ArticlePDF Available

Religious Zeal and the Uncertain Self

Authors:

Abstract

In two studies, personal uncertainty threats caused compensatory religious zeal. In Study 1 an academic uncertainty manipulation heightened conviction for religious beliefs and support for religious warfare. In Study 2 a relationship uncertainty manipulation caused non-Muslim's to derogate Islam. Together, these findings demonstrate that two aspects of religious zeal—conviction for one's own beliefs and derogation of others'—are caused by personal uncertainty.
This article was downloaded by:[McGregor, Ian]
On: 15 July 2008
Access Details: [subscription number 794859089]
Publisher: Psychology Press
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954
Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Basic and Applied Social Psychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t775648090
Religious Zeal and the Uncertain Self
Ian McGregor a; Reeshma Haji a; Kyle A. Nash a; Rimma Teper a
aYork University, Toronto, Canada
Online Publication Date: 01 April 2008
To cite this Article: McGregor, Ian, Haji, Reeshma, Nash, Kyle A. and Teper,
Rimma (2008) 'Religious Zeal and the Uncertain Self', Basic and Applied Social
Psychology, 30:2, 183 — 188
To link to this article: DOI: 10.1080/01973530802209251
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01973530802209251
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf
This article maybe used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction,
re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly
forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be
complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be
independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings,
demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or
arising out of the use of this material.
Downloaded By: [McGregor, Ian] At: 20:37 15 July 2008
Religious Zeal and the Uncertain Self
Ian McGregor, Reeshma Haji, Kyle A. Nash, and Rimma Teper
York University, Toronto, Canada
In two studies, personal uncertainty threats caused compensatory religious zeal. In
Study 1 an academic uncertainty manipulation heightened conviction for religious
beliefs and support for religious warfare. In Study 2 a relationship uncertainty manipu-
lation caused non-Muslim’s to derogate Islam. Together, these findings demonstrate
that two aspects of religious zeal—conviction for one’s own beliefs and derogation of
others’—are caused by personal uncertainty.
The uncompromising attitude is more indicative of an
inner uncertainty than of deep conviction. The implac-
able stand is directed more against the doubt within than
the assailant without.
Eric Hoffer, 1954, p. 41
Some Christians were thrown to the lions by Romans,
but far more were killed by other Christians over dis-
agreements about the precise extent of Jesus’ divinity
(Durant, 1944). That people should so readily kill for
their religious beliefs seems absurd to the outside
observer. Yet religiously animated killings perennially
blight human history. Still today, religious zeal
continues to inspire killing in the Middle East and else-
where. What is the psychological appeal of religious
zeal? The present research investigates the idea that
religious zeal is appealing because it helps people cope
with personal uncertainty.
Uncertainty has been identified as the most basic
cause of anxiety in humans, and indeed in all verte-
brates. When an important goal is at risk of being
blocked, but the organism is motivated to remain
oriented toward the goal, an uncertain motivational
state results in which approach and avoidance tenden-
cies are simultaneously active. It is specifically this
uncertain predicament that, if not resolved, results in
the experience of anxiety (Gray & McNaughton, 2000).
Threats that are more certain, in contrast, activate dif-
ferent responses that are mediated by different brain
systems. This important distinction between uncertain
and certain threats is illustrated by the way a hungry,
foraging rat responds to the smell of a cat (with anxiety)
versus the actual presence of a cat (with fear). A hungry
rat that smells a cat will continue to forage but will do
so with periodic, vigilant, scanning behaviors that are
relieved by anxiolytic drugs but not panicolytic drugs.
In contrast, a rat confronted with an actual cat will
show unconflicted fight, flight, or freeze reactions that
are relieved by panicolytic drugs but not anxiolytic
drugs.
In our research we use two experimental manipula-
tions to induce a state of uncertainty in undergraduate
psychology students. The first involves asking them to
summarize an extremely complicated paragraph about
a statistics procedure. We describe it as a common tool
in psychology. We do not tell them that the paragraph is
taken out of context from an advanced graduate text,
with random sections deleted to make it bewildering to
read. In a pilot study, after participants had completed
either this bewildering statistics task or the simple con-
trol condition task, they rated how the manipulation
had made them feel, using a 5-point scale. Participants
reported that the difficult statistics manipulation made
them feel significantly more uncertain (M¼2.66) than
in the control condition (M¼1.68), t(113) ¼4.98,
p<.0005. Moreover, this effect on uncertainty was
stronger than for any of the other adjectives assessed:
good (p<.005), successful (p<.005), stupid (p<.05),
happy (p<.05), smart (p<.05), likeable (ns), empty
Correspondence should be sent to Ian McGregor, York University,
4700 Keele Street, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada. E-mail:
ianmc@yorku.ca
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY, 30:183–188, 2008
Copyright #Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 0197-3533 print=1532-4834 online
DOI: 10.1080/01973530802209251
Downloaded By: [McGregor, Ian] At: 20:37 15 July 2008
(p<.05), meaningful (ns), anxious (ns), ashamed (ns),
insecure (ns), lonely, (ns), out of control (ns). The only
other adjectives that came close were frustrated
(p<.001) and confused (p<.001), both of which are
closely related to the experience of uncertainty. The
effect on reported uncertainty also remained statistically
significant (p<.001), with all the other adjectives
included as covariates.
