ArticlePublisher preview available

Illustrations, analogies, and facilitative transfer in prose learning

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Five groups of a total of 80 undergraduates received 5 different initial passages which could be characterized as abstract with illustrations, abstract with analogies, concrete, unembellished abstract, or a control passage. The 1st 4 passages were concerned with heat flow or electrical conductivity, while the control passage was concerned with an unrelated topic. The groups then received an abstract 2nd passage concerned with a topic different from the initial passage (e.g., electrical conductivity given initial heat flow). Results support the prediction, based on assumptions about knowledge structures, that the 1st 3 groups above would recall significantly more from the 2nd passage than would the latter 2 groups. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal
of
Educational
Psychology
1976,
Vol.
68, No. 2,
205-209
Illustrations,
Analogies,
and
Facilitative
Transfer
in
Prose
Learning
James
M.
Royer
and
Glenn
W.
Cable
University
of
Massachusetts
Amherst
Five
groups received
five
different
initial
passages
which
could
be
character-
ized
as
abstract
with illustrations,
abstract
with analogies, concrete, unem-
bellished abstract,
or a
control passage.
The
first
four
passages were con-
cerned with heat
flow
or
electrical conductivity, while
the
control passage
was
concerned with
an
unrelated topic.
The
groups then received
an
abstract
second
passage
concerned
with
a
topic different
from
the
initial
passage
(e.g.,
electrical conductivity given initial
heat
flow).
The
prediction, based
on
assumptions about knowledge structures,
was
that
the
first three groups
mentioned
above
would
recall significantly more
from
the
second passage
than
would
the
latter
two
groups.
The
results
confirmed
this
prediction.
The
research reported
in
this
article
is
concerned
with
the use of
illustrations
and
physical analogies
to
produce facilitative
transfer
in
prose learning. Recently, Royer
and
Cable (1975)
reported
one set of
condi-
tions under which facilitative transfer
would
occur.
They
hypothesized
that
such
transfer would
occur^when
two
conditions
were met: first, when
the
target
passage
(the second passage
in a
two-passage
se-
quence)
is
difficult
to
comprehend
and,
sec-
ond,
when
the
initial
passage
to
which
the
subjects
are
exposed establishes
a
"knowl-
edge
bridge" between information
the
sub-
ject
already
knows
and the
information
contained
in the
target
passage.
To
test
this
hypothesis, Royer
and
Cable
(1975)
constructed
two
passages,
one of
which
was
concerned with
the
flow
of
heat
through metals
and the
second with elec-
trical conductivity
in
metals. These pas-
sage
topics were chosen because both phe-
nomena (i.e.,
heat
flow
and
electrical con-
ductivity)
are
understandable given
a
basic
knowledge
of the
internal
structure
of
metals. Each
of
these
passages
was
then
prepared
in two
versions: concrete
and ab-
An
earlier version
of
this article
was
presented
at
the
meeting
of the
American Educational Research
Association,
Washington, D.C., 1975.
Requests
for
reprints should
be
sent
to
James
M.
Royer,
Department
of
Psychology,
University
of
Massachusetts,
Amherst,
Massachusetts
01002.
stract.
The
concrete version contained
as
many concrete referents
as
possible
and
also
contained physical analogies
for
por-
tions
of the
content.
For
example,
the
reg-
ular
crystalline
structure
of
metals
was
represented
as
being analogous
to a
tinker
toy
model where
the
discs
in the
model
represented
the
molecules
in the
metal
and
the
sticks between
the
discs repre-
sented
the
chemical bonds between
the
molecules.
The
second version
of
each pas-
sage
was
abstract
in
character
and was as
devoid
of
concrete referents
as
possible
and
contained
no
physical analogies.
The
rationale
behind
the
Royer
and Ca-
ble
(1975)
study
was
that
a
subject reading
an
initial concrete
passage
followed
by ex-
posure
to an
abstract
passage
differing
in
content
would
have
a new
knowledge
structure
established
via
exposure
to the
initial passage
into which
the
content
pf
the
second
passage
could
be
assimilated.
In
contrast,
a
subject receiving
an
initial
abstract
passage,
or an
unrelated control
passage,
would
not
have
a
relevant
knowl-
edge structure into which
the
abstract
sec-
ond
passage material could
be
assimilated
and
should learn less
from
the
passage.
