ArticlePublisher preview available

Reaction time in the cold

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract

"Visual RT's were elicited from 620 soldiers sorted into 14 different groups representing a variety of ambient temperatures, windspeeds and windchills… . 1. At low windspeed… low ambient temperature has no effect on RS… . [reciprocal of RT]. 2. At windspeeds of 10 mph and greater, low ambient temperature produces a significant decrease in RA. 3. Windspeed produces a significant decrease in RS. 4. Mild exercise produces a small recover in RS. 5. If men of low 'physiological cold tolerance' are removed from the more severe environmental conditions and if Ss wear protective clothing, RS is essentially a linear decreasing function of windchill." (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal
oj
Applied
Psychology
Vol.
42, No. 1,
19S8
Reaction Time
in the
Cold
Warren
H.
Teichner
1
Quartermaster
Research
and
Development Center
As
reported
by
Forlano
(3) and by
Teich-
ner (7)
studies
of the
effects
of
cold envi-
ronments
on the
simple reaction time (RT)
suggest
that
RT is not
affected
by low
ambi-
ent
temperatures
down
to
50°
F.
How-
ever, temperature
is
only
one of the
factors
which make
up
cold environments.
The
cool-
ing
power
of air
actually depends
on
both
its
temperature
and
speed
of
movement (wind-
speed).
The
effect
of
each
of
these, singly
and in
combination, must
be
studied
before
safe
generalizations
can be
made about
the
effect
of the
cold
on RT.
Further,
the
com-
bined action
of
temperature
and
wind (wind-
chill
2)
in
determining
the
cooling rate
of
exposed
bodies
has
been formulated
quantita-
tively; thus, there
is a
basis
for a
rational
ap-
proach
to the
combined
effects
problem.
It
was the
concern
of the
present investigation,
therefore,
to
study
the
effects
of the
cold
on
RT
through variation
of all
three physical
factors.
As
long
as 5s
wear protective clothing,
as
they have
in
previous studies,
S-R
relation-
ships
may be
misleading.
That
is,
with
no
information
beyond
the
stimulus conditions
and
S's
response,
it is not
possible
to
deter-
mine whether
the
environment
was
actually
effective
in
cooling
the
body. Failure
to find
a
temperature
effect
in
previous studies
may
have been
the
result
of
lack
of
actual body
cooling.
Thus,
studies which
fail
to
measure
1
Now at the
University
of
Massachusetts.
2
Windchill
is a
measure
of
that
part
of the
total
cooling
of a
body
due to the
action
of
wind.
The
term
is not
usually applied
to
temperatures above
freezing.
Values
of
windchill used
in
this
study
were
obtained
from
reference
(6)
based
on
Siple
and
Passel's
(5)
formula:
Ki>
=
(Viw
+
100
+
10.45
-
mi)
(33 -
T)
where:
KO
Total
cooling
in
kilogram calories
per
square
meter
per
hour,
IOT
=
Wind velocity
in
meters
per
second,
Ta
= Air
temperature
in
degrees centigrade.
body
cooling cannot yield information
of
gen-
eral
value
nor are the
results amenable
to
theoretical considerations, either physiologi-
cal
or
psychological.
The
present study
was
designed,
therefore,
to
obtain
body surface
temperatures
for
relationship
to the
effects
of
cold environments.
Method
Six
hundred
and
forty
infantrymen
from
Fort
Devens
were
used
as 5s.
20-man
groups were used,
one
per day
until
the
total
number
was
exhausted.
On
arrival
at the
laboratory
the 20 5s
were ran-
domly
sorted into
five-man
subgroups
and
each
op-
eration
of the
investigation
was
phased
to
handle
the
sequential appearance
of the
four
subgroups.
Two
subgroups were
studied
before
the
noon meal
and two
after
it.
Twenty
5s
were eliminated
for
medical reasons prior
to
starting.
5s
were taken
to a
dressing room
(55-60°
F.)
which
interconnected with
the
climatic chamber,
where
they undressed,
a
multi-point thermocouple
harness
was put on
them
and
they were dressed
in
appropriate clothing. These procedures were per-
formed
"by the
numbers"
so
that
all five Ss
were
dressed
at the
same time, thus avoiding individual
overheating. While
5s
were
in the
dressing room
standard instructions were read
to
them which
ex-
plained
the
details
of the
procedure
to
follow.
