ArticlePDF Available

Rediscovering the Later Version of Maslow's Hierarchy of Needs: Self-Transcendence and Opportunities for Theory, Research, and Unification

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The conventional description of Abraham Maslow's (1943, 1954) hierarchy of needs is inaccurate as a description of Maslow's later thought. Maslow (1969a) amended his model, placing self-transcendence as a motivational step beyond self-actualization. Objections to this reinterpretation are considered. Possible reasons for the persistence of the conventional account are described. Recognizing self-transcendence as part of Maslow's hierarchy has important consequences for theory and research: (a) a more comprehensive understanding of worldviews regarding the meaning of life; (b) broader understanding of the motivational roots of altruism, social progress, and wisdom; (c) a deeper understanding of religious violence; (d) integration of the psychology of religion and spirituality into the mainstream of psychology; and (e) a more multiculturally integrated approach to psychological theory. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Content may be subject to copyright.
Rediscovering the Later Version of Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs:
Self-Transcendence and Opportunities for Theory, Research,
and Unification
Mark E. Koltko-Rivera
New York University and Professional Services Group, Inc.
The conventional description of Abraham Maslow’s (1943, 1954) hierarchy of needs is
inaccurate as a description of Maslow’s later thought. Maslow (1969a) amended his
model, placing self-transcendence as a motivational step beyond self-actualization.
Objections to this reinterpretation are considered. Possible reasons for the persistence
of the conventional account are described. Recognizing self-transcendence as part of
Maslow’s hierarchy has important consequences for theory and research: (a) a more
comprehensive understanding of worldviews regarding the meaning of life; (b) broader
understanding of the motivational roots of altruism, social progress, and wisdom; (c) a
deeper understanding of religious violence; (d) integration of the psychology of religion
and spirituality into the mainstream of psychology; and (e) a more multiculturally
integrated approach to psychological theory.
Keywords: Maslow, motivation, self-transcendence, worldview, terrorism
Abraham H. Maslow (1908 –1970) was argu-
ably one of the most important psychologists of
modern times. In a recent survey, Maslow was
found to be the 14th-most-frequently cited
psychologist in introductory psychology text-
books; on the basis of various indicators, some
researchers proclaimed Maslow as the 10th
most eminent psychologist of the 20th century
(Haggbloom et al., 2002).
The hierarchy of human needs outlined by
Maslow (1943, 1954) is one of his most en-
during contributions to psychology. However,
the standard textbook version of this hierar-
chy is inaccurate as a reflection of Maslow’s
later descriptions of his motivational theory.
In this article, I describe Maslow’s later state-
ment of his theory and suggest some potential
reasons why an inaccurate version of the the-
ory has been maintained for more than 3
decades in psychology texts; I then point out
important implications that Maslow’s later
theory statement has for theory and research
in contemporary personality and social
psychology.
I have three objectives here. First, I wish to
correct the way that Maslow’s theory is de-
picted in current textbooks; accurate description
of a theory is a prerequisite to its scientific
testing. Second, I wish to promote theory and
research efforts regarding motivational self-
transcendence (described below); I believe that
this construct has important behavioral implica-
tions. Third, I wish to focus attention on moti-
vational self-transcendence, a construct that
cuts across psychological specialty areas, as a
means to further unification within disciplinary
psychology.
It is also important to indicate what my ob-
jectives are not. I am not primarily interested
here with any of the following: empirical evi-
dence regarding Maslow’s theory; critical ap-
praisal of Maslow’s theory; comparisons be-
tween Maslow’s theory and other motivational
theories; attempts to revise Maslow’s scheme in
light of other theoretical, rational, or empirical
Mark E. Koltko-Rivera, Department of Applied Psychol-
ogy, New York University, and Research Department, Pro-
fessional Services Group, Inc., Winter Park, Florida.
An early version of some material presented in this article
was presented at the 104th Annual Convention of the Amer-
ican Psychological Association, Toronto, Ontario, Canada,
August 1996. I thank Kathleen Schmid Koltko-Rivera for
helpful comments on earlier versions of this article and
Sonya Grover for assistance in obtaining research materials.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed
to Mark E. Koltko-Rivera, Research Department, Professional
Services Group, Inc., P.O. Box 3390, Winter Park, FL 32790-
3390. E-mail: mark@psg-fl.com or koltkorivera@yahoo.com
Review of General Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 10, No. 4, 302–317 1089-2680/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.10.4.302
302
considerations. Although these issues are wor-
thy of attention, they are beyond the scope of
this article. Nor would I wish to give the im-
pression that what Maslow had to say about
motivational self-transcendence is anything but
a beginning, something for others to develop.
The Popular Misconception of Maslow’s
Motivational Hierarchy
Maslow’s motivational scheme, as originally
construed, is depicted in the bottom five entries
in Table 1 (i.e., physiological or survival needs
through self-actualization; Maslow, 1943,
1954). Maslow posited that human needs are
arranged in a hierarchy:
It is quite true that man lives by bread alone—when
there is no bread. But what happens to man’s desires
when there is plenty of bread and when his belly is
chronically filled?
At once other (and “higher”) needs emerge and
these, rather than physiological hungers, dominate the
organism. And when these in turn are satisfied, again
new (and still “higher”) needs emerge and so on. This
is what we mean by saying that the basic human needs
are organized into a hierarchy of relative prepotency.
(Maslow, 1943, p. 375)
In addition to physiological needs, Maslow
posited needs for safety, belongingness and
love, esteem, and self-actualization, in ascend-
ing order on the hierarchy. He described self-
actualization this way:
It refers to the person’sdesire for self-fulfillment,
namely, to the tendency for him to become actualized
in what he is potentially.
The specific form that these needs will take will of
course vary greatly from person to person. In one
individual it may take the form of the desire to be an
ideal mother, in another it may be expressed athleti-
cally, and in still another it may be expressed in paint-
ing pictures or in inventions. (Maslow, 1943, pp. 382–
383)
This formulation still stands as a strong state-
ment regarding the structure of human motiva-
tion. By the time of Maslow’s death, many
studies had been published about the needs hi-
erarchy (Roberts, 1972). The hierarchy attracted
scholarly attention following Maslow’s death
(e.g., Lester, Hvezda, Sullivan, & Plourde,
1983) and continues to attract research attention
today. For example, Chulef, Read, and Walsh
(2001) found broad support for Maslow’s the-
ory in their research into the hierarchical struc-
ture underlying human goals. Many undergrad-
uate texts in psychology perpetuate this version
of the model, with self-actualization at the top
of the hierarchy (e.g., Atkinson, Atkinson,
Smith, Bem, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1996; Hall,
Lindzey, & Campbell, 1998; Larsen & Buss,
2002; Mischel, 1999; Myers, 2003; Pervin &
John, 2001). However, this description was not
Table 1
A Rectified Version of Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs
Motivational level Description of person at this level
Self-transcendence Seeks to further a cause beyond the self
a
and to experience
a communion beyond the boundaries of the self through
peak experience.
b
Self-actualization Seeks fulfillment of personal potential.
Esteem needs Seeks esteem through recognition or achievement.
Belongingness and love needs Seeks affiliation with a group.
Safety needs Seeks security through order and law.
Physiological (survival) needs Seeks to obtain the basic necessities of life.
Note. The earliest and most widespread version of Maslow’s hierarchy (based on Maslow,
1943, 1954) includes only the bottom five motivational levels (thus excluding self-
transcendence). A more accurate version of the hierarchy, taking into account Maslow’s later
work (especially Maslow, 1969a) and his private journal entries (Maslow, 1979, 1982),
includes all six motivational levels.
a
This may involve service to others, devotion to an ideal (e.g., truth, art) or a cause (e.g.,
social justice, environmentalism, the pursuit of science, a religious faith), and/or a desire to
be united with what is perceived as transcendent or divine.
b
This may involve mystical experiences and certain experiences with nature, aesthetic
experiences, sexual experiences, and/or other transpersonal experiences, in which the person
experiences a sense of identity that transcends or extends beyond the personal self.
303MASLOW AND SELF-TRANSCENDENCE
Maslow’s final statement concerning the moti-
vational hierarchy.
Maslow’s Description of a Motivational
Step Beyond Self-Actualization
Maslow developed compelling doubts about
self-actualization’s suitability as a motivational
capstone; these doubts were first related to the
phenomena of peak experiences and their atten-
dant cognitive activity. Maslow described peak
experiences, which include such phenomena as
mystical experiences, aesthetic experiences,
emotional experiences involving nature, and so
forth; he considered peak experiences in several
papers in the late 1950s and early 1960s, some
of which appeared in his hugely popular collec-
tion Toward a Psychology of Being (Maslow,
1959/1999a, 1961/1999b; 1959/1999c; see also
Maslow, 1962a, 1962b, 1963, 1964). Maslow
addressed the motivational significance of peak
experiences in a series of lectures in the early
1960s, later published as Religions, Values, and
Peak Experiences (Maslow, 1964/1970). He
called the special cognitive activity that attends
such phenomena “Being-cognition,” or “B-
cognition” for short.
Originally, Maslow thought that Being-
cognition was the province of self-actualization,
although in a very paradoxical way: Peak expe-
riences often led the self-actualizing individual
to transcend the personal concerns of the very
self that was being actualized. As Maslow put it,
As he that is, the person in the peak experiencesgets
to be more purely and singly himself he is more able to
fuse with the world, with what was formerly not-self,
for example, the lovers come closer to forming a unit
rather than two people, the I-Thou monism becomes
more possible, the creator becomes one with his work
being created, the mother feels one with her child.