Following
the
same argument, subjects
re-
ceiving
a
concrete passage
as the
second
passage should
not
differ
(regardless
of the
type
of
first
passage)
in
second
passage
learning. This should
be the
case since
the
content
of the
concrete passages would
be
205
... Many foreign scholars have done research on the function of illustrations, some of which is presented below. In terms of cognition, Royer and Cable (1976) found that more units of thought were recalled from a text when it was preceded by an illustration that provided 'context for understanding the text. [5] In terms of affect, Rigney and Lutz (1976) found that texts with illustrations were more enjoyable than texts without illustrations. ...
... In terms of cognition, Royer and Cable (1976) found that more units of thought were recalled from a text when it was preceded by an illustration that provided 'context for understanding the text. [5] In terms of affect, Rigney and Lutz (1976) found that texts with illustrations were more enjoyable than texts without illustrations. [6] In terms of learning effectiveness, Levin (1973) found that illustrations contributed to excellent reading. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although the progress of modern information technology and society has made modern junior high school English textbooks gradually develop in the direction of multi-modality, in actual teaching, teachers make less use of textbook illustrations and have difficulties in using them. Based on the two dimensions of classification and function of illustrations, the article Save the Sharks! from the Unit 13 Reading section of the Grade 9 English textbook of PEP is used as an example to discuss the effective use of textbook illustrations in reading teaching in conjunction with actual teaching. Teachers can use the illustrations in the three stages of reading, namely pre-reading, while-reading and post-reading, to help them enrich the classroom content and present a better quality of reading classroom.
... Examples of such research can be found in Rumelhart and Norman's (1981) efforts to teach adults aspects of the "Ed" text editor by using analogy-based models and in Gentner and Gentner's (1983) investigations of the effects of analogies on the understanding of electric circuits. Several researchers have also compared the comprehension and memory of information contained in short expository texts presented with or without analogies (e.g., Hayes & Tierney, 1982;Rigney & Lutz, 1976;Royer & Cable, 1976, Schustack & Anderson, 1979Simons, 1984) or concrete models (Mayer, 1975(Mayer, , 1976(Mayer, , 1978(Mayer, , 1979a(Mayer, , 1979bMayer & Bromage, 1980). Results have shown that analogies can be effective learning aids, but research has been limited so far to adults and older children. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this experiment, we investigated whether children can profit from the use of explanatory analogies to acquire information from expository text. Five-year-old (n = 24) and seven-year-old (n = 34) children listened to four texts describing relatively unfamiliar topics with or without explanatory analogies. The children were asked to recall the information described in the texts and to communicate this information to another child. They were also asked a number of inferential questions about the topics. The children in the analogy condition recalled and communicated more of the information contained in the texts and were more likely to remember the content units shared by the analogous concepts than were the children in the no-analogy condition. This effect was stronger for the older children than for the younger children. Although the children made a number of erroneous inferences about the topic, these inferences were not related to the presence of the analogies. These results show that analogies can help children acquire information from expository text, presumably because they make it possible for the children to transfer an explanatory structure from a familiar domain to an unfamiliar one.
... In the absence of such knowledge, memory is generally poor. Experimentally providing subjects with background information or with ways to link the new information with already stored information greatly enhances memory (Ausubel, 1960;Ausubel & Fitzgerald, 1961, 1962Bransford & Johnson, 1972, Experiment 1;Royer & Cable, 1975Royer & Perkins, 1977; see also Thorndyke, 1977;Wilkes & Aired, 1978). ...
Article
Full-text available
Proposes a prototypical schema theory of memory. Such a theory assumes the operation of 4 central encoding processes: selection (a process that chooses only some of all incoming stimuli for representation), abstraction (a process that stores the meaning of a message without reference to the original syntactic and lexical content), interpretation (a process by which relevant prior knowledge is generated to aid comprehension), and integration (a process by which a single, holistic memory representation is formed from the products of the previous 3 operations. The supportive and critical evidence for these processes is evaluated in light of the need for any theory of memory to account for 3 fundamental observations; accuracy, incompleteness, and distortion. The central retrieval process of schema theory, reconstruction, is also discussed in this context. Evidence indicates that the memory representation is far richer and more detailed than schema theory would suggest. (6 p ref)
... The Big Ideas provide an effective way for teachers to organize the content of units of instruction. [15] The broad concept of this unit can also be approached from the perspective of the language and style of the work, in addition to the entry points above. Big Idea 3: Depending on the emotion expressed, the prose is written from different perspectives, with different language styles and artistic techniques. ...