When
dressed, they
were
taken into
the
climatic chamber
which
was
pre-set
for the
appropriate environmental
conditions.
In the
chamber,
the five 5s sat
side
by
side about
three
ft.
apart,
before
a
long table
in
front
of a
large
observation window. They
faced
sideways
to
the
direction
of air
movement
and
were
in
front
view
of
technicians operating
the
equipment out-
side
of the
chamber. From their positions, however,
5s
were unable
to
observe
the
operation
of the
equipment.
5s sat
quietly
and
cooled
for the first 25
min.
During this time
the
instructions were read again
and
procedures demonstrated.
After
this they per-
formed
on a
manual dexterity task
for
about
20
min.
On
completion
of
this task
(45
min.
of
exposure),
5s
were seated
and 25
successive
RTs
were obtained.
This
procedure
was
completed
in
7-10 min.
Follow-
ing
this,
5s ran in
place slowly
for
three min. (mild
exercise),
performed
five
min. more
on the
psycho-
motor
task
and
then
10
more successive
RTs
were
obtained.
Each
5 was
provided with
a
Morse
key
fastened
to the
table.
At a
verbal ready signal,
Ss
closed
the
54
... Collectively, these changes can lead to decrements in cognitive task performance in ambient air ≤10°C (Falla et al., 2021;Hancock et al., 2007;Pilcher et al., 2002). Specific decrements have been reported to include impaired executive function (Racinais et al., 2017), working memory (Shurtleff et al., 1994;Thomas et al., 1989), attention/vigilance (Sun et al., 2022;Watkins et al., 2014), and psychomotor processing (Teichner, 1958) with acute (30-120 min) passive cold air (range: −15 to 10°C) exposure. Impaired cognitive performance in the cold is not a universal finding, as no effects on executive function (Makinen et al., 2006;Muller et al., 2012;Yang et al., 2021), working memory (Faerevik et al., 2021;Makinen et al., 2006;Muller et al., 2012), attention/vigilance (Makinen et al., 2006), or psychomotor processing (Faerevik et al., 2021;Makinen et al., 2006) have also been reported with cold air exposure. ...
... Cooling skin temperature alone (without reductions in core temperature) causes vasoconstriction, mild shivering, and thermal discomfort (Shurtleff et al., 1994). Under these conditions, cognition is proposed to be impaired due to increased distraction and decreased arousal from thermal discomfort leading to fewer attentional resources available to complete cognitive tasks (Teichner, 1958). Whereas whole-body cooling sufficient to induce mild hypothermia (decrease in core temperature by 0.5-2.0°C) ...
... This study tested the effects of cold air exposure leading to skin and core temperature decreases on executive function, working memory and psychomotor processing. We hypothesized that cognitive performance would be impaired with decreases in skin temperature due to increased thermal discomfort (Teichner, 1958), with further reductions in performance with progressively greater core cooling (Ellis, 1982). We found that neither reductions in skin temperature, nor core temperature of ∆-0.3°C and ∆-0.8°C significantly impacted executive attention based cognitive process (i.e., temporal preparation effect (simple reaction time task), flanker effect (vertical flanker), set-size effect (item working memory)). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study tested the effects of skin and core cooling on cognitive function in 0°C cold air. Ten males completed a randomized, repeated measures study consisting of four environmental conditions: (i) 30 min of exposure to 22°C thermoneutral air (TN), (ii) 15 min to 0°C cold air which cooled skin temperature to ~27°C (CS), (iii) 0°C cold air exposure causing mild core cooling of ∆‐0.3°C from baseline (C‐0.3°C) and (iv) 0°C cold air exposure causing mild core cooling of ∆‐0.8°C from baseline (C‐0.8°C). Cognitive function (reaction time [ms] and errors made [#]) were tested using a simple reaction test, a two–six item working memory capacity task, and vertical flanker task to assess executive function. There were no condition effects (all p > 0.05) for number of errors made on any task. There were no significant differences in reaction time relative to TN for the vertical flanker and item working memory capacity task. However, simple reaction time was slower in C‐0.3°C (297 ± 33 ms) and C‐0.8°C (296 ± 41 ms) compared to CS (267 ± 26 ms) but not TN (274 ± 38). Despite small changes in simple reaction time (~30 ms), executive function and working memory was maintained in 0°C cold air with up to ∆‐0.8°C reduction in core temperature.
