ThesisPDF Available

The ecology and social behavior of a resident manta ray (M. alfredi) population off Maui, Hawaii

Authors:
  • Hawaii Association for Marine Education and Research

Abstract and Figures

Late maturity, few offspring, and a residential nature, typical of Manta alfredi, make this species particularly vulnerable to localized anthropogenic threats, and much less likely to recover from depleted populations. Understanding the population characteristics and reproductive ecology of this species is critical for its successful management. Paired-laser photogrammetry, combined with photo-identification and active tracking were used to describe the population characteristics, demographics, habitat range and use, and reproductive ecology of a resident population of manta rays (Manta alfredi) in Hawai‘i. Paired-laser photogrammetry proved to be a simple, non-invasive, accurate (mean error of 0.39%), and precise (CV = 0.54%) method for sizing free-ranging manta rays. A total of 286 surveys were conducted between 2005 and 2010 at a known aggregation site off Maui, Hawai‘i. A total of 290 different individual manta rays were photo-identified. A discovery curve showed no asymptotic trend, indicating the number of individuals using the area was much larger than the total identified. Resights and manta follows revealed a home range spanning Maui County waters with visits to the four-islands but did not include the Big Island, supporting independent, island-associated stocks. High resight rates within and across years at the study site provided strong evidence of site fidelity. Findings were consistent with a population of manta rays moving into and out of the Maui aggregation area, with a varying portion of the total population temporarily resident at any given time. Males, accounted for 53% of all individuals, and resided for shorter periods than females around the study site. Manta rays were usually absent at first light with numbers increasing throughout the day. Shark predation was evident in 33% of individuals, and alarmingly, 10% had an amputated or non-functional cephalic fin, likely caused by entanglement in monofilament fishing line. Repeated measurements on 154 different manta rays, produced a mean CV of 1.46%, providing further support for the paired-laser system. Sexual dimorphism was evident with the largest female (3.64 m DW) 19% larger than the largest male (3.05 m DW). Sexual maturity in females, based on evidence of pregnancy and mating scars, was conservatively determined to be 3.37 m DW. The DW at which 50% of the males were likely to begin maturation and accelerated clasper growth was between 2.70 and 2.80 m DW. The absence of individuals smaller than 2.50 m DW suggests age class segregation may be occurring in this population. xiii Although mating trains and late-term pregnant females were observed at all times of the year, they were more likely to occur during the winter months. Females seemed capable of ovulating multiple times during a year if their initial mating attempts are unsuccessful. Estrus may last at least several days based on repeated sightings of the same female in a mating train over several days. Sexual maturity appears delayed in both males and females until their body size exceeds 90% of their maximum size, an indicator that large body size provides a reproductive advantage. Larger females had higher pregnancy rates, and were more likely to reproduce in successive years. No evidence was found to support direct physical competition between males for access to available females. Endurance rivalry may be occurring in which females select males who are able to stay with her mating train over a long duration, possibly days. Although male body size was not a predictor of proximity to the female within a mating train, larger males may benefit from greater physical resources, allowing them to follow a female for longer periods, especially if no feeding is taking place while in a mating train. Further investigation on male reproductive strategies is needed. The Maui aggregation site seems to be an important staging area for breeding members of this population. This small, demographically independent population appears vulnerable to the impacts from nontarget fisheries, primarily from entanglement in fishing line, and could suffer from exploitation by unregulated “swim-with manta ray” programs. Proper conservation measures are recommended to ensure the future preservation of this unique habitat and to minimize anthropogenic impacts in this region that may negatively impact the population. Management on an island-area basis is recommended. Long-term, repeated measurements using paired-laser photogrammetry on known individuals of a population could provide additional population growth parameters to assist in effective management of this poorly understood species.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ECOLOGY AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR OF A RESIDENT MANTA RAY
(MANTA ALFREDI) POPULATION OFF MAUI, HAWAI‘I
A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE DIVISION OF THE
UNIVERISTY OF HAWAI‘I AT MĀNOA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE
REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
IN
PSYCHOLOGY
DECEMBER 2010
By
Mark H. Deakos
Dissertation Committee:
Karl A. Minke, Chairperson
Patricia A. Couvillon
Louis M. Herman
Adam A. Pack
Joseph R. Mobley, Jr
ii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
LIST OF TABLES............................................................................................................ vi!
LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................viii!
ACKNOWLEDGMENT.................................................................................................... x!
ABSTRACT..................................................................................................................... xii!
1!GENERAL INTRODUCTION.................................................................................... 1!
1.1!Introduction......................................................................................................... 1!
1.2!A Brief Evolutionary History of Elasmobranchs................................................ 1!
1.3!Manta Rays .........................................................................................................2!
1.3.1!Coloration............................................................................................... 3!
1.3.2!Sensory Systems..................................................................................... 5!
1.3.3!Morphology ............................................................................................ 7!
1.3.4!Reproduction .......................................................................................... 8!
1.3.5!Range and Habitat ................................................................................ 11!
1.3.6!Food and Foraging ............................................................................... 13!
1.3.7!Population Size..................................................................................... 14!
1.3.8!Behavior ............................................................................................... 14!
1.3.9!Lifespan ................................................................................................ 17!
1.3.10!Threats .................................................................................................. 17!
1.3.11!Aquariums ............................................................................................ 20!
1.3.12!Manta Ray Protection........................................................................... 21!
1.3.13!Research ............................................................................................... 21!
1.4!Dissertation Overview ...................................................................................... 22!
1.5!General Methods............................................................................................... 23!
2!USING PAIRED-LASER PHOTOGRAMMETRY AS A SIMPLE AND
ACCURATE SYSTEM TO MEASURE THE BODY SIZE OF FREE-RANGING
MANTA RAYS (MANTA ALFREDI) ....................................................................... 25!
iii
2.1!ABSTRACT...................................................................................................... 25!
2.2!INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................ 26!
2.3!METHODS ....................................................................................................... 28!
2.3.1!Study Area and Population................................................................... 28!
2.3.2!Equipment ............................................................................................ 29!
2.3.3!Accuracy and Precision........................................................................ 29!
2.3.4!Manta Ray Measurements.................................................................... 34!
2.3.5!Statistics ............................................................................................... 37!
2.4!RESULTS ......................................................................................................... 38!
2.4.1!Accuracy and Precision........................................................................ 38!
2.4.2!Manta Ray Measurements.................................................................... 38!
2.5!DISCUSSION ................................................................................................... 44!
2.5.1!Accuracy and Precision........................................................................ 44!
2.5.2!Manta Measurements ........................................................................... 45!
2.5.3!Future Research.................................................................................... 47!
2.5.4!Summary .............................................................................................. 48!
3!CHARACTERISTICS OF A MANTA RAY (MANTA ALFREDI) POPULATION
OFF MAUI, HAWAII, AND IMPLICATIONS FOR MANAGEMENT................ 49!
3.1!ABSTRACT...................................................................................................... 49!
3.2!INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................ 50!
3.3!METHODS ....................................................................................................... 52!
3.3.1!Main study area .................................................................................... 52!
3.3.2!Surveys ................................................................................................. 53!
3.3.3!Photo-identification .............................................................................. 55!
3.3.4!Abundance and Survivorship ............................................................... 56!
3.3.5!Population Range ................................................................................. 56!
3.3.6!Population Structure............................................................................. 57!
iv
3.3.7!Use of the Aggregation Area................................................................ 57!
3.3.8!Threats .................................................................................................. 58!
3.4!RESULTS ......................................................................................................... 59!
3.4.1!Surveys ................................................................................................. 59!
3.4.2!Abundance and Survivorship ............................................................... 59!
3.4.3!Population Range ................................................................................. 62!
3.4.4!Population Structure............................................................................. 62!
3.4.5!Use of the Aggregation Area................................................................ 64!
3.4.6!Threats .................................................................................................. 68!
3.5!DISCUSSION ................................................................................................... 69!
3.5.1!Abundance and Survivorship ............................................................... 69!
3.5.2!Population Range ................................................................................. 69!
3.5.3!Population Structure............................................................................. 71!
3.5.4!Use of the Aggregation Area................................................................ 71!
3.5.5!Threats .................................................................................................. 73!
3.5.6!Population Management....................................................................... 76!
3.5.7!Conclusions .......................................................................................... 77!
4!THE REPRODUCTIVE ECOLOGY OF MANTA RAYS (MANTA ALFREDI) OFF
MAUI, HAWAII, WITH AN EMPHASIS ON BODY SIZE.................................. 79!
4.1!ABSTRACT...................................................................................................... 79!
4.2!INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................ 79!
4.2.1!Reproductive Cycles ............................................................................ 82!
4.2.2!Role of Body Size ................................................................................ 83!
4.3!METHODS ....................................................................................................... 84!
4.3.1!Data Collection..................................................................................... 84!
4.3.2!Data Analysis ....................................................................................... 86!
4.3.3!Reproductive Cycles ............................................................................ 86!
v
4.3.4!Role of Body Size ................................................................................ 87!
4.4!RESULTS ......................................................................................................... 88!
4.4.1!Reproductive Cycles ............................................................................ 90!
4.4.2!Role of Body Size ................................................................................ 93!
4.5!DISCUSSION ................................................................................................... 98!
4.5.1!Reproductive Cycles ............................................................................ 98!
4.5.2!Role of Body Size .............................................................................. 101!
4.5.3!Conclusion.......................................................................................... 104!
5!SUMMARY AND GENERAL DISCUSSION....................................................... 105!
5.1!Using paired-laser photogrammetry as a simple and accurate system to measure
the body size of free-ranging manta rays (Manta alfredi) ............................. 106!
5.2!Characteristics of a manta ray (Manta alfredi) population off Maui, Hawaii,
and implications for management .................................................................. 107!
5.3!The reproductive ecology of manta rays (Manta alfredi) off Maui, Hawai‘i, with
an emphasis on body size ............................................................................... 109!
5.4!SUMMARY.................................................................................................... 111!
LITERATURE CITED .................................................................................................. 113!
vi
LIST OF TABLES
Table 1. Disc Width (DW) in meters for M. alfredi reported at pre-birth, birth, maturity,
and maximum size, by gender, and for different regions around the world. ............ 10!
Table 2. The total number of different individuals photo-identified and the estimated
abundance, where available, for manta ray (Manta alfredi) populations around the
globe.......................................................................................................................... 16!
Table 3. Comparison of the mean disc ratio (DR) between male and female, and between
adult male and juvenile male manta rays (M. alfredi). ............................................. 40!
Table 4. The number of surveys conducted and mean sighting rates for years 2005
through 2009, broken down into time of day (600-1000, 1000-1400, 1400-1600) and
season (Nov-Apr, May-Oct). Sighting rates are calculated as the mean number of
manta rays observed per hour of survey effort. ........................................................ 60!
Table 5. Surveys, mean sighting rates, mean rate of new individuals, proportion of males,
and proportion of mating trains with standard deviations (SD) listed by month and
season........................................................................................................................ 65!
Table 6. Summary of surveys conducted by month and season showing the total number
and proportion of surveys observed with mating trains and pregnant females. The
proportions of surveys with trains and pregnant females were significantly higher
during the winter season. .......................................................................................... 89!
Table 7. The resight history of 21 nuclear females (NF) observed between years 2005 and
2010. Numbers indicate the month in which they were observed as a NF during that
year with train size indicated in brackets. Bolded IDs indicate females observed in a
train during a summer month.................................................................................... 91!
Table 8. The resight history and disc width (DW), if available, of 20 pregnant females
during the years 2005 through 2010. P indicates she was observed pregnant and her
pregnancy was new for that year; N indicates she was observed enough times during
that year to determine she was unlikely pregnant; U indicates the she was observed
during that year but not sufficiently often to determine if she was visibly pregnant;
and a dash (-) indicates she was not sighted during that entire year. Bolded IDs
indicate females observed pregnant during a summer month. Estimated Pregnancy
Rates (EPR) based on a minimum of 3 yrs with sufficient data, and Estimate
Consecutive Pregnancy Rates (ECPR) based on a minimum of 2 consecutive yrs
with sufficient data are shown. ................................................................................. 92!
Table 9. The resight history and disc width (DW), if available, of 22 individual nuclear
primary escorts (N1Es) during the years 2005 through 2010. N1E indicates he was
the primary escort in a mating train, and the bracketed numbers represent the month
followed by the train size. S indicates the male was sighted but not as a N1E during
vii
that year. A dash (-) signifies he was not sighted during any surveys for that entire
year. Bolded IDs indicate a male observed escorting in a train during a summer
month. ....................................................................................................................... 97!
viii
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1. M. alfredi showing: (a) unique ventral markings, and (b) dorsal shoulder
patches with white chevron......................................................................................... 4!
Figure 2. A pair of green, underwater laser pointers mounted in parallel to an underwater
video housing. ........................................................................................................... 30!
Figure 3. (a) Pipe of known length being measured on the ocean floor showing the
projected points of light 60 cm apart; (b) a photograph of a manta ray (M. alfredi)
from above showing the projected points of light along the spinal axis of the disc
from which a DL measurement can be obtained. ..................................................... 32!
Figure 4. A plot of the expected, undistorted length of an object (in pixels) if measured
with a flat lens against the actual measured length of the same object (in pixels)
distorted by the wide-angle lens. The data are fitted with a linear regression
equation..................................................................................................................... 39!
Figure 5. Sixty-four manta ray DW measurements plotted against its corresponding DL
measurement and fitted with a linear regression. Measurements are in meters........ 41!
Figure 6. The distribution of manta ray (M. alfredi) disc widths by gender and age class.
Females were considered adults if obviously pregnant or showed visible mating
scars. Not all nuclear females were pregnant or had mating scars. Males with clasper
lengths extending beyond the pelvic fins were classified as adult, even with the
pelvic fins were classified as transition, and shorter than the pelvic fins were
classified as juvenile. White boxes represent female categories, gray boxes represent
male categories, numbers represent sample sizes, and circles represent outliers. All
measurements are in millimeters. ............................................................................. 42!
Figure 7. The proportion of males which are sexually mature for each disc width size
category. The black column indicates the size category for which nearly 50% of the
males can be considered mature (DW50). Numbers above the column indicate the
sample size. ............................................................................................................... 43!
Figure 8. Map showing the study area 450 m from the shoreline. The start point of each
survey and the clockwise survey route are shown. ................................................... 54!
Figure 9. Discovery curve illustrating the cumulative number of new manta ray
identifications against the cumulative number of all identifications. Dark circles
represent surveys conducted in the winter (November – April) and light circles
represent surveys conducted in the summer (May - October). ................................ 61!
Figure 10. Map showing the range of individual manta rays (M. alfredi) either matched
with photo-identifications (solid arrows) or tracked with an acoustic tag (dashed
arrows). ..................................................................................................................... 63!
ix
Figure 11. The proportion of individual manta rays identified plotted against the number
of surveys in which they were observed. .................................................................. 67!
Figure 12. The sighting history of a notable female (ID#5003) observed multiple times as
a NF. Numbers in brackets indicate the number of animals in the mating train....... 94!
Figure 13. Distribution of manta ray disc widths. Heavy black lines = means, box
boundaries = 25th and 75th percentiles, whiskers = smallest and largest observed
values that are not statistical outliers, circles = statistical outliers, numbers = sample
sizes. *not significantly different, p = 0.232, #not significantly different, p = 0.363.
................................................................................................................................... 95!
Figure 14. The sighting history of a notable male (ID#13007) with a DW of 2.94 m,
observed pursuing a female on seven occasions. The behavioral role(s) observed are
indicated for each sighting as well as the train size in brackets................................ 99!
x
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The completion of this dissertation could not have been completed without the support,
hard work, dedication, collaboration and patience of many people. To my dive buddies,
Robin Deakos, Jonathan Whitney, Allan Ligon, Edward Lyman, Donna Brown, Lars
Bejder and Jason Larese who often endured ankle breaking surface swims, crippling
winds and swell, merciless currents, and horrendous visibility to assist with data
collection, my sincere gratitude. For their assistance with the paired-laser
photogrammetry design and calibrations, my thanks go out to Robin Deakos, Scott Spitz,
and Brian Brainstetter. For the grueling task of matching hundreds of manta ray photo-
identifications, thank you to Allan Ligon, Jonathan Whitney, Lesley Czechowic and Elisa
Weiss. The active tracking portion of this study could not have been achieved without the
expertise of Tim Clark, and the commitment of Jonathan Whitney and Lee James. A big
mahalo to Ultimate Whale Watching Adventures for providing the research vessel and
the University of Hawai‘i Zoology department for supplying the Vemco VR100 acoustic
receiver and the VH110 directional hydrophone. To my committee members Louis
Herman, Adam Pack, Joe Mobley, Karl Minke, and Patricia Couvillon, thank you for
your support, patience, and guidance. For their valuable feedback on earlier versions of
this manuscript I give my utmost appreciation to my committee, Tracy Adams, John
Deakos, Mari Smultea, William White, Jill Kubatko, Susan Yin, Scott Spitz and
anonymous reviewers. A special thanks to G. Scott Mills for his constructive comments,
edits and advice while we cruised the open ocean aboard the Oscar Elton Sette. Jason
Baker and Lars Bejder were particularly helpful with comments on the population
chapter. I look forward to publishing collaborative works with them on manta ray
population modeling in the near future. To Hal Whitehead, thank you for the
development of SOCPROG and for the advice on computational analysis. Additional
gratitude goes to Andrea Marshall (a.k.a. Manta Queen), William White, and Guy
Stevens for sharing their observations on manta ray populations on the other side of the
globe. I wish them the very best in their continued efforts to understand and protect
manta rays worldwide. Thanks to Joe Mobley for assistance with the animal care permit
issuance through the University of Hawai‘i and to Dave Pence who’s training and
xi
guidance gave me the skills to conquer the underwater world. Research was conducted
under the University of Hawai‘i Animal Care & Use Committee, Protocol No. 08-591-2,
and Assurance number A3423-01. I am indebted to Tracy Adams for her relentless
support, endurance to see me through this project, for tolerating my many years without a
real job, and for not asking me when I would be finished during my final year of writing.