In the second study, we rely on a manipulation of
uncertainty that has been shown in past research to
cause personal uncertainty but not general negative
affect or lowered self-worth (McGregor, Zanna,
Holmes, & Spencer, 2001). Thus, in addition to having
strong face validity, the two manipulations of personal
uncertainty have demonstrated past specificity for
uncertainty related affect. Moreover, they are theoreti-
cally close to the very basic uncertainty processes
described by Gray and McNaughton (2000). In both
studies, participants are confronted with goal impe-
dances that can not be simply fled from. Psychology stu-
dents know that statistics is an important part of their
chosen major, and dilemmas, by definition, involve goal
conflicts that one feels caught up in. Although the
uncertainty manipulations in both studies quite likely
have downstream effects on a wide variety of negative
thoughts in diverse content areas, there is good theoreti-
cal and empirical support for their primary effect on
uncertainty.
The dependent variables in the two studies assessed
aspects of religious zeal. Zeal refers to tenacious con-
viction and intolerance of dissent for an idealistic
cause (McGregor, Gailliot, Vasquez, & Nash, 2007;
McGregor & Marigold, 2003; McGregor, Nail,
Marigold, & Kang, 2005). Social commentators and
biographers have long observed that zeal erupts during
periods of personal or cultural turbulence. Hitlers
prototypical zeal, for example, coalesced during a
phase of intense personal and national chaos, and
the rise of fascism leading up to the Second World
War has been similarly attributed to developmental,
economic, and national insecurity (Adorno, Frenkel-
Brunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950; Fromm, 1941;
Muslin, 1992). Personal uncertainty, in particular,
has been implicated as an important cause of extreme
and rigid patterns of thinking and acting (e.g.,
Baumeister, 1991; Durkheim, 1951; Kruglanski, 1989;
McGregor, 2003, 2004). Correlational research has
found links between various measures of uncertainty-
aversion and extremity of conservative ideology
(Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003), and
experimental research has found that zealous reactions
are aroused by, and mask, personal uncertainty
(McGregor, 2006b; McGregor & Marigold, 2003;
McGregor et al., 2001, 2005; van den Bos, Poortvliet,
Maas, Miedema, & van den Ham, 2005).
Our research focuses on personal uncertainty as a
cause of religious zeal. Extreme instances of religious
zeal also tend to erupt during personal and historical
periods of heightened uncertainty, when identities are
conflicted or threatened by competing worldviews
(Armstrong, 2000; Durant, 1950; James, 1958; McCann,
1999). Surprisingly, however, there is no controlled
experimental evidence implicating personal uncertainty
as a cause of religious zeal. Experiments have found that
other psychological threats can arouse belief in God,
afterlife, and supernatural agency (Kay, Gaucher,
Napier, Callan, & Laurin, in press; Norenzayan &
Hansen, 2006; Willer, in press) and that religious contro-
versy causes particularly extreme negative feelings
among people who are most averse to uncertainty (van
den Bos, van Ameijde, & van Gorp, 2006; see also
van den Bos, 2001). Our research, however, is the first
to assess religious zeal as a function of experimentally
manipulated personal uncertainty. In Study 1 we mani-
pulate academic uncertainty and assess participants’
subsequent zeal for their own religious beliefs. In Study
2, for multimethod convergence we manipulate relation-
ship uncertainty and assess participants’ subsequent
derogation of others’ religious beliefs. Together, the
two studies investigate both sharp edges of religious
zeal: rigid conviction about personal opinions and dero-
gation of competing claims.
STUDY 1
Method
Twenty male
1
volunteers in a large 2nd-year personality
psychology course used electronic ‘‘clicker’’ devices to
1
Women also participated, but with null effects, jtsj<1. Whereas
for men random assignment resulted in 10 participants per condition,
for women it resulted in 25 participants in the control condition but
only 16 in the experimental condition, suggesting differential attrition
(clicker pick-up was anonymous and participants could stop respond-
ing without being identified). Women may have been more threatened
by the statistics materials than men. Only 28%of women, as compared
to 70%of men, rated the control condition materials as ‘‘extremely
easy’’ or ‘‘very easy.’’ The differential attrition and difficulty ratings
suggest that, perhaps because of the stereotype threat that women
associate with math (see Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999), female stu-
dents may have found the statistics-related uncertainty materials too
threatening to complete. This post hoc exclusion of women because
of differential attrition in the two conditions is a limitation of Study
1. Past research under more controlled circumstances, however, has
found the Study 1 uncertainty threat to cause other kinds of nonreli-
gious zeal among both men and women (McGregor et al., 2005, Study
3). Study 2 used a different manipulation of uncertainty to concep-
tually replicate the results of Study 1 with a sample that included both
women and men.
184 MCGREGOR ET AL.
Downloaded By: [McGregor, Ian] At: 20:37 15 July 2008
participate in the study. They anonymously picked up
their clickers at the beginning of class after being told
that the study would be related to that days lecture
material. Questions were projected at the front of a
lecture theatre, and clicker responses were wirelessly
recorded on a central computer. After indicating their
gender, participants viewed a list of prevalent religious
orientations and indicated which they most identified
with. Only participants who indicated definitive
religious orientations were retained for the study—two
who identified as ‘‘Pagan’’ and one who identified as
‘‘Other’’ were not included.
Participants randomly assigned to the uncertainty
condition read over an extremely difficult passage
taken out of context from a graduate statistics text.