... Two major hypotheses can explain these findings. The arousal hypothesis, which states that a slight decrease in Tco drives attention towards the cognitive tasks thus ameliorating performance [15], and the distraction hypothesis, stating that cold stress deviates attention from a primary task [16]. ...
... Distraction caused by cold discomfort can be demanding for central attention resources and thus decrease CP. Teichner [16] proposed the distraction hypothesis where cold stress is considered the cause of a deviation of attention away from the primary task provoking an impairment of performance such as an increased number of missing signals and a slowing of RT rather than rapid mistakes [16]. All studies included in the present review were performed in a simulated environment, allowing a control of confounding factors compared to in-field studies. ...
... Distraction caused by cold discomfort can be demanding for central attention resources and thus decrease CP. Teichner [16] proposed the distraction hypothesis where cold stress is considered the cause of a deviation of attention away from the primary task provoking an impairment of performance such as an increased number of missing signals and a slowing of RT rather than rapid mistakes [16]. All studies included in the present review were performed in a simulated environment, allowing a control of confounding factors compared to in-field studies. ...
Article
Full-text available
Several aspects of cognition can be affected after cold exposure, but contradictory results have been reported regarding affected cognitive domains. The aim of the current systematic review was to evaluate the effects of specific cold exposure on cognitive performance in healthy subjects. A systematic search was performed using MEDLINE (through PubMed), EMBASE (Scopus) and PsycINFO databases according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines. Inclusion criteria were healthy subjects exposed to a cold environment (either simulated or not) and cognitive performance related to cold exposure with an experimental design. The literature search identified 18 studies, eight studies investigated the effect of cold air exposure and ten the effect of cold water immersion on cognitive performance of healthy subjects. There were several differences among the studies (environmental temperature reached, time of exposure, timing, and type of cognitive test administration). Cold exposure induced in most of the experimental settings (15 of 18) an impairment of CP even before accidental hypothermia was established. The most investigated and affected cognitive domains were attention and processing speed, executive function, and memory. Gender differences and effects of repeated exposure and possible acclimation on cognitive performance need further studies to be confirmed.
... Similarly, exposure to cold environments (~-20 to 10 • C) has also been shown to induce cognitive decrements (Taylor et al., 2016), where performance on complex cognitive tasks are again thought be most affected (Martin et al., 2019). The early distraction hypothesis proposed that stress elicited from cold exposure shifts attention toward the environmental stressor and away from the cognitive task (Teichner, 1958). That said, findings from recent studies suggest that cognitive deficits may be attributed to catecholamine dysregulation in the cold (Muller et al., 2012). ...
... There were also some decrements in cognitive performance (i.e., slower target detection times) noted following exercise in the cold in the absence of any core temperature differences between conditions (Adam et al., 2008). These effects were attributed to increased perceptual demand, lower mood, and a potential distraction effect whereby cold exposure may have drawn attention away from cognitive tasks on to thermal discomfort (e.g., Muller et al., 2012;Teichner, 1958), thereby counteracting potential cognitive benefits following moderate exercise (Adam et al., 2008). Interestingly, Ji et al. (2017) identified that working-memory accuracy deteriorated in response to 60-min passive cold exposure, but following moderate exercise no such accuracy decrements were observed. ...
