ArticlePDF Available

Using Loan Plus Lender Literacy Information to Combat One-Sided Marketing of Debt Consolidation Loans

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The marketing of debt consolidation loans is intended to offer a financial remedy to consumers faced with mounting debt and credit problems and unable to meet their monthly payments. The authors argue that debt consolidation loan marketing overemphasizes the short-term benefits (e.g., lower monthly payments) and downplays the considerable downside of these loans (e.g., longer repayment and more total interest paid). Two experiments demonstrate that a financial literacy intervention combining information about loans and lenders can help consumers understand and respond to debt consolidation loan marketing (whereas a basic financial numeracy intervention does not). Implications for consumers, marketers, public policy makers, and researchers who work in the area of financial literacy are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
LISA E. BOLTON, PAUL N. BLOOM, and JOEL B. COHEN?
The marketing of debt consolidation loans is intended to offer a financial
remedy to consumers faced with mounting debt and credit problems and
unable to meet their monthly payments. The authors argue that debt con-
solidation loan marketing overemphasizes the short-term benefits (e.g.,
lower monthly payments) and downplays the considerable downside of
these loans (e.g., longer repayment and more total interest paid). Two
experiments demonstrate that a financial literacy intervention combining
information about loans and lenders can help consumers understand and
respond to debt consolidation loan marketing (whereas a basic financial
numeracy intervention does not). Implications for consumers, marketers,
public policy makers, and researchers who work in the area of financial
literacy are discussed.
Keywords:loan marketing, financial literacy, money management
Using Loan Plus Lender Literacy Information
to Combat One-Sided Marketing of Debt
Consolidation Loans
Although the availability of consumer credit has many
advantages for consumers and the economy, the downside
for consumers has become increasingly evident. Consumers
carry high-interest credit card balances, debt repayment
consumes an increasing proportion of income, savings rates
decline, and mortgage defaults and bankruptcy rates soar.
For example, personal bankruptcy rates were up almost
30% in 2008, and 14.7% of U.S. families had debt exceed-
ing 40% of their income in 2007 (before the recession)
(U.S. Congress Joint Economic Committee 2009). In 2007,
the average (median) balance for those carrying a balance
rose 30.4% (25%) to $7,300 ($3,000); in the preceding
three years, the average (median) rose 16.7% (9.1%) (Fed-
eral Reserve Board 2009). As these financial risks have
risen, the market has responded by offering “remedies” or
solutions to consumers’ financial problems.
*Lisa E. Bolton is Associate Professor of Marketing, Smeal College
of Business, Pennsylvania State University (boltonle@psu.edu). Paul N.
Bloom is Adjunct Professor of Social Entrepreneurship and Marketing,
Fuqua School of Business, Duke University (paul.bloom@duke.edu). Joel
B. Cohen is Distinguished Service Professor Emeritus, Warrington Col-
lege of Business, University of Florida (joel.cohen@warrington.ufl.edu).
The authors gratefully acknowledge funding support from the National
Endowment for Financial Education (www.NEFE.org). This research was
also funded in part by a Smeal Research Grant awarded to Lisa E. Bolton.
David Mick served as guest editor for this article.
Remedies refer to products or services designed to
reduce risk or offer solutions to problems in a variety
of substantive domains, such as health and security (e.g.,
smoking cessation aids, identity theft protection) (Bolton,
Cohen, and Bloom 2006). In the financial domain, reme-
dies include various offerings designed to help consumers
reduce financial risks by paying down debt—such as credit
counseling, home equity loans, payday loans, debt consoli-
dation, debt settlement, and other “solutions.” Perhaps iron-
ically, prior research has shown that a striking number of
these financial remedies involve loans—taking out debt to
solve problems associated with overspending and too much
debt. For example, home equity loans and lines of credit, as
well as mortgage advertising, are often promoted for debt
consolidation and credit problems (Perry and Motley 2009;
Williams 1999).
DEBT CONSOLIDATION LOAN MARKETING
The marketing of debt consolidation loans is intended to
offer a financial solution to consumers faced with mounting
debt and credit problems and worried about meeting their
monthly payments. Typically, such loan offers promise to
consolidate various consumer debts into a single loan, over
a longer period, and perhaps with a lower interest rate, thus
delivering a lower monthly payment. Although consolidat-
ing various loans at higher interest rates into a single loan
at a lower interest rate lowers monthly payments and may
alleviate immediate burdens for financially squeezed con-
sumers, there is a significant downside.
© 2011, American Marketing Association
ISSN: 0022-2437 (print), 1547-7193 (electronic) S51
Journal of Marketing Research
Vol. XLVIII (Special Issue 2011), S51–S59
S52 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, SPECIAL ISSUE 2011
The Downside of Debt Consolidation
An important downside of debt consolidation is the trade-
off between the short-term relief of lower monthly payments
and the long-term financial burdens, especially much higher
total interest paid. Although consumer financial literacy is
certainly part of the problem, our initial qualitative analysis
of loan advertising found few advertisements that mentioned
higher interest or longer loan terms (Bloom, Bolton, and
Cohen 2010). Instead, the majority ignored the downside
and emphasized lower monthly payments and made “hassle-
free” claims. These arguably one-sided claims may appeal
to consumers faced with looming debt problems whose state
of mind may induce them to downplay the negative aspects
of debt consolidation. In addition, many debt-burdened con-
sumers can only obtain consolidation loans with relatively
high interest rates rather than the strongly promoted lower
rates. Simply put, these loans over time may mean much
more debt, not less.
Another downside of debt consolidation occurs if the
financial remedy—by promising to manage or reduce risk—
undermines financially protective behavior. That is, why
avoid risk if a remedy exists to take care of the problem?
The net effect is a form of moral hazard or risk compensa-
tion (e.g., Calkins and Zlatoper 2001; Richens, Imrie, and
Copas 2000; Rodgers 1996; Rogers and Greenfield 1999),
in which consumers trade away some of the safety gains
of the remedy by continuing to engage in risky behavior
and even increase their exposure to risk. For example, rather
than minimizing use of credit cards, debt consolidation loan
marketing might lead consumers to increase spending lim-
its on their cards or even acquire additional cards. Our
research confirms this downside. In a sample of college stu-
dents, exposure to a debt consolidation loan message low-
ered financial risk perceptions and increased risky financial
behavioral intentions, especially among those most at risk
(Bolton, Cohen, and Bloom 2006). A similar pattern also
occurred among adult consumers exposed to real and fic-
titious advertisements making debt reduction or no-hassle
claims (Bloom, Bolton, and Cohen 2010). Thus, this down-
side of debt consolidation loan marketing seems robust and
widespread.
Whether the advantages of debt consolidation loans
outweigh their disadvantages is likely to vary across
consumers and situations. Some empirical evidence sug-
gests that these loans may leave consumers no better off
(Moorman and Garasky 2008; Williams 1999). Further-
more, these and other loan products and services promising
various forms of “debt relief” are receiving increasing regu-
latory scrutiny (Federal Trade Commission 2009; Goodman
2010).
Debt Consolidation Loans and Financial Literacy
This research develops an information-based interven-
tion to help consumers better understand debt consolidation
loans, especially their downside. Not surprisingly, consoli-
dation loan marketers tend to overpromote the benefits and
underplay the disadvantages; therefore, our efforts focus
on providing a more balanced view of these loans. Our
intervention approach focuses on two dimensions—loan-
focused and lender-focused.
Loan-focused literacy. A loan-focused approach attempts
to improve financial literacy by increasing consumer
knowledge about loans—how and why loans work and their
advantages and disadvantages. Prior research on financial
literacy has tended to focus on the quantitative aspects of
financial literacy, which we refer to as financial numer-
acy, such as the ability to correctly answer questions about
compound interest and inflation. For example, research
suggests that U.S. consumers exhibit low levels of finan-
cial numeracy skills and that higher skills are associated
with improved financial decisions, including saving, retire-
ment planning, and borrowing (Bernheim 1995; Lusardi
and Mitchell 2009; Lusardi, Mitchell, and Curto 2010;
Moore 2003). This arithmetic form of financial literacy is
broadly applicable across consumer personal financial deci-
sions, in contrast with some recent efforts to develop more
advanced or domain-specific measures of investment liter-
acy (Lusardi and Mitchell 2009) and debt literacy (Lusardi
and Tufano 2009). For example, debt literacy (measured by
consumer ability to correctly answer questions about com-
pound interest, paying off credit card debt with minimum
payments, and the time value of money) was strikingly low
as well as associated with self-reported overindebtedness.
