ArticlePDF Available

A quick, inexpensive trap for use with nest boxes.

Authors:
Linkhart, B. D. and R. T. Reynolds. 1987. Brood
division and postnesting behavior of Flam-
mulated Owls. Wi/son Buff. 99:240-243.
McCallum, D. A. 1994. Flammulated Owl (Otus
flammeolus). In A. Poole and F. Gill
(eds.).The birds of North America, No. 93.
Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadel-
phia, PA, and The American Ornitholo-
gists' Union, Washington, D C.
Mueller, H. C., D. D. Berger, and G. Allez. 1979.
Age and sex differences in size of
Sharp-shinned Hawks. Bird-Banding
50:34-44.
Pyle, P. 1997. Identification guide to North
American birds. Part I. 732 pp. Slate
Creek Press, Bolinas, CA.
News, Notes, Comments
A Quick, Inexpensive Trap for Use with Nest Boxes
Stutchbury and Robertson (1986) describe a
simple and effective trap design for capturing
cavity-nesting species in artificial nesting boxes.
Their trap consisted of a square aluminum plate
that is affixed to the inside of the nest box using
strips of masking tape (diagrams contained in
Stuchbury and Robertson 1986). To set their trap,
the plate is supported upright by using a stick or
shoot of grass. Here, we present a modification of
this original trap design, which allows for easier
field use and is also more cost effective.
Our trap is designed specifically for the side-
opening style nest box promoted by The North
American Bluebird Society (Fig. 1; for box plans
see http://www.nabluebirdsociety.org). This style
of box allows easy access for installation of the trap
within the box and subsequently for access to
captured birds. Our innovation involves replacing
the aluminum plate with a piece of 1/4-in hardware
cloth, which is both more cost efficient (approx-
imately $0.01 ea) and more easily affixed within the
box. Additionally, traps can be manufactured within
seconds in the field, allowing for a flexible trapping
schedule. Hardware cloth is constructed of stiff,
•nterwoven wire and is available at most hardware
stores. The hardware cloth is cut into a square (6.5
x 6.5 cm) and duct tape (5.8 x 6.5 cm) is folded
around the bottom to provide additional mass to the
mechanism. A second piece of duct tape (5.8 x 6.5
cm) is used to affix the trap to the inside of the box,
•mmediately above the entrance hole (Fig. 1). The
trap is light (about 4 g), allowing for the use of a thin
shoot of grass to support the trap within the box.
Jul - Sep 2004 North Amencan Bird Bander
During the course of our studies, we captured
about 500 male and female adult Eastern
Bluebirds (Sialia sialis). The trapping technique is
similar to that described by Stuchbury and
Robertson (1986). Trapping was most effective
during morning hours (0600 - 1100 h), and when
chicks were between 3 and 12 d old. During this
period of the nesting cycle, nestlings have the
highest energy demand and both the male and
female are intensively provisioning. Additionally,
nestlings are sedentary, minimizing unintentional
trap tripping by nestlings. Individuals were usually
caught within 20 min of installing the trap; however,
if unsuccessful, the trap was used on a box for a
maximum of 1 hr.
The trap is inexpensive and proficient use requires
minutes of training, unlike mist-nets. Trapping was
90% effective when used within the suggested time
frame, and no injuries were incurred while using
the trap. The original design by Stuchbury and
Robertson (1986) is effective, yet we believe these
modifications provide a substantial increase in
efficiency and adaptability.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We extend our thanks to H. Gangloft for illustrative
services. Additionally, we thank P. Lowther for
suggesting we submit this note.
Page115
Fig. 1. Side-opening nest-box design
with trap set within the box.
Recent Literature
LITERATURE CITED
Stuchbury, B.J. and R.J. Robertson. 1986. A
simple trap for catching birds in nest boxes.
