ArticlePDF Available

Alien abduction experiences.

Authors:

Abstract

The focus of this chapter is alien abduction experiences (AAEs). Although there is no universal agreement regarding what contextual or experiential features are necessary or sufficient to define AAEs, a number of descriptions have been proposed. The most common features of AAEs include capture and examination. Other elements present in some abduction reports include conference, tours of the UFO, otherworldly journey, theophany, return, and aftermath. If nothing else, the descriptions illustrate that the AAE is a dynamic, elaborate, and involved experience, rich in contextual detail, with considerable perceptual, psychological, cognitive, and physical concomitants. Specific topics addressed in the chapter include prevalence, aftereffects, biological markers, individual differences, psychopathology, clinical issues and risks, and theories of and explanations for AAEs. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
74
Alien Abduction Experiences
Stuart Appelle1
State University of New York at Brockport
Steven Jay Lynn
State University of New York at Binghamton
Leonard Newman
Syracuse University
and
Anne Malaktaris
State University of New York at Binghamton
Word Count First Edition (11,903) vs. Word Count Second Edition (10,810)
Corresponding Author:
Steven Jay Lynn, Ph.D.
Psychology Department
Binghamton University
Binghamton, NY 13902
Email: stevenlynn100@gmail.com
Ms. Jessica White (a pseudonym) is a quiet, retired lady of 60 years who lives alone in
her small home near Lake Placid, New York. One morning she experienced a horrifying
nightmare in which she felt a “creature” with cold hands and large eyes pressing down on her
with a suffocating and paralyzing force. She tried to move but her body did not respond; in fact,
she had the strange sense that her consciousness was out of her body, watching her rendered
helpless. She tried to scream, but words would not escape her mouth. She heard humming,
1 Deceased
75
strange noises, and experienced an eerie sense that other creatures were nearby, watching in a
cold, detached way. The feeling that she was somehow violated, surely emotionally, possibly
physically, lingered throughout the day and returned in what she called “flashbacks” during the
week.
The only stories Jessica could relate her bizarre experience to were accounts she had read
about people abducted by aliens. Perhaps that is what had happened to her, she mused. Her
thoughts of alien abduction plagued her for the next few weeks to the point that she sought her
therapist’s “permission” to consult a lay hypnotist to fathom what had happened to her. Her
therapist, skeptical at first, finally was persuaded that it might be helpful for Jessica to gain
closure no matter what memories the hypnotist was able to recover.
During hypnosis, Jessica reported frightening images of being examined by three
emaciated-looking aliens with a variety of advanced, highly technological medical instruments,
the likes of which she had never seen before. After the medical proceedings, she was returned
from the spacecraft to her bed with telepathic instructions to forget the entire episode.
When Jessica recounted this story to her therapist, her therapist informed her that
she done some reading since their last meeting and thought Jessica’s strange and terrifying
experience might be explained by sleep paralysis, caused by a disruption in the sleep cycle and
often associated with terrifying hallucinations and the sense of menacing figures perceived to be
close to or even on top of the immobile person. Perhaps Jessica’s “memories” retrieved during
hypnosis were her elaboration of the strange events in terms of what she had learned about alien
abduction experiences from her readings and exposure to media like the X-files programs she
loved to watch.
Although Ms. White listened attentively to the explanation her therapist provided, she,
76
nevertheless, continued to "trust her feelings" and steadfastly maintained, to her close friends at
least, that she had encountered alien life forms. Not surprisingly, the issue was not discussed
much in treatment from that point on.
Definition
Ms. White's account includes many features of alien abduction experiences (AAEs).
Many AAEers report temporary paralysis and out-of-body sensations. AAEs are often described
as vivid, emotionally intense, realistic, and extremely distressing. In 1992, Hopkins, Jacobs, and
Westrum (1992) noted that the experience is associated with waking up paralyzed with a sense
of a strange figure or figures present, "missing time" (unaccounted for periods in conscious
memory), and seeing strange balls of light in one's room -- hallucinations consistent with sleep
paralysis phenomena. More recently, Susan Clancy (2006) asserted that sleep paralysis is pivotal
to AAEs, a hypothesis we will consider in greater depth when we examine possible explanations
of AAEs.
AAEs are characterized by subjectively real memories of being taken secretly and/or
against one's will by apparently non-human entities, usually to a location interpreted as an alien
spacecraft (i.e., a "UFO") and subjected there to complex physical and psychological procedures.
Such experiences can occur as an isolated event, but more commonly are recalled as repeated
episodes over several years. AAEs have been reported to appear in consciousness as intact or
partial recollections (sometimes long after the experience was remembered as having occurred),
but often seem to emerge (or are enhanced) only after aggressive retrieval techniques such as
hypnotic regression.
Whereas no consensus exists regarding the features of the AAE (Gotlib, Appelle,
Rodeghier, & Flamburis, 1994), descriptions have been proposed. Bullard (1987), basing his
77
analysis on a sample of 270 abduction reports, described the most common features of AAEs as
"capture" (caught and taken aboard a UFO) and "examination" (subjected to physical, sexual,
mental, and/or spiritual examinations). Other potential elements of abduction reports include
"conference” (communication with abductors); "tour" (guided examination of the UFO);
“otherworldly journey" (transport to another place on earth or an unearthly environment);
“theophany" (receipt of religious or spiritual messages); "return” (egress from the UFO and
return to earth); and "aftermath" (post-abduction experience effects).
Jacobs (1992), based on hundreds of personally investigated AAEs, has
categorized their content into primary, secondary, and ancillary events involving physical
activities (e.g., removal from earthly surroundings to an alien craft or environment, taking of
tissue samples, insertion of implants), mental activities (e.g., telepathic manipulation,
psychological testing, information exchange), and reproductive procedures (e.g., egg/sperm
collection, embryo implantation, removal of fetus, actual or simulated sexual activity,
presentation of human alien babies). By the 1990s, an increasing number of
reports emphasized sexual intercourse with aliens, with some women claiming to have multiple
offspring (kept by the aliens) that resulted from these acts (Hopkins, 1987; Jacobs, 1992, 1998;
Newman & Baumeister, 1997). If nothing else, these descriptions illustrate that the AAE is a
dynamic, elaborate, and involved experience, rich in contextual detail, with considerable
perceptual, psychological, cognitive, and physical concomitants.
Prevalence
A 2012 survey of 1,114 Americans, commissioned by the National Geographic Channel,
estimated that 36% of the population, or 80 million citizens, believe that UFOs are alien
spacecraft (ABC News, 2012). Researchers have not, as yet, conducted comparable surveys of
78
people who believe that they have been abducted by aliens, but the number of reported AAEs is
not small. When Bullard (1994) surveyed 13 investigators of UFO-related phenomena, he found
that even this small number of researchers had a total of 1,700 separate abduction reports in their
files. In 1993, Whitley Strieber (author of the best-selling book Communion [1987], a detailed
account of purported abductions) claimed that he had received 55, 000 letters from people who
believed they were abductees, and as many as 200 a week were still arriving (Strieber, 1993).
As of 1992, Hopkins, Jacobs, and Westrum, all proponents of the “reality” of the
AAE, estimated that as many as that 3.7 million Americans had been abducted. However, this
high prevalence rate was based, in part, on survey items that tapped sleep paralysis experiences,
not endorsement of the actual experience of being “abducted” by aliens (note critiques of
methodological, conceptual, and psychometric aspects of this survey by Dawes & Mulford,
1993; Goertzel, 1994). Klass (1993) estimated that if Hopkins et al.’s (1992) conclusion were
correct, then 340 Americans have been abducted every day since 1961, a number he regards as
implausibly high. Even if the numbers are much lower than some of these estimates, many
thousands of Americans believe aliens have abducted them.
Aftereffects
Bullard (1987, 1996) has cataloged the reported physical and psychological sequelae of
the AAE. The following statistics are based on his 1987 analysis of 270 abduction reports and
supported by his later (1994) survey of AAE investigators. Bullard cautioned that these statistics
may under-represent actual prevalence rates: systematic attention to aftereffects is rare, few
investigators bother to record or report them, and long-term follow-up is uncommon.
Bullard's analysis revealed physical conditions reported as immediate
consequences of an AAE: injury --cuts, bruises, scrapes, puncture wounds (11 cases); eye and
79
vision problems --burning, inflamed or bloodshot eyes, watering, irritation, conjunctivitis,
sensitivity to light, pupil dilation, impaired or blurred vision (22 cases); skin burns and irritation
--"sunburn" or redness of skin, blisters, itchiness, rash (23 cases); gastrointestinal distress
--nausea, diarrhea, constipation, gastric pain (13 cases); equilibrium and coordination problems
--balance, disorientation, fainting (14 cases); and thirst and dehydration (12 cases). Intermediate
and long-term physical aftereffects included 13 instances where the experient claims to have
been healed of some pre-existing ailment, 5 instances of weight loss, and 9 cases of recurring
conditions such as the skin or balance problems that immediately followed the initial AAE.
Bullard identified fear, anticipation, anxiety, and recurring nightmares as aftereffects of AAEs,
sequelae consistent with PTSD (Laibow & Laue, 1993; Powers, 1994a; Wilson, 1990).
On the other hand, Clancy (2005, p. 149) reported that not one of the many abductees she
interviewed answered “yes” to the question, “If you could do it all over again, would you choose
not to be abducted?” Even if the details of the abduction stories they told were unpleasant, many
abductees came to reinterpret them in a way that made them feel that their lives had more
meaning and purpose and that made them feel a part of something larger than themselves.
Paranormal experiences (e.g., apparitions, events interpreted as apparent telepathy) and
personality changes are also widely reported. Ring (1992) has argued that such experiences and
personality changes also accompany near-death experiences (see Greyson, this volume) and
reflect a common propensity for, and a common consequence of, "extraordinary encounters."