My thanks to the Adams family for their generous hospitality and allowing me into their
home. And lastly, I am grateful to my primary advisor, Louis Herman, and the Deakos
family for their continued support throughout this extended endeavor.
xii
ABSTRACT
Late maturity, few offspring, and a residential nature, typical of Manta alfredi, make this
species particularly vulnerable to localized anthropogenic threats, and much less likely to
recover from depleted populations. Understanding the population characteristics and
reproductive ecology of this species is critical for its successful management. Paired-laser
photogrammetry, combined with photo-identification and active tracking were used to
describe the population characteristics, demographics, habitat range and use, and
reproductive ecology of a resident population of manta rays (Manta alfredi) in Hawai‘i.
Paired-laser photogrammetry proved to be a simple, non-invasive, accurate (mean error
of 0.39%), and precise (CV = 0.54%) method for sizing free-ranging manta rays. A total
of 286 surveys were conducted between 2005 and 2010 at a known aggregation site off
Maui, Hawai‘i. A total of 290 different individual manta rays were photo-identified. A
discovery curve showed no asymptotic trend, indicating the number of individuals using
the area was much larger than the total identified. Resights and manta follows revealed a
home range spanning Maui County waters with visits to the four-islands but did not
include the Big Island, supporting independent, island-associated stocks. High resight
rates within and across years at the study site provided strong evidence of site fidelity.
Findings were consistent with a population of manta rays moving into and out of the
Maui aggregation area, with a varying portion of the total population temporarily resident
at any given time. Males, accounted for 53% of all individuals, and resided for shorter
periods than females around the study site. Manta rays were usually absent at first light
with numbers increasing throughout the day. Shark predation was evident in 33% of
individuals, and alarmingly, 10% had an amputated or non-functional cephalic fin, likely
caused by entanglement in monofilament fishing line. Repeated measurements on 154
different manta rays, produced a mean CV of 1.46%, providing further support for the
paired-laser system. Sexual dimorphism was evident with the largest female (3.64 m
DW) 19% larger than the largest male (3.05 m DW). Sexual maturity in females, based
on evidence of pregnancy and mating scars, was conservatively determined to be 3.37 m
DW. The DW at which 50% of the males were likely to begin maturation and accelerated
clasper growth was between 2.70 and 2.80 m DW. The absence of individuals smaller
than 2.50 m DW suggests age class segregation may be occurring in this population.
xiii
Although mating trains and late-term pregnant females were observed at all times of the
year, they were more likely to occur during the winter months. Females seemed capable
of ovulating multiple times during a year if their initial mating attempts are unsuccessful.
Estrus may last at least several days based on repeated sightings of the same female in a
mating train over several days. Sexual maturity appears delayed in both males and
females until their body size exceeds 90% of their maximum size, an indicator that large
body size provides a reproductive advantage. Larger females had higher pregnancy rates,
and were more likely to reproduce in successive years. No evidence was found to support
direct physical competition between males for access to available females. Endurance
rivalry may be occurring in which females select males who are able to stay with her
mating train over a long duration, possibly days. Although male body size was not a
predictor of proximity to the female within a mating train, larger males may benefit from
greater physical resources, allowing them to follow a female for longer periods,
especially if no feeding is taking place while in a mating train. Further investigation on
male reproductive strategies is needed. The Maui aggregation site seems to be an
important staging area for breeding members of this population. This small,
demographically independent population appears vulnerable to the impacts from non-
target fisheries, primarily from entanglement in fishing line, and could suffer from
exploitation by unregulated “swim-with manta ray” programs. Proper conservation
measures are recommended to ensure the future preservation of this unique habitat and to
minimize anthropogenic impacts in this region that may negatively impact the population.
Management on an island-area basis is recommended. Long-term, repeated
measurements using paired-laser photogrammetry on known individuals of a population
could provide additional population growth parameters to assist in effective management
of this poorly understood species.
1
1 GENERAL INTRODUCTION
1.1 Introduction
Elasmobranchii are cartilaginous fish that include sharks, skates, and rays, many of which
are severely threatened by human activities (W. White & Kyne, 2010). Despite a growing
interest in elasmobranch research, accurate life history and behavioral information of
most species remain incomplete. Without an understanding of the basic biology and
ecology of these species, effective management policies are difficult to implement (Bres,
1993). The research presented here examines one of the largest living elasmobranch
species, Manta alfredi.
Because the behavior and biology of manta rays and other elasmobranchs are not
commonly known, this dissertation begins with a brief overview of elasmobranch
evolution followed by a summary of existing information on manta rays. Following this
introduction, questions are raised addressing the gaps in our understanding of resident
manta rays with a special emphasis on anthropogenic threats and effective management
based on their life history and habitat use.
1.2 A Brief Evolutionary History of Elasmobranchs
Archeological evidence suggests that the first vertebrates emerged about 500 million
years ago (mya) during the Cambrian period, and gave rise to two successful evolutionary
lines of fishes 100 million years later, Chondrichthyes and Osteichthyes. Although
Chondrichthyans retained the flexible, cartilaginous skeleton of their ancestors,
Osteichthyans replaced cartilage with bone giving rise to present-day bony fish,
comprising over 25,000 living species, the most abundant among vertebrates.
Today’s living sharks, rays, and chimaeras, which include over 1100 species, can trace
their common ancestry to early Chondrichthyans (for review see Leonard Compagno,
2
Dando, & Fowler, 2005). The chimaeras consist of only 40 species (4% of
Chondrichthyans), making up the subclass Halocephali, and are characterized by a single
gill slit covered by an operculum. The remaining 96% of Chondrichthyans belong to the
class Elasmobranchii (sharks and rays) easily recognized by the five to seven paired gill
openings on each side of the head. Sharks account for about 500 species and the rays or
"batoid" fishes (superorder Batoidea), include over 600 species. The first sharks appeared
about 400 mya while the first rays did not appear until about 150-200 mya.
Rays can be thought of as flat-bodied sharks, with large, modified pectoral fins, a likely
adaptation to their bottom dwelling existence (Holmgren, 1940). The pectoral fins have
become fused to the side of the head forming a flattened disc and the anal fin is no longer
present. Ray propulsion is no longer done with the trunk and tail but with their large,
expanded pectoral fins (Rosenberger, 2001). The 5-7 pairs of gill slits sit below the
pectoral fins unlike sharks whose gills slits are positioned above the pectoral fins. Six
orders of living rays are recognized and include the Pristiformes (sawfishes), Rhiniformes
(sharkfin guitarfishes or wedgefishes), Rhinobatiformes (guitarfishes), Torpedoniformes
(electric rays), Rajiformes (skates), and Myliobatiformes (stingrays).
The largest rays belong to the Myliobatiforme order, and more specifically to the family
Mobulidae, also known as “devil rays.” The eleven species of devil ray all possess a
characteristic pair of cephalic fins that protrude out from the front of the head. These
unique structures aid in guiding food and water into their mouths and are furled during
travel giving the appearance of a pair of horns. Devil rays represent two distinct genera,
Mobula, composed of nine species, and Manta composed of two species. Mobula rays
have their mouths positioned ventrally while manta rays have their mouths projecting
forward instead of downward.
1.3 Manta Rays
Manta rays are unique in that they have evolved to take advantage of large abundances of
zooplankton that inhabit the open water. Their large, rectangular mouths project forward
3
instead of downward to facilitate feeding. The spiracles, (a pair of small vestigial gill
slits), although still present, are no longer used. Instead, water enters through the manta
ray’s mouth while they swim, passes over the gills, providing oxygen to the blood. They
have evolved large pectoral fins that are used like wings to propel themselves through the
water. Their skin is covered with dermal denticals (small tooth-like structures) much like
their shark cousins. A mucus coating covers their skin, creating an important defense
against infection.
Manta rays are the largest rays in the Mobulidae family and until recently, the genus was
thought to consist of just a single species, Manta birostris. Recent evidence based on
morphology and meristic (quantitative features of fish) data has confirmed at least a
second species in the genus, Manta alfredi (for review see AD Marshall, Compagno, &
Bennett, 2009). M. birostris herein referred to as “oceanic manta rays” due to their
pelagic habitat range, can be differentiated from M. alfredi visually in the field by their
much larger size, their coloration, and the presence of a caudal spine. At the base of the
tail just below the dorsal fin, oceanic manta rays have retained a calcified mass that
contains a small, embedded spine, essentially a vestige of their ancestry. M. alfredi,
herein referred to as “resident manta raysdue to their site fidelity to coastal habitats, do
not possess this calcified mass. When differentiating dead specimens, differences in the
appearance of the skin and denticle morphology, as well as the number of teeth present
on the lower jaw can also be used to identify species. On the bottom jaw exists 12-16
rows of small cusped teeth in oceanic manta rays, and 6-8 rows in resident manta rays,
with no teeth existing in the upper jaw for either species. These very small teeth barely
penetrate the skin covering and are another vestige of an evolutionary era when their
ancestors used their teeth to feed.
1.3.1 Coloration
Each manta ray has a ventral spot pattern that is unique. These patterns are visible at birth
(Andrea D. Marshall, Pierce, & Bennett, 2008), and appear to remain unchanged over the
life of the individual (T. B. Clark, 2001; Homma, Maruyama, Itoh, Ishihara, & Uchida,
4
1999; Kitchen-Wheeler, 2010). These unique patterns allow researchers to discriminate
and track individuals over time.
The most common resident manta morph has a black dorsal cape with white shoulder
patches and a white chevron that stretches anteriorly from the insertion point of the dorsal
fin (for review see AD Marshall et al., 2009). The ventral side is typically cream to white
in color with variable dark markings that can occupy the entire ventral surface but most
are concentrated towards the center of the disc (Figure 1).
Figure 1. M. alfredi showing: (a) unique ventral markings, and (b) dorsal shoulder
patches with white chevron.
A melanistic morph exists that is almost completely black on both the dorsal and ventral
surfaces except for a white blaze along the mid-line that is variable in size. The much less
common leucistic “white manta” morph has an almost entirely white dorsal surface and a
ventral surface that is much lighter in overall coloration. Less than 20 of these have been
observed worldwide (AD Marshall et al., 2009).
5
The tremendously large body size of manta rays has likely been beneficial in reducing
predation pressure. With less predation pressure, the benefits of counter-shading become
less important, possibly allowing morph variants to proliferate successfully (G.
Notarbartolo-di-Sciara & Hillyer, 1989).
1.3.2 Sensory Systems
Although most elasmobranchs have relatively small brains, both devil rays and
galeomorph sharks independently evolved large telencephalons and cerebellums,
characterized by high brain:body ratios (Northcutt, 1977). What function the enlarged
telencephalon and cerebellum areas play or why they evolved together remains unclear
(Hofmann, 1999).
As manta rays move throughout their aquatic environment, they process information
through many different sensory channels allowing for a complex repertoire of signals and
behaviors to be used for finding food, mates, escaping predators, and to facilitate social
interactions with conspecifics. These sophisticated senses incorporate: 1) an olfactory and
gustatory system, 2) a visual system, 3) a mechanosensory system, which includes
hearing and touch, and 4) an electrosensory system (Bleckmann & Hofmann, 1999).
Elasmobranchs have a very good sense of olfaction and taste (Hodgson & Mathewson,
1978; Kleerekoper, 1978; GH Parker, 1914) and can use these senses to detect
biochemical products released by other organisms that may be prey, mates, or
conspecifics.
Although often thought of as secondary to olfaction, elasmobranchs have a well-
developed visual system (Gruber & Cohen, 1978; Hart, Lisney, & Collin, 2006). The
acute vision of elasmobranchs not only assists with detecting prey and avoiding
predators, but is also used for inter- and intraspecific communication (Hart et al., 2006).
A Manta ray’s eyes are located laterally just behind the cephalic fins, giving them the
ability to see forward and downward very easily. Binocular vision is likely when looking
6
downward but the ability to see upward and behind their body appears to be impaired (M.
Deakos, pers. observ., 2005-2010).
The movement of animals through the water when traveling, turning, or struggling,
creates water displacement and water pressure waves (sound) that can provide a source of
biological information for those capable of detecting these signals. Three mechanisms
exist in elasmobranchs to detect acoustic (hydrodynamic) events: the inner ear, the lateral
line, and the sense of touch.
The hearing sensitivity of manta rays is unknown but that of most elasmobranchs is very
acute. Conditioning studies have shown sharks most sensitive to low frequency sounds in
the vicinity of 100 Hz (D. Nelson, 1967), the frequency often produced by struggling
prey (e.g., D. Nelson & Gruber, 1963).
All elasmobranchs have a lateral line system much like that present in bony fish (D.
Nelson, 1967) and aquatic amphibians (Lannoo, 1987). In dorsoventrally flattened
batoids, they occur primarily around the expanded pectoral fins (Bleckmann & Hofmann,
1999). Short-tailed Stingrays (Dasyatis brevicaudata) were capable of detecting weak
vertical jets of water created by buried bivalves, their primary food source, through the
elaborate network of lateral line canals on its ventral surface (Montgomery & Skipworth,
1997). Nothing is known about the lateral line system of manta rays.
All elasmobranchs are electroreceptive, detecting electric stimuli passively (Collin and
Whitehead, 2004), but some skates and rays are electrogenic (Bratton & Ayers, 1987),
capable of using electricity actively to communicate or to stun and capture their prey.
Electrosense is used in round stingrays (Urolophus halleri) by reproductively active
males to locate mates, and by females to locate buried consexuals (Tricas, Michael, &
Sisneros, 1995). The electrosensitivity of manta rays is unknown.
Despite their sensitivity to low frequency sound, few elasmobranchs can produce their
own sounds beyond the usual hydrodynamic noises (Myrberg, 1981). Coles (1916)
7
observed and killed 11 oceanic manta rays off North Carolina, USA, and noted a harsh
bear-like cough emitted during the kill. Although reported as manta vocalizations, the
sounds were more likely the product of air being released from various air-filled cavities.
Based on their physiology, it seems unlikely that any audible sounds could be produced
by submerged mobulids (G. Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, 1988). Sound production in
elasmobranchs has been reported only for the cownose ray (Rhinoptera bonasus), which
produces sounds by scraping together its dental plates (Myrberg, 1981).
The first and only cognitive experimental research study performed on a captive oceanic
manta ray was carried out at the Lisboa Oceanarium in Portugal (Ari & Correia, 2008).
The young male manta used visual cues (the feed bucket) and olfactory cues (scent of
shrimp) to condition to a feed session that was given at the same time and place each day.
The manta’s sense of smell was acute, showing detection of 0.3 L of shrimp extract in
4700 m3 of water, but visual cues were equally likely to generate a food searching
response. The characteristics of the bucket that were being used as the visual cue (size,
shape, color) were not determined. During later feed sessions that were not cued, the
young male was able to remember the time and location of the feed session. This was
attributed to use of an internal biological clock and long-term spatial memory.
1.3.3 Morphology
The sheer size of manta rays makes it difficult to obtain morphological measurements
unless dead specimens become available (G. Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, 1987). The few
morphological reports that do exist on maximum disc width, size at maturity, coloration,
tooth counts, and presence of a tail spine have been variable and confusing (AD Marshall
et al., 2009), and the use of non-standard metrics has made it difficult to compare
between reports.
Although the standard method of reporting size in manta rays is disc width (measured
from wing tip to wing tip), proportional measurements using disc length (DL; measured
from the tip of the snout to the posterior edge of the pectoral fins), are sometimes used for
comparison, especially when a specimen’s wings have been severed or are have
8
deteriorated to the point that an accurate disc width (DW) measurement cannot be made
(Andrea D. Marshall et al., 2008). DWs have been reported to be 2.2 – 2.3 times larger
than their DLs (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953; AD Marshall et al., 2009). Determining
growth occurs proportionally through all stages of the animal’s life is important when
making these morphology conversions. For manta rays, other than differences seen in the
anterior head region, eye diameter, interspiracle length, and cephalic fin length, which
may develop disproportionately, all other morphometric measurements appear to develop
proportionately from the fetus to adulthood (Andrea D. Marshall et al., 2008).
A summary of existing documentation for DW measurements on resident manta rays is
shown in Table 1.
1.3.4 Reproduction
Manta rays are ovoviparous, giving birth to a live young that hatches from an egg, carried
inside the mother. After hatching from the egg case that is attached to the female’s
oviduct, the pup continues to feed on the mother’s uterine milk until fully developed. The
pup is born live as a miniature version of the adult.