J. Field OmithoL Winter: 64-65.
Thomas J. Robinson •,3, Lynn M. Siefferman 2,
and Thomas S. Risch 1
1Department of Biological Sciences
Arkansas State University
P.O. Box 599
State University, AR 72467
2Department of Biological Sciences
Auburn University
331 Funchess Hall
Auburn, AL 36849
3Corresponding author e-mail:
trobinson @ astate.ed u
BANDING HISTORY AND BIOGRAPHIES
A lark or two. W. E. Lanyon. 1993. Amer. Birds
47:1050-1057. Box 531, Keene Valley, NY 12943
(Color-banding helped author determine relation-
ships among Eastern and Western meadowlarks
in Wisconsin field, show stability of song patterns
of individuals from year to year and other social
details in classic 1950s study that showed that
these two species almost never hybridize in spite of
their nearly identical plumages. Further field and
captive studies in New York, Wisconsin, and
elsewhere demonstrated that males learned songs
from birds surrounding natal sites and that rare
instances of hybridization produced a high rate of
fertile young, but that the eggs of such young are
nearly all infertile.) MKM
BANDING EQUIPMENT AND TECHNIQUES
Automated doors for waterfowl banding traps.
E. P. Ashley and N. R. North. 2004. Wildl. Soc. Bull.
32:273-275. Can. Wildl. Serv., Ontario Region, Big
Creek Natl. Wildl. Area, R.R. 3, Port Rowan, ON
N0E 1 M0 (Describes modifications to baited swim-
in water[owl traps to reduce predation and prevent
escape. This automatic door, made of readily
available materials, can be programmed to open
Page 116
and close at specific times so that the amount of
time captured animals remain in the trap is
reduced, as is the potential for escape through an
otherwise open funnel door.) SG
Radiotelemetry studies: are we radio-handi-
capping Northern Bobwhites? F. S. Guthery
and J. J. Lusk. 2004. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 32:194-201.
Dept. Forestry, 008C Agric. Hall, Oklahoma State
Univ., Stillwater, OK 74078 (This is a re-analysis
of data from previously published papers that
estimated bobwhite survival rates through radio-
telemetry. The authors determined whether
populations would reach a stable age distribution
using published survival rates. Most previously
published survival rates appear to underestimate
true survival rates. Consequently, the authors
suggest that survival rates based on telemetry
data be interpreted cautiously and that telemetry
may not be the best approach for a given research
question.) SG
Self-injury and capture myopathy in net-
captured juvenile Red-legged Partridge with
necklace radiotags. U. Hofile, J. Millan, C.
Gortazar, F. J. Buenestado, I. Marco, and R.
Villafuerte. 2004. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 32:273-275.
Natl. Res. Inst. on Game Biol., Instituto de
North American Bird Bander Vol. 29 No 3
... Following our monitoring program protocol, we banded and measured both female and male social parents following hatching, that is, on 2 and August 3, 2022, respectively, using a trap designed by Robinson et al. (2004). Parents were aged based on the relative amount of white on the edges of the tenth primary covert (Pyle, 1997). ...
Article
Full-text available
Because plumage coloration is frequently involved in sexual selection, for both male and female mate choice, birds with aberrant plumage should have fewer mating opportunities and thus lower reproductive output. Here we report an Eastern Bluebird (Sialia sialis) female with a brown phenotype that raised a brood of four chicks to fledging. The brown female and her mate were only related to their social offspring to the second degree and one of the offspring was a half‐sibling. We propose four family tree scenarios and discuss their implications (e.g., extra‐pair paternity, conspecific brood parasitism). Regardless of the tree, the brown female was able to find a mate, which may have been facilitated by the bottleneck created by the severe snowstorms in February 2021.
... Adults banded in previous years that reused nest boxes were resighted with binoculars to identify their combinations. Upon capture at the nest box (following Robinson, Siefferman, & Risch, 2004) when chicks were between 2 and 4 days of age, unbanded adults were sexed, aged and banded. All field activities were approved by the Arkansas State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC). ...