However, research on financial literacy tends to rely on
correlational relationships, and causal evidence (e.g., for
the effectiveness of financial literacy programs) is lacking
and/or mixed (Braunstein and Welch 2002). For debt con-
solidation loans, the relationships among annual percentage
rate (APR), loan lengths, monthly payments, and total inter-
est paid are of particular interest and constitute what we
refer to as domain-specific literacy or “loan literacy.”
Lender-focused literacy. Lender-focused literacy refers
to consumer knowledge about lenders, including how and
why particular lenders act as they do. The emphasis on
the why is deliberate because it represents a previously
untapped dimension of financial literacy. For example,
consumers may categorize lenders as financial institutions
(which may follow different rules) rather than marketers
attempting to persuade them to make a particular loan pur-
chase. The Fannie Mae National Housing Survey (Fannie
Mae 2002) indicates that 39% of consumers erroneously
believe that lenders are obligated to give them the best
rate for which they qualify—which, among other things,
might lead consumers to search less than they should to
obtain the best loan terms. Moreover, if consumers fail
to categorize lenders as marketers, they are less apt to
bring to mind or make appropriate adjustments for more
generalized understanding of persuasion tactics. Friestad
and Wright (1994) refer to persuasion knowledge as a
schemer schema and propose that consumers can use such
knowledge to cope with persuasion attempts. More recently,
Wright (2002) argues that consumers have social intelli-
gence about the marketplace (i.e., marketplace metacogni-
tion) that they can use to interpret marketing tactics. Most
prior research has tended to (1) examine general persuasion
knowledge rather than tactics specific to a class of prod-
ucts or services as we do in this research and (2) rely on
measures of preexisting knowledge rather than manipula-
tions of it, thus failing to isolate this type of knowledge
from other knowledge as well as related consumer charac-
teristics (Brown and Krishna 2004; Campbell and Kirmani
2000; Friestad and Wright 1994; Williams, Fitzsimons, and
One-Sided Marketing of Debt Consolidation Loans S53
Block 2004). Given that lender literacy is likely to be low,
our lender-focused intervention is designed to improve con-
sumer knowledge about lender motives and behaviors while
activating marketplace metacognition.
EMPIRICAL OVERVIEW
Our basic premise is that both loan-focused and lender-
focused dimensions are needed to improve consumer
understanding of and response to debt consolidation loan
marketing. Loan literacy, by providing information about
how and why loans work, should help consumers evalu-
ate loan appeals and assess the degree to which consol-
idation loans will reduce debt. Likewise, lender literacy,
by providing information about lenders, should help con-
sumers correct mistaken beliefs about lenders and better
assess their claims. Indeed, we believe that both interven-
tions are necessary. That is, lender literacy might increase
skepticism about lenders and their claims, but consumers
will also require loan literacy to know how to improve their
evaluation of the loans themselves. (Viewed another way,
loan literacy might provide information helpful to evaluat-
ing the loans, but lender literacy will prompt a more care-
ful evaluation of loan claims made by lenders.) Thus, we
hypothesize that loan- and lender-focused approaches will
be most effective when combined.
We present two experiments to test the effectiveness of
loan- and lender-focused literacy on consumer response
to debt consolidation loan marketing. When testing effec-
tiveness, we argue that a successful intervention should
(1) increase intentions to engage in financially protective
behaviors, such as prudent money management (because
one downside of debt consolidation loan marketing is its
negative impact on such behaviors); (2) reduce the favora-
bility of loan evaluations (which are likely overoptimistic
given one-sided marketing of these loans); and (3) increase
the perceived importance of interest in loan decisions
(because total interest paid is virtually ignored in consol-
idation loan marketing). The latter two criteria represent
consequences for consumers in the market for debt consol-
idation, whereas the first criterion represents broader con-
sequences for consumers in general whether in the market
for loans or not.
EXPERIMENT 1: LOAN-FOCUSED AND
LENDER-FOCUSED LITERACY
In Experiment 1, we test the unique effects of loan-
focused and lender-focused literacy dimensions following
exposure to debt consolidation loan marketing. Because
we were less concerned with assessing the impact of our
eventual intervention than with understanding the separate
effects of each dimension (using exposure to typical debt
consolidation loan advertising by itself as a baseline), we
used a relatively homogeneous convenience sample of col-
lege students. College students are also an important popu-
lation in which to study the impact of loan marketing given
rising levels of debt and potential susceptibility to loan mar-
keting. (The average college graduate has nearly $20,000 in
debt; average credit card debt has increased 47% between
1989 and 2004 for 25- to 34-year-olds and 11% for 18- to
24-year-olds; and nearly one in five 18- to 24-year-olds is
in debt hardship, up from 12% in 1989 [Draut 2008].)
Method
Subjects and design. The experiment was a 2 (loan-
focused intervention/not) ×2 (lender-focused interven-
tion/not) between-subjects design. Participants were 127
undergraduate students who participated for extra credit
in an introductory marketing course, screened to eliminate
anyone with advanced finance coursework.
Materials and procedure. All participants were first
exposed to two advertising messages for debt consolida-
tion loans (a real and fictitious advertisement emphasiz-
ing the advantages of debt consolidation, including debt
reduction and no-hassle claims) to familiarize them with
such loans and provide a common baseline. In all condi-
tions, participants also read basic financial numeracy infor-
mation (titled “Loan Basics”) intended to be neutral and
factual in tone. This information included a description
of APR and an explanation and example of how loan
length affects monthly payments and total interest owed.
Doing this enables us to test whether loan- and lender-
focused dimensions of financial literacy have any added
value beyond basic financial numeracy.
Participants were then randomly assigned to receive
further information, either a loan-focused or a lender-
focused intervention or both. Participants who received a
loan-focused intervention message read information (titled
“Learn the Truth About Loans”) that indicated “When
loan repayment is stretched over time, you end up pay-
ing more overall interest” and that explained various loan
outcomes (e.g., not qualifying for lower interest rates and
winding up in more debt). Participants who received a
lender-focused intervention message then read information
(titled “Learn the Truth About Lenders”) that indicated
“Lenders are really sellers who act in their best inter-
est, not yours” and that explained various lender prac-
tices (e.g., hidden fees and penalties and deceptive prac-
tices). The conclusion drawn in each intervention message
stated “Don’t count on debt consolidation [loans/lenders]
to come to your rescue! When it comes to debt, these
[loans/lenders] aren’t the solution.” The interventions were
also similar in layout and graphics. After exposure to the
intervention, participants rated the information on two five-
point scales (with endpoints “uninformative/informative”
and “worthless/valuable”).
Participants then reported their money management
intentions on seven-point scales (with endpoints “definitely
will not/definitely will”) for the following items: “Pay off
the entire balance on credit cards each month,” “Keep three
months’ worth of living expenses as a cushion in savings,
and “Budget and track monthly living expenses.” Partici-
pants also indicated their evaluation of debt consolidation
loans on two seven-point scales (“ineffective/effective” and
“risky/safe”) and rated the importance of interest and non-
interest criteria when making loan decisions as follows:
“Imagine for a moment that you decide to take out a debt
consolidation loan. Please rate how important each of the
following are to you.” Participants then rated (on five-
point scales with endpoints “unimportant/important”) the
following items: “convenience of a single monthly pay-
ment,” “low monthly payments,” “long repayment period,
“low APR,” “low total interest paid,” and “easy applica-
tion process.”
S54 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, SPECIAL ISSUE 2011
Finally, participants responded to a ten-item quiz that
measures loan and lender financial literacy as a confirma-
tion of the intended effects of our interventions. The loan-
focused section included five multiple-choice questions
regarding loan terms and their effects on monthly payments
and interest owed. For example, participants were asked to
“imagine a debt of $1,000. How much will be owed after 5
years at an interest rate of 10% compounded annually” and
to select among six interval responses. The lender-focused
section included five true/false questions regarding lenders.
For example, participants were asked whether “lenders are
obligated to offer you the best loan terms possible given
your credit record.” We drew items from past financial lit-
eracy measures in the literature when possible.1
Results
We conducted analyses as a function of lender interven-
tion, loan intervention, and their interaction. Descriptive
means for key dependent variables appear in Table 1.
