ArticlePDF Available

Cognitive research and the design of instruction

Taylor & Francis
Educational Psychologist
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The utilization of cognitive psychological theory and findings from research to inform the design of instruction is illustrated in this paper. Physics learning studies demonstrate that students' pre‐instructional world knowledge is often logically antagonistic to the principles of Newtonian mechanics taught in introductory physics. Under these conditions psychological theory predicts that learning will be inhibited, a prediction consistent with both the experiences of physics teachers and the results of empirical investigation. Informed by cognitive research on problem solving, semantic memory, and knowledge acquisition, instruction has been designed to encourage the reconciliation of world knowledge and physics content among beginning physics students.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article was downloaded by: [Monash University Library]
On: 10 February 2014, At: 02:15
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Educational Psychologist
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hedp20
Cognitive research and the design of science instruction
Audrey B. Champagne
a
, Leopold E. Klopfer
b
& Richard F. Gunstone
c
a
Learning Research and Development Center , University of Pittsburgh , 3939 O'Hara Street,
Pittsburgh, PA, 15260
b
Learning Research and Development Center , University of Pittsburgh ,
c
Monash University , Melbourne, Australia
Published online: 01 Oct 2009.
To cite this article: Audrey B. Champagne , Leopold E. Klopfer & Richard F. Gunstone (1982) Cognitive research and the design
of science instruction, Educational Psychologist, 17:1, 31-53
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461528209529242
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the
publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations
or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any
opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the
views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be
independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses,
actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever
caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Educational Psychologist
1982,
Vol. 17, No. 1, 31—53
Cognitive Research and the Design
of Science Instruction
Audrey B. Champagne and Leopold E. Klopfer
Learning Research and Development Center
University of Pittsburgh
Richard F. Gunstone
Monash University
Melbourne, Australia
The utilization of cognitive psychological theory and findings from
research to inform the design of instruction is illustrated in this paper.
Physics learning studies demonstrate that students' pre-instructional world
knowledge is often logically antagonistic to the principles of Newtonian
mechanics taught in introductory physics. Under these conditions
psychological theory predicts that learning will be inhibited, a prediction
consistent with both the experiences of physics teachers and the results of
empirical investigation. Informed by cognitive research on problem solv-
ing, semantic memory, and knowledge acquisition, instruction has been
designed to encourage the reconciliation of world knowledge and physics
content among beginning physics students.
Most science textbooks can be criticized by drawing
attention to the fact. . . .that these books are
chiefly concerned with the statements of results.
Usually the most general results are put near the
beginning of the textbook. A textbook in physics
begins by telling about molecules and the constitu-
tion of matter or by giving some of the most com-
pactly formulated statements about the principles
öf mechanics .... The degree of enthusiasm of
the ordinary student for these introductions which
he gets in the textbooks is very slight indeed ....
The student, confronted by these verbal additions
to his experience, gets into the habit of thinking of
science as verbal additions to experience, and he
faithfully learns the words and keeps them in store
against the time when the teacher demands them.
(Judd, 1915, pp. 334-336)
Introduction
One recent trend in cognitive psychology
has increasingly focused the attention of
researchers on learning tasks representative of
those which students encounter in school in-
struction (Greeno, 1980). Developments such
as this hold the promise of an improved
theoretical basis for instruction. A theoretically
organized and systematically verified
psychological foundation is an essential
The address of Audrey B. Champagne is Learning
Research and Development Center; University of Pitts-
burgh; 3939 O'Hara Street; Pittsburgh, PA 15260.
requisite for effecting substantial im-
provements in instruction. However, the ex-
istence of such theory offers no guarantee that
instruction systematically and veridically incor-
porating principles derived from the theory
will be designed. A necessary condition for the
systematic application of theory to instruc-
tional practice is a science of design as
". . .a body of intellectually tough, analytic,
partly formalizable, partly empirical, technical
doctrine about the design process (Simon,
1981,
p. 132)." Only when a science of in-
structional design exists will the design process
cease to hide behind the cloak of "judgment"
or "experience."
This article is motivated by the need to
make explicit the design process as it applies to
the design of instruction, explicit description
of the process being a necessary first step in the
development of a science of instructional
design. We describe how we have applied the
theory and empirical findings of cognitive
psychology to devise a course of action aimed
at changing an existing situation into a desired
one (Simon, 1981, p. 129). Specifically, the
course of action is the instruction, the existing
situation is the cognitive state of the
uninstructed student, and the desired situa-
tion is a student's cognitive state that approx-
imates specified features characteristic of the
cognitive state of an expert in the field (the
ideal state).
Copyright 1982 by Division 15 of the American Psychological Association, Inc.
31
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
32
AUDREY B. CHAMPAGNE, LEOPOLD E. KLOPFER AND RICHARD F. GUNSTONE
The design process to be explicated in this
article represents an alternative approach to
the design of instruction. Central to this pro-
cess is the analysis of customary instructional
tasks for the purpose of specifying the underly-
ing cognitive processes and structures that are
necessary for the successful completion of the
task. The specification of these processes and
structures for a variety of school subject-matter
domains represents an important part of the
recent empirical findings of cognitive science
research. Thus, if detailed process and struc-
ture descriptions appear to be helpful in
designing instruction in physics (as we intend
to demonstrate), they should also be helpful in
designing instruction in other domains.
We begin by describing the practical
relevance of the instructional problem that in-
terests us, namely, student difficulties in the
learning of classical Newtonian mechanics.
The Instructional Problem
There is a general agreement among physics
instructors and students that mechanics is dif-
ficult to teach and to learn (Kolody, 1977).
Students have difficulty comprehending
classical mechanics, and physics instructors
often express disappointment with the out-
come of their efforts to instruct students in
classical mechanics. This instructional problem
has been discussed at length in the literature of
physics education where various underlying
causal factors contributing to the problem have
been suggested (Gerson & Primrose, 1977;
Halley & Eaton, 1974; Hudson & Mclntire,
1977).
Two distinct perspectives on this learn-
ing problem are identifiable.
One perspective assumes that learning dif-
ficulties occur when the learner is deficient in
skills which are assumed to be prerequisite to
the study of physics (e.g., Arons, 1976;
Renner, Grant, & Sutherland, 1978). The
other perspective links the observed learning
difficulties with the fact that students coming
to introductory physics courses have firmly
embedded conceptualizations of how and why
objects move, and that these conceptualiza-
tions are in clear conflict with the canonical
view of that subject-matter domain which the
student will be required to learn. One line of
science education research and psychological
research on semantic memory is particularly
relevant to the second perspective.
Research Background
Pre-instructional Knowledge and Students '
Interpretation of Instruction
The research we examine furnishes a context
for describing the existing situation, the
uninstructed student's cognitive state, and
distinguishing it from the desired situation.
Various empirical studies conducted by science
educators (including Brumby, in press; Driver,
1973;
Driver & Easley, 1978; Fleshner, 1963;
Gunstone & White, 1981; Leboutet-Barrell,
1976;
Rowell & Dawson, 1977; Singer &
Benassi, 1981; Viennot, 1979) and
psychologists (including Clement, 1979;
Green, McCloskey & Caramazza, 1980;
Selman, Jaquette, Krupa, & Stone, in press)
demonstrate that, for several science content
areas:
1.
Students have descriptive and ex-
planatory systems for scientific phenomena
that develop before they experience formal
study of the subject.
2.
These descriptive and explanatory
systems differ in significant ways from those
the students are expected to learn as the
result of formal study.
3.
These alternative conceptual systems
show remarkable consistency across diverse
populations.
4.
These alternative conceptual systems are
remarkably resistant to change by exposure
to traditional instructional methods.
5.
These alternative conceptual systems are
not facilitative to the learning process.
Students interpret instructional events (for
example, experiments and expository text)
in the context of the conceptual scheme
they currently hold, not the one that the ex-
periments or the text are designed to
convey.
These effects are particularly striking in the
context of mechanics where prior to formal
instruction young people and adults have a
conception of motion that is more
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
COGNITIVE RESEARCH AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN
33
Aristotelian' than Newtonian (Champagne,
Klopfer, Solomon, & Cahn, 1980; Clement,
1979;
Driver, 1973; Driver & Easley, 1978;
Leboutet- Barrell, 1976; Singer & Benassi,
1981;
Viennot, 1980). Other research findings
show that remnants of the Aristotelian concep-
tion persist with many "successful" physics
students, that is, with students receiving high
grades in introductory physics courses (Cham-
pagne, Klopfer & Anderson, 1980; Gunstone
& White, 1981). This research provides em-
pirical support for what physics teachers have
long observed, namely, that traditional in-
struction does not facilitate an appropriate
reconciliation of preinstructional knowledge
with the content of instruction. (Ausubel,
Novak, & Hanesian, 1978).
A study by Leboutet-Barrell (1976) indicates
that high school and college students have
misconceptions about force and motion which
persist despite instruction. The misconceptions
are described as pre-Galilean. In a study by
Cole and Raven (1969) with 12 to 15 year-olds,
it was necessary to give the students "oppor-
tunities to reject irrelevant factors in
understanding the principle of flotation."
Rowell and Dawson (1977) also explicitly con-
sidered common misconceptions when design-
ing instruction on the law of floating. Despite
efforts to refute misconceptions in instruction,
some misconceptions persisted. Instructional
work by Fleshner (1963) in the Soviet Union
indicates that students' intuitive ideas may co-
exist with ideas derived from instruction. In a
study by Driver (1973), 11 and 12 year-old
students were interviewed prior to, during,
and after instruction on several topics of a
physical science course. Although alternative
theoretical frameworks to explain observations
were introduced to the students and used dur-
ing the instruction, Driver reports that
counter-examples and conflicting evidence did
not produce changes in students' thinking.
Our own work (Champagne, Klopfer, &
Anderson, 1980; Champagne, Klopfer,
Solomon, & Cahn, 1980) has demonstrated
that prior knowledge affects students' com-
prehension of science instruction. We have
been particularly interested in the difficulty
that beginning physics students have in learn-
ing classical mechanics. Our research has
demonstrated that it is not the students' lack
of prior knowledge which makes the learning
of this topic so difficult, rather their con-
flicting knowledge. They come to instruction
with well-formed notions about the motion of
objects notions that have been reinforced by
their experiences. However, their notions may
stand in contradiction to the tenets of classical
physics, and these notions tend to interfere
with or inhibit the learning of mechanics. This
research demonstrates specific ways in which
students' conceptions influence (a) their
understanding of science texts and lectures, (b)
their observations, and (c) their interpretations
of their observations. Often the influence of
the students' conceptions is to inhibit their
understanding or distort their observations and
interpretations of experiments.
