ArticlePDF Available

Teaching children with autism to detect and respond to sarcasm

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
Author's personal copy
Teaching children with autism to detect and respond to sarcasm
Angela Persicke
a,b
, Jonathan Tarbox
a,b,
*, Jennifer Ranick
a,b
, Megan St. Clair
a,b
a
Center for Autism and Related Disorders, United States
b
Autism Research Group, United States
Many aspects of human communication depend upon the ability to use and understand non-literal language (Capelli,
Nakagawa, & Madden, 1990) including metaphor, metonymy, rhetorical questions, understatements, hyperbole, indirect
requests, and verbal irony (MacKay & Shaw, 2004). In a recent study on psycholinguistic literature, Gibbs (2000)
investigated the use of irony in everyday language by systematically reviewing sixty-two 10-min conversations. The
results indicated that ironic language accounted for 8% of all conversational turns. In addition, per each taped
conversation, there were approximately 4.7 instances of irony, indicating that ironic utterances may occur about once
every 2 min in everyday speech. A further analysis of 289 utterances revealed that in 28% of cases, sarcasm was the
specific form of irony used. These findings suggest that the general use of verbal irony and specific use of sarcasm is
common in everyday language and communication.
Sarcasm is a form of verbal irony that is characterized by an incongruity between what is said and what is meant (Capelli
et al., 1990). Sarcasm can be linguistically defined as a form of non-literal language in which someone subjectively states the
opposite of what is objectively known to be true in reality. In this form of ironic language, the speaker intends to
communicate a message contradictory to its literal meaning and the listener must subsequently reason a figurative
interpretation. Understanding this specific form of counterfactual, non-literal language has been found to serve significant
social and communicative functions. Mainly, verbal irony and sarcasm are used to either indirectly convey attitudes and
beliefs or for the purposes of generating humor (Harris & Pexman, 2003; Pexman et al., 2011). Thus, comprehension of
sarcasm requires an ability to identify the social cues necessary to differentiate between the speaker’s intended meaning and
the literal meaning of the utterance. The ability to judge the literal meaning of a statement while simultaneously inferring
the speaker’s intended meaning is said to reflect an advanced understanding of other people’s thoughts or intentions,
i.e., perspective-taking (Uchiyama et al., 2012).
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7 (2013) 193–198
ARTICLE INFO
Article history:
Received 27 June 2012
Received in revised form 7 August 2012
Accepted 9 August 2012
Keywords:
Sarcasm
Irony
Multiple exemplar training
Relational frame theory
ABSTRACT
Previous research has demonstrated that children with autism often have difficulty using
and understanding non-literal language (e.g., irony, sarcasm, deception, humor, and
metaphors). Irony and sarcasm may be especially difficult for children with autism
because the meaning of an utterance is the opposite of what is stated. The current study
evaluated the effectiveness of a training package, including rules and in vivo multiple
exemplar training, to teach three children with autism to detect and respond appropriately
to sarcastic statements. The training package was effective and generalization was
obtained across novel exemplars, settings, and people.
ß2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
* Corresponding author at: Center for Autism and Related Disorders, 19019 Ventura Blvd, 3rd Floor, Tarzana, CA 91356, United States.
Tel.: +1 818 345 2345.
E-mail address: j.tarbox@centerforautism.com (J. Tarbox).
Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
Journal homepage: http://ees.elsevier.com/RASD/default.asp
1750-9467/$ – see front matter ß2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.08.005
Author's personal copy
Research conducted with typically developing children has found that the ability to derive sarcastic intent from verbally
ironic utterances naturally begins to emerge around the age of 5 or 6, and continues to develop throughout middle childhood
(Creusere, 2000; Dews et al., 1996; Harris & Pexman, 2003; Pexman et al., 2011). On the other hand, research conducted on
children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) has found this population to be distinctively deficit in perspective taking
abilities necessary for the comprehension of verbal irony (Filippova & Astington, 2008; Happe
´, 1994; Pexman et al., 2011). In
a study conducted by Kaland et al. (2002), twenty-one children and adolescents with Asperger’s Disorder and twenty
typically developing age-matched controls were presented with several short stories that were used to assess the ability of
participants to infer physical and mental states. The content of these short stories included sarcastic remarks, among other
forms of figurative speech. In order to determine whether the children could identify the use of sarcasm, following each story
the researchers asked a comprehension question (i.e., ‘‘Is it true what X says?’’) and a justification question (i.e., ‘‘Why does X
say that?’’). Participants with Asperger’s Disorder had far more difficulty with the tasks and were impaired in their ability to
infer mental states in their responses and instead provided irrelevant or idiosyncratic explanations.
These deficits may have significant negative implications for social functioning, when taking into account the prevalence
of verbal irony and sarcasm used in everyday language described earlier. Indeed, researchers have highlighted the
importance of understanding non-literal language for everyday communication and overall social integration (Rajendran,
Mitchell, & Rickards, 2005). For example, if a child with autism shoots a basket and misses and a peer sarcastically says ‘‘Nice
shot,’’ and the child with autism responds as though the peer is being literal (e.g., ‘‘No, it wasn’t a nice shot, I missed’’), then
such a misunderstanding would likely set the occasion for the child with autism to be the subject of further ridicule. Research
on the prevalence of bullying among individuals with ASD has indicated that this population has a greater risk of
victimization compared to typically developing peers (Van Roekel, Scholte, & Didden, 2010). Higher rates of bullying among
individuals with ASD may very well be due to deficits in social interactions and an overall lack of understanding of the
behavior of others, i.e., perspective-taking.
