ArticlePublisher preview available

Note-taking and passage style

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

High-formality and low-formality versions of a passage were read by 120 undergraduate education students who either took notes for a presentation to professionals or to students, or simply read the text. A free-recall test showed superiority for notetakers and for those reading low-formality text. Despite passage style and type of instruction, Ss took essentially verbatim notes, but in recall, informal material was paraphrased significantly more than formal prose. A conditional probability analysis showed that an idea unit was more likely to be recalled if it had been written in notes than if it had only been read. Results support the encoding function of note-taking and its relation to informal prose. (13 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal
of
Educational
Psychology
1981,
Vol.
73,
No.
2,242-250
Copyright
1981
by the
American
Psychological
Association, Inc.
(K)22-Ofi6.VHI/7302-0242$00.7S
Note-Taking
and
Passage Style
Burke
H.
Bretzing
and
Raymond
W.
Kulhavy
Arizona
State
University
High-formality
and
low-formality
versions
of a
passage
were
read
by
under-
graduate
education students
who
either took notes
for a
presentation
to
pro-
fessionals
or to
students,
or
simply
read
the
text.
A
free-recall
test
showed
su-
periority
for
notetakers
and for
those
reading
low-formality
text.
Despite
passage
style
and
type
of
instruction,
individuals
took
essentially
verbatim
notes,
but in
recall,
informal
material
was
paraphrased
significantly
more
than
formal
prose.
A
conditional
probability
analysis
showed
that
an
idea
unit
was
more
likely
to be
recalled
if
it,
had
been
written
in
notes
than
if it had
only
been
read.
These
results
support
the
encoding
function
of
note-taking
and its
rela-
tion
to
informal
prose.
Much
of the
research
on
note-taking
be-
havior
has
centered
on
questions about what
people
remember
from
the
notes themselves
(Bretzing
&
Kulhavy, 1979;
Hartley
&
Davies,
1978). Although such questions
are
important
ones,
we
believe
that
an
effective
analysis
of
note-taking activity must involve
some
differential predictions relating notes
to
both
the
style
and
interpretation
of the
target
information.
In
written instruction, passage style
is
probably
an
important variable
for
deter-
mining
the
content,
of
notes
and the
degree
to
which
a
student
is
able
to use the
notes
to
increase recall.
In the
present study,
we
chose
to
study passages containing essen-
tially
the
same information,
but
written
at
opposite ends
of a
continuum
we
have
la-
beled
formality.
Here,
the
term
formality
refers
to the
same type
of
separation Spiro
(1977)
attempted
to
make when
he
discussed
the
idea
of
integrity
in
prose structure.
Al-
though
our
definitions
are
similar
to
those
of
Spiro,
we
chose
the
formality label because
it is
easier
to
comprehend
for
most people,
and it
also made
our
task
of
instructing
passage
raters
a
simpler
one.
The
pivot
of
our
definition
is the
degree
to
which
new
information
can be
made
to
relate
to
com-
munity
values
of
prior knowledge.
An in-
formal
passage would
be
written
to
allow
maximum
contact
with general information
available
to all or
most readers
in a
given
Requests
for
reprints
should
be
sent
to
Burke
H.
Bretzing,
Department
of
Educational
Psychology,
Ar-
izona
State
University,
Tempe,
Arizona
85281.
subject
population.
For
example,
a
low-
formality
passage would
be
designed
for
communication with learners
who
were well
acquainted with
the
terminology
and ex-
amples used
in the
text.
On the
other hand,
a
highly formal
passage
is
written
to
maxi-
mize
structured communication with
as few
personally identified instances
as
possible.
We
hypothesized
that
both
the
type
and
content
of
notes would vary
as a
function
of
the
formality built into
the
passage.
Our
second variable involved manipulat-
ing
task
demands prior
to the
note-taking-
reading
activity.
Pichert
and
Anderson
(1977)
showed
that
the
perspective
em-
ployed during reading determines
the
con-
tent
of
recall.
When subjects read material
from
a
particular viewpoint, subsequent
re-
call
was
biased.
Thus,
different
schemata
may
be
tapped, resulting
in
altered inter-
pretations
of
text.