That is, the greatest attainment of identity, auton-
omy, or selfhood is itself simultaneously a transcend-
ing of itself, a going beyond and above selfhood. The
person can then become relatively egoless. (Maslow,
1961/1999b, p. 117, footnotes omitted)
In conclusionI wish to underscore one main par-
adox I have dealt with above . . . which we must face
even if we don’t understand it. The goal of identity
(self-actualization . . .) seems to be simultaneously an
end-goal in itself, and also a transitional goal, a rite of
passage, a step along the path to the transcendence of
identity. This is like saying its function is to erase
itself. Put the other way around, if our goal is the
Eastern one of ego-transcendence and obliteration, of
leaving behind self-consciousness and self-observa-
tion, . . . then it looks as if the best path to this goal for
most people is via achieving identity, a strong real self,
and via basic-need-gratification. (Maslow, 1961/
1999b, p. 125)
Several points in the preceding quotation are
noteworthy. First, Maslow was puzzled by the
situation that he described and was not shy about
admitting that his thought was still under devel-
opment concerning the relationship between self-
actualization and self-transcendence. Second,
Maslow recognized a paradox—at least an appar-
ent contradiction—in combining self-actualization
and self-transcendence. Third, Maslow recognized
a sequence: Self-actualization preceded self-
transcendence. One can see in these points the
prefigurement of his later thought.
After his 1961 paper, Maslow began to think
that Being-cognition characterized a different
motivational level than self-actualization. At
least by October 1966, as shown by his unpub-
lished critique of self-actualization theory
(Maslow, 1996), Maslow thought that “self-
actualization is not enough” (p. 31) for a full
picture of the optimally functioning human be-
ing. Maslow came to a clearer sense of what
was missing during the following year. As he
recorded in his private journal in an entry dated
May 28, 1967,
All sorts of insights. One big one about SA self-
actualizationstuff, brought on, I think, mostly by my
deep uneasiness over popular pressarticles on the
topic. . . . I realized I’d rather leave it behind me. Just
too sloppy & too easily criticizable. Going thru my
notes brought this unease to consciousness. It’s been
with me for years. Meant to write & publish a self-
actualization critique, but somehow never did. Now I
think I know why. I think I had useda hidden,
unconscious hence mistakencriterion of selection
for examples of self-actualizationbeyond health.
(Maslow, 1979, Vol. 2, p. 794; Maslow, 1982, p. 204)
Maslow here surmised that, in describing self-
actualization, he had contaminated that construct
with notions that belonged to another, as-yet-
unnamed motivational level. The contaminant was
Being-cognition, which properly belonged some-
where on a level above self-actualization on the
hierarchy (i.e., beyond health”). In this journal
entry, Maslow related an insight: He had mistak-
enly put forth, as examples of self-actualization,
people who seemed to exhibit Being-cognition,
even though these people had gone beyond self-
actualization and operated from a higher state of
motivation:
304 KOLTKO-RIVERA
Why did I get so excited over Arthur E. Morgan
1
just
from reading his book—so sure he was a self-
actualizing person? It’s because he was using the B-
language! What I’ve done was pick B-people i.e.,
mistakenly, as examples of self-actualization!Inad-
dition to all the other overt and conscious criteria for
defining self-actualizers. People in the B-realm using
B-language, the awakened, the illuminated, the “high-
plateau” people who normally B-cognize and who
have the B-values very firmly and actively in hand—
even tho not consciously.... There are plenty of
“healthy” people & even self-actualizing people . . .
who are far from B-realm and from unitive perception.
Idid read into my selectees for healthy self-
actualizationa criterion beyond “health.” (Maslow,
1979, Vol. 2, pp. 794 –795; Maslow, 1982, p. 204).
In this journal entry, Maslow also reflected
on his previous depiction of some American
presidents as self-actualizers: “Eisenhower. He
does fit. So does Truman. . . . And yet they are
clearly not B-people” (Maslow, 1979, Vol. 2, p.
794; Maslow, 1982, p. 204). Two weeks later,
Maslow related this insight in some detail to the
personologist Harry Murray. As Maslow noted
in his journal entry of June 11, 1967,
I told him i.e., Murrayof my new discovery of the
difference between Eisenhower-Truman SA and the
health-beyond-health of the B-person. The B-person
may be more symptom-loaded and have more value
pathology
2
than the symptom-free “healthies.” Maybe
one is symptom-free only by virtue of not knowing or
caring about the B-realm, never having experienced the
B-realm in the highest peaks (now that must be
changed also; must separate Eisenhower-Truman-type
peaks from those with full cognition of the B-realm).
Having value-pathology symptoms is “higher” (&
B-healthier?) than being symptom-free. One can get
fixated at Eisenhower-Truman SA level of health and
nonillness & then be perfectly content, happy, . . .
without even being aware of the B-realm in an expe-
riential way....Ifonetries to transcend healthy SA of
the Eisenhower-Truman level, then troubles (of the
highest type) begin. Value pathologies can be a very
high achievement. And one can respect profoundly
those in whom one can see—through the symptoms of
frustrated idealism—the beautiful B-realm that they
are reaching for and may therefore get to.
The ones who are struggling & reaching upward
really have a better prognosis than the ones who rest
perfectly content at the SA level.
(I’ve really been touting value pathology & singing
its praises!) (Maslow, 1979, Vol. 2, pp. 798 –799; first
paragraph also in Maslow, 1982, p. 206)
Here Maslow made an important distinction:
one could be self-actualizing and “healthy,” yet
still not experience Being-cognition, which char-
acterizes certain peak/mystical/transcendent expe-
riences. Those who were satisfied with self-
actualization without Being-cognition were, ac-
cording to Maslow, at a lower stage of
motivational development than those who were
motivated to seek experiences of Being-cognition.
Over the next 3 months, Maslow refined
these insights into a public presentation. On
September 14, 1967, in San Francisco, Maslow
delivered a public lecture, titled “The Farther
Reaches of Human Nature,” in which he de-
scribed the higher levels of his by-then familiar
needs hierarchy; he then took a large step fur-
ther:
The major emphasis in Humanistic psychology rests on
the assumptions regarding “higher needs.” . . . These
higher human needs are . . . biological, and I speak
here of love, the need of love, for friendship, for
dignity, for self-respect, for individuality, for self-
fulfillment, and so on.
If however, these needs are fulfilled, a different
picture emerges. There are people who do feel loved
and who are able to love, who do feel safe and secure
and who do feel respected and who do have self-
respect. If you study these people and ask what moti-
vates them, you find yourself in another realm. This
realm is what I have to call transhumanistic,
3
meaning
that which motivates, gratifies, and activates the fortu-
nate, developed, i.e., alreadyself-actualizing person.
These people are motivated by something beyond the
basic needs. The . . . point of departure, into this
transhumanistic realm comes when they answer the
following kind of questions: “What are the moments
which give you . . . the greatest satisfaction? . . . What
are the moments of reward which make your work and
your life worthwhile?”
The answers to those questions were in terms of ulti-
mate verities....For example,truth, goodness, beauty
. . . and so on. What this amounts to is that this third i.e.,
humanisticpsychology is giving rise to a fourth,
1
Arthur Ernest Morgan (1878 –1975) was president of
Antioch College and an early proponent of what might now
be called humanistic education. It is unclear which of Mor-
gan’s many books is meant in Maslow’s journal entry, but
possibilities include Morgan (1946), Morgan (1957), or a
prepublication version of Morgan (1968).
2
Value pathology, for Maslow, was another term for
what he called metapathologies. These are “the spiritual-
existential ailments that result from the persistent depriva-
tion of metaneeds i.e., the higher needs in Maslow’s hier-
archy—the lack of fulfillment of metamotivations. They
include cynicism, apathy, boredom, loss of zest, despair,
hopelessness, a sense of powerlessness, and nihilism”
(Hoffman, 1996, p. 206).
3
What Maslow here called “transhumanistic” he later
termed transpersonal (Koltko-Rivera, 1998). Both terms
refer to a motivational state in which the person seeks
something beyond personal benefit, for example, the fur-
therance of some greater cause, union with a power beyond
the self, and/or service to others as an expression of iden-
tification beyond the personal ego. This motivational state
expresses a need for self-transcendence.
305MASLOW AND SELF-TRANSCENDENCE
“transhumanistic psychology” dealing with transcendent
experiences and transcendent values.
The fully developed (and very fortunate) human
being working under the best conditions tends to be
motivated by values which transcend his self. They are
not selfish anymore in the old sense of that term.
Beauty is not within one’s skin nor is justice or order.
One can hardly class these desires as selfish in the
sense that my desire for food might be. My satisfaction
with achieving or allowing justice is not within my
own skin . . . . It is equally outside and inside: there-
fore, it has transcended the geographical limitations of
the self. Thus one begins to talk about transhumanistic
psychology. (Maslow, 1969a, pp. 3– 4)
Maslow here noted that some individuals have
gone beyond even self-actualization as a salient
motivation. Such individuals arrive at the top of
Maslow’s new hierarchy of motivation with a
strong motive toward self-transcendence. That
is, such individuals seek a benefit beyond the
purely personal and seek communion with the
transcendent, perhaps through mystical or
transpersonal experiences; they come to iden-
tify with something greater than the purely in-
dividual self, often engaging in service to oth-
ers. (As Maslow put it in his unpublished Oc-
tober 1966 paper, “the good of other people
must be invoked” Maslow, 1996, p. 31. For a
further discussion of the meaning of transcen-
dence for Maslow, see Maslow 1969/1993c.)
This represented a major course change for
Maslow. In a paper published shortly before
Maslow gave his “Farther Reaches” lecture (al-
though likely composed at least a year before
that lecture), Maslow had written about how
self-actualizing individuals were motivated by
metamotivations, devoting themselves to callings
or vocations “beyond themselves” (Maslow,
1967/1993a, p. 291)—that is, devoting them-
selves to aspects of self-transcendence. Yet, in
the “Farther Reaches” lecture, Maslow clearly
differentiated self-actualization from self-tran-
scendence.
Maslow, who had helped to form humanistic
psychology as a distinct approach within psychol-
ogy, went on to help found the Journal of
Transpersonal Psychology. He defined transper-
sonal psychology as a separate “force” within
psychology, differing from the humanistic as self-
transcendence differs from self-actualization.