Given the potential social importance of ironic language, identifying effective interventions for teaching individuals with
ASD to understand and use ironic language would seem to be a priority. However, the vast majority of previous research has
focused on identifying deficits and constructing hypothetical explanations for them. Such hypothetical explanations range
from general theories of weak central coherence and theory of mind abilities (Happe
´, 1994; Jolliffe & Baron-Cohen, 1999;
Martin & McDonald, 2004), to domain specific impairments in mental inference abilities and the ability to attribute mental
states (Kaland et al., 2002). While this research contributes to our knowledge regarding the existence of deficits, it does not
address language as learned behavior that is amenable to teaching. Considering the multitude of studies demonstrating that
children with ASD have substantial difficulty comprehending sarcasm, it is important for current research to expand its focus
from identification of deficits to interventions for remediating them.
Persicke, Tarbox, Ranick, and St. Clair (2012) recently conducted a study that taught metaphorical reasoning, a form of
non-literal language, to children with ASD. Participants were presented with short stories and were then asked a series of
three metaphorical questions about each story. Answering the questions correctly depended on the ability to figure out the
meaning of the metaphors in relation to particular aspects of the story. During teaching, participants were first told how to
reason through a metaphor by identifying multiple features of the metaphorical statement, then identifying multiple
features of the thing which the metaphor was used to describe, and finally, identifying the feature that both had in common,
thus solving the metaphor. For example, ‘‘Why did I call the boy a race car? Well, the boy is a person, he’s wearing clothing,
and he was running really fast. Race cars are vehicles, they are made of metal, and they go really fast. So when I said the boy is
a race car, I meant he is really fast.’’ Participants were then trained across multiple exemplars of metaphors until
generalization to untrained metaphors was obtained. All participants demonstrated generalization across multiple
untrained metaphors, and two of the three participants began creating their own metaphors, a performance that was never
targeted during the intervention. The results of the study suggested that multiple exemplar training can be effective for
teaching metaphorical reasoning, a form of non-literal language, to children with ASD.
In the study by Persicke et al. (2012), children with ASD were taught to relate two stimuli in terms of a shared feature
(the feature shared between an object and the metaphor used to describe it). Sarcasm requires relating of a different
sort. In sarcastic statements, the intended meaning of the statement is opposite to the literal meaning. However, the
results of the Persicke study are encouraging for the possibility of teaching sarcasm because, from a behavioral
standpoint, there is no reason to assume that teaching one type of relating would be any more or less possible than
teaching another. Research in Relational Frame Theory (RFT), a contemporary behavioral account of human language
and cognition, suggests that the behavior of relating, per se, is learned operant behavior (see Rehfeldt & Barnes-Holmes,
2009, for a volume-length application of RFT to autism and related disorders). Furthermore, preliminary research
suggests that some forms of relating can be strengthened in individuals with ASD via multiple exemplar training
(Murphy, Barnes-Holmes, & Barnes-Holmes, 2005; Tarbox,Zuckerman,Bishop,Olive,&OHora,2011). Understanding a
sarcastic statement involves responding to the literal meaning of the statement and the inferred intended meaning of
the speaker in terms of the relation of opposition. According to RFT research, this ability should be teachable through
practice across multiple exemplars with clear and immediate feedback.
The purpose of the current study was to teach children with ASD to accurately detect and appropriately respond to
sarcastic comments in the course of normal everyday conversation. Training began in a contrived format, with the use of
video clips and directly prompted performance. Training progressed to more natural formats, culminating in natural
conversational interactions, and continuing until generalization to untrained sarcastic statements was observed.
A. Persicke et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7 (2013) 193–198
194
Author's personal copy
1. Method
1.1. Participants and setting
Participants included 3 children receiving applied behavior analytic services from a community-based in-home service
provider. Children ranged in age from 6 to 7 years old and had current diagnoses of autistic disorder. At the time of the study,
participants were receiving 2–10 h per week of behavioral therapy, and had been receiving in-home behavioral therapy for
approximately 3–4 years. To be included in the study, participants’ parents and clinical supervisors needed to prioritize the
detection of sarcasm as an important component of the children’s overall treatment plan. In addition, participants all had
prior training in skills that may be prerequisites for understanding sarcasm, including prosody, body language and facial
expressions, physical context of conversation, conversational audience, desires, emotions, sensory perspective taking, cause
and effect, preferences, knowing, beliefs, and intentions. None of the participants had previous direct training in detecting or
responding to sarcasm. Additionally, this skill was not targeted by the participants’ clinical treatment teams throughout the
course of the study, and parents were asked to not provide any training outside of specified research sessions. Sessions were
conducted at participants’ homes, with the exception of generalization sessions, which were conducted at various locations
in the community. Training sessions were video recorded using a digital video camera mounted on a tripod. For baseline and
post-training sessions, video recording was surreptitiously prepared before sessions so that participants could not detect
they were being recorded. Approximately two to three sessions were conducted each week. Training sessions lasted no more
than 30 min and baseline/post-training sessions no more than 1 h. All sessions were conducted by the first and third authors,
both of whom had 4–6 years of experience teaching skills to children with autism. To ensure treatment fidelity, all
procedures were rehearsed before the study began and fidelity was anecdotally monitored on a regular basis by the first
author through video tape review.