Psychophysiology
Except for multiple abduction cases (i.e., more than one person claims to have been
abducted) there exist no unambiguous instances of an individual having been observed during
the time they (later) report to have had an AAE. As such, the condition of an abduction
80
experient at the time of an AAE is unknown. However, McNally and his associates
(McNally,Lasko, Clancy, Macklin, Pitman, & Orr, 2004) found that memories associated with
AAEs could provoke powerful emotional reactions. The researchers compared the physiological
response of people with AAEs with non-AAE reporters. Abductees exhibited greater
psychophysiological reactivity to scripts featuring their traumatic abduction memory than to
positive or neutral scripts. Moreover, the abductees’ heart rate, sweating, breathing, muscle tone,
and emotional experience were similar to or greater than the reactions of combat vets and rape
victims to script-driven imagery. Conclusions regarding these findings are open to the
interpretation that (a) the reports of experients accurately reflect lived experiences, or (b) that the
belief that one has been traumatized during an AAE generates emotional responses similar to
those provoked by recollections of actual traumatic experiences.
Cultural and individual differences
Beliefs in the existence of extraterrestrial life and cultural beliefs have been implicated as
the "ground" from which the abduction scenario emerges. As McNally and Clancy (2005)
observed, anomalous experiences often prompt a search for explanations, and in their absence,
people “rely on personally plausible cultural narrative to interpret these otherwise baffling …
episodes” (p. 113).
Belief in extraterrestrial life and alien abductions
The belief in AAEs is premised on the existence of extraterrestrial life. Recent surveys
have shown an increase in the number of people who believe in extraterrestrial life (Swami,
Chamorro-Premuzi, & Shafi, 2010). Swami et al. (2009) surveyed 320 Austrian and 257 British
individuals regarding beliefs about extraterrestrial life and found that higher religiosity and right-
wing political orientation were negatively associated with such beliefs.
81
In a later survey of over 600 community adult volunteers (Swami, Chamorro-
Premuzi, & Shafi, 2010), education was negatively associated with beliefs about extraterrestrial
life, religiosity was negatively associated with beliefs about extraterrestrial life in general, and
conscientiousness positively predicted beliefs about extraterrestrial life in general and alien
visitation and government cover-ups in particular. Moreover, men were more likely than women
to believe in the existence of UFOs and alien visitation.
Patry and Pelletier ‘s (2001) survey of 400 volunteers revealed that whereas 35%
of respondents considered the idea seriously, only 30% completely rejected the idea of alien
abduction. Half of participants indicated that they would believe an alien abduction tale if a
friend told the story, and 79% of individuals felt that reporting an alien abduction would have
negative consequences. Four percent of individuals believed they knew someone personally who
claimed to have been abducted by aliens, and 2% believed that aliens at some point had abducted
them. These findings imply that the general public in Canada is at least somewhat receptive to
alien abduction narratives.
Cultural factors
To assess culturally derived beliefs, Lawson (1977) hypnotized volunteers and instructed
them to imagine an encounter with aliens. Contrary to his hypothesis, Lawson found that
hypnotiically suggested reports were not substantively different from claims of “genuine”
experients. Unfortunately, Lawson provided specific suggestions to report information pertinent
to what abductees claim, such as to imagine that aliens examined them. Accordingly, it is not
surprising that instructed and non-instructed reports shared many features, precluding a definitive
conclusion that participants’ fantasies stemmed from culturally derived beliefs.
Lynn and Pezzo (1994) assessed cultural narratives in which individuals were asked to
82
role-play (simulate) the performance of excellent hypnotic participants. Volunteers were told
that they would be hypnotized and asked to recall a puzzling event that involved: (a) driving in
the country, (b) leaving the car to witness mysterious moving lights in the sky, and (c) afterward,
having no clear recollection of being in the car and noticing 2 hours of unaccounted for missing
time. After a brief hypnotic induction, individuals were "age regressed" to the time when the
puzzling event occurred and asked to describe what they could see, feel, and hear. A structured
interview followed.
Lynn and Pezzo (1994) found that when individuals were specifically instructed to
simulate an encounter with aliens, 95% identified the lights as a UFO and 91% stated that they
interacted with aliens and boarded the spacecraft. In a second condition, researchers
administered a questionnaire (which queried respondents' about their beliefs in UFOs, if they had
sighted what they believe to be a UFO, memory gaps associated with the sighting, and so on),
devised by OMNI Magazine (Weintraub, 1987). After receiving this “priming” questionnaire,
during the interview, 62% of the participants reported they witnessed a UFO, and all but one, or
95%, reported witnessing a UFO in response to a direct question. In addition, 62% reported that
they interacted with aliens and a third of the participants reported they boarded the spacecraft. In
contrast, only 19% of the participants who received no specific instructions or priming
information identified what they saw as a UFO in response to an initial opened-ended question.
When asked more direct, leading questions in the interview, 52% identified the lights as UFOs,
24% indicated that they had interacted with the aliens, and 14% reported they boarded the UFO.
As Lynn and Pezzo (1994) hypothesized, participants endorsed items more frequently as they
received more cues to report experiences consistent with culture-based UFO narratives. Finally,
the simulated reports contained many of the features (e.g., telepathic contact, small bodies, large
83
eyes and heads of occupants, forced or compelled actions, sexual contact with aliens) reported by
AAEers. Combined, these studies indicate that certain elements of alien contact narratives are
widely available to college students.
Cultural beliefs may also be associated with the geographical distribution of abduction
reports and variations in their content. Bullard (1987) noted that 47% of the abduction reports he
studied came from the United States. Argentina, Brazil, Australia, and the United Kingdom also
provided a large number of reports, collectively accounting for about one third of abduction
reports. Africa, Asia, continental Europe, and the Middle East were only barely represented,
although the Associated Press news service (Associated Press, 1996) more recently reported "a
flurry of media reports on abductions by extraterrestrials" in Israel. It is unknown whether this
geographical variation reflects differences in the prevalence of AAEs or differences in reporting
and investigating them.
Similarly, the typical content of the AAE varies with geographical origin. In particular,
Bullard (1994) reported that compared to North American accounts, British and Australian
reports differed in terms of narrative descriptions and the experients' reactions to the reported
events. Moura (1994), a Brazilian psychologist, found that in her country, reported interactions
between abduction experients and aliens were far friendlier, and the experience was viewed more
positively than similar reports in the United States. Still, many of the content elements remain
constant across cultures (Carpenter, 1997). Although systematic, comprehensive, and large-scale
cross-cultural content analyses of cases are lacking, culture remains an important variable to
explore in future research.
Fantasy-proneness
One possibility is that cultural beliefs regarding alien visitations are transformed into
84
deeply felt personal experiences by a small number of individuals (i.e., “fantasizers” or fantasy-
prone personalities) who exhibit a propensity for making imaginative experiences seem "as real
as real," thereby confusing fantasy with reality (i.e., fantasy-prone personalities, Wilson &
Barber, 1981, 1983b). Indeed, there are interesting similarities between the experiences of
fantasizers and those of abduction experients. For example, fantasizers report a high incidence
of false pregnancies, paranormal and out-of-body experiences, the appearance of apparitions, and
vivid sleep imagery, which feels "as if they are seeing something that really exists out there or
that they are looking into another dimension" (Wilson & Barber, 1981, p. 365). Each of these
experiences has parallels with AAEs (Bullard, 1987; 1994), suggesting that fantasizers and AAE
reporters may belong to the same population. However, the empirical support for this hypothesis
is mixed.
Ring and Rosing (1990) found that participants who reported AAEs were more likely to
report childhood experiences of paranormal phenomena, "non-physical beings," and to "see into
other realities that others didn't seem to be aware of" (factors consistent with fantasy-prone
characteristics originally reported by Wilson and Barber) than participants who did not report
AAEs. However, the AAE reporters were no more fantasy-prone than non-AAE reporters.
Bartholomew, Basterfield, and Howard (1991) studied over 100 abduction
experients and concluded that the vast majority (87%) reported histories consistent with one or
more major characteristics associated with the fantasy-prone profile. However, they assessed
fantasy-proneness by retrospective analysis of biographical data, rather than by an independent
test of fantasy-proneness. Moreover, among all the fantasy prone characteristics they examined,
only the frequency of reported paranormal phenomena approached the frequency of
characteristics found by Wilson and Barber (1981) in their fantasy-prone population.
85
Rodeghier, Goodpaster, and Blatterbauer (1991) used the ICMI
measure of fantasy proneness (Wilson & Barber, 1983a) and found a distribution of scores no
different than would be expected from a random sample of the population based on Lynn and
Rhue’s (1988) published norms. Similarly, Spanos, Cross, Dickinson, and Dubreuil (1993)
found no statistical differences between groups (i.e., intense vs. non-intense UFO related
experiences) on fantasy-proneness as measured by the ICMI. They did, however, find a
correlation between ICMI scores and an intensity-of-experience scale. Nevertheless, even for the
group of persons who reported "intense experiences" (e.g., missing time and communication
with aliens), the mean ICMI score was at the midrange of the distribution of the general
population (Lynn & Rhue, 1988), leading Spanos et al. to conclude that their "findings clearly
contradict the hypotheses that UFO reports... occur primarily in individuals who are highly
fantasy prone" (p. 629). Relatedly, Hough and Rogers (2007) failed to find differences between
AAE reporters and non-reporters on measures of self-perceived fantasy proneness.
Negative findings aside, it is premature to conclude that fantasy proneness is not
associated with AAEs. First, many studies in this area have methodological limitations (e.g.,
unvalidated, ad hoc measures; small sample sizes) that preclude drawing strong conclusions.
Second, it is questionable whether any of the participants in Spanos et al.'s (1993) research
claimed to have classic abduction experiences, limiting the potential relevance of this research to
fantasy-proneness and AAEs. Third, respondents in such studies are undoubtedly aware that
many of the markers of fantasy proneness could indicate deviance or psychopathology (see Lynn
& Rhue, 1988). Therefore, characteristics or experiences associated with fantasy proneness
might not be reported in situations that evoke socially desirable responding, thereby
underestimating the correlation between fantasy proneness and AAEs (Newman & Baumeister,
86
1996b). In conclusion, fantasy proneness remains a plausible yet not very well tested hypothesis
with mixed and not very compelling support.
