ArticlePDF Available

The Treatment of Traumatic Memories: Synthesis, Realization, and Integration

Authors:
  • Metropolitan Psychotherapy Associates

Abstract

This paper is based on Pierre Janet's dissociation theory and his concept of the non-realization of a traumatic event. A model of treatment that integrates Janet's dissociation-integration theory with contemporary trauma-based models of therapy is delineated. The nature of traumatic memories is described, and a stage-oriented model for their treatment in patients with multiple personality disorder (MPD) is presented. Ideally a discrete phase in the overall treatment of MPD, this phase can itself be subdivided into the following stages: (1) preparation; (2) synthesis; and (3) realization/integration. Although a number of treatment recommendations are offered, the emphasis here is more on clarifying concepts than on the description of techniques. Multiple Personality Disorder (MPD) usually results from severe and repeated traumatization in childhood (Putnam, Guroff, Silberman, Barban, & Post, 1986; Ross, Norton, & Wozney, 1989; Schultz, Braun, & Kluft, 1989). The core pathogenic problem, i.e., the presence and disrupted impact of dissociated traumatic memories, is often independent of the form or complications of the MPD. Experienced therapists concur that this disorder cannot be completely resolved until these traumatic memories have been successfully processed. Modern clinicians working with MPD have developed effective treatment strategies to achieve this (e.g.. Fine, 1991; Putnam, 1989,1992; Ross, 1989; Steele & Colrain, 1990), but in our opinion the conceptual language in which these strategies are couched is inadequate. In two related papers, some of the authors drew attention to the uncritical use of the term "abreaction," and they argued in favor of the concept of dissociation over repression in describing and explaining the origins and maintenance of traumatic memories (Van der Hart, & Brown, 1992; Van der Hart, Steele, & Brown, manuscript in preparation). Recently,
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
The Treatment of Traumatic Memories:
Synthesis, Realization, and Integration
Onno van der Hart, Ph.D., Kathy Steele, R.N., M.N., C.S.
Suzette Boon, Ph.D. and Paul Brown, M.D.
Acknowledgement: The authors thank Barbara Friedman, M.A., M.F.C.C., for her helpful
comments on an earlier draft of this article.
Onno van der Hart, Ph.D., is a professor in the Department of Clinical Psychology, Utrecht
University, The Netherlands, and a psychologist at the Regional Institute for Ambulatory
Mental Health Care Amsterdam South/New West, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Kathy
Steele, R.N., M.N., C.S., is a psychotherapist with the Metropolitan Psychotherapy Associates,
Atlanta, Georgia, and is Clinical Administrator of the Post-traumatic Stress and Dissociative
Disorders Program at Parkwood Hospital, Atlanta, Georgia. Suzette Boon, Ph.D., is a research
fellow in the Department of Psychiatry, Free University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and a
clinical psychologist at the Regional Institute for Ambulatory Mental Health Care Amsterdam
South/New West, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Paul Brown, M.D., is a psychiatrist at the
Northpark Hospital Centre, Bundoora, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.
ABSTRACT
This paper is based on Pierre Janet's dissociation theory and his concept of the
non-realization of a traumatic event. A model of treatment that integrates Janet's
dissociation-integration theory with contemporary trauma-based models of
therapy is delineated. The nature of traumatic memories is described, and a
stage-oriented model for their treatment in patients with multiple personality
disorder (MPD) is presented. Ideally a discrete phase in the overall treatment of
MPD, this phase can itself be subdivided into the following stages: (1)
preparation; (2) synthesis; and (3) realization/integration. Although a number of
treatment recommendations are offered, the emphasis here is more on clarifying
concepts than on the description of techniques.
Multiple Personality Disorder (MPD) usually results from severe and repeated
traumatization in childhood (Putnam, Guroff, Silberman, Barban, & Post, 1986; Ross, Norton,
& Wozney, 1989; Schultz, Braun, & Kluft, 1989). The core pathogenic problem, i.e., the
presence and disrupted impact of dissociated traumatic memories, is often independent of the
form or complications of the MPD. Experienced therapists concur that this disorder cannot be
completely resolved until these traumatic memories have been successfully processed.
Modern clinicians working with MPD have developed effective treatment strategies to achieve
this (e.g.. Fine, 1991; Putnam, 1989,1992; Ross, 1989; Steele & Colrain, 1990), but in our
opinion the conceptual language in which these strategies are couched is inadequate. In two
related papers, some of the authors drew attention to the uncritical use of the term
"abreaction," and they argued in favor of the concept of dissociation over repression in
describing and explaining the origins and maintenance of traumatic memories (Van der Hart,
& Brown, 1992; Van der Hart, Steele, & Brown, manuscript in preparation). Recently,
1
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
Peterson also underlined the need to substitute more appropriate concepts for the term
"abreaction" (Peterson, 1993). Subsequently, in their treatment manual on processing
traumatic memory, Sachs and Peterson (1994) only retained the term "spontaneous
abreaction" as a synonym for flashback, and otherwise used the term "memory processing"
when referring to treatment of traumatic memory.
The purpose of this paper is to develop a phase-oriented treatment model based on
Janet's concepts of dissociation and the "non-realization" of traumatic memories. Within this
model a number of existing treatment techniques are comprehensively integrated. Since other
publications— in particular Steele and Colrain (1990) — already provide excellent practical
guidelines, the present emphasis is on conceptual aspects of the treatment model.
The authors first describe MPD, in Janet's terms, as a "disorder of non-realization."
They then explore the nature of traumatic memories, describing their various forms, and the
dissociative reactions which they may evoke. Next, they describe the treatment of traumatic
memories as a distinct core phase within the overall treatment of MPD. This phase can be
clinically and theoretically divided into the following stages: (1) preparation stage; (2)
synthesis stage; and (3) realization/integration stage. Finally, they discuss "synthesis" of
traumatic memories versus expression of emotions; the substitution of traumatic memories
with positive mental images; and finally "realization" as a social act, as a continuous mental
operation, and as the beginning of integration.
MPD as a Disorder of Non-Realization
Janet (1904, 1919/25) observed that traumatized individuals appear to have had the
evolution of their lives arrested.
They are "attached" to an obstacle which they cannot go beyond. The happening
we describe as traumatic has brought about a situation to which the individual
ought to react.
Adaptation is required, and adaptation is achieved by modifying the outer world
and by modifying oneself. Now, what characterizes these "attached" patients is
that they have not succeeded in liquidating the difficult situation (Janet,
1919/25, p. 660).
Janet (1935, 1945) added that these traumatized individuals have not "realized" the
traumatic event. Thus, realization requires putting the event into words, relating it as a
narrative, and reconciling the experience within the personality, thereby restoring continuity to
the individual's personal history. Non-realization of the trauma can exist to varying degrees.
Many traumatized people have a subliminal awareness of their traumatization, but cannot bear
to put it into words. They tend to evade all references to the event. If they are confronted with
it, they become highly anxious, a phenomenon which Janet (1904) called a phobia for the
[traumatic] memory, and which Van der Kolk (1988) regarded as an inability to tolerate the
feelings associated with the trauma. Their anxiety is, in fact, an act of separating themselves
from the traumatic memory, a flight from the act of realization (Janet, 1945). Complete non-
realization is found in those patients who do not make any allusion anymore to the verbal
formula which expresses the suppressed event, nor to the anxiety which stops them: for them,
the event ... seems never to have occurred. (Janet, 1945, p. 184)
Janet called this the hysterical form of non-realization, which was most clearly
demonstrated in MPD patients (Janet, 1910). In a comparable contemporary vein, Ross (1989,
p. 59) characterized MPD as occurring when a little girl who is abused imagines it happens to
somebody else. As an adaptation to continuing abuse, she creates alter personalities who alone
suffer the abuse. Non-realization is complete in this child in the sense that for her, the abuse
seems not to exist at all. For her traumatized alter personalities, the experience of the
traumatic events continues to exist as dissociated "traumatic memories" and, as such, these too
are also not realized.
A central focus in the treatment of traumatized patients is the processing of their
traumatic memories. This enables them to overcome their phobia for, and avoidance of these
2
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
traumatic memories, to reverse the dissociation of these memories, to realize the distressing
experiences, and to integrate them into the whole of their personality. The total process of
therapy, moving as it does inexorably towards integration may, thus, also be described in
terms of increasing realization.
The Nature of Traumatic Memories
One of the central features of traumatized patients, such as those suffering from MPD,
is that their traumatic memories continue to exist as highly distressing dissociated
psychophysiological states. These traumatic memories are states in which the traumatic events
are partially or completely, re-experienced. In the clinical literature on PTSD they are
regarded as forms of intrusive recall (e.g., Blank, 1985), while in the MPD literature, they are
often referred to as "spontaneous abreactions." The latter expression was the subject of the
authors' recent critique (Van der Hart & Brown, 1992), as it is based on the Freudian hydraulic
model of pent-up emotions.
However denoted, traumatic memories are part of experiential recall, and must be
distinguished from ordinary or narrative memory (Van der Kolk &; Van der Hart, 1991).
Traumatic memory is characterized by a sense of timelessness and immutability, has no social
function, and is reactivated by trigger stimuli. It also follows the principle of restitutio ad
integrum, the tendency of all elements of the trauma to be reactivated, once one element is
called into play (Janet, 1907,1928).
The term "traumatic memories" should also be introduced in the clinical literature on
MPD to describe those states which are partial or complete re-experiences of the trauma.
Because of its lack of specificity, Jung's (1921/2) alternative expression "complex" is much
less adequate.
When only in the dispositional or dormant state, traumatic memories are not usually a
hindrance. When reactivated, they then exert their harmful influence. Like "parasites in the
mind," they overgrow consciousness (Charcot, 1887; Janet, 1894). Traumatic memories never
exist as wholly isolated states. They are always "part of some personality" (McDougall, 1926,
p. 543). In describing acutely traumatized combat soldiers, Myers (1940) thus titled their
traumatized state the "emotional personality." He observed that this state went on to alternate
with the "apparently normal personality." He spoke about the "apparently normal," because
the traumatic memory could still manifest itself indirectly, for instance through intrusive
somatic dissociative phenomena such as motor paralyses.
In MPD, this doubling of the personality is more complex. During actual trauma itself,
through the process of dissociation, a number of alter personalities sequentially experience
different aspects of the trauma. The authors prefer to call this process linear dissociation (as
proposed by B. Friedman, personal communication, (November, 1993). Alter personalities
may also contain a range of different (non-successive) dimensions of the traumatic event, in
which case the authors speak of simultaneous dissociation. Janet (1907) distinguished the
visual, auditory and kinesthetic dimensions, among others. Braun (1988a&b) systematized
these dimensions in his BASK model. BASK is an acronym for behavior, affect, sensation,
and knowledge (in which we would also include the cognitions of the individual at the time of
the trauma). For many MPD patients, traumatic memories can be manifested both in a linear
and a simultaneous manner.
It is now widely recognized among clinicians and researchers in the field of post-
traumatic stress, as it was by Janet before them, that traumatic memory is reactivated under
particular conditions, specifically in situations which are reminiscent of the original traumatic
situation (Courtois, 1988; Gelinas, 1983; Steele & Colrain, 1990; Solomon, Garb, Bleich &
Grupper, 1987; Van der Hart & Friedman, 1992; Van der Kolk, 1994). These situations are
called triggers or reactivating stimuli (Van der Hart, Boon, Friedman & Mierop, 1992). For
practical purposes, the following types of reactivating stimuli may be distinguished: (1)
sensory data; (2) time-related stimuli; (3) daily life events; (4) events during the therapeutic
session; (5) emotional states; (6) physiological states; (7) stimuli recalling intimidations by
perpetrators; and (8) current trauma (Van der Hart & Friedman, 1992; Van der Hart et al.,
3
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
1992).
Blank (1985) distinguished four types of reactivated traumatic memories in Vietnam
combat veterans with PTSD: (1) vivid dreams and nightmares of the traumatic event; (2)
dreams from which the dreamer awakens, and then has difficulty shaking off the sense of the
recollected traumata, possibly even manifesting motor behavior related to the dream content;
(3) conscious flashbacks, in which the subject vividly experiences images of traumatic events;
and (4) unconscious flashbacks, wherein the survivor has a sudden, discrete experience that
leads to actions, and where the manifest psychic content is only indirectly related to the
trauma. Furthermore, in unconscious flashbacks the survivor does not have a conscious
awareness of reliving traumatic events, either at the time of the flashback or later. Amnesia
can follow both conscious and unconscious reminiscences.
In MPD the re-experiencing of a trauma by alter personalities may, in some cases,
involve complete loss of contact with present reality as well as amnesia for both the
occurrence and its contents. The authors therefore suggest the following distinction between
two forms of reactivated traumatic memories: (1) partial re-experiences, and (2) complete re-
experiences. The former concern the reactivation and re-experience of the trauma, while the
executive personality and its habitual consciousness also remains, to a certain degree, in touch
with the current situation. By contrast, during a complete re-experience, the trauma becomes
the total current reality for the dominant personality.