We
asked students
to
take
notes designed
to
prepare
them
to ei-
ther deliver
a
lecture
to a
group
of
high
school
students
or to an
assembly
of
profes-
sionals
who
were
familiar
with
the
con-
tent—thus
viewing
the
passage
from
a
dif-
ferent
perspective.
Obviously,
we are
pri-
marily
interested
in the
interaction between
passage style
and
note-taking instructions.
It
seems reasonable
to
argue
that
a
combi-
nation such
as
high formality
and
profes-
sional
orientation
should lead
to
greater
verbatim production,
a
more rigid note-
taking
style,
and
lower overall recall because
of
the
tighter demands
of the
formal/pro-
fessional
mode. Alternately, those learners
who
receive
a
low-formal, student-oriented
treatment seem more
likely
to
produce
a
... This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Austin et al., 2004;Baker & Lombardi, 1985;Bretzing & Kulhavy, 1981;Crawford, 1925;Einstein et al., 1985;Hartley & Marshall, 1974;Howe, 1970;Kiewra et al., 1989Kiewra et al., , 1995Kiewra, DuBois, et al., 1991;Kiewra, Mayer, et al., 1991;Locke, 1977;Luo et al., 2016Luo et al., , 2018Olive & Barbier, 2017;Peverly et al., 2003Peverly et al., , 2007Scerbo et al., 1992;Titsworth, 2004;Titsworth & Kiewra, 2004Note completeness Crawford, 1925Flanigan & Titsworth, 2020;Peverly et al., 2007Proportion or percent correct Bui & McDaniel, 2015Maddox & Hoole, 1975;Peverly et al., 2013;Reddington et al., 2015Note-taking efficiency Einstein et al., 1985Flanigan & Titsworth, 2020;Howe, 1970;Kiewra et al., 1989, 1995Word count Flanigan & Titsworth, 2020Hartley & Marshall, 1974;Horbury & Edmonds, 2020;Howe, 1970;Kiewra et al., 1989;Luo et al., 2018;Marsh & Sink, 2010;Morgan et al., 1988 lecture was scripted so that the instructor discussed the same content, in the same order, for both videos. The delivery of the lecture was the sole manipulation. ...
... Students may benefit from training on effective note-taking strategies that avoid copying the lecture verbatim to increase thoughtful consideration of lecture content. Measuring verbatim strings in students' notes (Bretzing & Kulhavy, 1981;Luo et al., 2018) may be important to consider with the mode in which notes are taken-typed or hand-writtenbecause verbatim note-taking can be more common when notes are typed (Morehead et al., 2019;Mueller & Oppenheimer, 2014;Urry et al., 2021). As well, a valuable direction for future work will be to quantify students' importance criterion and identify factors that influence it. ...
Article
Full-text available
General Audience Summary Students routinely take notes during class as a way of learning information from lectures. Despite the widespread practice of taking notes and relying on them to study for exams, the quality of students’ notes (and their subsequent effects on learning) is unclear. This is partially because assessments of students’ notes have rarely been considered in terms of the choices students make when taking notes. We proposed a two-phase model in which students’ notes are evaluated for (a) identification of concepts from the lecture that should be included in their notes and (b) completeness of students’ notes about the concepts that are included. Students viewed a video-recorded lecture and took notes over the content. Identification of concepts was low, with only about 46% of main ideas and supporting details included in students’ notes. However, students tended to take complete and correct notes for those concepts by including about 86% of the important information about the main ideas and supporting details. The type of content from the lecture (main idea vs. supporting detail) had a larger impact on idea identification than on completeness. The style of lecture delivery (fluent or disfluent) did not impact any measure of note quality or test performance. However, students who watched the fluent lecture, compared to the disfluent lecture, believed that they had learned the content better and also rated the instructor as more effective. Thus, the quality of students’ notes is multifaceted such that the decision to include concepts from a lecture can be measured separately from the completeness of those concepts. These results suggest that students have difficulty identifying concepts to include in notes, and instructional approaches that focus on building skills at identifying information from a lecture could be worthwhile for improving note quality.