Maslow pondered making this distinction the
topic for his address as president of the American
Psychological Association (APA). As he wrote in
his journal almost a year after his “Farther
Reaches” presentation (entry of August 25, 1968),
Possible beginning for presidential address: “I will
choose, as a strategy of presentation, to begin with a
vast oversimplification & then later fill in the details,
struggle with the complexities, and so forth.
Let me call behavioristic psychology the first
psychology, Freudian psychology the 2nd, and human-
istic psychology the 3rd. And then what I think of as a
real possibility, what I am fascinated with, is the psy-
chology of transcendence & of ends, the transhuman or
transpersonal (really should be “transcendent”)psy-
chology the 4th psychology.(Maslow, 1979,
Vol. 2, p. 1059; Maslow, 1982, p. 267)
Maslow, recuperating from a heart attack, never
delivered an APA presidential address (Hoff-
man, 1999), but he did prepare a different paper
for the American Psychologist, a sort of
“would-be” presidential address, in which he
stated, “I am Freudian and I am behavioristic
and I am humanistic, and as a matter of fact I
am developing what might be called a fourth
psychology of transcendence as well” (Maslow,
1969b, p. 724).
In summary, Maslow identified a construct,
self-transcendence, that went beyond self-
actualization in his motivational hierarchy; he
then helped to establish a corresponding per-
spective, transpersonal psychology, that was in-
tended to go beyond the perspective of human-
istic psychology as originally construed, with a
corresponding professional journal. Thus, the
typical textbook version of Maslow’s hierarchy
of needs is seriously inaccurate as a reflection of
Maslow’s later formulation of theory. A recti-
fied version of Maslow’s hierarchy includes the
top line of Table 1, self-transcendence.
The implications of this change in Maslow’s
thought must not be underestimated. The earlier
model positions the highest form of motiva-
tional development at the level of the well-
adjusted, differentiated, and fulfilled individual
self or ego. The later model places the highest
form of human development at a transpersonal
level, where the self/ego and its needs are tran-
scended. This represents a monumental shift in
the conceptualization of human personality and
its development. At the level of self-actualiza-
tion, the individual works to actualize the indi-
vidual’s own potential; there is thus, at least
potentially, a certain self-aggrandizing aspect to
this motivational stage, as there is with all the
stages below it in Maslow’s hierarchy. At the
level of self-transcendence, the individual’s
own needs are put aside, to a great extent, in
favor of service to others and to some higher
306 KOLTKO-RIVERA
force or cause conceived as being outside the
personal self. Certainly the image of the best-
developed human being that emerges from
Maslow’s hierarchy is very different, depending
on which of these two stages is placed at the top
of the motivational hierarchy.
Objections to This Reinterpretation of
Maslow
Some scholars studying Maslow’s work have
come to very different conclusions than I have.
For example, Daniels (1982), following a close
study of many of Maslow’s published writings,
has taken the position that Maslow never devel-
oped a final coherent theory of self-actualization
and that Maslow described self-actualization in
ways that intertwined it deeply with self-
transcendence. I agree that Daniels has pointed
out several unresolved theoretical issues involv-
ing Maslow’s theory of self-actualization. How-
ever, concerning a supposed intertwining of
self-actualization and self-transcendence in
Maslow’s thought, I must note that Daniels re-
viewed and cited neither Maslow’s pivotal
“Further Reaches” talk and publication
(Maslow, 1969a) nor Maslow’s posthumously
published journals (Maslow, 1979, 1982); these
sources, which I quote extensively earlier in this
article, demonstrate conclusively that although
Maslow initially conflated self-actualization
and self-transcendence, during the last 3 years
of his life he came to consider the two con-
structs as quite distinct.
It may also be claimed that Maslow’s writing
subsequent to the “Farther Reaches” lecture
seemed still to conflate self-actualization and
self-transcendence, suggesting that Maslow
never actually settled on the addition of a sixth
stage to his formal model. For example, in his
famous “Theory Z” paper, Maslow stated,
I have recently found it more and more useful to
differentiate between two kinds (or better, degrees) of
self-actualizing people, those who were clearly
healthy, but with little or no experience of transcen-
dence, and those in whom transcendent experiencing
was important and even central.
It is unfortunate that I can no longer be theoretically
neat at this level. I find not only self-actualizing per-
sons who transcend, but also nonhealthy people, non-
self-actualizers who have important transcendent ex-
periences. It seems to me that I have found some
degree of transcendence in many people other than
self-actualizing ones. (Maslow, 1969/1993b, p. 270)
There is a subtle but important distinction to be
made here. In this passage, Maslow noted the
mutual orthogonality of self-actualization and the
experience of transcendence: Self-actualizing in-
dividuals may or may not have peak/transcendent
experiences, and individuals who have peak/
transcendent experiences may or may not be self-
actualizing. However confusing this state of af-
fairs may have been to Maslow, it has no bearing
on the issue of a separate motivational stage for
self-transcendence. The crucial issue is the domi-
nant motivation at work in the individual’s life.
All individuals experience hunger; however, hun-
ger is the defining experience only for individuals
centered on the physiological or survival level of
Maslow’s hierarchy of needs. Similarly, many in-
dividuals have transcendent experiences, regard-
less of their position on Maslow’s hierarchy. This
fact has nothing to do with the matter of a separate
stage for self-transcendence. Maslow no more
confined the experience of transcendence to the
stage of self-transcendence needs than he confined
the experience of hunger to the motivational stage
where physiological needs predominate.
In sum, for Maslow the experience of tran-
scendence is one thing; having one’s motiva-
tional life be centered at the level of self-
transcendence is entirely another. Only the lat-
ter is what I shall refer to hereafter as
motivational self-transcendence or self-tran-
scendence as a motivational status.
Why Has the Misconception Persisted?
Why has the popular misconception of
Maslow’s theory persisted for so long? Why
does it pervade the literature so thoroughly?
These questions arise because, even though iso-
lated individuals have noted self-transcendence
as a neglected step within Maslow’s motiva-
tional hierarchy (e.g., Koltko-Rivera, 1998;
Roberts, 1982), there is no mention of self-
transcendence as a motivational status distinct
from self-actualization in almost any textbook
treatment of Maslow’s theory. There are a few
exceptions (e.g., Battista, 1996; Fadiman &
Frager, 2002; Zimbardo & Gerrig, 1999); in
addition, some texts describe self-actualization
as embracing self-transcendence (e.g., Ryck-
man, 2000, p. 439). By far, however, self-
transcendence is not described in textbooks as a
separate motivational step in the hierarchy. In-
deed, this inaccuracy has not even been cor-
307MASLOW AND SELF-TRANSCENDENCE
rected in the third, posthumous edition of Mo-
tivation and Personality (Maslow, 1987). At
least three factors may have played a part in
propagating this misconception.
First, there was little opportunity for Maslow
to publicize his amended theory before his
death. On July 8, 1966, he learned that he had
been elected to the presidency of APA
(Maslow, 1979, Vol. 2, p. 739; Maslow, 1982,
p. 192). As noted above, although he began
studying peak experiences in the mid-1950s, his
sense of the limitations of the self-actualization
construct coalesced in May and June 1967, just
a few months before beginning his term as APA
president. In the fall of 1967, more or less
simultaneously with the beginning of his presi-
dential term, he began to occupy a 1-year Ford
Foundation fellowship (Hoffman, 1999, p. 283),
during which term (in September) he gave the
“Farther Reaches” talk as his first public pre-
sentation on self-transcendence as a successor
step to self-actualization. After a few months
lecturing at Harvard, MIT, and Yale on his
Theory Z management approach and after ap-
pearing at a December 1967 conference on tran-
scendence, Maslow was hospitalized in inten-
sive care following a serious coronary event.
Much of 1968 was spent in convalescence or in
working to involve psychologists in the cause of
civil rights (Hoffman, 1999), although Maslow
was too ill to deliver a presidential address at
the annual APA meeting in the fall of that year.
Maslow left his long-time teaching position at
Brandeis in early 1969 for a fellowship in Cal-
ifornia. Although his time in California was
productive, it was all too brief: He died of a
coronary attack on June 8, 1970. Thus, there
simply was very little opportunity for Maslow
to incorporate the self-transcendence aspect of
his needs hierarchy into the theory as it ap-
peared in book form.
Second, the journal publication of Maslow’s
amended theory has been difficult for many
psychologists to find. Maslow’s clearest presen-
tation of his amended theory (the “Farther
Reaches” lecture; Maslow, 1969a) appeared in
what was, at the time, an obscure venue, as the
first article published in the then-fledgling Jour-
nal of Transpersonal Psychology. By some ed-
itorial quirk, the published lecture was not even
collected into the posthumous collection of es-
says that bears the same name as the lecture
itself (Maslow, 1971). Maslow’s personal jour-
nals, where his later thought also was described,
were not published until almost a decade after
his death (Maslow, 1979, 1982).
Third, the organized psychology of Maslow’s
day simply may not have been ready to incor-
porate Maslow’s concept of self-transcendence
into the quasi-official canon of acceptable the-
ory. This interpretation of events would be con-
sistent with Maslow’s experience. In the 1950s,
Maslow’s first major paper on Being-cognition
during peak experiences was rejected by both
the Psychological Review and the American
Psychologist (Hoffman, 1999, p. 206; Maslow,
1979, Vol. 2, p. 774; Maslow, 1982, p. 200). To
bring this material to light, he arranged to
present it as his APA Division 8 (Personality
and Social Psychology) presidential address at
the 1956 APA national meeting, a situation in
which his ideas could not be rejected for public
presentation; he later published these remarks in
a non-APA venue (Maslow, 1959/1999a). Sub-
sequent to this experience, despite a history of
more than 20 publications in APA core journals
from 1932 to 1957 (see publication lists in Hoff-
man, 1999, and Maslow, 1987), after 1957,
Maslow would only publish two more items in
APA journals: his APA would-be presidential
address (Maslow, 1969b) and the provocatively
titled comment “Are Our Publications and Con-
ventions Suitable for the Personal Sciences?”
(Maslow, 1961).