Partial re-experiences are reflexive, incomplete, and fragmentary, with much of the
content occurring outside the awareness of the executive personality (Steele & Colrain, 1990;
Loewenstein, 1991). Any dimension of the trauma can be reactivated and "relived." During a
flashback, the trauma survivor is usually still relatively aware of his or her current
surroundings and situation. There is a kind of doubling of consciousness, i.e., of reliving the
past (to which the person also begins to respond) while at the same time partially staying in
the present. The dissonance occurring between these parallel experiences can induce survivors
to feel "crazy."
An important distinction is made in the literature between conscious flashbacks and
unconscious flashbacks (Blank, 1985). During a conscious flashback, there is usually a strong
visual component of the traumatic memory, implying that there is a cognitive dimension to the
re-experienced trauma. Trauma survivors are also partially aware of their present
circumstances, and usually remember the contents of the flashback afterwards. This combined
awareness of both the trauma and the present situation can give rise to a confused sense of
time. Although both the trauma and the present situation are experienced in the present, the
current circumstances may be experienced in a more depersonalized manner.
It can also occur that the executive personality is more aware of other sensory
dimensions of the trauma than the visual one. Janet (1898) described in detail examples
pertaining only to hearing certain sounds such as screaming, to certain physical sensations
such as pain, and to emotions such as fear and anger. These elements were dissociated from
the rest of the traumatic memory, and may be re-experienced at a subconscious level by other
alter personalities. The executive personality may then be unable to comprehend what s/he is
(re) experiencing.
As previously mentioned, Blank (1985) distinguished the so-called unconscious
flashback. In this the trauma survivor has a sudden, discrete experience leading to actions,
where the manifest psychic content is only indirectly related to the original trauma. The
survivor does not have conscious awareness of reliving the trauma, either at the time of the
flashback or later (Blank, 1985, p. 297). The authors regard this phenomenon as being due to
the dominance of the behavioral dimension of the traumatic memory, with other (e.g.,
cognitive) dimensions dissociated from the executive personality.
Complete or near complete re-experiences were described in the classical literature as
hysterical attacks, or as somnambulistic crises (Briquet, 1859; Charcot, 1887; Janet, 1889,
1907). Recently, Loewenstein (1991, 1993) spoke of major dissociative reenactments. During
these, the executive personality is completely disoriented, and experiences him or herself back
in the original traumatic situation without any grounding in present reality. When the
behavioral dimension is also involved, one usually speaks of a behavioral re-enactment (e.g.,
4
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
Van der Kolk, 1987b). Patients may be completely oblivious to the current situation and
circumstances, such as the presence of persons in the same room. It is also possible that these
persons are perceived, but only in the traumatic context. For example, when a rape is re-
experienced during a therapy session, the therapist may be perceived as the rapist. In complete
re-experiences, the patient has amnesia afterwards, not only for the contents of the experience
but often for the fact that anything unusual occurred at all. Instead, the subject experiences a
puzzling loss of time or "amnesia for amnesia" (Kluft, 1987).
The partners of some MPD patients report observing them having had severe
nightmares, during which a trauma was apparently re-experienced. In such cases, alters gain
executive control and completely relive the traumatic experience, sometimes with an intense
behavioral re-enactments.
Dissociative Reactions to Reactivated Traumatic Memories
Re-experiencing a trauma can also be a traumatic experience in its own right. Just as in
the original acute trauma, further dissociative responses may occur. Depersonalization, for
example, not only can take place during or directly after traffic accidents (Noyes, Hoenck, &
Kupperman, 1977), but also during or after a reactivated traumatic memory. The authors
distinguish two broad categories of dissociative responses to reactivated traumatic memories:
1. Brief Reactive Dissociative disorder. Sometimes the dissociative reaction is so
intense, that it can be compared to acute stress disorder, the diagnostic category proposed for
inclusion in DSM-IV (Spiegel & Cardena, 1991). It occurs during or directly after a
distressing event, and is characterized by one or more of the following intense and often
persisting symptoms: (1) stupor; (2) derealization; (3) depersonalization; (4) perceptual
distortions; (5) subjective sense of numbing; (6) amnesia following the distressing event; (7)
sudden experience of terror, or other signs of physiological arousal.
2. Switching to Alters. For MPD patients, the distressing event has usually been
experienced by several alter personalities. During a reactivated traumatic memory the
personalities can alternate again. In response to this reactivation, it is also possible for
switches to occur either to alters who know only of the trauma, or to those who are completely
ignorant of it.
This switching may occur very rapidly. Many agitated alters, especially child alters,
alternate rapidly with one another without being able to reduce the attendant terror. Since the
primary psychological function of dissociation is to avoid re-experiencing traumatic elements,
it would appear that rapid switching defeats the original defensive purpose. It instead
generates intense dysphoria and dysfunction, and promotes further dissociation in reaction to
the dysphoric rapid switching process itself.
In summary, traumatic memories are sensorimotor states that may be partially or
completely re-experienced by the individual. In addition they may be conscious or
unconscious. They may remain temporarily in a quiescent state, but are often reactivated by
stimuli that resemble some aspect of the trauma, either literally or symbolically. The evocation
of one dimension of dissociated memory tends to reactivate all dissociated components, and
reactivated traumatic memories may themselves re-traumatize the patient.
A Phase Model for the Treatment of Traumatic Memories
The introduction of phase-oriented treatment models for traumatically-induced
disorders, dissociative and non-dissociative, is widespread, and the treatment of traumatic
memories per se is usually seen as a distinct phase. In Janet's original treatment approach to
post-traumatic stress, three phases could be distinguished (Van der Hart, Brown & Van der
Kolk, 1989): (1) containment, stabilization, and symptom reduction; (2) modification of
traumatic memories; and (3) personality integration and rehabilitation. Janet's stage model is
very similar to modern models of treatment for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and
dissociative disorders such as MPD (Boon & Van der Hart, 1991; Braun, 1986; Brende, 1984;
5
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
Brown & Fromm, 1986; Herman, 1992; Horowitz, 1986; Parson, 1984; Kluft, 1987; Putnam,
1989; Ross, 1989; Sachs, Frischholz & Wood, 1988; Sakheim, Hess & Chivas, 1988). For
example, Sachs et al. (1988) identified five phases in the treatment of MPD: (1) making and
sharing the diagnosis; (2) identifying the various personality states, and understanding their
purpose and function; (3) sharing with the therapist, and with each personality state, the
specific traumata associated with those personality states; (4) integrating the various
personality states into a single functional whole; and (5) learning new coping skills that enable
functioning of the unified personality, and prevent future personality splitting.
In this article, the prime focus is on Janet's stage two, the modification of traumatic
memories, which corresponds to phase three in the MPD treatment model of Sachs et al.
(1983). Their definition of this phase as the sharing of specific traumata with the therapist and
other personality states is well chosen. Successful treatment of traumatic memories involves
relinquishing related dissociations. This means that alters both involved in the traumatic
experience and independent of it must share with each other their respective post-traumatic
dissociations. The authors call this communication the synthesis of the traumatic memory. In
the older literature, the term "re-synthesis" was used (cf. Brown, 1920/1; Myers, 1920/1).
However, this term incorrectly implied that the traumatic memory had not been dissociated.
This is in contrast to the authors' view that dissociation is an immediate defensive reaction to
trauma.
The purpose of synthesis is to alleviate the dissociation by helping alters to share their
respective traumatic experiences of the trauma both with each other, and with the therapist.
This orientation is, at the very least, semantically different from those approaches which
encourage abreaction and catharsis of emotions. The authors consider "abreaction" to be an
incorrect explanatory principle, and an inappropriate treatment technique (cf. Van der Hart &
Brown, 1992).
However, the present approach does not stand for blanket discouragement of the
expression of emotions. On the contrary. When the patient needs to express the cry which she
suppressed during the actual abuse, she is told that she can now cry safely, and she is
encouraged to do so. This expression of emotion leads to realization and completion.
Abreaction or uncontrolled catharsis of overwhelming traumatic affects leads to states of
hyperarousal and, at times, to complete psychological decompensation. The hydraulic view of
affect ("It is in there, and needs to get out!") contrasts with the Janetian notion that the affect,
as well as the other elements of the trauma, needs to be realized and integrated or
"metabolized."
Therapeutic synthesis of traumatic memories is a purposeful mental action, based on a
conscious decision either by the whole personality, or by the alters involved in the traumatic
experience. For this endeavor to succeed the patient, or the alters involved, needs the complete
support of the therapist, not only through his or her sharing in the traumatic experience (as
emphasized by Sachs et al., 1988), but also through encouragement and guidance
When synthesis of a trauma occurs, the memory ceases to be re-experienced in a
traumatic way. The trauma becomes a "neutral" historical fact. In order to achieve this, the
patient, or the group of alters concerned, undergoes a process of "realization" of the personal
disaster which has occurred. The patient substitutes and integrates into his or her life history or
autobiography, a narrative memory of the event in place of the experiential traumatic
reminiscences (Janet, 1919/25), i.e., into the whole of the personality. This integrative mental
work is both a time and an energy-consuming reorganization of the personality.
The treatment phase of traumatic memories can be divided into the following stages,
each of which will be described below: (1) preparation stage, (2) synthesis stage, and (3)
realization/integration stage.
Stage 1: Preparation
The treatment of traumatic memories can be a stormy process and so must be planned
carefully. The preliminary groundwork follows the general guidelines for establishing and
maintaining a secure therapeutic setting, with particular attention paid to boundaries,
consistency, and predictability (Kluft, 1991,1993; Courtois, 1991; Fine, 1991,1993; Putnam,
6
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
1992; Steele, 1989; Steele & Colrain, 1990). In addition, in working with victims of childhood
trauma and with dissociative disorders, the following strategies should be employed: (1)
identification of alters and their respective conflicts; (2) development of a reasonable degree
of internal communication and cooperation among alters; (3)containment of traumatic
intrusion phenomena; (4) development of ego strength in the host personality, as well as in the
various alters; and (5) development of self-care capacities, i.e., capacities to tolerate affects, to
self-soothe, to tolerate aloneness, and to regulate self-loathing (McCann & Pearlman, 1990);
and (6) the introduction and use of hypnosis. The latter is for the purpose of enabling the
patient to develop control over dissociation, for containment and affect modulation, for
creation of a safe psychological space, internal communication, symptom relief and/or
substitution, and for management of other dissociative crises.
There are two important caveats regarding the application of hypnosis: (1) It must be
implemented within the context of well-conducted psychotherapy and therefore should be
used only by therapists with specialized training; and (2) particularly in the later phase of
treatment, care should be taken to prevent the defensive use of trance to avoid painful issues.
During the earlier synthesis phase, however, hypnosis is particularly useful.
Once treatment of traumatic memories has been initiated, the stage is set to prepare for
the synthesis and realization/integration of traumatic memories. The preparation phase
addresses: (1) safety factors; (2) issues of controlling or containing reactivated traumatic
memories; (3) exploration of traumatic memories; (4) correcting cognitive errors and
distortions; and (5) careful explanation and planning of synthesis sessions (Steele & Colrain,
1990).
Issues of Safety
Steele and Colrain (1990) presented an excellent overview of the preparations required
to ensure safety both during and between synthesis sessions. Preparation pertains to: (1)
intrapsychic safety; (2) interpersonal safety, both of the patient and the therapist; and (3) a safe
therapeutic environment. The latter is best provided in a minority of cases by a short term
hospital stay. Additional safety issues which should also receive careful consideration include:
(1) the degree of cooperation and communication among alters; (2) the general level of
hyperarousal and hypersensitivity to affective stimulation, as well as the level of affective
maturity; (3) the potential for suicide and self-harm; (4) the general level of coping of the
individual and the alters involved in maintaining function in the world (i.e., will these alters
participate in the integrative memory work, and what effect will that have on functioning?);
(5) the range of support systems and resources available; and (6) the level of motivation to
work through traumatic memories.
Controlling or containing reactivated traumatic memories
Traumatic memories do not usually emerge in a planned or controlled manner. They
are often triggered by a reactivating stimulus. When the timing is not right for their synthesis,
either in the overall course of treatment, or because the particular therapeutic session is too far
advanced, these re-experiencing phenomena should be controlled or contained using any of
the following techniques either alone or, still better, in combination: (1) identifying the
reactivating stimulus and, when indicated, relating it to the original trauma; (2) removing the
reactivating stimulus; (3) neutralizing the reactivating stimulus (involves teaching the patient
to regard the trigger as belonging to two different contexts, the present neutral one and the
past traumatic one, and helping the patient realize that the object itself holds no emotional
charge); (4) promoting and emphasizing the safe present; (5) suggesting temporary amnesia
for, or creating more mental distance from, the traumatic memory [for example, hypnotic
interventions such as the movie screen technique, the use of adult observers, and the use of
affective regulation techniques as described by Fine (1991) and Kluft (1989)]; (6) cue-
conditioning to help the patient return to the safe present (the patient can be trained to respond
to a certain cue in returning to the present if he or she should get lost whilst reliving a
traumatic memory); (7) screening traumatized alter personalities from the outside world; (8)
introducing a co- or deputy therapist; and (9) medication and hospitalization as crisis
intervention (Van der Hart & Friedman, 1992; Van der Hart et al., 1992).