In free-ranging manta rays some speculate that they may give birth in relatively shallow
water where the pups remain for several years before expanding their range (Ginis,
2002). The only reported birth was for a resident manta female in captivity at the
Okinawa Expo Aquarium following what was determined to be a 12 -13 month gestation
period (Senzo Uchida, Toda, & Matsumoto, 2008). The newborn was abandoned
immediately after birth and the mother was seen mating again within hours.
Pregancy rates for resident manta rays have been estimated using long term sighting
records of females observed pregnant every 2-3 years on average, with some females
becoming pregnant in consecutive years (Homma et al., 1999; AD Marshall & Bennett,
2010). Although giving birth to a single pup at a time appears to be the norm for both
species based on disections of pregnant females (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953; Coles,
9
1916; Lamont, 1824; Lesueur, 1824), two pups may be conceived on occasion (AD
Marshall & Bennett, 2010).
The first detailed description of a manta ray mating sequence was for oceanic manta rays
observed off Ogasawara Island, Japan (Yano, Sato, & Takahashi, 1999). The sequence of
events involved: (1) a male following directly behind a female for 20-30 minutes making
several attempts to bite her pectoral fin as she traveled at a speed of 10 km/hr; (2) the
male succeeds in biting the female’s pectoral fin positions his ventral side against hers;
(3) the male inserts his clasper into the female’s cloaca for 60 – 90 seconds; (4) the male
remove’s his clasper and continues to hold her pectoral fin in his mouth for several
minutes; and (5) the male releases the female’s pectoral fin and both individuals swim
apart. This mating sequence was observed in 10-20 m water depth over rocky reefs and
100-200 m from the beach.
Similar mating train behavior has been described for resident manta rays off Yaeyama
Island, Japan (Homma et al., 1999), and at North Male Atoll in the Republic of the
Maldives (Stevens & Rubin, 2008). In the Maldives, mating trains are reported to consist
of 1 – 21 males chasing a single, fast swimming female. The number of males in the train
increases then decreases before copulation takes place.
Marshall & Bennett (2010) reported strong lateralization of mating scars in females with
99% of the scars visible on the left pectoral fin. Yano et al. (1999) reported mating scars
on both wings of the female but only fresh scars were observed on the left wing.
Mating events appear to be restricted to certain times of the year but the time of year will
vary by region (Homma et al., 1999; AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010; Stevens & Rubin,
2008). Off Ogasawara Island, Japan, most mating trains were observed between March
and October with occasional chases observed outside the summer season (Yano et al.,
1999). Mating behavior in Yaeyama Island was reported during the spring and autumn
and lasting for about a one month duration (Homma et al., 1999). The wet season in this
area begins in May and lasts through June, and the typhoon season (May – November),
typically peaks in September. In the Maldives, the majority of mating trains were
10
Table 1. Disc Width (DW) in meters for M. alfredi reported at pre-birth, birth, maturity, and maximum size, by gender, and for
different regions around the world.
Age
Gender
Location
DW Size (m)
Method of
Measurement
Reference
Pre-birth
Male
S. Africa
1.33
dead specimen
(Andrea D. Marshall et al.,
2008)
Smallest Free-
Swimming
Male
S. Mozambique
1.50
field estimate
(AD Marshall & Bennett,
2010)
Maturity
Female
S. Africa
3.90
field estimate
(AD Marshall & Bennett,
2010)
Male
S. Mozambique
3.00
field estimate
(AD Marshall & Bennett,
2010)
Max
Unknown
S. Mozambique
5.50
field estimate
(AD Marshall & Bennett,
2010)
Female
Ogasawara, Japan
5.00
field estimate
(Yano et al., 1999)
Female
Wakayama, Japan
5.00
field estimate, using
boat as comparison
(Yanagisawa, 1994)
Female
Okinawa, Japan
4.65
unknown
(S Uchida, 1994)
Male
Ogaswara, Japan
4.00
field estimate
(Yano et al., 1999)
11
between October and November, towards the end of their wet season (April to October),
with occasional trains observed outside this season (Stevens & Rubin, 2008). In
Mozambique, fresh mating scars were observed primarily during the summer months
(AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010).
Although mating trains can be reliably seen, acts of copulation in free ranging manta rays
are rare (AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010; Yano et al., 1999).
1.3.5 Range and Habitat
Resident manta rays are more likely to be observed in shallow coastal areas around rocky
and coral reef habitats where productive upwellings exist, commonly sighted inshore,
within a few kilometers of land. They have been observed as far north as the Yaeyama
Islands, Japan, Hawai‘i, the Canary Islands, the Red Sea, Sri Lanka, and Thailand, and as
far south as the Solitary Islands, Australia, French Plynesia, Senegal, Durban, South
Africa, the Maldives, and Perth, Australia. No sightings exist for the eastern Pacific and
except for two sightings off the coast of Senegal from northwest Africa, sightings for the
eastern Atlantic are extremely rare (Cadenat, 1958). In general, they can be found in
tropical and subtropical regions of the Pacific, Atlantic, and Indian oceans within 30
degrees of latitude to the north and south of the equator (Marshall et al. 2009).
Congregations occur around rich food sources or at specific locations on the reef known
as cleaning stations (Losey Jr, 1972) where individuals solicit host cleaner fish to remove
parasitic copepods from their body’s surface. Strong site fidelity occurring at specific
feeding and cleaning stations (e.g., Homma et al., 1999) has created popular tourist
attractions where visitors pay to swim or scuba dive with the manta rays (T. B. Clark,
2001; Dewar et al., 2008).
Some well known resident manta ray aggregation areas worldwide that have become
popular tourist destination areas include Komodo Marine Park, Indonesia (Dewar et al.,
2008), Yap, Micronesia (Homma et al., 1999), Palau, Yaeyama, Okinawa, Japan
12
(Kashiwagi, Ito, Ovenden, & Bennett, 2008), Kona , Hawai‘i (T. B. Clark, 2001), Bora
Bora, French Polynesia (de Rosemont, 2008), Mozambique (A. D. Marshall, 2009), the
Republic of Maldives (C. R. Anderson, Shiham, Joaquim, Kitchen-Wheeler, & Stevens,
2008; Stevens & Rubin, 2008), and Ningaloo Western Australia (Daw & McGregor,
2008; McGregor, Van Keulen, Waite, & Meekan, 2008). In the Republic of Maldives,
divers are taken to watch manta rays at the same diving spot each time demonstrating
daily site fidelity is strong.
Long term sighting records at established aggregation sites suggest that M. alfredi may be
philopatric to these areas and may exhibit smaller home ranges (Dewar et al., 2008;
Homma et al., 1999; A. Kitchen-Wheeler, 2008; A. D. Marshall, 2009). Areas of high
productivity could eliminate the need for manta rays to migrate to other areas (T. B.
Clark, 2001) by providing sufficient food resources year-round. Thus far, no evidence of
inter-island migration exists from a known manta ray population in Kona, Hawai‘i (T. B.
Clark, 2001).
1.3.5.1 Seasonality
Although most resident manta populations appear to have year-round residents (e.g.
Bora-Bora, Hawai‘i, Mozambique) some populations (e.g. the Republic Maldive Islands,
Yap Island,) occur seasonally (Dewar et al., 2008). In the Republic Maldive Islands, the
manta rays are known to migrate from one side of the island to the other coinciding with
the seasonally alternating monsoon currents that are responsible for the plankton blooms
(C. R. Anderson et al., 2008; Homma et al., 1999). In Yaeyama Island, some individuals
are year-round residents but others have been reported to migrate annually to Kerama
Island, about 350 km to the east (Homma et al., 1999). In Yap, manta rays use different
channels in the summer and winter (T. B. Clark, 2001). Fishers targeting manta rays will
set gill nets at the same channel each year for guaranteed catches (Homma et al., 1999).
Some migrations are simply diurnal, where manta rays migrate between feeding and
cleaning stations (Homma et al., 1999).
13
1.3.6 Food and Foraging
Manta rays appear to feed on small planktonic organisms such as euphasids, copepods,
mysids, decapod larvae, and possibly shrimp, (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953; T. B. Clark,
2001; Last & Stevens, 1994). Stomach contents from an oceanic manta specimen from
South Carolina contained fragments resembling the shells of shrimps as well as remnants
of a small crab (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953).
When feeding, manta rays unfurl their cephalic fins to help direct the plankton rich water
into their mouths and over the five pairs of gills. Finger-like projections on the gill
arches, known as gill-rakers, strain and capture the food. This type of feeding is termed
ram-jet feeding. The manta rays sometimes swim in repeated summersaults through a
dense patch of plankton.
Coles (1916) reported mobulids using their cephalic fins to herd small minnows up
against the beach and funnel them into their mouth. And although manta rays have been
reportedly seen gulping schools of small mullet (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953), it is more
likely that the mullet were simply feeding on the same patches of plankton targeted by
the manta rays.
Very little is known about the feeding behavior of resident manta rays. They have been
seen feeding during the daytime and at night, as single individuals, and in large
aggregations. In some regions, artificial lights above and below the water are used to
attract large amounts of zooplankton, which in turn attracts resident manta rays looking to
take advantage of an easy meal. Dive operators in Kona, Hawai‘i, have conditioned
manta rays to aggregate each night around a group of scuba divers whose underwater
lights serve to attract plankton into the area.
The abundance of plankton is much less in the open ocean and tends to be more dense
around upwellings and around island groups (Lalli & Parsons, 1993). Nutrient rich
upwellings induced by trade winds can also create conditions suitable for high primary
14
production and thus may create important feeding areas for manta rays (G. Notarbartolo-
di-Sciara & Hillyer, 1989).
Temperature is also implicated in the pattern of zooplankton diversity which is correlated
closely with sea-surface temperature and decreases rapidly with depth (Rutherford,
D'Hondt, & Prell, 1999). Plankton are known to take regular and even daily vertical
migrations (Pillar, Armstrong, & Hutchings, 1989) and manta rays may adjust their
feeding regime in order to take advantage of these diel cycles. In Bateman Bay, Western
Australia, resident manta rays are observed feeding about 40% of the time, throughout
the year, on small calanoid copepods (McGregor et al., 2008).
1.3.7 Population Size
Based on photo-identifying the unique spot patterns on the underside of manta rays,
population sizes have been estimated for various aggregation areas around the world.
These reported population sizes are listed in Table 2.
1.3.8 Behavior
1.3.8.1 Cleaning Stations
Although manta ray congregations do occur around rich food sources (C. R. Anderson et
al., 2008), manta rays are also known to congregate at specific locations along the reef
referred to as “cleaning stations.” A cleaning station is where individuals solicit host
cleaner fish to remove parasites and clean wounds from their body’s surface (Losey Jr,
1972). When more than one host cleaner fish species shares in the cleaning behavior,
each may focus on a different region of the manta’s body in order to reduce interspecific
competition (A. D. Marshall, 2009).
Although Mobula species are reported to be free from parasites, Mantas can have parts of
their bodies thickly covered with several species of parasitic crustaceans (Coles, 1916). In
Japan, the parasitic copepods are thought to belong to the Pandaridae family (Homma et
al., 1999). For clients, the main benefits to being cleaned are likely a reduction in
15
ectoparasite load but this has been difficult to measure due to the methodological
problems in quantifying ectoparasite density (Coté, 2000).
Host cleaner fish vary by region. In Yaeyama Island, Japan, the cleaner wrasse
(Labroides dimidiatus) and other small shrimps commonly do most of the cleaning
(Homma et al., 1999). In Hawai‘i, predominantly Hawaiian cleaner wrasses (Labroides
phthirophagus) and saddle wrasses (Thalassoma duperrey) are observed removing
parasitic copepods from the surface of the manta ray’s body (M. Deakos, pers. Obs.). T.
duperrey will concentrate mostly on the external body surface, whereas L.
phtyirogphagus will concentrate mainly inside the mouth and around the gill slits. In
Mozambique, various butterfly fish (family Chaetodontidae) specialize in bite wounds
while Seargant Major fish (Abudefduf saxatilis) concentrate more around the mouth
region (Andrea Marshall & Bennett, 2008; A. D. Marshall, 2009).
Aggregations around cleaning stations are commonly used as reliable areas for guided
swim-with manta tours. In some locations, these cleaning stations are active year-round,
while in others the presence of manta rays at inshore reefs is seasonal or erratic (Dewar et
al., 2008).
1.3.8.2 Breaching
Manta rays are occasionally observed leaping partially or completely out of the water;
sometimes one after the other. The purpose of this behavior is unclear (Homma et al.,
1999) but some have suggested that it may be related to mating displays, giving birth, or
an attempt to get rid of parasites or remoras (Remorina albescens) attached to the surface
of the manta ray (E. Clark, 1969). One oceanic manta was observed with seven large
remoras attached to it’s body (Coles, 1916). Manta rays have been observed trying to
remove remoras by rubbing against rocks or the sandy bottom (Homma et al., 1999). The
splash created when reentering the water during a breach can be audible for some
distance away if the seas are calm (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953) and therefore may be
used as an acoustic form of communication.
16
Table 2. The total number of different individuals photo-identified and the estimated abundance, where available, for manta
ray (Manta alfredi) populations around the globe.
Location
Total Different
Individuals
Estimated
Population Size
Time period
Source
Yonara Channel, Japan
185
n/a
1977-1997
Takashi Itoh
(Homma et al., 1999)
Yaeyama, Okinawa, Japan
303
n/a
1987 - 2006
(Kashiwagi et al., 2008)
Yap Island, Micronesia
54
n/a
1990-1997
Bill Acker
(Homma et al., 1999)
Kona, Hawai‘i, USA
170
n/a
1979 - 2010
www.mantapacific.org
French Frigate Shoals, Hawai‘i
40
n/a
1998-1999
(T. B. Clark, 2001)
Ningaloo Reef, Western Australia
300+
n/a
n/a
(McGregor et al., 2008)
Bora Bora, French Polynesia
85
n/a
2002-2005
(de Rosemont, 2008)
Southern Mozambique
449
890
2003-2008
(A. D. Marshall, 2009)
Republic of the Maldives
n/a
2000+
n/a
(C. R. Anderson et al., 2008)
17
1.3.9 Lifespan
The average lifespan of a manta ray is unknown. The longest reported period between the first
and last sighting of the same individual resident manta is 27 years (1980-2006) off Yaeyama
Island, Japan (Kashiwagi et al., 2008).
1.3.10 Threats
1.3.10.1 Natural Predators
Large sharks (Homma et al., 1999) and killer whales (Orcinus orca) (Visser & Bonoccorso,
2003) have been reported to prey on manta rays. It is unknown how many attacks result in
fatalities and whether or not the shark will consume the entire manta ray. Cookie cutter shark
bites have also been observed on manta rays, most likely occurring at night during deeper dives
(Homma et al., 1999).
1.3.10.2 Anthropogenic Threats
The status of most manta ray populations worldwide is poorly understood. They are classified by
the IUCN Red List for Threatened Animals as “near-threatened” (A. D. Marshall et al., 2006),
meaning that manta rays could be threatened with extinction in the near future if conservation
efforts are not implemented.
The number of manta rays that exist worldwide is unknown, and little is known about their
population ecology. The recent reclassification of the genus has new implications for the
conservation assessment of both species (AD Marshall et al., 2009). Each population and
population stock faces its own regional specific ecological pressures. Understanding how a
particular population or population stock is affected by anthropogenic impacts in its region is
critical to understanding its conservation status and for directing effective management.
18
1.3.10.2.1 Directed-Fisheries-
Traditional hunting for manta rays in places like eastern Australia and the Sea of Cortez (Gulf of
California), involved fishers on small boats harpooning slow swimming animals traveling just
below the surface. They were hunted for their skin, the oil from their large livers, and simply for
shark bait, but never in enough quantity to be of commercial importance (Bigelow & Schroeder,
1953). More recently, large international markets have emerged for shark fins, meat, and hides,
resulting in the rapid rise in value for shark products. A new market has also emerged in Asia,
creating a demand for dried manta gill rakers to be used in traditional Chinese medicines and in
the treatment of cancer (Shen, Jia, & Zhou, 2001). These demands, combined with the existing
demand for manta ray cartilage to be used as filler in shark fin soup (Alava, Dolumbaló,
Yaptinchay, & Trono, 2002), has led to an exponential increase in the Indonesian fishery in just a
few years, threatening to drive manta ray populations into commercial extinction (Dewar, 2002;
W. T. White, Giles, Dharmadi, & Potter, 2006). In addition, as a result of over-fishing, fishermen
have turned to hunting manta rays as an alternative source of income, leading to a ten-fold
increase in manta ray harvesting.
These directed fisheries targeting manta rays have caused populations to decline and even
disappear in areas such as Mexico (Homma et al., 1999; G Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, 1995; W. T.
White et al., 2006), the Philippines (Alava et al., 2002), Indonesia (Dewar, 2002; W. T. White et
al., 2006), India, Sri Lanka, and other parts of Southeast Asia (A. D. Marshall et al., 2006). An
estimated 1,500 manta rays were taken over a period of six months in Lamakera, Indonesia
(Anon, 1997). Drift gill nets 700 – 1000 m long and 35 m high are set 7 m below the water
surface during the migratory passage of mobulids and are reported to entrap as many as 50 manta
rays in a single net (Homma et al., 1999). Divers from Palawan Island, Philippines, reported that
the local manta ray population had been reduced to a third of its original population over a
seven-year period (Homma et al., 1999). In Yaeyama Island, Japan, common aggregations of 50
manta rays in 1982 were reduced to just 30 in 1992, and further reduced to no more than 15 by
1999 (Homma et al., 1999).