Article
As climate change continues to alter temperature and precipitation patterns, numerous species have declined. However, populations of some species that show responses to climate change, such as eastern bluebirds ( Sialia sialis ), have increased or remained stable nationwide. To understand how species are adapting to climate change, we estimated demographic parameters and their responses to climatic variability, using nesting and banding‐recapture data between 2003 and 2018 in a northeastern Arkansas eastern bluebird population. Increasing variability in precipitation in the nonbreeding season negatively affected hatchability. Hatching success was negatively affected by increasing variability in maximum temperatures and the number of hot days during the breeding season, but positively affected by increasing winter snow depth. Adult survival was positively affected by increasing snow depth and variability in the number of hot days during the breeding season, but negatively affected by increasing variability in nonbreeding season temperatures. Our results demonstrate that for this study population, annual breeding parameters, though canalized against interannual environmental variation, were affected by seasonal climatic variability. Although climate change may benefit bluebird survival due to increasing variability in winter temperatures and the number of hot days, climatic variability negatively affected breeding parameters and is expected to increase. Because breeding parameters are typically the drivers of population growth rate in short‐lived species, these results raise concern for the future of this population of eastern bluebirds.
... Somewhat more sophisticated designs often operate automatically, through the use of a spring-operated mechanism (e.g. de Haven and Guarino 1969) and/or with a trigger stick to prop open a trap door (e.g. Stutchbury and Robertson 1986; Robinson et al. 2004). Many of these traps have been designed with small, nest box breeding northern hemisphere songbirds in mind. ...
... Most Eastern bluebird adults were captured using a modified prop trap (Robinson et al., 2004;Friedman et al., 2008) post-hatch, although some females were captured during late incubation (~4-6 days pre-hatching). Adults were banded with a US Geological Survey metal leg band and three colored plastic bands to allow for easy field identification. ...
... Most adults were captured using a modified prop trap (Friedman, Brasso, & Condon, 2008;Robinson, Siefferman, & Risch, 2004) after offspring had hatched, although many females were captured during late incubation (~4e6 days before hatching). All adults were banded with a U.S. Geological Survey metal leg band and a unique combination of three colour bands for easy field identification. ...
Article
Recent research suggests that individuals across a variety of taxa express consistent behavioural differences, or personality, which may affect reproductive success. Most previous studies of avian systems investigated the effects of individual personality on offspring recruitment and other fitness correlates, even in species with biparental care. Here we tested the potential for behavioural similarity within mated pairs of wild eastern bluebirds to impact reproductive success. Specifically, we quantified nest defence behaviour of males and females to address three hypotheses: (1) the individual personality hypothesis, which predicts that individuals with more aggressive personalities will have more nestlings survive to fledging; (2) the pair coordination hypothesis, which predicts similarly behaving pairs will have more nestlings survive to fledging; and (3) the pair intensity hypothesis, which predicts pairs with more aggressive personalities will have more nestlings survive to fledging. Pairs that had similar nest defence strategies produced more fledglings in support of the pair coordination hypothesis. We found no support for either the individual personality hypothesis or the pair intensity hypothesis. Thus, particularly in species with biparental care, it is important to consider the behaviour of both pair members to predict effects on reproductive success.
... We visited nestboxes with complete clutches daily and recorded the date that the first egg was laid. During late incubation, we captured adults using a nestbox trap (Robinson et al. 2004) and marked them with unique combinations of colour bands for later identification. We recorded the age of birds that were recaptured from previous years (second-year vs. after-second year). ...