7
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
Exploration of the Traumatic Memory
Either parallel to, or following safety preparation, the therapist begins to collect
information about the content of the traumatic memories.
1) Charting the Context of the Trauma. The authors first recommend defining the
historical context, including the personal history (development, school history, training and
occupation, supportive figures, pets, objects and other attachment objects, etc.), and the family
circumstances (social, cultural and economic factors); domestic factors (e.g., who lived in the
home, who shared bedrooms, family rules and discipline) (Steele & Colrain, 1990).
2) Completing the Story of the Trauma. Although most alters involved cannot, by
definition, relate a narrative of the unassimilated trauma, there are often some alters who have
a cognitive awareness without any associated affect, and who will be able to relate what
happened. Often this is an observing alter who watched happened "from a distance." In
completing the story, it is important to collect information from the whole sequence, including
directly before the trauma, during the trauma itself, and directly thereafter. This not only
provides a context for the trauma, but also ensures temporal continuity.
When an observing alter is asked to relate what happened, care is taken to inquire if the
traumatized alters can be protected from hearing the story. In this way, untimely reactivation
of the whole traumatic memory can be prevented.
3) Identifying the Alters and Their Role in the Trauma. It is preferable to know prior
to the actual synthesis which alters are involved, and what are their functions in the traumatic
experience (Sachs, Braun, & Shepp, 1988). Ignorance of which alters are involved, and which
should participate, may allow some alters to withdraw and refrain from sharing their
experiences with the others. Dissociated aspects of the traumatic memory will then continue to
exist, and can eventually be reactivated. Braun's (1988a&b) BASK Model may serve as a
checklist to determine whether all relevant dimensions of the trauma are covered by the alters
known to the therapist.
The purpose of this inquiry is therefore not acquisition of details for their own sake, but
rather to become aware of all of the dissociated elements, or "pathogenic kernels" (Van der
Hart & Op den Velde, 1991), which must be synthesized in order to neutralize the traumatic
memory. Often the personal meaning in these "pathogenic kernels" is more important than the
objective factual details. Personal or subjective meaning of the trauma is most often
experienced in the form of existential crises. Pathogenic kernels frequently develop in
response to, and contain in an undiluted and unassimilated form, intense existential crises that
are related to the trauma. Such crises are partially related to "stuck points" created by
discrepancies between prior belief schemes and new schemata generated by the traumatic
experience (Resick & Schnicke, 1992). These existential issues will be discussed further in a
later section of this paper, as well as specific approaches to the synthesis and integration of
pathogenic kernels.
Apart from knowing which alters were involved in the trauma, and which should
therefore participate in the synthesis session, the therapist should discover which of the other
alters can remain in the session as well, and which of them should be protected from it, e.g.,
by encouraging withdrawal behind an inner, dissociative "wall." It is also important to
ascertain whether there are alters who still actively object to the memory work. If so, these
should be engaged in negotiation to prevent internal punishment for premature revelation of
trauma-related "secrets."
Correcting Cognitive Distortions
This phase of therapy also enables the patient to recognize and confront any cognitive
distortions or errors that create dysfunction and misperception of the self and others (Fine,
1988, 1990, 1991, 1993; Fish-Murray, Kolb, & Van der Kolk, 1987; Janoff-Bulman, 1992;
McCann & Pearlman, 1990; Orzek, 1985; Ross, 1989; Ross & Gahan, 1988). Cognitive
schemas that were developed in environments of chronic abuse and neglect are inevitably
negative, with little associated flexibility or adaptive personality functioning. They remain
fixed along with the traumatic memories. The latter are reinforced by intense post-traumatic
affects. The negative cognitions, in their turn, reinforce these negative affects. During
8
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
synthesis the cognitions also become available for therapeutic exploration and change.
The trauma is encoded within specific cognitive schema, and continues to be
interpreted within this context by the patient until the therapist assists in the reconstruction of
new assumptions, beliefs, and expectations about the self and others. Cognitive distortions
frequently pose obstacles to initial memory work, and contacting the alters involved before the
actual synthesis helps the therapist to motivate them for this intense and painful work.
Resistances often emanate from deeply embedded beliefs about the trauma, e.g., "It lasted
forever"; about the perpetrator, e.g., "He'll know if I ever tell"; and about the self, e.g., "It was
my fault," or "It didn't happen to me."
Thus, in a case of sadistic sexual abuse by the father, a female patient's "father-alter"
had first to acknowledge that this was the part other personality which, by identifying with the
abuser, had helped her to survive the trauma. At the time, her defensive dissociation helped
her to believe that the intolerable abuse did not occur to her but to somebody else. Another
alter, in the same person, had to accept in treatment that she was not really crazy. She believed
that expressing her anger directly against her father would have made him abuse her even
more.
Assisting the patient, and alters, to change inappropriate and distorted cognitions is an
approach which may be required through out the whole course of therapy (Fine, 1988,
1990,1991; Ross & Gahan, 1988; McCann & Permian, 1990). Fine has extensively delineated
the necessity of cognitive work throughout the course of MPD treatment, and has utilized
Beck's (1979) cognitive therapy approach (Fine, 1990, 1991, 1993). Many cognitive errors and
distortions may be accessed prior to synthesis, but a number emerge in the course of
eradicating the post-traumatic dissociation. These distortions must be identified and processed
following synthesis. Others even emerge after the synthesis, when integration is taking place.
It must be realized that such cognitive distortions have created a gestalt of schema that
comprise the individual's entire worldview, a view that has been held for a lifetime. The
survivor's defenses are based on these cognitions, and usually have been incorporated into the
entire character structure itself. Change on such a scale of magnitude progresses slowly, yet
such change is necessary if the survivor is to heal. New, adaptive cognitions must be
reinforced over and over in the course of therapy in order to completely replace old, negative
injunctions (Steele & Colrain, 1990).
Explaining and Planning the Synthesis Session
During the preparation phase, it is necessary to give a clear explanation of the various
steps and procedures to be followed, and to provide a cognitive framework for the assimilation
of the traumatic memories.
A global explanation will have been given earlier in therapy. Now it is augmented in a
detailed manner. It must be repeated a number of times, initially with the host personality (or
the alter who is most active in therapy), followed the observing alters who are able to provide
information about the trauma, and then the other alters who are involved in the traumatic
memories. Sometimes the observing alter will be the one to inform these latter alters.
Prior to synthesis, the possibility of using artwork or other non-verbal therapeutic
modalities is explored. For example, severe abuse which is dominated by intense physical pain
destroys language and impedes verbal communication (Scarry, 1985). Thus, initial use of non-
verbal modalities in severely traumatized individuals may facilitate verbal communication.
Symbolic images may also provide a framework and a "container," perhaps for the first time,
for the traumatic experiences of all the alters.
The rationale provided for this treatment approach is that reactivated traumatic
memories continue to exert their intrusive and disturbing effects because of their dissociative
nature. They have yet to become shared and integrated parts of the alters involved, as well as
of the whole of the personality. To achieve this, the alters involved with the trauma share their
respective part-experiences with each other, unifying them into one whole, and sharing this
eventually with the remaining alters. The therapist emphasizes that the success of the
procedure is dependent on the completeness of the synthesis. This implies that all dissociated
"pathogenic kernels" must be synthesized. To share the trauma with each other, the alters need
to experience it intentionally, while also staying in the safe present, and keeping contact with
9
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
the therapist.
In many instances, all of the alters involved may accomplish the relief of the
dissociation, and the synthesis of the traumatic memory, in one session. This is called parallel
synthesis. With greater severity and duration of the trauma, resolution may take a number
sessions, which is called serial synthesis. Whether synthesis is accomplished in one or in
several sessions, it is crucial for the patient to pursue his or her own optimal pace. If two or
more sessions are needed, great care should be taken that the traumatic memories are well
contained between the sessions.
Stage 2: Synthesis
Synthesis of a traumatic memory involves its controlled reactivation in a collaborative
effort by the therapist and the patient. The involved alters re-experience and share their
respective parts of the trauma with each other, and with other alters. In this way the existing
dissociations are relieved.
A century ago Delboeuf formulated this reactivation as follows: Using hypnosis the
therapist "puts the subject back into the state in which his trouble manifested itself, and
combats the same trouble with spoken words, but in a state of rebirth" (1889, p. 71; cf.
Macmillan, 1991, p. 82). The "therapist's words" help the patient to stay in touch with the
present, and with the therapist, while the patient is, and the alters involved are, returning to the
traumatic experience. All dissociated aspects of this experience must be re-experienced. The
memory must eventually be shared with every part of the personality. That the presence of the
therapist is an essential factor in this process was aptly formulated by Jung (1921/2):
The support and understanding of the therapist raise the level of the patient's
consciousness and enable him to consciously bring the autonomous traumatic memory once
more under the control of his will. (p. 15).
While (re) experiencing the trauma, the patient enters an altered state of consciousness,
and loses the sense of chronological time. There is no sense of past or present relative to the
trauma, and so the trauma seems timeless. Synthesis must counteract this and add a sense of
continuity and finitude (Steele & Colrain, 1990). Using details collected during the
preparation stage, the therapist helps the patient to evolve from the period directly before the
trauma, through the trauma itself, to the period directly after it. The patient thereby
experiences a beginning and an end to the trauma (Putnam, 1989).
In addition to ensuring the chronological context, it is critical to help the patient
recover the subjective experience of the trauma, which is often held in the various alters. This
subjective experience includes a number of dimensions: the various meanings given to the
trauma (e.g., "this can't happen," "this has never happened to anyone else," "this is too terrible
to ever tell," etc.); internalized injunctions that are either implied or verbalized (e.g., "if you
tell, I'll kill you," "you know you like this," etc.); the various BASK dimensions including
cognitions, and existential crises (Steele & Colrain, 1990).
Existential Crises
Yalom (1980) saw an existential crisis as "a conflict that flows from the individual's
confrontation with the givens of existence" (p. 8). The crisis occurs at the moment when the
issues shift from the abstract to the experiential dimension, most likely when some form of
realization occurs. They become so real and intolerable that disintegrative anxiety occurs
(Steele & Colrain, 1990). Lifton (1980) aptly referred to the moment of existential crisis in the
traumatic memory as the "ultimate horror." Dissociation of the personality often occurs at this
point, and particular alters may be the embodiment of different aspects of the crisis. There are
six important aspects relating to: (1) death or psychological annihilation, (2) isolation, (3)
meaninglessness, (4) freedom and responsibility, (5) identity, and (6) intolerable pain.
Janoff-Bulman (1985, 1992) extensively discussed the destructive impact of trauma on
basic ontological assumptions (i.e., belief in a meaningful and benevolent world, personal
invulnerability, and positive self-worth). These assumptions are shattered by trauma, and, in
the case of early and prolonged child abuse, never develop. In both cases, the result is a
propensity to existential crises. The moment of the crisis is unbearable, and becomes fixed in
10
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
the traumatic memory without a temporal or meaningful context. It is accompanied by
disintegrative anxiety and terror that is diffuse, intense, and non-verbal, i.e., the classic state of
hyper-arousal in response to overwhelming trauma (Van der Kolk, 1987a, 1988, 1994;
Krystal,1988).
For each trauma there may be one or a series of existential crises. Since these moments
are so laden with affect, it is crucial to recognize them in the synthesis session, so they may be
processed during realization.
Parallel and Serial Synthesis
Two fundamental approaches to the synthesis of traumatic memories in MPD patients
can be distinguished: serial and parallel synthesis. The first often involves a number of
sessions, each returning to the original sequence of events. The second is concentrated in one
session with the therapist and patient striving towards maximum involvement of all alters
concerned.
Serial Synthesis
The rule in classic serial synthesis of traumatic memories is to follow the patient, and
to stay where the patient is at (Putnam, 1989,1992). Clinicians pursuing this approach risk not
knowing which alters are involved, and which should be excluded from participating at any
given point in the actual synthesis work.
The essence of serial synthesis is to bring the patient, after the necessary preparations,
back before the traumatic event. Then, while constantly remaining oriented to the therapist
and to the present, the patient is chronologically guided through the experience (i.e., by
asking, "And after that, what happened next?") Using the BASK Model (Braun, 1988a&b), for
example, the therapist attempts to elicit various dissociated traumatic responses, and to include
missing aspects requiring synthesis.