The boundary-deficit personality
Hartmann (1984) found that nightmare sufferers share a constellation of traits
characterized by weak discrimination between basic cognitive categories such as self and non-
self, fantasy and reality, and dream and waking experiences. These thin "boundaries" are said to
characterize individuals who are sensitive, artistic, empathetic, vulnerable, imaginative, have a
weak sense of sexual or personal identity, have difficulty distinguishing periods of time, or who
are perceived by others as "different."
Based on Hartmann's construct of thin boundaries, Kottmeyer (1988) argued that
the characteristics of what he termed the boundary-deficit personality also describe the abduction
experience. Spanos et al. (1993) administered a number of scales relevant to Kottmeyer's
hypothesis and found that, compared to the scores for control subjects, their close
encounter/UFO experient groups reported higher self-esteem, lower "schizophrenia," higher
well-being, lower "perceptual aberration," lower perception of an "unfriendly world," lower
"aggression," and no difference in "social potency." Moreover, Spanos et al. found no difference
between control and close encounter participants on measures of absorption, fantasy-proneness,
and imaginal propensity, all of which should be elevated according to the thin boundary or
boundary-deficit hypothesis.
The findings reported so far either are inconsistent with, or clearly contradict, predictions
derived from the boundary explanation. However, other studies have yielded results more
consonant with boundary-deficits in AAEers, including a weak sense of personal or sexual
identity (Slater, 1985), schizoid tendencies (Parnell & Sprinkle, 1990), greater sensitivity to non-
87
ordinary realities (Ring & Rosing, 1990), and a high rate of reported suicide attempts (Stone-
Carmen, 1994).
Emotional intelligence and The Big Five personality factors
Hough and Rogers (2007) found no support for the hypothesis that people who report
AAEs score relatively low on measures of emotional intelligence, which reflects the perception,
appraisal, and regulation of emotions and emotional knowledge. These negative findings support
McNally et al.’s (2004) contention that AAEers are no less capable of monitoring their own
emotional states than their non-experient counterparts. Hough and Rogers (2007) also failed to
find that any of the so-called “big five” personality factors (i.e., neuroticism, extraversion,
openness to experiences, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) was correlated with AAEs.
The "psychically sensitive" personality
Abduction experients often report increased paranormal abilities in the aftermath of an
AAE (Bullard, 1994; Ring, 1992), and many report long histories of ostensibly paranormal
events preceding their AAEs (Basterfield, 1994; Bullard, 1987, Randles, 1988; Ring, 1992).
Ring (1992) found a constellation of traits in common between abduction experients and
individuals who claim to be "electrically sensitive" (to have anomalous effects on electrical
devices). Based on a comparison of the personality characteristics of 20 abduction experients
and those of highly successful ESP subjects, Johnson (1994) reported that at least some
experients share traits with individuals who appear to have performed well in "remote viewing"
experiments in which individuals are asked to use purported paranormal abilities to obtain
information regarding objects, persons, or events at a distant or unseen locaton. Randles (1988)
has described the paranormal histories of AAErs as "the key to the entire [abduction] mystery."
Of course, self-reports of paranormal experience are no substitute for the objective assessment of
88
paranormal performance.
Psychopathology
Psychological disorders that might account for AAEs include symptoms of psychosis
(e.g., hallucinations and delusions), folie- à-deux (shared psychotic symptoms brought about by
a close relationship between percipients), conversion reactions (i.e., physiological
manifestations of a psychosomatic nature; e.g. marks, blotches, and discolorations of skin),
dissociative disorders (amnesia, fugue, and other conditions resulting in time loss,
disorientation), dissociative identity disorder (which in addition to "missing time" may be
characterized by messages from and/or "dual identities"), and Munchausen's syndrome (i.e.,
self-inflicted injury or false claims of physical symptomatology).
Whereas psychopathology explains some isolated AAE cases, assessment by both
clinical examination and standardized tests has shown that, as a group, experients and the general
population do not differ in psychopathology prevalence. Bloecher, Clamar, and Hopkins (1985)
discuss the findings of Slater (1985), who conducted a masked evaluation of 9 abduction
experients. Using the Rorschach and the Thematic Apperception Test, Slater found no evidence
of psychopathology that could account for the reported AAEs. Similarly, Jacobson and Bruno
(1994) examined the AAE narratives of 12 individuals and failed to find narrative elements that
suggested "any currently recognized psychiatric syndrome" (p. 306). Nonetheless, hospital
records showed that two of their participants had suffered from a major psychiatric illness around
the time of their AAE, illustrating the danger of relying on self-report alone.
Spanos et al. (1993) compared control participants to people who reported UFO
related experiences. The UFO reporters were divided into those who merely saw unidentified
lights and those who reported more elaborate close encounters. The authors found that their
89
encounter participants scored no lower than controls on any of the multiple measures of
psychological health administered. The researchers concluded that their findings "provide no
support whatsoever for the hypothesis that UFO reporters are psychologically disturbed" (p.
628). McNally and Clancy (2005) found negligible evidence of current distress or
psychopathology among self-identified abductees. Holden and French (2002) likewise conclude
that there are no data to suggest that serious psychopathology is more common in AAErs than in
the general population.
Still, researchers have found that abduction experients are not representative of the
general population and may exhibit manifestations of psychopathology. For example, although
Parnell and Sprinkle (1990) found no evidence for overt psychopathology among experients, the
researchers also found that participants claiming communication with aliens exhibited a
propensity for unusual feelings, thoughts, and attitudes and were suspicious, distrustful,
imaginative, and exhibited schizoid tendencies. Rodeghier et al. (1991) found that people with
AAEs, compared with non-experients, reported more loneliness, less happiness, and poorer sleep
patterns. Perhaps most troubling was the fact that Stone-Carmen (1994) found that 57% of her
participants reported suicide attempts (compared with 1.28 % in the general population).
Clinical issues and risks
It is not uncommon for abduction experients to present to mental health professionals
with AAE-related symptoms (Gotlib, 1996). One concern we will evaluate in depth later is that
abduction accounts might represent false memories created or elaborated by suggestive
psychotherapeutic procedures such as hypnosis (see Gotlib, 1993). Given that the spontaneous
emergence during therapy of an AAE completely unsuspected by the client is apparently quite
rare (based on the general lack of references to such cases in the clinical literature; see, however,
90
Gotlib, 1996), suggestive influences could enrich an existing AAE or fragmentary AAE memory,
create whole new experiences beyond those initially presented, and harden conviction regarding
the reality of the experience.
Consider the following example of leading questions asked of an alleged abductee:
Dr. Fiore: Now I'm going to ask you a few questions at this point. You will remember
everything because you want to remember. When you were being poked everywhere, did they do
any kind of vaginal examination?
Sandi: I don't think they did.
Dr. Fiore: Now you're going to let yourself know if they put a needle in any part of your
body, other than the rectum.
Sandi: No. They were carrying needles around, big ones, and I was scared for a while they
were going to put one in me, but they didn't. [Body tenses].
Dr. Fiore: Now just let yourself relax. At the count of three you're going to remember
whether they did put one of those big needles in you. If they did, know that you're safe, and it's
all over, isn't it. And if they didn't, you're going to remember that too, at the count of three. One .
. . two . . . three.
Sandi: They did. (From Fiore, 1989, p. 26)
An ethics committee comprised of individuals interested in the AAE (Gotlib, Appelle,
Rodeghier, & Flamburis, 1994) has formally cautioned therapists against practices that might
produce false memories. Clinicians should avoid implicitly or explicitly suggesting an AAE and
avoid using suggestive memory recovery procedures with anyone who claims to have
experiences consistent with an AAE or its potential antecedents (e.g., sleep paralysis,
hypnagogic/ hypnopompic images).
91
Clinicians should assess for possible antecedent conditions stemming from psychological
or organic dysfunction. Whereas psychotherapy carries its risks, so does withholding treatment.
Mental health practitioners must feel free to treat experients, lest they exacerbate their client's
feelings of rejection and helplessness and perpetuate the social and professional stigma
associated with the experience. The risks of providing therapy can be minimized- and positive
outcomes best assured - when treatment educates clients about possible explanations for the
AAE and encourages them to understand the AAE and its possible repercussions in terms of its
meaning in their life (see Clancy, 2007).
Theories
In our discussion of predisposing factors, we have presented a number of theories and
explanations of AAEs. We discuss other theories that merit consideration now.
Hoaxes
According to the hoax explanation, reports of alien abduction are stories made up to
deliberately deceive, motivated by opportunities for monetary or psychosocial rewards (e.g.,
support group membership, talk show appearances, presentations at conferences). However, to
take advantage of such opportunities, the abduction experient must go public. In the vast
majority of cases there is simply nothing in the reporter's behavior that would imply such
motivation, and the experient who goes public is a rarity. Much more commonly, an assurance
of anonymity is desired.
On the other hand, deliberate misrepresentation can occur in the absence of incentives for
deception. The term Münchausen syndrome, or factitious disorder, refers to individuals who
feign physical or psychological illness, with the motivation to assume the sick role (DSM IV;
American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p.474).
92
Despite some parallels with abduction accounts, it is unlikely that factitious disorder can
account for abduction hoaxes. Sufferers are likely to have an extensive history of
hospitalizations or treatment interventions, be extremely resistant to relinquishing the role as
"patient," and tend to be reluctant, vague, and inconsistent when asked to provide detailed
information. These and other differential diagnoses are not characteristic of the vast majority of
abduction experients.
Psychoanalytic and psychodynamic theories
Some commentators (e.g., Sagan, 1993; Vallee, 1969) have suggested that similar themes
appear in both folklore (e.g., encounters with fairies, elves, angels) and contemporary abduction
accounts and have argued that this implies a common origin in the unconscious (see Bullard,
1991). A number of psychoanalytic theories have been proposed to explain the manifestation of
AAEs.