Article
Full-text available
The fitness-related consequences of egg size, independent of the influences of parental quality, are poorly understood in altricial birds. Not only can egg size and parental quality influence growth and survival, but each could influence the development of condition- dependent plumage coloration in offspring. The Eastern Bluebird Sialia sialis is an altricial, multi-brooded, cavity-nesting passerine in which juveniles display dichromatic UV-blue plumage. Previous research suggests that plumage coloration acts as a signal of individual quality among juvenile and adult Eastern Bluebirds. Here, we separate the effects of egg size and parental quality (defined by egg size laid) on nestling growth and plumage ornamentation by exchanging clutches of large eggs with clutches of small eggs. Nestlings were significantly larger immediately post-hatching when hatched from a large egg, but to maintain a larger size, nestlings needed to have hatched from a large egg and to have been reared by high-quality parents. Nestlings were brighter when reared by high-quality parents and this relationship was strongest later in the breeding season. Nestlings exhibited greater UV chroma if hatched early in the season, but UV chroma was not significantly affected by egg size or parental quality. These findings demonstrate varying influences of both egg size and parental quality on offspring growth and plumage ornamentation but suggest that quality of post-hatching investment is more influential than pre-hatching investment.
... Total clutch mass was calculated as the combined mass of all eggs in the nest. To reduce the possibility of nest abandonment, we used a nest-box trap to attempt to capture all females during late incubation or early chick rearing (day 14-31 after the laying of the first egg in their first nest) (Robinson et al. 2004). All females were given a unique combination of color bands to allow for easy identification of individuals in the field. ...
Article
Full-text available
Because investment in eggs is costly for birds, a female's body condition is expected to influence that investment. Moreover, resource availability can fluctuate by year, and older and younger females may acquire or allocate resources differently. Additionally, in multi-brooded species, females may make tradeoffs in investment in first and second clutches. Because environmental conditions often change through the breeding season and early maturity of second broods can increase females' fitness, patterns of females' allocation to each clutch may change with the season. Here, we investigate associations between females' quality (body condition and age) and pre-hatching investment (clutch size, average egg mass per clutch, and total clutch mass) in adults of the Eastern Bluebird (Sialia sialis), a multi-brooded cavity-nesting passerine. In the two years of the study, relationships differed; in 2003, females that were heavier relative to their body size produced larger eggs and invested in greater overall clutch mass, and younger females produced larger clutches. In 2004, females whose first clutches were greater in overall mass laid second clutches proportionately smaller in overall clutch mass. Together, these data suggest that overall clutch mass may represent a significant cost to female bluebirds. We also found variation in investment in eggs within the season; second clutches tended to be smaller than first clutches but tended to contain larger eggs. By increasing resources per offspring late in the breeding season, females may increase the survival or condition of late-season offspring. Debido a que los huevos son una inversión costosa para las aves, se espera que la condición del cuerpo de las hembras influencie esta inversión. Más aún, la disponibilidad de recursos puede fluctuar anualmente, por lo que las hembras pueden asignar los recursos de modo diferente dependiendo de la edad de éstas. Adicionalmente, en las especies de nidadas múltiples, las hembras pueden realizar soluciones de compromiso entre las inversiones de la primera y segunda nidada. Debido a que las condiciones ambientales usualmente cambian a lo largo de la estación reproductiva, y a que la madurez temprana de las segundas nidadas puede aumentar la adecuación de las hembras, los patrones de asignación de las hembras a cada nidada pueden cambiar con la estación. Aquí investigamos las asociaciones entre la calidad de las hembras (condición corporal y edad) y la inversión efectuada antes de la eclosión (tamaño de la nidada, peso medio del huevo por nidada y peso total de la nidada) en adultos de Sialia sialis, un ave paseriforme de nidadas múltiples que anida en cavidades. Durante los dos años del estudio, las relaciones fueron distintas. En 2003, las hembras que fueron más pesadas en relación a sus tamaños corporales produjeron huevos más grandes e invirtieron en mayores pesos totales de la nidada, y las hembras más jóvenes produjeron nidadas más grandes. En 2004, las hembras cuyas primeras nidadas presentaron mayores pesos totales de la nidada pusieron segundas nidadas proporcionalmente menores en cuanto al peso total de la nidada. En conjunto, estos datos sugieren que el peso total de la nidada puede representar un costo significativo a las hembras de S. sialis. También encontramos variación en la inversión en los huevos dentro de una misma estación; las segundas nidadas tendieron a ser menores que las primeras nidadas pero tendieron a contener huevos más grandes. Mediante un incremento en los recursos por cría al final de la estación reproductiva, las hembras pueden incrementar la supervivencia o la condición de los pichones al final de la estación.