Sometimes the whole process must be repeated a number of times, so that all alters
involved in the original traumatic event can make their respective contribution. Should
synthesis not be completed, reactivation of the same traumatic memory can occur following
the session. Putnam spoke of this in regard to the resulting contradictions and the problems of
credibility which ensue:
The patient may relate very different versions of the same event and express widely
divergent emotions. In MPD patients, different alters will often embody these contradictory
perceptions. In some cases, specific events or specific versions of an event will have clear
fantasy components; in many other cases, the therapist and patient will never be sure what
actually occurred, and what was fantasy, in these cases. I do not know of any way to sort out
truth from fantasy in these cases. (Putnam, 1989, p.245)
First, it is important to be aware that different alters may indeed have experienced
widely different aspects of the trauma. An alter identifying with the abusive perpetrator might
relate a quite different version from an alter experiencing the pain and fear during abuse;
another alter who is completely dissociated from all pain and fear might even experience
sexual arousal.
Secondly, it has frequently been observed that some alters may have fantasy
experiences during a traumatic event, which should also be synthesized. Charcot (1887)
described a classic case of adult traumatization in which a fantasy played a major part in the
post-traumatic symptomatology. His patient Lelog was in a traffic accident with a horse-
drawn wagon, after which his legs were paralyzed. Although he fell against the ground, and
was unconscious, there were no neurological signs indicating a somatic cause of the paralysis.
Instead it was discovered that as he fell. Just before losing consciousness, he saw the wheels
of the car approaching him, and strongly believed that he would be overrun. This fantasy was
dissociated and gave rise to his paralysis.
Hypnotic Techniques
To ensure the safe and effective course of synthesis, the therapist may draw upon
specific hypnotic techniques. Some of these, such as creating a safe imaginary place for alters
11
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
not yet ready to participate in the actual synthesis, usually have been implemented during
earlier therapy phases or during the preparation stage. Other hypnotic techniques may be used
to ensure gradual synthesis over a number of sessions. One such technique involves the use of
an imaginary video screen on which elements of the trauma can be displayed. The patient may
then relate associated thoughts, emotions and sensations. Another approach involves placing
the incomplete traumatic memory in "safe keeping" (e.g., in an imaginary vault or similar
location). More extensive descriptions of these and other relevant hypnotic techniques can be
found in Brown and Fromm (1986), Hammond (1990), Kluft (1988, 1989), Peterson (1991;
1993), Putnam (1989), Ross (1989), Spiegel (1981), Sachs and Peterson (1994), Steele and
Colrain (1990), and Van der Hart, Boon, and Van Everdingen (1990).
Parallel Synthesis
Parallel synthesis is a much more structured approach. The development of the idea of
parallel synthesis, as well as the several techniques involved in its execution, like many other
ideas and techniques in the field of MPD, may be attributed to several different sources. The
authors have attempted to attribute credit as accurately as possible, while acknowledging that
on occasion it is difficult, if not impossible, to determine a primary source in a field with such
a strong orientation to workshops and oral tradition. Kluft, Comstock and Sachs, for example,
taught variations of parallel synthesis in workshops and conference presentations during the
1980's, and Judith Peterson did the same during the early 1990's (Peterson, 1991). The
variation described below was traced to Stephen Ray, as personally related to the authors by
Barbara Friedman (personal communication, September 1990) and Pamela Reagor (personal
communication, November 1990). In the literature, Kluft (1988,1989), Fine (1991,1993), and
Sachs and Peterson (1994) have made significant contributions to this concept and associated
techniques.
Parallel synthesis is based on a detailed simultaneous inventory of the traumatic event.
Making this inventory involves seeking answers to questions such as what actually happened,
which alters were involved in the trauma, and in which ways, e.g., on the dimensions of the
BASK Model. It also involves determining with the patient, or, preferably, with an alter who
has an overview, which alters should attend the synthesis, and which alters should temporarily
be protected from such knowledge and participation.
The actual synthesis starts by repeating the rationale and the actual steps involved. The
therapist emphasizes that the success of the technique depends on the completeness with
which it is executed. At the beginning of the synthesis session, the patient is guided into a
state of intense concentration (i.e., hypnosis). Those alters who are required to synthesize their
respective parts of the traumatic memory are brought forward, and others permitted to attend
are also invited. By contrast, those who should withdraw are instructed to go their safe places
behind the pre-established protective barrier.
A written account of the traumatic event has been divided into 10 segments. The
therapist counts from 1 to 10, for example, beginning at "1 "with the onset of the trauma, and
ending with "10" after the traumatic experience is over. Each segment that the therapist
subsequently mentions is preceded by its appropriate number. This counting gives a clear
structure to the session, reminding the patient of the stage reached in the process, and how
much more is still to come. During the synthesis, all alters involved are encouraged to share
their respective experiences, so that they coalesce into a single whole, and relieve existing
dissociations.
After the last count, the therapist offers additional appropriate support to all alters
involved, and asks what percent age of the total traumatic event has been brought together.
Often a significant percentage, particularly pertaining to the worst pain and fear, is left behind,
and must be more explicitly included in the next round. Within a given session, the whole
procedure is executed no more than three times. This is often more than adequate.
It is useful to intersperse this "titrated" (re) experiencing with episodes of hypnotic
"resting," in which the patient is induced into a very deeply relaxed state. This employs
hypnotic time distortion, with the suggestion that the patient may rest as long as is necessary
before the next synthesis module during the session. Suggestions include: "seconds seem like
minutes, and minutes like hours," "in a short time you may feel relaxed, refreshed and
12
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
renewed as though you had rested for a long time."
Where there is little outstanding dissociated material, say up to five percent, this may in
some cases be processed safely by the patient on his or her own. However, reassurances by
patients that they can do this last bit alone, must be tempered by the therapist's clinical
judgment. In one female patient the last five percent "at the end" was responsible for the
activation of serious suicidal tendencies. It pertained to an earlier suicide attempt made in
response to a rape by her father at the age of nine. She had subsequently believed she was
pregnant, and failed in an attempt to induce an abortion. Thus it is important to be sure that the
remaining dissociated elements do not contain material that will be overwhelming, and that
they do not link to further traumas that are not yet ready to be synthesized.
Parallel synthesis may involve many variations. Fine emphasized the importance of
first working with "like-clusters" of personalities so that affect and knowledge is shared
among groups of alters grouped according to affective or cognitive themes. In this way, the
affect is processed and modulated prior to being shared with other alters within other clusters
(Fine, 1991,1993). Fine advocated fractionated synthesis in order to avoid re-traumatization to
the patient. Comstock (1986) suggested various approaches to managing the treatment of
traumatic memories through modulating affect and content. As presented already in his
workshops during the early 1980's, Kluft (1988,1989) developed specific approaches to
fractionate traumatic material, in which it is suggested to the patient and alters that they need
not re-experience their part in the trauma to the full extent, but for instance to a degree of 4 on
a scale of 1 to 5. They are further instructed that they "need only experience that which is
necessary to know, to understand, and to heal." Another variation is the instruction that, if
necessary, one specific alter or one dimension of the traumatic experience, may be left out
during the first attempt and may be included later. This transforms parallel synthesis into a
highly structured serial synthesis. It is also the case when subsequent sessions are
systematically focused on the synthesis of successive episodes of a complex traumatic event.
While the parallel approach initially involves active participation by the therapist,
utilization of detailed information regarding the traumata, and sharing the experiences of the
respective alters involved, it also lends itself to relatively autonomous work by the patient.
Some patients become so accustomed to its application, that they can do the necessary work
with certain traumatic memories without the therapist being informed about all of the relevant
details. At first counting is often experienced as very helpful, but after a while some patients
indicate that they do not need it anymore. This implies that they are not utilizing dissociation
as much as before, and that they are now better able to tolerate and modulate affects related to
the traumatic memory.
An important issue is the degree of involvement of the host personality (where one
exists) in the synthesis of the traumatic memory. When this has been present during the
trauma, the answer is of course in the affirmative. In other cases it depends on the host's ego
strength — or "psychological tension" in Janet's terms (cf. Van der Hart& Friedman, 1989) —
and motivation, among other factors. In general, it is important to tailor synthesis to the
patient's needs.
Termination of the Synthesis Session
Towards the end of the synthesis session, while the patient is still in an altered state of
consciousness, the therapist ensures that all alters involved are supported, and suggests a
positive sense of accomplishment and unity. After having shared their terrible ordeals, they
can now share their warmth and wellbeing, not only with each other, but also with all other
alters as well. The therapist may use additional healing imagery, such as projecting the image
of a white, healing light, which embraces them, and brings them all the more closely together
(Kluft, personal communication January 1987). The authors believe that such experiences
provide a powerful model for future integration and unification. Finally, the patient is brought
back to a state of full awareness and orientation to the present time and place (Putnam, 1992).
Aftercare
Good preparation for a synthesis session requires knowing how (and, in some cases,
with whom) the patient will return home after the session, and what type of support will be
available, if needed (Putnam, 1992). It is sometimes useful to have the patient contact the
13
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
therapist at a pre-deter-mined time at the end of the day, or at the beginning of the next day,
and to make available a follow-up session at short notice.
Stage 3: Realization and Integration
With successful synthesis the traumatic memory ceases operating at a sensorimotor
level. McDougall (1920/1) formulated this as follows:
As soon as the dissociation is overcome, though the same train of thoughts may
recur, its power to produce distress is greatly weakened by the patient's
accompanying awareness of his present surroundings and his knowledge that the
experience belongs to the past. The process of readjustment of his emotional
attitude can then begin, or, in other words, he makes progress in 'autognosis'
(1920/1, p. 27; 1926,p.457).
What McDougall called the "process of readjustment" requires both acceptance of the
memory as real, and a conciliation between this narrative memory and the patient's
personality. The traumatic memory has now been transformed from an intrusive re-
experiencing, to a trauma-related narrative within the overall stream of consciousness. At this
point, therapy shifts to mitigating the impact of the trauma on the self and on relationships
with others. Herman stated that the core experiences of this stage are "empowerment and
reconnection" (1992, p. 197).
Janet (1935, 1945) called this conciliation "realization" of the event and its
consequences. Realization is the formulation of a belief about what happened (the trauma),
when it happened (in the past), and to whom it happened (to self). The trauma becomes
personalized, relegated to the past, and takes on symbolic rather than sensorimotor properties.
In order to resolve existential crises, realization requires the development of new cognitive
schema, such as accepting the event and resigning oneself to its occurrence, reframing one's
attitudes to the event, the perpetrator, and oneself, and reorganizing one's overall behavior
(Janet, 1945). Yet, it is more than cognitive process; realization is an experience of one's
history as one's own. It provides a transition from the non-verbal dissociated realm of
traumatic intrusion into secondary mental process in which words can construct meaning and
form, thereby facilitating the transformation of traumatic memory into narrative memory.
Integration or assimilation involves multi-dimensional change in the life of the survivor.
Realization then becomes lived experience. As one survivor said, "I don't have to live my old
story anymore now that I know it; I'm creating a new one every day. I'm no longer stuck with
one script!"
Realization evolves over time as more of the events and their meanings are digested
and resonate throughout the mind and as the various dissociated layers are uncovered and
integrated. The trauma thus becomes gradually assimilated or integrated into the patient's
personality; it is incorporated into the patient's narrative memory or autobiography (Janet,
1919/25). The patient should not only be able to narrate the story of the abuse without re-
experiencing it, but:
must know how to associate the happening with the other events of his life, how
to give it its place in that life-history which each one of us is perpetually
building up and which for each of us is an essential element of our personality.
A situation has not been satisfactorily liquidated, has not been fully assimilated,
until we have achieved, not merely an outward reaction through our movements,
but also an inward reaction through the words we address to ourselves, through
the organization of the recital of the event to others and to ourselves, and
through the putting of this recital in its place as one of the chapters in our
personal history. (Janet, 1919/25, p.662)
Clinical experience teaches us that this realization can be a very difficult — if not
initially impossible —challenge for some patients. This is sometimes due to the fact that
surviving dissociated elements of the traumatic memory have been reactivated. But even when
synthesis of the traumatic memory has been completely successful, patients may initially feel
unable to reconcile the narrative memory with their self-representation. The realization for
14
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
example that their mother always hated them, that their father sadistically abused them, that
they themselves came to perform horrible acts, is overwhelming and defies categorization or
attribution of meaning.
Integration and realization involve confronting enormous loss. Grief and mourning are
a crucial transition between realization and integration. The patient must learn to grieve deeply
the lost childhood that can never be recovered; the loneliness and the pain that have been and
must continue to be endured; the lost time, money, education, jobs, relationships, and energy
spent avoiding or struggling with the aftermath of the trauma; and finally, the awful truth that
must be carried throughout the rest of his or her life. Yet grief, with all its pain and anguish, is
ultimately healing. It enables the survivor to relinquish unrealistic expectations, to
acknowledge what "is," and therefore, to move fully into the present with new clarity and
purpose — with sadness, but also with newfound wisdom.