1.2. Response measurement and interobserver agreement
Data were collected on the accuracy of each participant’s responses to sarcastic and sincere comments made by therapists
and family members across all phases of the study. A correct response to a sarcastic comment was defined as smiling,
laughing or stating a reciprocal sarcastic comment. An incorrect response to a sarcastic comment was defined as responding
as though the sarcastic comment was literal, as in disagreeing with it. However, for the purposes of remaining as stringent as
possible, any response that did not meet the definition of a correct response was scored as an incorrect response. Correct
responses to sincere comments were defined as anything other than disagreeing with the comment. Data were only collected
on sincere comments in order to ensure that participants were learning to discriminate between sarcastic and sincere
comments and not rotely respond to all therapist comments as sarcastic after training. Accuracy data on sarcastic trials were
summarized and graphed as percent correct.
A second independent observer scored videos from 36% of sessions in all phases. Trial-by-trial interobserver agreement
(IOA) was calculated by dividing the number of agreements by the sum of agreements and disagreements and multiplying by
100. IOA averaged 99.4% across all participants and phases, with no individual session scoring lower than 97% agreement
(range = 97.1–100%).
1.3. Procedure
1.3.1. Baseline
Baseline sessions were designed to be indistinguishable from regular everyday conversational interactions between
participants and adults. As a regular part of participants’ in-home therapy, participants engaged in conversation with
therapists and family members during frequent breaks from therapy. ‘‘Sessions’’ of sarcasm trials were embedded into these
breaks, without any overt indication that testing or data collection was occurring. Each session consisted of the therapist
making three sarcastic comments over the course of an hour or more. In addition, one sincere comment was made on the
same subject as each sarcastic comment (i.e., sarcastic: ‘‘Worms would taste so good!’’ and sincere: ‘‘I would never eat
worms.’’), spread over the course of the same hour. In other words, therapists had numerous conversational interactions with
participants over the course of an hour and the six comments were randomly interspersed throughout. None of these six
comments were made as the initial interaction with the participants on a given day.
Table 1 displays a sample of the sarcastic comments included in the study (contact the corresponding author for a full
list of comments). Sarcastic comments were created prior to study sessions and included discrete contextual cues
Table 1
Sample context and corresponding sarcastic and sincere comments.
Context Sarcastic comment Sincere comment
warm sunny day ‘‘It’s definitely going to snow today.’’ ‘‘It’s so warm outside today.’’
non-preferred food item ‘‘You would love to eat broccoli every day.’’ ‘‘I know you don’t like broccoli.’’
messy room ‘‘You didn’t make a mess at all.’’ ‘‘This room is so messy.’’
preferred activity ‘‘Playing video games is never fun.’’ ‘‘It’s so much fun to play video games’’
A. Persicke et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7 (2013) 193–198
195
Author's personal copy
(e.g., object size) or the participant’s individual preferences (e.g., favorite foods or toys). Sarcastic comments that would
require participants to make inferences about the therapists’ preferences were avoided. Participants were not given any
feedback regarding correct or incorrect responses to any of the six comments. However, if the participant did not respond,
the therapist would say, ‘‘Did you hear me?’’ and repeat the comment, as he/she normally would during conversation.
Comments were repeated no more than 3 times before the response was recorded as a non-response.
1.3.2. Rules and videos
The first phase of training consisted of providing rules, modeling, practice, and specific feedback, across multiple
exemplars. At the beginning of each session, the following rule was presented, ‘‘When someone says the opposite of what
they mean, they are probably being sarcastic.’’ Short video clips (10 s or less) were then shown that contained clear
contextual cues allowing the participant to make a comparison between the sarcastic/sincere comment and what was
occurring in the clip. For example, one video clip showed a child riding a skateboard at an extremely slow pace and the
sarcastic comment was, ‘‘Wow, he is going really fast.’’ After each video clip, the therapist would say ‘‘This is being sarcastic’’
and would make a sarcastic comment. Following the comment, the therapist would ask the participant leading questions
about the comment, including what was said, if the therapist meant what was said, why it was said, and the appropriate
response to the comment. The purpose of asking these questions was to help the child develop a repertoire of asking himself
questions to determine the meaning of ambiguous comments. Ten-to-fifteen practice trials were then conducted, in which
the therapist played the video clips again and stated either sarcastic or sincere comments. Correct responses were reinforced
with praise and incorrect responses were corrected using the leading questions described above. Exaggeration of the
therapist’s facial expression and intonation, including eye rolling and smiling, was faded across training trials. The video and
rules phase of training was concluded when a participant scored 80% or higher on practice trials across 3 consecutive
sessions.
1.3.3. In vivo training
The second phase of training consisted of rules and in vivo rehearsal across multiple exemplars. This phase was adapted
from a lesson on detecting sarcasm in the Skillsßcurriculum (www.skillsforautism.com). Each session consisted of 10–12
trials. At the beginning of the session, the therapist stated the rule and asked the participant to repeat the rule. Sarcastic and
sincere comments were then stated during natural conversation throughout the session. If the participant responded
correctly the therapist provided verbal praise. If the participant responded incorrectly, the therapist gave corrective feedback
in the form of leading questions. Initially, sarcastic comments were stated with exaggerated intonation and facial expression
and this was faded across successive sessions. Specifically, during the first training session, facial expression and intonation
were exaggerated, during the second training session the exaggerations were decreased, and finally, were absent for the
third training session. During any given session, approximately half of all comments had never before been presented during
training (e.g., if 10 comments were stated, 5 of these comments were novel). All sarcastic comments, including novel
comments, were predetermined for each session. In vivo training sessions were conducted in three different settings (e.g.,
home, park, and coffee house) and with three teachers (i.e., two therapists and one parent or nanny) to promote
generalization. Therapists used modeling, practice and feedback to train caregivers to state sarcastic comments prior to each
session in which the caregivers participated. Training was concluded after the participant achieved 80% correct or higher
with novel exemplars across 3 consecutive sessions.