Manipulation checks. Ratings of the interventions indi-
cate that participants’ perceptions of their worth and value
did not differ (M =400560967; F <1). As intended, par-
ticipants’ scores on the financial literacy quiz revealed a
main effect of the loan intervention (Mloan =5015610307vs.
Mno-loan =4055610827; F4111235=4077, p<005); moreover,
the lender intervention marginally increased the propor-
tion of high-scoring participants on the lender-related items
(Õ2=3066, p=006). Stronger effects seem likely in a pop-
ulation lacking the education and sophistication of college
students; however, both achieved their intended purposes.
Money management. As expected, analysis of variance
(ANOVA) of money management intentions (coefficient
Á=076) revealed a two-way interaction of loan and lender
interventions 4F4111235=4081, p<005, partial ×2=0035.
1In our sampling of loan literacy levels, 24% of college students
answered the compound interest question described in the text correctly,
12% overestimated, and 64% underestimated. On the five-item loan liter-
acy quiz, 55% answered less than half correctly. In a convenience sample
of adults, 24% answered correctly, 25% overestimated, and 51% underes-
timated; on the five-item loan literacy quiz, 59% answered less than half
correctly. Although comparisons are fraught, Lusardi and Tufano (2008)
find that only 36% of adult consumers correctly answered their question
about compound interest loans. Turning to lender literacy, the Fannie Mae
National Housing Survey (Fannie Mae 2002) indicates that 39% of con-
sumers (erroneously) believe that lenders are obligated to give them the
best rate for which they qualify. In our own (convenience) sampling, 39%
of college students and 27% of adults held this erroneous belief.
Table 1
LOAN- AND LENDER-FOCUSED DIMENSIONS OF FINANCIAL
LITERACY (EXPERIMENT 1)
Financial Money Importance
Literacy Management Loan of Low
Intervention N Intentions Evaluation Interest
None (control
group) 34 5.92 (.85) 2.97 (1.15) 4.15 (.69)
Loan-focused 34 5.62 (1.30) 3.19 (1.46) 4.21 (.75)
Lender-focused 31 5.55 (1.50) 2.89 (1.38) 3.84 (1.13)
Loan- and
lender-focused 28 6.19 (1.11) 2.21 (1.18) 4.82 (.57)
In the presence of the lender intervention, follow-up simple
effects indicate that the loan intervention increased money
management intentions 4F4111235=4013, p<005, Cohen’s
d=0435; in its absence, the loan intervention had no effect
4F4111235=1007, p=030). These results indicate that the
interventions work best in concert to enhance intentions to
engage in financially protective responding.
Loan evaluations. The ANOVA of the loan ratings (coef-
ficient Á=086) revealed a main effect of the lender inter-
vention 4F4111235=5024, p<005), qualified by its interac-
tion with the loan intervention 4F4111235=3072, p=005,
partial ×2=002). In the presence of the lender inter-
vention, the loan intervention decreased loan evaluations
4F4111235=3094, p<005, Cohen’s d =049); in its absence,
the loan intervention had no effect (F <1). That is, loan and
lender interventions are both required to decrease favorable
evaluations of debt consolidation.
Loan decisions. For the data regarding loan decision
making, the ANOVA of the interest-related items (coef-
ficient Á=076) revealed a significant effect of the loan
intervention 4F4111235=5089, p<0055, qualified by its
interaction with the lender intervention 4F4111235=4008,
p<005, partial ×2=002). In the presence of the loan inter-
vention, the lender intervention increased the perceived
importance of interest rates 4F4111235=9022, p<001,
Cohen’s d =057); in the absence of the loan intervention,
the lender intervention had no effect (F <1). (A correspond-
ing analysis of noninterest items is nonsignificant; p>022.)
Again, loan and lender interventions work best in concert
to improve loan decision making, specifically by increasing
the perceived importance of low interest.
In summary, a combined loan- and lender-focused inter-
vention increased money management intentions, reduced
loan evaluations (which were likely overoptimistic given
the one-sided marketing of these loans), and improved loan
decision making (by increasing the perceived importance of
low interest). Thus, loan and lender literacy together have
the potential to improve personal financial responding by
consumers. Strikingly, single interventions had no effect,
making it less likely that the mere increase in the amount
of information explains the success of our combined inter-
vention. Instead, we argue that loan-focused interventions
provide consumers with the knowledge to evaluate loans
more appropriately but that lender-focused interventions are
also needed to help consumers respond to the marketing
claims made by lenders.
EXPERIMENT 2: TESTING A COMBINED
INTERVENTION
The primary goal of Experiment 2 is to test the effi-
cacy of an intervention combining loan- and lender-focused
dimensions in a field sample of adult consumers. As a
secondary objective, we also compare the combined inter-
vention with an intervention providing only basic financial
numeracy information (i.e., explaining APR and com-
pounding interest over time). From a policy perspective, the
latter may be considered less intrusive and also consistent
with the kinds of disclosure information already used for
some other financial products (e.g., credit card disclosures).
It thus serves as a theoretically and substantively relevant
control group.
One-Sided Marketing of Debt Consolidation Loans S55
Method
Subjects and design. The experiment was a three-
group (no intervention, financial numeracy only, finan-
cial numeracy +combined intervention) between-subjects
design. Participants were 132 adult consumers drawn from
a commercial panel who owned at least one credit card
and who had not previously purchased a debt consolidation
loan. The sample skewed slightly toward women (55%),
with desirable heterogeneity of age (median 45–54 years),
income (median $40,000–49,999), and education (median
four-year college).
Materials and procedure. All participants were first
exposed to an advertising message we created for debt
consolidation loans that embodied central characteristics
of actual advertisements in emphasizing the advantages
of debt consolidation, including debt reduction and no-
hassle claims. Participants then provided their opinion of
the debt consolidation program featured in the advertise-
ment (on three seven-point scales with endpoints “a bad
idea/a good idea,” “ineffective/effective,” and “risky/safe”),
intended as a baseline measure of attitudes toward debt
consolidation after ad exposure and before the intervention.
Participants were then exposed to one of three differ-
ent intervention levels. Some participants were exposed
to basic financial numeracy information, and some
participants were exposed to this basic numeracy infor-
mation and a combined intervention emphasizing loan-
and lender-focused dimensions of financial literacy (as in
Experiment 1). The remaining participants received no
intervention and served as an ad-only control group for
comparison purposes.
Participants then reported their money management
intentions on seven-point scales (with endpoints “very
unlikely/very likely”) for the following items: “Pay off the
entire balance on credit cards each month,” “Keep three
months’ worth of living expenses as a cushion in sav-
ings,” “Budget and track monthly living expenses,” “Refi-
nance to extend the term of outstanding loans” (reverse
coded), “Take out a new loan when existing loans have
not been paid off” (reverse coded), and “Spend money in
advance of receiving it (e.g., buy-now-pay-later, pay-day
borrowing” (reverse coded). Participants also indicated their
evaluation of debt consolidation loans on four seven-point
scales (“negative/positive,” “a bad idea/a good idea,” “inef-
fective/effective,” and “risky/safe”). Participants also rated
the importance of interest and noninterest criteria when
making loan decisions (see Experiment 1 for wording).
Finally, participants responded to various background ques-
tions, including education and income (used as covariates
for control purposes).
Results
We conducted analyses as a function of the interven-
tion, post-ad-exposure debt consolidation attitudes (coeffi-
cient Á=096; standardized M =01SD =1), and their interac-
tion. Descriptive means for key dependent variables appear
in Table 2.
Money management. The ANOVA of money manage-
ment intentions (coefficient Á=077) revealed a signif-
icant effect of intervention condition 4F4211225=3054,
p<0055, a nonsignificant effect of post-ad attitudes (F <1),
Table 2
TESTING A COMBINED INTERVENTION (EXPERIMENT 2)
Money Interest
Corrective Management Loan Importance
Intervention N Intentions Evaluation (Relative)
None (control 48 5.68 (1.09) 2.93 (1.87) .71 (.84)
group) b = −041 (.17) b =1054 (.21) b = −023 (.12)
Basic financial 42 5.40 (1.27) 3.30 (1.47) .64 (.71)
numeracy b =01840205b=074 (.24) b = −018 (.14)
Loan- and 42 6.13 (1.06) 2.47 (1.92) .83 (.79)
lender-focused b =002 (.16) b =094 (.19) b =013 (.11)
Notes: Means, standard deviations, and coefficients for the initial con-
solidation attitude covariate are shown for each condition.
and their expected two-way interaction 4F4211225=2097,
p=005, partial ×2=0035.2For the no-intervention ad-only
condition, the coefficient for the attitude covariate was neg-
ative and significant (b = −04160177, t = −2044, p<005).