The first task of this stage is for the therapist to listen empathically to the narration of
the story, repeatedly, if need be. Telling the story is an important component of realization and
integration; the narrative makes the story more real and personal, as well as shifting the
trauma from an autistic re-experiencing, to a relational sharing and linking in with the
therapeutic alliance. The survivor is no longer alone in the trauma. Words begin to allow the
patient to form new semantic structures that will assist him or her to assimilate the trauma
over time. Ager and Jensen (1990) stated that the telling becomes a "testimony," a ritual that is
healing and restorative, and gives the story not only a personal, but an interpersonal and social
dimension, so that it can be put in its broader context.
Cognitive work should also continue during this stage (Ross, 1989). The patient
develops more insight into the origins of cognitive distortions, while the affective and
behavioral realms follow suit. The capacity for self-care becomes established, and with it, a
new-found confidence and self-esteem. Personal psychological needs are acknowledged, and
the survivor begins to take an active role in creating an environment in which trust, healthy
dependence, autonomy, power, esteem, and intimacy can flourish (McCann & Pearlman,
1990). In short, the patient learns (or relearns) to love, to work, and to play.
Substitution
Sometimes realization of a traumatic event requires making the original trauma more
acceptable (Janet, 1935). This can be done in two principal ways: either by changing the
content of the traumatic memory itself, or by altering aspects of the already transformed
narrative recall.
The first approach is called the substitution technique, and was originally developed by
Janet (1889, 1898; cf. Van der Hart, Brown, & Van der Kolk, 1989; Van der Hart, Brown, &
Turco, 1990). In it he suggested a completely different course to the original traumatic event.
Thus, with his patient Marie, for whom the unanticipated first menstruation had been
traumatic, he used hypnosis to regress her before it and gave her appropriate preparatory
instruction. He then suggested that she would now "remember" her first menstruation as
having occurred in a normal and painless way.
A number of therapist have recently reintroduced this technique (Lamb, 1985; Miller,
1986). With regard to traumatic events involving perpetrators (e.g., childhood sexual assault),
clinicians are mostly reluctant to employ substitution, since it might be perceived by the
patient as an invalidation of the crime, something which has been done so often by the
perpetrators and by others who knew about the abuse. However, minor variations can be
employed with survivors of child abuse. After synthesis, for example, the patient is
encouraged to express anger, and to do symbolically what he or she had wanted to do against
the perpetrator (s) during or after the original traumatic event. Watkins (1949), who applied
this technique in the treatment of traumatized combat veterans, stated that it may help the
patient to bring the original frustrating situation to a more satisfactory conclusion.
However, for true realization to occur, substitution must occur in parallel with retention
of the actual traumatic memory. What actually happened and what one wished to happen must
exist simultaneously, so that substitution does not lead to denial.
The second approach consists of changing narrative and semantics aspects of the
15
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
traumatic event. These often involve reframing the patient's own interpretations (e.g., when
the patient blames herself for the abuse she suffered as a child from her father), exploring
different meanings, and correcting cognitive distortions, all of which have been discussed
above.
Whichever the approach, the goal is always to "liquidate the happening" (Janet,
1919/25, p. 681), i.e., to effect closure. In an era when the veracity of memories of abuse is
being questioned, and the possibility has been raised of "memories" being suggested by the
therapist, it is prudent to consider carefully in advance the implications of memory
substitution. This technique offers the potential for resolution and relief. In the highly
hypnotizable and suggestible patient, it can also lead to confusion, and should thus be used
with caution.
Resolution of Existential Crises
Resolution of existential crises identified in the synthesis stage, particularly those
associated with a sense of meaninglessness, is of prime importance in the process of
realization and integration. Chronically re-enacted existential crises often dominate the present
reality of the survivor. These crises are resolved once they, and the associated memories are
integrated into the personality structure. The patient then accepts that the moment of crisis has
past, and chronic responses can be transformed into more adaptive cognitions, affects, and
behaviors. First, the patient must accept that she or he has, indeed, survived. The crisis of
death has been continually re-experienced in the present as suicidality, self-destructive
tendencies, internal alters who kill or threaten to kill other alters, dead or dying alters, and a
sense of foreshortened future. One patient described the synthesis and realization of the
existential crisis of death as follows:
I couldn't remember for a very long time; I couldn't bear to remember that I was
so close to death so many times. When I finally got to the point of being able to
remember, something became crystal clear: I wasn't dead and I wasn't dying
anymore. I realized the reason why I was always so suicidal was because
somehow I had to be close to death all the time. Somehow I got stuck in the
very moment I thought I was going to die. Terror is a very sticky thing, you
know, it sticks you like glue right to the thing that happens, right to the part you
are most afraid of. Once I got unstuck, I didn't have to be dying anymore. Now I
can focus on living, and I have a lot of catching up to do.
Psychological annihilation is experienced and associated with the onslaught of
overwhelming and primitive states of hyper-arousal which have been termed "affective
storms." In those who do not have the capacity to soothe themselves or to modulate or tolerate
affect, the latter can occur with frightening regularity. Severe trauma is often accompanied by
a terrifying loss of all self boundaries: "My body seemed like it was flying apart, and then my
insides shattered into a thousand million pieces, and there was no more 'me' anymore."
Although dissociation during trauma is usually considered a natural defense, the process itself
is quite often experienced as intensely dysphoric and terrifying: "I melted into the wall, and
although it hurt and I knew this was the end, I spread myself so thin I was invisible." "I shrank
myself until it was like a black hole in space, and it sucked me in there and I was the black
hole." "I became no-one and went to nowhere into a black nothing."
Once the process of dissociation is more under the survivor's central control, these
dysphoric and annihilatory experiences gradually cease. The work on the central self, begun
early in treatment, and continued throughout, should by now have contributed significantly to
the patient's capacity to tolerate affects without being overwhelmed by them. There is a
growing realization that feelings need not be life-threatening events. Through synthesis,
dissociative re-enactment of annihilation in the trauma ceases. The survivor comes to
understand that there are experiences that none of us can tolerate. Having tolerated these in the
past by dissociating, nonetheless, she or he gradually learns how to achieve distance from
them in the present without splitting or disintegration of the personality.
The experience of intolerable physical and emotional pain is the basis of further
existential crises which impede healing. Repetitive intrusion of overwhelming affect and
sensation related to the abuse convinces the patient that she or he cannot tolerate living. The
16
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
fear is not one of dying, but rather of living with such intolerable pain. This process underlies
much of the suicidal ideation of survivors, since death can be seen as leading to relief from
pain.
Severe pain is thus a devastating and fragmenting experience (Scarry, 1985). It
destroys the self, eradicates the internal locus of control, and dismantles one's world view.
Emotional self-care functions do not develop adequately in many childhood abuse victims. As
they are unable to tolerate or regulate affect or the hyperaroused state generated by pain, they
therefore continue to be overwhelmed by it. Tolerance for pain varies, and survivors with low
thresholds readily enter states of overwhelming hyper-arousal. Once dissociated affects and
sensations are integrated, significant relief is obtained, and alters who contained the pain begin
to heal.
Grief is an important part of the emotional pain which must be worked through. With
the passage of time, episodes of grief gradually decrease in intensity and duration. Survivors
come to understand and accept that loss is an inevitable part of trauma, and that it is ultimately
a lifelong task to assimilate the ebb and flow of re-experienced grief with equanimity.
Assigning Meaning and Processing Information
Once the survivor has accepted that she or he will neither die nor be annihilated, and
has successfully resolved the pain, the difficult task of reconstructing meaning continues. A
sense of meaninglessness accompanies the obsessive "approach-avoidance" process involved
in attempting to realize what has happened: "This is real" versus "I made it up"; "This is real"
versus "It didn't happen to me. "As realization proceeds, the survivor is helped to make sense
out of what has been senseless. Herman (1992) wrote eloquently about the struggle to assign
meaning to the trauma:
[The survivor] stands mute before the emptiness of evil, feeling the
insufficiency of any known systems of explanation. Survivors of atrocity of
every age and every culture come to a point in their testimony where all
questions are reduced to one, spoken more in bewilderment than in outrage:
Why? The answer is beyond human understanding. (p.178)
Assigning meaning is a basic need underpinning the expression of all cognitions and
affects. Intrapsychic meaning provides the frame for organizing self concepts, and ultimately
all cognitive schema (Frankl, 1963; Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Ulman & Brothers, 1988; Yalom,
1980). Trauma not only shatters existing meaning, but also impedes provision of new meaning
to the trauma itself. The extent to which cognitive transformation is required depends upon the
severity and chronicity of the trauma. Single trauma mainly requires a focus on integration of
the traumatic memories, while prolonged and repeated trauma may require transformation of
the entire existing framework of thought. Thus, for those individuals who experience chronic
trauma in childhood, early meaning structures and cognitive schema are faulty, and
completely new ones must be constructed. This is an arduous and time-consuming process,
but the trauma cannot be integrated without such reconstruction.
The processing of traumatic memories has been described in terms of relief of
dissociation, realization, synthesis and integration. In Janet's words, it is a process aimed at "a
conciliation between two terms, between on the one hand the narrative of the trauma, and on
the other the personality of the subject" (Janet, 1935, p. 357). Integration of traumatic
memories into existing frameworks of thought, as for example in single trauma, was called
"transformation of the first term" by Janet, and "assimilation" by Piaget and his followers
(Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Kleber & Brom, 1992. The much more radical reconstruction of
existing frameworks of thought required in conditions characterized by chronic trauma, such
as in MPD, was described as "transformation of the second term" by Janet, and
"accommodation" by Piaget.
Such frameworks of thought were first called "cognitive schema" by Sir Henry Head in
1920. The concept was elaborated by Bartlett (1932), and taken up by those researching
traumatic neuroses in the 1970's (e.g., Horowitz, 1976/1986). Mandler (1979) stated that "a
schema is formed on the basis of past experience with objects, scenes, or events and consists
of a set of (usually unconscious) expectations about what things look like and/or the order in
17
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
which they occur" (p. 263). Consistent with Janet's two forms of conciliation, Hollon and
Garber (1988) stated that schema-discrepant information such as trauma may be assimilated
into existing schema ("That never happened," "I caused it to happen") or schema may be
altered to accommodate the new information ("There is no safe place in the world." "This
happened because people chose to hurt me; it wasn't my fault.").
Schema formed following trauma tend to be narrow and fixed, and tend to "determine
to what extent new information is absorbed and processed" (Van der Kolk & Van der Hart,
1991, p. 440). Frequently the survivor misinterprets the meaning of current experiences as
being dangerous and hostile (the meaning given to the interaction in the trauma) . Survivors
often speak of the helplessness and hopelessness they feel as they perceive a "random and
chaotic" world, where nothing is predictable or safe. In addition, many struggle with issues of
spirituality, rejecting their faith, and questioning "Why would God allow this to happen?" The
concept of God inevitably acquires a negative connotation as well as a negative transference:
that of the sadistic abuser, or of the passive non-offending parent who knew but did not
protect.
Thus, without ontological, psychological, or spiritual meaning structures that offer
safety, hope, control, trust, and esteem, the survivor lives in a seemingly random, chaotic, and
cruel inner and outer world. This continuing traumatic experience sets up vicious cycles that
perpetuate the dissociative and related defenses, and which ultimately prevent the individual
from resolving the trauma and from moving toward health. During the realization/integration
stage of treatment, new meaning structures are therefore developed, and an internal locus of
control is gradually restored. The world then gradually becomes a less hostile and formidable
place.
Once new meaning, cognitions and expectations have been established, the possibility
of improving relationships with others becomes more plausible; in the words of Lifton (1988),
"The survivor seeks vitality both in immediate relationships and ultimate meaning, the one
impossible without the other" (p. 26).
Survivors with severe PTSD and dissociative disorders depend on dissociation and
denial to maintain the pre-traumatic identity and intact related basic assumptions: "That never
happened" (temporal dissociation, or amnesia), or "It happened to that other little girl"
(parallel dissociation, and the development of alters). The victim identity, denied as it is of
selfhood, is intolerable, and the survivor goes to great lengths to develop any other identity:
"Nothing really happened; I just made it up because I am crazy... because I am bad... because I
am evil." Byway of contrast, some survivors tenaciously cling to the victim identity, since the
trauma overshadows or even destroys any other: they simply become what happened to them.
Survivors are also unable to develop a post-trauma identity because of fragmentation
and lack of realization. They have a peculiar and frightening sense that they are not 'whole " or
"real" (cf. Putnam, 1990). Those with MPD have many identities conflicting with one another.
The vicissitudes of multiple identities in one body have been amply documented in both the
professional and the popular literature. Survivors express the terror of experiencing a complete
lack of identity or sense of self: "I don't know who I am until somebody asks me to do
something"; "Maybe, after all these parts integrate I won't be anybody; maybe there isn't a
'real me' in here; maybe I'm just empty and nothing." For the dissociative patient, realization
of the self must coincide with realization of the trauma.