19
1.1.1.1.1 Ecotourism-
In light of diminishing populations of manta rays from directed fisheries, new interest has
sparked in the area of ecotourism as an alternative and more sustainable use of the resource.
Homma et al. (1999) estimated a dead manta ray sells for about $400 US in the Philippines and
ecotourism could provide the nation with a total annual revenue of $4,800,000 US. If tourists
paid about $400 US each to view manta rays in the wild, 12,000 tourists annually would be
needed to generate the same type of revenue gained from direct hunts and the practice becomes
sustainable as the resource becomes renewable.
The development of manta ray ecotours is becoming a popular recreational activity and a
booming industry in many parts of the world where manta rays are known to aggregate (Dewar
et al., 2008; Yano et al., 1999). These programs can generate tens of thousands and even tens of
millions of dollars of tourist revenue to local communities annually. However, there are serious
concerns over the impact that poorly managed ecotours can have on the behavior of the animals
and the habitats they rely on. Some governments are requiring licenses to operate these ecotours
to help ensure that they do not damage the resource upon which it relies.
1.3.10.2.1.1 Boat*Traffic*
Manta rays can be frequently observed traveling just below the surface and will often approach
or show little fear towards man or vessel (Coles, 1916), which can also make them extremely
vulnerable to boat strikes by vessels traveling at high speed and unaware of an animal in their
path. Several manta rays aggregating in Ningaloo Reef, Western Australia, possess deep scars
where they were most likely struck by a boat propeller (F. McGregor pers. comm., 2007).
Another incident was reported in Hawai‘i in which a manta ray died due to injuries sustained to
the head from a boat propeller (T. Clark, pers. comm., 2006).
In the Florida Keys, a cooperative effort between the National Park Service and NOAA’s Florida
Key Marine Sanctuary restricted boat traffic through a nurse shark mating and nursery ground by
20
using navigation control buoys during the peak of the mating season (Carrier & Pratt 1998).
They found that the presence of boats during mating activities of nurse sharks was disruptive and
often resulted in unsuccessful mating attempts.
1.1.1.1.2 Mooring-and-Anchor-Lines-
Although rare, manta rays on occasion entangle themselves in anchor and mooring lines. It is
believed that when a line makes contact with the front of the head between the cephalic fins, the
reflexive response by the manta is to immediately close the cephalic fins, thereby trapping the
rope and entangling the manta ray when they begin to roll in an attempt to get free. This
hypothesis was recently supported by video footage of a manta ray colliding with a cameraman
and swimming off with the camera after locking its cephalic fins around the camera. Bigelow &
Shroeder (1953) documented several records of a manta ray entangling in an anchor line in the
same way, sometimes towing the boat along for some distance. On at least two occasions, a
manta ray in Hawai‘i was reported to perish after entangling in a mooring line (A. Cummins,
pers. comm., 2007, K. Osada, pers. comm., 2009).
1.3.11 Aquariums
The Okinawa Ocean Expo in Motobu, Okinawa Island, Japan, is the only aquarium in the world
that has successfully housed and bred resident manta rays (Senzo Uchida et al., 2008). The
Atlantis Resort in the Bahamas (Russell, 2008) and more recently the Georgia Aquarium are the
only two facilities in the Western Hemisphere to keep manta rays in captivity. The manta rays on
display at the Atlantis Resort are removed from the wild and housed temporarily before being
returned back to the wild.
The minimum number of aquariums worldwide housing manta rays is a product of the difficulty
in keeping manta rays alive during captivity. There is concern about the extraction of even a
small number of individuals from the wild, especially when the mortality rate is so high.
21
1.3.12 Manta Ray Protection
In many parts of the world, measures have been taken to reduce anthropogenic threats on local
manta ray populations. For example, codes of conduct for manta ray dive operators have been
implemented in Kona, Hawai‘i (unpublished), Western Australia (Daw & McGregor 2008),
Mozambique (A. Marshall, pers. comm., 2007), Bora Bora (M. deRosemont, pers. comm.), and
in the Maldives (G. Stevens, pers. comm., 2007). Elements of the code include minimizing the
number of divers around the manta rays, keeping divers in tight controlled groups, restricting the
touching of the animals, and using approach methods that minimize stress on the manta rays. In
Mozambique, mooring balls are banned in areas where the manta rays are known to aggregate,
and boats are required to minimize their speed. Marine protected areas (MPA) have been
established in the Maldives, Mexico, Mozambique, and Yap, to help eliminate fishing pressure
and provide a safe refuge for the manta rays.
In Hawai‘i, a state law was passed that prevents the intentional killing or extraction of manta
rays from all Hawaiian State waters with an exception for persons granted a special take permit.
A special take permit requires the applicant to demonstrate that removal will not harm the
population. The proposed removal application must evaluate the potential biological removal
(PBR) of the targeted population. PBR is the maximum number of animals, not including natural
mortalities, which may be removed from a stock while allowing that stock to reach or maintain
its optimum sustainable population (Wade, 1998).
1.3.13 Research
In contrast to sharks, which have been the subject of numerous research studies, little research
has been done on manta rays. This difference is primarily due to the difficulty in studying these
large animals in the field. Landings from fisheries provide a source of information on
morphology, including the size demographics of a fished population, descriptions on the size at
sexual and physical maturity, litter sizes, and other important reproductive information. Satellite,
acoustic, and pop-up archival tags are becoming more common for tracking animals that travel
22
large distances and can provide valuable information on home ranges, dive profiles, and diel
patterns of behavior. Mark recapture studies are useful for estimating a population size and
range, and for developing individual life histories.
1.4 Dissertation Overview
Data on the life history and ecology of resident manta rays is limited, despite the species being
circumglobally distributed and aggregation areas being popular attractions for marine tourism
(A. D. Marshall, 2009). Like most sharks, manta rays can be considered equilibrium strategists
(Winemiller & Rose, 1992), having late maturation, long gestation periods, and producing only a
few, large offspring in their lifetime. Their life history characteristics of slow growth and low
fecundity make them a greater risk for population decline from human exploitation (Frisk,
Miller, & Fogarty, 2001; Holden, 1974).
The aim of this dissertation was to improve our understanding of the ecology and behavior of a
manta ray (M. alfredi) population in Hawaiʻi that frequents an aggregation site located off
Maui’s northwestern coastline. An improved understanding of the general ecology of this
population would improve management strategies to protect this population locally and other
populations globally. Three studies are described:
1) Using paired-laser photogrammetry as a simple and accurate system to measure the
body size of free-ranging manta rays (Manta alfredi)
This study examines the use of paired-laser photogrammetry as a tool for measuring free-ranging
manta rays remotely. The accuracy and precision of the technique is quantified and size
demographics of the population are described. Minimum and maximum sizes of males and
females at physical and sexual maturity are examined.
23
2) Characteristics of a manta ray (Manta alfredi) population off Maui, Hawai‘i, and
implications for management
This study applies mark-recapture methodology to estimate the population size of resident manta
rays observed in the study area. Life history information is used to quantify site fidelity. Data
from two acoustically tagged individuals and photo-identification comparisons with other
aggregation areas are used to determine home ranges and the occurrence of interisland-
movements. Basic demographics are described and temporal uses of the study area are examined.
Natural and anthropomorphic threats are discussed and incorporated into suggested strategies for
local management of this population.
3) The reproductive ecology of role manta rays (Manta alfredi) off Maui, Hawai‘i, with an
emphasis on body size
Understanding reproductive strategies and mating behavior is critical for the proper management
of marine fish species (S. Rowe & Hutchings, 2003). In this study, the reproductive ecology of
this population is examined by looking for seasonal trends in mating, pregnancy rates, and mate
choice with an emphasis on how body size plays a role in male and female reproductive success.
1.5 General Methods
The majority of surveys were conducted at a single manta aggregation area off the west coast of
the island of Maui, Hawai‘i, between 2005 and 2010. An area approximately 30,000 m2 (200 m
X 150 m) in size, 450 m offshore, with a depth range of 5 - 30 m was used as the monitoring
boundary for the study site. This location was chosen because of the high reliability of
encountering manta rays and thereby maximizing encounter rates.
The habitat in this area consisted primarily of fringing coral reef that extends away from the
shoreline for approximately 550 m. The substrate cover at depths of 5 m to 15 m was composed
24
of lobe (Porites lobata), rice (Acroporidae spp.), cauliflower (Pocillopora meandrina), and
finger coral (Porites compressa), with intermittent patches of sand and rubble. The substrate
cover at depths between 15 m and 30 m was composed of sandy bottom and sea grass (Halimeda
spp.), interspersed with patches of cauliflower and lobe coral.
The main cleaning stations were situated near the starting point of the survey where manta rays
have been seen soliciting predominantly Hawaiian cleaner wrasses (Labroides phthirophagus)
and saddle wrasses (Thalassoma duperrey) to remove parasitic copepods from the surface of
their bodies. Mating trains have also been observed in this area.
Surveys were conducted with open-circuit SCUBA, over a six-year period between 2005 and
2010. A combination of photo-identification, paired-laser photogrammetry, and occasional active
tracking were used as primary methods for collecting data. Detailed accounts of the data
collection and analysis procedures used in each study are provided in Chapters 3, 4, and 5.
25
2 USING PAIRED-LASER PHOTOGRAMMETRY AS A SIMPLE
AND ACCURATE SYSTEM TO MEASURE THE BODY SIZE OF
FREE-RANGING MANTA RAYS (MANTA ALFREDI)
2.1 ABSTRACT
Morphometrics are useful for describing and managing animal populations but measurements
can be difficult to obtain, especially on large, free-ranging, aquatic animals. The accuracy and
precision of paired-laser photogrammetry was tested as a simple, accurate, precise, and non-
invasive remote sensing system for measuring the body size of free-ranging, resident manta rays
(Manta alfredi), a newly described species that is poorly understood. Based on repeated
measurements of a pipe of known size, the paired-laser system proved accurate (mean error of
0.39%) and precise (CV = 0.54%). Repeated measurements on 154 different manta rays visiting
a cleaning station off Maui, Hawai‘i, produced a mean CV of 1.46%. Disc length (DL)
measurements were more precise than disc width (DW) measurements and an empirically
derived disc ratio (DR) function was applied to convert DL to DW measurements for standard
comparison with other studies. Sexual dimorphism was present with the largest female (3.64 m
DW) 18% larger than the largest male (3.03 m DW). Sexual maturity in females, based on
evidence of pregnancy and mating scars, was conservatively determined to be 3.37 m DW. The
DW at which 50% of the males were likely to be mature (based on clasper length) was between
2.70 and 2.80 m DW. The absence of individuals smaller than 2.50 m DW suggests age class
segregation may be occurring in this population. Paired-laser photogrammetry proved to be a
simple, non-invasive, accurate, and precise method for sizing free-ranging manta rays. Repeated
measurements on known individuals over time could provide population growth parameters
needed for adequate management of this poorly understood species.
26
2.2 INTRODUCTION
Body size measurements or morphometrics are important components in understanding the life
history of an organism. Morphometrics have been used to study individual growth (e.g., S.
Clark, Odell, & Lacinak, 2000), physical and sexual maturity (e.g., Waters & Whitehead, 1990),
phenotypic differences in closely related species (e.g., Perryman & Lynn, 1993), and size class
segregation in a population (e.g., Cubbage & Calambokidis, 1987). Identifying the existence of
sexual dimorphism in a population through morphometrics can help to understand reproductive
strategies, intrasexual competition, and mate choice (e.g., Breuer, Robbins, & Boesch, 2007).
Obtaining morphometrics on free ranging animals can be challenging, especially if the animals
are large in size, and particularly if they live in aquatic environments. Most methods are
intrusive, disruptive, and usually involve the capturing or killing of the animal, subjecting both
researcher and animal to the risk of injury. The ability to obtain size measurements remotely
eliminates many of these risks.
Photogrammetry is a non-invasive, remote sensing technique that uses photography or digital
imagery to measure objects, or in the case of animals, morphometrics. The technique has been
used successfully to measure the body length of large marine animals such as whales (Best &
Ruther, 1992; Cubbage & Calambokidis, 1987; S. S. Spitz, Herman, & Pack, 2000), dolphins
(Perryman & Lynn, 1993), seals (Bell, Hindell, & Burton, 1997), and sharks (Klimley & Brown,
1983). With photogrammetry, measurements can be collected quickly, with minimal disturbance
to the animal or their associates. However, photogrammetric techniques can be expensive and
cumbersome, requiring an aircraft (e.g., Cosens & Blouw, 2003), a boat with a tall mast (e.g.,
Dawson, Chessum, Hunt, & Slooten, 1995), or multiple cameras operating simultaneously (e.g.,
Klimley & Brown, 1983), limiting their applicability. Paired-laser photogrammetry uses two
parallel laser pointers mounted onto a single camera to project two points of light onto a target,
showing a scale of known size from which the size of the target can be inferred. The technique is
relatively simple, compact, and can be implemented by a single photographer. It has been used
27
successfully to measure morphometrics on large free-ranging animals such as the horn length in
Alpine ibex (Capra ibex) (Bergeron, 2007), and the dorsal fins of killer whales (Orcinus Orca)
(Durban & Parsons, 2006) and bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncates) (L. Rowe & Dawson,
2008). It has also been used underwater, with moderate success, to measure small fish at close
range (Mueller, Brown, Hop, & Moulton, 2006; Yoshihara, 1997). This study investigates the
usefulness of underwater paired-laser photogrammetry for measuring one of the largest fish in
the oceans, the manta ray.
Manta rays are the largest ray in the Mobulidae family and still poorly understood. They feed on
small planktonic organisms such as euphasids and copepods (T. B. Clark, 2001; Homma et al.,
1999; Last & Stevens, 1994; G. Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, 1987) and possibly on small shrimp,
crabs, and fish (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953). They are ovoviviparous and are believed to give
birth to a single live young every 2-3 years (Homma et al., 1999) following a gestation period of
12-14 months (A. D. Marshall et al., 2006). Following parturition, the pup is weaned
immediately with no further parental care (Senzo Uchida et al., 2008). Natural predators include
large sharks (Homma et al., 1999) and killer whales (Orcinus orca) (Visser & Bonoccorso, 2003)
depending on the region.
The genus Manta was thought to consist of just a single species, Manta birostris, but recent
evidence from morphology has confirmed a second species in the genus, Manta alfredi (AD
Marshall et al., 2009). M. birostris, sometimes referred to as “oceanic manta rays,” can grow to a
disc width (DW) (measured from wing tip to wing tip) of 6.70 m (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953),
and possibly as large as 9.10 m (L. Compagno, 1999). These manta rays occur in temperate, sub-
tropical, and tropical waters globally, spending the majority of their time in deep water, paying
occasional visits to coastal areas with productive upwellings, oceanic islands, and offshore
pinnacles and seamounts (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953; L. J. V. Compagno, 1999; AD Marshall
et al., 2009).
28
M. alfredi, referred to in the present study as “resident manta rays”, have been observed in the
Pacific, Atlantic, and Indian ocean between latitudes 30o N and 30 o S (AD Marshall et al., 2009).
Smaller and more tropical than their oceanic relatives, they are more likely to be observed in
shallow coastal areas (G. Notarbartolo-di-Sciara & Hillyer, 1989) with rocky and coral reef
habitats near productive upwellings, as well as around tropical islands, atolls, and bays (AD
Marshall et al., 2009). In some parts of the world they can be reliably seen congregating around
rich food sources and cleaning stations (T. B. Clark, 2001; Dewar et al., 2008; Homma et al.,
1999). Cleaning stations consist of specific locations along the reef where individuals solicit host
cleaner fish that feed on parasites and other unwanted materials on their skin (Losey Jr, 1972).
Worldwide, only a handfull of resident manta rays have been successfully measured for
morphology. In waters off southern Mozambique, the smallest free swimming individuals were
estimated at 1.5 m DW and the largest estimated at 5.5 m DW (AD Marshall et al., 2009). Males
in this region appear to mature around 3.0 m DW while females in coastal waters off South
Africa appear to mature at approximately 3.9 m DW (AD Marshall et al., 2009).
The aim of the present study was to examine the practicality, accuracy, and precision of paired-
laser photogrammetry as a simple, non-invasive, remote sensing system for measuring free-
ranging, resident manta rays from a population off Maui, Hawai‘i. The maximum size of male
and females at physical maturity, and their minimum size at sexual maturity were quantified,
providing useful biological parameters from which to infer new information about the biology
and ecology of this species.
2.3 METHODS
2.3.1 Study Area and Population
All manta ray surveys were conducted opportunistically over a three-year period between 2007
and 2009 at a single manta ray cleaning station off the west coast of the island of Maui, Hawai‘i.
Approximately 450 m offshore, a region 8000 m2 (200 m X 400 m) in size, with a depth range of
29
5-30 m, was chosen as the monitoring boundary for the study area because of the high reliability
of observing manta rays and thereby maximizing encounter rates. The habitat consists primarily
of fringing coral reef that extends away from the shoreline approximately 550 m. The main
cleaning stations were situated near the starting point of the survey where the Hawaiian cleaner
wrasse (Labroides phthirophagus) and saddle wrasse (Thalassoma duperrey) remove small
copepod parasites from soliciting manta rays. Manta ray mating trains were also observed in this
area consisting of a single female pursued by one or more males (Yano et al., 1999). The exact
location of the site is being withheld to avoid the potential commercial exploitation of this
unique area.
