ArticlePDF Available

What are musical identities, and why are they important

Authors:

Abstract

Music is a fundamental channel of communication: it provides a means by which people can share emotions, intentions and meanings even though their spoken languages may be mutually incomprehensible. It can also provide a vital lifeline to human interaction for those whose special needs make other means of communication difficult. Music can exert powerful physical effects, can produce deep and profound emotions within us, and can be used to generate infinitely subtle variations of expressiveness by skilled com- posers and performers. At the same time, music plays a greater part in the everyday lives of more people than at any time in the past. This is partly the result of the extremely rapid technological developments that have occurred in the last two decades or so, allied to the increasing commercialization and economic power of the music industry. In the developed coun- tries of the world at least, the widespread availability and relative inexpensiveness of the Walkman, the Internet, the MIDI interface, the video recorder and more means that a vast diversity of musical styles and genres is available to us as listeners. The ways in which people experience music—as 'consumers', fans, listeners, composers, arrangers, performers or critics—are far more diverse than at any time in the past, as are the range of contexts in which this takes place. One result is that music can be used increasingly as a means by which we formulate and express our individual identities. We use it not only to regulate our own everyday moods and behaviours, but also to present ourselves to others in the way we prefer. Our musical tastes and preferences can form an important statement of our values and attitudes, and composers and performers use their music to express their own distinc- tive views of the world. Nicholas Cook (1998) puts this succinctly: 'In today's world, deciding what music to listen to is a significant part of deciding and announcing to peo- ple not just who you "want to be" . . . but who you are. "Music" is a very small word to encompass something that takes as many forms as there are cultural or sub-cultural identities' (p. 5). This concept of identity enables us to look at the widespread and varied interactions between music and the individual. The concepts of identity and the self have undergone some radical changes in psychological theory in recent years, to which we will return
CHAPTER 1
WHAT ARE MUSICAL
IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE
THEY IMPORTANT?
DAVID J. HARGREAVES, DOROTHY MIELL AND
RAYMOND A.R. MACDONALD
Music is a fundamental channel of communication: it provides a means by which people
can share emotions, intentions and meanings even though their spoken languages may
be mutually incomprehensible. It can also provide a vital lifeline to human interaction
for those whose special needs make other means of communication difficult. Music can
exert powerful physical effects, can produce deep and profound emotions within us,
and can be used to generate infinitely subtle variations of expressiveness by skilled com-
posers and performers.
At the same time, music plays a greater part in the everyday lives of more people than
at any time in the past. This is partly the result of the extremely rapid technological
developments that have occurred in the last two decades or so, allied to the increasing
commercialization and economic power of the music industry. In the developed coun-
tries of the world at least, the widespread availability and relative inexpensiveness of the
Walkman, the Internet, the MIDI interface, the video recorder and more means that
a vast diversity of musical styles and genres is available to us as listeners. The ways in
which people experience music—as ‘consumers’, fans, listeners, composers, arrangers,
performers or critics—are far more diverse than at any time in the past, as are the range
of contexts in which this takes place.
One result is that music can be used increasingly as a means by which we formulate
and express our individual identities. We use it not only to regulate our own everyday
moods and behaviours, but also to present ourselves to others in the way we prefer.
Our musical tastes and preferences can form an important statement of our values and
attitudes, and composers and performers use their music to express their own distinc-
tive views of the world. Nicholas Cook (1998) puts this succinctly: ‘In today’s world,
deciding what music to listen to is a significant part of deciding and announcing to peo-
ple not just who you “want to be”... but who you are. “Music” is a very small word to
encompass something that takes as many forms as there are cultural or sub-cultural
identities’ (p. 5).
This concept of identity enables us to look at the widespread and varied interactions
between music and the individual. The concepts of identity and the self have undergone
some radical changes in psychological theory in recent years, to which we will return
2MUSICAL IDENTITIES
later in this chapter. The idea of the self as a kind of focus, or relatively unchanging
core aspect of individuals personalities, has given way to a much less static and more
dynamic view of the self as something which is constantly being reconstructed and
renegotiated according to the experiences, situations and other people with whom we
interact in everyday life. Globalization and technological advance have led to rapid recent
changes in many peoples lifestyles, and our self-identities are changing correspondingly
in ever more complex ways.
This book is about the role that music plays in this process, and we introduce the
concept of musical identities as a crucial means of doing so. In this opening chapter, we
shall attempt to answer some basic questions: what are musical identities, who has them
and how do they form and develop? We tackle these questions from a social psycho-
logical perspective, as indeed do the majority of contributors to the book. The same
questions are also of current interest in other disciplines: DeNoras (2000) sociological
exploration of the role of music as a means of structuring everyday experience is a
notable recent example. We have made an ambitious attempt to break new ground:
our aim is to set out the territory and ask critical questions rather than necessarily to
provide definitive answers.
The two-part structure of the book is based on the conceptual distinction between
what we will call identities in music (IIM) and music in identities (MII). Part One, on
IIM, deals with those aspects of musical identities that are socially defined within given
cultural roles and musical categories. As we shall see, the ways in which young people
do or do not define themselves as musicians, for example, and the role of specific influ-
ences such as the school and the family are central reference points for young peoples
self-concepts with respect to music. In the same way, the culturally defined features
of musician, composer, performer, improviser or teacher are central to the identities of
professional musicians.
Part Two, on MII, focuses on how we use music as a means or resource for developing
other aspects of our individual identities. Our identities can be thought of as complex,
hierarchical networks of inter-related constructs: some of these are overarching,
superordinate constructs incorporating others which exist at a more subordinate level.
For example, Bem (1981) proposed that gender schemas, namely our self-definitions
as masculine or feminine, are at the core of most of the other constructs we use to
define ourselves, such that most of the new information we receive about people and
their behaviour initially is coded and interpreted in terms of gender norms. This is one
aspect of MII that is reviewed in this book, by Nicola Dibben, and we deal also with
other aspects including national identity, youth identity and identity as a disabled per-
son.
Four main sections follow this opening section of the chapter. In the first of these,
we take a brief look at the development of music psychology over its relatively short
history, and attempt to show how various theoretical and empirical developments lead
naturally to the investigation of musical identities. We then look more closely at how
peoples self-concepts and identities have been defined and described within social
psychology, and outline the main theoretical concepts that might be useful when we try
to explain musical identities. The next section represents our attempt to define and
explain the development of musical identities, looking at IIM and MII in turn. In the
WHAT ARE MUSICAL IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE THEY IMPORTANT?3
final section, we look ahead to the remaining contents of the book, and summarize the
main orientation and contribution of each chapter.
Musical identity and the development of music psychology
There has been an explosive increase of interest in the psychological basis of musical
thinking, behaviour and development over the last two decades or so which shows no
sign of abating. Music psychology has very clear overlaps with a number of related
disciplines such as cognitive science and computing, sociology, cultural studies, anthro-
pology, education, medicine and health studies, acoustics, broadcasting, marketing and
communication studies, as well as with music and musicology. The research is reported
in journals in all of these fields, as well as in specialist music psychology journals such as
Music Perception, Musicae Scientiae and Psychology of Music. The subdisciplines of
cognitive, developmental and social music psychology are now clearly identifiable, and
a brief historical outline of the theoretical and empirical developments which led to
their emergence shows how the investigation of musical identities is a natural next step.
Psychometrics, acoustics and cognitive psychology
Before the 1980s, the discipline was characterized by a preponderance of psychometric
and acoustical studies. Some well-known early texts such as Lundins (1967) An Objective
Psychology of Music and Shuter-Dyson and Gabriels The Psychology of Musical Ability
(1968, 1981) reflect this orientation very clearly, which can probably be traced right back
to Seashores (1938) The Psychology of Music. The main emphasis was on the objective
measurement of acoustic abilities, and there was very little interest in the study of musi-
cal behaviour as such.
With the publication of two books, both entitled The Psychology of Music, by John
Davies (1978) and Diana Deutsch (1982, 1999), a new era began which broadened the
horizons of the discipline, and which had a strong grounding in cognitive psychology.