Fourth, notions concerning self-transcen-
dence were not well received within APA for
many years after Maslow’s death. This is sug-
gested by the dispute in the mid-1980s regard-
ing the unsuccessful effort to form an APA
Division of Transpersonal Psychology (“Debat-
ing the Legitimacy,” 1986; “In Defense,” 1986;
May, 1986a, 1986b). It is interesting to observe
that a book published by APA almost a decade
after Maslow’s death noted that “he was a
leader in humanistic psychology and a founder
of the Journal of Humanistic Psychology (Hil-
gard, 1978, p. 531), omitting what would have
been equally accurate to state, namely that
Maslow was also a leader in transpersonal psy-
chology and a founder of the Journal of
Transpersonal Psychology. This anecdotal evi-
dence is consistent with the idea that institu-
tional psychology was largely unwilling to rec-
ognize transpersonally oriented changes in
Maslow’s theory.
308 KOLTKO-RIVERA
Fifth, there is no denying that there are prob-
lems inherent in Maslow’s motivational model,
problems that Maslow failed to resolve. The
reality is that so-called “higher” motivations,
such as self-actualization and self-transcen-
dence, can appear as the dominant motivations
in individuals who seem not to have firmly
resolved the needs for survival, safety, and so
forth. This is a serious problem for a strictly
hierarchical model such as Maslow’s, where
lower, prepotent needs must be addressed suc-
cessfully before higher needs come to the fore.
It may be the case that, in light of this, subse-
quent theorists were loathe to recognize the
addition of yet another level onto the hierarchy.
However, it is clear that Maslow meant to do so,
which settles the question of his theory’s con-
tent, although not its validity.
If organized psychology was resistant to
Maslow’s ideas about self-transcendence, one
wonders why this might have been so, a ques-
tion that falls outside the scope of this article.
Maslow himself was of the (perhaps uncharita-
ble) opinion that many prominent psychologists
of his day did not understand the psychology of
peak or transpersonal experiences because they
had not experienced these themselves.
4
Although Maslow’s opinion was highly spec-
ulative, other observers have noted a tendency
among psychologists to avoid issues that in-
volve spirituality, presumably including mysti-
cal or peak experiences (see discussion in Rich-
ards & Bergin, 1997). It has been claimed that
this may be because of problematic or uninves-
tigated assumptions regarding either spirituality
or science (Jones, 1994; Slife, Hope, &
Nebeker, 1999; Sperry, 1988). It has been noted
that, perhaps out of a desire to gain scientific
respectability, psychology has historically
tended to stigmatize serious researchers of reli-
gion (R. Hogan, quoted in P. Young, 1979).
Sociological and affective reasons have also
been advanced for the neglect of religion by
psychology (Baumeister, 2002). These issues
may have had an effect on the receptivity of
disciplinary psychology to the notion of
self-transcendence.
The preceding exposition demonstrates that
the accepted version of Maslow’s needs hierar-
chy is not accurate as a reflection of his later
thought on the matter. The time has come to
rewrite the textbooks.
However, theories can be described accu-
rately without making an impact on a scientific
discipline; the humoral theory of personality,
for example, generates little contemporary
scholarship. For a rectification of Maslow’s the-
ory to have more than solely historical interest,
it must offer some advantage to theory or re-
search. As it happens, there are several such
advantages.
Implications of the Rectified Version of
Maslow’s Motivational Theory
The rectified version of Maslow’s hierarchy
of needs has several important implications for
theory and research in personality and social
psychology. These implications include more
comprehensive approaches to (a) personal and
cultural conceptions of the purpose of life; (b)
the motivational underpinnings of altruistic be-
havior, social progress, and wisdom; and (c)
suicidal terrorism and religious violence; in ad-
dition, the rectified theory provides a basis for
(d) closer integration of the psychology of reli-
gion and spirituality into both personality and
social psychology and (e) a more multicultur-
ally integrated approach to psychological the-
ory. Each of these areas is addressed below. In
some areas, Maslow’s rectified theory allows
deeper understanding of a single construct; in
others, the theory allows us to make connec-
tions across theories. Making such connections,
of course, is highly desirable; it has long been
recognized that building bridges between for-
merly unrelated theories is important for the
advancement of psychology as a science (e.g.,
Staats, 1981, 1999).
Self-Transcendence, Worldview, and
Purpose of Life
Worldviews are sets of assumptions held by
individuals and cultures about the physical and
social universe (Koltko-Rivera, 2004). One im-
portant aspect of a worldview involves notions
4
As Maslow mentioned in his journal, after putting forth
U.S. Presidents Eisenhower and Truman as examples of
people who were self-actualizers yet without experience of
Being-cognition, “Same for the APA board of directors—
very capable & sound, etc., but no B-cognition” (journal
entry for May 28, 1967; Maslow, 1979, Vol. 2, p. 794;
Maslow, 1982, p. 204).
309MASLOW AND SELF-TRANSCENDENCE
about the purpose or meaning of life; this has
been described as a central issue for individual
psychology (Baumeister, 1991). It has rarely
been noted, but Maslow’s needs hierarchy de-
fines a framework that identifies and organizes
notions about the purpose of life (Koltko-
Rivera, 2004). Maslow noted that each stage of
the motivational hierarchy can be characterized
by a distinctive worldview:
Apeculiar characteristic of the human organism when it
is dominated by a certain need is that the whole philos-
ophy of the future tends also to change. For our chroni-
cally and extremely hungry man, . . . life itself tends to be
defined in terms of eating. Anything else will be defined
as unimportant. Freedom, love, community feeling, re-
spect, philosophy, may all be waved aside as fripperies
that are useless since they fail to fill the stomach.
All that has been said of the physiological needs is
equally true of the safety needs. . . . Again, as in the
hungry man, we find that the dominating goal is a
strong determinant not only of his current world-
outlook and philosophy but also of his philosophy of
the future. (Maslow, 1943, pp. 374, 376)
The inclusion of self-transcendence in
Maslow’s hierarchy allows for a richer concep-
tualization of the meaning-of-life worldview di-
mension. Others have also noted the importance
of self-transcendent goals in forming a sense of
the purpose of life (e.g., Emmons, 1999). By
making our models of worldview more compre-
hensive, we in turn gain a better articulation of
theory in personality and social psychology.
Self-Transcendence, Altruism, Social
Progress, and Wisdom
History has demonstrated that a significant mi-
nority of the human species functions primarily
from the position that it is more important to serve
some selfless greater purpose than to serve one’s
own purposes. (One thinks in this respect of the
Mother Teresas, Albert Schweitzers, and Gandhis
of the world, as well as many lesser known indi-
viduals who put their lives at risk for social justice
and environmental and religious causes.) A com-
prehensive theory of human personality and social
behavior must account for such individuals. There
are, of course, various scientific approaches to
altruism (e.g., Barash, 2003; Sober & Wilson,
1998). The self-transcendence aspect of Maslow’s
theory should be seriously considered in relation
to these issues.
On a somewhat different plane, the sociolo-
gist Rodney Stark has asserted, on the basis of
the historical record, that despite the association
of monotheistic religion and violence (Stark,
2001), monotheistic religion has been a driving
force behind social progress and the advance of
science (Stark, 2003, 2005). It seems plausible
that some of this effect might be associated with
a motivational stance centered in self-transcen-
dence, putting aside self in favor of the greater
good, the search for truth, and so forth.
5
Wisdom is traditionally considered to be a
form of the highest human development. It is
noteworthy that a central aspect of Sternberg’s
(1998, 2003) balance theory of wisdom in-
volves self-transcendence. As Sternberg defined
the construct,
Wisdom is defined as the application of successful
intelligence and creativity as mediated by values to-
ward the achievement of a common good italics
addedthrough a balance among (a) intrapersonal, (b)
interpersonal italics added, and (c) extrapersonal
interests italics added, over (a) short and (b) long
terms, in order to achieve a balance among (a) adap-
tation to existing environments, (b) shaping of existing
environments, and (c) selection of new environments.
Thus, wisdom is not just about maximizing one’s
own or someone else’s self-interest, but about balanc-
ing various self-interests (intrapersonal) with the inter-
ests of others (interpersonal) and of other aspects of
the context in which one lives (extrapersonal), such as
one’s city or country or environment or even God. ...
Extrapersonal interests might include contributing to
the welfare of one’s school, helping one’s community,
contributing to the well-being of one’s country, or
serving God italics added, and so forth. Different
people balance these interests in different ways. At one
extreme, a malevolent dictator might emphasize his or
her own personal power and wealth; at the other ex-
treme, a saint might emphasize only serving others and
God. (Sternberg, 2003, pp. 152, 154)
The relationship between Maslow’s notion of
self-transcendence and Sternberg’s notion of a
common good and interpersonal and extraper-
sonal interests is easy to see. Recognition of the
inclusion of self-transcendence into Maslow’s
motivational hierarchy thus allows us to make a
connection between Maslow’s motivational the-
ory and cognitive psychology, inasmuch as the
latter is where Sternberg has positioned his the-
ory of human wisdom (see Sternberg & Ben-
Zeev, 2001, pp. 325–326).
5
I certainly would not wish to imply that monotheistic
religions have some sort of monopoly on self-transcendence. I
simply wish to associate the notion of a religious connection to
science with the construct of motivational self-transcendence.
310 KOLTKO-RIVERA
Self-Transcendence, Terrorism, and
Religious Violence
Self-transcendence is not just about the Mother
Teresas and Gandhis of the world. Surrendering
personal needs to a power or cause conceived as
being beyond the self has certainly been an impe-
tus not only for service but for religious violence
as well (Fields, 2004). The suicide terrorist attacks
of September 11, 2001, may need to be framed
within a context that we might call the negative
pole of self-transcendence.