2.3.2 Equipment
Two underwater, green laser pointers (Lasermate Professional: output power < 5 mw,
wavelength = 532 nm, 180 mm in length, 25 mm in diameter) were mounted in parallel with
their centers 0.60 m apart onto a 160 X 680 X 5 mm aluminum plate. A 0.60 m separation
provided a reference scale small enough to measure the smallest manta ray in the population but
large enough to minimize error while maintaining portability. The platform was mounted to the
bottom of a Sea & Sea VX-HC1 underwater housing (Figure 2). The housing enclosed a Sony
HDR-HC1, high definition video camera with a lens focal zoom length of 5.1 – 51 mm
(equivalent to 48 – 480 mm on a 35 mm still camera), lens aperture F/1.8-2.1, and 2.76
megapixels effective still resolution. The camera was fitted with a wide-angle lens attachment
(68 mm diameter, 41 mm length, 0.7 magnification). Allen screws, threaded around each laser-
mounting bracket, allowed for fine-scale adjustments of each laser pointer to ensure they were
exactly parallel.
2.3.3 Accuracy and Precision
Potential sources of measurement error include: (1) image distortion caused by light refraction
and the wide-angle lens; (2) non-parallel alignment of the lasers; and (3) parallax error.
30
Figure 2. A pair of green, underwater laser pointers mounted in parallel to an underwater video
housing.
2.3.3.1 Image distortion
Image distortions can occur when light refracts as it passes at an angle from water (refractive
index ~1.00) to air (refractive index 1.33) inside the underwater camera housing. Further
distortion occurs when the light passes through the wide-angle lens. Wide-angle lenses are
designed to severely bend rays of light around the periphery of the field of view (Swaminathan &
Nayar, 1999) with pixels toward the center of the image being the least distorted and pixels
toward the edges of the image being the most distorted. Some of the distortion, in particular
around the edges of the picture, readjusts slightly due to the refraction occurring in air trapped in
the camera housing (between water and lens) before the light reaches the lens. By approximating
the amount of distortion occurring in the image, a correction factor can be applied to compensate
for the distortion.
To quantify the amount of distortion that was occurring, a piece of graph paper was
photographed underwater and the image examined in Adobe Photoshop ®. The image was
composed of 16 columns and 10 rows of squares making up a total of 160 squares. Since the
31
squares at the center of the image had the least distortion, the dimensions of these squares were
used to represent the expected dimensions of a non-distorted square (as if the image had been
photographed with a flat lens). The diagonal length across two of these center squares was
measured (in pixels) using the Adobe Photoshop ® line tool. Additional lengths were taken
diagonally across 4, 6, and 8 squares with the center of the diagonal passing over the center of
the image, essentially expanding the diagonal measurement by 2 square increments. The
expected, undistorted lengths for these dimensions were calculated by multiplying the
undistorted diagonal length of the two center squares by 2, 3, and 4 respectively.
The expected, undistorted lengths were plotted along the y-axis. The actual measured lengths
were plotted on the x-axis. The data were fitted with a regression curve and a Pearson
Correlation Coefficient was calculated to determine its fit. The function of the curve was applied
to all measurements to correct for the distortion caused by the light refraction and wide-angle
lens.
2.3.3.2 Parallel alignment of lasers
Non-parallel alignment of the laser pointers can cause the spacing between the points of light to
change depending on the distance from the target, creating inaccurate measurements. To ensure
the lasers were parallel, a plastic pipe with two clear marks on the center of the pipe spaced 0.60
m apart, was placed on the ocean bottom at a depth of approximately 10 m. With the paired-laser
system in hand, a scuba diver positioned over the center of the pipe adjusted the Allen screws so
that the points of light projected exactly onto the markings (Figure 3A). The laser pointers were
confirmed to be parallel when the spacing between the points of light remained 0.60 m, even as
the diver moved towards and away from the pipe. On 4 occasions (12 Aug 2008, 21 Sep 2008, 9
Oct 2008, and 9 Jan 2009) the laser pointers were removed from the holding brackets,
remounted, and the spacing adjusted to ensure they were parallel.
32
Figure 3. (a) Pipe of known length being measured on the ocean floor showing the projected
points of light 60 cm apart; (b) a photograph of a manta ray (M. alfredi) from above showing the
projected points of light along the spinal axis of the disc from which a DL measurement can be
obtained.
33
2.3.3.3 Parallax error
Parallax error can be a problematic source of error with paired-laser photogrammetry (e.g.,
Durban & Parsons, 2006). This occurs when the laser projections are not perpendicular to the
surface being measured. To investigate how the measurement of an object of known length
varies with the horizontal angle of the target to the axis of the lasers (parallax), measurements of
a 1.94 m pipe were taken with the diver positioned above the end of the pipe rather than over the
center. The distance of the diver above the end of the pipe was calculated by multiplying the
tangent of the desired parallax angle (in this case 80, 70, 60, 50, and 40˚) by half the length of the
pipe (0.97 m) to produce parallax angles of 10, 20, 30, 40, and 50˚ off the perpendicular axis.
The pipe was measured 5 times at each angle. The lens correction function was applied to each
measurement and a mean percent error was calculated for all measurements at each angle.
2.3.3.4 Pipe measurements
Accuracy of a measurement is the degree to which the ‘measured’ matches the ‘actual’. Precision
of a measurement is the degree to which repeated measurements of the same target show the
same results. Both accuracy and precision of the paired-laser system was determined by
measuring a 1.94 m pipe on 4 separate days over a 5 mo period. The diver positioned himself
over the center of the pipe at a distance that would allow the full length of the pipe to be captured
within the camera’s field of view along the horizontal plane. With the pipe perpendicular to the
direction of the laser projections, the 2 points of light were projected onto the center of the pipe
and a photograph was taken using the minimum focal length of 5.1 mm (Figure 3A). After a
measurement was taken, the diver moved a short distance away before repositioning to take a
second independent measurement. After a minimum of 4 independent measurements, the pipe
was moved to a new location and a new series of measurements was taken. Accuracy was
determined by measuring the percent error of the estimated length against the known length of
the pipe. Precision was measured by calculating the percent coefficient of variation (CV) from
repeated measurements of the pipe.
34
2.3.4 Manta Ray Measurements
2.3.4.1 Surveys
A survey consisted of a 55 to 75 minute SCUBA dive (single tank, open-circuit) with a beach
entry to the study site. Each survey began from the exact same location. From this
start point, a rectangular search pattern was initiated, enclosing an area approximately 8000 m2
(200 m X 400 m). The water depth ranged from 6 m to 30 m. When manta rays were
encountered, disc length (DL) and DW measurements were attempted.
DL is defined here as the length from the tip of the snout to the posterior edge of the pectoral fins
(Francis, 2006). To measure the DL, the diver positioned above the manta ray such that its dorsal
plane was perpendicular to the direction of the laser projections, and the DL was captured along
the horizontal axis of the field of view. A photograph was taken with the points of light projected
onto the center of the anteroposterior axis of the disc (Figure 3B). The diver then turned the
camera 180˚ horizontally and repositioned above the manta ray before each repeated
measurement. When repositioning, care was taken to stay out of the manta ray’s field of view so
as not to surprise the animal and cause it to flee. At least 4 independent measurements were
attempted on each manta ray when possible.
The same method applied when measuring the DW, except that the wing tips of the manta ray
were aligned with the horizontal axis of the field of view. A photograph was taken with the
points of light projected onto the center of the mediolateral axis of the disc. It was important that
the photograph be taken when both wings were completely open so as not to underestimate the
DW.
Sizes were taken from above the manta ray to: (1) minimize disturbance to the manta ray since
they do not appear to see directly above their dorsal plane; (2) eliminate any chance of projecting
a point of light into the manta ray’s eye since the eyes are not visible from above; and (3)
35
minimize parallax error since the diver, from above, is better able to align the laser projections
perpendicular to the axis of the manta ray.
Precision of the paired-laser system on free-ranging manta rays was assessed by calculating the
CV from repeated measurements taken on the same animal during the same survey and across
different surveys.
2.3.4.2 Disc ratio
The relationship between DW and DL was examined by dividing the DW by its corresponding
DL to obtain a disc ratio (DR). The mean DR was compared between females and males to
determine if this proportional relationship was the same across sexes, and the same comparison
was made between adult and juvenile males to determine if the proportional relationship was
constant across age classes. The measured DW for each manta ray was plotted against its
corresponding DL and fitted with a linear regression curve. The function of the regression curve
was used to convert measurements of DL to an estimate of DW so that direct comparisons could
be made with other studies.
2.3.4.3 Photo-processing
Each photograph of a manta ray size was examined in Adobe Photoshop ®. If the dorsal plane of
the manta ray in the image did not appear perpendicular to the axis of the laser projections, the
image was discarded to eliminate parallax error. For all other images, the number of pixels
between the 2 points of light and between each end of the target was measured using the line
tool. The length of the target (in pixels) was divided by the distance between the points of light
(in pixels) and multiplied by the known distance between the points of light (0.60 m) to obtain
the length of the target in m.
36
2.3.4.4 Photo-identification
Photo-identification involves taking photographs of distinctive characteristics from an animal in
order to identify and track individuals of a wild population over time. This technique has been
used extensively with large and long-lived vertebrates (for review see Würsig & Jefferson, 1990)
for population estimates (e.g., Graham & Roberts, 2007), life history information (e.g., Brault &
Caswell, 1993), lifespan information (e.g., Langtimm et al., 2004), migration patterns (e.g.,
Calambokidis et al., 2006), and social relationships (e.g., Bejder, Fletcher, & Bräger, 1998) of
recognized individuals. Since each manta ray possesses a distinct pattern of spots on the ventral
surface that are present from birth (Andrea D. Marshall et al., 2008), and the pattern appears to
remain unchanged over time (T. B. Clark, 2001; Homma et al., 1999; Yano et al., 1999), this
species is well suited for photo-identification studies.
During each manta ray encounter, attempts were made to photograph the ventral spot pattern of
each individual sighted. Manta rays frequently make close passes near a diver allowing the diver
to be positioned such that a ventral identification photograph can be taken. When possible, the
genital area was also captured in the photograph for sex identification. Immediately after each
manta ray was photo-identified, a hand signal was also photographed to indicate the sex and age
class of that individual. Photo-identifications were taken prior to moving above the animal for
size measurements.
Photographs were downloaded to a MacBook Pro computer and the best photo-identification for
each individual from a survey was imported into Finbase, a publicly available photo-
identification program created in Microsoft Access (Adams, Speakman, Zolman, & Schwacke,
2006). The photo was matched against photos of all previously identified individuals from the
study site and determined to be a match or a new individual. A detailed catalog was kept of each
individual in the population and its sighting history. The very distinct markings on the underside
of each manta ray make the likelihood of missing a match, or falsely identifying a match,
unlikely.
37
2.3.4.5 Sex and age class
Since claspers are present and visible in males from birth (Andrea D. Marshall et al., 2008), the
sex of the manta ray was determined by the presence or absence of claspers. Females were
documented as sexually mature if they were obviously pregnant, or showed visible mating scars
(spot scarring and abrasions usually visible on the dorsal side of the end of the left wing; AD
Marshall & Bennett, 2010). A pregnant female close to term was exceptionally rotund in girth
and could be identified quite easily. A female that appeared to be pregnant but was questionable
was not given an age class. A female being pursued by multiple males in a mating train was
documented as a nuclear female.
Among males, calcification of the claspers occurs rapidly over a relatively narrow range of
growth (W. T. White et al., 2006), with the majority of calcification occurring once the claspers
have extended beyond the length of the pelvic fins (A. Marshall, pers. comm.). Since the onset
of clasper calcification in many shark species coincides with a rapid rate of clasper growth and
gonadal maturation (e.g., Jones, Hall, & Potter, 2008), claspers extending beyond the pelvic fins
were used as a reliable indicator of sexual maturity in male manta rays.
2.3.5 Statistics
Pearson Correlation Coefficients were calculated for both linear regressions describing the
relationship between undistorted and actual measured lengths, and the relationship between the
DW and DL of individual manta rays. The precision of a single measurement was tested using a
Wilcoxon signed-rank test for matched pairs by comparing the first measurement of a manta ray
with the mean of repeated measurements on the same manta ray. The variability of repeated DL
and DW measurements was compared using a Mann-Whitney U-Test. This test was also used to
compare the mean DR between females and males, and between adult and juvenile males.
Significance for all statistical tests was set at p < 0.05. Confidence intervals are reported at 95%.
38
2.4 RESULTS
2.4.1 Accuracy and Precision
The diagonal distance across the 2 center squares in the underwater photograph of the graph
paper was 245 pixels. Therefore, expected diagonal distance across 4, 6, and 8 of the center
squares were estimated as 2 X 245 = 490, 3 X 245 = 735, and 4 X 245 = 980 pixels respectively.
Actual measured diagonal values were 491, 741, and 992 pixels respectively. The expected,
undistorted lengths were plotted against the actual, measured lengths (Figure 4). The linear
regression of best fit produced a Pearson correlation coefficient of 1.0 (df = 3, p < 0.001).
Five pipe measurements were made from angles of 10, 20, 30, 40, and 50˚ away from the
perpendicular axis to the center of the pipe. This resulted in mean errors of -4.92, -6.13, -8.79, -
22.25, and -39.29% respectively. A pipe 1.94 m in length was measured on 4 separate occasions
over a 6 mo period for a total of 92 independent measurements. Without a wide-angle lens
correction function applied, the overall mean estimated pipe length was 1.97 m (95% CI = ±
0.02, CV = 0.61%). With the lens correction applied the estimated mean length was 1.94 m (95%
CI = ± 0.02, CV = 0.54%). The lens correction function reduced the mean error from 1.39% to
0.39%, and reduced the maximum error from 2.76% to 1.43%.
2.4.2 Manta Ray Measurements
2.4.2.1 Surveys
A total of 87 surveys were conducted during which the DL of 274 manta rays was measured. The
DW of 82 of these manta rays was also measured. Photo-identification matching revealed 154 of
these manta rays were distinct individuals.
The variance of repeated DW measurements on the same individual (mean CV = 3.05%) was
significantly greater than the variance of repeated DL measurements on the same individual
39
(mean CV = 1.46%) (Mann-Whitney U-Test: Z(0.05) = -2.692, n = 264, 51, p = 0.007) indicating
that DL is a more precise measurement than DW.
Figure 4. A plot of the expected, undistorted length of an object (in pixels) if measured with a
flat lens against the actual measured length of the same object (in pixels) distorted by the wide-
angle lens. The data are fitted with a linear regression equation.
2.4.2.2 Disc ratio
The mean DR for all 82 individuals measured was 2.33 (95% CI = ± 0.02). No significant
differences were found between the DR of females and males, or between adult and juvenile
males (Table 3). For each individual manta ray, the measured DW was plotted against its
corresponding DL (Figure 5). The linear regression of best fit produced a Pearson Correlation
40
Coefficient of 0.923 (df = 63, p < 0.001). The relationship between DW and DL was best
described by the following linear regression:
DW = 1.958DL + 0.469 (r2 = 0.923)
Results of a Wilcoxon signed rank test found no differences between the first measured DL of an
individual manta ray and the mean of repeated independent DL measurements of the same
individual (Z = -0.632, n = 274, p = 0.527).
Table 3. Comparison of the mean disc ratio (DR) between male and female, and between adult
male and juvenile male manta rays (M. alfredi).
Mean Disc
Ratio (m)
n
% CV
Males
2.34a
37
3.13
Females
2.33a
27
3.50
Adult Males
2.33b
23
3.24
Juvenile Males
2.35b
12
3.46
All Individuals
2.33
64
3.27
Mann-Whitney U-Test
a Z = -0.768, df = 63, p = 0.442
b Z = -0.452, df = 34, p = 0.668
2.4.2.3 Sex and age class
Of the 154 individual manta rays measured in this population, 71 (46%) were females, and 83
(54%) were males (Figure 6). Females were on average significantly larger than males (Mann-
Whitney U-Test: Z = -0.0867, n = 71, 83, p < 0.001). The largest female (3.64 m DW) was 18%
larger than the largest male (3.00 m DW). The smallest female (2.50 m DW) was only slightly
smaller than the smallest male (2.51 m DW).
41
The smallest pregnant female at 3.37 m DW (n = 16) was also the smallest female with visible
mating scars (n = 19). Using the size of this female as a conservative minimum size for sexual
maturity in females, at least 48% of the females measured in this population were likely to be of
mature size. The smallest female observed in a mating train was 3.24 m DW (n = 12). One third
of all nuclear females were never observed pregnant or to have mating scars.
Figure 5. Sixty-four manta ray DW measurements plotted against its corresponding DL
measurement and fitted with a linear regression. Measurements are in meters.
42
Figure 6. The distribution of manta ray (M. alfredi) disc widths by gender and age class. Females
were considered adults if obviously pregnant or showed visible mating scars. Not all nuclear
females were pregnant or had mating scars. Males with clasper lengths extending beyond the
pelvic fins were classified as adult, even with the pelvic fins were classified as transition, and
shorter than the pelvic fins were classified as juvenile. White boxes represent female categories,
gray boxes represent male categories, numbers represent sample sizes, and circles represent
outliers. All measurements are in millimeters.