It was laden with imposing mathematical formulae
and statistical symbols and truncated prematurely to
render it incomprehensible. Participants were given
4 min to read it, to try their best to understand it,
and then to indicate how easy it was for them to
understand. Response options were skewed as follows
to create the expectation that the passage should be
easily understood: 1 (extremely easy), 2 (very easy),
3(easy), and 4 (hard). Participants randomly assigned
to the control condition received the same instructions
as participants in the uncertainty condition, except
they read a simple introductory passage from an
undergraduate statistics text instead of the difficult
one. (The materials for both conditions were on the
back side of the lecture outline and had been shuffled
and distributed at the beginning of the class.) This
uncertainty manipulation targeted undergraduate
psychology students’ common uncertainty about their
math and statistic ability. In past research with
psychology student participants this manipulation
has caused self-reported confusion and defensive con-
viction for opinions about abortion, capital punish-
ment, suicide bombing, and the U.S. invasion of
Iraq (McGregor & Jordan, 2007; McGregor, Nail
et al., 2005).
After the uncertainty manipulation, participants
spent a few minutes using their clickers to answer
questions about lecture topics. This delay was pro-
vided to allow time for defensive zeal reactions to
emerge (Wichman, Brunner, & Weary, in press).
And then for the dependent variable participants then
rated their agreement with eight statements about
their religious zeal (a¼.75): I am confident in my
belief system; I aspire to live and act according to
my belief system; My belief system is grounded in
objective truth; Most people would agree with my
belief system if they took the time to understand it
rather than just relying on stereotypes about it; If
my belief system were being publicly criticized I would
argue to defend it; I would support a war that
defended my belief system; If it came down to it I
would sacrifice my life to defend my belief system;
In my heart I believe that my belief system is more
correct than others’. Ratings were made on the follow-
ing 5 point scale: 5 (strongly agree), 4 (agree), 3
(neither agree nor disagree), 2 (disagree), and 1
(strongly disagree). Participants were explicitly
instructed to refer to their identified religious
orientation when answering each question.
Results
Religious belief system identifications were 5%Muslim,
10%Buddhist, 20%Jewish, 20%Atheist, and 45%
Christian. The manipulation check revealed that parti-
cipants in the academic uncertainty condition rated the
statistics passage as ‘‘hard’’ (M¼3.70), but in the con-
trol condition they rated it as ‘‘very easy’’ (M¼2.20),
t(18) ¼4.57, p<.001, d¼2.15. As predicted, on the
main dependent variable participants in the academic
uncertainty condition reported more overall religious
zeal (M¼3.60) than participants in the control con-
dition (M¼3.04), t(18) ¼2.40, p<.03, d¼1.13. More-
over, and particularly disturbing, exploratory analyses
revealed that participants in the academic uncertainty
condition reported that they were significantly more
willing to support a war that defended their religious
beliefs (M¼3.00) than participants in the control con-
dition (M¼1.80), t(18) ¼2.45, p<.03, d¼1.15. Thus,
not only did the academic uncertainty manipulation
cause participants to tend to ‘‘agree’’ with zealous state-
ments about their religious beliefs, it further moved
them toward equivocal ‘‘neither agree nor disagree’’
acceptance of religious war from their usual and
more pacific ‘‘disagree’’ stance. Study 2 conceptually
replicates and extends the results of Study 1 with male
and female participants and provides multimethod
convergence.
STUDY 2
Whereas Study 1 assessed zeal about participants own
religious beliefs, Study 2 assessed the tendency to dero-
gate others’ religious beliefs. Specifically it assessed non-
Muslim’s tendency to derogate Islam. Exaggerated con-
sensus and intolerance of dissent are cardinal features of
zeal (McGregor et al., 2001; 2005). Uncertainty not only
motivates endorsement of extremes but also motivates
delusion about how objective those extremes are and
derogation of dissenters (McGregor & Jordan, 2007;
McGregor et al., 2001). Indeed, delusional consensus
was reflected in the Study 1 finding that after the aca-
demic uncertainty threat, participants rated their
RELIGIOUS ZEAL 185
Downloaded By: [McGregor, Ian] At: 20:37 15 July 2008
religious beliefs as particularly objective and likely to
garner the agreement of others.
In Study 2 we focus on uncertainty-motivated
derogation of dissenting religious views. Non-Muslim
participants ruminated about a relationship dilemma
ongoing in their lives and then evaluated Islam. We
expected the uncertainty manipulation to cause dero-
gation of Islam.
Method
Thirty-four female and 19 male non-Muslim under-
graduates received either course credit or $5 for their
participation. The experiment was advertised as explor-
ing ‘‘relationships, opinions, personality, and deci-
sions’’ and was administered on computers in private
cubicles in lab sessions with as many as 6 participants
at a time. After answering gender, demographic, and
personality questions that took approximately 10 min
to complete, participants were randomly assigned to
describe either a currently unresolved relationship
dilemma of their own (uncertainty condition) or a
relationship dilemma a friend was facing, for which
the participant had a clear and certain opinion about
what the friend should do (control condition). All part-
icipants were allocated 3 min to complete these materi-
als, after which the computer automatically began the
next portion of the experiment. Uncertainty and con-
trol condition materials were adapted from McGregor
et al. (2001).
The main dependent variable was then assessed
after 3 min of filler materials that provided the delay
required for the onset of zealous reactions to uncertainty
(Wichman et al., in press). Participants rated their agree-
ment with the following statements about Islam: Most
people who practice Islam value peace; Equality is an
important concept in Islam; Islam promotes essentially
the same good values as other world religions; The
Qur’an and Bible contain similar stories; Islam pro-
motes religious tolerance; Islam would be an okay religi-
on if it did not have such oppressive rules; There is
something in Islam that invites terrorism; Canada
should have more stringent immigration regulations
for people from Islamic countries; The majority of
people who practice Islam are religious zealots; Islam
is a cult on a larger scale. Participants rated each state-
ment on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree)to5(strongly
agree). The five positive and five reverse-scored negative
items were averaged for a measure of Islam evaluation
(a¼.80). To afford a comparison between religious zeal
and ingroup bias effects, the items assessing Islam
evaluation were mirrored by a counterbalanced block
of 10 questions that assessed participants’ evaluation
of Canada. For the main analysis, we regressed Islam
evaluation on manipulated uncertainty condition, with
Canada evaluation and gender as covariates (neither
covariate interacted with condition, jtsj<1).