Article
The 2017 Senior Secondary Language Curriculum standards set out the core literacies of senior secondary language students in four dimensions: language, thinking, aesthetics and culture. The core language literacy is in line with the new era's focus on the concept of “deep learning”. Teachers can fully mobilize students' learning, build on the language, develop students' learning ability and creativity, improve their language skills, broaden their horizons, enrich their emotions, cooperate and communicate with their peers, etc., so that learning is consciously pursued towards quality all-round. Here we are concerned that the new version of the unified version of the high school language textbook, which is based on the new curriculum standards, has changed significantly, highlighting the advantages of the traditional “unit teaching”. In this context, based on our understanding of core literacy and new educational theories, we propose to move from "unit teaching" to “holistic unit teaching” in language teaching, relying on textbooks.
... Pictures provide a contextual framework in which is easier to understand the text. In fact, when a passage was preceded by an illustration providing a context to understand the text information, this improved learning made easier to recall text information (e.g., Royer and Cable, 1976;Sherman, 1976;Bernard, Petersen and Ally, 1981). Note that graphic contextual information help readers more that the verbal anticipation (i.e., a title for the passage; Sherman, 1976). ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
Visual and linguistic factors in literacy acquisition: Instructional Implications For Beginning Readers in Low-Income Countries. A literature review prepared for the Global Partnership for Education, c/o World Bank.
... The CM-c training most likely altered student's overall information processing strategies, enabling them to implicitly interact with a different CM learning format. This is in line with previous studies suggesting that the familiarity with particular formats can positively influence learning performance in similar formats (e.g., Royer and Cable, 1976;Royer, 1979). Our results could be explained by the nature of the tasks (passive vs. active learning task). ...
Article
Full-text available
Concept maps are graphical tools for organizing and representing knowledge. They are recommended for biology learning to support conceptual thinking. In this study, we compare concept map construction (CM-c, i.e., creating concept maps) and concept map study (CM-s, i.e., observing concept maps). Existing theories and indirect empirical evidence suggest distinct effects of both formats on cognitive, metacognitive and emotional aspects of learning. We developed a CM-c training, a CM-s training, and a brief introduction to concept maps (control training) for junior high school students. We investigated effects on learning performance, concept map quality, cognitive load (cognitive effects), accuracy of self-evaluation (metacognitive effects) and enjoyment (emotional effects) of these trainings in a subsequent learning phase (CM-c learning vs. CM-s learning) in a quasi-experimental two-factorial study with 3 × 2 groups (N = 167), involving the factors training type and learning type. Results reveal that CM-c training increased learning performance and concept map quality. Effects of CM-c training on learning performance transferred onto learning with CM-s. Self-evaluation was slightly more accurate after CM-c training than CM-s training. Students reported moderate, and highly varying enjoyment during CM-c and CM-s learning. The superiority of CM-c over CM-s in learning performance and concept map quality probably lies in its characteristic of being an active learning strategy. We recommend practitioners to favor CM-c training over CM-s training, and foster students’ active engagement and enjoyment.
... In addition to supporting explicit memory, integration of prior, relevant knowledge is also thought to support generation of new understandings never explicitly learned. In line with this idea, seminal studies show that individuals encode and recall new events in ways that cohere with the particular type of knowledge stored in memory (Ausubel & Fitzgerald, 1961, 1962Bartlett, 1932;Bransford & Johnson, 1972;Royer & Cable, 1975, 1976Thorndyke, 1977). Together, these studies demonstrate that new memories are not veridical, but rather are flexible interpretations constructed through integrating prior, related knowledge. ...