The cognitive psychology of music deals with the internalized rules, strategies and oper-
ations which people employ in musical behaviour, and this early work included studies
of the effects on listeners of tones, intervals and scales; of the perception of and memory
for melody; and of the internal representation of harmony and larger scale aspects of
musical structure (see Sloboda, 1985).
Although this tradition continues in contemporary research, some of the early research
was criticized subsequently for its narrow focus on the minutiae of musical experience,
for the artificiality of some of its experimental paradigms and laboratory testing situ-
ations, for the unrepresentativeness of its participant groups and because many of the
experimental stimuli employed bore very little relation to actual musical materials.
Whilst these criticisms may still apply to some research studies, the cognitive psychol-
ogy of music has now developed to include much more complex and ecologically valid
questions about musical behaviour and understanding, including such issues as musical
expressiveness and performance; the emotional effects of music; creativity in compos-
ition and improvisation; and practical issues for musicians such as sight reading and
practice techniques.
4MUSICAL IDENTITIES
The investigation of these issues needs to draw on theories and techniques which go
well beyond the cognitive paradigms of the 1980s, however, such that these no longer
hold centre stage in contemporary music psychology. Twenty years or so later, it is clear
that the discipline as a whole has diversified into various subdisciplines. Alongside the
contemporary cognitive psychology of music (e.g. DeLiège and Sloboda, 1997), whose
scope has broadened considerably, we can clearly identify the developmental psychology
of music (see, for example, Hargreaves, 1986; DeLiège and Sloboda, 1996) and the social
psychology of music (see, for example, Hargreaves and North, 1997), and it is easy to
foresee similar developments in other clinical and applied areas.
Social psychology
In the broadest sense, the music psychologists job is to investigate the multifaceted
ways in which we engage with musiccreating, performing, listening, appraisingand
to try to explain the mechanisms underlying its powerful influence on our behaviour.
This necessarily means that musical behaviour must be investigated in all of the social
and cultural contexts in which it naturally occurs. In a recent attempt to characterize
the discipline, Hargreaves and North (1997) suggested that psychologists have neglected
the social dimension, and that a social psychology of music ought to include the effects
of the immediate social environment as well as the impact of broader-based cultural
norms. The social psychologists role is thus to investigate the effects of particular
listening and performing/composing situations, as well as those of cultural standards
and norms which the historian and the musicologist might investigate (p. 5).
The social psychology of music is less easy to characterize than the developmental or
cognitive subdisciplines since it is less clearly based on a circumscribed set of function-
ally related phenomena, or on age-related patterns of behaviour. However, its potential
scope is delineated by Hargreaves and North (1997), who draw on Doises (1986) dis-
tinction between four types of explanation or levels of analysis in social psychological
research. The highest of these are the social-positional and the ideological levels, which
deal with the effects of broad institutional or group membership and cultural statements
of belief, or norms, respectively. Within music psychology, these might include studies
of the effects of social class, educational institutions or the media on musical behaviour.
The other two levels of explanation are more microscopic, localized or situation-
specific; these are the inter-individual and the intra-individual levels, respectively.
The former includes the analysis of small group effects, such as those of conformity or
leadership in musical behaviour, and the latter deals with the specific mechanisms with
which individuals engage with their musical environments, which might include aspects
of individual differences such as gender, age or personality.
The social psychological approach argues for a far greater emphasis on the study of
musical behaviour in everyday life situations than in the laboratory and the classroom,
and indeed to widen the sphere of investigation beyond specific, formal musical situ-
ations such as the concert hall or the practice studio. Another aspect of this argument
concerns the functions that music fulfils in peoples everyday lives. What is music for?
is a central question for the social psychology of music, and it has also been discussed in
anthropology and sociology (e.g. Merriam, 1964) as well as from an evolutionary point
WHAT ARE MUSICAL IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE THEY IMPORTANT?5
of view (Cross, 2001). From the psychologists perspective, the functions of music fall
into three broad domains, namely the cognitive, the emotional and the social.
Our contention, along with Hargreaves and North (1997), is that music psychology
as a whole has placed a disproportionate emphasis on the first two of these domains,
and that the social functions of music in the lives of individuals have been seriously
neglected. Because music is essentially a social activityit is something we do along
with and for others, either as listeners or as co-creators–—there is a strong argument
that the social functions of music subsume the cognitive and emotional functions in
certain respects.
The research evidence suggests that the social functions of music are manifested
in three principal ways for the individual, namely in the management of interpersonal
relationships, mood and self-identity (see review by Hargreaves and North, 1999a). First,
people use music as a means of developing and negotiating interpersonal relationships.
Ones musical preferences can define which social groups one does and does not belong
to, and this is particularly clear in the case of teenage music preferences (see Tarrant
etal., Chapter 8). Secondly, an increasing body of evidence shows that people use music
as a means of regulating their mood, and that this is mediated by the immediate social
environment in which listening takes place. This can explain patterns of musical taste
and preference which are linked with specific listening situations and social circumstances
(see, for example, North and Hargreaves, 2000).
The third area forms the central rationale for this book. We suggest that one of the
primary social functions of music lies in establishing and developing an individuals
sense of identity, and that the concept of musical identity enables us to look at the wide-
spread and varied interactions between music and the individual.
Developmental psychology
The central subject matter of the developmental psychology of music is the description
and explanation of the patterns of age-related changes that occur across the life span in
various aspects of musical behaviour. Several specific models and theories of musical
development have been proposed (see, for example, reviews by Hargreaves and
Zimmerman, 1992; Swanwick and Runfola, 2002), and an impressive body of evidence is
accumulating about the course of normative musical development, as well as about the
specific effects of musical training and education. This rapidly growing body of research
and theory has the potential to illuminate the ways in which musical identities develop
across the life span, although this issue has not been raised as such in the discipline so far.
One of the most striking features of the recent developmental literature is the study
of infant musicality, which is beginning to show how biological and social influences
jointly shape musical development. Papoušek (1996) relates early musicality to the devel-
opment of speech and communication. He suggests that speech, as a uniquely human
form of communication, represents an unusually effective means of biological adapta-
tion (p. 38). Early musicality is seen as an integral part of early speech and language, and
thus also presumably possesses adaptive significance. Speech-related and musical sounds
are partly learnt in the infants social environment, and Papoušek and Papoušek (1982)
have demonstrated how parents scaffold the infants vocal behaviour towards different
levels of expertise, and towards the acquisition of emotional meanings.
6MUSICAL IDENTITIES
Parents reinforce the musical aspects of early vocalizations, and also sing songs and
lullabies. Gradually, vocal/musical play gives rise to speech and words on the one hand,
and to more specific musical activities such as imitation and improvising on the other,
so that singing develops as another sphere of activity in its own right. Early musicality
thus encapsulates the interaction between biological predispositions and the social
world: the development of babies sense of their environment is inherently social.
Colwyn Trevarthen (1999, and Chapter 2 in this volume) has developed this idea into
a theory of a basic motivation for musicality which he sees as providing the origins of
later forms of musical expression and artistic creation. He proposes that communicative
musicality is the dynamic sympathetic state of an individual that allows co-ordinated
companionship to arise. He considers that music communicates with young babies
because it engages with what he calls an intrinsic motive pulse (IMP) which is gener-
ated within the brain. This comprises a rhythmic time sense, which is able to detect reg-
ularities in musical elements, a sensitivity towards the acoustic elements of the qualities
of the human voice, and the ability to perceive narrative structures in vocal or musical
performances.
In proposing his theory, Trevarthen uses the term communicative musicality to
characterize the broad features of early interaction and development, and this goes well
beyond music itself. Early interactions between parent and child develop into narratives
of mutually constructed meaning, which are characterized by their intersubjectivity.
Investigations of talk, singing and other rhythmic games with infants show that the
general features of interactive musicality are displayed in the anticipatory movements
and emotions which develop between infants and their caregivers. These early inter-
actions can be seen as forming the foundations of musical self-identity: early musical
identities are based on learning ones own position and role in relation to the reactions
and communications of the other people around, and they are subject to constant
development, renegotiation and change.