Religious violence has been with us for mil-
lennia (Ellens, 2002, 2004; Stark, 2001). Al-
though it is easy to dismiss perpetrators of re-
ligious violence as simply malign, unintelligent,
or deranged, such facile explanations are often
inaccurate and overlook the way in which per-
petrators of such violence have spiritual moti-
vations (Lincoln, 2003; Stern, 2003)—what
Maslow would have called motives of self-
transcendence. We have much to learn in terms
of what separates the founder of a soup kitchen
or a home for lepers from a suicide bomber;
however, the uncomfortable truth is that there is
a dimension in which they are similar: devotion
to a cause or purpose beyond the self. Certainly,
if we are to appropriately respond to the threat
of religious terrorism in the 21st century, we
will need to understand self-transcendence, in
both its positive and negative poles. (As Kfir
2002has pointed out, Maslow’s notion of the
peak experience is also relevant to discussions
of the motivations behind terrorist acts.)
In investigating the relationship of self-
transcendence to violence (or to altruism, for
that matter), we will be confronted with some of
the less-developed aspects of Maslow’s theory.
Some of those engaged in self-transcendent vi-
olence or altruism are quite young. It would
seem unlikely that these individuals have suc-
cessfully negotiated the prior steps of Maslow’s
motivational hierarchy. Is there something pe-
culiar about self-transcendence in this respect?
If so, is there an inherent difference between
those who reach the motivational stage of self-
transcendence after traversing the other levels
of the hierarchy and those who confront moti-
vational self-transcendence out of sequence, so
to speak? Is the theory itself in need of read-
justment? Future developments in theory and
research in this area will be exciting to consider.
Integrating the Psychology of Religion
and Spirituality With Personality and
Social Psychology
Robert Hogan, the first section editor for the
Personality Processes and Individual Differ-
ences section of the Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, noted that religion has acted
as one of the most powerful social forces in
human history (as quoted in P. Young, 1979).
Moving from an historical perspective to a
present-centered one, Baumeister (2002, p. 165)
has noted, “Like TV, money, sex, and aggres-
sion, religion is an important fact of life, and
psychology cannot pretend to be complete un-
less it understands religion alongside these
other phenomena.” Despite this, scientific psy-
chology’s attitude toward the study of religion
or spirituality can be described, at best, as am-
bivalent. Although some rigorously edited spe-
cialty journals publish psychological research
involving religious or spiritual variables, in the
APA core journals such research is exceedingly
rare. For example, only 2 of the 150 articles
published in the volumes of the Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology (JPSP) for the
year 2000 dealt with religion or spirituality in
any substantive way (i.e., Gilbert, Brown, Pinel,
& Wilson, 2000; Tetlock, Kristel, Elson, Green,
& Lerner, 2000)—and in one of those (Gilbert
et al., 2000), the variables were only part of the
introduction and discussion and were not actu-
ally studied in the empirical investigation fo-
cused on in the article.
6
There are several likely reasons why the psy-
chology of religion and spirituality is largely
isolated from the mainstream of personality and
social psychology (as distinct from the reasons
for religion’s outright neglect by psychology as
a whole). Classical psychoanalytic theory, of
course, considered religion as essentially an ex-
pression of neurosis and defense against anxiety
(see Freud, 1930/1961a, 1927/1961b). Most
other major theories of personality, it seems,
simply ignore religion and matters of spiritual-
ity. (Exceptions would include the theories of
6
Abstracts from all articles appearing in the year 2000
issues of JPSP were consulted for this survey. I omit from
the total of 2 a study (Epley & Dunning, 2000) whose only
relation to religion or spirituality was the use of biblical
metaphors in the title and text, which were not integral to
the authors’ study or discussion.
311MASLOW AND SELF-TRANSCENDENCE
Jung, 1951/1969a, 1938/1969b; Allport, 1950;
and Maslow, 1964/1970; see also Pargament,
2002.) In the absence of much theory with
which to address nonpathological religion, it is
unsurprising that contemporary personality and
social psychology have largely ignored religion
and spirituality.
There is thus plenty of room for attempting to
build a bridge between personality and social
psychological theory, on the one hand, and re-
ligious and spiritual phenomena, on the other.
Inclusion of the self-transcendence step in
Maslow’s motivational hierarchy builds such a
bridge. We must make sense of the great deal of
research literature that finds positive associa-
tions between spiritual or mystical experience
and measures of psychological health (e.g., Ar-
gyle & Hills, 2000; Byrd, Lear, & Schwenka,
2000; Hunt, Dougan, Grant, & House, 2002;
Koltko, 1991; Koltko-Rivera, 2006; Mallory,
1977; Murphy et al., 2000; Pargament, 2002;
Waldron, 1999; J. S. Young, Cashwell, &
Shcherbakova, 2000). It is also necessary to
relate to personality theory the burgeoning clin-
ical literature that speaks to a need to address
spiritual issues in therapy (e.g., Constantine,
1999; Engels, 2001; Richards & Bergin, 1997,
2000; Shafranske, 1996). To better comprehend
these matters in terms of personality theory, it
may help to associate at least some spiritual
phenomena and religious behavior with the
highest motivational stage in the rectified ver-
sion of Maslow’s theory.
Maslow— himself an atheist
7
clearly con-
sidered spiritual phenomena and peak experi-
ences to be crucial aspects of human experience
(Maslow, 1964/1970, 1969a). Certainly one can
imagine Maslow agreeing with the proposition
that religious experience can be rooted in any of
the steps of the needs hierarchy, allowing us to
understand that some superficially similar expe-
riences would have vastly differing persono-
logical implications for different individuals.
8
Including the step of self-transcendence allows
us to apply a long-standing motivational theory
to some aspects of religious experience and to
understand how spiritual and even mystical ex-
periences can be associated positively with
mental health and other personality variables.
Similarly, a theoretical focus on self-
transcendence can help to integrate the field of
positive psychology (Seligman & Csikszentmi-
halyi, 2000) even more closely with disciplinary
psychology in general. Spirituality and tran-
scendent experience are domains addressed by
positive psychology (Pargament & Mahoney,
2002; Peterson & Seligman, 2004). The con-
struct of self-transcendence as a motivational
status can thus build a bridge between positive
psychology and a major theory of motivation.
Self-Transcendence and Multicultural
Psychological Theory
A culturally aware psychology recognizes
that spirituality is a basic dimension of the hu-
man condition (Sue, Bingham, Porche´-Burke, &
Vasquez, 1999). The cultures of the world are
home to a variety of traditional personologies,
which have as a common factor one or more
motivational constructs similar to self-transcen-
dence. Although this is not the place for a
comprehensive treatment of the immense lit-
erature on this topic, one can discern such
constructs in traditional Hindu or yogic psy-
chology (Scotton & Hiatt, 1996), the tradi-
tional psychologies of central and western
Asia (Scotton, 1996), the traditional psycholo-
gies of Africa (Bynum, 1999), and the tradi-
tional psychologies embedded within shamanic
cultures (Krippner, 2002).
A theoretical focus on motivational self-
transcendence also creates connections to the
cross-cultural values research literature. Look-
ing at values from the perspective of motiva-
tional goals that they express, Shalom Schwartz
has conducted research involving values with
participants from more than 50 countries (a
program summarized in Smith & Schwartz,
1997). Schwartz has found that 10 motivation-
ally distinct types of values are empirically or-
ganized into two bipolar dimensions: Openness
to Change versus Conservation and Self-
Enhancement versus Self-Transcendence. In
7
As he put it, “For me no God ever existed” (Maslow,
1979, Vol. 1, p. 525).
8
Thus, in terms of Allport’s (1959) original concept of
religious orientation, one might hypothesize that higher
intrinsic religiosity is related to higher need for self-
transcendence and that higher extrinsic religiosity is related
to higher degrees of needs found on the other levels of
Maslow’s rectified hierarchy. In terms of the quest dimen-
sion found in later conceptualizations of religious orienta-
tion (Batson, Schoenrade, & Ventis, 1993), one might hy-
pothesize that higher quest is related to higher needs for
self-actualization and self-transcendence.
312 KOLTKO-RIVERA
Schwartz’s research, the Self-Transcendence
pole reflects values promoting universalism
(“understanding, tolerance and protection for
the welfare of all people and nature”; Smith &
Schwartz, 1997, p. 86) and benevolence (“pre-
serving and enhancing the welfare of people to
whom one is close”; Smith & Schwartz, 1997,
p. 86). Thus, it appears that Schwartz’s notion
of self-transcendence has a great deal of resem-
blance to Maslow’s notion (although the two are
not identical). For our purposes here, the most
important issue is to note that the construct of
motivational self-transcendence provides a
bridge between the cross-cultural values litera-
ture and a major theory of motivation.
Another point of connection involves a dif-
ferent construct in the cross-cultural literature:
individualism– collectivism. It was mentioned
earlier that there is a certain potential for self-
aggrandizement implicit within the construct of
self-actualization that is not present within the
construct of self-transcendence. This tension
parallels a dimension of cultural and individual
worldview difference, a dimension known as
individualism– collectivism (Kagitc¸ibasi, 1997;
Triandis, 1995) or relation to group (Koltko-
Rivera, 2004). This bipolar dimension reflects
an individual’s or a culture’s preference con-
cerning situations in which there is a conflict of
agenda between what the individual wants and
what the individual’s reference group requires
(e.g., I wish to be a poet or a theorist, but my
family wishes me to be a lawyer or a plumber).
Those who position themselves at the individu-
alism pole take the view that the individual’s
agenda should prevail, whereas those who po-
sition themselves at the collectivist pole take the
view that the reference group’s agenda takes
priority. This dimension appears robustly across
cultures (Hofstede, 1984; Schwartz, 1992) and
has been considered crucial to an understanding
of cultural differences (Triandis, 1995, 1996).
The incorporation of the self-transcendence
construct within Maslow’s motivational theory
may allow us to more clearly conceptualize at
least some of the motivational issues underlying
the individualism– collectivism polarity.
Thus, including self-transcendence as a stage
of Maslow’s needs hierarchy allows us to make
a firmer connection between mainstream per-
sonality theory and the traditional psychologies
of the world and may permit us to more fully
understand a crucial dimension of cross-cultural
difference. This effort cannot but help to make
for more culturally informed theories of the
person and of society.