That is, more favorable attitudes toward debt consolidation
after exposure to a debt consolidation loan advertisement
led to a decline in intentions to manage money carefully—
consistent with the downside that such one-sided marketing
undermines financially protective behaviors.
We then conducted a spotlight analysis at more and
less favorable post-ad-exposure debt consolidation attitudes
(i.e., +1 SD and 1 SD) to understand the nature of
the two-way interaction. For the contrast of the combined
intervention versus no-intervention, the combined interven-
tion had no effect on money management intentions when
post-ad attitudes were already less favorable (i.e., 1 SD;
6.09 vs. 6.08, t =012, p=091) but, as intended, increased
money management intentions when attitudes were more
favorable (i.e., +1 SD; 6.12 vs. 5.22, t =2069, p<005,
Cohen’s d =083). However, by itself, the financial numer-
acy intervention actually lowered prudent money manage-
ment intentions (compared with the no-intervention control
group) when post-ad-exposure consolidation attitudes were
less favorable (i.e., 1 SD; 5.28 vs. 6.05, t = −2022, p<005,
Cohen’s d =071) and had no effect when consolidation atti-
tudes were more favorable (+1 SD; 5.65 vs. 5.22, t =1016,
p=025). These results suggest that an intervention com-
bining loan and lender literacy dimensions can successfully
encourage money management intentions (which are under-
mined when debt consolidation loan attitudes are favor-
able), in marked contrast with a basic financial numeracy
intervention (which seems to inadvertently exacerbate this
downside among people whose attitudes are less favorable).
Loan evaluations. The ANOVA of postintervention loan
evaluation ratings (coefficient Á=097) revealed an effect of
intervention condition 4F4211225=4086, p<0055, an effect
of post-ad-exposure attitudes 4F4111225=73038, p<0015,
and their expected two-way interaction 4F4211225=3065,
p<005, partial ×2=0025. For the no-intervention condi-
tion, the coefficient for the attitude covariate was significant
2For completeness’ sake, education and income effects were nonsignif-
icant (p>015) for all dependent variables with one exception: The high-
est levels of education (i.e., graduate-level vs. undergraduate or lower)
increased money management intentions 4F4311225=2058, p=0065.
S56 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, SPECIAL ISSUE 2011
(b =105460217, t =7045, p<001). That is, as post-ad atti-
tudes toward debt consolidation increased, loan evaluations
also increased.
We then conducted a spotlight analysis for more and
less favorable post-ad-exposure debt consolidation attitudes
(i.e., +1 SD and 1 SD) to understand the nature of the two-
way interaction. For the contrast of the combined interven-
tion versus no-intervention, the combined intervention had
no effect on loan evaluations when post-ad attitudes were
less favorable (i.e., 1 SD; 1.25 vs. 1.42, t = −041, p=068)
and, importantly, reduced loan evaluations when attitudes
were more favorable (i.e., +1 SD; 3.13 vs. 4.49, t = −3033,
p<001, Cohen’s d =073). Conversely, relative to the no-
intervention ad-only control group, the financial numer-
acy intervention increased loan evaluations when post-ad
attitudes were less favorable (i.e., 1 SD; 2.31 vs. 1.42,
t= −2012, p<005, Cohen’s d =048) and had no effect when
attitudes were more favorable (i.e., +1 SD; 3.80 vs. 4.49,
t= −1056, p=012). This undesirable effect of basic finan-
cial numeracy information is similar to that observed for
money management intentions. These results indicate that
an intervention combining loan and lender literacy dimen-
sions successfully targeted and reduced favorable attitudes
toward debt consolidation after exposure to debt consoli-
dation loan advertising, while the basic financial numeracy
intervention did not and sometimes increased debt consol-
idation attitudes.
Loan decisions. The ANOVA of the index reflecting the
perceived importance of interest versus noninterest items
(coefficient Á=080 and .76, respectively) in making loan
decisions revealed an interaction between intervention con-
dition and post-ad attitudes 4F4211225=3004, p=005, par-
tial ×2=0035; intervention condition and post-ad attitudes
were nonsignificant (respectively, F <1; F4111225=1063,
p=020). For the no-intervention ad-only condition, the
coefficient for the post-ad-attitude covariate was negative
(b = −02360127, t = −1091, p=006). That is, as attitudes
toward debt consolidation increased following ad exposure,
the relative importance of interest as a decision criterion
declined. This result is consistent with the one-sided nature
of such advertising, which emphasizes the advantages of
debt consolidation loans (e.g., lower monthly payments and
no-hassle claims) while underplaying the importance of
lower interest.
We then conducted a spotlight analysis at more and
less favorable post-ad-exposure debt consolidation attitudes
(i.e., +1 SD and 1 SD) to understand the nature of the two-
way interaction. Again, the combined intervention (versus
no-intervention) had no effect on interest importance when
post-ad attitudes were less favorable (i.e., 1 SD; .81 vs.
1.04, t = − 1003, p=031) but, as intended, increased the rela-
tive importance of interest when attitudes were more favor-
able (i.e., +1 SD; 1.07 vs. .59, t =2008, p<005, Cohen’s
d=057). The financial numeracy intervention had no effect
on interest importance when post-ad-exposure consolida-
tion attitudes were less favorable (i.e., 1 SD; .97 vs. 1.04,
t= −028, p=078) or more favorable (i.e., +1 SD; .61 vs.
.59, t =007, p=094). Thus, the intervention combining loan
and lender literacy dimensions successfully improved loan
decision making by increasing the perceived importance of
interest; the basic financial numeracy intervention did not.
In summary, more favorable debt consolidation attitudes
following ad exposure are associated with less prudent
money management intentions and the reduced impor-
tance of interest—consistent with the undesirable effects of
debt consolidation loan marketing. An intervention provid-
ing basic financial numeracy information had no desirable
effects and may even backfire by creating more favorable
loan attitudes. It would seem that something more than
financial numeracy information is needed to help con-
sumers respond to fairly one-sided debt consolidation loan
marketing, perhaps because such information is perceived
as relatively neutral, more challenging, and less imme-
diately relevant. In contrast, an intervention comprising
loan- and lender-focused information was successful. The
combined intervention undermined favorable loan evalu-
ations and improved financially protective behaviors and
loan-related decision making (through the perceived impor-
tance of low interest).
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The main contributions of this research are twofold.
First, we provide evidence that debt consolidation loan
marketing can have potentially harmful consequences on
consumers’ personal finances. Specifically, favorable debt
consolidation loan attitudes undermine financially protec-
tive behaviors (e.g., prudent money management) and
worsen loan decisions (e.g., by reducing the perceived
importance of low interest when choosing a loan). We
show that a well-constructed financial literacy intervention
improves these responses and helps correct unreasonably
favorable debt consolidation loan attitudes that are based
on one-sided information. Second, we expand the litera-
ture on financial literacy by (1) decomposing literacy into
two dimensions, loan- and lender-focused (the latter almost
ignored until now), and (2) manipulating these dimensions
rather than relying on measurement to demonstrate their
combined efficacy. As expected, a combined intervention
helps consumers respond to debt consolidation marketing—
by overcoming more favorable loan attitudes, encouraging
money management intentions, and improving loan deci-
sions by increasing the perceived importance of interest.
Financial Products and Financial Literacy
Low levels of financial literacy are a matter of increasing
global concern (Organisation for Economic Co-operation
and Development 2008). Although most measures of finan-
cial literacy typically focus on basic concepts (e.g., under-
standing inflation and compound interest), consumers need
to make complex decisions regarding cash flow manage-
ment, credit, savings and retirement, and home ownership.
Meanwhile, there has been an explosion of financial prod-
ucts, and these products have grown increasingly com-
plex, with the risk and responsibility for decisions about
them being shifted toward consumers (e.g., choice in retire-
ment savings) (Organisation for Economic Co-operation
and Development 2008). Lack of financial literacy has been
linked to problems with debt and lower levels of retirement
planning and saving (Lusardi and Mitchell 2007; Lusardi
and Tufano 2009; Van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie 2007).