There is also a much more specific sense in which the musicality of early infant
behaviour has been described, however. Sandra Trehub and her colleagues, for example
(see Trehub etal., 1997), have been interested in the biological predispositions which
infants bring to listening. Their extensive programme of research has shown that young
babies are precocious in their sensitivity to rhythms, to melodic contours, to simple
frequency ratios such as octaves and even to some aspects of harmony, in ways which
previously were thought impossible. It appears that they possess strong biological
predispositions to respond to the musical features of the sounds they encounter,
although cross-cultural studies will need to investigate the relationship between these
predispositions and particular tonal systems.
In the broader field of developmental psychology as a whole, there can be little doubt
that the main theoretical trend over the last two decades or so has been the investigation
of specific social and cultural influences in more detail and depth: this has influenced
the course of empirical research as well as the development of developmental theory.
The essence of what has become known as the socio-cultural approach within develop-
mental psychology is the notion that the accumulation of knowledge can only be explained
within its physical and cultural context: that we must think in terms of situated cognition
(see, for example, Butterworth, 1992).
WHAT ARE MUSICAL IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE THEY IMPORTANT?7
The interest in situated cognition introduced the idea of learning as cultural practice,
and an interest in the ecology of childrens learning (e.g. Crook, 2000). This has led to
an emphasis on the interactions between the teacher and the learner and between pairs
or groups of learners rather than on the child as an individual learner, as was the case in
the past. Children are seen as taking on social practices and cultural rules as active
partners in the process of what Rogoff (1990) calls guided participation: individual
development is based on a developing and accumulative series of shared social under-
standings. This is the essence of the socio-cultural perspective on development, and is
now so widely accepted that the discipline of social developmental psychology, or
developmental social psychology, which is based on this perspective, might be said to
have emerged (see, for example, Durkin, 1995).
We suggest that the study of the development of musical identity is best approached
from this theoretical point of view. Childrens development of musical identities, which
have their origins in biological predispositions towards musicality, are shaped by the
individual groups and social institutions that they encounter in their everyday lives.
These form an integral part of those identities rather than merely providing the frame-
work or context within which they develop, and this perspective enables us to explain
identities in music (IIM) as well as music in identities (MII).
This approach also puts a new perspective on the investigation of individual
differences in musical behaviour: issues such as age, gender and personality differences
have been studied largely within the psychometric perspective. However, such lines of
research are conducted from the outside: they do not deal with peoples experience
of the features which define them as individuals. The concept of musical identity takes
us a stage further in enabling us to understand the individuals musical behaviour from
the inside: to explain some of the processes and mechanisms by which individuals
monitor and conceptualize their own musical development. This represents a significant
advance for the social and developmental psychology of music, and is the central task of
this book.
Concepts of self and identity in social psychology
The notion of identity has long been a topic of interest within psychology, although it
has been addressed in many different ways and from some very different theoretical
perspectives. As early as 1890, William James, one of the founders of psychology, was
perhaps the first theorist to try and understand the selfwhich he called the most puz-
zling puzzle with which psychology has to deal. In the century of research and writing
since then, this puzzle has stimulated a good deal of effort to explain, unpack and,
most recently, challenge the meanings of self and identity.
Self-concept, self-esteem and self-identity
There is a good deal of current research on different aspects of the self and its develop-
ment, and it might be useful to start by clarifying some of the current terminology in
this field. We might say that the self-system is made up of a number of self-concepts, or
self-images, which are the different ways in which we see ourselves. These self-concepts
8MUSICAL IDENTITIES
can be context- or situation-specific (e.g. how I see myself as being able to cope under
stress, or in an emergency), or domain-related (e.g. how I see myself as a linguist, or a
musician). Self-identity is the overall view that we have of ourselves in which these dif-
ferent self-concepts are integrated, although the ways in which individuals accomplish
this remain a central and unresolved theoretical question. Self-esteem is the evaluative
component of the self, and has both cognitive and emotional aspects: how worthy we
think, and feel we are.
Self-image and self-esteem have received most of the research attention. The develop-
ment of different components of self-image in childhood and adolescence has received
particular attention. The self-image includes aspects of personality style, appearance
and the social roles that we play. Those components relating to the specific domain of
music, for example, might include saxophonist, jazz fan or music teacher’—all
of which are instances of what we have termed identities in music (IIM). Harter (1999)
has suggested that these domain-specific self-images typically become integrated into
a generalized self-concept at around the age of 8 years, although the existence of such
acore self-concept or self-identity is itself problematic.
The self-image develops by a process of monitoring our own behaviour, and making
social comparisons. We constantly compare ourselves with others, so that particular
situations and social groups exert a powerful influence on what we do and what we say.
We also compare our behaviour with what we expect ourselves to do on the basis of our
self-image, which is built up from past experience, and with what we would like to do,
i.e. with our ideal self-image. Rogers (1961) suggested that when these comparisons
give rise to incongruity (between either ideal self and self-image, or between self-image
and actual behaviour), psychological distress can be the result. An accomplished
musician with classical western training may be disturbed to be asked to improvise in
informal situations, for example. Equally, someone whose ideal self is built on their
ability to improvise may feel embarrassed about their ignorance of musical theory, or
their inability to read a score.
The psychological distress experienced by such discrepancies is often felt in terms of
lowered self-esteemthe other factor which has received a good deal of psychological
research attention in the last century. Self-esteem can involve overall evaluations of
ourselves, e.g. as a musician, or of very specific aspects of our self-image, such as our
aptitude as a piano improviser. The factors that influence self-esteem and its develop-
ment have been studied extensively, and one of the key findings is the importance of the
influence that other people can have on an individuals sense of worth.
This influence of other peoples views can be felt partly through the indirect process
of comparing ourselves and our behaviour with similar others to obtain a sense of our
relative effectiveness and worth, even when those others may be unaware of their effect
on us. It can also operate more directly, however, when others comment directly on our
abilities, appearance and general behaviour. Such judgements are particularly influen-
tial when they are made by significant othersfor a child, this would mean parents and
siblings primarily, but could also include teachers. Family and school contexts can
therefore be crucially important for a childs developing sense of self and particularly
for their self-esteem. Some striking evidence for this in the case of what we might call
musical self-esteem is presented in Chapters 3, 4 and 5 of this book.
WHAT ARE MUSICAL IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE THEY IMPORTANT?9
Theories of self and identity
William James (1890) distinction between two components of identity, the I and the
me, has had a long-lasting impact. The me is that part of our identity which can
be observed and known, whilst the I is that part that is able to reflect on the me,
i.e. which has subjectivity and is the knower. The I therefore constituted the real and
unchanging self for James, whereas the me was seen as subject to change since it is
composed of social categories. James identified four aspects of the me: the spiritual
self, the material self, the social self and the bodily self, all of which were seen as plural
in that they come in different forms.
This view has been at the heart of the later theories which were influenced by the
work of the influential neo-Freudian psychoanalyst Erik Erikson, who coined the term
ego identity (see, for example, Kroger, 1993). The concept of a reflexive I influenced
by social encounters is also to be found in the theories of Cooley (1902), who wrote of
the looking glass self in which we gain our identities partly by seeing reflections of
what other people think of us. It is also a central part of the work of the sociologist and
social philosopher George Herbert Mead (1934), who made a distinction between the
personal and the social aspects of self in describing the I and the me. Mead saw
language as the supreme symbolic system for communicating and for negotiating inter-
actions, in that it allows people to carry on internal conversations with themselves and
to anticipate the responses of other actors. This was the essence of symbolic inter-
actionism, which pre-figured social constructionist theory.
Another account of the development and maintenance of self-esteem and the role
played by others is social identity theory, which was developed by Henri Tajfel and
colleagues (e.g. Tajfel, 1978). This proposes that individuals have a fundamental moti-
vation to develop and maintain a high level of self-esteem, and that this is established
through identification with groups of people who have a positive image, since social
identity and personal identity are conceptually distinct, yet inextricably linked. Individ-
uals attempt to maximize the differences between their own group (the in-group) and
others (out-groups) on those dimensions that favour the in-group. They maintain a
positive social identity by boosting the value of the in-groups attributes in comparison
with members of out-groups. For adolescents striving to establish their identities and to
increase their self-esteem, identifying with particular genres of music which they rate
highly (e.g. intelligent drumnbass) and distancing themselves from less valued genres
(e.g. pop) allows them to establish favourable social and personal identities (see Tar-
rant etal., Chapter 8).