2
Results
Islam evaluations were significantly more negative in the
uncertainty condition (M¼3.24) than in the control
condition (M¼3.53), t(49) ¼2.16, p<.05, d¼.62. It is
important to emphasize that this uncertainty effect was
significant even when attitudes toward Canada (which
were not affected by the uncertainty manipulation;
p>.17) were statistically controlled. Thus, uncertainty
specifically caused zeal about religious beliefs. This lack
of an uncertainty effect on Canadian national pride is
the usual finding in our laboratory (McGregor, Haji,
& Kang, in press). National pride may be too vague of
a phenomenon to serve as a topic of zeal for
Canadians (except perhaps during international hockey
competitions).
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Religious rapture, moral enthusiasm, ontological won-
der, and cosmic emotion, are all unifying states of mind,
in which the sand and grit of self-hood incline to disap-
pear. (James, 1902, p. 240)
Personal uncertainty caused two aspects religious zeal.
In Study 1 academic uncertainty increased conviction
for religious beliefs and support for religious warfare.
In Study 2 relationship uncertainty caused derogation
of Islam among non-Muslims. These results extend the
range of defensive zeal research. Past research has found
that individuals respond to the same uncertainty manip-
ulations used in the present research with defensive zeal
about social issues (McGregor & Jordan, 2007;
McGregor & Marigold, 2003; McGregor et al., 2001;
2005). Our research demonstrates that uncertainty-
induced defensive zeal processes can also bias religious
convictions. Indeed, religious ideology may be a parti-
cularly reliable and attractive domain for defensive
zeal because religious ideals are difficult to objectively
2
Although the Gender Uncertainty interaction effect did not pre-
dict Islam evaluation, there was a marginal effect of gender on Islam
evaluation, such that women were marginally more negative toward
Islam (M¼3.28) than men did (M¼3.52), t(49) ¼1.75, p<.10,
d¼.50. This marginal tendency for non-Muslim women to like Islam
less than men is potentially interesting in its own right, but the null
Uncertainty Gender interaction effect is most important here, given
that Study 1 only used male participants. The Study 2 result shows that
at least one aspect of religious zeal after uncertainty occurs regardless
of gender, which is consistent with the absence of gender effects in past
research on zeal about nonreligious topics (e.g., McGregor et al.,
2001).
186 MCGREGOR ET AL.
Downloaded By: [McGregor, Ian] At: 20:37 15 July 2008
refute. As such, religious zeal can provide a reliable safe
haven of hopeful and certain ideals that one can focus
on to make the ‘‘sand and grit of selfhood’’ disappear
(James, 1902, p. 240). Our results help explain one piece
of the puzzle of why religious conflicts can be so intrac-
table. Religious zeal can be a motivated defense that
people seize onto quell self-uncertainty.
This is an important finding because most people
would likely be loathe to admit that the fervency of their
religious ideals might be, at least in part, a psychological
defense (McGregor, 2007). Moreover, given the social
cost of religious extremism, and the dire need for clear
illumination of the phenomenon, the direct causal
demonstrations in our research could be particularly
instructive.
At a more general level still, these findings add to a
growing body of research demonstrating that various
threats with links to uncertainty cause compensatory
conviction and zeal about various topics that are far
removed from the topics of the threats (reviewed in
McGregor, 2006a). All of the threats that cause such
defensive zeal reactions share a common property of
being experiential threats to important goals or values
from which one cannot easily disengage. All of the vari-
ous zeal reactions reflect unconflicted ideals or actions,
which can return the individual to an unconflicted state
of unmitigated approach-motivation (McGregor et al.,
2007). Ideals have been isolated, both theoretically and
empirically, as being closely linked with approach-
motivation processes (Amodio, Shah, Sigelman, Brazy,
& Harmon-Jones, 2004; Higgins, 1997). Accordingly,
we interpret the current results as reflecting a very basic
kind of compensatory approach-motivation that people
(and other animals; Sullivan, 2004) turn to for relief from
the anxiety that can result from unresolved conflict and
uncertainty. Future research assessing patterns of neural
activation after uncertainty threats are currently under
way to more directly assess this defensive approach-
motivation hypothesis.
REFERENCES
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford,
R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality New York: Harper.
Amodio, D. M., Shah, J. Y., Sigelman, H., Brazy, P. C., & Harmon-
Jones, E. (2004). Implicit regulatory focus associated with asym-
metrical frontal cortical activity. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology,40, 225–232.
Armstrong, K. (2000). The battle for God: A history of fundamentalism.
New York: Ballantine.
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Escaping the self. New York: Basic Books.
Durant, W. (1944.) Caesar and Christ. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Durant, W. (1950). The age of faith. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Durkheim, E. (1951). Suicide: A study in sociology. Glencoe, IL: The
Free Press.
Fromm, E. (1941). Escape from freedom. New York: Holt, Rinehart,
and Winston.
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxi-
ety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychol-
ogist,52, 1280–1300.
Hoffer, E. (1954). The passionate state of mind. New York: Harper and
Row.
James, W. (1958). The varieties of religious experience. New York:
Mentor. (Original work published 1902)
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003).
Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological
Bulletin, 129, 339–375.
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callan, M. J., & Laurin, K.
(in press). God and government: Testing a compensatory control
explanation for the support of external systems. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology.
Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cog-
nitive and motivational bases. New York: Plenum.
McCann, S. J. H. (1999). Threatening times and fluctuations in
American church memberships. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin,25, 325–336.
McGregor, I. (2003). Defensive zeal: Compensatory conviction about
attitudes, values, goals, groups, and self-definition in the face of per-
sonal uncertainty. In S. Spencer, S. Fein, & M. Zanna (Eds.), Moti-
vated social perception: The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 9, pp. 73–92).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McGregor, I., & Marigold, D. C. (2003). Defensive zeal and uncertain
self: What makes you so sure? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,85, 838–852.
McGregor, I. (2004). Zeal, identity, and meaning: Going to extremes
to be one self. In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, & T. Pyszczynski
(Eds.), Handbook of experimental existential psychology (pp. 182–
199). New York: Guilford.
McGregor, I. (2006a). Offensive defensiveness: Toward an integrative
neuroscience of compensatory zeal after mortality salience, personal
uncertainty, and other poignant self-threats. Psychological Inquiry,
17, 299–308.
McGregor, I. (2006b). Zeal appeal: The allure of moral extremes. Basic
and Applied Social Psychology,28, 343–348.
McGregor, I. (2007). Personal projects as compensatory convictions:
Passionate pursuit and the fugitive self. In B. R. Little, K.
Salmela-Aro, & S. D. Phillips (Eds.), Personal project pursuit:
Goals, action and human flourishing (pp. 171–195). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
McGregor, I., Gailliot, M. T., Vasquez, N., & Nash, K. A. (2007).
Ideological and personal zeal reactions to threat among people with
high self-esteem: Motivated promotion focus. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin,33, 1587–1599.
McGregor, I., & Jordan, C. H. (2007). The mask of zeal: Low implicit
self-esteem, and defensive extremism after self-threat. Self and Ident-
ity,6, 223–237.
McGregor, I., Haji, R., & Kang, S.-J. (in press). Can ingroup affir-
mation relieve outgroup derogation? Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology.
McGregor, I., Nail, P. R., Marigold, D. C., & Kang, S-J. (2005).
Defensive pride and consensus: Strength in imaginary numbers.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,89, 978–996.
McGregor, I., Zanna, M. P., Holmes, J. G., & Spencer, S. J. (2001).
Compensatory conviction in the face of personal uncertainty: Going
to extremes and being oneself. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology,80, 472–488.
Muslin, H. (1992). Adolf Hitler: The evil self. Psychohistory Review,
20, 251–270.
RELIGIOUS ZEAL 187
Downloaded By: [McGregor, Ian] At: 20:37 15 July 2008
Norenzayan, A., & Hansen, I. G. (2006). Belief in supernatural agents
in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,32,
174–187.
Shrira, I., & Martin, L. L. (2005). Stereotyping, self-affirmation, and
the cerebral hemispheres. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
31, 846–856.
Spencer, S. J., Steele, c., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and
women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology,35, 4–28.
Sullivan, R. M. (2004). Hemispheric asymmetry in stress processing in
rat prefrontal cortex and the role of mesocortical dopamine. The
International Journal of the Biology of Stress,7, 131–143.
van den Bos, K. (2001). Uncertainty management: The influence of
uncertainty salience on reactions to perceived procedural fairness.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,80, 931–941.
van den Bos, K., Poortvliet, P. M., Maas, M., Miedema, J., & van den
Ham, E.-J. (2005). An enquiry concerning the principles of cultural
norms and values: The impact of uncertainty and mortality salience
on reactions to violations and bolstering of cultural worldviews.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,41, 91–113.
van den Bos, K., van Ameijde, J., & van Gorp, H. (2006). On the psy-
chology of religion: The role of personal uncertainty in religious
worldview defense. Basic and Applied Social Psychology,28, 333–341.
Wichman, A. L., Brunner, R. P., & Weary, G. (in press). Immediate
and delayed effects of causal uncertainty inductions on uncertainty
accessibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.
Willer, R. (in press). No atheists in foxholes: Motivated reasoning
and religious ideology. In J. T. Jost, A. C. Kay, & H. Thorisdot-
tir (Eds.), Social and psychological bases of ideology and system
justification.
188 MCGREGOR ET AL.
... The first two experiments were conducted in our lab and operationalized threat as negative performance feedback in the academic context. The threat inductions are similar to other paradigms designed to induce academic uncertainty (McGregor et al., 2005(McGregor et al., , 2008Nash et al., 2011). Our initial goal with these experiments was to test whether BIS activation would mediate the effects of threat on defense, as was demonstrated by Agroskin et al. (2016). ...
... Classic academic uncertainty manipulations (McGregor et al., 2005(McGregor et al., , 2008McGregor & Jordan, 2007;Nash et al., 2011) require participants, usually Psychology students, to summarize a complicated text passage on structural equation modeling that contains formulae, mathematical symbols, and statistical terms. Notably, the procedure subtly leads participants to believe that failing to understand the text was a sign that they were not suitable to earn an academic degree. ...
... Notably, the procedure subtly leads participants to believe that failing to understand the text was a sign that they were not suitable to earn an academic degree. This treatment creates feelings of confusion, frustration, and anxiety (McGregor et al., 2008). It is usually contrasted with a nonfrustrating control condition in which participants must summarize a more accessible text about the usefulness of statistics. ...