Other research (Champagne, Klopfer, &
Anderson, 1980) demonstrates that the belief
in the proposition, heavier objects fall faster
than lighter objects, is not readily changed by
instruction, thus demonstrating the strong in-
fluence that prior knowledge has on the effec-
tiveness of instruction in this case the prior
knowledge having an inhibiting effect on lear-
ning. In a study of beginning college physics
students, about four students in five believed
that, all other things being equal, heavier ob-
jects fall faster than lighter ones. These results
were particularly surprising since about 70% of
the students in the sample had studied high
school physics, some for two years. A chi-
square test showed that students in the sample
who had studied high school physics did not
score significantly better than those who had
not. This finding has been replicated in
follow-up studies.
In a report of a similar study of the
knowledge of gravity possessed by beginning
first-year physics students at Monash Universi-
ty, all of whom had successfully completed two
years of high school physics, Gunstone &
White (1981) conclude: (a) "... students
know a lot of physics but do not relate it to the
everyday world;" and (b) "In many instances
the students used mathematical equations to
1
Aristotle considered rest to be the natural state of ob-
jects.
In the absence of any cause, an object does not move;
conversely, when an object is moving, it must have been
caused, usually by a force. Aristotle also argued that the
speed of an object is directly proportional to the force ac-
ting on it, and inversely proportional to the resistance of
the medium through which the object is moving. In the
Newtonian physics of today, it is stated that an object will
continue in its existing state (cither at rest or moving with
constant speed in a straight line) unless it is acted on by a
net force. The acceleration of the object is directly propor-
tional to this net force and inversely proportional to the
mass of the object.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
34 AUDREY B. CHAMPAGNE, LEOPOLD E. KLOPFER AND RICHARD F. GUNSTONE
explain predictions, though often inap-
propriately, which indicates that they had lots
of physics knowledge to hand [sic] but were
unskilled in seeing which bit applied to the
given situation (p. 299)." In their conclusions,
Gunstone and White note that "... much
more attention may have to be given to in-
tegrating the knowledge acquired in school to
general knowledge (p. 298)."
The difficulty is compounded by the fact
that many of the terms used in classical
mechanics are also used in everyday life
terms such as acceleration, momentum, speed,
and force. The meanings of these terms as used
by physicists are quite different from the way
in which they are used in everyday life. Thus,
we observe that students misinterpret
mechanics instruction because they interpret
physics lectures and textbooks in the context of
their everyday understanding of the terms
rather than in the way in which the teacher or
text is using the terms.
Theoretical analysis. These descriptions of
the interactive effects of knowledge on
understanding are consistent with findings
emerging from cognitive psychology that
demonstrates the impact of existing knowledge
in memory on the comprehension of text
(Anderson, Reynolds, Schauert, & Goetz,
1977;
Bransford & McCarrell, 1974; Lindsay &
Norman, 1972). This research demonstrates
that all incoming stimuli which are
remembered are subject to reorganization by
the learner. The incoming stimuli are primar-
ily restructured by the learner in terms of the
learner's own past experiences, and only secon-
darily in terms of the organizing principles of
the material
itself.
In instructional situations
generally, students engage in active, mean-
ingful'structuring of text they read and lectures
they hear in order to remember and under-
stand incoming information.
Cognitive psychologists have studied ways in
which prior semantic knowledge influences
comprehension of verbal materials. The early
studies in the area of reading comprehension
aimed at demonstrating that something other
than the linguistic structure of a sentence is re-
quired to explain a person's comprehension of
that sentence. The "something other" is
described as the person's world knowledge and
is often characterized as a "schema," "plan,"
or "script." Bransford and McCarrell (1974)
review studies which indicate that the process
of understanding text involves creation of
"semantic descriptions" that use both the
reader's world knowledge and the sentence in-
put. In this research, the contexts for inter-
pretation of text either were common world
knowledge or were induced by the experi-
menter. Anderson et al. (1977) indicate that
an individual's "private" representation of
the world can affect text comprehension. In
general, studies of text comprehension
indicate the facilitative effect of schemata or
world knowledge. However, studies of physics
learning indicate that world knowledge is
logically antagonistic to the content to be
learned and often persists after physics
instruction.
Cognitive Contents of Uninstructured
Physics Students' Cognitive State.
From our analysis of empirical studies in-
vestigating students' preinstructional concep-
tions of the motion of objects, we conclude
that the following are characteristic of the con-
tents of the cognitive state of uninstructed
physics students:
1.
Concepts are poorly differentiated. For
example, students use the terms speed,
velocity, and acceleration interchangeably;
thus,
the typical student does not perceive
any difference between two propositions
such as these (a) the speed of an object is
proportional to the [net] force on the ob-
ject; (b) The acceleration of an object is pro-
portional to the [net] force on the object.
2.
Meanings physicists attribute to terms
are different from the everyday meanings
attributed to the terms.
3.
Propositions are imprecise and the im-
precision derives from several different
sources.
(a) Some of the imprecision of proposi-
tions is attributable to the meanings
students have for technical concepts
which are different from the canonical
meaning. Example: More force means
more speed.
(b) Other imprecision can be interpreted
as errors of scale (Gunstone & White,
1981).
Example: Gravity pulls harder on
objects that are closer to the earth. (This
proposition, in the context of an object
falling a distance of three meters, is cor-
rect only in theory because the difference
in the force of gravity (approximately
1 part in lO"'-
5
) is too small to measure.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
COGNITIVE RESEARCH AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN
35
If, however, the difference in distance
from the earth were large (several hun-
dred kilometers), the difference in the
force of gravity is significant.)
(c) Other propositions are just wrong and
may arise because of students' attempts
to inappropriately formulate general
rules of motion from their experiences in
the real world. Example: Heavy objects
fall faster than lighter objects.
Implications for instruction. Research
reviewed in this section demonstrates that the
cognitive contents of the uninstructed student
differs from the desired state with respect to
propositions and the meaning of concepts.
Uninstructed students apply propositions that
link force with motion, whereas Newtonian
mechanics links force with change in motion.
Moreover, the meaning uninstructed students
attribute to technical terminology is different
from the technical meaning. For example, the
technical meaning of acceleration is a change
in the magnitude of velocity or direction of
velocity of an object, while the meaning
uninstructed students attribute to acceleration
is speeding up.
Application of the Simon paradigm to the
design process requires detailed specification
of the meaning concepts have for the
uninstructed person and the principles that
uninstructed persons apply in the analysis of
motion. Such specifications are prerequisite to
the process of specifying the goals of instruc-
tion, and they allow for (a) the generation of
hypotheses about why certain instructional
practices are not successful, and (b) the con-
struction of possible mechanisms that will
result in the desired changes in the learners'
cognitive state.
Differences like those described above bet-
ween the uninstructed learners' cognitive state
and the desired cognitive state provide dear
specifications of changes instruction should
produce. The instructional goal, then, is to
bring about the specified changes in the
learners' cognitive state. We hypothesize that
the observed differences between the
uninstructed and desired cognitive states result
in certain of the difficulties that students ex-
perience in learning mechanics, an interpreta-
tion that is consistent with cognitive theory.
We further hypothesize that the observed in-
teractive effects of prior knowledge and in-
struction may be more pronounced for
mechanics than for other subjects.
The development of practical principles of
motion is necessary for coping with the moving
objects that are encountered in daily life.
Thus,
all students begin the formal study of
mechanics with an experientially verified set of
principles that allow them to predict the mo-
tion of objects under the conditions prevalent
in the real world. In addition, the same words
that arc used to describe and explain motion in
everyday language also are used by physicists.
Contrast this situation with thermodynamics
or chemistry where the words used for
technical concepts are not a part of everyday
language (mole, enthalpy, entropy) and where
principles need not be developed to cope with
frequently encountered situations. In these
and similar subject areas, traditional expository
instruction is more successful. However, in
mechanics, instructional strategies need to be
applied that can make students aware of dif-
ferences between their everyday meanings of
words and principles of motion and those of
the instruction.
Before presenting detailed hypotheses
related to strategies which will produce the
desired changes in the cognitive contents of
uninstructed students, other relevant
characteristics of the learners' cognitive state
will be described.
Structural and Representational Features
of
Physics
Knowledge and Physics
Problem Solving Strategies
The preceding analysis focused on the con-
tents of memory propositions and meaning
of concepts and hypothesized how students
interpretation of instruction understanding
of lectures, text and experiments is in-
fluenced by significant differences between the
subject matter to be learned and the students'
cognitive contents. This section focuses on the
organization of the contents of memory: its
structural features and modes of representa-
tion; the differences between the structural
organization and representations of expert
physicists, novices and uninstructed physics
students; and the implications of these dif-
ferences for physics instruction.
Descriptions of the structural features and
representations of physics knowledge derive
principally from research on physics problem
solving. Researchers in the domain of problem
solving are concerned with both the strategies
and structures that problem solvers are
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
36
AUDREY B. CHAMPAGNE, LEOPOLD E. KLOPFER AND RICHARD F. GUNSTONE
observed to apply in the successful solution of
problems (e.g., Greeno, 1978; Newell &
Simon, 1972). We shall review, in turn, the
pertinent research findings on structural
organization and problem solving strategies.
Structural organization and representation.
Research on physics problem solving provides
descriptions of the structural organizations and
representations characteristic of expert and
novice problem solvers. The solution of physics
problems requires both the availability of pro-
blem solving strategies and the understanding
of physical situations which are observed
directly or described in the text of the pro-
blem. Current theories of semantic memory
and natural language understanding (Ander-
son, 1976; Anderson et al., 1977; Bransford &
McCàrrell, 1975; Kintsch, 1974; Lindsay &
Norman, 1972; Norman & Rumelhart, 1975;
Quillian, 1968; Schank, 1972; Winograd,
1972) tie the existence of relevant schemata to
the process of making inferences and coming
to understand a situation.
In the context of physics, understanding im-
plies (a) the construction of mental representa-
tions of physical situations that include the ob-
jects that are a part of the physical situation,
(b) the concepts and scientific principles that
are relevant to the situation, and (c) the rela-
tionships that exist between objects, concepts
and principles (Winograd, 1972). Essential to
the construction of a mental representation is
the process of inference. Making valid in-
ferences is dependent on schemata that are
relevant, correct and complete. Thus,
understanding physical situations as physicists
understand them requires both that the rele-
vant schema is present and that the features of
the physical situation evoke the schema.