In many ways, these psychological questions about the self are very much in keeping
with the common sense perspective on identity that those of us in the Western world
assume to be typical and a pervasive experience, i.e. we experience our selves as being
self-contained, internally coherent, different in important ways from others around us,
and relatively stable and consistent over our lifetimes. Psychologists in the main have
also taken this view of individuals, and their research has focused on investigating the
typical range of differences between people in self-esteem and other aspects of self-
concept, and what sets of personality traits might account for the key dimensions
underlying these differences.
10 MUSICAL IDENTITIES
However, social constructionist approaches within psychology diverge from this in
suggesting that the self cannot be characterized in this way. Instead, they suggest that it is
formed and developed continuously through conversation and interaction with others.
In other words, we are not just influenced by others, but are in effect made up of inter-
actions with otherswe are ultimately social and not personal beings. Social construc-
tionist ideas have diverse origins in a number of disciplines and, although there are many
social constructionist perspectives, all of them clearly diverge from those of James, Erikson
and Tajfel, in which the personal and social aspects of the self are differentiated.
The beginnings of this approach may perhaps be traced to Meads (1934) emphasis
on the interplay between self and society: that we cannot develop an understanding of
one without the other, and that this is achieved through interaction with others. We
play with a range of social roles in order to understand the self from the perspective of
others in different roles (a development of Cooleys looking glass self). Language is
seen as vital to this process as it allows people to reflect on their own behaviour as well
as that of others, and to come to a reflexive understanding of their selves as a result.
Others have built on Meads ideas in the social constructionist tradition, and their view
of the self is well expressed by Bahktin (1981):
I am conscious of myself and become myself only while revealing myself for another, through
another, and with the help of another . . . every internal experience ends up on the boundary. ..
To be means to communicate. .. To be means to be for the other; and through him, for one-
self. Man has no internal sovereign territory; he is all and always on the boundary (p. 287).
Social constructionist theories suggest that people have many identities, each of which
is created in interaction with other people, rather than having a single, core identity.
These identities can be contradictory; for example, a musician can be a different person
on stage than when in solitary rehearsals, and be different again when engaged in each
of a number of non-musical activities. In social constructionist terms, identities are also
always evolving and shiftingeach interaction can lead to new constructions.
Language plays a central role in social constructionist accounts of developing identities,
and Western theories of the psychology of the self have perhaps clung for so long to the
view of a single unitary self because most of the Western languages include words such
as I and me which imply that a consistent personal agent exists which underlies our
actions. In the language of some other more collectivist cultures, such as Japan, the self
is referenced very differently, and is signified by many more words, depending on the
other participants in the interaction. Bruner (1990) suggests that we make ourselves
and our identities through our autobiographical narrativesthe stories about ourselves
that we tell others and indeed ourselves. This is perhaps one reason why we have a sub-
jective feeling of having a core identitya relatively unchanging sense of self that has
a historyrather than the shifting and multiple identities of the constructionists
account. We construct particular narratives for ourselves as they fit our Western ideas
of what people are.
As we said at the very start of this chapter, music is a fundamental channel of
communication, and we argue that it can act as a medium through which people can
construct new identities and shift existing ones in the same way as spoken language.
The continual construction and reconstruction of the self through autobiographical
WHAT ARE MUSICAL IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE THEY IMPORTANT?11
narratives can occur in music as well as in language, and Gergens (1994) definition of
meaningful discourse aptly describes some of the properties of musical communication:
Meaningful language is the product of social interdependence. It requires the co-ordinated actions
of at least two persons, and until there is mutual agreement on the meaningful character of
words, they fail to constitute language. If we follow this line of argument to its ineluctable conclu-
sion, we find that it is not the mind of the single individual that provides whatever certitude we
possess, but relationships of interdependency.. .. We may rightfully replace Descartess dictum
(Cogito ergo sum) with communicamus ergo sum. We communicate, therefore I am (p. viii).
There is an obvious parallel between this process and Trevarthens notion of communi-
cative musicality, which we discussed earlier. The early interactions and narratives
which are constructed between babies and their caregivers display inherently musical
features, but also exist in other activities such as linguistic play and physical movement,
as well as in musical activities themselves. These can be seen as the origins of musical
identities, which are therefore not individual but mutual constructions, as Gergen
suggests.
Conceptualizing musical identities
We have now set the scene for our discussion of musical identities, and have made some
basic conceptual distinctions. Is it true to say that everyone has a musical identity? Very
few people claim to have no interest in or liking for any aspect of music whatsoever.
Most people have strong musical likes and dislikes regardless of their level of musical
expertise: even those who might proudly (and erroneously) describe themselves as
being tone deaf are likely to have clear-cut preferences. Individual patterns of prefer-
ence, described in the literature as musical taste, can be an integral part of ones self-
concept, and this is particularly clear in adolescence. Musical taste has been shown to be
related to age, level of musical training and aspects of cognitive style and personality,
and the notions of taste cultures and taste publics have been proposed to explain how
social groups might have distinctive patterns of preferences and values (see Hargreaves,
1986; Kemp, 1996).
However, we have already pointed out that peoples musical likes and dislikes vary
according to their moods, the time of day, their social situation and many other cir-
cumstances which are constantly changing. The complexity of these reactions and their
relationship to other aspects of everyday life are apparent in an increasing body of
recent research (e.g. North and Hargreaves, 2000; Sloboda etal., 2001). In other words,
music can have short-term, transitory effects as well as a more deep-seated influence on
our beliefs and behaviour. It is perhaps useful to think of a continuum of the levels at
which we engage with different kinds of music in different situations. The background
music that is playing in a store or restaurant may engage us at such a low level that we
are unaware of it: yet even this can been shown to have a strong influence, for example,
on our spending behaviour (see, for example, North etal., 1998). This is at the opposite
end of the engagement continuum from the peak experiences which people report,
showing that music can produce extremely strong emotional and physical reactions
(see, for example, Gabrielsson and Lindström, 1993).
12 MUSICAL IDENTITIES
In between these two extremes, most of us listen to music at varying levels of engage-
ment to regulate our moods in different contexts, and whilst involved in different activ-
ities. It seems reasonable to suggest that our broad patterns of preference, and indeed
even our transitory likes and dislikes, form part of our musical identities: presumably
those that exist at higher levels of engagement are more integral to those identities. Many
people regard themselves as fans, amateur critics or buffs within styles and genres that
particularly interest them, and can indeed be just as knowledgeable as professional
critics within these specific domains. The complexity and ever-changing nature of this
process is readily apparent, and it is in this sense that aspects of our musical identities
constantly are being reconstructed.
Although tastes and preferences in listening form part of the musical identities of
most people, they may nevertheless play a minor or insignificant role for others. As we
pointed out earlier, self-systems are made up of a number of different self-concepts,
some of which are domain-specific: the importance of the domain of music will vary
considerably in the self-identities of different individuals. This variation is very likely to
depend on the level of specialist interest or professional training in music. Professional
performers or composers are likely to be so highly involved that they see most aspects of
their lives in relation to music: in Chapter 6, Jane Davidson suggests that professional
solo performers take on additional performance identities alongside their everyday
identities. The musical activities of these individuals are at the core of their identities,
and this perhaps represents the most clear-cut form of the first of the two broad areas of
musical identity that we have delineated, and which we now go on to explore in more
detail.
Identities in music (IIM)
These are defined by social and cultural roles within music, and might be categorized in
a number of different ways. They might be derived from broad, generic distinctions
within musical activities: we could speculate that the culturally defined roles of the
composer, the performer, the improviser or the teacher are central to the self-definitions
of professional or skilled musicians. Cook (1998) has discussed some of the relation-
ships between these generic roles in terms of the question of authenticity, suggesting
that they embody outmoded and hierarchical value systems which derive from 19th
century European classical music, and which can be traced back to Beethoven. This
view of musical authenticity implies that its creators exist on a higher plane than
its reproducers, or performers, which in turn implies that music is something which
exists out there, in a sense independently of those activities which bring it to life.