Article
Full-text available
The anxiety-to-approach model of threat and defense suggests that psychological threat and defense reflects the operation of the behavioral inhibition system (BIS) and the behavioral approach system (BAS). Based on this model, we hypothesized that threat should lead to defense especially when both the BIS and the BAS are highly active. We tested this hypothesis in three experimental studies. In Studies 1 (n = 125) and 2 (n = 158), students were told that their performance in a task was below (vs. in the range of) that of university graduates to induce academic uncertainty (vs. certainty). Those participants who exhibited high anxious affect (indicative of BIS activation) and high approach-related affect (indicative of BAS activation) in response to the academic uncertainty treatment became more ethnocentric. In Study 3, we used the openly accessible ManyLabs 4 data set, a multilab attempt to reproduce a classic mortality salience study. Students (n = 795) were either led to reflect about their own mortality or about watching television. Again, those who experienced high levels of both anxious and approach-related affect responded unfavorably to the author of an essay that criticized their country. These results support the idea that anxiety and approach motivation promote the emergence of psychological defense.
... However, the current Russian aggression against Ukraine has been a huge shock for Russian society; this may alter existing trends. For example, war increases uncertainty, and greater uncertainty often enhances the religiosity of respondents (McGregor et al. 2008, Hogg et al. 2009). ...
Article
Are Russians religious? In this paper, religiosity in Russia (2011–2021) is decomposed into age, cohort and period effects using panel data analysis techniques. It is shown that age effects on religiosity are U-shaped: respondents are more religious when young or old with the minima at the age of 41–45 for women and 46–60 for men. The most religious cohorts in Russia were born in 1960–1980. There is a sharp decline in religiosity in younger generations. There is also a large gender religiosity gap in older generations: women are more religious than men. However, in younger cohorts, the gender religiosity gap closes. Time effects suggest that religiosity in Russia was increasing before 2016, while in 2016, a substantial decline occurred. Since 2016, there has been no growth in religiosity.
... Darüber hinaus geben eindeutige Abgrenzungen nach außen der Gruppe eine abgeschlossene Form. In Konsequenz wird eine Diffusion und Durchlässigkeit zwischen unterschiedlichen sozialen Identitäten erheblich erschwert wird -etwa indem strenge Aufnahmerituale für Neumitglieder vorgesehen sind oder schlichtweg aufgrund der Tatsache, dass die Mitglieder durch ihre identifizierbare Zugehörigkeit zur radikalen Gruppe fortan kaum in anderen, etwa moderateren Gruppen aufgenommen oder akzeptiert werden.Unterstützung gewinnt die Uncertainty-Identity-Theorie durch eine Reihe empirischer Studien, die unter anderem zeigten, dass das Erleben persönlicher Unsicherheit eigene religiöse Überzeugungen festigt sowie die Bereitschaft verstärkt, für die eigene Religion in den Krieg zu ziehen und andere Religionsgruppen abzuwerten(McGregor et al. 2008). Unter US-amerikanischen Unterstützer:innen der republikanischen und der demokratischen Partei führte die Interaktion von hoher persönlicher Unsicherheit und hoher wahrgenommener Entitativität der Parteien zu einem Anstieg der wahrgenommenen Polarisierung beider Lager hinsichtlich relevanter politischer Fragen(Sherman et al. 2009). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Unabhängig von der Frage nach der Legitimität von Radikalisierung stellt der so bezeichnete Prozess aus psychologischer Perspektive eine zunehmende Gefährdung des Selbst und der Anderen dar. Radikalisierung beschreibt das Aufgehen von Individuen in radikalen Gruppen und kann in Hass und Gewalt gegen die Mitglieder andersdenkender Gruppen zum Ausdruck kommen. Radikalisierung motiviert dazu, diesen Gegner:innen bzw. Feind:innen nach Möglichkeiten zu schaden und lässt Konflikte eskalieren, aber auch die persönliche Selbstfürsorge, wie die körperliche Unversehrtheit der eigenen Person, mehr und mehr in Vergessenheit geraten. Auch für Freund:innen und Familie abseits der radikalen Gruppe stellt Radikalisierung häufig eine enorme Belastung dar. Denn vom zerstörerischen Strudel der Aggressionen kann das gesamte Umfeld einer radikalisierten Person erfasst werden. Der Gedanke, dass Radikalisierung für eine besondere Bedürftigkeit oder Notlage radikalisierter Personen stehen könnte, mag dabei nicht als Erstes in den Sinn kommen. Doch im folgenden Beitrag wollen wir die Perspektive einnehmen, Radikalisierung anhand von unbefriedigten sozialen und psychologischen Bedürfnissen zu erklären.
... This study will take a stand from the perspective of Uncertainty-Identity Theory as previous studies have shown how uncertainty and extremism show strong ties (Hogg 2013(Hogg , 2014Hogg and Adelman 2013) and how uncertainty is managed through group entitativity can be conducive to explain radicalism and zealotry (Hogg et al. 2010;Kruglanski et al. 2006;McGregor et al. 2008). At the extreme end of the spectrum, group entitativity embodies consensus and internal homogeneity, adhering to a rigid ideological belief system associated with a shared worldview and ritualized practices (Hogg and Adelman 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
In the face of uncertainty, people tend to look for ways to manage discomfort, often through religion. Growing conservatism in the Muslim society in Indonesia has encouraged people to restore meaning when dealing with uncertainties triggered by a crisis. This study aims to explore the dynamics of meaning restoration through hijra, a collective spiritual transformation process. Using a qualitative approach, findings showed that people going through hijra were driven by significance loss and potential significance gain, both taking validation in relational significance and group identity. In Indonesia, where religion is acknowledged as central to identity, religious groups become a prescription sought for closure, and all are directed towards managing uncertainty and restoring significance. The study found that individuals started the hijra journey provoked by either personal significance loss or the need to gain significance and resorted to the collective orientation of relational significance to gain closure. Once a new collective identity is established through the spiritual transformation, significance is restored.