Conclusion
It is time to change the textbook accounts of
Maslow’s hierarchy of needs. Inclusion of self-
transcendence at the top of the needs hierarchy
is a more accurate reflection of Maslow’s the-
ory, but there are more benefits to be gained
from this rectification of theory than historical
accuracy alone. The construct of self-transcen-
dence as a motivational status provides a means
to a deeper understanding of other important
constructs and builds bridges between bodies of
theory that are currently isolated. Incorporating
self-transcendence into Maslow’s theory can
help psychology develop a better grasp of how
different people and cultures construe the mean-
ing of life. Considering the construct of self-
transcendence can help us better understand the
motivational underpinnings of both altruism
and religious violence, as well as human wis-
dom. This construct can allow us to more firmly
connect the psychology of religion and spiritu-
ality with the mainstream of theory in person-
ality and social psychology. The construct al-
lows us to more clearly relate mainstream psy-
chological theory to the traditional psychologies
of the world and may ultimately help us to build
a more culturally informed psychology. In sum-
mary, incorporating self-transcendence into
Maslow’s hierarchy of needs gives us a theo-
retical tool with which to pursue a more com-
prehensive and accurate understanding of hu-
man personality and behavior.
References
Allport, G. W. (1950). The individual and his reli-
gion. New York: Macmillan.
Allport, G. W. (1959). Religion and prejudice. Crane
Review, 2, 1–10.
Argyle, M., & Hills, P. (2000). Religious experiences
and their relations with happiness and personality.
International Journal for the Psychology of Reli-
gion, 10, 157–172.
Atkinson, R. L., Atkinson, R. C., Smith, E. E., Bem,
D. J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1996). Hilgard’s
introduction to psychology (12th ed.). Fort Worth,
TX: Harcourt Brace College.
313MASLOW AND SELF-TRANSCENDENCE
Barash, D. P. (2003). The survival game: How game
theory explains the biology of cooperation and
competition. New York: Times Books.
Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L.
(1993). Religion and the individual: A social-
psychological perspective. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Battista, J. R. (1996). Abraham Maslow and Roberto
Assagioli: Pioneers of transpersonal psychology.
In B. W. Scotton, A. B. Chinen, & J. R. Battista
(Eds.), Textbook of transpersonal psychiatry and
psychology (pp. 52– 61). New York: Basic Books.
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of life. New
York: Guilford Press.
Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Religion and psychology:
Introduction to the special issue. Psychological
Inquiry, 13, 165–167.
Bynum, E. B. (1999). The African unconscious:
Roots of ancient mysticism and modern psychol-
ogy. New York: Teachers College Press.
Byrd, K. R., Lear, D., & Schwenka, S. (2000). Mys-
ticism as a predictor of subjective well-being. The
International Journal for the Psychology of Reli-
gion, 10, 259 –269.
Chulef, A. S., Read, S. J., & Walsh, D. A. (2001). A
hierarchical taxonomy of human goals. Motivation
and Emotion, 5, 191–232.
Constantine, M. (Ed.). (1999). Spiritual and religious
issues in counseling racial and ethnic minority
populations Special issue.Journal of Multicul-
tural Counseling and Development, 27(4).
Daniels, M. (1982). The development of the concept
of self-actualization in the writings of Abraham
Maslow. Current Psychological Reviews, 2, 61–
75.
Debating the legitimacy of transpersonal psychology.
(1986). Common Boundary, 4(4), 6 –7, 15.
Ellens, J. H. (2002). Psychological legitimization of
violence by religious archetypes. In C. E. Stout
(Ed.), The psychology of terrorism: Vol. 3. Theo-
retical understandings and perspectives (pp. 149
162). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Ellens, J. H. (Ed.). (2004). The destructive power of
religion: Violence in Judaism, Christianity, and
Islam (Vols. 1– 4). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Emmons, R. A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate
concerns: Motivation and spirituality in personal-
ity. New York: Guilford Press.
Engels, D. W. (Ed.). (2001). Spirituality in counsel-
ing Special issue.Counseling and Values, 45(3).
Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “holier
than thou”: Are self-serving assessments produced
by errors in self- or social prediction? Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 861– 875.
Fadiman, J., & Frager, R. (2002). Personality and
personal growth (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River,
NJ: Prentice Hall/Pearson Education.
Fields, R. M. (2004). The psychology and sociology
of martyrdom. In R. M. Fields, Martyrdom: The
psychology, theology, and politics of self-sacrifice
(pp. 23– 81). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Freud, S. (1961a). Civilization and its discontents (J.
Strachey, Trans.). New York: Norton. (Original
work published 1930)
Freud, S. (1961b). The future of an illusion (J. Stra-
chey, Trans.). New York: Norton. (Original work
published 1927)
Gilbert, D. T., Brown, R. P., Pinel, E. C., & Wilson,
T. D. (2000). The illusion of external agency.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
690 –700.
Haggbloom, S. J., Warnick, R., Warnick, J. E., Jones,
V. K., Yarbrough, G. L., Russell, T. M., et al.
(2002). The 100 most eminent psychologists of the
20th century. Review of General Psychology, 6,
139 –152.
Hall, C. S., Lindzey, G., & Campbell, J. B. (1998).
Theories of personality (4th ed.). New York:
Wiley.
Hilgard, E. R. (Ed.). (1978). American psychology in
historical perspective: Addresses of the presidents
of the American Psychological Association, 1892–
1977. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Hoffman, E. (Ed.). (1996). Future visions: The un-
published papers of Abraham Maslow. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hoffman, E. (1999). The right to be human: A biog-
raphy of Abraham Maslow (Rev. ed.). New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Hofstede, G. (1984). Culture’s consequences
(Abridged ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Hunt, H., Dougan, S., Grant, K., & House, M. (2002).
Growth enhancing versus dissociative states of
consciousness: A questionnaire study. Journal of
Humanistic Psychology, 42(1), 90 –106.
In defense of transpersonal psychology Letters to the
editor. (1986, July). APA Monitor, p. 2.
Jones, S. L. (1994). A constructive relationship for
religion with the science and profession of psy-
chology: Perhaps the boldest model yet. American
Psychologist, 49, 184 –199.
Jung, C. G. (1969a). Aion: Researches into the phe-
nomenology of the self. In C. G. Jung, The col-
lected works of C. G. Jung, Vol. 9, Part II: Aion
(2nd ed.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press. (Original work published 1951)
Jung, C. G. (1969b). Psychology and religion. In
C. G. Jung, The collected works of C. G. Jung, Vol.
11: Psychology and religion, West and East (2nd
ed., pp. 3–105). Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univer-
sity Press. (Original work published 1938)
Kagitc¸ibasi, C. (1997). Individualism and collectiv-
ism. In J. W. Berry, M. H. Segall, & C. Kagitc¸ibasi
(Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology:
314 KOLTKO-RIVERA
Vol. 3. Social behavior and applications (2nd ed.,
pp. 1– 49). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Kfir, N. (2002). Understanding suicidal terror
through humanistic and existential psychology. In
C. E. Stout (Ed.), The psychology of terrorism:
Vol. 1. A public understanding (pp. 143–157).
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Koltko, M. E. (1991, August). How are transper-
sonal experiences and personal maturity related?
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Psychological Association, San Fran-
cisco. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.
ED335566)
Koltko-Rivera, M. E. (1998). Maslow’s “transhu-
manism”: Was transpersonal psychology con-
ceived as “a psychology without people in it”?
Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 38(1), 71– 80.
Koltko-Rivera, M. E. (2004). The psychology of
worldviews. Review of General Psychology, 8,
3–58.
Koltko-Rivera, M. E. (2006). Mysticism, psychopa-
thology, and personality. Manuscript in prepara-
tion.
Krippner, S. C. (2002). Conflicting perspectives on
shamans and shamanism: Points and counter-
points. American Psychologist, 57, 962–977.
Larsen, R. J., & Buss, D. M. (2002). Personality
psychology: Domains of knowledge about human
nature. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Lester, D., Hvezda, J., Sullivan, S., & Plourde, R.
(1983). Maslow’s hierarchy of needs and psycho-
logical health. Journal of General Psychology,
109, 83– 85.
Lincoln, B. (2003). Holy terrors: Thinking about
religion after September 11. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Mallory, M. M. (1977). Christian mysticism: Tran-
scending techniques. Amsterdam: Van Gorcum
Assen.
Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motiva-
tion. Psychological Review, 50, 370 –396.
Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality.
New York: Harper.
Maslow, A. H. (1961). Are our publications and
conventions suitable for the personal sciences?
American Psychologist, 16, 318 –319.
Maslow, A. H. (1962a). Lessons from the peak-
experiences. Journal of Humanistic Psychology,
2(1), 9 –18.
Maslow, A. H. (1962b). Notes on being-psychology.
Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 2(2), 47–71.
Maslow, A. H. (1963). Further notes on the psychol-
ogy of being. Journal of Humanistic Psychology,
3(1), 120 –135.
Maslow, A. H. (1964). Further notes on the psychol-
ogy of being. Journal of Humanistic Psychology,
4(1), 45–58.
Maslow, A. H. (1969a). The farther reaches of human
nature. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology,
1(1), 1–9.
Maslow, A. H. (1969b). Toward a humanistic biol-
ogy. American Psychologist, 24, 724 –735.
Maslow, A. H. (1970). Religions, values, and peak
experiences. New York: Penguin. (Original work
published 1964)
Maslow, A. H. (1971). The farther reaches of human
nature. New York: Viking.
Maslow, A. H. (1979). The journals of A. H. Maslow
(R. J. Lowry, Ed.; Vols. 1–2). Monterey, CA:
Brooks/Cole.
Maslow, A. H. (1982). The journals of Abraham
Maslow (1-vol. ed.; R. J. Lowry, Ed., & J. Freed-
man, Abridger). Brattleboro, VT: Lewis.