Some of these problems reflect basic numeracy difficul-
ties, such as poor understanding of interest rates (Lee and
Hogarth 1999) and a general tendency to underestimate
One-Sided Marketing of Debt Consolidation Loans S57
exponential growth and thereby undersave and overborrow
(Eisenstein and Hoch 2005; Stango and Zinman 2008).
Although we agree that such numeracy difficulties are prob-
lematic, our research suggests that basic financial numeracy
is insufficient to help consumers understand and respond to
more specific financial products, including debt consolida-
tion loans.
Moreover, financial problems may also arise from
“lay theories” of personal finance (see Molden and
Dweck 2006), including widespread and harmful “con-
sumer finance myths” (Emmons 2004). These lay theo-
ries may be especially harmful when the downside of
financial remedies are not understood. For example, the
myth that “all that matters is your monthly payment”
leads people to favor lower interest from longer duration
loans, thus pushing payment burdens into the future. In
the case of borrowing, the monthly payment myth coin-
cides with the belief that resources will be less constrained
in the future (Zauberman and Lynch 2005). Similarly, the
myth that lenders are obligated to offer the lowest rate
for which consumers qualify may reduce consumer search
and lead consumers to accept suboptimal loan terms. As
our work attests, a financial literacy intervention that tar-
gets such myths, specifically erroneous lay beliefs about
loans and lenders, can improve financial decision making.
These myths or lay theories are also evident in the heuris-
tics and biases that drive underinvesting for retirement
(Bernartzi and Thaler 2007) and, more generally, represent
a promising avenue for consumer research to understand
(and improve) personal financial behaviors.
That being said, improving financial literacy seems chal-
lenging. The little research that exists suggests that the
effectiveness of financial literacy programs is poor and/or
uncertain (for a review, see Lusardi 2004; Lusardi and
Mitchell 2007; see also Bell, Gorin, and Hogarth 2009;
Cole and Shastry 2009). Importantly, some specific pro-
grams that have shown promise (e.g., Quick Enrollment,
Save More Tomorrow; Choi, Laibson, and Madrian 2006;
Keller and Lusardi 2012; Thaler and Benartzi 2004) may
work primarily by simplifying decisions (e.g., through
defaults and planning aids) rather than improving financial
literacy. Our research also speaks to this challenge insofar
as an intervention to improve loan literacy (i.e., information
about loan terms and the dis/advantages of consolidation
loans) had no impact unless coupled with an intervention
to improve lender literacy (i.e., information about lender
motives and tactics). Although these findings suggest that
efforts to improve personal financial behaviors are difficult,
we note that the combined approach was successful, imply-
ing that an emphasis on both dimensions in financial edu-
cation programs might increase their effectiveness. Indeed,
the low rates of lender literacy observed herein suggest
that consumers may view lenders as agents acting in con-
sumers’ interests, rather than as sellers acting in their own
self-interest. Questionable marketing may contribute to this
perception with appeals that encourage trust combined with
deceptive practices, such as appearing to offer credit coun-
seling or signaling nonprofit status when the firm has other
objectives. A better understanding of marketplace metacog-
nition and lay theories of personal finance—regarding loans
and lenders or, more generally, financial products and their
providers—would be of great benefit in understanding how
the marketing of financial products intended to help con-
sumers can sometimes lead to harm.
Limitations
We acknowledge several limitations in this research pro-
gram. First, we rely on self-report measures of inten-
tions rather than actual behavior. Studying the impact of
loan marketing and interventions on actual behavior over
a period sufficient to encompass changing economic and
personal conditions would be valuable but is, of course,
challenging. To obtain added insights, we conducted a
survey using a commercial financial panel of U.S. con-
sumers and found correlational evidence linking more favor-
able attitudes toward debt consolidation with lower FICO
scores (Bloom, Bolton, and Cohen 2010). Second, our loan-
focused and lender-focused interventions were multifaceted,
and further research might be useful to pinpoint which
aspect of the information within each intervention had the
most impact. It is perhaps not surprising that an effortful
intervention might be necessary to affect entrenched behav-
iors in the domain of personal finances. Investigating the
longer-term impact of such interventions is warranted.
Implications
This research has several pragmatic implications. First,
consumers in serious financial difficulty who are consid-
ering debt consolidation loans may be especially vulner-
able to current promotional practices, given low levels of
financial literacy and the motivation to downplay longer-
term negative aspects of debt consolidation (heightened
by fears of immediate creditor actions). Findings showing
that both loan and lender literacy are needed to improve
financial decision making can be used by organizations
that currently advocate for and provide consumer finan-
cial education (e.g., the National Endowment for Financial
Education; credit counselors, schools, churches, and other
educators) to create and/or improve informational interven-
tions. Credit counselors are particularly valuable to edu-
cate distressed consumers who are targeted by marketers
promising “debt relief.” For that reason, we have developed
an audiovisual form of our intervention that is currently
under trial by a credit counseling agency that provides
financial literacy seminars to consumers. Second, increas-
ing awareness of why and how consumers are seduced
by the apparent remedy of debt consolidation loans may
increase the willingness of third parties to provide finan-
cial education about loan marketing. For example, the mil-
itary (arguably a leader in this area) provides financial
literacy programs during training, universities are already
involved in providing financial assistance to students (and
may want to reconsider their collaboration with debt con-
solidation marketing), and large employers could expand
existing training that is part of benefits or employee assis-
tance. (More generally, though, we note that the question
of how to deliver financial literacy education effectively
to consumers remains a challenge—as the current state of
financial literacy attests.) Finally, responsible lenders, when
put on notice as to serious consequences for consumers due
to overpromoting the benefits of debt consolidation while
underplaying downside consequences, might want to con-
sider more balanced marketing approaches, perhaps as a
S58 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, SPECIAL ISSUE 2011
way to differentiate themselves from less legitimate com-
petition and/or to gain broader support from policy makers
and regulators, such as the newly created Consumer Finan-
cial Protection Bureau.
REFERENCES
Bell, Catherine, Daniel Gorin, and Jeanne Hogarth (2009), “Does
Financial Education Affect Soldiers’ Financial Behaviors?”
Networks Financial Institute Working Paper 2009-WP-08.
Benartzi, Shlomo and Richard H. Thaler (2007), “Heuristics and
Biases in Retirement Savings Behavior,Journal of Economic
Perspectives, 21 (3), 81–104.
Bernheim, Douglas (1995), “Do Households Appreciate Their
Financial Vulnerabilities? An Analysis of Actions, Percep-
tions, and Public Policy,” in Tax Policy and Economic Growth,
Washington, DC: American Council for Capital Formation,
1–30.
Bloom, Paul N., Lisa E. Bolton, and Joel B. Cohen (2010), “Help-
ing Consumers Respond Responsibly to the Advertising and
Availability of Debt Consolidation Loans,” Interim Reports to
the National Endowment for Financial Education.
Bolton, Lisa E., Joel B. Cohen, and Paul N. Bloom (2006), “Does
Marketing Products as Remedies Create “Get Out of Jail Free
Cards’?” Journal of Consumer Research, 33 (June), 71–81.
Braunstein, Sandra and Carolyn Welch (2002), “Financial Liter-
acy: An Overview of Practice, Research, and Policy,” Federal
Reserve Bulletin, 88 (November), 445–57.
Brown, Christina L. and Aradhna Krishna (2004), “The Skep-
tical Shopper: A Metacognitive Account for the Effects of
Default Options on Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research,
31 (December), 529–39.
Calkins, Lindsay Noble and Thomas J. Zlatoper (2001), “The
Effects of Mandatory Seat Belt Laws on Motor Vehicle Fatal-
ities in the U.S.,” Social Science Quarterly, 82 (December),
716–32.
Campbell, Margaret C. and Amna Kirmani (2000), “Consumers’
Use of Persuasion Knowledge: The Effects of Accessibility
and Cognitive Capacity on Perceptions of an Influence Agent,
Journal of Consumer Research, 27 (June), 69–83.
Choi, James J., David Laibson, and Brigitte C. Madrian (2006),
“Reducing the Complexity Costs of 401(k) Participation
Through Quick Enrollment,” NBER Working Paper 11979
(January).
Cole, Shawn A. and Gauri Kartini Shastry (2009), “Smart Money:
The Effect of Education, Cognitive Ability, and Financial Lit-
eracy on Financial Market Participation,” Harvard Business
School Finance Working Paper No. 09-071 (February).
Draut, Tamara (2008), “Economic State of Young America,
(May 6), [available at http://archive.demos.org/pub1568.cfm].