... We suspected that the pair was nesting nearby, but not in the nest box. On 8 June, however, we captured a female Blue Grosbeak in the nest box by using a nest-box trap (Robinson et al. 2004); she was incubating the four eggs described above, which appeared to be pipping . When we revisited the nest again on 13 June, we found four nestlings approximately 5 days old and apparently in good condition. ...
Article
Full-text available
ABSTRACT.—On 21 May 2003, we discovered a completed Blue Grosbeak (Passerina caerulea) nest in an Eastern Bluebird (Sialia sialis) nest box. On 28 May, the nest contained four whitish-tan eggs with light-brown, streaky and spotty markings, an unusual color pattern for Blue Grosbeak eggs. Species’ identification was confirmed by capturing the breeding female in the nest box, and confirmed again later with identification of the chicks as Blue Grosbeaks. To our knowledge, this is the first published account of cavity nesting, artificial or otherwise, for this species. Received 27 September 2004, accepted 31 May 2005.
... The number of nestlings surviving to fledging was recorded for each nest. Most adults were captured using a modified prop trap (Robinson et al. 2004; Friedman et al. 2008 ) between nestling days4 and 9. Some females were captured before hatching by trapping them in the nest box during late incubation . Females were not trapped earlier than 4–6 days prehatch in order to prevent abandonment. ...
Article
Full-text available
Consistent individual differences in behavior, or personality, have been demonstrated in a variety of species other than humans, including mammals, birds, and invertebrates. Behavioral consistency has been shown to affect dispersal, foraging, exploration, and antipredator responses, which may have an impact on parental and offspring survival. Despite increasing research in behavioral consistency, the repeatability of nest defense behavior has rarely been assessed in wild bird populations. Furthermore, previous studies investigating nest defense behavior have utilized laboratory studies or mounted predators to elicit defensive behavior. It is important to assess personality in wild populations to fully understand the fitness consequences of behavioral consistency across natural contexts and to utilize live predators or competitors for accurate assessment of defensive behavior. We used an ecologically relevant, live, invasive, nest site competitor, the house sparrow (Passer domesticus), to elicit nest defense behavior in a wild population of Eastern bluebirds (Sialia sialis) to determine if nest defense behavior is repeatable within and between years for males and females (males: 2009, N = 17; 2010, N = 18; both years, N = 9. Females: 2009, N = 22; 2010, N = 15; both years, N = 11). We also determined if individual behavior changes as a function of season, parental age, brood size, or the number of house sparrows around the nest site. We found that females demonstrated repeatable behavior both within and between years. Male nest defense behavior was only repeatable in 1 year and was influenced by season in the other year. Parental age, brood size, and the number of house sparrows around the nest site did not affect nest defense behavior. We conclude that Eastern bluebirds demonstrate consistent nest defense behavior, or personality, although males are more plastic than females.
Article
I studied the diet and foraging behavior of fledgling Black-crowned Night-Herons (Nycticorax nycticorax) in a mixed-species nesting colony of Black-crowned Night-Herons and Snowy Egrets (Egretta thula) in New Orleans, Louisiana. In 1 of 5 years, cannibalism of nestlings that had fallen or climbed out of nests was common, accounting for 66 of 94 (70.2%) prey items taken by fledglings. Juveniles took younger conspecifics by both predation and scavenging. Isolated incidents of cannibalism among Black-crowned Night-Herons have been reported previously, but not on a colony-wide scale.