1.3.4. Post-training and follow-up
Post-training sessions were identical to baseline (i.e., the procedures were identical and the sarcastic comments were the
same) and none of the sarcastic comments included in this phase were present during training. In addition, in order to rule
out any possible practice effects, additional novel probes were conducted that included sarcastic comments that were never
before present during any prior baseline or training phases. Finally, follow-up probes were conducted at one, two, and three-
months for Reggie and Kevin (Hans was not available for follow-up sessions). Follow-up sessions also included completely
novel comments that were never present during any prior sessions.
2. Results
Fig. 1 depicts the percentage of correct responses to sarcastic statements across all phases of the study. Note that asterisks
represent accuracy on novel exemplars. In baseline, two of the three participants, Reggie and Kevin, did not respond correctly
to any sarcastic comments, for example, saying with frustration ‘‘No, worms are not yummy!’’ Hans responded correctly to 1
out of 3 comments during baseline sessions 2 and 4. All participants showed immediate increases in correct responding to
sarcastic statements at the onset of the rules and video training phase and continued to respond correctly during the second
phase of training, in vivo multiple exemplar training. Hans and Reggie had greater difficulty than Kevin during in vivo
training, but both achieved mastery after 9 in vivo training sessions.
During post-training, all participants demonstrated correct responding to the vast majority of sarcastic comments
presented. Two-to-four additional probes of novel exemplars (never before included in any previous session), depicted by
asterisks, were conducted for all three participants. Following these novel probes, three monthly follow-up probes were
conducted with Reggie and Kevin and accurate responding maintained. Note that none of the exemplars presented during
A. Persicke et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7 (2013) 193–198
196
Author's personal copy
any post-training sessions were ever present during training. Therefore, all responding during post-training sessions
represents generalization across exemplars. Hans demonstrated generalized correct responding to 22 untrained exemplars
and Reggie and Kevin demonstrated generalized correct responding to 31. In addition, one setting in which training was
never conducted was included in post-training sessions for all three participants and all demonstrated correct responding.
Finally, generalization across people was assessed by including two people in post-training sessions who were never present
during training (i.e., one therapist and one parent or nanny). All three participants demonstrated correct responding to these
people during post-training.
3. Discussion
This study used rules, video clips, and in vivo training across multiple exemplars to successfully teach three children with
autism to detect and respond appropriately to sarcastic comments. These results are encouraging because they provide
further evidence that non-literal language deficits can be remediated using simple behavioral teaching procedures such as
the provision of rules and multiple exemplar training. Perhaps most importantly, all three participants not only mastered the
skill, but also demonstrated generalization to novel people, settings, and exemplars. That is, all participants demonstrated
the ability to respond appropriately to new sarcastic statements, made by people who did not participate in training, and in
locations that were not included in training. The ability to respond appropriately to sarcasm, then, was established as a
generalized operant, rather than teaching children to memorize appropriate responses to particular sarcastic comments.
The verbal repertoire targeted in this study was relatively narrow but the success of the intervention has relatively broad
implications for the remediation of complex and subtle social behavior in individuals with ASD. There is a common
misconception that behavioral intervention procedures are effective for teaching simple skills but that addressing deficits in
complex language and cognition may require cognitive and/or relationship-oriented approaches. The results of the current
study do not support this conclusion. Much further research into the application of behavioral procedures for teaching a
wider variety of perspective-taking and pragmatic language skills is needed, but this study provides preliminary evidence
that complex skills such as these are amenable to remediation through behavioral teaching strategies.
The current results have implications for the utility of RFT for constructing programs for teaching complex language and
cognition to individuals with ASD. The core concept of RFT, that cognition consists of relating behavior, which is learned via
[(Fig._1)TD$FIG]
Fig. 1. Percentage of correct responses to sarcastic comments across all phases of the study for Reggie, Hans, and Kevin.
A. Persicke et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7 (2013) 193–198
197
Author's personal copy
multiple exemplar training, seems to be supported by this study. The complex ability of responding to sarcastic comments, in
which the intended meaning is the opposite of the literal meaning, was successfully taught via practice across multiple
exemplars with clear and immediate feedback. In addition, the rules provided in the earlier sessions of training directed
participants to attend to the relational properties of the context, that is, the fact that sarcastic comments are the opposite of
what is true in the current context.
The ability of participants to expressively produce sarcastic language was never targeted during the study. However, two
of the three participants created their own sarcastic comments during research sessions, and the third participant’s mother
reported that he created his own sarcastic comments when conversing with her outside of sessions. These findings, although
uncontrolled and anecdotal, suggest that the participants may have learned how to produce their own sarcastic language
without direct training. Future research on teaching sarcasm or other forms of non-literal language should include
systematic evaluation of the expressive repertoire and/or other untrained repertoires.
A limitation of this study is that the rules and video phase may not have been necessary for the participants to learn
the skill. This phase was included to ensure that the participants were attending to the discrete contextual cues in the
video that contradicted the sarcastic statement stated by the therapist. It seems likely that this phase helped establish
the repertoire that was perfected in the in vivo phase, but the current data do not allow for an assessment of this
possibility. It is possible that the participants would have learned the skill just as quickly from in vivo training alone and
future research should attempt to identify which treatment components are truly necessary for teaching sarcasm.