Article
To date, no review has focused specifically on the potential modulating role of environmental temperature on the effects of exercise on cognitive function. Despite this, a range of occupations and performance contexts exist (e.g., military personnel, emergency services, sport) where the maintenance of cognitive function in environmentally challenging environments is crucial. Therefore, this systematic review aimed to evaluate the experimental research investigating how manipulating environmental temperature influenced the effects of acute bouts exercise on cognitive functioning from pre-to post-exercise, or during exercise. Studies to be included were assessed by two authors reviewing title, abstract, and then full-text. From the searches conducted, twenty articles were identified which met the inclusion criteria. For the purpose of this review, exercise involved in each study was categorised into low, moderate, and vigorous dosages (dependent on intensity and duration). The results indicate that moderate dosages of exercise help stimulate improved cognitive performance from pre-to-post exercise in temperate conditions, where cold exposure appears to blunt these effects. In addition, hot environments led to cognitive decrements during and post exercise which were often identified in studies that implemented prolonged moderate or vigorous exercise protocols. Therefore, suggesting a combination of heightened physiological strain from increased dose of exercise, alongside heat exposure, can be detrimental to optimal cognitive functioning, whereby executive functioning tasks appeared to be most affected. The findings from this systematic review highlight the potential modulating role of environmental temperature on the effects of exercise on cognitive function. Thus, highlighting the importance of considering the role of environmental temperature for individuals either exercising to elicit desired cognitive benefits or for those involved in physically demanding occupations or performance domains.
... These cold-induced neurocognitive impairments, which exhibit a large interindividual variability, have been ascribed to the depression of synaptic transmission and of central and peripheral nerve conduction velocity (4)(5)(6), the reduction in cerebral blood flow and oxygen (O 2 ) delivery (7), and/or, perhaps, the desensitization of catecholamine release in response to mental demands (8,9). The magnitude of hedonic perception engendered by cold stress appears to also contribute to the cognitive decline, via a "distraction" effect; that is, thermal discomfort disturbs and diverts attention away from the required cognitive task (10). Yet, regardless of the underlying mechanisms, the impact of cold on cognition has primarily been assessed during short-term (i.e., 3 h; cf. ...
Article
Full-text available
Divers are at enhanced risk of suffering from acute cognitive deteriorations, due to the low ambient temperatures, and the narcotic action of inert gases inspired at high pressures. Yet, the behavioral effects of cold and inert-gas narcosis have commonly been assessed in isolation, and during short-term provocations. We, therefore, evaluated the interactive influence of mild hypothermia and narcosis engendered by a subanaesthetic dose of nitrous oxide (N 2 O; a normobaric intervention analogue of hyperbaric nitrogen) on cognitive function during prolonged iterative exposure. Fourteen men partook in two ~12-h sessions (separated by ≥4 days), wherein they performed sequentially three 120-min cold (20°C) water immersions (CWIs), while inhaling, in a single-blinded manner, either normal air, or a normoxic gas mixture containing 30% N 2 O. CWIs were separated by 120-min rewarming in room-air breathing conditions. Prior to the first CWI and during each CWI, subjects performed a finger dexterity test, and the Spaceflight Cognitive Assessment Tool for Windows (WinSCAT) test assessing aspects of attention, memory, learning and visuo-spatial ability. Rectal and skin temperatures were, on average, reduced by ~1.2°C and ~8°C, respectively ( P<0.001). Cooling per se impaired ( P≤0.01) only short-term memory (~37%) and learning (~18%); the impairments were limited to the first CWI. N 2 O also attenuated ( P≤0.02) short-term memory (~37%) and learning (~35%), but the reductions occurred in all CWIs. Further, N 2 O invariably compromised finger dexterity, attention, concentration, working memory and spatial processing ( P<0.05). Present results demonstrate that inert-gas narcosis aggravates, in a persistent manner, basic and higher-order cognitive abilities during protracted cold exposure.