43
The smallest adult male was estimated at 2.60 m DW (n = 57) and the largest juvenile male was
estimated at 2.77 m DW (n = 24). Transition males, whose claspers were exactly even with the
edge of the pelvic fins, were rare. Only two were measured, each with a DW of 2.76 m and 2.80
m. When DW among males was separated into 0.1 m incremental categories, the DW category at
which approximately 50% of the males were considered mature (DW50) was 2.7 to 2.8 m (Figure
7).
Figure 7. The proportion of males which are sexually mature for each disc width size category.
The black column indicates the size category for which nearly 50% of the males can be
considered mature (DW50). Numbers above the column indicate the sample size.
44
2.5 DISCUSSION
2.5.1 Accuracy and Precision
When measuring a target of known size, the paired-laser photogrammetry system was accurate to
a mean error of 0.39% (0 – 1.43%) and precise to a mean CV of 0.54% (0.02 – 4.01%). This is
comparable or better than reports for other photogrammetric systems with accuracy ranging from
0.47 - 6.6% (Bergeron, 2007; Cosens & Blouw, 2003; Cubbage & Calambokidis, 1987;
Perryman & Lynn, 1993; S. S. Spitz et al., 2000) and precision (CVs) ranging from 0.84 to
9.03% (Best & Ruther, 1992; Cosens & Blouw, 2003; Cubbage & Calambokidis, 1987; Dawson
et al., 1995; J. Gordon, 1990; Klimley & Brown, 1983; Perryman & Lynn, 1993; S. S. Spitz et
al., 2000).
Potential sources of error with the paired-laser system were easily controlled for by (1) using
study mount to ensure the laser pointers remained parallel, (2) discarding images showing
evidence of parallax, and (3) applying a simple, empirically determined correction function to
control for image distortion caused by light refraction and the wide-angle lens.
For measuring manta ray sizes, the paired-laser photogrammetric system proved to be simple to
use. A single diver was able to take multiple measurements with little or no change to the manta
ray’s behavior. Occasional reactions by a manta ray during measurements usually occurred when
the manta ray performed an abrupt change in direction (e.g., when being bitten by a cleaner
wrasse) thus bringing the diver into view and causing the manta ray to move rapidly away.
Although multiple measurements should be taken for insurance, in some situations, such as when
multiple animals in a mating train pass through the area rapidly, time may only allow for a single
measurement to be taken per individual. The first measurement proved to be just as precise as the
mean of repeated measurements on a manta ray.
45
Measurements of DW were less precise than measurements of DL. This was most likely due to
the difficulty in photographing the manta ray with its wings completely open. Even with dead
specimens where the fin tips have become curled or the texture has become loose, DW
measurements can be unreliable (Andrea D. Marshall et al., 2008; G. Notarbartolo-di-Sciara,
1987), and caution should be taken when using this metric. DL proved to be a more accurate
metric for measuring the body size of free-ranging manta rays.
2.5.2 Manta Measurements
For direct comparisons with other studies, DL can be converted to a more conventional DW
estimate by applying a DR function. The relationship between DW and DL was constant
regardless of sex or age class. Morphometric proportions, including the DW and DL of a
measured male manta ray fetus from southern Mozambique were the same as those measured for
three juvenile manta rays from South Africa (Andrea D. Marshall et al., 2008), adding further
support for isometric growth of this species.
The mean DR was 2.33 (95% CI = ± 0.02), similar to those reported for specimens in South
Africa ranging between 2.21 and 2.37, and a fetus from southern Mozambique with a DR of 2.43
(Andrea D. Marshall et al., 2008). DRs for oceanic manta rays include 2.2 reported for an
individual from the eastern North Atlantic (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953) and a range of 2.16 –
2.29 from 4 specimens examined in Indonesia (Andrea D. Marshall et al., 2008).
The largest measured manta ray was a female estimated at 3.64 m DW, substantially smaller than
the 5.50 m DW maximum estimate observed in southern Mozambique (AD Marshall et al.,
2009) and the 4.30 m DW maximum estimate observed in Japan (Kashiwagi et al., 2008).
Geographic variability in size is common for oceanic manta rays ranging in size from 4.94 m
DW in Indonesia (W. T. White et al., 2006) to a 6.45 m DW from the eastern North Atlantic
(Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953). Additional size measurements of geographically independent
populations of M. alfredi should be investigated for comparison.
46
The largest measured female was 18% larger then the largest measured male. This supports the
existence of sexual dimorphism in this resident population. In comparison, the largest female
reported by Kashiwagi et al. (2008) from a resident population of manta rays off the coast of
Japan had a DW of 4.30 m, while their largest male had a DW of 3.60 m, a 19% difference in
size. It should be noted that measurements in this study were taken by extending a piece of rope
between two divers positioned above the manta ray as it swam (Kashiwagi, pers. comm.).
Sexual dimorphism can occur when natural selection for high female fecundity in a species is
stronger than sexual selection for males (Wiklund & Karlsson, 1988). In most vertebrates,
natural selection for larger males is well understood, with larger males having an advantage in
male-male competition for mating access to females (Thornhill & Alcock, 1983). Natural
selection can also favor larger females, with larger females having greater fecundity (Fairbairn,
1997). Female manta rays give birth to a single, large, well-developed pup every 2 to 3 yr
(Homma et al., 1999). Since the pup receives no parental care immediately after parturition,
larger pups should have greater survivorship (e.g., McMahon, Burton, & Bester, 2000). This
immediate independence favors large pups and larger mothers are more able to produce larger
offspring (e.g., Pack et al., 2009).
Newborn manta rays have been reported with a DW of 1.1 m–1.5 m (Homma et al., 1999; AD
Marshall et al., 2009). The absence of manta rays < 2.5 m DW from the study area suggest that
young manta rays may be geographically segregating and may not visit the study area until later
in their development. Segregation by body size has been noted for other mobulid species:
Mobula thurstoni, Mobula japanica, Mobula munkiana, and Mobula tarapacana (G.
Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, 1988).
In several species of sharks, females are known to move into specific nursery areas to give birth
(e.g., Simpfendorfer & Milward, 1993). The pups remain in the protected area for a length of
time before dispersing, presumably for protection against predation. Similarly, female manta rays
may retreat to more protected habitats to give birth, where the pup will reside locally until it
47
reaches a certain age or size. Since no female manta ray has been observed giving birth in the
wild, it is not yet known where they go to have their young.
Small manta rays have been observed and photographed in shallow waters along Maui’s
southeastern shores (B. Blinski, pers. comm.), but not one has been systematically measured.
This southeast Maui area is approximately 20 km from the study site and may constitute an area
where females give birth and young animals reside until they are more mature and begin to
expand their range. Future efforts should focus on obtaining body size measurements from manta
rays frequenting this area.
Using pregnancy and mating scars as an indicator of sexual maturity in females, a DW of 3.37 m
constitutes a conservative estimate of the size at sexual maturity achieved by females in this
Maui population. Although females measuring 3.24 m DW were observed as nuclear females in
mating trains, the lack of observed mating scars and pregnancy suggest immature females may
also be pursued by males in mating trains. Otherwise, 3.24 m DW may represent a lower limit on
sexual maturity in females. Males appear to reach sexual maturity between 2.75 and 2.80 m DW,
at the time when their claspers grow rapidly and begin to extend beyond their pelvic fins.
2.5.3 Future Research
Paired-laser photogrammetry is a practical tool for collecting and comparing morphometric data
on resident manta rays throughout their range. By visiting areas where manta rays are known to
aggregate, it is relatively easy to obtain length measurements from a large part of the population
in a relatively short period of time. The ability to relate individual identities with morphometrics
can be applied to longitudinal studies looking at growth rates, and allows for the incorporation of
life history information about those individuals. By measuring the body size of captive and free-
ranging animals of known ages, future applications include identifying age specific survival
rates, age at first pregnancy, and other important variables for modeling population growth.
Morphometrics on free-ranging manta rays can also help to identify stock depletion, evident
from fewer older and larger animals in the population (Cubbage & Calambokidis, 1987),
48
primarily in regions where they are overfished (A. D. Marshall et al., 2006; W. T. White et al.,
2006). This is particularly important with large, slower-growing species such as manta rays that
are at greater risk of population decline from exploitation (Frisk et al., 2001).
2.5.4 Summary
The equipment needed to carry out paired-laser photogrammetry is simple, allowing a single
diver to collect a large number of manta ray sizes quickly, with high accuracy and precision.
Information about the individual’s identity, sex, and age class can be obtained simultaneously.
These types of information from known-aged animals can be applied to population growth
models and used for population management. By adjusting the distance between the lasers, the
projected points of light can be customized for measurements of other species. Limitations to the
use of this system include the ability to fit the target being measured within the field of view of
the camera, the distance from the target at which the light points are still visible (largely
dependent on the clarity of the water), and the ability to get into position such that the target is
perpendicular to the axis of the laser projections.
49
3 CHARACTERISTICS OF A MANTA RAY (MANTA ALFREDI)
POPULATION OFF MAUI, HAWAII, AND IMPLICATIONS FOR
MANAGEMENT
3.1 ABSTRACT
Late maturity, few offspring, and a residential nature, typical of Manta alfredi, make this species
particularly vulnerable to localized anthropogenic threats. A total of 229 surveys were conducted
between 2005 and 2009 at a manta ray aggregation site off Maui, Hawai‘i, to describe this
population’s abundance, characteristics, and temporal use of the area. Photo-identifications
revealed 290 different individuals. A discovery curve showed no asymptotic trend, indicating the
number of individuals using the area was much larger than the total identified. Resights and
manta follows revealed that manta rays used Maui County waters but did not appear to mix with
a neighboring island population off the Big Island suggesting the possibility of independent
island associated stocks. High resight rates within and across years provided strong evidence of
site-fidelity. Findings were consistent with a population of manta rays moving into and out of the
Maui aggregation area, with a varying portion of the total population temporarily resident at any
given time. Males accounted for 53% of all individuals. Manta rays were usually absent at first
light with numbers increasing throughout the day. More frequent mating trains were observed
during the winter months. Mating appears to occur primarily during the winter. Shark predation
was evident in 24% of individuals, and 10% had an amputated or non-functional cephalic fin.
This small, demographically independent population appears vulnerable to the impacts from non-
target fisheries, primarily from entanglement in fishing line, and could suffer from exploitation
by “unregulated swim-with manta ray” programs. Management on an island-area basis is
recommended.
50
3.2 INTRODUCTION
Understanding the basic biology and ecology of a species is necessary for the proper
conservation and management of that species. For elasmobranchs, which are slow-growing, slow
to mature, and have low fecundity (Holden, 1974), understanding how populations are affected
by anthropogenic impacts comes with added urgency as they are less likely to recover from
population depletion (Hoenig & Gruber, 1990; Pratt & Casey, 1990). Furthermore, populations
that are isolated geographically are subject to regional ecological pressures and may require a
management strategy that is tailored to that specific population. Batoids (rays) are among the
most susceptible marine taxa to fisheries exploitation (Dulvy et al., 2008; Dulvy & Reynolds,
2002) since their large body size is associated with later maturation, thereby putting them at
greater risk of overexploitation, extirpation, and in some cases extinction. Manta rays (Manta
sp), the largest of the batoids are especially vulnerable.
The status of most manta ray populations worldwide is poorly understood. They are classified by
the IUCN Red List for Threatened Animals as “near-threatened” (A. D. Marshall et al., 2006).
Fisheries targeting manta rays in many parts of the world (L. Compagno, 1999; Dewar, 2002; A.
D. Marshall et al., 2006; G. Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, 1987) are fueled by an increasing demand for
branchial filter plates and cartilage. The branchial filter plates are used in traditional Chinese
medicines, and the cartilage for filler in shark-fin soup (Alava et al., 2002; W. T. White et al.,
2006). These directed fisheries have caused significant population declines in areas such as
Mexico (Homma et al., 1999), the Philippines (Alava et al., 2002), Indonesia (Dewar, 2002; W.
T. White et al., 2006), India, Sri Lanka, and other parts of Southeast Asia (A. D. Marshall et al.,
2006).
Manta rays are ovoviviparous, giving birth to a single pup every 2-3 years (Homma et al., 1999;
AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010). The only manta ray birth ever witnessed was captured on video
at the Okinawa Churaumi Aquarium in Japan (Senzo Uchida et al., 2008). The mother gave birth
to a single pup following a twelve month gestation period. Parturition was immediate and the
51
mother was observed mating within a few hours after giving birth. No information exists on the
development and growth of free-ranging manta ray pups.
Mating behavior in manta rays has been described as a mating train, where multiple males pursue
and attempt to mate with a single female (Yano et al., 1999). Although these mating trains can be
observed at all times of the year, seasonal peaks have been reported for the summer months (July
– August) in Ogasawara, Japan (Yano et al., 1999), and the austral summer (October –
November) in Mozambique (AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010).
The number of species within the Manta genus has long been debated among scientists (Beebe &
Tee-Van, 1941; Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953; T. B. Clark, 2001; L. J. V. Compagno, 1984;
Fowler, 1941; Last & Stevens, 1994; J. Nelson, 1984; Nishida, 1990; Whitley, 1936) but recent
evidence supports at least two species: Manta birostris and Manta alfredi (AD Marshall et al.,
2009). M. birostris are the larger of the two species, found in tropical, sub-tropical and temperate
waters. Although occasionally seen visiting shallow, coastal areas, they spend the majority of
their time in pelagic waters, migrating over thousands of kilometers (Marshall, pers. comm.,
2009). Their disc width (DW: measured from wing tip to wing tip) can span 6.7 m (Bigelow &
Schroeder, 1953) with one specimen reportedly as large as 9.1 m (Last & Stevens, 1994). Manta
alfredi are more likely to be observed in shallow coastal areas around rocky and coral reef
habitats where productive upwellings exist. They can be found in tropical and subtropical regions
of the Pacific, Atlantic, and Indian Oceans within 30 degrees of latitude to the north and south of
the Equator (AD Marshall et al., 2009). Congregations can occur around rich food sources or at
specific locations on the reef known as cleaning stations (Losey Jr, 1972) where individuals
solicit host cleaner fish to remove parasitic copepods from their body’s surface. Strong site
fidelity occurring at specific feeding and cleaning stations (e.g., Homma et al., 1999), has created
popular tourist attractions where visitors pay to swim or scuba dive with the manta rays (T. B.
Clark, 2001; Dewar et al., 2008). M. alfredi are much smaller than their oceanic cousins with
females reaching a maximum DW between 3.6 m (Deakos, 2010) and 5.5 m (AD Marshall et al.,
2009) depending on the region. The maximum lifespan is unknown but the longest reported time
52
period between first and last sightings of a M. alfredi is 27 years (1980-2006) off Yaeyama
Island, Japan (Kashiwagi et al., 2008).
For management purposes, differentiating between M. birostris and M. alfredi is extremely
important since each could be exposed to a very different set of anthropogenic impacts. While M.
birostris may be targeted by large-scale directed fisheries, or succumb to bycatch in longline and
tuna purse seine operations (Paulin, Habib, Carey, Swanson, & Voss, 1982; Romanov, 2002), M.
aflredi populations may be more vulnerable to nearshore anthropogenic impacts such as coastal
development, storm water runoff, pollutant loadings, boat strikes, entanglement in fishing and
mooring lines, and increased pressure from “swim-with manta” programs. A basic understanding
of the abundance, home range, and use of popular aggregation areas by M. alfredi is needed for
effective management.
The aim of this study was to use photo-identification and active tracking to describe for the first
time, the abundance, minimum geographic range, and population structure of M. alredi
frequenting a known aggregation area in waters off West Maui, Hawai‘i. Temporal patterns and
reproductive use of the area were investigated. Both natural and anthropogenic threats were
quantified and their implications for management of this population discussed.
3.3 METHODS
3.3.1 Main study area
All surveys were conducted at a single manta aggregation area off the west coast of the island of
Maui, Hawai‘i. The exact location of the site is being withheld to avoid the potential commercial
exploitation of this unique site. An area approximately 30,000 m2 (200 m X 150 m) in size, 450
m offshore, with a depth range of 5 - 30 m was the monitoring boundary for the study site
(Figure 8). This area was chosen because of the high reliability of encountering manta rays and
thereby maximizing encounter rates. Habitat consisted primarily of fringing coral reef that
extended away from the shoreline for approximately 550 m. The main cleaning stations were
53
situated near the starting point of the survey where manta rays are seen soliciting predominantly
Hawaiian cleaner wrasses (Labroides phthirophagus) and saddle wrasses (Thalassoma duperrey)
to remove parasitic copepods from the surface of their bodies. Mating trains were also observed
in this area.
3.3.2 Surveys
Surveys, carried out with open-circuit SCUBA, were conducted opportunistically over a five-
year period between 2005 and 2009. Surveys were done at different times of the day and
attempts were made to conduct at least one survey during each month of each year. Due to an
apparent diurnal trend on manta ray sighting rates, 10 days in which a pair of surveys were
conducted in a single day were compared using a Wilcoxon Signed-Rank Test to determine if
sighting rates later in the day were significantly different than sighting rates earlier in the day.
The majority of surveys were conducted during late afternoon since more manta rays were likely
to be encountered during that time.