Maslow, A. H. (1987). Motivation and personality
(3rd ed.; R. Frager, J. Fadiman, C. McReynolds, &
R. Cox, Eds.). Addison Wesley.
Maslow, A. H. (1993a). A theory of metamotivation:
The biological rooting of the value-life. In A. H.
Maslow, The farther reaches of human nature (pp.
289 –328). New York: Penguin/Arkana. (Reprinted
from Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 1967,
7(2), 93–127).
Maslow, A. H. (1993b). Theory Z. In A. H. Maslow,
The farther reaches of human nature (pp. 270–
286). New York: Penguin/Arkana. (Reprinted
from Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 1969,
1(2), 31– 47)
Maslow, A. H. (1993c). Various meanings of tran-
scendence. In A. H. Maslow, The farther reaches
of human nature (pp. 259 –269). New York: Pen-
guin/Arkana. (Reprinted from Journal of
Transpersonal Psychology, 1969, 1(1), 56 66).
Maslow, A. H. (1996). Critique of self-actualization
theory. In E. Hoffman (Ed.), Future visions: The
unpublished papers of Abraham Maslow (pp. 26
32). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Maslow, A. H. (1999a). Cognition of being in the
peak-experiences. In A. H. Maslow, Toward a
psychology of being (3rd ed., pp. 81–111). New
York: Wiley. (Reprinted from Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 1959, 94, 43– 66).
Maslow, A. H. (1999b). Peak-experiences as acute
identity experiences. In A. H. Maslow, Toward a
psychology of being (3rd ed., pp. 113–125). New
York: Wiley. (Reprinted from American Journal
of Psychoanalysis, 1961, 21, 254 –260).
Maslow, A. H. (1999c). Some dangers of being-
cognition. In A. H. Maslow, Toward a psychology
of being (3rd ed., pp. 127–138). New York: Wiley.
(Reprinted from Journal of Individual Psychology,
1959, 15, 24 –32).
May, R. (1986a, May). Transpersonal Letter to the
editor.APA Monitor, p. 2.
May, R. (1986b). Transpersonal or transcendental?
The Humanistic Psychologist, 14, 87–90.
315MASLOW AND SELF-TRANSCENDENCE
Mischel, W. (1999). Introduction to personality (6th
ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt College.
Morgan, A. E. (1946). Nowhere was somewhere:
How history makes utopias and utopias make his-
tory. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Caro-
lina Press.
Morgan, A. E. (1957). The community of the future
and the future of community. Yellow Springs, OH:
Community Service.
Morgan, A. E. (1968). Observations (V. H. Bresne-
hen, Ed.). Yellow Springs, OH: Antioch Press.
Murphy, P. E., Ciarrocchi, J. W., Piedmont, R. L.,
Cheston, S., Peyrot, M., & Fitschett, G. (2000).
The relation of religious belief and practices, de-
pression, and hopelessness in persons with clinical
depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 68, 1102–1106.
Myers, D. G. (2003). Psychology (7th ed.). New
York: Worth.
Pargament, K. L. (2002). The bitter and the sweet:
An evaluation of the costs and benefits of reli-
giousness. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 168 –181.
Pargament, K. I., & Mahoney, A. (2002). Spiritual-
ity: Discovering and conserving the sacred. In
C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of
positive psychology (pp. 646 659). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Pervin, L. A., & John, O. P. (2001). Personality:
Theory and research (8th ed.). New York: Wiley.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character
strengths and virtues: A handbook and classifica-
tion. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Richards, P. S., & Bergin, A. E. (1997). A spiritual
strategy for counseling and psychotherapy. Wash-
ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Richards, P. S., & Bergin, A. E. (2000). Handbook of
psychotherapy and religious diversity. Washing-
ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
Roberts, T. B. (1972). Maslow’s human motivation
needs hierarchy: A bibliography. (ERIC Docu-
ment Reproduction Service No. ED069591)
Roberts, T. B. (1982). Comment on Mathes’s article.
Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 22(4), 97–98.
Ryckman, R. M. (2000). Theories of personality (7th
ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learn-
ing.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and
structure of values: Theoretical advances and em-
pirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experi-
mental Social Psychology, 25, 1– 65.
Scotton, B. W. (1996). The contribution of Buddhism
to transpersonal psychiatry. In B. W. Scotton,
A. B. Chinen, & J. R. Battista (Eds.), Textbook of
transpersonal psychiatry and psychology (pp.
114 –122). New York: Basic Books.
Scotton, B. W., & Hiatt, F. J. (1996). The contribu-
tion of Hinduism and Yoga to transpersonal psy-
chiatry. In B. W. Scotton, A. B. Chinen, & J. R.
Battista (Eds.), Textbook of transpersonal psychi-
atry and psychology (pp. 104 –113). New York:
Basic Books.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000).
Positive psychology: An introduction. American
Psychologist, 55, 5–14.
Shafranske, E. (Ed.). (1996). Religion and the clini-
cal practice of psychology. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Slife, B. D., Hope, C., & Nebeker, R. S. (1999).
Examining the relationship between religious spir-
ituality and psychological science. Journal of Hu-
manistic Psychology, 39(2), 51– 85.
Smith, P. B., & Schwartz, S. H. (1997). Values. In
J. W. Berry, M. H. Segall, & C. Kagitc¸ibasi (Eds.),
Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Vol. 3.
Social behavior and applications (2nd ed., pp.
77–118). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto others: The
evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Sperry, R. W. (1988). Psychology’s mentalist para-
digm and the religion/science tension. American
Psychologist, 43, 607– 613.
Staats, A. W. (1981). Paradigmatic behaviorism, uni-
fied theory, unified theory construction methods,
and the zeitgeist of separatism. American Psychol-
ogist, 36, 239 –256.
Staats, A. W. (1999). Unifying psychology requires
new infrastructure, theory, method, and a research
agenda. Review of General Psychology, 3, 3–13.
Stark, R. (2001). One true God: Historical conse-
quences of monotheism. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Stark, R. (2003). For the glory of God: How mono-
theism led to reformations, science, witch-hunts,
and the end of slavery. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Stark, R. (2005). The victory of reason: How Chris-
tianity led to freedom, capitalism, and Western
success. New York: Random House.
Stern, J. (2003). Terror in the name of God: Why
religious militants kill. New York: HarperCollins/
Ecco.
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). A balance theory of wisdom.
Review of General Psychology, 2, 347–365.
Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Wisdom, intelligence, and
creativity synthesized. Cambridge, England: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2001). Complex
cognition: The psychology of human thought. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Sue, D. W., Bingham, R. P., Porche´-Burke, L., &
Vasquez, M. (1999). The diversification of psy-
chology: A multicultural revolution. American
Psychologist, 54, 1061–1069.
316 KOLTKO-RIVERA
Tetlock, P. E., Kristel, O. V., Elson, S. B., Green,
M. C., & Lerner, J. S. (2000). The psychology of
the unthinkable: Taboo trade-offs, forbidden base
rates, and heretical counterfactuals. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 853– 870.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectiv-
ism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Triandis, H. C. (1996). The psychological measure-
ment of cultural syndromes. American Psycholo-
gist, 51, 407– 415.
Waldron, J. L. (1999). The life impact of transcen-
dent experiences with a pronounced quality of
noesis. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 30,
103–134.
Young, J. S., Cashwell, C. S., & Shcherbakova, J.
(2000). The moderating relationship of spirituality
on negative life events and psychological adjust-
ment. Counseling and Values, 45, 49 –57.
Young, P. (1979, April). Robert Hogan Interview.
APA Monitor, pp. 4 –5.
Zimbardo, P. G., & Gerrig, R. J. (1999). Psychology
and life (15th ed.). New York: Longman/Addison
Wesley Longman.
Received April 15, 2005
Accepted December 12, 2005
317MASLOW AND SELF-TRANSCENDENCE
... While Frankl discovered the truth of self-transcendence early in his career because of his painful experience with two world wars, Maslow discovered the same thing much later in his 7 career. He found out from his observation that the pursuit of self-actualization is a dead end because actualized people can still be arrogant jerks without the awakening to B-values, such as truth, goodness, and beauty, which are needed to transcend egotism (Koltko-Rivera, 2006;Maslow, 1971). ...
Article
Full-text available
From: Wong, P. T. P. (2021, March 17). Transcend the pandemic: how to redesign the future of work [President’s Column]. https://www.meaning.ca/article/transcend-the-pandemic-how-to-redesign-the-future-of-work/
... From a theoretical perspective, research on neighborhood resource-based theory describes how social cohesion relates to conditions, behaviors and stressors that affect human health [45]. Extensions of the theory of human motivation [46,47] and network theory of social capital [48] also inform this topic. Scholars describe how the social environment relates to biological pathways linked to public health [49][50][51][52][53][54]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social cohesion is a key factor within social determinants of health and well-being. Urban green spaces can provide environments that potentially facilitate meaningful and positive social interactions that promote social cohesion, equity, human health, and well-being. However, the lack of integration of existing research on social cohesion, urban green spaces, and public health in diverse (e.g., racially and ethnically) communities limits culturally relevant strategies to address health disparities. In this manuscript, we synthesize research on the potential of urban green spaces to promote social cohesion and public health in historically excluded communities. Particularly, we explore the development of social cohesion as it relates to the social environment, built environment, leisure opportunities, green space maintenance, safety, and green gentrification. We highlight key factors and their application to meet opportunities and challenges to social and public health. A conceptual framework is presented to provide an overview and illustrate connections found in the literature.
... It should be noted that Maslow also considered the human need for self-actualization and concluded that it can lead to some form of bad behavior (Kaiser and Peschl 2020). Toward the end of his life, however, Maslow noted that the stage of self-transcendence enables individuals to rise above themselves and their needs and relate to the external world without such limitations (Koltko-Rivera 2006). Only then can the individual approach everything respectfully, humbly, and constructively. ...