Recent work by Chi, Feltovich, and Glaser
(1981) describes the following explicit dif-
ferences in schemata of experts and novices: (a)
The schemata of experts are based on physical
principles (for example, energy conservation
and Newton's Second Law), but the schemata
of novices are based on physical objects (for ex-
ample, springs and inclined planes) and men-
tal constructs (for example, friction and gravi-
ty),
(b) The contents of the schemata of experts
and novices do not differ significantly in infor-
mation content; however, the novices' struc-
tures lack important relations, specifically rela-
tions between the surface features of the pro-
blem and the scientific principles which are the
basis for solutions, (c) Experts translate
surface features of the problems into canonical
objects, states, and constructs, while the
novices represent the problem in terms of the
literal objects and constructs described in the
text of the problem, (d) Links exist in the ex-
perts'
representations of knowledge structures
between the abstract representation of features
of the problems and the physical principles
which are the basis for the solution of the pro-
blem, (e) Experts' schema are organized
hierarchically along the dimension of abstract-
ness;
in contrast, the different levels of the
novices' knowledge are not well integrated,
thus preventing easy access from one level of
abstraction to another.
Research conducted by science educators pro-
vides descriptions of the organization and rep-
resentations of mechanics knowledge (motion-
of-objects schemata) in uninstructed students.
Motion-of-objects schemata of uninstructed
students are situation-specific, thus suggesting
that no naive abstract representation is extant in
die schemata to make them appear to be ap-
plicable to a large number of physical situations
(Gunstone, 1980).
This last characteristic was exemplified in
our work with middle-school students (Cham-
pagne, Klopfer, Soloman, & Cahn, 1980). We
have observed that, given four physical situa-
tions,
all of which could be explained by using
Newton's Second Law (F= ma), students never
give any indication that they perceive that a
common explanatory system might be applied
to all four of the situations. In fact, they never
notice that a proposition they have used to ex-
plain the motion in one of the situations is
directly contradicted by a proposition they use
to explain the motion in another situation.
This failure to see the contradiction suggests
that they are unaware of any need for con-
sistency across situations. For example,
students do not recognize that the same
physical laws apply to objects in free fall and to
objects sliding down an inclined plane. At one
point during a class discussion, for example,
students agreed that two carts of unequal
mass,
but equal volume, would strike the
ground at approximately the same time when
dropped from the same height. When they
were asked to compare the times for the carts
to slide down an incline, however, only one of
them argued that the times would be about
the same.
Problem solving strategies of experts and
novices. Cognitive research on problem solv-
ing has generated detailed specifications of
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
COGNITIVE RESEARCH AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN
37
problem solving strategies for many categories of
problems (Greeno, 1978; Newell & Simon,
1972;
Larkin, Note 1). One finding from this
research that is particularly pertinent to the in-
struction we propose is reported by Larkin. Her
analyses of thinking-aloud protocols indicate that
expert physicists perform a preliminary
qualitative analysis before proposing equations
for the quantitative solution of the problem. In
contrast, novices immediately begin the search
for an equation and proceed to match the
information presented in the problem with terms
in the equation.
Given that the beginning students' ex-
planatory schemata are so situation-specific, it is
hardly surprising that their problem solving
strategy is similarly bound to the perceived situa-
tions.
The students' main strategy for solving
motion-of-objects problems is to try to recall a
rule or relationship which they believe to be ap-
plicable to the specific situation at hand. Rarely,
if ever, is there any evidence that the beginning
students are aware of general problem solving
strategies, related to general physical principles or
laws,
which are applicable across many situations.
Differences in Structure and their
Instructional Implications
Empirically-derived descriptions of the
characteristics of the schemata of uninstructed
students, novices, and experts are summarized in
Table 1. This summary makes evident the con-
trasts and similarities in the characteristics of the
three groups' schemata with respect to principles,
surface features, and second-order features, each
of which is briefly explained in the first column.
Also summarized in Table 1 are descriptions of
the problem solving strategies for each group.
Precise descriptions of the differences in the
organization and representation of the physics
knowledge of individuals at different levels
of competence provide a further basis for the
specification of the goal and objectives for begin-
ning mechanics instruction and allow us to
generate hypotheses about (a) how current in-
strucrional practice may impede the attainment
of the goal and (b) alternative instructional
mechanisms that will facilitate the attainment of
the objectives.
Contrasting the problem solving strategies and
organization of the mechanics knowledge of ex-
perts,
novices and uninstructed students
yields the following goal for beginning
mechanics instruction: Development of a well in-
tegrated motion-of-objects schema that is
organized in a way that produces (a) the solution
of mechanics problems via the method of
qualitative analysis and (b) the analysis of the
motion of objects in the real world using the
tenets of Newtonian mechanics. The detailed
specifications of the mechanics knowledge
organization to be accomplished as the result of
instruction constitute the instructional objectives
subsumed under the goal.
Empirical evidence demonstrates that current
instructional practice does not facilitate the at-
tainment of identified goal. Students generally
do not learn to relate their mechanics knowledge
to real situations as the result of either high
school or beginning college physics instruction.
Based on a cursory analysis of physics texts and
our knowledge of physics instruction, we con-
clude that preliminary qualitative analysis of
physics problems is seldom if ever taught ex-
plicitly. In fact, problem solving instruction in
physics textbooks makes no attempt to link the
physical features of the real-world situations
described in physics to the abstract concepts and
principles of the Newtonian framework.
Physics texts teach the problem solution
strategy that novices typically use. The first step
in sample solutions is the presentation of the
equation that will yield a quantitative solution to
the problem. There is no attempt to instruct the
student in the expert physicists' analytic pro-
cedures which result in abstract representations of
physical situations in terms of abstract concepts.
These concepts are vital because they in turn can
be linked to principles or laws of mechanics and
formal expressions of the principle or law (for-
mulas) which can then be applied to reach a
quantitative solution of the problem.
The traditional practice is counterproductive
in two respects: (a) It teaches a problem solution
strategy that does not approximate the strategy
exemplified by the ideal state, and (b) It does not
encourage the development of links in cognitive
structure between real-world situations and the
abstract representations of physical situations that
characterize the to-be-approximated schemata.
Experiences contributing to the expert's
cognitive state. An interesting theoretical ques-
tion, with implications for both practice and
theory, should now be posed: In the absence of
direct instruction, how do experts come to
develop the qualitative analysis strategy and the
conceptual links between physical situations and
the appropriate abstract representations?
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
Table 1
Problem Solving Strategies and Schemata of Uninstructed Students, Novices, and Experts
PROBLEM SOLVING STRATEGIES
UNINSTRUCTED
STUDENTS
NOVICES
EXPERTS
The typical procedure for solving problems is to find a general rule which appears to cover the physical situation de-
scribed in the problem, and then to use the relationship described in the identified rule deductively to derive an answer
to the problem. Rules to be employed in this process may be recalled from experiences with similar physical situations,
or they may be recollections of authoritative statements from books or people.
The principal procedure for solving problems is to instantiate variables in equations. This procedure may be chained
through a series of equations. Abstracted solution methods are lacking.
Associated with the organizing schema are its specific conditions of applicability and the necessary problem solution
methods. Experts abstract a basic solution strategy from the surface features of the problem and engage in qualitative
analysis of the problem prior to determining a quantitative solution.
SCHEMATA
FEATURES OF
SCHEMATA
CHARACTERISTICS OF
UNINSTRUCTED STUDENTS'
SCHEMATA
CHARACTERISTICS OF
NOVICES' SCHEMATA
CHARACTERISTICS OF
EXPERTS' SCHEMATA
c
o
>
O
o
3
G
m
O
i
g
z
3
PRINCIPLES
Ideas of some degree of general-
ity that express relationships;
principles are applied to solving
problems; can serve to organize
schemata.
Principles are generalized rules
derived from everyday exper-
iences (world knowledge). They
are imprecise propositions. The
imprecision is due to vagueness
about the meaning of concepts,
errors of scale, and inappropriate
formulations of general rules.
The principles (rules) have
limited scope and tend to be
situation-specific. The notion
that an abstract principle can
apply to a range of different
physical situations is lacking or
poorly developed. There appears
to be no awareness of the need
for consistency along the rules
that cover different physical
situations.
Principles are relationships
between physical variables ex-
pressed as equations or rules.
Some of the principles are the
major physical laws expressed in
equation form, but there is no
evidence that they serve as
organizers of schemata.
Principles are major physical
laws,
which are highly abstract
and express relationships of
great generality. Included with
each principle are the conditions
under which the principle ap-
plies.
Each principle has an
associated schema, which is
oriented by the content and ap-
plicability conditions of the
principle. The applicability con-
ditions usually are expressed in
terms of second-order features.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
OBJECTS AND PHYSICAL
CONDITIONS (SURFACE
FEATURES)
Physical objects and conditions
described or presented in a pro-
blem situation; physical features
of objects and their states of
motion or position that are
directly perceivable from verbal
description; diagrams or direct
observation of the physical
situation.
Concrete objects and the direct-
ly observable properties of ob-
jects are present. A reasonable
inference is that the objects and
properties define the specific
physical situation which, in
turn,
directs the search in memory for
a general rule that covers it.
Physical objects and their sur-
face features are the basis for
categorizing problems. It is infer-
red that an object or a config-
uration of objects functions as
the organizing element (node) in
its schema for the problem. The
content of the representations
may be concrete objects or ab-
stractions at the level of
diagrams.
Concrete objects, their physical
configurations, and diagrams of
objects are present in the
schemata, but none of these is
prominent. It is inferred that
objects serve primarily as
vehicles for identifying second-
order features, and that some-
times they trigger the activation
of a particular principle-based
schema.
n
o
o
z
m
po
RI
n
z
o
i
o
I
1
3
o
PHYSICS CONCEPTS AND
SYMBOLS (SECOND-ORDER
FEATURES)
Idealizations of physical objects
(e.g.,
an elephant is represented
as a point mass), and constructs
or entities
(e.g.,
energy, force);
conventional representations of
physical entities
(e.g.,
vector
components).
There is no evidence that
second-order features are repre-
sented in these schemata. Con-
cepts and terms are present, but
many are poorly differentiated.
The meanings of the terms are
their real-world meanings, rather
than their technical meanings
in physics.
Some conventional representa-
tions of physical entities are
present, and idealizations of
physical objects may be used in
problem representations. Con-
cepts and terms related to the
objects which dominate the pro-
blem-solution schema are
present. Novices report taking
terms directly from the problem
statement to identify equations
that could be approximately em-
ployed in solving the problem.