In conclusion, this study used rules, video clips, and in vivo training across multiple exemplars to teach the
generalized skill of detecting and responding appropriately to sarcastic statements with 3 children with autism. All 3
participants successfully demonstrated this skill with untrained exemplars, as well as novel exemplars not contacted in
any phase of the study. Furthermore, the two participants who were available for maintenance probes demonstrated
maintenance of the skill across 3 months. This study provides further evidence that behavioral teaching procedures can
be used to teach complex language and cognitive skills, including non-literal language, to children with ASD. Much
research is still needed in areas such as deception, humor, and intentions, among many others, but the current data
suggest that ASD research can be successful in progressing from merely identifying deficits in complex language and
cognition to remediating them.
References
Capelli, C. A., Nakagawa, N., & Madden, C. M. (1990). How children understand sarcasm: The role of context and intonation. Child Development, 61, 1824–1841.
Creusere, M. A. (2000). A developmental test of theoretical perspectives on the understanding of verbal irony: Children’s recognition of allusion and pragmatic
insincerity. Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 29–45.
Dews, S., Winner, E., Kaplan, J., Rosenblatt, E., Hunt, M., Lim, K., et al. (1996). Children’s understanding of the meaning and functions of verbal irony. Child
Development, 67, 3071–3085.
Filippova, E., & Astington, J. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79, 126–138.
Gibbs, R. W. (2000). Irony in talk among friends. Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 5–27.
Happe
´, F. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and
normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.
Harris, M., & Pexman, P. M. (2003). Children’s perceptions of the social functions of verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 36, 147–165.
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). A test of central coherence theory: Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome: Is
local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71, 149–185.
Kaland, N., Møller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E., Gottlieb, D., & Smith, L. (2002). A new ‘advanced’ test of theory of mind: Evidence from children and
adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 517–528.
MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2004). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20,
13–32.
Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 34, 311–328.
Murphy, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2005). Derived manding in children with autism: Synthesizing Skinner’s Verbal Behavior with relational
frame theory. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 445–462.
Persicke, A., Tarbox, J., Ranick, J., & St. Clair, M. (2012). Establishing metaphorical reasoning in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6,
913–920.
Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1097–1112.
Rajendran, G., Mitchell, P., & Rickards, H. (2005). How do individuals with Asperger syndrome respond to nonliteral language and inappropriate requests in
computer-mediated communication? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 429–443.
Rehfeldt, R. A., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (Eds.). (2009). Derived relational responding applications for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities: A
progressive guide to change. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Tarbox, J., Zuckerman, C. K., Bishop, M. R., Olive, M. L., & O’Hora, D. P. (2011). Rule-governed behavior: Teaching a preliminary repertoire of rule-following to
children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 125–139.
Uchiyama, H. T., Saito, D. N., Tanabe, H. C., Harada, T., Seki, A., Ohno, K., et al. (2012). Distinction between the literal and intended meanings of sentences: A
functional magnetic resonance imagining study of metaphor and sarcasm. Cortex, 48, 563–583.
Van Roekel, E., Scholte, R. H. J., & Didden, R. (2010). Bullying among adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence and perception. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 40, 63–73.
A. Persicke et al. / Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7 (2013) 193–198
198
... Il sarcasmo può essere definito come: «Ironia amara e pungente, ispirata da animosità e quindi intesa a offendere e umiliare, che a volte può anche essere espressione di profonda amarezza rivolta, più che contro gli altri, contro sé stessi» (Treccani, 2022); oppure come: «una forma di ironia caratterizzata da una incongruenza tra ciò che viene detto e ciò che si intende e può essere linguisticamente definito come una forma di linguaggio non letterale in cui qualcuno afferma soggettivamente il contrario di ciò che è oggettivamente noto per essere vero» (Persicke et al., 2013). La comprensione del sarcasmo richiede la capacità di identificare gli indizi sociali necessari per distinguere tra il significato voluto dal parlante e il significato letterale dell'enunciato. ...
... La validità sociale dell'insegnamento di questo tipo di abilità è da ricercare nel fatto che questi deficit possono avere implicazioni negative significative sul funzionamento sociale (se si tiene conto della prevalenza dell'ironia verbale e del sarcasmo utilizzati nel linguaggio quotidiano descritto in precedenza). Pertanto, data la potenziale importanza sociale del linguaggio ironico, l'identificazione di interventi efficaci per insegnare alle persone all'interno dello spettro a comprendere e utilizzare il linguaggio ironico sembrerebbe una priorità all'interno del campo dell'Analisi del Comportamento (Persicke et al., 2013). ...
... I componenti del BST, inoltre, sono stati utilizzati per insegnare abilità sociali quali aumentare le capacità di conversazione come salutare, mantenere e terminare conversazioni (Kornacki, Ringdahl, Sjostrom e Nuernberger, 2013;Nuernberger, Ringdahl, Vargo, Crumpecker e Gunnarsson, 2013); cambiare argomento di conversazione e mantenere il contatto oculare (Stewart, Carr e LeBlanc, 2007); negoziare e fare complimenti (Hui Shyuan Ng, Schulze, Rudrud e Leaf, 2016); cambiare gioco (Peters e Brooks, 2016); insegnare abilità di richiesta (Loughrey et al., 2014); e numerose altre abilità di gioco (Green et al., 2020). Il BST si è quindi dimostrato efficace nell'insegnamento di una varietà di abilità a popolazioni diverse e vi è un supporto preliminare per l'utilizzo della BST per insegnare abilità sociali e di teoria della mente a bambini con autismo (Persicke et al., 2013;Stewart et al., 2007). Nonostante esistano diversi tipi di BST generalmente il modello include: ...