... However, results of empirical studies on the benefits of encoding have been mixed (see Kiewra, 1985a;Kobayashi, 2005 for reviews). On the one hand, considerable research has indicated that students who took lecture notes (Barnett, Di Vesta, & Rogozinski, 1981;Bretzing, Kulhavy, & Caterino, 1987;Einstein et al., 1985) or text notes (Bretzing & Kulhavy, 1981;Lahtinen, Lonka, & Lindblom-Ylänne, 1997;Peverly, Brobst, Graham, & Shaw, 2003;Rickards & Friedman, 1978) generally outperformed non-note-takers who merely listened to lectures or read texts on various tasks (e.g., comprehension, recall, retention) in the absence of reviewing notes, supporting the encoding function hypothesis with overall small to modest positive effects (Kobayashi, 2005). On the other hand, a number of other studies have shown no significant difference in performance between note-takers who did not review notes and non-note-takers (e.g., Howe, 1970;Kiewra et al., 1991), or have indicated that taking notes can even interfere with learning (e.g., Peck & Hannafin, 1983). ...
... Overall, a majority of the notes taken by students were verbatim copies or close paraphrases of content presented in the environment, around 20% of the note segments involved introduction of new ideas and information through elaboration, and a very small proportion of notes were metacognitive. This was consistent with previous findings in classrooms that reformulated notes were rarer than verbatim/paraphrased notes (Boch & Piolat, 2005;Bretzing & Kulhavy, 1981). ...
Article
Full-text available
Note-taking is important for academic success and has been thoroughly studied in traditional classroom contexts. Recent advancements of technology have led to more students taking notes on computers, and in different situations than are common in traditional instructional contexts. However, research on computer-based note-taking is still an emerging area, and findings from these studies are mixed. In this exploratory study, we conducted multilevel analysis to comprehensively investigate the relationship between note-taking measures and subsequent student success at science inquiry among middle school students, using two scenarios of an open-ended learning environment named Virtual Performance Assessments. Analysis revealed an advantage for content elaborative note-taking over content reproductive note-taking conditional on the source of notes taken, but other measures were less consistent between the two scenarios. Implications of the findings and limitations of this research are also discussed.
... 問題と目的 大学生の学習方略の中でも,ノート作成は大学で学 ぶための基礎的方略として学習者と指導者の双方に重 視され,よく用いられるものの一つである (Bonner & Holliday, 2006;Van Meter et al., 1994) (Kiewra, 1989;Kobayashi, 2005) (Aiken et al., 1975;Kiewra, 1989;Kobayashi, 2005;Peters, 1972) 。そのため,実際 の講義中のノート作成では,要点の把握や推論などが 十分に行われない傾向がある (Bretzing & Kulhavy, 1981;Chen, 2013;Peper & Mayer, 1986) 。 一方, 「事後の見直し(review) 」がともなう場合に ノート作成が有効であることは比較的安定して報告さ れている (Carter & Van Matre, 1975;Kiewra, 1985;Kobayashi, 2006) 。これは,認知資源が圧迫されない状況で,自分 のペースで学習しなおすことが,情報の統合を促進す るためだと考えられる (Kiewra, 1989) 。このように,講 義中のノート作成と見直しについては研究が蓄積され, 学習者自身による体制化や精緻化が行われることが理 解促進につながることも示されてきた (Kiewra, 1989;Kobayashi, 2006) 3.25(0.12) 3.55(0.14) ...
Article
In the present study, students' perception of their post-lecture note-making strategies and changes in their strategies during a course, and the relations between qualitative characteristics of the students' post-lecture notes and the students' performance on examinations, were investigated. The participants were university students: Study 1, N=171; Study 2, N=114; and Study 3, N=45. The results of Study 1, which was conducted in a lecture course, suggested that the amount of notes written and the frequency of the use of figures predicted the students' test performance, and that the students' perception of the utility and cost of making post-lecture notes differed, depending on whether the notes could be referred to during examinations. The students participating in Study 2 prepared post-lecture notes twice during the exercise course. Their performance on fact problems suggested that the amount of writing and the level of organization of the notes had a positive effect, whereas copying a summary had a negative effect. The level of organization and the copying of summaries also predicted the students' performance on problems requiring application of the material and on tasks requiring explanation. The change in the perception of the post-lecture notes was statistically significant, although the size of the effect was small. In Study 3, the lecturer explicitly explained characteristics of effective post-lecture notes. The degree of organization in the post-lecture notes of the participants in Study 3 predicted their test scores on problems requiring application of the material; their perception of strategies also showed a significant increase.