Representations of physical
objects in their idealized form
are prominent, with the content
of the representations deter-
mined by the organizing schema.
Physical entities are represented
according to the conventions of
the
field.
Features abstracted
from the problem statement are
also present. Some experts
report that these features help to
select the basic approach to pro-
blem solutions, thus indicating
direct links between these
second order features and
prin-
ciples. Concepts relevant to the
organizing schema are present.
Associated with each concept
are its interconnections of
relations with other concepts
and with the schema's major
physical law.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
40
AUDREY B. CHAMPAGNE, LEOPOLD E. KLOPFER AND RICHARD F. GUNSTONE
For the purpose of our discussion, we con-
sider three factors that differentiate the ex-
periences of experts from the experiences of
novices; these are: (a) additional formal in-
struction, (b) more extensive practice in solv-
ing problems, and (c) more extensive verbal in-
teractions about physics. The Chi et al. (1981)
study suggests that the contents of novices'
data structures for particular types of physics
problems are similar to those of experts with
respect to objects, concepts, and terms;
however, experts' data structures contain many
more linkages. The more extensive data base,
which experts acquire as a result of their
greater exposure to formal instruction, is not
necessary for the successful solution of prob-
blems of the type on which the analysis of
expert-novice differences is based. However,
the additional links in experts' knowledge
structures are necessary for the successful and
efficient solution of mechanics problems.
We hypothesize that these links develop as a
result of extensive practice in problem solving
and that their development is facilitated by
verbal interactions. The professional activities
of physics experts require either verbal interac-
tions with others or the organization of physics
information for the purpose of communicating
it to others. We hypothesize that this type of
experience is important to schema change
because the individual must make explicit the
meaning attributed to technical terminology
and the rules for applications of proposition
and principles.
This analysis leads us to hypothesize that
providing beginning students with oppor-
tunities to engage in the quantitative analysis
of physics problems will facilitate the develop-
ment of physics knowledge organized in ways
that approximate that of the organization of a
physics expert. Our selection of this instruc-
tional strategy to attain this goal is based on
the recognition that: (a) The cognitive objec-
tives of beginning mechanics instruction
should approximate the skills and knowledge
applied by experts in the solution of mechanics
problems; (b) Explicit instruction in the
knowledge and skills required for successful
novice performance is not now a part of physics
instruction; and (c) Part of such instruction
must focus on producing a schema change in
students which results in the incorporation and
integration of mechanics principles and inter-
pretations of real-world phenomena.
We hypothesize that providing learners with
opportunities for verbal interactions will
facilitate the development of correct usage of
technical vocabulary and help students become
aware of the principles they apply in the
analysis of physical situations and how their
principles are different from those being
taught. This hypothesis is consistent with
cognitive theory of schema change.
Schema change theory. The processes by
which existing schemata are modified are just
beginning to be understood (Greeno, 1980).
Information processing models of schema
development generally have not gone beyond
the level of describing stages. Nonetheless,
several valuable ideas concerning the develop-
ment of schemata and suggestions for modify-
ing schemata have been offered.
Two principal mechanisms for schema
modification have been discussed by
Rumelhart and Ortony (1977). Each
mechanism is, in a sense, the antithesis of the
other. Specialization occurs in a schema when
one or more of its variables are fixed to form a
less abstract schema. Conversely, generaliza-
tion occurs in a schema when some fixed por-
tion is replaced by a variable to form a more
abstract schema. The generalization
mechanism can be applied in a
motion-of-
objects schema.
The typical uninstructed student has the
motion schema: A push produces motion. As a
result of appropriate instructional experiences,
the student's motion schema could become: A
force produces acceleration. The fixed portion,
push,
in the initial schema has been replaced
by a more general variable, force, which can
take on several values in addition to push.
Similarly, the general variable, acceleration,
which can have different values, has replaced
the initial schema's fixed portion, motion. The
modified schema is considerably more abstract
and, hence, should have a much broader range
of applicability.
The hypothesized generalization mecha-
nism only describes the changes and is, in fact,
not a mechanism for producing them. If, as
Rumelhart and Ortony (1977) imply, the gen-
eralization mechanism is a mechanism for pro-
ducing change, a reasonable implication is that
the modification of the motion-of-objects
schema might be accomplished quite simply
by describing to students the needed modifica-
tions in definitions of terms and restating sim-
ple propositions. Empirical evidence on
mechanics learning demonstrates that this in-
structional strategy is not generally effective
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
COGNITIVE RESEARCH AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN
41
and suggests that, while the gradual modifica-
tion of schemata doubtlessly involves general-
ization and specialization, in highly integrated
schemata more dramatic changes, amounting
essentially to a shift to a new paradigm (in
Kuhn's [1962] sense), must also take place.
To bring about schema change on such a
large scale, a dialectical process appears to be
necessary. Riegel (1973) points out that the
thinking of both adults and children is dialec-
tical, and he proposes that dialectics is "the
transformational key" in cognitive develop-
ment. Anderson (1977) suggests that "...
the likelihood of schema change is maximized
when a person recognizes a difficulty in his
current position and comes to see that the dif-
ficulty can be handled within a different
schema (p. 427)."
As the mechanism for promoting dialectics
in the classroom, Anderson advocates the use
of a Socratic teaching method. By participating
in the dialogues which occur in Socratic
teaching, the student is forced to deal with
counterexamples to proposals and to face con-
tradictions in his or her ideas. To overcome the
attacks of adversaries in the dialogues, the stu-
dent must construct a new framework of ideas
that will stand up to criticism. The newly con-
structed framework is, of course, a new
schema, so it may be said that schema change
has occurred as a result of the student's par-
ticipation in the dialogues.
Instructional Issues
Specification of Instructional
Objectives and Strategies on the
Basis of Cognitive Analysis
Table 2 summarizes instructional objectives
and strategies for mechanics instruction deriv-
ed from the analysis of the cognitive states of
uninstructed students, novices, and experts,
groups who differ with respect to (a) the quan-
tity and extent of formal mechanics instruc-
tion, (b) experience in solving mechanics pro-
blems, and (c) the extent of their verbal in-
teractions about mechanics.
Objectives. The objectives presented in
Table 2 are based on the analysis of contrasting
cognitive states and represent the first in a
series of steps in the detailed specification
of instructional objectives. These objectives
specify features of the ideal state which the
learner will approximate but do not detail how
far the learner will move along the continuum
from beginning student to expert as the result
of a particular course or sequence of instruc-
tion. Further refinement of the objectives for a
certain instructional sequence must take into
account many other factors, including the con-
tent the instruction will cover, the time avail-
able for instruction, and the age and academic
aptitude of the students for whom the instruc-
tion is intended. However, the analysis here il-
lustrates one important principle employed in
the cognitive approach to design. That princi-
ple is the comparison of the cognitive states of
individuals at different levels of competence
(Greeno, 1976). Furthermore, the design re-
quires that the initial specification of objec-
tives be based on the cognitive features that
distinguish the learner for whom the instruc-
tion is designed from individuals competent in
the field.
An observation worthy of comment is the
difference between these objectives based on
cognitive analysis and objectives that derive
from the logical analysis of the subject matter
or from the identification of to-be-learned
behaviors. Objectives derived from these two
processes are deficient in at least two respects.
First, cognitive analysis identifies significant
objectives not identified by either analysis of
behaviors or logical analysis. Second, logical
analysis does not identify the structural
organization of knowledge which the instruc-
tion should produce. For example, logical
analysis would not identify qualitative analysis
of physics problems as an objective of instruc-
tion, nor would it produce information about
the optimal structural organization of
mechanics knowledge for competent problem
solving.
Instructional
strategies.
The first approxima-
tion of instructional objectives derives from the
comparison of the cognitive states of unin-
structed students and experts. Possible instruc-
tional strategies for the attainment of the
objectives derive from comparisons of the cogni-
tive states of uninstructed students, novices,
and experts, and from examinations of the
mechanics-relevant experiences of novices and
experts. Having specified the differences in
cognitive states and relevant experiences we can
generate hypotheses for explaining the observed
differences in cognitive states on the basis of dif-
ferences in experiences. These hypotheses, in
turn suggest possible instructional strategies.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
PROPOSITIONS IN SCHEMATA
U - presence in schema of incorrect pro-
positions.
Example: motion implies force.
N - presence in schema of conflicting pro-
positions which are applied in different
situations.
Example: Motion implies force pro-
position applied in the analysis of
rèal-
world situations. Change in motion
implies force proposition applied in the
quantitative solution of physics problems.
E - propositions present in schema are in-
ternally consistent and widely applicable.
Example: Change of motion implies force
proposition is applied in analyzing all
pertinent problems.
Students apply change in motion implies
force proposition in real-world situations.
Students contrast implications of the dif-
ference in the two relationships between
force and motion expressed by the proposi-
tions (1) Motion implies force, and (2)
Change in motion implies force.
Instructional dialogue to change contents
of
mechanics
schema:
Provides opportunity
for students to (1) be explicit about the pro-
positions they assume in invoking the pre-
sence of forces in physical situation (For
example, U generally invokes forces only in
situations where there is motion.) and (2)
make explicit the relationship between
motion and force in the propositions they
use.
8
o
z
en
m
!»
S
Z
D
S
O
>
o
z
STRUCTURAL FEATURES
Concept integration of U and N is sparse,
with fewer links among concepts than for E.
Example: N - experientially derived
motion-of-objects schema is not inte-
grated,
or reconciled with Newtonian
mechanics schema.
Integration of representations in U and IM is
poor, while they are well-integrated in E.
Example: N representations of surface
features of physical situations are poorly
integrated with abstract representations
of physical situations; these, in
turn,
are
poorly integrated with propositions
which link canonical objects and physical
constructs used in abstract representa-
tions;
E - representations of surface
features of physical situations are inte-
grated both with abstract representations
of physical situations and with proposi-
tions linking the canonical objects and
constructs of abstract representations.
Students qualitatively analyze mechanics
problems:
(1) Produce an abstract representation of a
physical situation.
(2) Recognize that situations with very dif-
ferent surface features can have the same
abstract representation. (For an example,
see Appendix A where the situations of 6
problems have the same abstract repre-
sentation.)
(3) Recognize that the problems can be
solved by the application of the same
mechanics principle.