Article
Trascorrere una vita sociale soddisfacente significa essere in grado di padroneggiare abilità verbali complesse come il saper rispondere a richieste indirette o l'essere in grado di utilizzare l'umorismo nelle sue diverse sfaccettature come, ad esempio, il sarcasmo (Najdowski, Bergstrom, Tarbox e Clair, 2017; Persicke, Tarbox, Ranick e St. Clair, 2013). Il presente studio ha valutato l'insegnamento delle abilità legate al sarcasmo ad un gruppo di ragazzi nello spettro ad alto funzionamento. Il pacchetto di intervento ha utilizzato procedure derivanti dall'Analisi Comportamentale Applicata (ABA) come il Behavioral Skill Training (BST) ed il training in vivo con Multiple Exemplar Training (MET) per inse-gnare a tre studenti con autismo a riconoscere e rispondere in maniera adeguata a commenti sarca-stici. L'insegnamento è stato suddiviso in due macro obiettivi: saper identificare il sarcasmo, ossia sa-per fare effettuare il tact di situazioni sarcastiche e successivamente rispondere e fare commenti in maniera appropriata (ad esempio sorridere). Sono stati raccolti i dati per ogni prova per ogni singolo partecipante. Tutti i partecipanti hanno acquisito l'abilità di identificazione del sarcasmo e di fare commenti sarcastici. L'insegnamento di abilità verbali complesse mette l'analisi del comportamento di fronte a nuove sfide. Quando insegniamo un repertorio sotto controllo di antecedenti multipli, e che prevedono una componente di motivazione sociale, l'insegnamento assume caratteristiche differenti dalla quotidiana pratica clinica.
... Therefore, the purpose of the current study was to extend behavioral research in this area by evaluating a training package that included multiple exemplar training (MET), rules, modeling, practice, and feedback (e.g., praise and error correction), for teaching four Autistic children and adolescents to use deception to play friendly tricks on others. The use of treatment packages that include MET and rules has been effective for teaching other perspective-taking skills such as detecting sarcasm (Persicke et al., 2013), social conflicts (Suarez et al., 2021), responding to disguised mands (Najdowski et al., 2017), and responding to the preferences of others during play (Najdowski et al., 2018). ...
... The findings of this study contribute to the existing behavior analytic research literature on teaching complex social skills and nonliteral language (e.g., Persicke et al., 2013;Bergstrom et al., 2016) by offering a systematic evaluation of a training package for teaching Autistic individuals to use deception to play friendly tricks on others. Therefore, it represents a small step in attempting to fill a gap in the literature on teaching complex social repertoires to individuals with developmental disabilities. ...
Article
Full-text available
Perspective taking is a critical repertoire for navigating social relationships and consists of a variety of complex verbal skills, including socially adaptive forms of deception. Detecting and being able to use socially adaptive deception likely has many practical uses, including defending oneself against bullying, telling white lies to avoid hurting others’ feelings, keeping secrets and bluffing during games, and playing friendly tricks on others. Previous research has documented that some Autistic ¹ children have challenges identifying deception and playfully deceiving others (Reinecke et al., 1997). The current study employed a multiple baseline across participants design to evaluate the use of multiple exemplar training, rules, modeling, practice, and feedback for teaching four Autistic children and adolescents to use deception to play friendly tricks on others. The procedure was successful for all participants, and generalization was achieved across novel, untrained tricks.
... They identified five studies that have remedied irony comprehension in various study fields, including children, second-language learners, and people with autism. These studies (Bouton, 1999;Kim & Lantolf, 2018;Persicke et al., 2013;Saban-Bezalel & Mashal, 2015;Szücs & Babarczy, 2017) demonstrated that explicit metapragmatic training led to a significant improvement in irony comprehension. This training consisted of defining irony and its contextual cues and then putting them into practice in the context of written stories, comic strips, or videos. ...
... Regarding our first objective (treatment effectiveness on the treated items), both participants showed an improvement in the treated stories, especially for irony and indirect requests. These results confirm previous studies that have shown that explicit training could improve irony comprehension in people with autism spectrum disorder (Persicke et al., 2013;Saban-Bezalel & Mashal, 2015). Our study yet extends these findings for the first time to irony and indirect requests comprehension in individuals with TBI. ...
... Variability in children's irony comprehension accuracies, however, suggests that this is not the case for all. A few recent studies have explored how to overcome the lack of metapragmatic knowledge through training (Bouton, 1999;Kim & Lantolf, 2018, Persicke et al., 2013Saban-Bezalel & Mashal, 2015). The majority have involved participants with a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (Persicke et al., 2013;Saban-Bezalel & Mashal, 2015) or second language speakers (Bouton, 1999;Kim & Lantolf 2018). ...
... A few recent studies have explored how to overcome the lack of metapragmatic knowledge through training (Bouton, 1999;Kim & Lantolf, 2018, Persicke et al., 2013Saban-Bezalel & Mashal, 2015). The majority have involved participants with a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (Persicke et al., 2013;Saban-Bezalel & Mashal, 2015) or second language speakers (Bouton, 1999;Kim & Lantolf 2018). Most relevant for the current study, two studies explored the effect of irony training with typically developing children (Lee et al., 2021;Szücs & Babarczy, 2017). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Irony comprehension requires going beyond literal meaning of words, and is challenging for children. In this pre-registered study we investigated how teaching metapragmatic knowledge in classrooms impacts 10-year-olds’ written irony comprehension (n = 41, 21 girls) compared to a control group (n = 34, 13 girls). At pre-test, participants read ironic and literal sentences embedded in stories while their eye movements were recorded. Next, the training group was taught about irony, and the control group was taught about reading comprehension. At post-test, the reading task and eye tracking were repeated. Irony comprehension improved after training, suggesting that metapragmatic knowledge serves an important role in irony development. However, the eye movement data suggested that training did not change the strategy children used to resolve the ironic meaning. The results highlight the potential of metapragmatic training and have implications for theories of irony comprehension.