Cook argues that this is inappropriate for contemporary musical experience:
There is, in short, a nexus of interrelated assumptions built into the basic language we use of
music: that musicianship is the preserve of appropriately qualified specialists; that innovation
(research and design) is central to musical culture; that the key personnel in musical culture are
the composers who generate what might be termed the core product; that performers are in
essence no more than middlemen. .. and that listeners are consumers, playing an essentially
passive role in the cultural process . .. in truth none of these things are natural; they are all human
constructions, products of culture, and accordingly they vary from time to time and from place to
place (p. 17).
WHAT ARE MUSICAL IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE THEY IMPORTANT?13
We agree, and the ways in which humans view themselves in relation to these culturally
defined roles are at the heart of our concept of identities in music. Most creators are not
solitary figures whose inspiration comes from some mysterious and unconscious muse,
but hard-working professionals whose work is constrained by the everyday demands of
working with others. Similarly, listeners are not passive consumers, but active partners
in a cultural process who use music to fulfil different functions according to different
social contexts and locations.
One body of research with an obvious bearing on the question of musical identity is
that which has attempted to investigate the personality characteristics of specialists in
different areas of musical activity. This has been reviewed painstakingly and expertly by
Kemp (1996), who has himself conducted a good deal of the empirical research in the
field. Kemp suggests that musicians as a whole have certain distinctive characteristics,
such as introversion, anxiety and pathemia (sensitivity and imagination), and goes on
to draw finer distinctions between the particular personality profiles of composers,
music teachers, instrumental performers, and so on.
The notion of the musical personality raises several obvious questions. Are people
with certain personality predispositions drawn to certain instruments or activities, or
does taking part in these activities give rise to the development of those personality
traits? Another central issue in personality theory is the personsituation debate: can
personality traits predict our behaviour across different situations, or is the situational
variation so great that it swamps any meaningful consistent individual differences in
behaviour? In some ways, these issues run parallel to those we raised earlier about the
nature of the self: do we construct core self-concepts which are relatively unchanging
across different situations and interactions, or do we adopt different selves in different
contexts? This is not the place to try and answer these big questions, and traditional
personality theory has some obvious limitations in dealing with these issues. For now,
we will simply note that musical personalities and musical identities are closely inter-
woven, and that our own emphasis is upon the way that both of these are constructed in
relation to other people and different situations.
Alongside these generic distinctions, we might also propose that specific identities in
music exist, which derive from special interest groups. Two obvious groupings might
be those relating to particular musical instruments and those relating to particular
musical genres. In the case of instruments, Kemps (1996) review of the research literature
on professional orchestral players suggests that orchestral string, brass, percussion and
woodwind players have distinctive personality profiles, as have keyboard players, singers
and conductors, and indeed that these may be reinforced by the stereotypical views that
groups of professional players might have of each other. This leads to the obvious spec-
ulation that specific musical identities might exist in relation to particular instruments,
although the theoretical implications of this idea must remain a question for the future.
As far as musical genres are concerned, Kemps review also touches on particular
aspects of the lifestyle and personalities of professional musicians in what he calls popu-
lar fields, such as pop, rock, jazz and commercial session work. This is an interesting
though undeveloped research area, and the role of genres in musical identity is much
more clearly apparent in the notion of taste publics, which we mentioned earlier.
These are social groups which exhibit distinctive musical style preferences such as for
14 MUSICAL IDENTITIES
jazz, country and western, folk, classical, or for subgroups within these broad genres.
Defining taxonomies or classifications of these styles is no easy task, not least because
the categories themselves change rapidly, particularly in pop music (see Hargreaves and
North, 1999b).
In summary, we are suggesting that identities in music might be based on generic
distinctions between broad categories of musical activity, as well as on specific distinc-
tions which cut across these categories, in particular instruments and genres. This is of
course extremely rough and ready, and musicologists and others could refine our argu-
ment considerably. Nevertheless, it serves as the starting point for our basic argument
that identities in music are based on social categories and cultural musical practices.
The development of these identities in childhood, which typically emerge at around
the age of 7 years, is based initially on specific activities within music. Alexandra Lamonts
research, which she describes in Chapter 3, provides a particularly clear example of the
way in which childrens self-definitions as musician or non-musician are based on
activities within the school curriculum. She found that taking formal instrumental
lessons was the critical factor in the self-description as musician. Half of the children
she studied described themselves as non-musicians because they did not have this formal
tuition even though they did play instruments within general class musical activities.
If childrens identities within music are grounded in social contexts, it seems even
more likely that their musical self-esteem has a similar origin. Reynolds (1992) has
reviewed the literature on what she calls the self-concept of musical ability, which is
how children see their ability in relation to others. She also reviews some psychometric
tests which have been formulated to measure these levels of musical self-esteem. The
crucial point is that childrens self-ratings of musical ability determine the likelihood of
their pursuing further activities in music, which in turn provide the opportunities for any
progress and development that might take place. To define oneself as a non-musician
at an early stage may preclude such developments irrespective of the childs actual level
of potential ability.
The mechanisms of the process by which childrens musical self-perceptions deter-
mine levels of motivation, and thence actual development and achievement, are
elaborated by Susan ONeill in Chapter 5. ONeill draws on Dwecks (1999) account of
self-theories, which incorporate the distinction between entity theorists’—those who
believe their abilities are fixed and innateand incremental theorists, who believe that
their abilities can be changed through practice and effort. People might implicitly hold
entity theories about their abilities in some fields (e.g. sport), and incremental theories
about those in others (e.g. music). ONeill provides a convincing account of the ways
in which childrens motivations to be successful in musical activities are dependent on
these processes of self-perception: those with what she calls mastery-oriented strategies
of motivation are more likely to persist despite instances of failure, for example, and to
pursue new challenges.
Music in identities (MII)
The second part of the book deals with how we use music as a means of, or as a resource
for, developing other aspects of our personal identities, including gender identity;
youth identity; national identity; and disability and identity. We suggested earlier that
WHAT ARE MUSICAL IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE THEY IMPORTANT?15
peoples levels of engagement with music can vary from having virtually no investment
to very high levels of commitment, and that these levels may vary markedly between
active and passive participation in different individuals. In a similar way, music plays
a greater or lesser role in other aspects of peoples identities. The musical preferences of
many 13-year-olds are a vital part of their overall identities, as Mark Tarrant and colleagues
clearly demonstrate in Chapter 8, whereas they may play an incidental role in the self-
concepts of others.
In order to explain the role of music in developing identities, we can draw on psycho-
logical theories of identity and of self-esteem, and three clear trends can be identified in
the developmental literature. First, generalized aspects of the self-concept become
increasingly differentiated with age. Whilst younger children might generalize being
good at one activity to various others, an understanding emerges in middle childhood
that they are good at some things but not at others (Harter, 1999): musical abilities may
well therefore begin to be differentiated from others at around this age. Secondly, there
seems to be a general shift away from an emphasis on physical characteristics and
activities in early childhood, such as sporting or musical interests, and towards more
psychological judgements involving feelings and emotions, such as how and why one
participates in those activities (Damon and Hart, 1988).
This developing focus on psychological characteristics provides the origin of the
mechanisms of self-perception that we described earlier, with its corresponding impact
on childrens motivation and ability in different activities. These mechanisms reflect the
third main trend in the development of self-identity, namely that childrens self-concepts
become increasingly based on comparisons with others in middle childhood through to
adolescence. Their own achievements and attitudes, for example in musical activities,
become based on comparisons with their peers. Once again, the message is clear:
childrens identities, including musical identities, are constructed and reconstructed by
making comparisons with other people, and this continues into adult life.