Qualitative
,analysis
of problems to change
structural features of mechanics schema:
Forges links between the physical situation,
its abstract representation using canonical
objects and mechanics constructs, and the
principles (Newton's second law, F = ma)
which link properties of the canonical
objects and constructs. Also forges links
between concepts
(e.g.,
between mass and
weight) to integrate them better, thereby
contributing to concept differentiation.
Table 2 (continued on next page)
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
Table 2 (continued)
PROBLEM SOLUTION STRATEGY
U solution strategy: search for a rule that
applies to the given situation.
Example: Problem of comparing speeds
of two falling objects evokes the rule
Heavy objects fall faster than lighter
objects.
N solution strategy: search for an equation
E solution strategy: qualitative analysis
Students engage in qualitative analysis
of physics problems before attempting
quantitative solutions.
Interactive dialogue: Demonstrates that the
same abstract representations and diagrams
derive from problems with different surface
features. Also demonstrates the usefulness
of a general principle for solving a large
number of problems with different surface
features.
Qualitative analysis of
mechanics
problems:
Provides practice in using the desired
strategy.
>
D
5»
3
o
>
"O
>
O
2
rn
K
o
S
O
>
O
ñ
S8
O
Z
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
COGNITIVE RESEARCH AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN
The potential for effectiveness of alternative
instructional strategies is then evaluated in
terms of relevant psychological theory.
The availability of empirical descriptions of
differences between uninstructed students and
novices is particularly important to the process
of selecting instructional strategies. Information
about these differences leads to the identifica-
tion of instructional strategies which are effec-
tive and those which are not effective or
counterproductive. The observation that
novices' cognitive contents resemble those of ex-
perts is an indication that the didactic method
of instruction is effective for imparting discrete
bits of information. However, the detailed
analysis of problem-solving behaviors of novices
suggests that their structural organization of in-
formation resulting from exposure to didactic
instruction is less than satisfactory.
In the case of mechanics, this analysis also
reveals that the problem-solving strategy taught
in physics textbooks is indeed demonstrated by
novices, but, as we discussed earlier, this
strategy does not produce certain desired links
in cognitive structure, specifically those between
physical situations and mechanics concepts.
This lack of structural integration is also
evidenced by the fact that many novices con-
tinue to apply non-Newtonian principles when
asked to analyze real-world situations.
The analysis of novice-expert differences is a
useful source of possible alternative instruc-
tional strategies. For example, the proposed in-
fluence of problem-solving and verbal interac-
tion in the development of correct and well-
integrated cognitive structures are derived
directly from cognitive difference analysis. This
interpretation of the differences is consistent
with cognitive theory and with educational
practice and philosophy. The cognitive analysis
provides an explicit causal link between the
strategy and the outcomes, thus making possi-
ble more convincing empirical tests of the effec-
tiveness of the strategy. Our assertion that
engaging in qualitative analysis of mechanics
problems will develop a better integration of
real-world situations and their abstract represen-
tation utilizing the concepts and symbols of
Newtonian physics is empirically testable.
Procedural Description of the
Proposed Instruction
Although the strategy of using dialogues in
instruction has been specified in relation to the
attainment of instructional objectives (Table
2),
we have not yet described how this strategy
is implemented. Illustrative procedures which
employ the strategy are outlined in this sec-
tion. In one mode of the dialogues strategy,
students engage in interactive dialogues with
each other.
First the students are presented with a set of
mechanics problems which require qualitative
answers. A typical set of six such problems is
shown at the right side of Appendix A. These
problems are qualitative restatements of pro-
blems from five different physics textbooks.
The physical situations or surface features of
these problems (in both the qualitative and
quantitative versions) are very different, but all
problems can be represented in the same
abstract form (diagram or verbal description
using mechanics concepts) and can be solved
using the same mechanics principle. Each stu-
dent produces a solution to each of the pro-
blems and then shares with the class the pro-
blem analysis, the solution of the problem,
and definitions of technical terms used in the
solution or the analysis. This procedure forces
students to be explicit about the idiosyncratic
meanings attributed to technical terms and the
principles and propositions that they apply in
the analysis of the problem. Each student can
contrast his or her solution strategy for a pro-
blem with the strategies presented by other
students.
When all of the problems in the set have
been considered by the class, the teacher will
present the physicists' analysis of the problem
by means of diagrams and verbal explanations
using the technical vocabulary of mechanics.
The expert analysis is based on the common
deep structure of the six problems, as shown in
Appendix A. The teacher will demonstrate
mat the abstract representation is the same for
each of the problems and that the same princi-
ple will produce a solution to all the problems
in the set. Then students will analyze their
solutions to the problems in light of the
physicists' solution and will specify how their
interpretations differ from that of the
physicists.
The teacher's presentation of the physicists'
analysis and solution of qualitative problems is
in the mode of an instructional dialogue. In
order to explain and illustrate this mode of the
dialogues strategy, we have analyzed physics
problems from introductory texts in the man-
ner shown in Appendix B. The general struc-
ture of the analysis is simple. Initially the
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
46
AUDREY B. CHAMPAGNE, LEOPOLD E. KLOPFER AND RICHARD F. GUNSTONE
textbook problem was rewritten as a
qualitative problem and subsidiary questions
were added asking about the assumptions and
physics principles used in obtaining the answer
to the problem. Then a realistic minimum
state in terms of relevant prior knowledge and
experience was selected, and a strategy for
working from that state to a successful solution
was worked out. At this stage, when we do not
have the insights to be derived from data, it is
assumed that other, more developed responses
can be accommodated by beginning at a later
point in this sample strategy.
The strategy has been outlined only. It in-
dicates a series of logical steps. Within each
step,
the essential concept(s) to be developed
and the purpose of the step in terms of the
problem solution are indicated. In some cases
a particular instructional methodology to be
used for a step is shown, while in the remain-
ing cases only instructional dialogue is an-
ticipated. For each the rationale for the pro-
cedure is described, referring to the techniques
identified by Collins (1977) and Collins and
Stevens (1981) when appropriate. A strategy
for handling a correct answer to the questions
asked so as to develop a solution strategy for
the problem is also given.
Concluding Remarks
In this article we have recounted how our in-
terest in a particular instructional problem in
introductory physics, students' difficulty in
learning mechanics, provided the occasion for
utilizing Simon's characterization of a science
of design as a guide in proceeding to design an
instructional strategy that can be used to help
students learn mechanics effectively. We have
shown how the theory and empirical findings
of cognitive science and the cognitive psychol-
ogist's analytical tools and procedures were
brought to bear on every stage of the design
process. We have sought to make explicit the
particular principles of instructional design
which both evolved and were applied in the
course of the inquiry. We suspect that these in-
structional design principles may be applied in
various school subject-matter domains, though
only their application in physics was illustrated
here.
The instructional strategy for guiding
uninstructed physics students in their learning
of Newtonian mechanics is now available, but
our inquiry is not at an end. We are now
preparing to investigate empirically several
issues which were raised during the process of
designing the instructional strategy. The major
hypothesis to be tested in our proposed
research is that engaging uninstructed physics
students in instructional dialogues focused on
the qualitative analysis of mechanics problems
will produce changes in the students' motion-
of-objects schemata. A further hypothesis is
that, after completion of the specified instruc-
tion, the students' cognitive state will approx-
imate significant features which are
characteristic of the cognitive state of a physics
expert.
We can admire the great psychologists of
earlier days, such as Charles H. Judd, whom
we quoted at the start of the paper, for their
keen perspective on students' learning of
abstruse school subjects like physics. Judd's in-
sight (or perhaps, intuition) seems so right and
true that we cannot help being amazed. Also
amazing is the realization that the situation
which Judd described seven decades ago still
rings true for physics textbooks and physics
learning today. Hardly anything seems to have
changed in the interim. Why is this so?
The main reason, we believe, is that,
although Judd recognized the problem, he
could not prescribe an effective solution.
Because he was unable to describe the problem
precisely, Judd had no basis on which to
evaluate the probability of the effectiveness of
possible instructional strategies. Today the
status is different. Empirical and theoretical
research in cognitive psychology make possible
the construction of theoretical models, on
which predictions can be based. The applica-
tion of a model of understanding of physical
phenomena leads to detailed specification of a
strategy for beginning physics instruction
which can be expected to produce desired
changes in students. The difference between
our present possibilities and those of Judd's
day is demonstrated in this article.
Reference Notes
1.
Larkin, J. H. Skill acquisition for solving physics pro-
blems. Paper presented at the meeting of the
Psychonomic Society, Phoenix, November 1979.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
COGNITIVE RESEARCH AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN
47
References
Anderson, J. R. Language, memory, and thought. New
York: Erlbaum, 1976.
Anderson, R. C. The notion of schemata and the educa-
tional enterprise: General discussion of the conference.
In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro, & W. E. Montague
(Eds.),
Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1977,
415-431.
Anderson, R. C., Reynolds, R. E., Schallen, D. L.,
& Goetz, E. T. Frameworks for comprehending
discourse. American Educational Research Journal,
1977,
14,
367-381.
Arons, A. B. Cultivating the capacity for formal reasoning:
Objectives and procedures in an introductory physical
science course. American Journal of Physics, 1976, 44,
834-838.
Atkins, K. R. Physics. New York: John Wiley & Sons,
1965.
Ausubel, D. P., Novak, J. D., & Hanesian, H. Educa-
tional psychology: A cognitive view (2nd ed.). New
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1978.
Bransford, J. D., & McCarrell, N. S. A sketch of a cogni-
tive approach to comprehension. In W. R. Weimer
and D. S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic
processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1974.
Brumby, M. N. Students' perceptions of the concept of
life.
Science Education, 1982, in press.
Champagne, A. B., Klopfer, L. E., & Anderson, J. H.
Factors influencing the learning of classical mechanics.
American Journal of
Physics,
1980, 45, 1074-1079.
Champagne, A. B., Kloper, L. E., Solomon, C. A., &
Cahn, A. D. Interactions of students' knowledge with
their comprehension and design of science ex-
periments. LRDC Publication 1980/9, Pittsburgh:
University of Pittsburgh, Learning Research and
Development Center, 1980.
Chi,
M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. Categoriza-
tion and representation of physics problems by experts
and novices. Cognitive Science, 1981, 5, 121-152.
Clement, J. Mapping a student's causal conceptions from a
problem solving protocol. In: J. Lockhead & J.
Clement (Eds.), Cognitive process instruction.
Philadelphia: Franklin Institute Press, 1979.
Cole,
H., & Raven R. Principle learning as a function of
instruction on excluding irrelevant variables. Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 1969, 6,
234-241.