A survey involved a 55 to 75 minute SCUBA dive. Divers entered the water from the beach and
transited at the surface 450 m to the survey start point before descending (Figure 8). A dive flag
was attached to the ocean floor in ten meters of water. Midway down the tether, a fluorescent
green target (30 cm x 20 cm) was attached and used as a visual cue to determine visibility. The
distance at which point the target was no longer visible was recorded from the north and south of
the flag and the mean was used as the visibility rating for that survey. A rectangular search
pattern was initiated from the start point (see Figure 8). When manta rays were encountered, the
search was interrupted in order to collect information on that individual. Once the desired
information was collected, the search pattern was resumed.
54
Figure 8. Map showing the study area 450 m from the shoreline. The start point of each survey
and the clockwise survey route are shown.
55
3.3.3 Photo-identification
Photo-identification involves taking photographs of distinctive characteristics of an animal to
identify and track individuals of a wild population over time. This technique has been used
extensively with large and long-lived vertebrates (for review see Würsig & Jefferson, 1990), for
population estimates (e.g.: Graham & Roberts, 2007), life history information (e.g.: Brault &
Caswell, 1993), lifespan information (e.g.: Langtimm et al., 2004), migration patterns (e.g.:
Calambokidis et al., 1996), and social relationships (e.g.: Bejder et al., 1998) of an individual.
Each manta ray is born with a unique pattern of spots on its ventral side (Andrea D. Marshall et
al., 2008), which appears to remain unchanged for the duration of the animal’s life (T. B. Clark,
2001; AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010; Yano et al., 1999), even after 20 years (Homma et al.,
1999). This makes manta rays highly suitable for photo-identification studies.
During each manta ray encounter, a diver equipped with either a Canon Powershot S70 in an
underwater housing, or a Sony HDR-HC1 video camera in a Sea & Sea VX-HC1 underwater
housing, attempted to photograph the ventral pattern of each individual. Images were
downloaded to a MacBook Pro computer and the best identification for each individual was
imported into Finbase, a publicly available photo-identification program created in Microsoft
Access (Adams et al., 2006). The photo was matched against photos of all previously identified
individuals from the study site and recorded as either a match or as a new individual. The very
distinct markings on the underside of each manta ray make the likelihood of missing a match, or
falsely identifying a match very unlikely.
56
3.3.4 Abundance and Survivorship
3.3.4.1 Discovery Curve
To illustrate the rate at which new individuals were encountered, a discovery curve showing the
cumulative number of individual manta rays identified was plotted against the cumulative
number of identifications made. Winter and summer season identifications were differentiated on
the curve to visually demonstrate if new individuals were entering the population more often
during a particular season.
3.3.5 Population Range
3.3.5.1 Active Tracking
Two manta rays were tagged on separate occasions with Vemco V16 continuous acoustic
pingers. Each pinger was programmed to emit a unique pulse frequency (52 and 56 kHz
respectively). The signal was received through a VH110 directional hydrophone (frequency
range 50 – 84 kHz) and decoded by a Vemco VR100 receiver/decoder that was kept onboard a
28 ft Glass Pro vessel. A crew of 3 rotated every 4 hours tracking the manta ray in real time from
the vessel. Tracking was continuous throughout the day and night until weather conditions made
it unsafe to continue. The acoustic detection range of the pingers was approximately 1 km. A
continuous track of the boat was recorded onto a Garmin GPSMAP 276C. The acoustic tags
were attached to the dorsal side of the right pectoral fin by a snorkeler swimming above the
manta ray. The tags were deployed using a modified Hawaiian sling and anchored to the manta
ray by embedding a small stainless steel barb under the skin. The barb was tethered to the
acoustic tag with 15 cm of stainless steel wire and crimps.
3.3.5.2 Regional comparisons
Photo-identifications from our study area were compared to opportunistic photo-identifications
taken of manta rays off the southwestern coast of Maui (n=18), Molokini Crater (n=11), and the
57
southeastern coast of Molokai (n=11). Comparisons were also made to a catalog of 146
individual manta rays from a well-monitored population off Kona on the Island of Hawai‘i (Big
Island) (www.mantapacific.org), to look for potential movements between Maui and the Big
Island.
3.3.6 Population Structure
Gender was determined by the presence of claspers in males and their absence in females.
Females were only classified as sexually mature if they had visible mating scars (spot scarring
and abrasions usually on the dorsal or ventral side of the left wing tip) or were obviously
pregnant (AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010). A pregnant female close to term was exceptionally
rotund and unmistakable. A female that appeared to be pregnant but was questionable was not
given an age class.
Among males, calcification of the claspers occurs rapidly over a relatively narrow range of
growth (W. T. White et al., 2006) and the majority of calcification occurs once the claspers have
extended beyond the length of the pelvic fins (AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010). Since the onset of
clasper calcification in many shark species coincides with a rapid rate of clasper growth and
gonadal maturation (e.g., Jones et al., 2008) claspers extended beyond the pelvic fins were used
as a reliable indicator of sexual maturity in male manta rays. Since juvenile females could not be
determined, comparisons between adults and juveniles were done only with males.
3.3.7 Use of the Aggregation Area
3.3.7.1 Temporal Trends
Sighting rates were computed as the total number of manta rays photo-identified divided by the
number of hours surveyed and were compared by time of day, by month, by season, and by year.
The start time of each survey was categorized as “AM” (6:00 – 10:00), “MIDDAY” (10:00 –
14:00) and “PM” (14:00 – 18:00) surveys. Surveys from November through April were
58
categorized as “winter season” surveys, and May through October were categorized as “summer
season” surveys. The effect of diver visibility and tidal state on sighting rate was also examined.
Linear regression was used to determine the correlation between sighting rates and diver
visibility. A Kruskal-Wallis Test was used to assess the significance of year, season, month, and
tidal states in explaining variance of manta ray sightings. Tide tables were used to determine the
tidal state, which was categorized as “incoming” (flood tide), “incoming/outgoing” (high tide),
“outgoing” (ebb tide), or “outgoing/incoming” (low tide) for each survey.
3.3.7.2 Reproduction and New Individuals
The presence or absence of mating trains and pregnant females were recorded for each survey as
well as the proportion of males to females. Chi-square statistics were used to compare the
proportion of mating trains between winter and summer seasons. The mean number of new
individual sighting rates (total number of newly identified individuals divided by the total
amount of time surveyed) was computed for each survey, by month, by season, and by year. A
Kruskal-Wallis Test was used to test the significant difference in the rate of new individuals
occurring by month and by season.
3.3.8 Threats
Physical characteristics of an individual were also recorded and included: a missing or damaged
cephalic fin, and the presence of a large wound, large scar, or large section of the body missing
(i.e. disc or tail) indicative of having been attacked by a large predator. Chi-square statistics were
used to compare the proportions of natural and anthropogenic injuries between gender and age
class. The probability level at which significance was determined was 0.05. Statistical analyses
were performed using SPSS version 17.0 (SPSS Inc., 2007)
59
3.4 RESULTS
3.4.1 Surveys
A total of 229 surveys were conducted between 2005 and 2009 (Table 4). Surveys carried out
later in the day were more likely to have a higher sighting rate (Wilcoxon Signed-Rank: Z = -
2.912, n = 20, p = 0.004). Due to this diurnal trend, the majority of dives were conducted in the
PM (82%) to increase encounter rates. The remaining surveys were conducted in the AM (8%)
and at MIDDAY (10%). Fifty-seven percent of surveys were conducted in the summer months
and 43% during winter months. A total of 1494 manta rays were encountered and photo-
identified, revealing 290 different individuals. Manta rays were observed on 201 (88%) surveys.
The number of manta rays encountered during each survey ranged from 0 to 31.
3.4.2 Abundance and Survivorship
3.4.2.1 Discovery Curve
The discovery curve (Figure 9) illustrates a decreasing trend of new individuals entering the
population with increasing identifications. The curve has a steep slope during early surveys and
begins to decrease with additional surveys but never reaches asymptote.
60
Table 4. The number of surveys conducted and mean sighting rates for years 2005 through 2009,
broken down into time of day (600-1000, 1000-1400, 1400-1600) and season (Nov-Apr, May-
Oct). Sighting rates are calculated as the mean number of manta rays observed per hour of
survey effort.
No. of Surveys
Year
AM
MIDDAY
PM
Winter
Summer
Total
2005
0
0
33
10
23
33
2006
1
1
22
6
18
24
2007
16
14
29
22
37
59
2008
1
8
86
42
53
95
2009
0
1
17
18
0
18
Overall
18
24
187
98
131
229
Sighting Rate
1.40
4.17
6.90
8.14
4.77
6.21
61
Figure 9. Discovery curve illustrating the cumulative number of new manta ray identifications
against the cumulative number of all identifications. Dark circles represent surveys conducted in
the winter (November – April) and light circles represent surveys conducted in the summer (May
- October).
62
3.4.3 Population Range
3.4.3.1 Active tracking
On separate occasions in December of 2008, an adult male and an adult female manta ray were
tagged with an acoustic transmitter in the study area. Both animals were in a mating train at the
time of tagging. The adult male was tracked for 28 hours and traveled across
the Auau channel to the north coast of the island of Lanai, a linear distance of 40 km from the
study area where he was tagged (Figure 10). The maximum depth traversed was 93 m. The adult
female was tracked for 51 hours and traveled to the northwest side of the island of Kahoolawe, a
linear distance of 32 km from the study area where she was tagged (Figure 10). The maximum
depth traversed was 324 m.!
3.4.3.2 Regional comparisons
Of the 290 individuals identified from the study area, 2 matches were made to south Molokai
(based on 11 photo-ids), 3 matches to Molokini Crater (based on 11 photo-ids), and one match to
a southwest Maui sighting (based on 18 photo-ids; Figure 10). No matches were found between
the 290 individual manta rays from the Maui study area with the 146 individuals photo-identified
in waters off Kona, Big Island, a transit distance of approximately 150 km from the study area.
3.4.4 Population Structure
The 290 photo-identified individuals were composed of 128 (44%) females, and 153 (53%)
males. Nine of these individuals were of unknown sex. At least 44% of the females were
considered to be sexually mature based on the appearance of being pregnant or with mating
scars. Among the males, 72% were considered sexually mature based on claspers extending
beyond the pelvic fins, 26% were recorded as immature, and 2% were never confirmed.
63
Figure 10. Map showing the range of individual manta rays (M. alfredi) either matched with
photo-identifications (solid arrows) or tracked with an acoustic tag (dashed arrows).
64
3.4.5 Use of the Aggregation Area
3.4.5.1 Temporal Trends
It was rare to see manta rays during early morning surveys. The sighting rate during AM surveys
was 1.40 manta rays per hour (SD = 3.24), 4.17 (SD = 4.25) for MIDDAY surveys, and 6.94 (SD
= 5.16) for PM surveys (Table 4). The number of surveys conducted and mean sighting rates for
years 2005 through 2009, broken down into time of day (600-1000, 1000-1400, 1400-1600) and
season (Nov-Apr, May-Oct). Sighting rates are calculated as the mean number of manta rays
observed per hour of survey effort. To eliminate the diurnal effect on sighting rates described
previously, analyses of sighting rates incorporated only the 187 PM surveys.
3.4.5.2 Other variables affecting sighting rate
The survey month was a significant predictor of the mean sighting rate (Table 5; Kruskal-Wallis
Test: χ2 = 26.14, df = 11, p = 0.006), with significantly greater sighting rates during the winter
months (Kruskal-Wallis Test: χ2 = 19.35, df = 1, p < 0.001). For years 2006, 2007, and 2008,
during which surveys were conducted in both summer and winter months, mean sighting rates
did not differ significantly across years (Kruskal-Wallis Test: χ2 = 0.91, df = 2, p = 0.634).
Sighting rates were not significantly affected by visibility (r2 = 0.031, p = 0.075) or tidal state
(Kruskal Wallis Test: χ2 = 5.616, df = 3, p = 0.132).
65
Table 5. Surveys, mean sighting rates, mean rate of new individuals, proportion of males, and proportion of mating trains with
standard deviations (SD) listed by month and season.
Month
No.
Surveys
Mean
Sighting Rate
SD
% with
Mating Trains
% Males
SD
Mean Rate of
New Individuals
SD
NOV
14
7.73
5.24
0
61
0.16
0.25
0.18
DEC
10
10.31
6.47
10
57
0.07
0.14
0.13
JAN
13
11.13
7.29
31
64
0.23
1.40
1.49
FEB
14
7.38
4.79
43
54
0.24
2.02
2.52
MAR
10
9.37
7.80
20
67
0.23
1.40
1.96
Winter
APR
22
7.90
4.28
32
58
0.23
1.85
1.79
Overall
83
8.76a
5.80
24b
60c
0.21
1.79d
1.84
MAY
15
4.45
3.08
7
63
0.26
0.45
0.87
JUN
16
6.67
4.36
13
49
0.30
0.66
0.97
JUL
24
5.26
4.14
17
48
0.29
1.35
1.55
AUG
17
3.40
2.88
0
54
0.28
0.60
0.85
SEP
19
6.78
3.95
11
50
0.29
1.68
1.79
Summer
OCT
13
6.49
5.04
8
56
0.25
0.67
0.64
Overall
104
5.49a
4.05
10b
53c
0.28
0.97d
1.31
Grand Total
187
6.94
5.16
16
56
0.25
1.33
1.62
a, b, c significantly different (p < 0.05)
d not significantly different (p > 0.05)
66
3.4.5.3 Residency
Of the 290 different individuals, 78 (27%) were observed only once, 212 (73%) were
observed more than once, 198 (68%) were resighted within a one year period, and 95
(33%) were resighted across multiple years (Figure 11). Resights were made on 76% of
the females, 74% of the males, 78% of the adult males, and 59% of the juvenile males. Of
the top ten most resighted individuals, 6 were male and 4 were female. The most
resighted individual was an adult male, seen 41 times between April 2005 and April
2009. The most resighted female was sexually mature and seen 30 times between April
2005 and December 2008.
The mean period between resights for all individuals was 181 days (SD = 195), ranging
from a single day to as long as 3.6 yrs. For the highest resighted individual, 31 (78%)
resights had a lag period of less than 2 months, but on two occasions his lag periods
lasted 7 and 10 months in duration.
3.4.5.4 Reproduction
Mating trains were observed during 10 months of the year with most surveys containing
mating trains between December and April. Significantly more mating trains were
observed during the winter season (24%) compared with the summer season (10%) (Chi-
square Test: χ2 = 195.2, df = 1, p < 0.001; Table 5). The proportion of males to females
during winter months (0.60) was not significantly different than the proportion during the
summer months (0.53; Kruskal-Wallis Test: χ2 = 3.65, df = 1, p = 0.056; Table 5).
3.4.5.5 New Individuals
The overall mean rate of newly encountered manta rays was 1.33 per hour of observation.
This rate decreased each year from 2.41 in 2005 to 0.77 in 2008 but increased again in
2009 to 1.02 (Table 5). The month played a significant role in the rate of new individuals
67
Figure 11. The proportion of individual manta rays identified plotted against the number
of surveys in which they were observed.
68
observed (Kruskal-Wallis Test: χ2 = 23.596, df = 11, p = 0.015) with a higher rate of
new individuals observed during the winter months (Chi-square Test: χ2 = 10.355, df =
1, p = 0.001; Table 5).
3.4.6 Threats
3.4.6.1 Natural
A total of 70 individuals (24%) had an injury that appeared to have been caused by a
shark attack based on wound characteristics described for shark predation on marine
mammals and turtles (Corkeron, Morris, & Bryden, 1987; MR Heithaus, 2001; M
Heithaus, Frid, & Dill, 2002). Males and females were both equally likely to have these
injuries (Chi-Square Test: (χ2 = 1.389, df = 1, p = 0.239), but juvenile males were
significantly less likely to possess these injuries when compared with adult males (Chi-
Square Test: (χ2 = 7.509, df = 1, p = 0.023). Only a single juvenile (3%) had shark related
injuries compared with 31 (30%) adult males. Since juvenile females could not be
determined, the proportion of injuries in adult and juvenile females could not be
compared.
3.4.6.2 Anthropogenic
Twenty-eight individuals (10%) had an amputated or disfigured, non-functioning
cephalic fin. The proportion of males and females with cephalic fin injuries were not
significantly different (Chi-Square Test: χ2 = 1.567, df = 1, p = 0.211). The proportion of
adult males and juvenile males with cephalic fin injuries were also not significantly
different (Chi-Square Test: χ2 = 1.676, df = 1, p = 0.433).
Eight individuals had physical evidence of entanglement in fishing line. These included
two with fish hooks embedded in the cephalic fin, two with monofilament line wrapped
around the cephalic fin, two with clear injuries where line had begun to cut part-way
69
through the cephalic fin, and two with visible scars from line that had been wrapped
around the cephalic or pectoral fin.
3.5 DISCUSSION
3.5.1 Abundance and Survivorship
The population of manta rays utilizing the Maui aggregation site consisted of at least 290
individuals, and the rate of new individuals shows no sign of leveling off, suggesting that
the overall population is much larger than all individuals identified. Other reported
population sizes include 185 different individuals identified over a twenty-year period
from an aggregation site off the Yaeyama Islands in Japan, and 54 different individuals
identified over a seven-year period off the Island of Yap in the Western Pacific (Homma
et al., 1999). An estimated 890 individuals, of which 449 individuals were identified over
a five-year period were reported for a resident population along the west coast of
Mozambique, Africa (A. D. Marshall, 2009). In areas where anthropogenic impacts are
not impeding population growth, the size of the local population may be a reflection of
local food availability and the carrying capacity this resource can sustain. For example,
the presence of manta rays around the atolls of the Republic of the Maldives coincide
with the seasonally alternating monsoon currents, supplying rich zooplankton blooms that
support a manta ray population numbering into the thousands (C. R. Anderson et al.,
2008).