... Nel concreto, utilizzando una forma di ironia o sarcasmo, la persona vuole comunicare un messaggio contrario al significato letterale delle parole utilizzate con l'obiettivo di generare umorismo o muovere critiche. Pertanto, la comprensione del sarcasmo e dell'ironia richiede la capacità da parte di chi ascolta di crearsi un'interpretazione figurata di ciò che ha sentito dire dall'interlocutore (Persicke et al., 2013). ...
Article
Il mondo analitico comportamentale sta investendo sempre più tempo a individuare modalità per insegnare comportamento verbale complesso (Persicke et al., 2022; St. Clair et al., 2022; Suarez et al., 2022). La Teoria della Mente (ToM) è un argomento complicato, che da più di 30 anni interessa diverse discipline umanistiche come la psicologia e l’educazione speciale. Nonostante le critiche mosse all’analisi del comportamento, gli insegnamenti che prevedono l’applicazione di training analitico comportamentali hanno mostrato di poter essere condivisibili anche con altre professioni che collaborano nel trattamento delle persone con disabilità (Gasiewski et al., 2021; Summers et al., 2022). In questo secondo articolo sul tema del perspective taking, continuiamo a fare riferimento agli studi di Adel Najdowski (PhD, BCBA-D) e Angela Persicke (MA, BCBA), attualmente Professoresse presso la Pepperdine University e con all’attivo più di 40 pubblicazioni e collabo- razioni con giornali di riferimento nel campo dell’analisi del comportamento (ad esempio «Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis»), trattando in particolare, le preferenze, i desideri, il sarcasmo e le emozioni. Oltre ad esempi e materiali, verranno discussi i possibili utilizzi dei training e limitazioni.
... However, sarcasm perception can be improved. For example, individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) enhanced their sarcasm perception after training in prosody, gesture, and facial expressions, and incorporating in vivo practice (Persicke et al., 2013). ...
Article
When verbal and nonverbal communications (NVC) lack congruency, nonverbal cues are often relied upon to perceive meaning. This study measured participant reliance on verbal and NVC to understand speaker intention, and if participants' accuracy of perception varied with cue use. Commonly used gestural cues for sarcasm perception were determined using videos from the Relational Inference in Social Communication (RISC) database in two video conditions: With Audio (WA) and Without Audio (WoA). Data showed perception of speaker intention was more accurate in the WA condition for literal, humor, and white lie videos. Perception of speaker intention showed no significant change in perception accuracy across conditions with sarcasm videos. The eyes and mouth were referred to most when viewing all videos in the WoA condition, but not significantly more reliable. However, the results showed that relying on cue clusters may improve sarcasm perception and that verbal cues may not be needed at all.
Book
Full-text available
Motivate your students to learn and apply strong communication principles in personal and business relationships as Adler/Proctor/Manning's popular LOOKING OUT, LOOKING IN, 16E with MindTap digital resources applies the latest research to students' everyday lives using magazine-style readings, the latest pop culture references and expanded discussion of social media.
Article
Full-text available
Verbal irony can serve many social functions: Speakers can mute the aggression conveyed by criticism or temper the praise conveyed by a compliment (the Tinge Hy- pothesis; Dews, Kaplan, & Winner, 1995), and speakers can also bring humor to a situation. A full understanding of ironic language requires one to make complex in- ferences about speaker intent, a task that can be challenging for children. This study was devised as a developmental test of the Tinge Hypothesis. Two experiments assessed 5- to 6- and 7- to 8-year-old children's abilities to detect and interpret the aggressive and humorous intent of speakers who made ironic criticisms, literal crit- icisms, ironic compliments, and literal compliments depicted in puppet shows. When children detected the use of irony, their aggression ratings provided support for the Tinge Hypothesis but their humor ratings indicated that the humor function was not recognized.
Article
Full-text available
Rule-governed behavior is generally considered an integral component of complex verbal repertoires but has rarely been the subject of empirical research. In particular, little or no previous research has attempted to establish rule-governed behavior in individuals who do not already display the repertoire. This study consists of two experiments that evaluated multiple exemplar training procedures for teaching a simple component skill, which may be necessary for developing a repertoire of rule-governed behavior. In both experiments, children with autism were taught to respond to simple rules that specified antecedents and the behaviors that should occur in their presence. In the first study, participants were taught to respond to rules containing "if/then" statements, where the antecedent was specified before the behavior. The second experiment was a replication and extension of the first. It involved a variation on the manner in which rules were presented. Both experiments eventually demonstrated generalization to novel rules for all participants; however variations to the standard procedure were required for several participants. Results suggest that rule-following can be analyzed and taught as generalized operant behavior and implications for future research are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
To comprehend figurative utterances such as metaphor or sarcasm, a listener must both judge the literal meaning of the statement and infer the speaker's intended meaning (mentalizing; Amodio and Frith, 2006). To delineate the neural substrates of pragmatic comprehension, we conducted functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with 20 normal adult volunteers. Participants read short stories followed by a target sentence. Depending on the context provided by the preceding stories, the target sentences were classified as follows: (1) metaphor versus literally coherent; (2) metaphor versus literally incoherent; (3) sarcasm versus literally coherent; and (4) sarcasm versus literally incoherent. For each task pair, we directly compared the activations evoked by the same target sentences in the different contexts. The contrast images were incorporated into a 2 (metaphor and sarcasm)×2 (literal coherency and incoherency) design. Metaphor-specific activation was found in the head of the caudate, which might be involved in associating statements with potential meanings, and restricting sentence meanings within a set of possible candidates for what the speaker intended. Sarcasm-specific activation was found in the left amygdala, which is an important component of the neural substrates of social behavior. Conjunction analysis revealed that both metaphor and sarcasm activated the anterior rostral medial frontal cortex (arMFC), which is a key node of mentalizing. A distinct literal coherency effect was found in the orbital MFC, which is thought to be involved in monitoring. These mesial frontal areas are jointly involved in monitoring literal coherency and mentalizing within social contexts in order to comprehend the pragmatic meanings of utterances.