One theory is based on a correlation between reported AAEs and reported childhood
abuse (e.g., Laibow & Laue, 1993; Powers, 1994a, 1994b; Ring & Rosing, 1990; Rodeghier et
al., 1991). Some theorists have suggested that this association reflects actual childhood abuse
manifesting as “screen” (or false) memories of alien abduction. According to Powers (1994a),
abduction by aliens "is less stressful than confronting the trauma of childhood abuse perpetrated
by relatives or family friends" (p. 49), and "recasting the experience [of early childhood abuse]
as a selection with such a grand purpose [i.e., for the aliens’ cosmic objectives] might restore
meaning to lives threatened by traumatic memories" (Powers, 1994b, p. 46). Lawson (1984,
1985) argued that the AAE is the unconscious' representation of the birth experience, and Grosso
(1985) extended Jung’s (1959) assertion that some "flying saucer" sightings represent archetypal
imagery associated with the collective unconscious. Psychoanalytic and psychodynamic theories
93
are not, as yet, supported by solid scientific evidence.
The sleep paralysis and sleep anomalies hypothesis
In the past decade, sleep paralysis and other sleep anomalies have gained increasing
attention as explanations for the AAE (e.g., Clancy, 2007). AAEs are commonly reported as
having occurred at bedtime, or during the course of sleep. They are frequently first remembered
as the content of an apparent (albeit "unusual") dream, or as otherwise having a dream-like
subjective quality. Furthermore, as noted above, AAEs are often accompanied by sleep
paralysis, a condition that (a) occurs during part of the sleep period when motor tonus is lost, and
(b) is marked by a variety of disturbing experiences, including humming noises, vibrations, vivid
hallucinations, increased heart rate, and the sense of one or more “presences” nearby or
rendering the body immobile (Cheyne, 2005). Interestingly, sleep paralysis experiences are
common, occurring on a one-time or more basis in as many of one-third to one-half of college
students (Fukuda, Ogilvie, Chilcott, Venditelli, & Takeuchi, 1998; Girard & Cheyne, 2004).
Baker (1990) characterized AAEs as "a classic, textbook description" of sleep-
related hallucinations. Part and parcel of hypnagogic states is the experience of perceptual
distortions and anomalies (e.g., round objects swell to gigantic size), a sensation of pressure and
heaviness on the body or, alternately, sensations of weightlessness and floating, feelings of
disorientation and fearfulness that extend to panic caused by the fact that the dreamer is unable to
move (Baker, 1992 a, b; 1995). Hufford (1982) has drawn attention to the similarities of AAEs
and a variety of sleep experiences across many cultures. The phenomena of sleep paralysis are
not common knowledge in the general public. Accordingly, people are unlikely to construe their
experiences in terms of sleep paralysis, but rather describe their often-terrifying experiences in
terms of paranormal folklore or along the lines of cultural narratives (Holden & French, 2002;
94
McNally & Clancy, 2005). In medieval times, the myths of the incubus (male demon) and
succubus (female demon)—evil spirits that descend on vulnerable sleepers and engage in sexual
relations with them—probably arose from sleep paralysis experiences (Sagan, 1995).
The hallucinatory experiences that accompany sleep paralysis can be reliably parsed into
vestibular-motor, intruder, and incubus hallucinations (Cheyne, 2005). Intruder hallucinations
typically involve a vague sense of threat accompanied by strange noises, apparitions, and
sensations. According to Girard and Cheyne (2004), intruder hallucinations are generated by a
threat-activated vigilance system that produces fear and impending threat. Incubus
hallucinations involve breathing difficulties, bodily pressure, and feelings of suffocation or
impending death. Finally, vestibular-motor hallucinations include sensations of flying, floating,
or flying, and out of body experiences. In a survey of 6,000 individuals reporting a sleep
paralysis episode within the previous week, Cheyne (2005) reported that intruder and incubus
hallucinations are highly correlated with and often interpreted as threat and assault by alien
entities, a perception typically not associated with commonly occurring positive attributions
(e.g., spiritual experiences) of sleep paralysis phenomena (Hufford, 2005).
Although many studies have explored sleep paralysis, there is still a need for research to
examine its link with AAEs. Gotlib (1996) provided a single case study of a client with both a
sleep disorder and AAEs. Spanos et al. (1993) found that compared to participants with less
involved UFO experiences, participants who reported intense alien encounters experienced them
more often in association with sleep-related phenomena (e.g., sudden awakenings). Rodeghier
(1994) reported a somewhat greater incidence of hypnagogic imagery in a subset of abduction
experients. McNally and (2004) found that all 10 people with AAEs they studied reported at
least one episode of sleep paralysis accompanied by hallucinations. In each case, the participants
95
interpreted these experiences, which were generally consistent with contemporary cultural
stereotypes (e.g., thin, gray body with large dark eyes, McNally & Clancy, 2005), as related to
aliens. Because eight of the abductees had undergone some form of hypnosis to assist in the
“recovery” of memories of AAEs, the researchers were not able to determine the contribution of
hypnosis versus sleep paralysis to these experients’ recovered memories. Although sleep
paralysis symptoms bear striking resemblance to AAEs, sleep paralysis cannot account for full-
blown stories of boarding alien spacecraft and being subject to painful medical examinations,
which may represent embellishments on sleep paralysis phenomena related to cultural narratives
about AAEs or suggestive psychotherapeutic interventions.
Escape-from-self and masochistic fantasy
Newman and Baumeister (1996a; see also Newman, 1997) detailed many similarities
between the UFO abduction experience and what might seem at first to be a very different kind
of phenomenon: ritualized masochism. The main features of masochism -- both actual activities
and fantasies --are pain, helplessness, loss of control, and humiliation. All of these themes
dominate abduction accounts, and Newman and Baumeister (1996a) hypothesized that abduction
accounts might derive from the same motivational roots that attract some people to masochism.
They argued that this accounts for the resemblance between the profile of the typical abductee
and the demographic profile of the typical masochist (e.g., a person of reasonably high
socioeconomic status from a Westernized individualistic society). The underlying motive, they
claim, is the wish of some individuals to "escape the self." The "threats, stresses, and other
burdens of modern egotism" create a need in some individuals to "escape from self-awareness
and identity;” in other words, people sometimes seek relief from constant pressures to be
independent, fulfilled, esteemed, and in control. Strategies that thwart self-esteem and control
96
may accomplish this, and Newman and Baumeister (1996a) argue that masochism is an extreme
but effective example (see Baumeister, 1989). Masochism can help people escape their
customary identity because the pain that is inflicted upon masochists captures their attention and
makes reflection and self-evaluation difficult; restraint and immobilization nullifies the usual
pressure to assert control over one's situation; and paradoxically the humiliation involved can
liberate people from trying to meet their usual standards of esteem and dignity.
Newman and Baumeister (1996a) suggested that among individuals inclined to
escape the self, the influence of abduction investigators, media, and popular culture create the
raw material for masochistic fantasy to manifest itself as an AAE (especially those aspects of the
AAE dealing with sexual or gynecological procedures). In support of their hypothesis, Newman
and Baumeister examined the abduction accounts presented in Bullard's (1987) compendium of
AAEs for specific references to humiliating displays (e.g., "being stretched out on a table naked
with lots of people watching"), a feature of masochistic fantasy more prevalent among female
masochistic fantasizers than males (see Baumeister, 1988). Their analysis of the Bullard data
showed that such features were present in the narratives of 50% of the males and 80% of the
females, a statistically significant difference consistent with the masochism hypothesis.
Electromagnetic effects
Anomalous responses to electromagnetic fields have been suggested as a cause of the
AAE. Persinger (1990) regarded such responses as a manifestation of labile temporal lobe
activity in response to the effects of stresses in the earth’s crust (a condition he argues propagates
electromagnetic fields). However, the presence of electromagnetic fields during AAEs has not
been demonstrated, nor has the prevalence of temporal lobe lability among abduction experients
been established. Spanos et al. (1993) assessed temporal lobe lability and found no differences
97
between control participants and abduction experients. Relatedly, Holden and French’s (2002)
research failed to find support for the temporal lobe lability hypothesis.
False memories
Holden and French (2002) suggested that abductees might be particularly prone to false
memories in that their tendency to dissociate, for example, may increase vulnerability to
pseudomemory formation. Clancy et al. (2002) used the Deese/Roediger-McDermott paradigm
(Roediger & McDermott, 1995; Deese, 1959; false recall/recognition of a word not actually
presented but semantically related to words actually presented on a list) to explore false recall
and recognition in (a) participants who reported remembering an alien abduction (recovered
memory group); (b) participants with no explicit memory of an abduction, but suspected such an
event occurred (repressed memory group); and (c) individuals who denied a history of alien
abduction (control).
Participants who reported recovered memories committed more false recall and false
recognition errors than control participants, but did not differ in true recall or recognition rates.
Participants in the recovered memory group were more prone to memory errors than the
repressed memory group, who were, in turn, more prone to error than the participants in the
control group. Those reporting recovered or repressed memory scored higher on measures of
hypnotic suggestibility, perceptual aberration, and magical ideation than control subjects (Clancy
et al., 2002). Importantly, individuals in the recovered memory group were more likely to
exhibit source monitoring deficits (i.e., remembering how, when, and where a memory is
acquired), which may explain why, after exposure to suggestive therapies or media sources, these
individuals eventually ‘recalled’ false memories. To document the generalizability of these
findings, it will be important for researchers to replicate these findings with paradigms that
98
assess complex memories that resemble more real life scenarios.
Hypnosis and hypnotizability
Researchers have used suggestive procedures to instate memories of complex and
sometimes implausible events such as putting slime in a teacher’s desk (Lindsay, Hagen, Read,
Wade, & Garry, 2004), events that occur in the first few days of life (Spanos et al., 1999), being
attacked by a dog (Porter, Yuille, & Lehman, 1999), witnessing demonic possession as a child
(Mazzoni, 2007; Mazzoni, Loftus, & Kirsch, 2001), and even memories of alien abduction
(Otgaar et al., 2007). Of course, AAEs do not arise in a contextual vacuum: much of their detail
emerges during sessions in which suggestive procedures such as hypnosis are used to recover
memories presumably related to the AAE. Bullard (1987) examined published AAE accounts
prior to 1987 and determined that hypnosis had been used 31% of the time. However, for those
reports that Bullard rated high in both reliability and information content, hypnosis was involved
about 70% of the time (Bullard, 1989). A later survey he conducted of active abduction
investigators (Bullard, 1994) suggested that hypnosis was by that time being used in about 70%
of all contemporary abduction investigations.