As the intrapsychic structure of survivors is stabilized and reinforced throughout
treatment, they will begin to feel more confident of selfhood. Many express feelings that they
deserve to exist or even to live for the first time. A core sense of shame dissolves, as the
therapist provides a positive identification for the patient in the context of the therapeutic
alliance. Survivors are urged to conclude that they have a right to a selfhood, and can begin
building on that foundation.
Realization as a Social Act
The existential crisis of isolation is experienced at the moment when the patient
realizes that she or he was completely alone in experiencing the trauma, and that there were
often no consistent supportive figures in their childhood. This isolation is re-enacted in the
18
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
immense struggles to find and maintain connection with the self and with others. It is
frequently associated with failures and narcissistic wounding; the constant sense of
abandonment and terrifying aloneness; the utter certainty and shame associated with the
feeling that one is ultimately unworthy to be loved (e.g., "If you really knew me, you would be
disgusted"), and finally, with the keeping of the secrets, not only from others, but from oneself
(Schultz, 1990). Chronic dissociation and depersonalization leave the individual unable to feel
sufficiently connected in the present. Disrupted internal object relations preclude mutually
satisfying and interdependent external relationships.
Realization is not only an intrapsychic process, it is also a social act. Janet (1935)
described realization as a way of unifying the total life experience, of linking the past with the
present and future. With regard to traumatic memories, this requires an ability to tell the story,
not only to one's self, but also to others. Furthermore, testimony must not only be given, it
must also be heard.
This leads to consideration of social processes that actually promote non-realization
and continued isolation of the traumatized individual: collective denial. Realization is usually
associated with social expectations. Thus, in the case of loss of a family member, those around
us, as well as society at large, expect us to realize this loss and to take it into account in our
social actions and in our personal belief systems (Janet, 1937). We are even provided with
personal, social, and religious rituals to assist in realization and integration. However, in the
case of childhood abuse, society, including the mental health field, has been extremely
reluctant to acknowledge (realize) either the occurrence or the impact of abuse (Goodwin,
1985).
There is no social expectation or demand for adults to realize their highly traumatized
childhoods. In fact, there is often enormous pressure from perpetrators and families to
continue the pattern of dissociation and denial that characterizes collective non-realization (cf.
Herman, 1992). The mental health community merely expects survivors to become
asymptomatic, and if any acknowledgement is given at all, it is in the form of: "That's all in
the past; forget about it and get over it." Goodwin (1985) formulated this social demand for
non-realization of abuse as follows:
We observe, in interactions with patients with MPD and abused children and
their families, a shared negative hallucination. (...) The MPD patient and the
physician cling to the series of false symptoms and false diagnoses in proportion
to their mutual need to blot out the reality of multiplicity, and to blot out the
unbearable experiences of real pain that triggered it (p. 14).
Realization as a social act may thus be hindered, less by the individual's reluctance to
acknowledge what happened, than by society's inability to accept the truth about abuse. The
survivor remains isolated in a dissociative "no man's land" between truth and denial.
Hopefully, as society becomes increasingly aware of abuse and its dramatic negative personal
and social consequences, this will change. Until that time, the therapeutic relationship will be
the primary medium through which the survivor can explore the terrifying abyss of isolation
and engage in the social act of realization. Perhaps for the first time, the survivor can re-
experience the trauma in the presence of an empathic listener, break the isolation and emerge
from the crisis, no longer alone or forgotten.
The secure boundaries of the therapeutic relationship, and the honesty, clarity and
consistency of the therapist allow the patient accept the impossibility of the hoped for
"perfect" relationship, while sensing that the therapist understands the anguish of this
perceived loss. The survivor learns to negotiate a balance between closeness and distance, to
constructively utilize time spent alone, to remain in touch with others even when alone, and to
understand the impact of the boundary violations experienced in the trauma. The therapist
aims to enhance the survivor's ability to discern and maintain awareness of interpersonal and
intrapsychic boundaries, and the capacity to view self and others as integrated, the "good"
with the "bad."
While the therapeutic relationship is a primary healing force, the therapist must also
assist the patient to develop additional support systems. Partners and other supportive family
members may be engaged in family therapy with the survivor (Sachs, Frischholz, & Wood,
19
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
1988). This area is often much neglected in the treatment of survivors. Intensive individual
therapy can often disrupt the delicate balance of domestic relationships, and this must be
attended to, if the survivor is not to sustain further loss. In many cases, this can be prevented
by early intervention. Partner support groups are a growing phenomenon as therapists become
more aware of the survivor's current family needs.
The survivor must also find ways of rejoining the community at large. These may
include a survivors support or therapy group. Survivors often begin by expressing their hopes
and dreams for the future. These may include studies, a job change, or volunteer work. The
therapist should encourage even the smallest desire of the survivor to be a part of the "world."
Adult survivors frequently perceive themselves as having no choice, no control over, or
capacity to direct the course of their lives. They passively await the next disaster, because they
are convinced they can do nothing about it. For some survivors the very process of making a
choice can lead to hyperarousal, and they become anxious and unable to decide (Van der Kolk
& Greenberg, 1987).
The roots of this loss of control are in childhood (Summit, 1983). Children are, by
definition, powerless, dependent, and vulnerable. By virtue of developmental limitations, they
are unable to take adequate responsibility for themselves or others. Yet, the abused child is
often "parentified," becoming the caretaker and/or the surrogate "spouse" of a parent. The
child develops an egocentric set of beliefs that she or he is responsible for the abuse. At the
same time, the abused child exists in an environment of inconsistency, arbitrary rules, and
unpredictable violence. The child lives in the throes of continual double binds in which
responses may be punished at one time, and rewarded at another (Bateson, 1972; Spiegel,
1986; Fine, 1990, 1991, 1993). She or he is unable to control or stop the abuse, and usually,
other adults also make no attempt to stop it. The child learns that any action results in the
same outcome as no action: nothing is effective. This is the genesis of learned helplessness
(Seligman, 1975; Garber & Seligman, 1980).
In one case, a woman had not only sustained childhood abuse and severe neglect, but
had then married a vicious sadist. Double binds of forced choice were so great that she
became catatonic at the thought of having to make even the smallest decisions. In tracing the
roots of this extreme response, it was discovered that her husband had repeatedly forced her to
choose which of their children he would torture, or which method of torture she would
"prefer." He persistently told her that he would kill her if she tried to leave, as well as telling
her it was her fault that he "had" to hurt her and the children. This patient was so fragmented
and her ability to make choices so severely incapacitated that she could not make the simplest
decisions, and found elaborate ways to have others make choices for her. This made her
extremely vulnerable to exploitation, and to further revictimization.
In the course of long and arduous treatment, the patient one day triumphantly reported
that she had been able to shop for groceries without assistance, the first "choice" she had been
able to make in well over a decade. Although this is an extreme example, it highlights the
survivor's difficulties in learning to be responsible and to successfully make choices.
In addition to an impaired capacity to choose and act, the survivor feels a core sense of
guilt, shame, and responsibility for the abuse. In the case above, the patient emphatically
believed that she had been responsible for both what had happened to her as a child, and for
hurting her children. All three children had MPD, and both she and the children had well
documented medical evidence of the torture. This led to the belief that she must be punished
for her "sins," and that she had no right to recover or to have a good life so long as her
children continue to suffer. It was only after a number of years of therapy that she could even
begin to acknowledge and realize her previous helplessness and lack of choice, and could
move gradually toward empowerment in the present.
Conclusions
This paper, and related literature on trauma, presents the treatment of traumatic
memories as a separate step in the phase-oriented therapy of MPD. In actual practice, it is
important to realize that this specific phase often alternates several times with other treatment
20
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
phases. Traumatic memories frequently present in a layered fashion. A number of fusions of
alter personalities — usually seen at a later phase in therapy — may have occurred before all
traumatic memories have been processed adequately.
In this regard, the realization and integration of past traumata should also be seen as an
ongoing mental process. Traumatic events, together with other significant aspects of the
patient's past, are shared with an increasing number of alter personalities, whose fusions
demand further reorganization of the patient's "autobiography" and personality. Only some
time after the last fusion may this reorganization be considered completed, albeit for the time
being. The more patients succeed in linking their past to the present and future, the more they
are able to live fully independent lives. Yet, for all of us, integration continues as a lifelong
process.
The psychology of realization and integration of chronic trauma still remains in its
infancy. Much more comprehensive work has been done regarding the processing of single
traumatic events or series of events that occurred in adulthood. Much of this work is not
directly applicable to MPD patients, since their defenses have had years to solidify and
become thoroughly incorporated into the personality structure. This is particularly true of the
dissociative defense, which often becomes the preferred method of coping with even the
smallest of inconveniences. Many MPD patients were never able to develop what are
considered to be basic adaptive assumptions about themselves and others. Their cognitive
schema and meaning structures are distorted and entrenched, and there are often no pre-
traumatic structures on which to begin to build a new foundation.
This lends a complexity to the therapeutic work that is inadequately addressed in the
literature on adult trauma. The realization and integration of a lifetime, or at the very least, of
an entire childhood of trauma is a monumental task, requiring all the skills of the therapist, the
energy and motivation of the patient, and the strongest therapeutic rapport. The work is slow,
gruelling, and, at times, tediously repetitious, again and again contrasting the old worldview
with the new.
There is still much to learn about this process, and much in this paper will need
investigation and elaboration. This has been a pioneering effort to clarify current conceptions
about traumatic memories, and to integrate the work of Pierre Janet with contemporary
trauma-based models of therapy. It provides a sound conceptual framework for the resolution
of traumatic memories in adult survivors of chronic childhood abuse.
References
Ager, I., &Jensen, S.B. (1990). Testimony as ritual and evidence in psychotherapy for political refugees.
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 3, 115-130.
Bartlett, F.C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York: Ballantine Books.
Beck, A.T. (1979). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities
Press.
Blank, A.S. (1985). The unconscious flashback to the war in Viet Nam veterans: Clinical mystery, legal
defense, and community problem. In S.M. Sonnenberg & J.A. Talbot (Eds.), The trauma of war:
Stress and recovery in Viet Nam veterans (pp. 293-308). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Press.
Bliss, E. (1986). Multiple personality, allied disorders and hypnosis. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Boon, S., & Van der Hart, 0. (1991). De behandeling van de multiple persoonlijkheidsstoornis
[Treatment of multiple personality disorder]. In 0. van der Hart (Ed.), Trauma, dissociatie en
hypnose [Trauma, dissociation and hypnosis] (pp. 159-187). Amsterdam/Lisse: Swets &
Zeitlinger.
Braun, B.G. (1986). Issues in the psychotherapy of multiple personality disorder. In B.G. Braun (Ed.),
Treatment of multiple personality disorder , (pp. 3-28). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Press.
Braun, B.G. (1988a). The BASK model of dissociation: Part I. DISSOCIATION, 1(1), 4-23.
Braun, B.G. (1988b). The BASK model of dissociation: Part II: Clinical applications. DISSOCIATION,
1(2), 16-23.
21
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
Brende, J.O. (1984). An educational-therapeutic group for drugs and alcohol abusing combat veterans.
Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 14, 122-136.
Briquet, P. (1859). Traite clinique et therapeutique de l'hysterie (2 vols.). Paris: J.-B. Bailliere & Fils.
Brown, W. (1920/1921). The revival of emotional memories and its therapeutic value (1). British
Journal of Medical Psychology, 1, 16-19.
Brown, D.P., & Fromm, E. (1986). Hypnotherapy and hypnoanalysis. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Eribaum
Associates.
Charcot, J.M. (1887). Leçons sur les maladies du systéme nerveux faites a la Salpêtriére, Tome III.
Paris: Progres Medical and A. Delahaye & E. Lecrosnie.
Comstock, C. (1986). The therapeutic utilization of abreactive experiences in the treatment of multiple
personality disorder. In B.C. Braun (Ed.), Dissociative disorders 1986: Proceedings of the Third
Annual Conference on Multiple Personality and Dissociative States. Chicago: Rush.
Courtois, C. A. (1988). Healing the incest wound: Adult survivors in therapy. New York: W.W. Norton
& Co.
Courtois, C.A. (1991). Theory, sequencing, and strategy in treating adult survivors. New Directions for
Mental Health Services, 5-7 (Fall), 47-60.
Delboeuf, J.R.L. (1889). Le magnétisme animal: A propos d'une visite a l’école de Nancy. Paris: Félix
Alcan.
Fine, C.G. (1988). Thoughts on the cognitive perceptual substrates of multiple personality disorder.
DISSOCIATION, 1(4), 5-10.