... This means that activities need to engender an engaged response from the learner. Engaged learning can be generated in activities as simple as taking notes during a lecture (Bretzing & Kulhavy, 1981;Peper & Mayer, 1978, 1986, creating a personal mind map of target information after a lecture has been completed, or, for example, building an infographic that consolidates the information into a sensible graphic display. Engaged responding can also be complex-for example, building a scale model of a timeline leading up to a historical event, writing and producing a homemade documentary, or working in the community on a social problem. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter inventories the essential components necessary to leverage theory and research in the deployment of effective instruction in higher education.
... In addition, the encoding hypothesis (Kiewra, 1989) suggests that a benefit of note-taking can arise from the additional activity of recording materials (at the time of learning), which leads to deeper processing of that information. This view is supported by studies which found that note-takers recalled significantly more target materials than non-note-takers when listening to a lecture (Einstein et al., 1985) and also when reading a passage on paper (Bretzing & Kulhavy, 1981), in the absence of the opportunity to review their notes. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The present study compared the effectiveness of typing on laptops to longhand writing when second language English learners took notes to help learn new vocabulary. The results showed that the benefit for longhand writing over typing was significantly modulated by participants’ expectations about the future availability of their notes: when participants were explicitly told that their notes would not be available to them in the future, there was a significant benefit for handwriting over typing. In contrast, in a ‘cognitive offloading’ condition, in which all participants expected to have full access to their notes in the future, no modality effect was observed. We explain this interaction by suggesting that participants in the typing conditions tended to offload due to strong pre-existing expectations about the pervasive accessibility of information typed into a laptop, and that this tendency to offload reduced future memory performance.
... A learner selects an underlining pen or a highlighting marker in the desired color from the toolbox at the top of the screen. As we described earlier, this function can help the learner understand the text, make inferences, and develop critiques (Bretzing and Kulhavy 1981;Annis 1985;Haenggi and Perfetti 1992;Lonka et al. 1994;Kobayashi 2007Kobayashi , 2014. The learner can underline or highlight portions of the text using the pen or the marker and adjust the length of the underlined and/or highlighted portion by moving the cursors at both ends of the underlined or highlighted sections. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper describes the development of a software program that supports argumentative reading and writing, especially for novice students. The software helps readers create a graphic organizer from the text as a knowledge map while they are reading and use their prior knowledge to build their own opinion as new information while they think about writing their essays. Readers using this software can read a text, underline important words or sentences, pick up and dynamically cite the underlined portions of the text onto a knowledge map as quotation nodes, illustrate a knowledge map by linking the nodes, and later write their opinion as an essay while viewing the knowledge map; thus, the software bridges argumentative reading and writing. Sixty-three freshman and sophomore students with no prior argumentative reading and writing education participated in a design case study to evaluate the software in classrooms. Thirty-four students were assigned to a class in which each student developed a knowledge map after underlining and/or highlighting a text with the software, while twenty-nine students were assigned to a class in which they simply wrote their essays after underlining and/or highlighting the text without creating knowledge maps. After receiving an instruction regarding a simplified Toulmin’s model followed by instructions for the software usage in argumentative reading and writing along with reading one training text, the students read the target text and developed their essays. The results revealed that students who drew a knowledge map based on the underlining and/or highlighting of the target text developed more argumentative essays than those who did not draw maps. Further analyses revealed that developing knowledge maps fostered an ability to capture the target text’s argument, and linking students’ ideas to the text’s argument directly on the knowledge map helped students develop more constructive essays. Accordingly, we discussed additional necessary scaffolds, such as automatic argument detection and collaborative learning functions, for improving the students’ use of appropriate reading and writing strategies.
... For instance, students reported that they record a large amount of the lecture material in their notes. This finding is intriguing because students' notes often contain less than 50% of the information from lecture (e.g., Bretzing & Kulhavy, 1981;Kiewra, 1984;Kiewra & Benton, 1988). The number of college years completed was positively related to students' confidence in their note-taking. ...
Article
Full-text available
Numerous researchers have investigated the factors that influence students’ note-taking. In the present study, we explored whether recent advances in technology are associated with changes in students’ self-reported note-taking and classroom experiences. We administered a survey to a sample of current and former university students to investigate why students take notes, how students take notes, students’ classroom experiences, and whether students’ note-taking and classroom experiences have changed in recent years. Students reported taking notes for a variety of reasons, including to enhance encoding and external storage. Students also reported high confidence in their note-taking ability. The two cohorts provided similar responses regarding why they take notes. Relative to former students, current students reported using technology to take notes more frequently and reported receiving PowerPoint slides from their professors more frequently. Understanding how technology impacts students’ note-taking and learning is an important goal for researchers.