Whereas hypnosis may produce a slight increase in accurate memories, this slim
advantage, if present at all, often comes at the expense of a tradeoff of increased errors and an
increased tendency to respond to misleading information (see Lynn, Barnes, & Matthews, 2009).
In fact, the elaboration of confidently held spurious memories could be the effect of misleading
questions rather than of hypnosis per se (see Mazzoni, Heap, & Scoboria 2010). The influence
of hypnosis may be related more to recall quantity rather than content. Bullard (1989, 1994)
compared AAEs that emerged during hypnosis with those obtained without hypnotic assistance.
Bullard (1989) concluded, "the form and content of abduction stories seems independent of
99
hypnosis" (p.3) (cf., Newman & Baumeister, 1996). In his later examination, Bullard (1994)
concluded that hypnosis is a significant factor in regard to the quantity of material reported, but
not in any direct way to the content. These findings are consistent with the literature, which
indicates that hypnosis often increases the sheer volume of responses, regardless of the accuracy
of the responses obtained (see Lynn et al., 2009). Although people who experience AAEs do not
appear to be more hypnotizable than the general population (Rodeghier, Goodpaster, &
Blatterbauer; 1991), many medium hypnotizable persons--the modal participants in the
population--report pseudomemories in experimental contexts (Lynn et al., 2009). Moreover, even
low hypnotizable individuals occasionally experience false memories (E.C. Orne, Whitehouse,
Dinges, & M. T. Orne, 1996). In short, even low hypnotizable persons might be influenced by
hypnotic suggestions that engender or reinforce AAEs.
Clancy, McNally, Schacter, Lenzenweger, and Pitman (2002) reported that
published narratives of alien abduction follow a characteristic pattern: 1) the abductee mentions
an (apparent) episode of sleep paralysis; 2) the sleep paralysis episode often includes multimodal
sensory hallucinations; 3) the abductee assumes that “something” has happened and seeks the aid
of a therapist to understand the anomalous experience; and 4) during hypnotic regression, the
abductee “recalls” the abduction (see also Lynn & Kirsch, 1996; Newman & Baumeister, 1996
for related accounts).
Medical-surgical procedure hypothesis
Forrest (2008) suggested that many of the frequently reported particulars of the AAE
resemble medical and surgical procedures that could be a precursor of the AAE. Forrest (2008)
argued that the altered state of consciousness some report during an AAE corresponds to
anesthesia, mysterious green figures could be surgeons in scrubs, and the surgical suite with
100
bright lights and strange equipment could represent the flying saucer. Moreover, medical-
surgical procedures often involve physical restraint and loss of control (and are thus a not
uncommon basis for masochistic fantasy and activity—Theroux, 1998), along with changes in
heartbeat and blood pressure. Although Forrest (2008) provided no evidence to support his
claims, he argued that abductees could be recovering memories of actual surgery related to
intraoperative awareness, which he estimates occurs in 1 to 2 percent of cases. Forrest suggested
that future studies include a full medical history, including medical and surgical procedures.
The extraterrestrial (ET) hypothesis
Perhaps the most provocative explanation of AAEs is that at least some of them are
veridical reports of lived experiences (Mack, 1994, 1999). Critics of this hypothesis argue that in
the absence of tangible proof (e.g., indisputable physical evidence of alien contact, artifacts from
an alien civilization, direct contact of an alien civilization with authorities), parsimony requires
that the ET hypothesis be dismissed. Reports have occasionally surfaced of actual physical
artifacts of alien encounters: specifically, objects or materials left embedded in the bodies of
experients (i.e., “alien implants”). However, these implants are rarely available for analysis,
because they are often said to disappear after removal. In addition, no alleged implant has been
found to consist of anything but terrestrial elements (Blackmore, 1998; Nickell, 2001; Sheaffer,
1997).
Nevertheless, proponents point to a number of features of the AAE as supporting the ET
hypothesis, including the consistency of the accounts, down to very specific details. For
example, Jacobs (1992) regarded "the strongest evidence presented [to be]...the congruence of
narrative and the richness of exact detail" (p. 239). Relatedly, based on an exhaustive analysis of
AAE content, Bullard (1987) concluded: No accident, random hoax, or purely personal fantasy
101
could reasonably explain so much consistency throughout this sizable body of reports." (p. 353).
In his later analysis, Bullard (1994) noted that both prominent and obscure elements of the AAE
recur across investigators: "The range of differences among major features and main patterns is
quite narrow.... Abduction reports seem to converge toward a unity of content irrespective of the
investigator" (p. 615).
Although consistency is well documented, the source of this consistency is still a subject
of debate. For example, Kottmeyer (1989) has argued that fantasy production could easily draw
upon material readily available in the media and popular culture. Previously, we cited data
implying that cultural narratives could account for similarities in abduction reports. Other
arguments that have been raised in support of a veridical interpretation of AAEs include alleged
corroborating testimony from very young children (Hopkins, 1994) and from multiple experients
describing their participation in the same abduction event (Haines, 1994). However, Otgaar and
colleagues (2009) were able to instate memories of being abducted by a UFO when they
provided children aged 7-12 with newspaper articles that provided information suggesting such
experiences were not uncommon. In fact, younger children (7-8 years old) were more likely to
report “memories” of being abducted than were children 11-12 years old. The issue of multiple
participants in a common abduction may the product of cross-contamination of memories and
fantasies between individuals, although this explanation has not, as yet, been empirically
evaluated.
Methodological issues
Many theories that seem parsimonious and reasonable have been advanced to explain the
AAE. However, methodological problems in this research area are legion. As we noted earlier,
(a) many of the studies reviewed rely on very small samples [e.g., Clancy (2007) N= 11] and
102
retrospective analyses of biographical data that are not representative of all important features
such as normal conscious memory and association with sleep, and (b) many studies did not use
standardized test and diagnostic procedures. Also, it is not always clear whether individuals
experienced a classic AAE or merely experienced witnessing a UFO or an alien. Moreover,
virtually nothing is known about whether the persons who present to their therapists with
experiences consistent with AAEs are representative of the population of persons who have
AAEs in the larger community. Questions can be raised about the validity of AAE reporters'
responses to questionnaires or interviews. Experients may be defensive or highly committed to
their beliefs and status as "abductees," which may engender response biases that distort their
scores on measures of psychopathology, fantasy-proneness, and hypnotizability. It may be as
important to study the unique circumstances and self-presentational concerns of AAE reporters,
as it is to study the nature of their experiences. Unfortunately, no studies to date have
manipulated the perceived demand characteristics of the experimental setting and the way that
experimental scales and procedures are presented to AAE reporters (e.g., psychopathology vs.
creativity/imagination). Clearly, scales with established and subtle validity indices would be
worth including in studies of AAEs.
Various theorists have emphasized the role of therapy and therapists in the production of
AAE reports. However, we know very little about how therapists actually manage such reports
and abduction experients in therapeutic situations. Because the use of hypnosis is often
superimposed on the broader dynamics of psychotherapy, the specific role of hypnosis in
treatment has not been isolated. Researchers should consider both the perceptions and behaviors
of the client as well as the therapist. Clearly, this will be a complex and challenging endeavor.
Conclusion
103
It may be asking too much for any theory or even any combination of variables identified
to date to account for all abduction reports. The evidence suggests that many, if not most, AAEs
are associated with sleep paralysis, cultural scripts, and the possible shaping influences of
suggestive procedures and hypnotizability. Yet it must also be said that many historical
abduction cases have not been associated with sleep paralysis, hypnosis has not been used in
many cases, and experients often have normal conscious memories associated with the reported
abduction (i.e., no “recovered” memory technique have been involved). If the hypotheses
tendered so far cannot completely explain the gamut of AAEs in prosaic terms, more esoteric
explanations may be required. The most prominent among these is that the AAE is veridical, but
no solid evidence supports this explanation. And in the absence of such evidence, the argument
strains credulity on many fronts. This notwithstanding, we might do well to emulate Meacham's
definition of "wisdom" (cited in Seppa, 1997) and "hold the attitude that knowledge is fallible
and strive for a balance between knowing and doubting." In this respect, the study of AAEs
might make us all a little wiser.
References
ABC News (2012). UFSs exist, say 36 percent in National Geographic Survey.
http://abcnews.go.com/Technology/ufos-exist-americans-national-geographic-
survey/story?id=16661311#.T_hlLnAkCVk. Accessed July 7, 2012.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders: DSM IV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Appelle, S. (1996). The abduction experience: A critical evaluation of theory and
evidence. Journal of UFO Studies, 6, 29-79.
Associated Press (1996, Sept. 22). UFO hysteria invades Holyland: Israeli media join craze of
104
sightings, alien abductions.
Baker, R. A. (1990). They call it hypnosis. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.
Baker, R. A. (1992a). Alien abductions or alien productions? Some not so unusual personal
experiences. Unpublished manuscript, Lexington, KY.
Baker, R. A. (1992b). Hidden memories: Voices and visions from within. Buffalo, NY:
Prometheus Books.
Baker, R. A. (1995). Alien dreamtime. The Anomalist, 2, 94-17.
Bartholomew, R. E., Basterfield, K., & Howard, G. S. (1991). UFO abductees and contactees:
Psychopathology or fantasy proneness? Professional Psychology: Research and
Practice, 22, 215-222.
Basterfield, K. (1994). Abductions: The paranormal connection. In A. Pritchard, D. E.
Pritchard, J. E. Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien discussions: Proceedings of
the Abduction Study Conference held at M.I.T (pp. 149-150). Cambridge, MA: North
Cambridge Press.
Baumeister, R. F. (1988). Gender differences in masochistic scripts. Journal of Sex Research,
25(4), 478-499.
Baumeister, R. F. (1989). Masochism and the self. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Blackmore, S. (1998, May/June). Abduction by aliens or sleep paralysis? Skeptical Inquirer,
22(3). 23-28.
Bloecher, T., Clamar, A., & Hopkins, B. (1985). Summary report on the psychological testing
of nine individuals reporting UFO abduction experiences. In Final Report on the
Psychological Testing of UFO "Abductees.” Mount Rainier, MD: Fund for UFO
Research.