Article
The current research was an investigation of the effects of prior, domain-relevant knowledge on self-derivation of new, integrated knowledge. Adults were presented with novel "stem" facts and tested for self-derivation of new knowledge through integration of the facts in memory. To examine the effects of prior knowledge on memory integration, we tested participants under three within-subject conditions, in which in advance of stem encoding, they were provided with either: (1) no domain relevant information (No Knowledge control), (2) generally relevant information about the domain (General), or (3) generally relevant information about the domain along with a stem fact that was directly necessary for self-derivation (General + Stem). Prior exposure to both General and General + Stem knowledge facilitated memory for the novel, explicitly-taught stem facts. Moreover, for both prior knowledge conditions, the amount of domain-relevant knowledge retained in memory was associated with trial-by-trial self-derivation success. Importantly, the type of prior knowledge modulated memory integration in different ways. Whereas General + Stem knowledge enhanced selective retrieval and integration of the stem facts, General knowledge supported learning of the individual stem facts, but not their integration with one another. Together, the findings indicate how malleable, domain-specific experience shapes encoding, integration, and flexible extension of new, related information.
... Continued studies have been conducted on illustration efficacy in facilitating the comprehension of written texts (Haring, 1982;Donald, 1979Donald, , 1983Haring & Fry, 1979;Holliday, 1975;Koenke & Otto, 1969;Royer & Cable, 1976;Peeck, 1974;Rohwer & Harris, 1975;Rusted & Coltheart, 1979a, 1979bTennyson, 1978;and Samuels, 1970). The use of pictures and images that are added to stories could be easily "comprehended, remembered, and recalled", unlike the stories that are not presented with pictures or images. ...
Article
Full-text available
The study explores the effect of using illustrations with written narratives in short stories on the readers who are following along with the events and linking these events together. The study also explores the impact of applying critical thinking skills in using illustrations with the written narrative on readers who are following along with the events of the short story and linking them together. A mixed method was used to answer the questions and obtain the results. The quantitative method was used, where a study sample consisting of 35 students who were asked to submit an exam twice. The first time the exam contained only the written text. However, the second exam contained both the written text and the illustrations from a book. The qualitative method was applied on the same sample of students, where they were interviewed and asked whether the illustrations helped them in answering the questions on the written narrative. Next, they were asked specifically about the six questions which were based on the students’ use of their critical thinking skills in linking the provided illustrations with the written narrative. The study concluded by presenting how illustrations have a great effect on the students’ following along with the events and understating the main ideas in the narrative story. The results also show that the written text cannot always stand alone, and especially when it comes to young readers because they still lack the experience and the ability to be able to understand things on their own. Received: 30 November 2021 / Accepted: 3 February 2022 / Published: 5 March 2022
Article
Full-text available
Two experiments investigated the extent to which the concreteness of titles affects metacognitive text expectations, study motivation, and comprehension test performance. Sixty-three American and 61 German students were presented with three titles (either concrete or abstract), based upon which the students estimated their expected ease-of-comprehension, and the expected interestingness, of three expository texts. Students also reported how motivated they were to study the texts. The students then studied the texts and completed comprehension tests. The results revealed that students expected texts with concrete (as opposed to abstract) titles to be easier to comprehend and more interesting, and were more motivated to study those texts. Structural Equation Modelling revealed that the effects of titles on reported study motivation were mediated by expected interestingness. In addition to that, expected interestingness and reported study motivation were partially mediated by expected ease-of-comprehension. Comprehension test performance was not affected. The results provide robust evidence for positive motivational effects of concrete titles. More specifically, the results indicate that concrete titles—which are specific and easy to imagine—promote students’ motivation to study expository texts by encouraging the students to expect that they will find the texts interesting, and that they will be able to understand the texts.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of the research was to test the prediction that nonspecific facilitated learning of a second prose passage occurs in the situation in which an initial passage read by the subjects contained concrete referents designed to increase the comprehension of a difficult to understand second passage. Two hundred forty subjects distributed equally in 12 groups read either two successive experimental passages or a control passage followed by an experimental passage and then recalled all of the information they could from the second passage. The results offered substantial support for the prediction and contain implications for enhancing the learning of educational materials. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This review, based on a survey of more than 1200 items in the research literature, begins by attempting to outline a theory of language comprehension and learning from language. A lengthy chapter is devoted to problems in the measurement of comprehension and of learning from connected discourse. Also considered, in successive chapters, are the role of various kinds of factors in promoting comprehension and learning from connected discourse--stimulus characteristics such as readability, listenability, vocabulary, grammatical structure, and logical organization-stimulus modality (audition vs. vision); manner of presentation; factors in learning and memory; and individual differences. Problems for further research are pointed out. (Author)