... The handout-notes, thus, points to the material provided by the teacher. We would like to leave out the very interesting (more classical (Bretzing, 1979;Bretzing and Kulhavy, 1981;Brown, 1988) and more modern (Hembrooke and Gay, 2003;Bohay et al., 2011;Mueller and Oppenheimer, 2014)) discussions related to the pulse between the hand-taken or typed notes. We think, as many of the performed studies have shown (Stacy and Cain, 2015;Bui et al., 2013), that the blind verbatim typed notes give less comprehension of concepts than the hand-taken notes, for which a selective process for the written sentences facilitates the medium and long term comprehension of concepts and recalling of data. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Nowadays there are many tools to teach, both analogical and electronic. In our classes we use either tools: slides (and their xerox copies), computer driven presentations, videos, exercise bundles, calendar of the subject, syllabus, blackboard, detailed program of the subject... In the teaching/studying process there are to reference systems: the teacher’s and the students’. In the former, it is the teacher who knows the subject and has designed it, whereas in the latter the students should acquire the competences relative to the module, grade... To that end, the students should follow the teacher’s indications and use the materials we give them directly or indirectly. Unfortunately, the students, in their reference system, and due to the way teachers do our job, do not have any linear narrative threat facilitating the acquisition of the knowledge and competences. In this communication we present a tool, the electronic notes via Jupyter notebooks, which provides the students with a linear narrative threat based on a static initial schema but adaptable to each student, and modifiable and extensible by each student, being executable as well.
Article
There are 3 unique cognitive mechanisms during note taking: generative processing, summarization, and sustained attention. Generative processing is active construction of associations between novel information and prior knowledge and experiences. Summarization forces identification of the most pertinent information to create a coherent synopsis. Sustained attention is selectively concentrating on novel information while ignoring irrelevant distraction. This investigation compared the operation of the 3 cognitive mechanisms in relation to the note‐taking effect—the advantage of note taking when there is no opportunity to review the notes. Experiment 1, through measurement of task‐relevant and task‐irrelevant distraction, showed that sustained attention is positively related and generative processing negatively related to retention. Experiment 2, through an instruction manipulation, showed that generative processing impeded and summarization facilitated retention. Therefore, generative processing cannot account for the note‐taking effect. Instead, these results suggest that summarization and sustained attention are the primary cognitive processes underlying the note‐taking effect. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Poster
Full-text available
College is a critical time when changes in students' attitudes, knowledge, personality characteristics, and self-concepts are affected by their face-to-face and online interactions with educators, peers, and the campus climate (Astin, 1997). The growing use of big data and analytics in higher education has fostered research that supports human judgement in the analysis of information about learning and the application of interventions that can aid students in their development and improve retention rates (Siemens & Baker, 2012). This information is often displayed in the form of learning analytics dashboards (LADs), which are individual displays with multiple visualizations of indicators about learners, their learning activities, and/or features of the learning context both at the individual and group levels (Schwendimann et al., 2017). The information presented in LADs is intended to support students' learning competencies that include metacognitive, cognitive, behavioral, and emotional self-regulation (Jivet et al., 2018). We investigated the impact of a student-facing LAD on students' self-concepts and viewing preferences to address the following questions: What are students' viewing preferences (i.e., for individual vs. comparative performance feedback)? How does viewing performance information affect the development of students' metacognitive skills and self-concepts? And, what are students' perceptions about the usability of LADs? In an end-of-term survey, 111 students at a large research university responded to 10 Likert scale and three open-ended questions. Overall, the students reported understanding the information that was presented to them through the LAD and that it was useful, although some students expressed concerns about its accuracy and wanted more detailed information. Students also reported that they preferred to view comparisons to other students over just viewing their own performance information, and that LAD use increased positive affect about performance. Students also reported that dashboard use affected how much they believed they understood the course material, the time and effort they were willing to put into the course, and that it lessened their anxiety. We concluded that course-specific or program-specific related outcomes may require different LAD design and evaluation approaches, and the nonuse of the LAD may be linked to self-confidence, forgetfulness, and a lack of innovative dashboard features. Our study was limited by the analysis of survey data (without trace data), and the sample size. This research contributes to the literature on student-facing learning analytics dashboards (LADs) by investigating students' reasons for interacting with dashboards, their viewing preferences, and how their interactions affect their performance and tying these insights to educational concepts that were a part of the LAD design. Further research is needed to determine whether presenting students with the option to turn on the dashboard for any or all of their courses over the course of the semester is important,