... Therefore, one plausible explanation for the limited improvements in cognitive flexibility in young men is the distraction hypothesis. Based on this hypothesis, it can be assumed that greater physical and psycho-emotional strain provide competing stimuli that interfere with the response elicited by the reaction signal, and thus produce increased latencies [39]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and Objectives: To date, understanding age-related changes in cognitive processes during heat exposure still needs to be better-understood. Thus, the main aim of the current study was to evaluate the effects of whole-body hyperthermia (WBH), i.e., a ≈2.5 °C increase in rectal temperature (Tre) from overnight-fast baseline value, on cognitive functioning in old and young men and to explore factors, such as stress and thermophysiological strain, that could influence such changes. Materials and Methods: Ten young (19-21 years of age) and nine old (61-80 years of age) healthy men underwent an experimental trial with passive lower-body heating in hot water immersion (HWI) at 43 °C (HWI-43 °C) until Tre reached 39 °C in old adults and 39.5 °C in young adults. Cognitive performance and cortisol concentration were assessed before and after HWI, and the physiological strain index (PSI) was assessed during HWI-43 °C. Results: PSI was lower and cortisol concentration was greater after HWI-43 °C in the old group compared with the young group (p < 0.05). Surprisingly, hyperthermia improved cognitive flexibility only in old adults, whereas short-term and visual recognition memories were maintained in both age groups. Conclusions: A ≈2.5 °C increase in rectal temperature can improve executive function in old adults, and this increase parallels the increased cortisol concentration and the lower thermophysiological strain under severe WBH conditions.
... Several studies document the repercussions on human response when exposed to temperatures ranging from − 20º to 10ºC, describing vigilance impairment and slower reaction time [26,27]. The distraction theory explains how severe reductions in ambient temperature disrupt focus and negatively impact cognitive function [28,29]. Additionally, prolonged cold exposure and reduced skin temperature hamper manual agility, muscular strength, and tactile sensation, all essential for ET-SA [30][31][32][33]. ...
... Several studies document the repercussions on human response when exposed to temperatures ranging from − 20º to 10ºC, describing vigilance impairment and slower reaction time [26,27]. The distraction theory explains how severe reductions in ambient temperature disrupt focus and negatively impact cognitive function [28,29]. Additionally, prolonged cold exposure and reduced skin temperature hamper manual agility, muscular strength, and tactile sensation, all essential for ET-SA [30][31][32][33]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Our study aimed to assess the ability of nonmedical civilians to self-apply extremity tourniquets in cold weather conditions while wearing insulating technical clothing after receiving basic training. Methods A field study was conducted among 37 voluntary participants of an expedition party to the Spanish Antarctic base. The researchers assessed the participant’s ability to self-apply five commercial extremity tourniquets (CAT, OMNA, RMT, SWAT-T, and RATS) over cold-weather clothing and their achieved effectiveness for vascular occlusion. Upper extremity self-application was performed with a single-handed technique (OHT), and lower extremity applying a two-handed technique (THT). Perceptions of self-application ease mean values ± standard deviation (SD) were compared by applying a 5% statistical significance threshold. Frequency count determined tourniquet preference. Results All the tested ETs, except the SWAT-T, were properly self-applied with an OHT, resulting in effective vascular occlusion in the upper extremity. The five devices tested were self-applied correctly in the lower extremities using THT. The ratcheting marine-designed OMNA ranked the highest for application easiness on both the upper and lower extremities, and the windlass CAT model was the preferred device by most participants. Conclusions Civilian extremity tourniquet self-application on both upper and lower extremities can be accomplished in cold weather conditions despite using cold-weather gloves and technical clothing after receiving brief training. The ratcheting marine-designed OMNA ranked the highest for application ease, and the windlass CAT model was the preferred device.
... This means that different levels of cold exposure can lead to different emotions. And 40 min of recovery time was not enough to compensate for the difference in temperature levels, and this phenomenon could be explained by the distraction hypothesis (Warren, 1985). Many scholars have shown that exercise can improve mood in the cold, so it's possible to mitigate the differential effects of cold exposure over a short period by setting different exercise intensities (Yang et al., 2021;Muller et al., 2011). ...
Article
Mood and cognitive function can change after cold exposure. Recovering from extreme cold is important, especially for people who work outdoors. This study was conducted to investigate cognitive function in the recovery period after being exposed to different temperatures of cold environments. 12 subjects underwent different cold exposures at −5 °C, −10 °C, and −15 °C in a climate chamber. Cognitive function was measured with the Neurobehavioral Core Test Battery (NCTB) and Stroop color-word test after 40 min of recovery at a room of 24 °C. The results showed that 40 min of recovery after cold exposure could not eliminate the differences between different degrees of extremely cold exposure in the mood effects (confusion-bewilderment). A decrease in ambient temperature could reduce visual perception and memory functions. Manual dexterity decreased by 13.6% in −10 °C compared with that in 24 °C due to colder ambient temperatures. At different degrees of cold exposure, selective attention decreased significantly as the ambient temperature dropped. The results from this study could be used to better organize work and rest periods according to task characteristics and duration of cold exposure.