Eisenstein, Eric M. and Stephen J. Hoch (2005), “Intuitive Com-
pounding: Framing, Temporal Perspective, and Expertise,” work-
ing paper, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania.
Emmons, William R. (2004), “Consumer-Finance Myths and
other Obstacles to Financial Literacy,” Federal Reserve Bank
of St. Louis Working Paper 2005-03.
Fannie Mae (2002), “Fannie Mae National Housing Survey: The
Growing Demand for Housing,” (accessed June 29, 2011), [avail-
able at http://www.fanniemae.com/global/pdf/media/survey/
survey2002.pdf].
Federal Reserve Board (2009), “Survey of Consumer Finances
2007,” (February), (accessed June 29, 2011), [available at http://
www.federalreserve.gov/pubs/bulletin/2009/pdf/scf09.pdf].
Federal Trade Commission (2009), “Proposed Rules: Telemarket-
ing Sales Rule,” 16 CFR Part 310, Federal Register, 16 (19),
4669–79.
Friestad, Marian and Peter Wright (1994), “The Persua-
sion Knowledge Model: How People Cope with Persuasion
Attempts,” Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (June), 1–31.
Goodman, Peter S. (2010), “Peddling Relief, Firms Put Debtors in
a Deeper Hole,” New York Times, (June 18), [available at http://
www.nytimes.com/2010/06/19/business/economy/19debt.html].
Keller, Punam Anand and Annamaria Lusardi (2012), “Employee
Retirement Savings: What We Know and What We Are Dis-
covering for Helping People to Prepare for Life After Work,”
in Transformative Consumer Research for Personal and Collec-
tive Well-Being: Reviews and Frontiers, David Mick, Simone
Pettigrew, Connie Pechmann, and Julie Ozanne, eds. New York:
Routledge.
Lee, Jinkook and J.M. Hogarth (1999), “The Price of Money:
Consumers’ Understanding of APRs and Contract Interest
Rates,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 18 (1), 66–76.
Lusardi, Annamaria (2004), “Savings and the Effectiveness of
Financial Education,” in Pension Design and Structure: New
Lessons from Behavioral Finance, Olivia S. Mitchell and
Stephen Utkus, eds. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 157–84.
and Olivia Mitchell (2007), “Financial Literacy and
Retirement Preparedness,” Business Economics, 42 (1), 35–44.
and (2009), “How Ordinary Consumers
Make Complex Economic Decisions: Financial Literacy and
Retirement Readiness,” NBER Working Paper No. 15350
(September).
, , and Vilsa Curto (2010), “Financial Literacy
Among the Young: Evidence and Implications for Consumer
Policy,” NBER Working Paper No. 15352 (September).
and Peter Tufano (2009), “Debt Literacy, Financial Expe-
riences and Overindebtedness,” NBER Working Paper 14808
(March).
Molden, Daniel C. and Carol S. Dweck (2006), “Finding
‘Meaning’ in Psychology: A Lay Theories Approach to
Self-Regulation, Social Perception, and Social Development,
American Psychologist, 61 (3), 192–203.
Moore, Danna (2003), “Survey of Financial Literacy in
Washington State: Knowledge, Behavior, Attitudes and Experi-
ences,” Technical Report 03-39, Social and Economic Sciences
Research Center, Washington State University.
Moorman, Diann C. and Steven Garasky (2008), “Consumer Debt
Repayment Behavior as a Precursor to Bankruptcy,” Journal of
Family and Economic Issues, 29 (June), 219–33.
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (2008),
“Taking Financial Literacy to the Next Level: Important Chal-
lenges and Promising Solutions,” in OECD-US Treasury Interna-
tional Conference on Financial Education Proceedings, Vol. 1.
Washington, DC: 7–8, [available at www.oecd.org/dataoecd/47/
15/41635334.pdf].
Perry, Vanessa G. and Carol M. Motley (2009), “Where’s the Fine
Print? Advertising and the Mortgage Market Crisis,California
Management Review, 51 (1), 29–44.
Richens, John, John Imrie, and Andrew Copas (2000), “Condoms
and Seat Belts: The Parallels and the Lessons,Lancet, 355
(January 29), 400–403.
Rodgers, Gregory B. (1996), “Bicyclist Risks and Helmet Usage
Patterns: An Analysis of Compensatory Behavior in a Risky
Recreational Activity,” Managerial and Decision Economics,
17 (September–October), 493–507.
One-Sided Marketing of Debt Consolidation Loans S59
Rogers, John D. and Thomas K. Greenfield (1999), “Beer Drink-
ing Accounts for Most of the Hazardous Alcohol Consumption
in the U.S.,” Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 60 (November),
732–39.
Stango, Victor and Jonathan Zinman (2008), “Exponential Growth
Bias and Household Finance,” Journal of Finance, 64 (6),
2807–49.
Thaler, Richard and S. Benartzi (2004), “Save More Tomorrow:
Using Behavioral Economics to Increase Employee Saving,
Journal of Political Economy, 112 (S1), S164–87.
U.S. Congress Joint Economic Committee (2009), “Vicious Cycle:
How Unfair Credit Card Company Practices Are Squeez-
ing Consumers and Undermining the Recovery,” (May 12),
[available at http://jec.senate.gov/public/?a=Files.Serve&File_id
=42840b23-fed8-447b-a029-e977c0a25544].
Van Rooij, Maarten, Annamaria Lusardi, and Rob Alessie (2007),
“Financial Literacy and Stock Market Participation,” Michigan
Retirement Research Center Research Paper No. 2007-162.
Williams, Flora L. (1999), “Costs and Benefits of Loan Consoli-
dation,” Financial Counseling and Planning, 10 (2), 61–73.
Williams, Patti, Gavan J. Fitzsimons, and Lauren G. Block (2004),
“When Consumers Don’t Recognize ‘Benign’ Intentions Ques-
tions as Persuasive Attempts,Journal of Consumer Research,
31 (December), 540–51.
Wright, Peter (2002), “Marketplace Metacognition and Social
Intelligence,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (March),
677–82.
Zauberman, Gal and John G. Lynch (2005), “Resource Slack
and Propensity to Discount Delayed Investments of Time Ver-
sus Money,Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
134 (1), 23–37.
Copyright of Journal of Marketing Research (JMR) is the property of American Marketing Association and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
... These results corroborate previous studies on the relationship between an individual's financial issues and the tendency to indebtedness, particularly the role of attitude (Carpena et al., 2016). As the attitude is formed over time, this study shows that actions that can create more conscious financial attitudes are of great importance in indebtedness mitigation, such as financial education actions , for example (Bolton et al., 2011, Gathergood, 2012, Fernandes et al., 2014, Huston, 2016 (1981), this study identified financial attitude as a construct capable of mitigating debt when less conscious relationships with money occur . ...
... Na exploração inicial dos (Carpena et al., 2016). Na medida em que a atitude é formada ao longo do tempo, este estudo mostra que ações que possam criar atitudes financeiras mais conscientes são de grande importância na mitigação do endividamento, como por exemplo, ações de educação financeira (Bolton et al., 2011, Gathergood , 2012, Fernandes et al., 2014, Huston, 2016 ...
... Ou seja, o desenvolvimento de convicções advindas do conhecimento e da vida financeira, que permitam gerenciar melhor seus gastos e atingir o equilíbrio financeiro, mitigando o risco de inadimplência(Norvilitis et al., 2006, Piccini & Pinzetta, 2014, Figueira & Pereira, 2014, Campara et al., 2016. Não basta adquirir o conhecimento financeiro, é necessário que ele possa se tornar convicção e atitude em relação ao uso de dinheiro(Bolton et al., 2011, Carpena et al., 2016, na medida em que a atitude é preditora do comportamento, até que o próprio comportamento possa ser um melhor preditor da sua própria repetição ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of the study: To estimate the degree of relationships between financial attitude, materialism, consumption by status, and consumer perception of the chosen payment method, and impacts on indebtedness.Methodology / approach: We used the survey technique, which collected data from 161 respondents. The quantitative data obtained were analyzed through structural equation modeling, as well as conditional mediation and moderation tests.Main results: Individuals with weak financial attitudes tend to be more materialistic and to consume more by status, and individuals with materialistic and status-driven tendencies are more prone to debt. No relationship was identified between the chosen means of payment and indebtedness. On the other hand, the mediation of the financial attitude in debt mitigation was observed.Originality / relevance: Despite the vast literature on the theories cited in the paper, the relationship between the financial attitude and the buying behavior of the Brazilian consumer had not yet been established, from an attitude perspective as a predictor of behavior.Theoretical / methodological contributions: The work contributes to studies on the inversely proportional advance relationship between financial attitude, status consumption and materialism, in which the latter constructs are discouraged by the individual's trait with that first characteristic. The study also identified the mediation of financial attitude as a debt reduction mechanism.