105
Bullard, T. E. (1987). UFO abductions: The measure of a mystery. Mount Rainier, MD: Fund
for UFO Research.
Bullard, T. E. (1989). Hypnosis and UFO abductions: A troubled relationship. Journal of UFO
Studies, 1, 3-42.
Bullard, T. E. (1991). Folkloric dimensions of the UFO phenomenon. Journal of UFO
Studies, 3, 1-58.
Bullard, T. E. (1994). The influence of investigators on UFO abduction reports: Results of a
survey. In A. Pritchard, D. E. Pritchard, J. E. Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien
discussions: Proceedings of the Abduction Study Conference held at M.I.T (pp. 571-
619). Cambridge, MA: North Cambridge Press.
Carpenter, J. S. (1997). Encounters: Now and then. MUFON UFO Journal, 353, 18.
Cheyne, J. A. (2005). Sleep paralysis episode frequency and number, types, and structure of
associated hallucinations. Journal of Sleep Research, 14, 319-324.
Clancy, S. A. (2005). Abducted: How people come to believe they were kidnapped by aliens.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Clancy, S. A., McNally, R. J., Schacter, D. L., Lenzenweger, M. F., & Pitman, R. K. (2002).
Memory distortion in people reporting abduction by aliens. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 111(3), 455-461.
Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate
recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17-22.
Forrest, D. V. (2008). Alien abduction: A medical hypothesis. Journal of the American Academy
of Psychoanalysis & Dynamic Psychiatry, 36, 431-442.
Fukuda, K., Miyasita, A., Inugami, M., & Ishihara, K. (1987). High prevalence of isolated sleep
106
paralysis: Kanashibari phenomenon in Japan. Sleep: Journal of Sleep Research & Sleep
Medicine, 10(3), 279-286.
Girard, T. A., & Cheyne, J. A. (2004). Individual differences in lateralisation of hallucinations
associated with sleep paralysis. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition,
9(1), 93-111.
Gotlib, D. (1996). Psychotherapy for the UFO abduction experience. Journal of UFO Studies,
6, 1-23.
Gotlib, D. (1993). FMS update. Bulletin of Anomalous Experience, 4, 11.
Gotlib, D., Appelle, S., Rodeghier, M., & Flamburis, G. (1994). Ethics code for investigation
and treatment of the abduction experience. Journal of UFO Studies, 5, 55-82.
Grosso, M. (1985). The final choice: Playing the survival game. Walpole, NH: Stillpoint.
Haines, R. F. (1994a). Multiple abduction evidence - What's really needed? In A. Pritchard,
D. E. Pritchard, J. E. Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien Discussions: Proceedings
of the Abduction Study Conference held at M.I.T (pp. 240-244). Cambridge, MA:
North Cambridge Press.
Hartmann, E. (1984). The nightmare: The psychology and biology of terrifying dreams. New
York, NY: Basic Books.
Holden, K. J., & French, C. C. (2002). Alien abduction experiences: Some clues from
neuropsychology and neuropsychiatry. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 7(3), 163-178.
Hopkins, B. (1994). The Hopkins image recognition test (HIRT) for children. In A. Pritchard,
D. E. Pritchard, J. E. Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien discussions: Proceedings
of the Abduction Study Conference held at M.I.T (pp. 127-133). Cambridge, MA: North
Cambridge Press.
107
Hopkins, B., Jacobs, D. M., & Westrum, R. (1992). Unusual personal experiences: An
analysis of the data from three national surveys conducted by the Roper organization.
Las Vegas, NV: Bigelow Holding Corporation.
Hough, P., & Rogers, P. (2007). Individuals who report being abducted by aliens: Investigating
the differences in fantasy proneness, emotional intelligence and the big five personality
factors. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 27, 139-161.
Hufford, D. J. (1982). The terror that comes in the night. Philadelphia, PA: University of
Pennsylvania Press.
Hufford, D. J. (2005). Sleep paralysis as spiritual experience. Transcultural Psychiatry, 41 (1),
11-45.
Jacobs, D. M. (1990). The rock and roll theory of UFOs. Journal of UFO Studies, 2, 141-143.
Jacobs, D. M. (1992). Secret life: First-hand documented accounts of UFO abductions. New
York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Jacobs, D. M. (1998). The threat. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Jacobson, E., & Bruno, J. (1994). Narrative variants and major psychiatric illnesses in close
encounter and abduction narrators. In A. Pritchard, D. E. Pritchard, J. E. Mack, P.
Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien discussions: Proceedings of the Abduction Study
Conference held at M.I.T (pp. 304-309). Cambridge, MA: North Cambridge Press.
Johnson, D. A. (1994). Personality characteristics of UFO abductees. In A. Pritchard, D. E.
Pritchard, J. E. Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien discussions: Proceedings of
the Abduction Study Conference held at M.I.T (pp. 316-319). Cambridge, MA: North
Cambridge Press.
Jung, C. G. (1959). Flying saucers: A modern myth of things. New York: Signet.
108
Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien discussions: Proceedings of the Abduction Study Conference
held at M.I.T (pp. 304-309). Cambridge, MA: North Cambridge Press.
Klass, P. J. (1993). Additional comments about the "Unusual Personal Experiences" survey.
Skeptical Inquirer, 17, 145-146.
Kottmeyer, M. (1988). Abduction: The boundary deficit hypothesis. Magonia, 37, 3-7.
Kottmeyer, M. (1989). Gauche encounters: Bad films and the UFO mythos. Unpublished
manuscript.
Laibow, R. L., & Laue, S. (1993). Posttraumatic stress disorder in experienced anomalous
trauma. In J. P. Wilson & B. Raphael (Eds.), International handbook of traumatic
stress syndromes. New York, NY: Plenum.
Lawson, A. H. (1977). What can we learn from the hypnosis of imaginary abductees? MUFON
UFO Symposium Proceedings. Seguin, Texas: Mutual UFO Network.
Lawson, A. H. (1984). Perinatal imagery in UFO abduction reports. Journal of Psychohistory,
12, 211-239.
Lawson, A. H. (1985). UFO abductions or birth memories. Fate, 38, 68-80.
Lynn, S. J., Barnes, S., & Matthews, A. (2009). Hypnosis and forensic science: Legal decisions
and opinions. In C. Edwards (Ed.), Handbook of forensic science. New York, NY:
Wiley.
Lindsay, D S., Hagen, L., Read, J. D., Wade, K. A., & Garry, M. (2004). True photographs and
false memories. Psychological Science, 15, 149-154.
Lynn, S. J., & Kirsch, I. I. (1996). Alleged alien abductions: False memories, hypnosis, and
fantasy proneness. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 151-155.
Lynn, S. J., & Pezzo, M. (1994, August). Close encounters of a third kind: Simulated hypnotic
109
interviews of alien contacts. Presented at the annual convention of the American
Psychological Association, Los Angeles.
Lynn, S. J., & Rhue, J. W. (1988). Fantasy proneness: Hypnosis, developmental antecedents, and
psychopathology. American Psychologist, 43, 35-44.
Mack, J. E. (1994). Abduction: Human encounters with aliens. New York, NY: Charles
Scribner's Sons.
Mack, J. E. (1999). Passport to the cosmos: Human transformations and alien encounters.
United Kingdom: White Crow Books.
Mazzoni, G. (2007). Did you witness demonic possession? A response time analysis of the
relationship between event plausibility and autobiographical beliefs. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 14, 277-281.
Mazzoni, G., Heap, M., & Scoboria, A. (2010). Hypnosis and memory: Theory, laboratory
research, and applications. In S. J. Lynn, J. W. Rhue, & I. Kirsch (Eds.). Handbook of
clinical hypnosis 2nd ed. (pp. 709-742). Washington, DC: APA.
Mazzoni, G. A. L., Loftus, E. F., & Kirsch, I. (2001). Changing beliefs about implausible
autobiographical events: A little plausibility goes a long way. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Applied, 7, 51-59.
Mazzoni, G., & Lynn, S. J. (2006). The use of hypnosis in eyewitness memory: Past and current
issues. In M. Toglia (Ed.), Handbook of eyewitness psychology: Volume 1:Memory for
events. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum.
McNally, R. J., & Clancy, S. A. (2005). Sleep paralysis, sexual abuse, and space alien abduction.
Transcultural Psychiatry, 42(1), 113-122.
McNally, R. J., Lasko, N. B., Clancy, S. A., Macklin, M. L., Pitman, R. K., & Orr, S. P. (2004).
110
Psychophysiological responding during script-driven imagery in people reporting
abduction by space aliens. Psychological Science, 15, 493-497.
Moura, G. (1994). The abduction phenomenon in Brazil. In A. Pritchard, D.E. Pritchard, J.E.
Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.). Alien abductions: Proceedings of the Abduction
Study Conference held at M.I.T (pp. 186-190). Cambridge, MA: North Cambridge
Press.
Newman, L. S. (1997). Intergalactic hostages: People who report abduction by UFOs. Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology, 16, 151-177.
Newman, L. S., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996a). Toward an explanation of the UFO abduction
phenomenon: Hypnotic elaboration, extraterrestrial sadomasochism, and spurious
memories. Psychological Inquiry, 7(2), 99-126.
Newman, L. S., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996b). Not just another false memory: Further thoughts
on UFO abduction narratives. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 185-197.
Nickell, J. (2001). Real-life X-Files: Investigating the paranormal. Lexington, KY: University
Press of Kentucky.
Orne, E. C., Whitehouse, W. G., Dinges, D. F., & Orne, M. T. (1996). Memory liabilities
associated with hypnosis: Does low hypnotizability confer immunity? International
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 44, 354-369.
Otgaar, H., Candel, I., Merckelbach, H., & Wade, K. (2009). Abducted by a UFO: Prevalence
information affects young children’s false memories for an implausible event. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 23, 115-125.
Parnell, J. O., & Sprinkle, R. L. (1990). Personality characteristics of persons who claim UFO
experiences. Journal of UFO Studies, 2, 45-58.
111
Patry, A. L., & Pelletier, L. G. (2001). Extraterrestrial beliefs and experiences: An application of
the theory of reasoned action. The Journal of Social Psychology, 141, 199-217.