... When people experience loss of control, they recognize a lack of order in their environment, leading to an elevated sense of randomness or uncertainty (Landau et al., 2015). A consequence of being in a state of uncertainty is to engage in uncertainty mitigation behavior (Faraji-Rad & Pham, 2017), such as stronger religious conviction (McGregor et al., 2008), supporting capital punishment (McGregor et al., 2001), and increased structure-seeking (Cutright, 2012). More importantly, people experiencing uncertainty may exhibit risk averse behavior (Chuang et al., 2012;Malmendier et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Subscription commerce is an integral part of today's consumption space. Researchers have identified different factors that influence subscription intent and different mechanisms that are used to evaluate subscription options. In this research, we investigate the effect of loss of perceived control on subscription intent and explore a new underlying mechanism used for the evaluation of subscription options. Across six studies in the field and laboratory, we show that when consumers lose their level of perceived control, they are likely to exhibit higher subscription intent for monthly subscription options than yearly subscription options, and we explain the mechanism through risk aversion. Particularly, we find that individuals experiencing loss of control are more likely to evaluate subscription options based on associated risk, and subsequently choose the option that appears less risky. Our findings contribute to the literature by identifying a novel risk‐based mechanism driving subscription choice and by finding a new consumer‐related antecedent of subscription choice in perceived control.
Article
Russia represents the largest Eastern Orthodox population in the World. Contrary to secularization trends in Western Europe, Russia has been undergoing a religious revival promoting traditional values. Extensive evidence indicates that in Christian nations women tend to be more religious than men. But what socio-economic factors explain the gender-religiosity gap in Russia? Using the Russian Longitudinal Monitoring Survey (RLMS-HSE), we compare gender-specific religiosities. Fixed-effects regressions show a U-shaped relation between respondents’ age and the probability of being religious with a minimum at the age of 15 for women, and 46 for men. Furthermore, we find that women are indeed more often religious than men and that the influence of religious parents or grandparents on women’s religiosity is higher. Women compared to men demonstrate increased religiosity in response to the death of a family member. However, better education and health status reduce women’s religiosity more than that of men. At the same time, men’s religiosity is more often influenced by that of their wives. In addition, men living in urban areas tend to be more religious than those in rural areas. This finding is Russia-specific and differs from the results found in other countries.
Chapter
Security is a vital subject of study in the twenty-first century and a central theme in many social science disciplines. This volume provides a comparative analysis of the ways in which the concept of security is theorized and studied across different disciplines. The book has two objectives: first, to explore the growing diversity of theories, paradigms, and methods developed to study security; and, second, to initiate a multidisciplinary dialogue about the ontological, epistemological, paradigmatic, and normative aspects of security studies in social sciences. Readers across nine fields are invited to reflect on their conceptualizations of security and to consider how an interdisciplinary dialogue can stimulate and enrich the understanding of security in our contemporary world. Analytically sharp yet easy to read, this is a cutting-edge volume exploring what security is and what it means in today's world.
Chapter
Security is a vital subject of study in the twenty-first century and a central theme in many social science disciplines. This volume provides a comparative analysis of the ways in which the concept of security is theorized and studied across different disciplines. The book has two objectives: first, to explore the growing diversity of theories, paradigms, and methods developed to study security; and, second, to initiate a multidisciplinary dialogue about the ontological, epistemological, paradigmatic, and normative aspects of security studies in social sciences. Readers across nine fields are invited to reflect on their conceptualizations of security and to consider how an interdisciplinary dialogue can stimulate and enrich the understanding of security in our contemporary world. Analytically sharp yet easy to read, this is a cutting-edge volume exploring what security is and what it means in today's world.
Chapter
Security is a vital subject of study in the twenty-first century and a central theme in many social science disciplines. This volume provides a comparative analysis of the ways in which the concept of security is theorized and studied across different disciplines. The book has two objectives: first, to explore the growing diversity of theories, paradigms, and methods developed to study security; and, second, to initiate a multidisciplinary dialogue about the ontological, epistemological, paradigmatic, and normative aspects of security studies in social sciences. Readers across nine fields are invited to reflect on their conceptualizations of security and to consider how an interdisciplinary dialogue can stimulate and enrich the understanding of security in our contemporary world. Analytically sharp yet easy to read, this is a cutting-edge volume exploring what security is and what it means in today's world.
Chapter
Security is a vital subject of study in the twenty-first century and a central theme in many social science disciplines. This volume provides a comparative analysis of the ways in which the concept of security is theorized and studied across different disciplines. The book has two objectives: first, to explore the growing diversity of theories, paradigms, and methods developed to study security; and, second, to initiate a multidisciplinary dialogue about the ontological, epistemological, paradigmatic, and normative aspects of security studies in social sciences. Readers across nine fields are invited to reflect on their conceptualizations of security and to consider how an interdisciplinary dialogue can stimulate and enrich the understanding of security in our contemporary world. Analytically sharp yet easy to read, this is a cutting-edge volume exploring what security is and what it means in today's world.