... While for dispersed accounts, the bills are considered one by one, emphasizing each value (KETTLE et al., 2016). These two perspectives have different effects on subsequent behavior as they help consumers organize personal finances (BOLTON et al., 2011;ORMAN, 2000;RAMSEY, 2003), choose better how to allocate debt repayments among multiple bills (AMAR et al. 2011;SOLL et al., 2013) and increasing motivation to get out of debts (KETTLE et al., 2016). ...
... Because of some inconsistencies, the research sought to investigate how consumers assess their financial situation. Moreover, how it could lead to subsequent behavior, since motivational behavior, psychological factors, financial literacy (BOLTON et al., 2011), available information in credit card statements (SOLL et al., 2013), minimum payment (NAVARRO et al., 2011), and management of bills concentrated or dispersed as small or completely tasks (JIN et al., 2015;GAL et al., 2012;BROWN et al., 2015;AMAR et al., 2016;ORMAN, 2000;RAMSEY, 2003), the influence of the type of debt/source-of-fundings on purchase preferences (TRULLY, 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Pesquisas anteriores sugerem que a concentração da dívida afeta a motivação dos consumidores e leva às melhores decisões financeiras. No entanto, pouco se sabe sobre sua relação com a presença de opções de compra, especificamente, escolhas materiais versus experiências. Para preencher esta lacuna, este estudo propõe investigar como a estratégia de gestão da dívida (concentrada vs. dispersa) influencia as preferências experienciais versus bens materiais. Dois experimentos foram conduzidos no laboratório e Mturk. Os principais achados sugerem evidências de que a concentração de vencimentos influencia os consumidores a pagar mais saldos de cartão de crédito. No entanto, esse efeito de concentração é atenuado pela distância temporal. Além disso, contas dispersas levam os consumidores a gastar mais, não com experiências como previa, mas com mercadorias. Os resultados sugerem que a concentração da dívida leva os consumidores a preferir bens materiais em detrimento de experiências. Isso pode ajudar os consumidores a perceber que fatores externos podem influenciar seu processo de compra mais do que as características das compras e pode ser útil para os consumidores adotarem uma estratégia de concentração de datas de vencimento para organizar melhor suas finanças. Os resultados contribuem para o desenvolvimento de teorias sobre como a gestão da dívida influencia o comportamento subsequente. Além disso, foi demonstrado como um novo antecedente para as preferências de compra de material (vs. experiência).
... Las escasas competencias financieras incrementan además el coste de la deuda (Huston, 2012), la probabilidad de impago y el sobreendeudamiento (Gathergood, 2012). Por otro lado, la cultura financiera desincentiva la contratación de créditos al consumo (Disney y Gathergood, 2013) y la consolidación de deudas (Bolton et al. 2011); mientras que incrementa la probabilidad de detectar fraudes y prácticas abusivas por parte de las entidades financieras y a denunciarlas (Aguiar-Diaz et al. 2023). ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
Este estudio describe los principales avances en el estudio de las implicaciones y los determinantes de la educación y las competencias financieras en diferentes temáticas abordadas en la literatura académica. Desde la perspectiva de las implicaciones de la educación financiera en la riqueza de los individuos, la literatura resalta la importancia de la cultura financiera en la propensión al ahorro y la planificación financiera, así como en las decisiones de inversión y endeudamiento. Las competencias financieras fomentan la canalización del ahorro hacia la inversión y permite al individuo obtener financiación a coste bajo, contribuyendo así a que pueda mantener un nivel de endeudamiento responsable. Diversos son los determinantes de la educación y las competencias financieras de la población, siendo los más estudiados aquellos que tienen que ver con las características económicas y sociodemográficas de los individuos. Así, la formación académica, el nivel de ingresos, la edad, el género, la situación profesional o las características de la unidad familiar determinan el nivel de cultura financiera de los individuos. Este análisis permite a su vez identificar colectivos de especial interés a los que dirigir de manera prioritaria los programas e iniciativas de educación financiera: los jóvenes, los mayores y otros colectivos vulnerables. Por otro lado, se analiza y describe el actual debate en torno a la existencia y las causas de una brecha de género en la cultura financiera de la población. Por último, el estudio pone en relieve la importancia de la educación y la cultura financiera en ámbito empresarial, describiendo los principales resultados obtenidos por la literatura académica que aborda el impacto de la cultura financiera en las estrategias y en los resultados empresariales. Este estudio, en definitiva, hace un recorrido por las principales enseñanzas extraídas de la literatura académica sobre educación financiera, permitiendo a su vez, identificar nuevos retos en la materia tanto para la academia como para profesionales, empresas, organismos multilaterales, supervisores y responsables políticos.
... Given that people with this type are strategical managers who enjoy meticulously developing their long-range plans, debt consolidation can be interpreted as a method they carefully planned and selected. It supports the existing study of Bolton et al. (2011), who argued that although debt consolidation can be an effective way of borrowing for consumers who have accumulated debt or have credit problems, they must examine their newly adjusted debt punctiliously. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although psychometric features have been considered for alternative credit scoring, they have not yet been applied to peer-to-peer (P2P) lending because such information is not available on platforms. This study proposed an alternative credit scoring model for P2P lending by extracting typical personality types inferred from the borrowers' job category. We projected a virtual space of borrowers by using the affinity matrix based on the Myers-Briggs type indicator (MBTI) that fits each job category. Applying the distance in this space to Lending Club data, we used locally weighted logistic regression to vary the coefficients of the variables, which affect loan repayments, with each MBTI type for predicting the default probability. We found that each MBTI type's credit scoring model has different significant variables. This study provides insights into breakthroughs in developing alternative credit scoring for P2P lending. Supplementary information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s40854-022-00347-4.
... Banks rarely remain without fault in these relationships. Bolton et al. (2011) report that bank clients often encounter a one-sided marketing of loan products (especially consolidation loans), and financial service providers downplay the impact of loans on financial well-being in the long run, emphasizing only the short-term benefits they provide. Consequently, their impact on life satisfaction in the long run (especially during the repayment phase) can be negative. ...
Article
Purpose The relationship between debt and happiness is an emerging research topic with significant implications for both theory and practice in economics and business. In China, where the consumer credit market is at an early stage of development, the topic remains under-investigated and the evidence on the debt–well-being link is scarce. The purpose of this study is to examine the association between debt holding and happiness and the moderating role of income in it. Design/methodology/approach Data used in the study were from three waves (2013, 2015 and 2017) of the China Household Finance Survey. Fixed-effect regressions on panel data were used for data analyses. Findings The results show that any type of debt holding is negatively associated with happiness. Among seven specific types of debts, four types show negative associations with happiness, which in the order from higher to lower associations, are medical, education, other and housing debt. In addition, negative associations between debt holding and happiness vary among income groups. The results suggest that any debt holding potentially decreases happiness for low- and middle-income consumers only. In addition, holdings of three specific types of debts (medical, education and housing debt) may decrease happiness for both low- and middle-income consumers, and holding two types of debts (business and other debt) may decrease happiness for middle-income consumers only. Research limitations/implications Data used in this study originate from one country only. It limits the generalizability of findings to other countries with different institutional backgrounds and different socio-economic characteristics of populations. The results have implications for researchers who study consumer debt behavior and business practitioners who do businesses with Chinese companies and consumers. Practical implications China is an emerging economy that is at the early stage of credit market development. The results of this study provide helpful information and insights for business practitioners to explore credit markets and serve credit product clients with various income levels in China. Social implications The results of this study are informative for public policies. When introducing credit market-related policies, policymakers should pay attention to people's happiness and to differential welfare effects of holdings of different types of debts and among consumers with various levels of incomes. Originality/value Unique contributions of this study include using data from the most recently available waves of the China Household Finance Survey (2013, 2015 and 2017) to study the associations between debt holding and happiness. In addition, the findings of this study enrich the literature of debt and happiness by adding evidence from China, the largest emerging economy in the world, which is helpful for future theory building and business practice on the relationship between debt holding and happiness.