Article
Full-text available
We examined processing of verbal irony in three groups of children: (1) 18 children with high-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder (HFASD), (2) 18 typically- developing children, matched to the first group for verbal ability, and (3) 18 typically-developing children matched to the first group for chronological age. We utilized an irony comprehension task that minimized verbal and pragmatic demands for participants. Results showed that children with HFASD were as accurate as typicallydeveloping children in judging speaker intent for ironic criticisms, but group differences in judgment latencies, eye gaze, and humor evaluations suggested that children with HFASD applied a different processing strategy for irony comprehension; one that resulted in less accurate appreciation of the social functions of irony.
Article
Difficulties with figurative language have been highlighted by many researchers and people with autistic spectrum disorders (ASDs) as a core problem of the condition. However, few tests exist which describe and assess the responses of children with autism to figurative language. This paper reports a study which evaluated figurative language skills in able children with ASD. A group of children with ASD and a group of age-matched peers with no ASD were compared using a test of figurative language devised for the investigation. The test focused on understanding the meaning of an utterance and understanding the intentionality behind an utterance. Six categories of figurative language were examined. In all six, the controls with no ASD performed more successfully than the group with ASD, at statistically significant levels in most cases. The study also showed qualitative idiosyncrasies among those with ASD. The results are discussed in terms of their clinical and theoretical importance.
Article
This article reports the findings of a single study examining irony in talk among friends. Sixty-two 10-min conversations between college students and their friends were recorded and analyzed. Five main types of irony were found: jocularity, sarcasm, hyperbole, rhetorical questions, and understatements. These different forms of ironic language were part of 8% of all conversational turns. Analysis of these utterances revealed varying linguistic and social patterns and suggested several constraints on how and why people achieve ironic meaning. The implications of this conclusion for psychological theories of irony are discussed.
Article
The "allusional pretense" theory of verbal irony (Kumon-Nakamura, Glucksberg, & Brown, 1995) claims that irony can be characterized as having 2 main features: (a) al- lusion to behavioral expectations, prior thoughts utterances, social conventions, and so forth and (b) pragmatic insincerity. Following a brief review of what is currently known about children's comprehension of irony, the focus of this article is on descrip- tion of a developmental study testing the allusional pretense theory. One result of this study is that, just like adults, 8-year-old children were found to be able to recognize both allusion and pragmatic insincerity behind ironic speech acts. The potential effect of propositional form on irony comprehension is also discussed.
Article
To recognize ironic sarcasm, adults may rely on either of 2 cues: the context in which the utterance is made, or the speaker's intonation. In 2 experiments comparing third graders (8–9 years old), sixth graders (11–12 years old), and adults, we investigated the development of children's ability to use these cues. In the first, children were able to recognize sarcasm when the speakers used sarcastic intonation but failed to do so without the intonation cue, even if the context strongly indicated a nonliteral interpretation. In the second experiment, subjects delivered dialogue with intonation they deemed appropriate—and justified their choices—based on contexts that either suggested sarcasm or not. Young children again appeared largely oblivious to contextually implied sarcasm. These results suggest that children initially depend more heavily on intonation than on context in recognizing sarcasm.
Article
Central coherence theory (Frith, U., 1989. Autism: Explaining the Enigma. Blackwell, Oxford.) is addressed by exploring linguistic processing in normally intelligent adults with either autism or Asperger syndrome, to test whether local coherence is impaired. Local coherence is the ability to make contextually meaningful connections between linguistic information in short-term or working memory. Experiment 1 demonstrated that individuals with an autism spectrum condition were less likely to use the sentence context spontaneously to provide the context-appropriate pronunciation of a homograph. Experiment 2 presented scenarios which had a situation and outcome which only cohered if a bridging inference was drawn. The clinical groups were less likely to select the most coherent (bridging) inference from competing alternatives. Experiment 3 demonstrated that individuals with an autism spectrum condition were less able to use context to interpret an auditorily presented ambiguous sentence. The findings from Experiments 2 and 3 suggest that individuals with autism or Asperger syndrome have a difficulty in achieving local coherence, while the evidence from Experiment 1 suggests a preference not to strive for coherence. Taken together, these results suggest that individuals with an autism spectrum condition are impaired in achieving local coherence, and they have a preference not to strive for coherence unless instructed to do so, or unless they make a conscious decision to do so. Moreover, the three experiments correlate with one another, which suggests that central coherence may be a unitary force in these different tasks. Of the two clinical groups, the autism group had the greater difficulty in achieving coherence. Possible explanations for the clinical groups' difficulty are explored.