Chapter
This chapter, entitled “Critical review of the concept of the learning organization and the learning enterprise,” from the third part of the book Managing learning Enterprises, entitled “Possibilities of implementing the concept of a learning organization and enterprise,” analyzes Senge’s philosophy of learning organization from the aspect of learning disciplines with the aim of establishing that it is impossible to apply the original concept without considering and applying the missing link—spirituality or spiritual practice. Based on the literature review, the complementarity of spiritual practice with the learning organization disciplines is demonstrated with the aim of providing guidelines for applying the original concept of the learning organization based on the synergistic efforts of individuals dedicated to learning and spirituality. This chapter also answers the question of previous authors as to whether the learning organization is just a myth, as well as whether the learning organization is still alive. It also refutes the thesis that spiritual practice causes passivity; on the contrary, it supports the transcendental aspirations of learning individuals and learning organizations.
... Self-actualization was defined by Maslow as the highest, most inclusive, or holistic consciousness that goes beyond the self and was added as a motivational step in the "Hierarchy of Needs" model [13]. Over time, many nursing theorists also mentioned the concept of self-actualization and finally a middle-class nursing theory was developed by Reed under the name of "Self-Transcendence Theory" [14]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose To examine the effect of individualized reminiscence therapy on the management of global distress and physical and psychological symptoms, life satisfaction and self-transcendence levels of palliative care patients. Methods In a single-center palliative care service in western Turkey, 48 patients without cognitive impairment and able to communicate were included in the study. However, 44 patients completed the study. Patients who met the inclusion criteria were randomly assigned to the reminiscence therapy (intervention), unstructured social interviewing (placebo), and control groups (16 people for each group) before the start of the study. The sessions for the interview and placebo groups were conducted face-to-face in the patient's room (while the patient was sitting or lying down) for 15 days (2 weeks), every other day, for a total of eight sessions (each session was approximately 30 min). Data collection instruments—the Memorial Symptom Assessment Scale, the Contentment with Life Assessment Scale, and the Self-Transcendence Scale—were collected at baseline (first day) and after the intervention (day 15th). Statistical significance level was accepted as p < 0.05. Results There was no decrease in physical and total symptom burden (p > 0.05). There were significant reductions in general distress and psychological symptoms in the intervention and placebo groups within the group (p < 0.05), but there were no significant differences between the control group and all groups when compared (p > 0.05). Group × time interactions were statistically significant for life satisfaction and self-transcendence (p < 0.001), and there was a substantial increase in the intervention group compared to the other groups. Conclusion It may be recommended that reminiscence therapy intervention be included in routine nursing care as it may contribute positively to the psychological recovery of palliative care patients approaching the end of life. Trial registration ClinicalTrails.gov (Registration number: NCT05242016). Prospectively registered on 1 February 2022.
... Additionally, authentic-self pursuit has been found to account for the promoting effect of awe on prosocial tendencies beyond self-diminishment 4 . Individuals who felt true to themselves and aligned with their values were more likely to empathize with and prioritize the welfare of others [88][89][90] , thereby increasing prosocial tendencies beyond self-diminishment 4 . Another study demonstrated that awe elicited from witnessing the work of medical personnel during the COVID-19 pandemic enhanced prosocial behaviour as a result of heightened empathy and connection, rather than just owing to the dynamics put forward in the small-self hypothesis 91 . ...
... This has led to the model being operationalized without due attention to the interdependence of needs. Additionally, Maslow's later refinement of the model, which made it less distinct and hierarchical, has also not been fully operationalized (Koltko-Rivera, 2006). Loh et al. (2000) argue that "despite overwhelming evidence that the intrinsic nature of the theory does not work, its relevance stems not from its feasibility, but from its 'usability'" (p. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper critically analyzed Abraham Maslow's well-known hierarchy of needs theory (1943), identified its shortcomings, and examined previous studies in the literature on qualitative research methods. Moreover, it discussed the suitability of the theory to human needs and its scientific nature, considering the qualifications, dynamics, variability, and nature of human needs. Maslow proposed a rigid hierarchical framework, categorizing human needs from physiological to self-actualization. Over time, the theory faced criticism for disregarding scientific principles and overlooking the objective and subjective aspects of human needs. Instead of rigorous scientific research, the theory heavily relied on Maslow's personal observations and clinical experiences. This paper posits that Maslow overlooked socio-cultural influences that shape motivation and emphasizes how social and economic contexts prioritize needs. Moreover, the lack of empirical evidence is identified as a weakness compromising the theory's validity. The article also, proposes an updated framework considering the objectivity and subjectivity of human needs, scientific research principles, technological connections, sustainability, socio-cultural influences, arbitrary human behavior and needs, personal preferences, and emotional factors. It advocates for acknowledging that needs circulate dynamically through interpersonal networks rather than being staged rigidly, thereby supporting theoretical evolution. Additionally, the paper identifies and offers potential solutions to eight fundamental issues associated with the theory. In this study, a literature review was conducted, and Maslow's hierarchy of needs was analyzed and its deficiencies were identified and eliminated. Igor Kospov's model was examined and explained to eliminate its flaws. Finally, instead of Maslow's hierarchy of needs, the article proposes a model or set of clustered needs that are compatible with human needs and scientific, dynamic, interactive, prioritized by humans, and influenced and influenced by culture, technological developments and the human environment. The model, or clustered set of needs, consists of several clusters. These clusters are interconnected and influence each other. In other words, the same person can take steps towards self-actualization and experience the indispensable feature of social belonging while he satisfies the physiological needs of drinking water and eating, because human needs can vary from person to person, from place to place, from time to time and from situation to situation, even within the same person.
... It is suggested that meeting human spiritual needs, such as the will to meaning and self-transcendence, contributes to well-being and fulfillment (Reed, 2014;Wong, 2016). Self-transcendence, as Maslow's highest form of human development, is recognized to be present in Sternberg's balance theory of wisdom (Koltko-Rivera, 2006) which integrates intelligence, creativity and values for the common good, balancing intrapersonal, interpersonal and extrapersonal interests, in relations with environments (Sternberg, 1998). ...
Article
Purpose Work is central to human life. It requires high mental and emotional involvement from organizational leaders, whose decisions have far-reaching impacts. Currently, industrial and technological societies struggle with growing problems of trust in leadership, mental health, feelings of emptiness and the search for relevant meaning, which highlights the importance of attending to spirituality at work. This study aims to fill two main gaps: the need for empirical research on spirituality in organizational leaders, and access to the meanings and experiences of leaders themselves. Design/methodology/approach Qualitative data from 25 Portuguese organizational leaders were collected through in-depth interviews. We followed a phenomenological approach to reveal participants' own meanings and experiences of spirituality. Content analysis was performed to identify emergent categories and a framework of spirituality development. Findings The results suggest the participant’s plural views and spirituality practices, which generate common achievements – Ways to Inner. Relations of leaders’ spirituality with their development and integration at work are also evidenced – Ways to Wisdom. A spirituality development framework is presented with aspects revealed in leaders’ ways, from deepening individual issues to the orientation for existential impacts. Originality/value This study proposes a framework of spirituality development based on empirical data, with the potential for adaptation to various professional contexts. Spirituality is approached as a human dimension in leaders, as whole beings and a decisive dimension of present organizational change towards a holistic work paradigm.
... This expansion involves experiencing communion beyond self-boundaries, often realized through peak experiences. Koltko-Rivera (2006) advocates for placing selftranscendence at the hierarchy's zenith, aligning it with Maslow's theory and enhancing our comprehension of human personality and behavior. ...
Article
While late-modern society promotes individual self-actualization, its moral dimension often falters in a calculating environment. Pilgrimages offer a potential solution. Through peak experiences and self-transcendence, pilgrims reconnect with their inner moral compass, fostering morally valid self-actualization. Amid late modernity, reflective individuals emerge, navigating moral choices within societal norms while pursuing self-actualization.This study affirms the Camino de Santiago's role in promoting self-actualization, as conceptualized in humanistic psychology. A survey of 500 participants, employing Kaufman's (2018) Characteristics of Self-Actualization Scale (CSAS), validates the pilgrimage's impact. Pilgrims reported heightened levels of appreciation, equanimity, self-acceptance, reality perception, authenticity, and a fortified moral compass. The pilgrimage nurtures the recognition of life missions, instills a sense of responsibility, and cultivates a desire to contribute to humanity's well-being while facilitating experiences of self-transcendence. Our research suggests that the Camino de Santiago pilgrimage can benefit organizations by fostering an authentic and self-actualized workforce comprising leaders and followers.
Book
Martyrdom is a controversial and disputed concept. Just as religion is often hijacked by politics, martyrdom is frequently ascribed to a narrow, partisan, and parochial foundation. This is the first book to present varied views on the topic of martyrdom, reaching beyond cliches and simplistic explanations to provoke deep consideration of the essential nature of human beings and society. The volume's authors—experts in the disciplines of psychology, theology, and politics—examine martyrdom in thoughtful and thought-provoking chapters. A closing conversation between the authors is designed to inspire further discourse and debate. Readers engaged in the exploration of social justice, conflict, psychology, religion, and the politics of memory will find this book unique and stimulating. The authors have appeared on public television and public radio, as well as ABC, CBS, and NBC news and discussion programs.
Article
Review of book, P. Scott Richards and Allen E. Bergin (Au.) Handbook of Psychotherapy and Religious Diversity. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2000, 518 pp., ISBN 1-557-98624-X. Reviewed by Timothy B. Smith.
Article
Shamans' communities grant them privileged status to attend to those groups' psychological and spiritual needs. Shamans claim to modify their attentional states and engage in activities that enable them to access information not ordinarily attainable by members of the social group that has granted them shamanic status. Western perspectives on shamanism have changed and clashed over the centuries; this address presents points and counterpoints regarding what might be termed the demonic model, the charlatan model, the schizophrenia model, the soul flight model, the degenerative and crude technology model, and the deconstructionist model. Western interpretations of shamanism often reveal more about the observer than they do about the observed; in addressing this challenge, the study of shamanism could make contributions to cognitive neuroscience, social psychology, psychological therapy, and ecological psychology.