Fine, C.G. (1990). The cognitive sequelae of incest. In R.P. Kluft (Ed.), Incest related syndromes of
adult psychopathology (pp. 161-182). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Fine, C.G. (1991). Treatment stabilization and crisis prevention: Pacing the therapy of the multiple
personality disorder patient. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14(3), 661-675.
Fine, C.G. (1993). A tactical integrationalist perspective on the treatment of multiple personality
disorder. In R.P. Kluft & C.G. Fine (Eds.), Clinical perspectives on multiple personality disorder
(pp. 135-153). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Fish-Murray, C.C., Koby, E.V., & Van der Kolk, B.A. (1987).Evolving ideas: The effect of abuse on
children's thought. In B.A. Van der Kolk (Ed.), Psychological trauma (pp.89-110). Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Frankl, V. (1963). Man's search for meaning. New York: Bantam Books.
Fromm, E. (1965). Hypnoanalysis: Theory and two case excerpts. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and
Practice, 2 127-133.
Garber,J., & Seligman, M. (Eds.) (1980). Human helplessness: Theory and application. New York:
Academic Press.
Gelinas, D. (1983). The persisting negative effects of incest. Psychiatry, 46, 312-332.
Goodwin, J. (1985). Credibility problems in multiple personality disorder patients and abused children.
In R. P. Kluft (Ed.), Childhood antecedents of multiple personality (pp. 2-19). Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Press.
Hammond, C.D. (1990) (Ed.). The handbook of hypnotic suggestion and metaphors. Washington, DC:
W. W. Norton & Co.
Herman, J. L. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books.
Hollon, S. D., & Garber, J. (1988). Cognitive therapy. In L.Y. Abramson (Ed.). Social cognition and
clinical psychology: A synthesis, pp. 204-253. New York: Guilford Press.
Horowitz, MJ. (1986). Stress response syndromes, 2nd ed., Northvale, NJ:J.Aronson. (Original
publication: 1976).
Horowitz, MJ. (1976). Stress response syndromes. New York: Jason Aronson.
Janet, P. (1889). L'Automatisme psychologique. Paris: Felix Alcan. Reprint: Societe Pierre Janet, Paris,
1973.
Janet, P. (1894). Histoire D'une idée fixe. Revue Philosophique, 37, 121-163.
Janet, P. (1898). Névroses et idées fixe. Paris: Félix Alcan.
Janet, P. (1903). Les obsessions et la psychasthénie (Vol. 1). Paris: Félix Alcan.
Janet, P. (1904). L'Amnésia et la dissociation des souvenirs par l'émotion. Journal de Psychologie, 1,
417-453. Also in P.Janet, (1911). L 'Etat mental des hysteriques, 2nd. ed. (pp. 506-544). Paris:
Felix Alcan. Reprint: Lafitte Reprints, Marseille, 1983.
Janet, P. (1907). The major symptoms of hysteria. London & New York: Macmillan.
Janet, P. (1910). Une Félida artificielle. Revue Philosophique, 69, 329-357; 483-529.
Janet, P. (1911). L'Etat mental des hystériques (Reprint: Lafitte Reprints, Marseilles, 1983 ed.). Paris:
Felix Alcan.
Janet, P. (1919/25). Les médications psychologiques. Paris: Félix Alcan. English edition Psychological
healing (2 vols ) New York Macmillan, 1925. Reprint Arno Press, New York, 1976.
Janet, P. (1928). L'Evolution de la mémoire et de la notion du tempts. Paris: A Chahine.
Janet, P. (1929). L'Evolution de la personnalité. (New Société Pierre Janet 1984 ed.). Paris: Chahine.
Janet, P. (1935). Réalisation et interprétation. Annales Médico-Psychologiques, 93, 329-366.
Janet, P. (1937). Les troubles de la personnalité sociale. Annales Médico-Psychologiques, 95, 421-468.
Janet, P. (1945). La coyance délirante. Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 4, 173-187.
Janoff-Bulman, R (1985): The aftermath of victimization: Rebuilding shattered assumptions. In C. R.
Figley (Ed ), Trauma and its wake, pp 15-35. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
22
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychology of trauma. New York:
Free Press.
Jung, C. G. (1921/1922.) The question of the therapeutic value of abreaction. British Journal of Medical
Psychology, 2, 13-22.
Kleber, R.J. , & Brom, D., with Defares, P. B. (1992). Coping with trauma: Theory, prevention and
treatment. Amsterdam: Lisse Swets & Zeitlinger.
Kluft, R.P. (1987). An update on multiple personality disorder. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 38,
(4), 363-373.
Kluft, R. P. (1988). On treating the older patient with multiple personality disorder: "Race against time "
or "Make haste slowly? " American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 30 (4), 257-266.
Kluft, R.P. (1989). Playing for time: Temporizing techniques in the treatment of Multiple Personality
Disorder. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 32, 90-98.
Kluft, R.P. (1991). Multiple personality disorder. In A Tasman & S Goldfinger (Eds ), American
Psychiatric Press Annual Review of Psychiatry, 10, 161-188.
Kluft, R. P. (1993). Basic principles in conducting the psychotherapy of multiple personality disorder. In
R. P. Kluft & C. G. Fine (Eds. ), Clinical perspectives on multiple personality disorder (pp 19
50). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Krystal, H. (1988). Integration and self-healing: Affect, alexithymia, and trauma. Hillsdale, NJ: The
Analytic Press.
Lamb, C. S. (1985). Hypnotically-induced deconditioning: Reconstruction of memories in the treatment
of phobias. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 28, 56-62.
Lifton, R.J. (1980.) The concept of the survivor. In J. E Dimsdale (Ed ), Survivors, victims, and
perpetrators: Essays on the Nazi holocaust (pp 113-126. New York: Hemisphere Publishers.
Lifton, R. J. (1988.) Understanding the traumatized self: Imagery, symbolization, and transformation. In
J.P. Wilson, Z. Harel & B. Kahana (Eds ), Human adaptation to extreme stress From the
Holocaust to Vietnam (pp 7-31). New York: Plenum Press.
Loewenstein, R.J. (1993). Posttraumatic and dissociative aspects of transference and countertransference
in the treatment of multiple personality disorder. In R. P. Kluft & C. G. Fine (Eds ). Clinical
perspectives on multiple personality disorder, pp 51-85. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Press.
Loewenstein, R.J. (1991) An office mental status examination for complex chronic dissociative
symptoms and multiple personality disorder. In R.J. Loewenstein (Ed ). Psychiatric Clinics of
North America Multiple Personality Disorder, 14(3), 567-604.
Macmillan, M. (1991). Freud evaluated: The completed arc. Amsterdam/New York North-Holland.
Mandler, J.M. (1979). Categorical and schematic organization of memory In C .R. Puff (Ed ), Memory
organization and structure. New York Academic Press.
McCann, I. L. , & Pearlman, L. A. (1990). Psychological trauma and the adult survivor: Theory,
therapy, and transformation. New York: Brunner/Mazel Publishers.
McDougall, W. (1920/1) The revival of emotional memories and its therapeutic value (III). British
Journal of Medical Psychology, 1, 23-29.
McDougall, W. (1926). An outline of abnormal psychology. London: Methuen.
Miller, A. (1986). Brief reconstructive hypnotherapy for anxiety reactions: Three case reports. American
Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 28, 138-146.
Myers, C. S. (1920/1). The revival of emotional memories and its therapeutic value (II). British Journal
of Medical Psychology, 1, 20-22.
Myers, C.S. (1940). Shell shock in France 1914-1.8 Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Noyes, R. , Hoenck, P. R. , & Kupperman, B.A. (1977). Depersonalization in accident victims and
psychiatric patients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 66, 451-458.
Orzek, A. M. (1985). The child's cognitive processing of sexual abuse. Child and Adolescent
Psychotherapy, 2, 110-114.
Parson, E. R. (1984). The reparation of self: Clinical and theoretical dimensions in the treatment of
Vietnam combat veterans. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 14, 4-56.
Peterson, ,J.A. (1991, November). Hypnotic techniques recommended to assist in associating the
dissociation: Abreaction. Paper presented at the 8th International Conference on Multiple
Personality/Dissociative States: Chicago, Illinois.
Putnam, F. W. (1989). Diagnosis and treatment of multiple personality disorder New York: Guilford
Press.
Putnam, F.W. (1990) Disturbances of self in victims of childhood sexual abuse In R. P. Kluft (Ed ),
Incest-related syndromes of adult psychopathology (pp 113-131). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Press.
Putnam, F.W. (1992). Using hypnosis for therapeutic abreactions. Psychiatric Medicine, 10 (1), 51-65.
Putnam, F.W., Guroff.JJ., Silberman, E.K., & Post, R.M. (1986). The clinical phenomenology of
multiple personality disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 47, 285-293.
Resick,P.A.,& Schnicke,M.K. (1992). Cognitive processing therapy for sexual assault victims. Journal
of Consulting and ClinicalPsychology, 60 (5), 748-756.
Ross, C.A. (1989). Multiple personality disorder: Diagnosis, clinical features and treatment. New York:
John Wiley & Sons.
23
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
Ross, C.A., Norton, G.R., & Wozney, K. (1989). Multiple personality disorder: An analysis of 236 cases.
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34, 413-418.
Ross, C.A. &: Gahan, P. (1988). Cognitive analysis of multiple personality disorder. American Journal
of Psychotherapy, 42, 229-239.
Sachs, R.G., Braun, E.G., &Shepp, E. (1988). Techniques for planned abreactions with MPD patients. In
B.G. Braun (Ed.), Dissociative Disorders 1988: Proceedings of the Fifth International
Conference on Multiple Personality/Dissociative States (p. 85). Chicago: Rush.
Sachs, R.G., Frischholz, E., & Wood, J.I. (1988). Marital and family therapy in the treatment of multiple
personality disorder. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 14, 249-259.
Sachs, R. G., & Peterson.J. A. (1994). Processing memories retrieved by trauma victims and survivors:
A primer for therapists. Tyler, Texas: Family Violence and Sexual Assault Institute.
Sakheim, D.K., Hess, E.P., & Chivas, A. (1988). General principles for short-term inpatient work with
multiple personality-disorder patients. Psychotherapy, 25, 117-124.
Scarry, E. (1985). The body in pain: The making and unmaking of the world. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Schultz, R. (1990). Secrets of adolescence: Incest and developmental issues. In R.P. Kluft (ed.), Incest-
related syndromes of adult psychopathology (pp. 133-159). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Press.
Schultz, R., Braun, B.G., & Kluft, R.P. (1989). Multiple personality disorder: Phenomenology of
selected variables in comparison to major depression. DISSOCIATION, 2(1), 45-51.
Seligman.M. (197'5). Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San Francisco: Freeman
Press.
Solomon, Z., Garb, R., Bleich.A, & Grupper, D. (1987). Reactivation of combat-related post-traumatic
stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 51-55.
Spiegel, D. (1986). Dissociation, double binds, and post-traumatic stress in multiple personality disorder.
In B.G. Braun (Ed.), Treatment of multiple personality disorder (pp. 61-77). Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Press.
Spiegel, D. (1981). Vietnam grief work using hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 24 289-
294.
Spiegel, D., & Cardeña, E. (1991). Disintegrated experience: The dissociative disorders revisited.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, (3), 366-378.
Steele, K. (1989). A model for abreaction with MPD and other dissociative disorder. DISSOCIATION,
2(3), 151-159.
Steele, K., & Colrain, J. (1990). Abreactive work with sexual abuse survivors: Concepts and techniques.
In M.A. Hunter (Ed.), The sexually abused male. Volume 2, Applications of treatment strategies
(pp. 1-55). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Summit, R.C. (1983). The child sexual abuse accommodation syndrome. Child Abuse and Neglect, 7
177-193.
Terr, L.C. (1991). Childhood traumas: An outline and overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148,
10-20.
Ulman, R.B., & Brothers, D. (1988). The shattered self: A psychoanalytic study of trauma. Hillsdale, NJ:
The Analytic Press.
Van der Hart, 0., Boon, S., Friedman, B., & Mierop, V. (1992). De reactivering van traumatische
herinneringen [The reactivation of traumatic memories]. Directieve Therapie, 12(1), 12-55.
Van der Hart, 0., Boon, S., & Van Everdingen, G.B. (1990). Writing assignments and hypnosis in the
treatment of traumatic memories. In M.L. Fass & D. Brown (Eds.), Creative mastery in hypnosis
and hypnoanalysis: A Festschrift for Erika Fromm (pp. 231-254). Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum
Associates.
Van der Hart, 0., Brown, P., & Van der Kolk, B.A. (1989). Pierre Janet's treatment of post-traumatic
stress. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 2 (4), 379-395.
Van der Hart, 0., Brown, P., & Turco, R.N. (1990). Hypnotherapy for traumatic grief: Janetian and
modern approaches integrated. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 32(4), 263-271.
Van der Hart, 0., & Brown, P. (1992). Abreaction re-evaluated. DISSOCIATION, 5(4), 127-138.