Article
Some extreme cold events have occurred in the past few years. Human perceptual responses and cognitive performances when exposed to extremely cold environments could be significantly influenced. Impaired cognition can result in human errors and other cascading impacts. This paper aims to investigate human thermal perception and cognitive performance in an extremely cold environment in a climatic chamber. The study involved and exposed twelve male participants who wore protective clothing (2.16 clo) to a -20 ℃ climate chamber. Critical parameters for evaluating human perceptual response and cognitive performance, including thermal sensation vote (TSV), thermal comfort vote (TCV), mood, health symptoms, and selective attention performance, were recorded and analyzed. The results showed that the TSV did not recover to the value before the cold exposure despite the subjects being rewarmed in a neutral environment for 40 minutes. The participants’ TSV reached the coldest after 20-25 minutes. Cold exposure increased discomfort, which required more than 40 minutes to recover. The extremely cold exposure resulted in increased tension, anger, confusion, decreased depression and fatigue. Anger and confusion had a strong correlation with tension-anxiety, and the correlation was stronger after recovery. The extremely cold exposure had a sustained impact on selective attention and heart rate. The reaction speed decreased by 36 %. The results could be used to form recommendations to protect workers’ health and performance when exposed to extremely cold environments.
Article
Many jobs like outdoor work and emergency rescue have to be exposed to extremely cold environments. The combined effects of the cold exposure and work intensity on human cognitive performance remain unclear. In this paper, the experiments of six Chinese young men exposed to an extremely cold environment (-10 °C) were conducted in a climatic chamber. The work intensity level was graded according to the metabolic rate corresponding to three walking speeds. Nine cognitive functions and one perceived were recorded to evaluate the subjects’ cognitive performance, including NCTB (seven items), Stroop, and RPE were measured. The increase of workload from moderate to high could lead to the acceleration of fatigue speed and the aggravation of fatigue degree 5 minutes earlier. Moderate work intensity is a noteworthy work level in extremely cold environment, which is an inflection point in the impact of fatigue and cognitive levels. The manual dexterity significantly increases by the workload intensity, and the high work intensity makes the hands more dexterous (29% increase). Extremely cold environment has a significant effect on short-term memory (decreased 33%). The selective attention was reduced by 16% in the extremely cold environment. With the moderate work intensity in extremely cold environment, the perceived judgment response speed would decrease. The combined effects of the extremely cold environment and the workload on the cognitive functions of psychomotor ability and attention or sensorimotor speed should be paid more attention to.
Article
Full-text available
Found that foreperiod length and muscular tension are independent in their effects on reaction time (RT). In general, RT varies inversely with magnitude of muscular tension. There appears to be an optimum foreperiod of reaction which, in the present massed practice task, was 5-6 sec. No practice effects due to the repeated elicitation of the reaction were found.
Article
Please refer to the American Philosophical Society, Vol 89, 1, April, 1945 for a copy of this article.
Article
An assessment is made of the current scientific status of simple reaction time (RT), based primarily on a literature review of the last 20 years. Considered are the effects on RT of stimulus-receptor factors, of central and motor factors, and of special factors such as prolonged readiness, certain common drugs, temperature, sleep conditions, etc. While further research probing is indicated, several advances have been noted during the past 20 years and the present status of simple reaction time is evaluated in terms of 10 reasonably well established generalizations. 163-item bibliography.
Set and muscular tension
  • R C Davis
Davis, R. C. Set and muscular tension. Indiana Univ. Publ. Sci. Ser. No 10, 1940.
The effect of ambient and body temperatures upon reaction time. ONR, SDC, Rpt
  • G Forlano
  • J E Barmack
  • J D Coakley
Forlano, G., Barmack, J. E., & Coakley, J. D. The effect of ambient and body temperatures upon reaction time. ONR, SDC, Rpt. 151-1-13, 1948.
Table of wind chill values
Table of wind chill values. QM Res and Dev. Center, Climatic Research Lab., 1943.