Plan of the book
The previous section represents our attempt to deal head on with the question posed in
the title of this opening chapter. This is an ambitious undertaking which raises more
questions than it answers; but we hope that others may be inspired to pursue some of
those questions. The chapters in the rest of the book make a start on different aspects of
the enterprise.
Chapter 2, by Colwyn Trevarthen, provides an account of the origins of musicality in
infancy which highlights the communicative power of music, and which thereby illumin-
ates not only the origins of musical identities, but also some of the processes which
govern their development. Because this provides a general theoretical underpinning
for the book as a whole, the chapter appears before the two main sections of the book.
Trevarthen elaborates upon his assertion that we are all musical: that every human
being has a biological and social guarantee of musicality. This is not a vague utopian
ideal, but rather a conclusion drawn by an increasing number of academic researchers
interested in developing our knowledge of the psychological foundations of music
listening and performance (e.g. Hodges, 1996; MacDonald and Miell, 2000). Trevarthens
16 MUSICAL IDENTITIES
chapter emphasizes that music is central in babies lives, and demonstrates the funda-
mental role that it plays in developing parentchild bonding. His ideas have wide-
ranging implications for many issues in the developmental, social and therapeutic aspects
of music psychology.
In Chapter 3, Alexandra Lamont discusses empirical research in discussing how the
structuring of musical activities within the school environment has a significant influ-
ence upon a childs developing musical identity. One of the most illuminating aspects
of her chapter is her reflection on the impact that instrumental music tuition provided
by peripatetic teachers can have in this respect. She demonstrates that children in
school who have no such instrumental music provision are more likely to see them-
selves as musicians, presumably through their involvement in general class musical
activities, than children in schools in which individuals are taken out of class lessons to
receive specialist tuition. This chapter has particular educational relevance: if children
do not view themselves as musical, it will be difficult if not impossible for them to
develop musically, as performers at least.
In Chapter 4, Sophia Borthwick and Jane Davidson discuss the influences that family
interactions can exert upon a childs developing musical identity. This chapter utilizes
script theory to highlight how certain key features of daily family interactions can have
a significant impact on childrens developing sense of musical identity. They trace the
influence of parents and siblings, noting in particular how parents perceptions of each
childs musicality can affect the interactions of all the siblings in a family. One par-
ticularly interesting finding is that the parents involved in the study were very keen for
their children to develop advanced musical skills, and viewed music as one of the most
important activities in which their children were engaged. At the same time, however,
these parents were not keen for their children to become professional musicians, and
this double bind situation often perplexed the children.
Susan ONeill, in presenting data from her in-depth interviews with young musicians
in Chapter 5, also highlights the fluid and constantly evolving nature of musical identities.
She explores the contradictions and complexities involved in adopting the label of
musician, and uses social constructionist theory to illuminate the delicate interplay
that exists between social perceptions of and personal beliefs about musicality. For
example, her interviews with four young female musicians reveal that issues such
as public performances and friends expectations of how a musician should behave
influence how these young people feel about themselves as musicians.
The final chapter in Part One, by Jane Davidson, introduces the notion of a performer
identity. She investigates the identity of the professional adult performer, and the
intrinsically social nature of musical performance. The stereotype of composers who
lock themselves away in solitude, wrestling with their creativity to produce original
works of genius, has very little basis in reality, since a growing body of research high-
lights the social features of musical creativity: a musicians creative output is inextricably
linked to a social and cultural milieu (Miell and MacDonald, 2000). Davidson
highlights some key distinctions between the musical identities of individuals who see
themselves as professional performers and others who are also technically accomplished,
but who see themselves as players but not performers.
WHAT ARE MUSICAL IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE THEY IMPORTANT?17
Part Two of the book (MII) looks at how music can influence and channel many
non-musical aspects of identity: at the impact of music on our sense of who we are. In
Chapter 7, for example, Nicola Dibben discusses some central and sometimes contro-
versial issues concerning gender identity and music. Her review shows that boys and
girls own gender development can influence their musical perceptions: girls often see
themselves as good singers, whereas boys have more confidence in composition, for
example. This gender distinction exists not only in musical performance, but also in
musical taste: preferences for specific styles seem to influence and be influenced by our
constantly evolving gender identity.
Mark Tarrant and his colleagues investigate young peoples musical identities from
the perspective of Social Identity Theory in Chapter 8. The distinction between in-groups
and out-groups is central to this approach, and their research highlights the ways in
which identification with musical styles affects adolescents identification with these
groups. This research shows that the music we choose signals many other non-musical
aspects about ourselves, and that young people use their liking of particular forms of
music to ally themselves with members of their peer group. Tarrant etal. note that
music can act as a powerful badge of identity for adolescents, perhaps more than any
other aspects of their lives, and that as such it represents a fundamental influence on
their identities.
In Chapter 9, Göran Folkestad explores the relationship between national identity and
music from a number of different perspectives. He provides examples that demonstrate
how our sense of belonging to a nation can be communicated through music, noting
that national anthems and the educational environment in which children learn about
music are both very influential in developing and sustaining our national identities.
Folkestad also discusses the influence of globalized popular music on peoples concepts
of national identity. Has the growth of popular music which does not recognize national
boundaries diminished our identification with music from our own country, and what
are the implications of this for music education? Folkestad provides some thought-
provoking answers to these questions which should generate further interest in this
topical yet neglected area.
The final two chapters of the book focus on the therapeutic applications of music,
and the ways in which musical participation that has explicit therapeutic and/or edu-
cational objectives might influence our sense of self. The relationship between musical
participation and an individuals psychological well-being is of increasing interest to
researchers and practitioners working this area. Of particular interest are the precise
connections between the educational, therapeutic, clinical and musical aspects of this
type of work (MacDonald, ODonnell etal., 1999; Ockleford, 2000), and Chapters 10
and 11 contribute towards the dialogue which is needed between researchers and clin-
icians. Both chapters discuss how musical participation can help develop an individuals
sense of ability in music, and feelings of ownership of a creative product, and this
highlights some of the parallels that can exist between therapeutic and educational
music interventions.
In Chapter 10, two of us (Raymond MacDonald and Dorothy Miell) focus on the
work of a music company that specializes in working with people who have special
needs. The chapter discusses both the musical and psychological developments that can
18 MUSICAL IDENTITIES
result from musical participation, and then considers how specific observed developments
in communication and musical ability can in turn influence the identities of those
involved in musical activities.
In the final chapter of the book, Wendy Magee discusses a music therapy intervention.
She carried out in-depth interviews exploring the music therapy experiences of her
clients and presents here the detailed analysis of one case study of a client with multiple
sclerosis, a chronic and progressive neurological disability. Magee highlights a number
of key changes in self-concept that appeared to arise as result of the clinical improvisa-
tions that took place during the music therapy sessions. In particular, she demonstrates
how music therapy gives opportunities for interactions that help to reduce the feelings
of hopelessness and isolation that often accompany severe illness.
In this opening chapter, we have tried to develop and map out what musical identities
might be, drawing largely on research in the social and developmental psychology of
music. The concept of identity is important because it enables us to understand individ-
uals musical development from the inside whilst clearly locating identity as an emergent
feature of our fundamentally social worlds. It provides us with a way of conceptualizing
the interaction between biological and social influences, and provides continuity between
our explanations of infant, child and adult behaviour. Studying the ways in which peo-
ple perceive themselves in relation to music has the potential to explain some
phenomena of musical behaviour and experience that might otherwise be inaccessible.
This is an important undertaking, particularly at a time when the nature of musical
experience itself is changing so rapidly in the globalized world.
References
Bahktin, M. (1981). Discourse in the novel. In M. Holquist (ed.), The Dialogic Imagination, (pp.
259422) (translated by C. Emerson and M. Holquist). Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
Bem, S.L. (1981). Gender schema theory: a cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review,
88, 354364.
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Butterworth, G. (1992). Context and cognition in models of cognitive growth. In P. Light and
G. Butterworth (ed.), Context and Cognition: Ways of Learning and Knowing, (pp. 113). Hills-
dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cook, N. (1998). Music: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cooley, C.H. (1902). Human Nature and Social Order. New York: Shocken.