Van der Hart, 0., Steele, K., & Brown, P. (manuscript in preparation) . Traumatic memories: Repression
and dissociation re-evaluated.
Van der Hart, 0., & Friedman, B. (1989). A reader's guide to Pierre Janet on dissociation: A neglected
heritage. DISSOCIATION, 2(1), 3-16.
Van der Hart, 0., & Friedman, B. (1992). Trauma, dissociation and triggers: Their role in treatment and
emergency psychiatry. In J.B. van Luyn et al. (Eds.), Emergency psychiatry today (pp.137-142).
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Van der Hart, 0., & Op den Velde, W. (1991). Traumatische herinneringen [Traumatic memories]. In 0.
van der Hart (Ed.), Trauma, dissociatie en hypnose [Trauma, dissociation, and hypnosis] (pp. 71-
90). Amsterdam/Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Van der Kolk, B.A. (1987a). The psychobiology of the trauma response: Hyperarousal, constriction, and
addiction to traumatic reexposure. In B.A. van der Kolk (Ed.), Psychological trauma (pp. 63-88).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Van der Kolk, B.A. (Ed.) (1987b). Psychological trauma. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
24
Dissociation, 1993, 6(2/3), 162-180.
Van der Kolk, B.A. (1988). The biological response to psychic trauma. In F.M. Ochberg (Ed.), Post-
traumatic therapy and victims of violence (pp. 25-38). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Van der Kolk, B.A. (in press, 1994). The body keeps the score: The evolving psychobiology of post-
traumatic stress. Harvard Review of Psychiatry.
Van der Kolk, B.A., & Greenberg, M. (1987). The psychobiology of the trauma response: Hyperarousal,
constriction, and addiction to traumatic exposure. In B.A. van der Kolk (Ed.), Psychological
trauma (pp. 63-87). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Van der Kolk, B.A., & Van der Hart, 0. (1989). Pierre Janet and the breakdown of adaptation in
psychological trauma. American Joumal of Psychiatry, 146, 1530-1540.
Van der Kolk, B.A., & Van der Hart, 0. (1991). The intrusive past: The flexibility of memory and the
engraving of trauma. American Imago, 48(4), 425-545.
Van Putten, T., & Emory, W.H. (1973). Traumatic neurosis in Vietnam returnees: A forgotten diagnosis?
Archives of General Psychiatry, 29, 695-698.
Watkins,J.G. (1949). Hypnotherapy of war neuroses. New York: The Ronald Press Company.
Yalom, I. (1980). Existential psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.
25
... Learners themselves as well as the institution could benefit from an explicit consideration of recalled educational experience. The adoption of maladaptive performance avoidance goals on negative performance experiences (Anderman & Maehr 1994) could be reshaped through altered attributions as happens in trauma therapy (Van der Hart et al. 1993). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Literature describing the impact of guided self-study (G-SS) in knowledge changes and skills improvements in undergraduate students is scarce. Objectives The aims of our study were to evaluate the feasibility of a G-SS programme in a full-time undergraduate physiotherapy degree course and to assess the effectiveness of the G-SS on changes in knowledge and development of skills (hands-on). Method Fifty-three first-semester undergraduate physiotherapy students were randomly divided into a G-SS group and a control group (CG). The G-SS group received six clinical cases and prepared each case during an 8-day cycle. The control group received self-study learning units of the original curriculum content. Primary outcome parameters were (1) time of task, (2) responsiveness of students and (3) programme differentiation. Knowledge changes and skills changes were tested using a multiple-choice questionnaire and the objective structured clinical examination (OSCE). Results Students’ responsiveness was 32%. This was below the a priori set 83%. No differences in programme differentiation were found. The OSCE grade was significantly higher in the G-SS compared to CG (p = 0.003). Conclusion The G-SS programme in its current form was not feasible regarding students’ responsiveness. Therefore, a slight modification of our study protocol (e.g., better time planning in the academic calendar) is needed to improve students’ willingness to participate in the G-SS programme. Clinical implications Adaptation of the school timetable should allow undergraduate physiotherapy students to prepare clinical cases under conditions of lower workload. Guided self-study as compared to CG is superior in knowledge change and (hands-on) skills improvement.
... This is in line with prior findings from a randomized controlled trial indicating that an additional written account of traumatic events, in this case defined as a form of exposure, is particularly helpful in patients with high dissociation, presumably because high levels of dissociation may be reflective of high peritraumatic dissociation, which go hand in hand with more fragmented memories (Resick et al., 2012). Hence, patients with high levels of dissociation may need more time and effort to synthesize traumatic memories before being able to integrate them in their wider memory networks (van der Hart et al., 1993). Therefore, reprocessing of traumatic memories can be helpful also in cases of severe comorbidities such as severe dissociation and AVH. ...
Article
Treatment guidelines for complex presentations of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are often cautious about the reprocessing of traumatic memories and recommend multicomponent treatments that are widely used in clinical practice. Yet, the role of reprocessing in these multicomponent treatments remains unknown. Using naturalistic data of 97 patients treated for PTSD, we used a linear mixed model to investigate the role of reprocessing for the outcome at discharge and at 6-month follow-up. Treatment effects were significant and large (g = 0.91-1.05). The final model showed good fit and explained 51% of the variance. There was a significant main effect of time (B = −8.1 [−11.5; −4.8], p < 0.001), as well as a reprocessing by time interaction (B = −17.2 [−30.5; −3.8], p = 0.012), indicating better outcomes with higher levels of reprocessing. Hence, maximizing the amount of reprocessing used in multicom-ponent treatments for PTSD may significantly enhance outcomes.
... But still, done in an empathic way the verbalising of the experience has been recommended by therapists both in psychoanalysis and in behavioural cognitive therapy (Follette & Ruzek, 2006, Foa et al., 2009. In emergencies we find that some survivors talk extensively about what happened to them, while others are more or less blocked (Hart, Steele, Boon & Brown, 1993;van der Kolk, 1994). In our meeting with the survivors of the earthquake, we found some, who refused to talk about what happened to them. ...
Article
The article describes how psychosocial support, in the immediate and mid-term disaster response for Haitian earthquake survivors in hospitals in the Dominican Republic, ameliorated some of the suffering and prevent the crisis from becoming crystallised into symptoms and complex grief. The support was centred on (1) providing information; (2) facilitating social regulation of emotions; (3) supporting a social support network for the patient; (4) focusing coping efforts; and (5) normalization of reactions. Finally it is described how the crisis made values of caring, compassion and responsibility expressed and shown by the survivors.
... It is conceivable that managing the call during and after the contact leads the helper to somehow integrate the traumatic experience of the callers into their own life experience. At the clinical level, this is what happens when the so-called integration phase of trauma occurs in traumatized patients [19,20]. It is the phase in which individuals give themselves the opportunity to orient to the future with new forces and new energies arising from the relationship in a prospective view. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Usually managed by trained volunteers, crisis helplines services are of primary importance in community care. However, literature has shown that volunteers may be exposed to a high risk of developing negative psychological consequences in relation to the service performed. Although there are numerous studies dedicated to the use of helplines, evidence regarding the experiences of helpers in the context of traumatic losses is still limited. Objective: This study aimed to explore lived experiences of the operators of a crisis line supporting traumatic bereavement. Specifically, it analyzed the psychological impact of interacting with the caller, and the resources and strategies used by the operators themselves. Methods: The study considered 35 operators of the helpline provided by the Italian NGO De Leo Fund, which offers psychological support to survivors of traumatic bereavement. The inclusion criteria were: (a) currently being or having been a helper at the De Leo Fund helpline; and (b) having completed at least 10 shifts in the helpline service. Data were collected through an ad hoc questionnaire, filled online. The thematic analysis technique used Atlas.ti software 8. Results: Four thematic areas emerged from the transcription of the semi-structured interviews. These were: (a) motivation and expectations; (b) emotional and relational impact of the experience as a helper; (c) strategies adopted by operators; and (d) criticalities and strengths. Conclusions: Monitoring helpers’ emotional experiences and the impact of their work overtime emerged in a marked way. The analysis of the strategies and experiences of the operators represents a fundamental factor for the implementation of specific training programs for the management of emergency situations.
... Sommige auteurs stelden dat seksueel misbruik zo traumatisch kan zijn dat de herinnering niet toegankelijk is voor het geheugen (zoals Briere & Conte, 1993;Herman & Schatzow, 1987). Tegelijkertijd claimden ze dat deze verdrongen herinneringen in therapie weer opgehaald konden worden (zoals Van der Hart et al., 1993). Uit onderzoek bleek echter dat traumatische gebeurtenissen over het algemeen juist goed in het geheugen worden opgeslagen Lindsay & Reed, 1994;Merckelbach & Wessel, 1995). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter presents drama-based participatory research conducted by the author with Afro-Colombian youth. This initiative aimed to explore how the embodiment and performance of historical documents is a distinctively unique approach to healing the intergenerational trauma of enslavement. In 2019, a research group at the National University of Bogotá (Colombia) identified historical letters dated 1743–1966, written by a slave foreman working in a mine on the Colombian Pacific coast and addressed to the slave master living in a city of the interior. The letters provide details on the living conditions of enslaved people and their strategies of resistance and adaptation. The National University identified a community on the Colombian Pacific coast whose biological ancestors were the enslaved people mentioned in the letters, and invited members of this community to participate in a theatre workshop where they would embody and perform the events described in the letters. Data show that the combination of dramaturgy based on historical documents, the embodiment of ancestral stories, public performance, and reflection help communities affected by intergenerational traumas to re-signify their experience, heal their collective wounds, and imagine a different future.
Chapter
Dissociative Identity Disorder (DID) is a complex illness that requires a multimodal approach to treatment. While psychopharmacology is reserved for co-morbid psychiatric illnesses, individual psychotherapy remains the cornerstone of treatment with a strong therapeutic alliance necessary for successful treatment outcomes. The primary goal of treatment is to reduce distress and impairment, promote cooperation and integration between the different identities, and improve overall functioning. The International Society for the Study of Trauma and Dissociation (ISSTD) provides treatment guidelines for DID that recommend a psychodynamic phase-oriented treatment approach, involving stabilization, development of adaptive coping skills, trauma processing, and integration of fragmented identities. Treatment must be individualized and paced to match the patient’s capacity and readiness for each phase. Adjunctive therapies such as cognitive behavioral therapy, eye movement desensitization and reprocessing, and hypnotherapy may be helpful. Family therapy can also be incorporated to address the family system dynamics and provide support for the patient. Despite the challenges associated with DID, it is possible to achieve significant improvement in DID symptomatology over time with consistent and expert-guided treatment. Future research is needed to continue to improve the understanding and treatment of DID.
Article
Full-text available
A research synthesis was conducted to examine the relationship between a written emotional expression task and subsequent health. This writing task was found to lead to significantly improved health outcomes in healthy participants. Health was enhanced in 4 outcome types—reported physical health, psychological well-being, physiological functioning, and general functioning—but health behaviors were not influenced. Writing also increased immediate (pre- to postwriting) distress, which was unrelated to health outcomes. The relation between written emotional expression and health was moderated by a number of variables, including the use of college students as participants, gender, duration of the manipulation, publication status of the study, and specific writing content instructions.
Article
Full-text available
This Australian study examined 310 respondents’ retrospective reports about the quality of their relationship with their caregivers during their childhood to identify the dynamics that predict clinical levels of dissociation in adulthood. The sample comprised 277 university participants and 32 inpatients and outpatients receiving treatment for a dissociative disorder. The authors developed the 32-item parent-child dynamics questionnaire (PCD-Q) to understand and identify aspects of the relational dynamics and home environment that gives rise to dissociation. The PCD-Q demonstrated excellent internal reliability and effectively differentiated between groups with different levels of dissociation. For females in the university sample, parent-child dynamics were clearly positive for 186 non-dissociative controls, mildly positive for the 27 individuals with elevated levels of dissociation consistent with a dissociative disorder or PTSD, and mildly negative for the 26 with clinical levels of dissociation, whereas the 30 females in the clinical group experienced clearly negative dynamics. The PCD-Q demonstrated protective factors from dissociation as parental care, being able to seek comfort, and developing self-esteem and self-worth; experiences of safety (absence of unpredictable parental behaviour, powerlessness and control, unfairness, secrecy, isolation, and dysfunction), and feeling seen and being supported on the road to independence. Nineteen items (59%) had large effect sizes, and 11 items (34%) had medium effect sizes. In males, despite the small sample significant differences were found between the 34 controls and 12 dissociators for half the PCD-Q items. The findings suggest parent-child dynamics may be equally as influential as trauma in the aetiology of dissociation. Highlights Three of the highest risk factors for clinical dissociation were as follows: 21-fold for females who were uncomfortable seeking comfort when hurt, unwell or upset 17-fold for females who did not have any control over their life while growing up Seven-fold for males whose parents did not support them to become independent