Collins, H., & Raven, R. Principle learning as a function
of instruction on excluding irrelevant variables. Journal
of
Research
in Science Teaching, 1969, 6,
234-241.
Collins, A., & Stevens, A. L. Goal and strategies of inquiry
teachers. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional
psychology (Vol. II). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum,
1981.
Driver, R. The representation of conceptual framewords in
young adolescent science students. Unpublished doc-
toral dissertation. University of Illinois, 1973.
Driver, R., & Easley, J. Pupils and paradigms: A review of
literature related to concept development in adolescent
science students. Studies in Science Education, 1978,
5, 61-84.
Fleshner, E. A. The mastery by children of some concepts
in physics. In B.
&
J. Simon, Educational psychology in
the
U.S.S.R.
Routledge & Kegan, 1963.
Gerson, R. & Primrose, R. Results of a remedial laboratory
program based on a Piaget model for engineering and
science freshmen. American Journal of
Physics,
1977,
45,
649-651.
Glaser, R. Components of a psychology of instruction:
Toward a science of design. Review of Educational
Research, 1976, 46, 1-24.
Green, B., McCloskey, M., & Caramazza, A. Curvilinear
motion in the absence of external forces: Naive beliefs
about the motion of objects. Science, 1980, 210,
1139-1141.
Greeno, J. G. Cognitive objectives of instruction: Theory
of knowledge for solving problems and answering
questions. In D. Klahr (Ed.), Cognition and instruc-
tion.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1976.
Greeno, J. G. Nature of problem-solving abilities. In
W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive
processes (Vol 5). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1978.
Greeno, J. G. Psychology of learning, 1960-1980: One
participant's observations. American Psychologist,
1980,
35, 713-728.
Gunstone, R. F. Structural outcomes of physics instruc-
tion.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Monash
University, Melbourne, Australia, 1980.
Gunstone, R. & White, R. Understanding of gravity.
Science Education, 1981, 65, 291-300.
Haber-Schaim, U., Gross, J. V., Dodge, J. H., &
Walter, J. A.
PSSC
Physics (4th ed.). Lexington: D. C.
Heath and Company, 1976.
Halley, J. W., & Eaton, B. A course in physics of human
motion. American Journal of Physics, 1974, 42,
820-829.
Hudson, H. T., & Mclntire, W. R. Correlation between
mathematical skills and success in physics. American
Journal of
Physics,
1977, 45,
470-471.
Hulsizer, R. J., & Lazarus, D. The word of physics.
California: Addison-Wesley, 1972.
Judd, C. H. Psychology of high school subjects. Boston:
Ginn & Co., 1915.
Kintsch, W. The representation of meaning in memory.
New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1974.
Kolody, G. O. How students learn velocity and accelera-
tion. Journal of College Science Teaching, 1977, 6,
224-227.
Kuhn, T. S. Structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago:
University of Chicago, 1962.
Leboutet-Barrell, L. Concepts of mechanics in young
people. Physics Education, 1976, 11, 462-466.
Lindsay, P. H., & Norman D. A. An introduction to
psychology. New York: Academic Press, 1972.
Miller, F., Dillon, T. J., & Smith, M. K. Concepts in
physics (2nd ed.). New York: Harcourt, Brace &
Jovanavich, 1974.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. Human problem solving.
Englewood
Cliff,
NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1972.
Norman, D. A., & Rumelhart, D. E. Explorations in cog-
nition. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1975.
Quillian, M. R. Semantic memory. In M. Minsky (Ed.),
Semantic information processing. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press, 1968.
Renner, J. W., Grant, R. M., & Sutherland, J. Content
and concrete thought. Science Education, 1978, 62,
215-221.
Riegel, K. F. Dialectic operations: The final period of cog-
nitive development. Human Development, 1973, 16,
346-370.
Rowell, J. A., & Dawson, C. J. Teaching about floating
and sinking: An attempt to link cognitive psychology
with classroom practice. Science Education, 1977, 61,
243-251.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
48 AUDREY B. CHAMPAGNE, LEOPOLD E. KLOPFER AND RICHARD F. GUNSTONE
Rumelhart, D. E., & Ortony, A. The representation of
knowledge in memory. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro,
& W. J. Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisi-
tion of knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1977.
Schank, R. C. Conceptual dependency: A theory of
natural language understanding. Cognitive
Psychology, 1972, 3, 552-631.
Sclmart, R. L., Krupa, M. P., Stone, C. R., &
Jaquette, D. S. Concrete operational thought and the
emergence of the concept of unseen force in children's
theories of electromagnctism and gravity. Science
Education, 1982, in press.
Simon, H. A. The sciences of the artificial (2nd ed.).
Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press, 1981.
Singer, B., & Benassi, V. A. Occult beliefs: Media distor-
tions,
social uncertainty, and deficiencies of human
reasoning seem to be at the basis of occult beliefs.
American Scientist, 1981, 69, 49-55.
Smith, A. W., & Cooper, J. N. Elements of physics. New
York: McGraw-Hill, 1979.
Viennot, L. Spontaneous reasoning in elementary dyna-
mics.
European
Journal of Science Education, 1979, 1,
205-221.
Winogard, T. Understanding natural language. Cog-
nitive Psychology, 1972, 3, 1-191.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
Appendix A
Six Physics Problems with Different Surface Features and the Same Deep Structure
QUANTITATIVE VERSION
QUALITATIVE VERSION
1.
Boy and Wagon Problem
A boy of mass 20 kg is standing in a wagon of mass 10kg. The
boy jumps off to the right with a speed of 20 m/sc. What happens
to the wagon? (Ignore friction.)
(Hulsizer & Lazarus, 1972, p. 187)
2.
Boy and Raft Problem
A 50 kilogram boy is standing on a 500 kilogram raft floating on
a lake. The raft is at rest. It can move on the surface of the lake
with negligible friction. Starting from rest, the boy begins to walk
with constant speed 1 meter/sec (relative to ground) and con-
tinues to walk for 20 seconds. How far does the raft move in
this time?
(Smith & Cooper, 1979, p. 152)
3. Rifle and Bullet Problem
A 3-g bullet is fired from a 2.4-kg rifle with a velocity of 360 m/s
north.
Find the momentum of the bullet and the recoil velocity
of the rifle, assuming that no other bodies are involved.
(Smith & Cooper, 1979, p. 93)
4. Skaters Problem
Two skaters are stationary in the center of a circular rink. They
then push on one another so that they fly apart. One of the
skaters has a mass of 90 kg and acquires an initial velocity of 0.8
m. sec. If the other skater has a mass of 75 kg, what is his initial
velocity?
(Atkins, 1965. p. 119)
A boy is standing in a wagon. The boy jumps off one end of the
wagon.
Ignoring friction, describe the motion of the wagon.
How does the velocity of the wagon compare with the velocity
of the boy?
A boy is standing on a floating raft on a lake. The raft is at rest. It
can move on the surface of the lake with negligible friction.
Standing from rest, the boy begins to walk with constant speed
towards the shore. Describe the motion of the raft. How does the
speed and direction of motion of the raft compare with the speed
and direction of the boy?
A bullet is fired from a rifle. Describe the motion of the rifle.
How does the velocity of the rifle compare with the velocity of
the bullet?
Two skaters are stationary in the center of a circular rink. The
skaters push on each other. Describe the motion of each skater.
How do their velocities compare?
i
3
i
i
o
Z
(5
a
E3
o
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
Appendix
A
(continued)
5. Carts
and
Spring Problem
Two heavy frictionless carts
are at
rest. They
are
held together
by
a loop
of
string.
A
light spring
is
compressed between them
(see
drawing). When
the
string
is
burned,
the
spring expands from
2.0
cm
to 3.0 cm, and the
carts move apart. Both
hit the
bumpers
fixed
to the
table
at the
same instant,
but cart
A
moved
0.45
meter while
cart
B
moved
0.87
meter. What
is the
ratio
of:
(a)
The
speed
of A to
that
of B
after
the
interaction?
(b) their masses?
(Haber-Schaim
et al.,
1976,
p. 321)
6. Compressed Spring Problem
Two objects
of
mass
m^ and
IVQ
are
held together
by a
strong
light thread,
and are
also acted
on by a
light spring that
is com-
pressed
as
shown
in the
figure. When
the
restraining thread
is
broken,
the two
objects
fly
apart with
velocities
I/A
and + i/o. Use the law
of conservation
of
momentum
to
solve
for
the
ratio
of the
velocities,
V^/VQ.
(Miller, Dillon,
&
Smith, 1974,
p. 112)
Two heavy frictionless carts
are at
rest. They
are
held together
by
a loop
of
string.
A
light spring
is
compressed between them.
The
string
is
burned
and the
spring expands. Describe
the
motion
of
the carts.
How
does
the
velocity
of
cart
A
compare with
the
velocity
of
cart
B?
Two objects
of
mass
m^ and
m^are held together
by a
strong
light thread,
and are
also acted
on by a
light spring that
is com-
pressed.
When
the
restraining thread
is
borken,
the two
objects
fly apart with velocities
v^ and
+yg.
Use the law of
conservation
of
momentum
to
solve
for the
ratio
of
the velocities,
V^/VQ.
F
m
1
PHYSICIST'S ANALYSIS
p Forces
of
equal magnitude
and
opposite
in
direction
are
^*" exerted
on two
unequal masses
at
rest.
How do the
veloc-
ities
of the
masses compare?
How do the
displacements
of
the masses compare?
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
COGNITIVE RESEARCH
AND
INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN
51
Appendix B
Example of Proposed Instructional Dialogue The Gun and Bullet Problem
Original Problem
"
1. A gun has a mass of 2.00 kg. It fires a bullet of mass 0.005 kg towards the right with a speed of
500 m/sec. How does the gun recoil? (Give its speed and direction of motion.)"
(Hulsizer & Lazarus, 1972. p. 187)
Problem Restated
in
Qualitative Form
When
a gun is
fired,
the
bullet leaves
the gun
with some speed.
How
does
the
bullet's speed
at the
muzzte
of the gun
compare with
the
gun's speed
at
that time?
How
does
the
direction
of the
bullet's
motion compare with
the
direction
of the
gun's motion?
Subsidiary questions:
(i)
What assumptions
did you
make
to
arrive
at
your answers?
(ii) What principles
of
physics/laws
of
motion
did you
apply
to the
situation
in coming
to
your answers?
Knowledge/skills assumed
in
following outlines
of
Instructional Dialogue strategies:
1.