Article
Full-text available
In 2 experiments, 176 undergraduates read stories from 1 of 2 directed perspectives or no directed perspective. Results show that an idea's significance in terms of a given perspective determined whether the idea would be learned and, independently, whether it could be recalled 1 wk later. These results are interpreted to mean that alternative high-level schemata can provide frameworks for assimilating a text, perhaps by providing "slots" for different types of information. Later, the schema from which an instantiated memorial representation of a passage was constructed may furnish the retrieval plan for recovery of detailed information. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Includes bibliographical references (leaves 63-66)
Article
Full-text available
Pretested 120 students on a battery of personality tests, including the Rokeach Dogmatism Scale and Internal-External Control Scale. Ss listened to a set of 3 5-min passages with 4 orthogonally crossed variables: position of the criterion passage on an imaginery scientific system in the set, note taking while listening, rehearsal immediately after listening, and testing. A free-recall test scored for number of words and ideas, and a multiple-choice test were then administered. It was found that there were more words generated and higher multiple-choice test scores when the study interval was used for review than for other activities. The number of ideas recalled was favorably influenced by note taking, rehearsal, and testing. There were no significant effects due to position of the passage in the set. Significant correlations were obtained between performance and the individual difference variables of anxiety and tolerance of ambiguity. A significant interaction between social desirability and performance was obtained for certain treatments. Implications for a minitheory of listening and note taking are discussed. (20 ref.)
Article
In meaningful learning tasks the acquisition process is influenced by the way in which the individual learner interprets and encodes the material. Early attempts to recall written materials largely determine subsequent retention, even when the learner's errors are corrected. To provide an indication of how information was interpreted and encoded by learners, adult students were asked to write notes on a meaningful prose extract they heard, and they were later asked to attempt recall. Whereas the meaningful items recorded in a subject's notes had a .34 probability of recall one week later, items not recorded in notes were recalled on only .047 of occasions, suggesting that the notes learners make provide a useful indication of the products of individual encoding processes In meaningful verbal learning and memory.
Article
High school students completed both multiple-choice and constructed response exams over an 845-word narrative passage on which they either took notes or underlined critical information. A control group merely read the text In addition, half of the learners in each condition were told to expect either a multiple-choice or constructed response test following reading. Overall, note takers showed superior posttest recall, and notetaking without test instructions yielded the best group performance. Notetaking also required significantly more time than the other conditions. Underlining for a multiple-choice test led to better recall than underlining for a constructed response test. Although more multiple-choice than constructed response items were remembered. Test Mode failed to interact with the other factors.
Article
The present paper presents a series of studies showing that relevant contextual knowledge is a prerequisite for comprehending prose passages. Four studies are reported, each demonstrating increased comprehension ratings and recall scores when Ss were supplied with appropriate information before they heard test passages. Supplying Ss with the same information subsequent to the passages produced much lower comprehension ratings and recall scores. Various explanations of the results are considered, and the role of topics in activating cognitive contexts is discussed.
Article
Four levels of notetaking (summary, paraphrase, verbatim, and letter search) were used to control depth of processing of a prose passage with 180 high school students, who then either reviewed their notes or read an interpolated text. A separate control group took no notes. On immediate and delayed post-tests, post hoc analyses with the depth (notetaking) condition showed the following ranking: summary = paraphrase > control = verbatim > letter search. A paraphrase notes × review × test-position interaction was significant, indicating that less forgetting occurred on a delayed post-test when students reviewed their paraphrase notes than when they read an interpolated text. Analysis of reading times showed that the additional time required for notetaking was only worthwhile when meaningful notes were taken.
Article
Thesis (Ph. D.)--Arizona State University, 1974. Vita. Includes bibliographical references (leaves [69]-72).