Persinger, M. A. (1990). The tectonic strain theory as an explanation for UFO phenomena: A
non-technical review of the research, 1970-1990. Journal of UFO Studies, 2, 105-137.
Porter, S., Yuille, J. C., & Lehman, D. R. (1999). The nature of real, implanted, and fabricated
memories for emotional childhood events: Implications for the recovered memory debate.
Law and Human Behavior, 23(5), 517-537.
Powers, S. M. (1994a). Dissociation in alleged extraterrestrial abductees. Dissociation, 7(1), 44-
50.
Powers, S. M. (1994b). Thematic content analysis of the reports of UFO abductees and close
encounter witnesses: Indications of repressed sexual abuse. Journal of UFO Studies, 5,
35-54.
Randles, J. (1988). Abduction. London, UK: Robert Hale.
Randles, J. (1994a). An experiment to test imaginary versus real abductions. In A. Pritchard, D.
E. Pritchard, J. E. Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien discussions: Proceedings
of the Abduction Study Conference held at M.I.T (pp. 394-395). Cambridge, MA: North
Cambridge Press.
Randles, J. (1994b). An analysis of British abduction cases. In A. Pritchard, D. E. Pritchard, J.
E. Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien discussions: Proceedings of the
Abduction Study Conference held at M.I.T. (pp. 174-176) Cambridge, MA: North
Cambridge Press.
Ring, K. (1992). The omega project: Near-death experiences, UFO encounters, and mind at
large. New York, NY: William Morrow.
112
Ring, K., & Rosing, C. J. (1990). The Omega project: A psychological survey of persons
reporting abductions and other encounters. Journal of UFO Studies, 2, 59-98.
Rodeghier, M. (1994). Psychosocial characteristics of abductees. In Andrea Pritchard, D. E.
Pritchard, J. E. Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp (Eds.), Alien discussions: Proceedings of
the Abduction Study Conference held at M.I.T. (pp. 296-303), Cambridge, MA: North
Cambridge Press.
Rodeghier, M., Goodpaster, J., & Blatterbauer, S. (1991). Psychosocial characteristics of
abductees: Results from the CUFOS abduction project. Journal of UFO Studies, 3, 59-90.
Roediger, H.L., III, & McDermott, K.B. (1995). Creating false memories? Remembering words
not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 21, 803-814.
Sagan, C. (1993, March 7). Are they coming for us? Parade.
Sagan, C. (1995). The demon haunted world: Science as a candle in the dark. New
York, NY: Random Hourse.
Sheaffer, R. (1997, January/February). Implants and gurus are everywhere;Skeptical Inquirer,
21(1). doi: www.csicop.org/si/show/implants_and_gurus_are_everywhere/
Seppa, N. (1997). Wisdom: A quality that may defy age. The APA Monitor, 28(2), 1-9.
Slater, E. (1985). Conclusions on nine psychologicals. In Final Report on the Psychological
Testing of UFO "Abductees." Mount Rainier, MD: Fund for UFO Research.
Spanos, N. P., Burgess, C. A., and Burgess, M. F. (1994). Past life identities, UFO abductions,
and satanic ritual abuse. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
42, 433- 446.
Spanos, N. P., Cross, P. A., Dickson, K., & DuBreuil, S. C. (1993). Close encounters: An
113
examination of UFO experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(4), 624-632.
Stone-Carmen, J. (1994). A descriptive study of people reporting abduction by unidentified
flying objects (UFOs). In A. Pritchard, D. E. Pritchard, J. E. Mack, P. Kasey, & C. Yapp
(Eds.), Alien Discussions: Proceedings of the Abduction Study Conference held at M.I.T
(pp. 309-315). Cambridge, MA: North Cambridge Press.
Strieber, W. (1993, July 5). Appearance on "Larry King Live., Cable News Network.
Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Shafi, M. (2010). Psychology in outerspace: Personality,
individual difference, and demographic predictors of beliefs about extraterrestrial life.
European Psychologist, 15, 220-228.
Swami, V., Furnham, A., Haubner, T., Stieger, S., & Voracek, M. (2009). The truth is out there:
The structure of beliefs about extraterrestrial life among Austrian and British
respondents. Journal of Social Psychology, 149(1), 29-43.
Theroux, P. (1998, June 15). Nurse Wolf. New Yorker, pp. 50-63.
Vallee, J. F. (1969). Passport to Magonia. Chicago, IL: Henry Regnery Co.
Wilson, J. (1990). Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and experienced anomalous trauma
(EAT): Similarities in reported UFO abductions and exposure to invisible toxic
contaminants. Journal of UFO Studies, 2, 1-18.
Wilson, S. C. & Barber, T. X. (1978). The creative imagination scale as a measure of hypnotic
responsiveness: Applications to experimental and clinical hypnosis. American Journal
of Clinical Hypnosis, 20, 235-249.
Wilson, S. C., & Barber, T. X. (1981). Vivid fantasy and hallucinatory abilities in the life
histories of excellent hypnotic subjects ("somnambules"): Preliminary report with
female subjects. In E. Klinger (Ed.), Imagery: Vol. 2. Concepts, results, and
114
applications. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Wilson, S. C. & Barber, T. X. (1983a). Inventory of Childhood Memories and Imaginings.
Framingham, MA: Cushing Hospital.
Wilson, S. C., & Barber, T. X. (1983b). The fantasy-prone personality: Implications for
understanding imagery, hypnosis, and parapsychological phenomena. In A. A. Sheikh
(Ed.). Imagery: Current theory, research, and application (pp. 340-390). New York,
NY: Wiley.
... In many African countries including Ghana, it is believed that people may suddenly be abducted by invisible beings said to be dwarfs, described as being extremely short with peculiar features. The abductees are alleged to be hidden by their captors in the forest for several days to years after which they return with some magical powers [6,7]. This belief may be an attempt to describe dissociative fugue. ...
... The cultural setting and beliefs about this condition may cause many to remain untreated as the origin of symptoms may be attributed to spiritual causes such as a spiritual abduction or "abduction by dwarfs" as observed in the above case [6,7]. The persistence of the condition may be a result of a lack of proper assessment and diagnosis. ...
Article
Full-text available
The rarity and close resemblance to other mental health conditions of dissociative fugue make it difficult to diagnose. Akin to a culture-bound syndrome, most African countries have their local explanation for this unique presentation and therefore people may not seek evidence-based health care but rather may resort to faith-based-treatment which may not give the best results. This is the case of a 39-year-old man who has experienced about five episodes of dissociative fugue over nine years. This case points out the comorbidities associated with the condition. We report this case to increase awareness of the condition and to bring to the fore the need for further studies into psychopathology and the treatment modalities.
... las NDEs solo en ciertas culturas o epocas? mese el caso de la jubilada E. B., quien men durante hipnosis que habfa sido secuestrada j · minada por extraterrestres en un par de ocasi (Appelle, Lynn, y Newman, 2000), lQue pape _ gala hipnosis en esta clase de informes? lScolögicarnente estables las personas que suele estas experiencias? ...
Article
Full-text available
Defino a !as experiencias anömalas como experiencias que o son muy raras (p. ej., sinestesia), o pare poner en tela de juicio nuestra concepciön usual de la realidad (p. ej., experiencias interpretadas co precogniciön), Los principales pioneros de la psicologfa clinica y de la personalidad a finales del siglo consideraron que la nueva ciencia de la psicologfa deberfa incluir el estudio cientffico de estas vivenci • que pueden tener un efecto dramätico en quienes las experimentan. Sin embargo, la desatenciön de la co ciencia por el conductismo radical y un cognitivismo posterior muy restringido marginalizaron este estu · Actualmente, hay la suficiente investigaciön empfrica y sofisticaciön metodolögica para iniciar un estu • serio delas experiencias anomal as dentro de la psicologfa. Hay razones diversas por !as que la psicologfa puede continuar ignorando esta importante area. Descriptores: experiencia anomala, experiencia espiritual, estilo cognitivo. ■ ABSTRACT Anomalous experiences are defined as experiences that are either very rare (e.g., synesthesia), or see challenge our usual view of reality (e.g., experiences interpreted as precognition). The major pioneers clinical and personal ity psychology at the end of the xrx century considered that the new science of psychol should include the scientific study of these experiences, which can have a dramatic effect on their experi Nonetheless, the disregard of consciousness by radical behaviorism, anda too restricted later cognitivi relegated this study to the fringes. Currently, there is enough empirical investigation and methodolo · sophistication to initiate a serious consideration of anomalous experiences within psychology. There various reasons why psychology cannot continue ignoring this important area. Key word: anomalous experience, spiritual experience, cognitive style. Introducci6n Considerese el caso de un transportista que se so-mete a una operaciön cardiaca de emergencia, des-pues de la cual comenta que mientras lo operaban se sintiö flotar y caer por un ttinel que lo llevö a una luz que le hizo sentir amor y tranquilidad (Greyson, 2000). lOcurren estas "experiencias cercanas a la
... As globalization increases, it becomes more difficult to answer such questions definitively, and more work is needed with remote cultures and in relation to disparate cultural traditions to investigate claims of a universal basis for AEs. In this regard, the mere identification of common features or patterns across cultures is no longer enough to support universality assumptions, especially considering the process of globalization as an intense diffuser of beliefs and cultural practices practically across the world, as is the case of alien abduction reports and UFO (Unidentified Flying Object) sightings (Appelle, Lynn, Newman, & Malaktaris, 2014). For example, it is known that in some countries, such as Brazil and Argentina, reports of UFO sightings became more frequent and widespread after the famous Roswell UFO incident in the United States. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research has begun to unveil the cognitive and personality correlates of anomalous experiences (AEs). Unfortunately, cultural differences have received markedly less attention. Questions exist regarding whether the Western description of AEs, as possibly nonordinary or exceptional, is valid across cultures. Scant research on AEs has been conducted in non-English-speaking countries, and few systematic cross-cultural comparisons have been carried out. Our goal is to critically review the scientific literature on AEs in light of cross-cultural research. We outline theoretical issues arising from cultural and cross-cultural investigations of AEs and discuss methodological challenges and shortcomings, followed by specific suggestions to address them. To do so, we review extant findings on psychosocial and cultural aspects of AEs and existing gaps in knowledge about the cultural underpinnings of AEs. We begin by highlighting the cultural and group differences in the report of AEs and the difficulties in isolating the experience from interpretations of the experience. We (a) evaluate the cultural source and the experiential source hypotheses related to AEs based on multiple evidentiary sources ranging from prevalence studies to experimental investigations of these experiences across cultures, (b) outline the implications of cross-cultural research with respect to mental health issues, and (c) conclude with a research summary and proposals for future investigations.