... This research presents a number of interesting and high-potential future research opportunities. Because of the complex and multi-faceted nature of personal finances and the effort involved in the assessment process, it is worth exploring whether a more microscopic, targeted assessment of specific aspects of one's financial condition (e.g., debt only) can be used, especially by financial counselors and advisors, to encourage focused behaviors within a specific area (Bolton, Bloom, & Cohen, 2011). We believe that this is a particularly fruitful research topic because of the seriousness of certain types of financial obligations such as student loan debt or high-interest payday loans among certain consumer segments (Fry, 2012). ...
Article
We consider the relationship between an individual's financial self‐awareness, defined as detailed knowledge about one's current financial assets, liabilities, and spending patterns, and financial outcomes. The results of three studies show that a higher level of financial self‐awareness is associated with positive financial decisions and satisfaction among individuals. This effect is mediated by perceived efficacy about handling personal finances. We also find that financial literacy strengthens the association between financial self‐awareness and investing and saving, but does not affect the association between financial self‐awareness and either financial satisfaction or spending decisions. Increasing the financial self‐awareness of clients offers a useful method for financial advisors and policy makers to encourage prudent financial decisions.
... Our findings are also relevant to financial products such as debt consolidation programs, which are marketed as debt relief by offering lower interest rates over a longer debt repayment period. In reality, debt consolidation programs often involve a higher total interest payment in the long run (Bolton, Bloom, and Cohen 2011). Our findings point to an additional deleterious effect that may come with total debt consolidation. ...
Article
Across three studies, the authors investigate the effect of student loan debt on spending. Evidence from consumer finance data and experimental scenarios reveals that borrowers with moderate student loan debt are less likely to spend than people with low (or no) debt. However, borrowers with high debt are more likely to spend relative to those with moderate debt. The latter effect is consistent with goal disengagement, as paying off high student loan debt seems difficult. Importantly, the spending propensity associated with high student loan debt is attenuated by presenting the debt in a monthly payment (vs. lump-sum) format, which reduces perceived payoff difficulty. From a public policy perspective, the authors recommend that estimated monthly payments be included in all student loan disclosures.
Preprint
Full-text available
Financial education has become exceptionally important in protecting the financial well-being of people and companies in the era following the COVID-19 pandemic. This study provides a systematic literature review of financial education experiments worldwide, adopting a new technique that combines a deep learning algorithm with multivariate statistical analysis. The findings suggest a progressive improvement in the e↵ectiveness of the intervention as time progresses. However, we observe a limited influence of digitization and monetary incentives on the e cacy of the intervention. This study o↵ers valuable perspectives on the transforming nature of financial education initiatives in a dynamic economic context. Abstract Financial education has become exceptionally important in protecting the financial well-being of people and companies in the era following the COVID-19 pandemic. This study provides a systematic literature review of financial education experiments worldwide, adopting a new technique that combines a deep learning algorithm with multivariate statistical analysis. The findings suggest a progressive improvement in the effectiveness of the intervention as time progresses. However, we observe a limited influence of digitization and monetary incentives on the efficacy of the intervention. This study offers valuable perspectives on the transforming nature of financial education initiatives in a dynamic economic context.
Article
Full-text available
Different disciplines such as economics, sociology, philosophy, and psychology have examined the different dimensions of money construct from different perspectives. In this paper, our aim is to review, summarize and categorize the previous literature on money that has used a consumer behavior point of view. By this aim, we reviewed 213 articles found in four recognized marketing categorized the research found in these journals under six perspectives. These six perspectives on money research include 1) perceiving money, 2) managing and allocating money 3) spending money 4) giving money, 5) having money, 6) saving money. We investigated the related research under each perspective in detail and mentioned future research areas. This review could help researchers to see the gaps in the literature and direct their future research into these areas.
Article
Full-text available
This monograph describes the marketing research that has been published in the top marketing journals since their inception relating to health care, broadly defined. Over 1,000 articles are summarized across the chapters relating to consumer behavior and food, consumer behavior and other consumption, and business marketing issues. Research from outside of marketing is also briefly reviewed. This monograph celebrates the research that has been accomplished and closes with suggestions for future research.
Article
Full-text available
Much of psychology focuses on universal principles of thought and action. Although an extremely productive pursuit, this approach, by describing only the "average person," risks describing no one in particular. This article discusses an alternate approach that complements interests in universal principles with analyses of the unique psychological meaning that individuals find in their experiences and interactions. Rooted in research on social cognition, this approach examines how people's lay theories about the stability or malleability of human attributes alter the meaning they give to basic psychological processes such as self-regulation and social perception. Following a review of research on this lay theories perspective in the field of social psychology, the implications of analyzing psychological meaning for other fields such as developmental, cultural, and personality psychology are discussed.
Article
Individuals are increasingly put in charge of their financial security after retirement. Moreover, the supply of complex financial products has increased considerably over the years. However, we still have little or no information about whether individuals have the financial knowledge and skills to navigate this new financial environment. To better understand financial literacy and its relation to financial decision-making, we have devised two special modules for the DNB Household Survey. We have designed questions to measure numeracy and basic knowledge related to the working of inflation and interest rates, as well as questions to measure more advanced financial knowledge related to financial market instruments (stocks, bonds, and mutual funds). We evaluate the importance of financial literacy by studying its relation to the stock market: Are more financially knowledgeable individuals more likely to hold stocks? To assess the direction of causality, we make use of questions measuring financial knowledge before investing in the stock market. We find that, while the understanding of basic economic concepts related to inflation and interest rate compounding is far from perfect, it outperforms the limited knowledge of stocks and bonds, the concept of risk diversification, and the working of financial markets. We also find that the measurement of financial literacy is very sensitive to the wording of survey questions. This provides additional evidence for limited financial knowledge. Finally, we report evidence of an independent effect of financial literacy on stock market participation: Those who have low financial literacy are significantly less likely to invest in stocks.
Article
Both the contract interest rate and the annual percentage rate indicate the price of a loan. Consumer's understanding of these two measures of the price of closed-end consumer loans was examined using the 1997 University of Michigan Survey of Consumers. Among the mortgage borrowers, at least 40% of respondents failed to understand the relationship between these two price measures; education, quantity and quality of information search, and region were found to influence their understanding. The authors draw implications for improving information disclosure in consumer credit markets.
Article
Current conditions in the U.S. mortgage industry provide a rich context for exploring the positioning and information used in advertisements for mortgage products. This article examines television, radio, and print advertising messages employed to promote prime and subprime mortgage products. An analysis of these advertising messages reveals marked differences in the marketing for prime and subprime mortgage offers. Prime mortgage market advertisements tended to emphasize ‘the American dream of homeownership’ and low rates, and often provide detailed loan terms. On the other hand, subprime loan advertisements were based on fear appeals, emphasizing debt problems and encouraging consumers to rely on the lender/broker for help. These important differences in the information available to consumers for various mortgage products may have exacerbated industry information asymmetries and contributed to the current mortgage crisis.
Article
A default option is the choice alternative a consumer receives if he/she does not explicitly specify otherwise. In this article we argue that defaults can invoke a consumer's "marketplace metacognition," his/her social intelligence about marketplace behavior. This metacognitive account of defaults leads to different predictions than accounts based on cognitive limitations or endowment: in particular, it predicts the possibility of negative or "backfire" default effects. In two experiments, we demonstrate that the size and direction of the default effect depend on whether this social intelligence is invoked and how it changes the interpretation of the default.
Article
Consumers develop over their life span a pragmatic expertise in marketplace metacognition and marketplace interactions. Marketplace metacognition and social intelligence refer to people's beliefs about their own mental states and the mental states, strategies, and intentions of others as these pertain directly to the social domain of marketplace interactions. Drawing from the recent study of evolutionary psychology, theory of mind, multiple life-span intelligences, and everyday persuasion knowledge, I discuss the importance to our field of studying marketplace metacognition and social intelligence and of research-based consumer education programs on those topics.
Article
This article evaluates the risk- and safety-related behavior of adult bicyclists. Probit regression models are applied to national survey data to determine the relationship between accident risks and helmet use. Compensatory behavior, measured in the form of chanes in the likelihood of helmet use, is quantified for discrete changes in risk factors. The results are supportive of the risk compensation hypothesis: changes in the risk of head injury tend to be offset by changes in helmet use. However, the analysis provides no evidence that the magnitude of the compensatory response overwhelms the direct impacts of changes in the risk environment.