"Physics" knowledge: it is assumed that students have completed a study of kinematics.
2.
General (or "world") knowledge: Awareness of medieval cannons, rifles, handguns (see
step A1 below).
Strategy A Outline of strategy to be used for responses to the qualitative problem of the form
"don't know" or "gun doesn't move."
Steps in the Strategy Purpose of Steps
Commentary on Steps
AI (a). Given scale drawings
of a small pistol, rifle, small
mortar, medieval cannon and
shells/bullets fired by each,
student is asked to match the
guns and shells/bullets.
A1(b). Ask student why
matchings were made.
A2.
Ask the student why
the bullet/shell comes out of
the gun when the gun is
fired.
A3.
Show the student a
drawing of a metal tube,
closed at one end and with a
lighted fire cracker placed
on it.
A1(a) and (b). To establish
that, in the real world, mass/
weight of gun ^> mass/weight
of bullet or shell.
Asked what will happen when
the fire cracker goes off.
To establish the role of ex-
plosion in this phenomenon.
If this notion is present, go
to A4; if not, go to A3.
To establish the effect of ex-
plosion on the mass in the
tube,
i.e., the cracker. If this
exercise does not establish
the notion, move further to
considering a medieval can-
non where the explosive and
propelled object are separate.
Collins (1977) has proposed a
series of production rules for
this form of instructional dia-
logue.
Strategy A1(a) is an
example of Rule 1: . Ask
about a known case. In his
subsequent reorganization of
these rules (Collins & Stevens,
1981),
this is an example
of Case Selection Strategy 1:
Pick a positive exemplar for
a set of factors.
Collins (1977) Rule 2: Ask
for any factors.
8
Collins (1977) Rule 2 (see
above).
Collins (1977) Rule 3: Ask
for intermediate factors. For
some students this will also
be a prompt to recall a rele-
vant previous experience,
relevant existing world know-
ledge.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
52
AUDREY B. CHAMPAGNE, LEOPOLD E. KLOPFER AND RICHARD F. GUNSTONE
Appendix B (continued)
Steps in the Strategy
Purpose of Steps
Commentary on Steps
A4.
Using two labor-
atory trolleys, one containing
a spring plunger (PSSC), and
a number of bricks.
(a) have students exper-
iment qualitatively with the
effect of placing two carts
with loaded spring between
and releasing the spring,
with varying masses on the
carts,
(b) have students exper-
iment with one cart carrying
various masses placed with
loaded .spring against their
hand and then released.
A5.
By drawing on A1-
A4,
assist the student to
establish (and, if appropriate,
to link to relevant existing
knowledge/experience) :
) that in the case of the
exploding carts, the ex-
panding spring exerts forces
on the carts placed at either
end of the spring, and that
these two forces are equal in
magnitude.
(ii) that, in the case of the
exploding carts, equal forces
from the spring acting on
carts of different mass result
in different cart velocities
after explosion,
(iii) that, in the case of ex-
ploding carts, when one cart
has a mass considerably lar-
gar than the other this situa-
tion is in some ways
anal-
ogous to a gun firing a
bullet. (Of the limitations to
the analogy, the most im-
portant to be drJwn out
here is that the relative mass
differences for gun and
bullet are much greater than
for the carts.)
A6.
Return to qualitative
problem.
After successful
solution of the problem as
asked,
use strategy B below if
appropriate.
To establish the separation of
all masses involved in an ex-
plosion;
to establish that
smaller mass pieces move
more quickly than larger mass
pieces.
To establish that a mass ex-
ploded away from a
"rigid"
body results in a force on
that rigid body.
To establish the generaliz-
ations needed in order to be
able to analyze and solve the
original qualitative problem.
Direct observation
Direct observation
This step is similar to Step A1
(b) (see Footnote a) in that
the production rules to be
applied will vary from subject
to subject, depending both on
each individual's existing
knowledge and beliefs, and
on each individual's interpre-
tations of steps A1 to A4.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
COGNITIVE RESEARCH AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN
Appendix B (continued)
Strategy B Outline of strategy to be used for responses to the qualitative problem of the form "speed
of bullet very much greater than speed of gun and in opposite direction"
(i.e.,
correct answer to
questions asked).
Steps in the Strategy Purpose of Steps
Commentary on Steps
B1 Ask student if they can
be more precise about
the relative values of the
bullet and gun speeds.
B2 Repeat subsidiary ques-
tions (i) and (ii) from the
qualitative problem.
To establish that the ratio
of speeds is the inverse ratio
of the weights/masses in-
volved.
If this is not forth-
coming,
go to strategy A,
beginning at A4. If some such
statement is produced, go to
B2.
To elicit a statement of
Newton's Third Law and to
have the student authorita-
tively link this law to the
gun/bullet phenomenon.
Collins (1977) Rule 4:
for prior factors.
Ask
See commentary on Step A5.
Numbers of other rules
(e.g..
Rules 3-11) may be applied in the exploration of individual re-
sponses. For example, it may be necessary to ask "Could the cannon ball be fired from the
pistol?"
(Rule 6: Pick a counter example for an insufficient factor.)
Previous experience suggests that a physical mark
(e.g.,
a chalk line) needs to be made to indicate
the position of carts before the explosion so that a reference is available for considering
post-explosion effects.
If friction effects provide an interfering concept, move further by using an air track for explosions
and then returning to trolleys.
Downloaded by [Monash University Library] at 02:15 10 February 2014
... This article only considers predicting as a strategy for simple declarative learning (e.g., facts, definitions). It does not include predicting as a strategy for inducing complex conceptual change, for which it is popular as well (e.g., Champagne et al., 1982;Hardy et al., 2006). It is also restricted to direct, task-specific mnemonic effects of predicting. ...
... Predicting already forms part of science teaching curricula, such as in the widely used "predict-observe-explain" curriculum (Champagne et al., 1982;Liew & Treagust, 1995). The psychological mechanisms by which these curricula work have remained opaque, however. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article attempts to delineate the procedural and mechanistic characteristics of predicting as a learning strategy. While asking students to generate a prediction before presenting the correct answer has long been a popular learning strategy, the exact mechanisms by which it improves learning are only beginning to be unraveled. Moreover, predicting shares many features with other retrieval-based learning strategies (e.g., practice testing, pretesting, guessing), which begs the question of whether there is more to it than getting students to engage in active retrieval. I argue that active retrieval as such does not suffice to explain beneficial effects of predicting. Rather, the effectiveness of predicting is also linked to changes in the way the ensuing feedback is processed. Initial evidence suggests that predicting boosts surprise about unexpected answers, which leads to enhanced attention to the correct answer and strengthens its encoding. I propose that it is this affective aspect of predicting that sets it apart from other retrieval-based learning strategies, particularly from guessing. Predicting should thus be considered as a learning strategy in its own right. Studying its unique effects on student learning promises to bring together research on formal models of learning from prediction error, epistemic emotions, and instructional design.
... However, learners are not the "blank slate" as John Locke described (Baird, 2011), rather, they often enter learning scenarios with prior conceptions (Duit & Treagust, 2003). The prior conceptions that are not consistent with scientific consensus are referred to as misconceptions or alternative conceptions (Champagne et al., 1982;Nieswandt, 2001) and can make it more difficult for learners to acquire correct scientific facts (Sinatra & Pintrich, 2003). The process of re-aligning or reorganizing an individual's current conceptual understanding is referred to as conceptual change (Posner et al., 1982), and has been an ardent topic in educational research for decades (Kang et al., 2010). ...
Article
As educators, we often seek to engage students’ prior knowledge to help them learn new and potentially difficult science content. However, sometimes our experiences with the world lead us to create misconceptions that run counter to the scientific consensus. Refutational texts have been shown to be more effective at changing individuals’ misconceptions compared to traditional (i.e., expository) texts. Sometimes, conceptual change materials might include irrelevant but interesting pictures geared toward increasing engagement or positive affect while learning. These are referred to as seductive pictures, and although they are intended to promote the situational interest of readers, they may hinder learning since they might cause cognitive overload. The current study sought to investigate the effects of text structure and seductive pictures on individuals’ knowledge of genetically modified foods, situational interest, and cognitive loads. We found significant learning advantages for the refutational text groups and significant disadvantages for the seductive pictures groups. Additionally, the individuals who were exposed to seductive pictures demonstrated higher levels of extraneous cognitive load than those who were not. Implications and limitations of the present study are discussed.
... Asking learners to generate predictions is a popular method for improving children's learning. Studies investigating prediction generation (or "hypothesis generation") in children tend to find that explicitly predicting an outcome before seeing it improves learning (e.g., of physics; [53][54][55]). The benefits of making predictions have been connected to successful activation of prior knowledge when learning new material, but less is known about the specific mechanisms by which predicting affects learning success, in particular when it comes to theory revision [56]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Bayesian models allow us to investigate children’s belief revision alongside physiological states, such as “surprise”. Recent work finds that pupil dilation (or the “pupillary surprise response”) following expectancy violations is predictive of belief revision. How can probabilistic models inform the interpretations of “surprise”? Shannon Information considers the likelihood of an observed event, given prior beliefs, and suggests stronger surprise occurs following unlikely events. In contrast, Kullback–Leibler divergence considers the dissimilarity between prior beliefs and updated beliefs following observations—with greater surprise indicating more change between belief states to accommodate information. To assess these accounts under different learning contexts, we use Bayesian models that compare these computational measures of “surprise” to contexts where children are asked to either predict or evaluate the same evidence during a water displacement task. We find correlations between the computed Kullback–Leibler divergence and the children’s pupillometric responses only when the children actively make predictions, and no correlation between Shannon Information and pupillometry. This suggests that when children attend to their beliefs and make predictions, pupillary responses may signal the degree of divergence between a child’s current beliefs and the updated, more accommodating beliefs.
... Asking learners to generate predictions is a popular method for improving children's learning. Studies investigating prediction generation (or "hypothesis generation") in children tend to find that explicitly predicting an outcome before seeing it improves learning (e.g., of physics; [53][54][55]). The benefits of making predictions have been connected to successful activation of prior knowledge when learning new material, but less is known about the specific mechanisms by which predicting affects learning success, in particular when it comes to theory revision [56]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Bayesian models allow us to investigate children’s belief revision alongside physiological states like “surprise”. Recent work finds that pupil dilation (or the “pupillary surprise response”) following expectancy-violations may be predictive of belief revision. How can probabilistic models inform interpretations of “surprise”? Shannon Information considers the likelihood of an observed event, given prior beliefs – suggesting stronger surprise occurs following unlikely events. In contrast, Kullback-Leibler divergence considers the “dissimilarity” between prior beliefs and updated beliefs following observations – with greater surprise indicating more change between belief states to accommodate information. To assess these accounts under different learning contexts, we use Bayesian models that compare these computational measures of “surprise” to contexts where children are asked to either predict or to evaluate the same evidence during a water displacement task. We find correlations between the computed Kullback-Leibler divergence and children’s pupillometry responses only when children actively make predictions, and no correlation between Shannon Information and pupillometry. This suggests that when children attend to their beliefs and make predictions, pupillary responses may signal the degree of divergence between a child’s current beliefs and updated, more accommodating beliefs.