Article
Full-text available
People approach pleasure and avoid pain. To discover the true nature of approach–avoidance motivation, psychologists need to move beyond this hedonic principle to the principles that underlie the different ways that it operates. One such principle is regulatory focus, which distinguishes self-regulation with a promotion focus (accomplishments and aspirations) from self-regulation with a prevention focus (safety and responsibilities). This principle is used to reconsider the fundamental nature of approach–avoidance, expectancy–value relations, and emotional and evaluative sensitivities. Both types of regulatory focus are applied to phenonomena that have been treated in terms of either promotion (e.g., well-being) or prevention (e.g., cognitive dissonance). Then, regulatory focus is distinguished from regulatory anticipation and regulatory reference, 2 other principles underlying the different ways that people approach pleasure and avoid pain.
Article
Full-text available
Considerable evidence now indicates that poignant self-threats cause compensatory zeal about opinions, values, goals, groups, and self-worth. Evidence for the self-threat -> zeal link is reviewed from research on zealous reactions to epistemic, self-worth, relationship, and mortality salience threats. Two new studies demonstrate that zealous pride and conviction insulate individuals from concern with uncertainty threats. The new studies further demonstrate that this insulation effect does not result from distraction, mood, or state self-esteem. It is proposed that the appeal of zeal arises from its ability to activate the approach-motivation system and to down-regulate the avoidance motivation system's vigilant concern with threats.
Article
Full-text available
Building on the work of S. M. Sales, who related contemporaneous economic threat to authoritarian behaviors, two studies tested the hypothesis that threat is associated with relatively more attraction to authoritarian churches and less attraction to nonauthoritarian churches. The hypothesis was supported in Study 1, when the annual percentage of changes in memberships (1928-1986) for two authoritarian and two nonauthoritarian denominations were examined in relation to several annual social, economic, and political threat indices, and in Study 2, when changes in the proportion of the population having membership in 25 representative denominations were examined over periods of relatively low threat (1955-1964), high threat (1965-1974), and low threat (1965-1979). Both studies suggest that social and political threat as well as economic threat may activate authoritarian behaviors.
Article
Intergroup hostilities are an important social concern in multicultural societies and the global community. Individuals with dispositionally high Personal Need for Structure (PNS) are particularly inclined toward outgroup derogation [Schaller, M., Boyd, C., Yohannes, J., O’Brien, M. (1995). The prejudiced personality revisited: Personal need for structure and formation of erroneous group stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 544–555]. The present research demonstrates that ingroup affirmation can eliminate high PNS individuals’ tendency toward outgroup derogation. Spontaneous (Study 1) and experimentally manipulated affirmations (Study 2) of consensual, positive ingroups eliminated the high PNS tendency to derogate outgroup targets. Study 3 experimentally manipulated the two key elements that are presumably bolstered by ingroup affirmations—self-certainty and self-worth—independent of the group context. The high PNS tendency to derogate outgroups was relieved only in the combined certainty and worth affirmation condition, just as it had been relieved in Study 2 by the ingroup affirmation. Results suggest a paradoxical strategy for relieving derogation of outgroups by affirming ingroups.
Article
This book provides an updated theory of the nature of anxiety and the brain systems controlling anxiety, combined with a theory of hippocampal function, which was first proposed thirty years ago. While remaining controversial, the core of this theory, of a 'Behavioural Inhibition System', has stood the test of time, with its main predictions repeatedly confirmed. Novel anti-anxiety drugs share none of the side effects or primary pharmacological actions of the classical anti-anxiety drugs on the actions of which the theory was based; but they have both the behavioural and hippocampal actions predicted by the theory. This text is the second edition of the book and it departs significantly from the first. It provides, for the first time, a single construct - goal conflict - that underlies all the known inputs to the system; and it includes current data on the amygdala. Its reviews include the ethology of defence, learning theory, the psychopharmacology of anti-anxiety drugs, anxiety disorders, and the clinical and laboratory analysis of amnesia. The cognitive and behavioural functions in anxiety of the septo-hippocampal system and the amygdala are also analysed, as are their separate roles in memory and fear. Their functions are related to a hierarchy of additional structures - from the prefrontal cortex to the periaqueductal gray - that control the various forms of defensive behaviour and to detailed analysis of the monoamine systems that modulate this control. The resultant neurology is linked to the typology, symptoms, pre-disposing personality and therapy of anxiety and phobic disorders, and to the symptoms of amnesia. © Jeffrey A. Gray and Neil McNaughton 2000 , 2003. All rights reserved.
Article
Previous research has focused on enhanced processing as a response to causal uncertainty (CU), but relatively little empirical attention has been given to how CU is activated and the temporal unfolding of this activation. The current research investigates the counterintuitive idea that people inhibit causal uncertainty immediately after its activation. We find that this inhibition weakens over time. Study 1 demonstrates this inhibition effect with self-report uncertainty. Study 2 demonstrates this effect with an implicit accessibility measure. Temporary inhibition of uncertainty may be a general response when uncertainty is activated.
Article
When women perform math, unlike men, they risk being judged by the negative stereotype that women have weaker math ability. We call this predicamentstereotype threatand hypothesize that the apprehension it causes may disrupt women's math performance. In Study 1 we demonstrated that the pattern observed in the literature that women underperform on difficult (but not easy) math tests was observed among a highly selected sample of men and women. In Study 2 we demonstrated that this difference in performance could be eliminated when we lowered stereotype threat by describing the test as not producing gender differences. However, when the test was described as producing gender differences and stereotype threat was high, women performed substantially worse than equally qualified men did. A third experiment replicated this finding with a less highly selected population and explored the mediation of the effect. The implication that stereotype threat may underlie gender differences in advanced math performance, even those that have been attributed to genetically rooted sex differences, is discussed.