Crook, C. (2000). Motivation and the ecology of collaborative learning. In R. Joiner, K. Littleton,
D. Faulkner and D.E. Miell (ed.), Rethinking Collaborative Learning, (pp. 161178). London:
Free Association Books.
Cross, I.C. (2001). Music, mind and evolution. Psychology of Music, 29, 95102.
Damon, W. and Hart, D. (1988). Self-understanding in Childhood and Adolescence. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Davies, J.B. (1978). The Psychology of Music. London: Hutchinson.
DeLiège, I. and Sloboda, J.A. (ed.) (1996). Musical Beginnings: Origins and Development of Musi-
cal Competence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
WHAT ARE MUSICAL IDENTITIES, AND WHY ARE THEY IMPORTANT?19
DeLiège, I. and Sloboda, J.A. (ed.) (1997). Perception and Cognition of Music. Hove, UK: Psychol-
ogy Press.
DeNora, T. (2000). Music in Everyday Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Deutsch, D. (ed.) (1982). The Psychology of Music. New York: Academic Press.
Deutsch, D. (ed.) (1999). The Psychology of Music, 2nd edn. San Diego: Academic Press.
Doise, W. (1986). Levels of Explanation in Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Durkin, K. (1995). Developmental Social Psychology. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Dweck, C.S. (1999). Self-theories: Their Role in Motivation, Personality and Development. Hove, UK:
Psychology Press.
Gabrielsson, A. and Lindström, S. (1993). On strong experiences of music. Jahrbuch der Deutschen
Gesellschaft für Musikpsychologie, 10, 114125.
Gergen, K. (1994). Toward Transformation in Social Knowledge, 2nd edn. London: Sage.
Hargreaves, D.J. (1986). The Developmental Psychology of Music. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
Hargreaves, D.J. and North, A.C. (ed.) (1997). The Social Psychology of Music. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Hargreaves, D.J. and North, A.C. (1999a). The functions of music in everyday life: redefining the
social in music psychology. Psychology of Music, 27, 7183.
Hargreaves, D.J. and North, A.C. (1999b). Developing concepts of musical style. Musicae Scientiae,
3, 193216.
Hargreaves, D.J. and Zimmerman, M. (1992). Developmental theories of music learning. In
R. Colwell (ed.), Handbook for Research in Music Teaching and Learning, (pp. 377391). New
York: Schirmer/Macmillan.
Harter, S. (1999). The Construction of the Self: A Developmental Perspective. New York: Guilford
Press.
Hodges, D.A. (ed.) (1996). Handbook of Music Psychology. Missouri: MMB Music.
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 1. New York: Holt.
Kemp. A.E. (1996). The Musical Temperament: Psychology and Personality of Musicians. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Kroger, J. (1993). Identity in Adolescence: The Balance Between Self and Other, 2nd edn. London:
Routledge.
Lundin, R.W. (1967). An Objective Psychology of Music, 2nd edn. New York: Ronald.
MacDonald, R.A.R and Miell, D.E. (2000). Creativity and music education: the impact of social
variables. International Journal of Music Education, 36, 5868.
MacDonald, R.A.R., ODonnell, P.J. and Davies, J.B. (1999). Structured music workshops for
individuals with learning difficulty: an empirical investigation. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 12, 225241.
Mead, G.H. (1934). Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Merriam, A.P. (1964). The Anthropology of Music. Chicago: Northwestern University Press.
Miell, D.E. and MacDonald, R.A.R. (2000). Childrens creative collaborations: the importance of
friendship when working together on a musical composition. Social Development, 9, 348369.
North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (2000). Musical preferences during and after relaxation and
exercise. American Journal of Psychology, 113, 4367.
20 MUSICAL IDENTITIES
North, A.C., Hargreaves, D.J. and McKendrick, J. (1998). The effects of music on atmosphere and
purchase intentions in a cafeteria. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 22542273.
Ockelford, A. (2000). Music in the education of children with severe or profound difficulties:
issues in current UK provision, a new conceptual framework and proposals for research.
Psychology of Music, 28, 197218.
Papoušek, H. (1996). Musicality in infancy research: biological and cultural origins of early musi-
cality. In I. DeLiège and J.A. Sloboda (ed.), Musical Beginnings: Origins and Development of
Musical Competence, pp. 3755. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Papoušek, H. and Papoušek, M. (1982). Integration into the social world: survey of research.
In P.M. Stratton (ed.), Psychology of the Human Newborn, (pp. 367390). London: John Wiley.
Reynolds, J.W. (1992). Music education and student self-concept: a review and synthesis of the
literature. Unpublished Masters thesis, University of South Florida.
Rogers, C.L. (1961). On Becoming a Person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Seashore, C.E. (1938). Psychology of Music. New York: Dover.
Shuter, R. (1968). The Psychology of Musical Ability. London: Methuen.
Shuter-Dyson, R and Gabriel, C. (1981). The Psychology of Musical Ability, 2nd edn. London:
Methuen.
Sloboda, J.A. (1985). The Musical Mind: The Cognitive Psychology of Music. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Sloboda, J.A., ONeill, S.A. and Ivaldi, A. (2001). Functions of music in everyday life: an explora-
tory study using the Experience Sampling Method. Musicae Scientiae, 5, 932.
Swanwick, K. and Runfola, M. (2002). Developmental characteristics of learners. In R. Colwell
(ed.), Second Handbook of Research on Music Teaching and Learning. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Tajfel, H. (ed.) (1978). Differentiation Between Social Groups: Studies in the Social Psychology
of Intergroup Relations. London: Academic Press.
Trehub, S., Schellenberg, E. and Hill, D. (1997). The origins of music perception and cognition:
a developmental perspective. In I. DeLiège and J.A. Sloboda (ed.), Perception and Cognition
of Music, pp. 103128. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Trevarthen, C. (1999). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human psycho-
biology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue (19992000), 155215.
... In the domain of music, which is recognized for its artistic expression [7] and cultural significance [8], the intrinsic motivations that drive individuals to pursue careers in the field play a critical role [9,10]. Music may serve as a vehicle for people to create new identities and modify old ones as it is a basic means of communication like spoken language [11]. Different people will place varying degrees of importance on the realm of music in their self-identities [11]. ...
... Music may serve as a vehicle for people to create new identities and modify old ones as it is a basic means of communication like spoken language [11]. Different people will place varying degrees of importance on the realm of music in their self-identities [11]. This variance most likely depends on the degree of professional training or specialized interest in music [11]. ...
... Different people will place varying degrees of importance on the realm of music in their self-identities [11]. This variance most likely depends on the degree of professional training or specialized interest in music [11]. To illustrate, professional musicians probably have such a deep involvement in music that they view most parts of their lives as connected to factors among music graduates in Fujian, China, that influence their choice of music as a professional career? ...
Article
Full-text available
Career choice has received significant attention in recent years, with research indicating a strong correlation between individual intrinsic motivations and career decisions. This study investigated the intrinsic factors influencing music graduates’ decisions to pursue a professional career in music in Fujian, China, using the Exploratory Design research method. This research design involved two phases. In phase one, the influencing factors were explored through interviews seeking to obtain qualitative information (N = 18). In phase two, the codes derived from the qualitative study were utilized to build a Likert scale for the quantitative questionnaire (N = 221). The qualitative results revealed that music graduates’ professional career choices were intrinsically influenced by their commitment to the music profession, sense of self-worth derived from successful performing and teaching experiences, self-purpose in sharing and teaching music, interest in music playing and music teaching, confidence in music-playing skills, and working relationship between teacher and student. These variables were confirmed by the quantitative study to have an impact on career choice. Commitment to the music profession was the most influential variable, followed by a sense of self-worth. Confidence in music-playing skills was the least influential variable. These findings deepen our understanding of the variables impacting music majors’ job choices. Understanding these determinants can help educational institutions, policymakers, and career counselors better understand music majors’ particular needs and concerns, leading to more supportive career pathways for aspiring musicians in the region.