... Since learning is a process of knowledge construction undertaken by an individual, new knowledge is elaborated and/or reconstructed from this basis [4]. The importance of the situation in which the knowledge is introduced is emphasized because relations between prior and new knowledge elements are constructed according to student's "overall understanding of the situation" [5]. The lack of perceived coherence of the structure of physics knowledge is among the principal causes of student failure to achieve conceptual understanding in physics [6]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The sequence of representations used to introduce new knowledge and the context in which this is done is critical to both effectively build on prior knowledge and establish conceptual coherence in physics. Ongoing reports indicate that current teaching activities fail address student difficulties to obtain conceptual understanding and-coherence between relevant physics representations and concepts. There is a need for fine-grained topic specific multiple representational teaching activities to enhance coherence in physics knowledge. In this paper we present a teaching activity for undergraduate physics, built on using motion diagrams to specifically relate the concepts and representations in kinematics, in particular for the concept of free fall. The teaching activity follows a conceptual qualitative approach to teaching kinematics concepts and is informed by the results of a broader Design Based Research study. In the teaching activity the concept of acceleration is introduced qualitatively as the net force to mass ratio followed by drawing motion diagrams to visualize the motion of the objects in free fall. The motion diagrams are implemented to support conceptual qualitative interpretation of mathematical representations such as graphs and equations, to enhance transfer of knowledge between representations and coherence between relevant concepts and representations. The contribution of this research lies in presenting an argument for a conceptual qualitative approach to teaching introductory mechanics and introducing a teaching activity based on the value offered of including motion diagrams in the teaching activity to enhance coherence between physics representations and between physics concepts.
Article
Full-text available
Misconception is a problem that is often found in learning biology concepts. Ecosystem concept is one of the biology concepts in which misconceptions are found a lot. As a learning problem, misconceptions need to be addressed as soon as possible. The purpose of this research is to produce a valid and reliable diagnostic test instrument for misconceptions in the form of a three-tier multiple choice test which is presented through Google Form platform. This development research applied the research and development method with a 4D development model. The developed diagnostic test instrument was validated by two expert validators and obtained a mean score of 3.27 (very feasible). Through the results of item validation, from 40 questions there are 30 valid items with an average correlation coefficient value of 0.67 and 10 invalid items with an average correlation coefficient value of 0.10. The diagnostic test instrument was also tested for readability to students and got an average value of 3.41 (very feasible) and was tested for readability to teachers and got an average value of 3.74 (very feasible). This shows that the developed product is very valid and reliable to be used as a diagnostic test instrument for misconceptions. Keywords: 4D Biology Ecosystem Misconception Three-tier multiple choice test This is an open access article under the CC BY-SA license.
Chapter
Full-text available
Die Bedeutsamkeit von Beispielen für das Lernen neuer (physikalischer) Konzepte ist lerntheoretisch Konsens (Fischer et al. 2003). Richtig eingesetzt, sind Beispiele in der Lage, die Abstraktheit bzw. Komplexität physikalischer Inhalte zu verringern, indem sie Anknüpfungspunkte an die Erfahrungen und somit an das bestehende Wissensnetz der Lernenden bieten (Kalyuga et al. 2010, Kircher et al. 2015, Schecker et al. 2018). Gleichzeitig ermöglichen Beispiele es durch ihre Verankerung in konkreten Kontexten einen für die Schüler*innen relevanten und interessanten Lebensweltbezug herzustellen (Muckenfuß 1995, Habig et al. 2018). Der Einsatz von Beispielen unterstützt so sowohl den kognitiven als auch affektiv-emotionalen Verstehensprozess. Aufgrund dieser Eigenschaften scheinen Beispiele gerade auch für Lernende mit geringerem Vorwissen von hohem Nutzen zu sein. Bezüglich einer optimalen Positionierung des Beispiels (vor oder nach der Regel) beim (Physik)Lernen gibt es jedoch sich entgegenstehende Befunde. Kulgemeyer (2018) spricht sich für die Reihenfolge Regel-Beispiel zum Erlernen von deklarativen Wissen (Faktenwissen) aus und verweist dabei auf Seidel et al. (2013), Champagne et al. (1982) und Korthagen & Kessels (1999), die alle Effekte zugunsten dieser Reihenfolge feststellen konnten. Demgegenüber steht der Ansatz der Basismodelltheorie (Oser & Baeriswyl 2001), der die Strukturabfolge (prototypisches) Beispiel - Konzept bei der Begriffs- bzw. Konzeptbildung vorsieht. Diese Vorgabe gründet sich v.a. auf den lerntheoretischen bzw. lernpsychologischen Ausarbeitungen von Aebli (1981; 1983) und Smith & Medin (1981) und beschreibt, dass durch das geführte Durcharbeiten eines einzigen – für das Konzept prototypische – Beispiel wesentliche Züge des Konzeptes aufgezeigt werden können, die dann in den vernetzten und verallgemeinerten Konzeptbegriff münden. Zwischen diese beiden Ansätze stellen sich die Studien von Kokkonen et al. (2021) sowie van Hout und Mettes (1976), die den Einfluss der Strukturabfolge im physikalischen bzw. ingenieurstechnischen Kontext überprüft haben und keine Effekte zugunsten der einen oder anderen Abfolge feststellen konnten. Aufgrund der uneindeutigen Forschungslage bezüglich der Reihenfolge der Strukturierungselemente in Bezug auf das Erlernen physikalischer Konzepte stellen wir die Forschungsfrage auf: Welchen Einfluss hat die Reihenfolge der Strukturierungsschritte „prototypisches Beispiel“ und „Regel/Konzept“ auf den Lernerfolg? Wir gehen davon aus, dass die Reihenfolge der Strukturierungsschritte sich auf den Lernerfolg der Lernenden auswirkt. Dabei nehmen wir an, dass das Vorwissen als moderierende Variable agiert, wobei die Richtung der Wirkung offen bleibt.
Article
Full-text available
Thirty physical education students and 30 music education students read a passage that could be given either a prison break or a wrestling interpretation, and another passage that could be understood in terms of an evening of card playing or a rehearsal session of a woodwind ensemble. Scores on disambiguating multiple choice tests and theme-revealing disambiguations and intrusions in free recall showed striking relationships to the subject’s background. These results indicate that high-level schemata provide the interpretative framework for comprehending discourse. The fact that most subjects gave each passage one distinct interpretation or another and reported being unaware of other perspectives while reading suggest that schemata can cause a person to see a message in a certain way, without even considering alternative interpretations.
Book
Scitation is the online home of leading journals and conference proceedings from AIP Publishing and AIP Member Societies
Article
INTRODUCTION The problem of how to describe t.he structure of B student's current concep­ tions in I!l given area is 8 fundamental one for researchers who Beek to develop 8 theoretical foundation for cognitively oriented instruction . Recent develop­ ments in the physics leaching community, for elample, have emphasized cogni­ lively oriented approaches to teaching and the need for understanding the cognitive processes tbat underlie one's ability to "do" physics. Ideally, we would Ilke to have 8 picture of the kinds of knowledge structures and reasoning processes that are present in beginning students and that ate present in experts. Detailed descriptions of experts' knowledge structures, including those tacit knowledge structures not represented explicitly in the curriculu.m, would pre· sumably belp to define more clearly what is to be learned by the student. Detailed descriptions of beginning students' preconceptions and misconceptions would have value not only 8S a sophisticated evaluation tool, but would also make it more possible to take ccmroon preconceptions and misconceptions into 8.ccounl during instruction. This paper attempts to show that it is possible to st.udy systematically certain types of beginning students' conceptions in physics; specifically, causal concep· tions in mechanics. The paper examines the conceptions 8 freshman student uses to understand a simple physicol system involving the horiz.ontal motion of 8 cart launched scross 8 table, The task given to the student does Dot ask him to find 8. long series of adions which will solve a problem. Rather he is Bsked for a prediction and explanation of the effects on the system resulting from a single action. Protocols of 8uch explanations are particularly interesting because they tend not to be limited to formal, deductive arguments, but to include informal arguments that reflect the structuring of the subject's physical intuitions. The methodology used in this study involves two phases: obtaining problem· solving protocols via taped interviews, and analyzing these protocols to produce a model of the conceptions that Wlderlie the student's responses in the in. krv.iew. Several considerations are important t.o the success of this U!chnique. An Import.ant consideration daring the interview process is the attempt to en.
Article
The study described in this paper investigates the combined effect of certain variables on student achievement in classical mechanics. Our purposes are (1) to describe the preinstructional knowledge of mechanics, mathematical skills, and reasoning skills of a sample of college physics students, (2) to correlate these variables with the student's success in learning classical mechanics, and (3) to develop an hypothesis about the relationships between these variables and student achievement in mechanics. We also offer some proposals for improving the teaching of classical mechanics.
Article
A pre-course test of algebraic and trigonometric skills has been correlated with performance in introductory physics without calculus. For 200 students in two sections it was found that mathematical abilities at the beginning of the course weakly correlate with performance in the course. A repeat of the test at the end of the course indicates that mathematical skills alone are not sufficient to guarantee success in physics, but that unless the student has the mathematical skills, the performance in the physics will be poor.
Article
Experience in a physical science course for preservice elementary teachers, inservice elementary teachers, and other nonscience majors has led to the identification of certain specific factors and procedures that seem to assist the attainment of the formal operational level of intellectual development on the part of students who are initially at a concrete or transitional stage.
Article
A course in elementary mechanics especially designed for students of athletics and dance is described. The course has been taught for two years at the University of Minnesota. Innovative features include a gymnasium laboratory utilizing an inexpensive photographic technique for analysis of human motion. The order of presentation and the selection of topics, as well as the illustrative examples, are all unconventional in an elementary mechanics course. Student response is described. © 1975, American Association of Physics Teachers. All rights reserved.