Chapter
There have been numerous reported cases of alien encounters and abductions throughout history. Many individuals claim to have been taken aboard an extraterrestrial spacecraft and subjected to various experiments, procedures, or observations. These experiences have been described by many as life-changing events, causing a dramatic shift in their attitude toward the existence of UFOs and existence in general. However, there is a lack of concrete evidence to support these claims. Some scientists have offered alternative explanations for these experiences, such as sleep paralysis and the possibility of false memories generated through hypnosis.
Article
Full-text available
p>Purpose: Starting from the classic definition of “alien abduction”, the present research, starting from the study published in 2020 on the proposed clinical classification of this particular phenomenon, is aimed at confirming the theoretical assumption of psychopathological origin. Methods: Clinical interview, based on narrative-anamnestic and documentary evidence and the basis of the Perrotta Human Emotions Model (PHEM) concerning their emotional and perceptual-reactive experience, and administration of the battery of psychometric tests published in international scientific journals by the author of this work: 1) Perrotta Integrative Clinical Interviews (PICI-2), to investigate functional and dysfunctional personality traits; 2) Perrotta Individual Sexual Matrix Questionnaire (PSM-Q), to investigate the individual sexual matrix; 3) Perrotta Affective Dependence Questionnaire (PAD-Q), to investigate the profiles of affective and relational dependence; 4) Perrotta Human Defense Mechanisms Questionnaire (PDM-Q), to investigate the defence mechanisms of the Ego. Results: Preliminary results from the interviews and the anamnestic form would suggest that the phenomenon of alien abductions has a greater tendency to occur in the female group, in the adult and mature group (and tends to diminish but not disappear with advancing age) and in the group geographically originating in central-northern Italy (due to lower religious influences but greater openness to the typical contents of ufological and mystery narratives). Moreover, the subsequent results would lead us to deduce with almost total certainty, concerning the selected sample, that the phenomenon of alien abductions has an absolute prevalence in the believing population concerning the existence of paranormal phenomena per se, even in the absence of objective and/or scientific evidence. It is a phenomenon that is almost completely linked (110/112, 98.2%) to a medium-low or not fully educated cultural level. Based on the PICI-2 it emerged that the primary emerging disorder is alternatively the delusional disorder, the dissociative disorder and the narcissistic disorder; followed, as secondary disorders, by the delusional disorder (if it is not considered as primary disorder), the schizoid disorder, the borderline disorder, the obsessive disorder and the psychopathic disorder. The analysis of functional traits also reported the marked dysfunctional tendency of the classes referring to self-control, sensitivity, Ego-Es comparison, emotionality, ego stability, security and relational functionality, confirming here too the marked dysfunctional tendency of the clinical population. According to the PSM-Q, almost 2/3 of the participants (73/112, 65.2%) present a dysfunctional tendency to sexual behaviour and a marked tendency to chronicle feelings of shame in avoidance behaviour or hyposexuality. Furthermore, 100% of the sample of the population surveyed report having suffered significant or serious psychological or physical abuse at a young age, or intra-parental relational imbalances, or in any case a sexual upbringing that was not open and lacking in free communication. According to the PDM-Q, 27.7% (31/112) are affected by affective dependency, with greater emphasis on types I (neurotic), VI (covert narcissist), V (borderline) and III (histrionic), in that order of descent. Finally, the PDM-Q reveals the widespread psychopathological tendency of the functional ego framework for the mechanisms of isolation, denial, regression, reactive formation, denial, projection, removal, withdrawal, instinct, repression and idealisation. Conclusion: This research confirms the psychopathological nature of the alien abduction phenomenon, which deserves to be treated using a psychotherapeutic approach (preferably cognitive-behavioural and/or strategic) and possibly also pharmacological in serious cases, depending on the symptoms manifested and the severity of the morbid condition.</p
Article
Objectives Thousands of people throughout the world are convinced that they have been abducted by aliens. We aimed to assess the emotional reaction to such an implausible event and propose some explanations alternative to severe psychopathology. Methods A total of 19 individuals who reported memories of having been abducted by aliens were compared to a control group of 32 participants. We employed a battery of tests measuring post-traumatic stress disorder, suggestibility, and dissociation within all participants. Results The abductee group showed higher scores in measures of post-traumatic stress disorders and dissociation, but lower in suggestibility. Nevertheless, these differences were statistically significant only in suggestibility. Conclusions The emotional reaction to memories of an implausible experience can be similar to an individual's response to a genuinely traumatic event. Dissociation might be involved in the clarification of some cases. Explanations alternative to psychosis for these testimonies are proposed.
Article
Full-text available
Unidentified Flying Objects (also called UFO) are usually related to the magical beliefs that accept the existence of extraterrestrial beings and alien abductions. Therefore, UFO experiences describe contacts with UFOs, extraterrestrial beings, covert alien visits and abductions. Although science provides enough advances that bet for the existence of life outside the earth, these experiences have been related and equated with paranormal beliefs under an integrative model given the magical content they manifest. The following paper presents a psychosocial study based on the statistical justification of the UFO Experiences Questionnaire (UFO-Q), which examines the underlying dimensions of UFO experiences and beliefs, as well as their social impact on the Spanish-speaking culture. The sample was of non-probabilistic convenience and consisted of 404 subjects selected from the Spanish general population. The Factorial Analyses confirmed that UFO experiences can be represented based on four dimensions: Extraterrestrial Beliefs (C1), Extraterrestrial Experiences (E1), Fearful Extraterrestrial Beliefs (T1) and Form Beliefs (C2). The analysis of items suggested that these beliefs could present two cognitive models: On the one hand, there is the magical-divergent model (irrational thinking), and on the other hand, there is also the critical-divergent (rational thinking). However, the Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analysis questioned both models because they only allowed the identification of the magical-divergent dimension (factor C2). This result invites us to review the validity of the integrative model that associate paranormal contents with extraterrestrial beliefs. Would it be possible that the relationship between both types of beliefs occurred only in the magical-divergent model?
Article
Full-text available
Zusammenfassung-Dieser Artikel gibt eine nach unserer Kenntnis erste transdisziplinäre Über-sicht über das Phänomen Schlafparalyse in deutscher Sprache. Die wichtigsten mit diesem Phäno-men zusammenhängenden Aspekte werden sowohl aus einer medizinischen und psychologischen, als auch aus einer sozialwissenschaftlichen Perspektive vorgestellt. Schlafparalyse wird (vor dem Einschlafen oder nach dem Aufwachen im Wachbewusstsein) als Zustand einer temporären Läh-mung bzw. Bewegungsunfähigkeit erlebt, der in den meisten Fällen von intensiven Halluzinatio-nen sowie Furcht begleitet ist. Nach einer Begriffsklärung werden verschiedene Subtypen und ver-wandte Schlafstörungen erläutert. Danach wird die Phänomenologie der Schlafparalyse mit ihren Hauptmerkmalen Wachbewusstsein, Atonie, Halluzinationen und Furcht detailliert beschrieben. Verbindungen zu anderen außergewöhnlichen Erfahrungen wie luzide Träume/Klarträume, außer-körperliche Erfahrungen und Erfahrungen der Entführung durch Außerirdische werden thema-tisiert. Schließlich werden (neuro-)physiologische und psychologische Theorien zur Ätiologie der Schlafparalyse sowie Therapien und Coping-Strategien vorgestellt. Nicht-materialistische und he-terodoxe Deutungen und Umgangsweisen für Schlafparalyse (beispielsweise der Glaube an Geister oder Dämonen) werden ebenfalls genannt. Schlüsselbegriffe: Schlafparalyse-Parasomnien-außergewöhnliche Erfahrungen-heterodoxe Glaubensvorstellungen-außerkörperliche Erfahrungen-luzider Traum-Abduktionserfahrung Sleep Paralysis Phenomenology-Interpretation-Coping Abstract-To the best of our knowledge, this article provides the first transdisciplinary overview of the phenomenon of sleep paralysis in German language. The most substantial key points are summarized not only from a medical and psychological but also from a socio-scientific perspective. Sleep paralysis is the experience of wakefulness (before falling asleep or after having woken up from sleep) accompanied by temporary paralysis and, in most cases, in-1 Max Fuhrmann, B. A., studierte an der Philipps-Universität Marburg "Vergleichende Kultur-und Re-ligionswissenschaft". Seine Abschlussarbeit (Fuhrmann, 2016) thematisiert den Zusammenhang von Schlafparalyse mit individuellen Glaubensvorstellungen.
Article
Full-text available
p>Starting from the general concept of “alien abduction experience”, the present work focuses on the essential aspects of the disorder defining the clinical and diagnostic contexts, laying the foundations for correct differential diagnosis, without neglecting the neural characteristics elaborated in the scientific community. The discussion concludes with the best therapeutic approaches suggested on the subject, paying particular attention to analyzing the related profiles and those related to the presumed discovery of alien implants in the human body of patients.</p
Article
Ss who reported UFO experiences were divided into those whose experiences were nonintense (e.g., seeing lights and shapes in the sky) and those whose experiences were intense (e.g., seeing and communicating with aliens or missing time). On a battery of objective tests Ss in these 2 groups did not score as more psychopathological, less intelligent, or more fantasy prone and hypnotizable than a community comparison group or a student comparison group. However, Ss in the UFO groups believed more strongly in space alien visitation than did comparison Ss. The UFO experiences of Ss in the intense group were more frequently sleep-related than the experiences of Ss in the nonintense group. Among the combined UFO Ss, intensity of UFO experiences correlated significantly with inventories that assessed proneness toward fantasy and unusual sensory experiences. Implications are discussed.