... Existing literature suggests that music activities can be effective in reducing stress and maintaining or increasing functioning and wellbeing (e.g., MacDonald et al., 2012;Ruud, 2013;Toyoshima et al., 2011). Indeed, music represents a popular means for self-regulation, an important source of pleasure and a medium to support identity formation (Hargreaves et al., 2002;Saarikallio, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Beneficial effects of music on wellbeing and group cohesion are well established. Furthermore, participation in music groups has been shown to be associated with increased orientation to the host culture, while orientation to culture of origin appears to remain unaffected. In order to gain insight into the effects of music activities on acculturative stress in adult migrants, a group of musically active migrants to Germany ( n = 80) was compared with migrants who had never played a musical instrument ( n = 86). We saw that music group members ( n = 42) reported lower levels of acculturative stress and a stronger orientation to mainstream culture compared to participants who are not actively making music. The association between music group membership and acculturative stress was mediated by orientation to host culture. Solo-musicians ( n = 38) did not differ from group-musicians and musically non-active subjects. We found no differences in orientation to the culture of origin between the study groups. The findings suggest that (group) music making can support acculturation processes in migrants.
... The author made a claim that a transition and hybridization process between traditional and modern musical idioms now characterize the dynamics of music making in Northern Ghana. This corroborates earlier assertions by Hargreaves et al. (2002)that some musical genres connected to different ethnic groups are socially formed and so play a substantial role in an individual's musical education. ...
Article
Full-text available
Music serves as a vital reflection of cultural identity, allowing people to express themselves, celebrate, and narrate stories. In the rich tapestry of African traditions, the Ashanti people of Ghana regard music as an indispensable element. For them, music is not just an accompaniment but a cornerstone enhancing their way of life, deeply woven into various celebrations. Many communities possess their distinct musical customs, some not formally taught in Ghanaian schools, forming an essential part of their local heritage. The study therefore sought to uncover and catalogue the previously unacknowledged origins of existing musical genres within the Ashanti community of Ghana. Using qualitative research paradigm and case study design, data were collected among six purposively selected musical ensembles in Kumasi Township and their leaders interviewed to unearth and illuminate the roots of these musical genres and highlight their substantial impact on the social tapestry of Ashanti community as well as in the teaching of music and dance in schools. It was seen that the Ashanti have some unpopular ensembles such as sanga, Akosua tumtum, dansuom, alateawe as well as bɔsoeɛ which have rich origin and source materials for music learning. It is envisaged that, these diverse musical genres that exist within the Ashanti community are utilised in the education sector. This inclusion aligns with the essence of cultural education, not only in Ghana but throughout Africa, recognizing the significance of every culture's impact.
... Day et al. (2006) define professional capital as theoretical and practical knowledge acquired through initial and continuing training, as well as professional experience in the educational field, all of which enable the teacher to develop effective pedagogical and didactic skills and competencies. On the other hand, Hargreaves et al. (2002) define it as knowledge, skills and competences acquired through critical reflection and experiential learning, which enable the education professional to make informed decisions and develop effective strategies to improve teaching and learning. Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (2019) considers this term as the combination of knowledge, skills, competencies, and values that an education professional possesses, and that allows them to exercise their work ethically and effectively in the educational field. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background/Motivation This article presents a systematic review aimed at examining the utilization of learning analytics (LA) to enhance teachers’ professional capital. Aim The study focuses on three primary research questions: (1) exploring the characteristics and approaches of LA in professional capital, (2) investigating suggestions from LA for assessing and improving professional capital, and (3) examining variables studied in enhancing the most intricate dimension of professional capital using LA. Methodology To address the research objectives, a systematic review was conducted focusing on the key concepts “learning analytics” and “professional capital.” Following the procedures outlined encompassed in four stages: identification, screening, inclusion, and adequacy. The PRISMA 2009 protocol guided the systematic review process. Principal findings The findings of the study underscore the efficacy of LA as a catalyst for improving professional capital, particularly through collaborative learning and the utilization of tools like forums and online learning platforms. Social capital emerges as a pivotal component in integrating diverse types of professional capital, fostering opportunities for knowledge creation and social networking. Conclusion/Significance In conclusion, the study highlights the paramount significance of addressing teachers’ professional capital development through collaborative approaches and leveraging technology, particularly in primary education. The article concludes by emphasizing the imperative for more research and knowledge dissemination in this field, aiming to ensure equity in learning and address the challenges posed by the COVID−19 pandemic.
Chapter
The Oxford Handbook of Music Education offers a comprehensive overview of the many facets of musical experience, behavior, and development in relation to this diverse variety of contexts. While the first volume primarily focuses on children during school-age years, this second volume collects an international list of contributors to explore how music learning takes place outside of the traditional classroom environment. Discussing a range of issues such as music education for the special needs population, music learning in adulthood, and music learning through media and technology these chapters help to broaden conceptions of music and musical involvement.
Article
The use of music in marketing campaigns is ubiquitous. Frequently, brands choose music that they think will be personally meaningful to consumers so that positive associations about the music may spillover to the brand, itself. Yet some anecdotal evidence suggests that such a strategy can sometimes backfire. The proposed conceptual framework of consumer responses to music explains the process through which consumers interpret and respond to music which is related to their personal autobiographical experiences. We propose that the more music is associated with autobiographical experiences, the more a consumer will develop a sense of ownership of the music, and subsequently, the more likely that they will make a determination about whether they will psychologically “license” the use of the music to the brand. This effect is proposed to be conditional on the appropriateness of the music usage. The framework then traces such licensing to a series of consequences: identity outcomes, music outcomes, and identity-related brand outcomes. We conclude by outlining directions for future research.
Chapter
Music is an omnipresent part of life for elementary school students in much of the United States. The music they engage with throughout the school day serves as one of their primary sources of social learning, integration, and acculturation. In US schools, anthems representing various cultural groups and student cliques echo in the hallway and on the playground while the national and school-related anthems are formally taught and performed at certain times throughout the school day. These anthems serve as a way to not only represent or symbolize certain social groups, but also evoke emotional responses, communicate ideas, and encourage social assimilation and cohesion. This chapter will explore the various musical anthems of a bilingual elementary school in Miami, Florida, to gain greater insight into the meaning and functions of the anthems and the role they play in the negotiation of their national and youth identities. In support of this central purpose, the following sub-questions guided this study: (1) What anthems do bilingual students engage with throughout the school day and how and when do they engage with them? (2) How does the US national, school, and other cultural and social groups’ anthems influence bilingual students’ national and youth identities? and (3) Why are these anthems meaningful and valuable in their lives?
Book
Full-text available
Music is a powerful process and resource that can shape and support who we are and wish to be. The interaction between musical identities and learning music highlights school music education’s potential contributions and responsibilities, especially in supporting young people’s mental health and wellbeing. Through the distinctive stories and drawings of Aaron, Blake, Conor, Elijah, Michael and Tyler, this book reveals the musical identities of teenage boys in their final year of study at an Australian boys’ school. This text serves as an interface between music, education, and psychology using narrative inquiry. Previous research in music education often seeks to generalise boys, whereas this study recognises and celebrates the diverse individual voices of students where music plays a significant role in their lives. Adolescent boys’ musical identities are examined using the theories of identity work and possible selves, and their underlying music values and uses are considered important guiding principles and motivating goals in their identity construction. A teaching and learning framework to shape and support multiple musical identities in senior secondary class music is presented. The relatable and personal stories in this book will appeal to a broad readership, including music teachers, teacher educators, researchers and readers interested in the role of music in our lives. Creative and arts-based research methods, including narrative inquiry and innovative draw and tell interviews, will be particularly relevant for research method courses and postgraduate research students.
Book
This stimulating Very Short Introduction to music invites us to really think about music and the values and qualities we ascribe to it. The world teems with different kinds of music-traditional, folk, classical, jazz, rock, pop-and each type of music tends to come with its own way of thinking. Drawing on a wealth of accessible examples ranging from Beethoven to the Spice Girls to Chinese zither music, Nicholas Cook attempts to provide a framework for thinking about all music. By examining the personal, social, and cultural values that music embodies, the book reveals the shortcomings of traditional conceptions of music, and sketches a more inclusive approach emphasizing the role of performers and listeners.