3.5.2 Population Range
Photo-identification matches combined with tracks from acoustically tagged animals
provide evidence that individuals from the study area are moving between the 4-islands
that represent the Maui County area (Maui, Molokai, Lanai, and Kahoolawe). with
distances between these neighboring islands ranging from 11 to 15 km. The closest
distance between the Big Island and the island of Maui is 49 km, which would seem be
within the range of attainment for individuals in this Maui population. M. alfredi in Japan
were reported to travel distances of 350 km (Homma et al., 1999), and individuals from a
70
population in the Maldives reportedly travelled 160 km (A.-M. Kitchen-Wheeler, 2008).
However, the absence of photo-identification matches between the Maui population and
the Kona population, for which individual identities have been well documented by
commercial dive operators for the past 10 years, brings to question if movement between
these islands is occurring. The deepest area transited by one of the acoustically tracked
individuals was 324 m. The 2000 m depth in the middle of the Alenuihaha channel could
present a barrier preventing individuals from crossing Maui to the Big Island. A more
likely explanation is that sufficient resources exist within the 4-island region to sustain
the Maui population, making the transit unnecessary. Species such as Cuvier’s (Ziphius
cavirostris) and Blainville’s (Mesoplodon densirostris) beaked whales, spinner dolphins
(Stenella longirostris), and bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) have been reported
to have independent island-associated stocks among the main Hawaiian Islands (Andrews
et al., 2006; Baird et al., 2009; McSweeney, Baird, & Mahaffy, 2007).
The deepest area transited by one of the acoustically tracked individuals was 360 m. The
2000 m depth in the middle of the Alenuihaha channel could present a barrier preventing
individuals from crossing Maui to the Big Island. A more likely explanation is that
sufficient resources exist within the 4-island region to sustain the Maui population,
making the transit unnecessary. Species such as Cuvier’s (Ziphius cavirostris) and
Blainville’s (Mesoplodon densirostris) beaked whales, spinner dolphins (Stenella
longirostris), and bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) have been reported to have
independent island-associated stocks among the main Hawaiian Islands (Andrews et al.,
2006; Baird et al., 2009; McSweeney et al., 2007).
The greatest depth needed to transit from Molokai to Oahu is 600 m. If depth does not
represent an inter-island barrier, future research should compare individuals photo-
identified on Oahu with those from the Maui study area. Additional acoustic tracking of
individuals and genetic sampling could help to confirm whether or not these individuals
are crossing the deep channels to neighboring islands outside the Maui County area.
71
3.5.3 Population Structure
The male to female ratio was near parity with a slight bias towards males (1:0.83).
Marshall & Bennett (2010) reported a strong female biased sex ratio (1:3.5) within a
population off the eastern coast of Mozambique. The attractiveness of an aggregation
area may vary according to the sex or age class of an individual. Aggregation areas in
close proximity to suitable pupping grounds may be more favorable to pregnant females
(AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010). The lack of female bias in this aggregation area may
reflect the absence of a nearby birthing area.
The disc width (DW) of 154 different individuals from this population were measured
using paired-laser photogrammetry (Deakos, 2010), and were all larger than 2.5 m. The
smallest free-swimming individuals for both M. birostris and M. alfredi have been
reported between 1.2 – 1.5 m DW (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953; L. J. V. Compagno,
Marshall, Kashiwagi, & Bennett, 2008). This suggests that very young animals in the
Maui population may be segregated geographically and staying out of the study area,
making them unavailable for sighting. In some coastal shark species, females seek out
discrete, inshore habitats where they give birth and the young spend their first weeks,
months, or years of life protected from predation by larger sharks (e.g., Castro, 1993).
Very young individuals in this population may exhibit similar behavior. This age class
may represent a significant portion of the population that is not accounted for in the
population estimate.
3.5.4 Use of the Aggregation Area
Frequent resights of individuals within and across years support long-term site fidelity to
the Maui study area. Although sight-fielity was highly variable between individuals,
males and females, or adult males and juvenile males were equally likely to revisit the
study site. The times between resights ranged from a single day to over three years with
an average of about 6 months between sightings. Even individuals with the strongest
fidelity to the study site, on occasion, were not resighted for periods of 6 months or more.
72
This is consistent with animals residing in the area for a period of time before dispersing
to a new area. The absence from the study area for long durations may be a product of
decreasing food resources or potential mates in the area during that period. Caution
should be taken when interpreting these results since effort was not continuous and
individuals could have been present in the study area when surveys were not conducted.
Time of day and the time of year were the best predictors of manta ray sighting rates.
They were typically absent in the early morning with sighting rates increasing as the day
progressed. The two individual manta rays equipped with acoustic tags both moved
offshore and out of the study area after sunset. Since both individuals were part of a
mating train when tagged, it is unclear if these offshore movements were representative
of all individuals or specific to individuals in mating trains. Both individuals remained in
the study area for several hours before moving offshore making it unlikely that their
movements were a response to being tagged.
This diurnal trend may be due to manta rays moving out of the study area at night to feed,
since they were never seen feeding while in the study area. Whether or not these animals
were feeding during the night was not confirmed. Although zooplankton distribution and
abundance can be highly variable across space and time (Greene, Wiebe, Pelkie,
Benfield, & Popp, 1998), certain changes in the vertical abundance of zooplankton,
termed deep vertical migration (DVM) can be predictable (for review see Hays, 2003).
Planktivorous elasmobranchs such as basking sharks (Cetorhinus maximus) and whale
sharks (Rhincodon typus) can take advantage of these predictable diel trends by resting
more during the day and foraging more at night when the plankton moves closer to the
surface (Rowat, Meekan, Engelhardt, Pardigon, & Vely, 2007; Sims, Southall, Tarling, &
Metcalfe, 2005). By feeding at night, manta rays could be taking advantage of more
easily accessible euphasid and copepod concentrations. Further research is needed to
better understand when and where this population is feeding.
Although mating trains were observed during 10 months of the year, most occurrences
were concentrated during the winter months, primarily January through April. This was
73
also the time when the proportions of new individuals sighted during a survey were
highest. It is possible that during the reproductive season more individuals visit the
aggregation area in search of mates rather than for use of the cleaning stations. A mating
system is based on the potential of one sex to monopolize key resources or mates of the
limiting sex (Emlen & Oring, 1977). The limiting sex is usually more heavily invested in
parental care, and the greater the imbalance, the more intrasexual competition exists
between members of the other sex (Darwin, 1871). Female manta rays are likely the
limiting sex since they provide the only parental investment in the form of a 12-month
gestation period, and multiple males appear to compete for access to a single female in a
mating train (Yano et al., 1999). The dispersion of females, or resources essential to
females, limits the ability for a male to monopolize multiple females.
A male dominance polygyny mating system could explain shorter residency times
calculated for males, who may move more frequently between aggregation areas in
search of reproductively available females. The shorter residency time for males would
create a greater turnover of males in the study area, making more males available for
sighting. This could explain why the estimates of abundance using mark-recapture were
much larger for males utilizing the study area compared to females, even though the
proportion of photo-identified males and females was nearly equal. Adult females may
benefit from residing longer in a popular aggregation area where she may have a greater
selection of potential mates, provided food resources are also available nearby.
3.5.5 Threats
Both natural threats and anthropogenic threats were documented in this population.
Large sharks (Homma et al., 1999) and killer whales (Orcinus orca) (Visser &
Bonoccorso, 2003) have been reported to prey on manta rays. Since killer whales are
extremely rare in Hawaiian waters (Mobley, Mazzuca, Craig, Newcomer, & Spitz, 2001),
the most likely predator would be large sharks such as the common tiger shark
(Galeocerdo cuvier). About one in four individuals showed injuries likely caused from a
shark attack. Although males and females were equally likely to possess these injuries,
74
adult males were 10 times more likely to have these injuries compared with juvenile
males. This may suggest that juveniles are less susceptible to attacks by sharks, possibly
because they may be geographically segregated in more protective areas during their
early years of development, whereas adults foraging in deeper waters are more
susceptible. The proportional difference could also be an artifact of adults having more
exposure to sharks during their lifetime, or if they are more likely to survive a shark
attack due to their larger body size.
If young manta rays are spending the early years of development in shallower, more
protective geographically segregated to areas that make them less prone to shark
predation, this might explain the low proportion of shark attack injuries on juveniles.
However, if shark attacks on juveniles are fatal, these would go undetected whereas
adults may be more likely to survive an attack.
One out of ten manta rays in the population had an amputated or non-functioning
cephalic fin, most likely due to entanglement in monofilament line. Considering the
function of the cephalic fins to guide food into the mouth during feeding, an animal
reduced to a single cephalic fin would likely suffer a reduction in feeding efficiency.
Individuals in this population with only a single functioning cephalic fin appeared healthy
but further research should investigate how the absence of a cephalic fin affects the size,
growth rate, and reproductive success of these individuals.
All amputated cephalic fins had straight edge cuts, consistent with being severed with
line. Some deformed cephalic fins had straight cuts half way through the fin, most likely
having shed the line before the fin was completely severed. Shark predation as the cause
of cephalic fin damage seems unlikely, as the 70 individuals with shark attack scars, 65
had scars either on the posterior part of their body or on the wing tip. Only five
individuals possessed attack scars anterior to the midline of the body. This suggests that
most attacks are occurring from behind or from the side where the shark is less likely to
be detected. Additionally, eight individuals were observed with either fish hooks
embedded into their cephalic fins, fishing line wrapped around a cephalic fin, or fishing
75
line scars around a cephalic fin and the pectoral fin, providing further support that
entanglement in fishing lines is a significant threat. Further research is needed to
determine the impaired fitness of a manta ray reduced to only a single cephalic fin. This
could be achieved by monitoring their growth and reproductive success over time.
Manta rays have also been known to die from entanglement in boat anchor lines (Bigelow
& Schroeder, 1953), and mooring lines. Two manta ray entanglements in mooring lines
were documented on video in Hawai‘i. The first was reported inside Molokini Crater,
Maui, on 12 Jun 2007 (A. Cummins, pers. comm., 2007), and the second off Kona,
Hawai‘i, on 19 Jun 2009 (K. Osada, pers. comm., 2009). Both manta rays perished and
were consumed by sharks immediately thereafter.
Additional acoustic tracking could assist in determining areas frequented by manta rays
that may be heavily fished and pose a higher risk of entanglement. Managing fishing
practices in these areas or simply educating fishers who utilize these areas could help to
reduce the frequency of manta ray entanglements.
Several manta ray aggregation sites worldwide are being utilized commercially to put
paying clients in the water to swim with the manta rays. Unregulated, these operations
can impose undue stress on the local manta ray population, potentially causing the
animals to abandon the area. Sustained pressure from divers, snorkelers, boaters, and jet
skiers visiting a manta ray aggregation site in Bora Bora, French Polynesia, reportedly
caused the manta rays to completely abandon this area (de Rosemont, 2008). The
biological significance of displacing manta rays from these aggregation sites is unknown
and worthy of investigation. A study conducted by Semeniuk (2009) in which tourists
interacted with a wild population of southern stingrays (Dasyatus americana) resulted in
higher parasite loads, higher injury rates, and suppression of the immune system in these
animals, putting their long-term survival at serious risk.
Recent success in Japan’s manta ray captivity program (Senzo Uchida et al., 2008) has
sparked global interest from aquariums looking to add manta rays to their exhibits. In
76
certain aggregation areas where manta rays are easily accessible, and where no regulatory
protection exists, populations may be exposed to indiscriminant non-sustainable
extraction of individuals for profit, especially those that are small and geographically
isolated.
3.5.6 Population Management
In many parts of the world, measures have been taken to reduce anthropogenic threats on
local manta ray populations. For example, codes of conduct for manta ray dive operators
have been implemented in Kona, Hawai‘i, Western Australia (Daw & McGregor, 2008),
Mozambique, Bora Bora, French Polynesia (de Rosemont, 2008), and in the Maldives (R.
Anderson, Adam, Kitchen-Wheeler, & Stevens, 2011). Elements of the code include
minimizing the number of divers around the manta rays, keeping divers in tight
controlled groups, restricting the touching of animals, and using approach methods that
minimize stress on the manta rays. In Mozambique, mooring balls are banned in areas
where the manta rays are known to aggregate, and boats are required to minimize their
speed. Marine protected areas (MPA) have been established in the Maldives, Mexico,
Mozambique, and Yap, to help eliminate fishing pressure and provide a safe refuge for
the manta rays.
In 2009, the State of Hawai‘i passed a law making it illegal to intentionally kill or extract
manta rays within state waters with an exception given to persons granted a special take
permit. Obtaining such a permit requires the applicant to demonstrate the potential
biological removal (PBR) of the targeted population. PBR is the maximum number of
animals, not including natural mortalities, which may be removed from a stock while
allowing that stock to reach or maintain its optimum sustainable population (Taylor,
Wade, De Master, & Barlow, 2000). This approach was originally designed and
implemented in 1994 as an amendment to the Marine Mammal Protection Act to ensure
more sustainable levels of incidental takes of marine mammals, especially in data poor
situations. PBR is the product of a minimum population estimate of the stock (NMIN),
one-half of the maximum population growth rate (RMAX), and a recovery factor FR.
77
PBR = NMIN * 0.5RMAX * FR
The model has been made simplistic for management purposes and utilizes parameters
that are readily available, and more importantly, that are conservative when biological
parameters are uncertain.
3.5.7 Conclusions
The findings of this study are consistent with a population of more than 290 manta rays
moving into and out of the Maui study area with a varying portion of the total population
temporarily resident in the study area at any given time. Although strong site-fidelity
exists to the study area, individuals range throughout the Maui County area. The Maui
County population appears to be geographically distinct from its neighboring island
populations but further research through active and passive tracking and genetics is
needed to confirm the existence of independent Hawaiian island stocks of M. alfredi.
The biological significance of the study area is not well understood but appears to be an
important staging area where individuals from the population make routine, year-round
visits to either rid themselves of parasites or to find available mates. The absence of very
young individuals (<2.5 m DW) and a biased sex ratio towards adult males indicates that
not all individuals in the population make use of the area equally and segregation is
occurring based on age class and sex. The predominance of adult males and the high
frequency of mating trains observed indicate the study area may also be a significant
mating area, primarily between the months of December through April.
If island-associated M. alfredi populations are indeed geographically independent from
neighboring stocks, with little or no transfer occuring between individuals, regional
management of these population stocks is needed to deal with specific threats that are
unique to each region. Small, isolated populations can be at serious risk of rapid and
unrecoverable decline (Musick, 1999), and the frequent occurrence of large aggregations
78
of manta rays to a small area makes them even more vulnerable to localized
anthropogenic impacts.
The greatest immediate threat to this population appears to be entanglement in
monofilament fishing line, which appears to result in disabling or dismembering the
cephalic fin, likely impacting an individual’s feeding efficiency. Anticipated threats in
the near future include: unregulated swim with manta ray programs adding increased
pressure on animals utilizing this natural aggregation area; and entanglement in proposed
mooring lines for this area.
The recent differentiation of the genus Manta into two separate species raises new
concerns about anthropomorphic impacts placed on highly resident populations. Due to
the slow population growth and low fecundity typical of elasmobranchs (Holden, 1974),
monitoring of changes in population size, population growth, and impact on these
parameters from anthropogenic impacts are recommended. An understanding of
population characteristics and basic ecological information is needed on a regional basis.
79
4 THE REPRODUCTIVE ECOLOGY OF MANTA RAYS
(MANTA ALFREDI) OFF MAUI, HAWAII, WITH AN
EMPHASIS ON BODY SIZE
4.1 ABSTRACT
A combination of photo-identification and photogrammetry was used to study the
reproductive ecology of a resident population of manta rays (Manta alfredi) in what
appears to be an important staging area off Maui, Hawai‘i. Reproductive cycles including
mating, birthing, and estrus were investigated. Although reproductive activities occur
year-round, mating trains and late-term pregnant females were significantly more likely
to be observed during the winter months, and mature females seem capable of ovulating
multiple times during a year if their initial mating attempts are unsuccessful. Sexual
maturity appears delayed until growth exceeds 90% of their maximum size, an indicator
that large body size provides a reproductive advantage in both sexes. Larger females had
higher pregnancy rates, and were more likely to reproduce in successive years, but did
not have more escorting males in their mating train. The mean pregnancy rate for all
females was close to biennial and the operational sex ratio was male biased with 2.68
adult males per reproductively available female. Males do not appear to compete
physically for access to females and body size was not a predictor of a male’s position in
the mating train. Further research is needed to determine how size plays a role in male
mating success.
4.2 INTRODUCTION
In many species, larger body size provides a reproductive advantage for males and
females (Ralls & Mesnick, 2002). For females, large size generally equates to greater
physiological resources for reproduction, and often results in the production of larger,
80
healthier, or more frequent offspring. For males that compete physically with one another
for access to mates, larger males generally win out over smaller males.
The proportion of maximum growth reached at sexual maturity can be an indicator of the
importance of large body size for reproductive success and can also be an indicator of the
availability and predictability of food resources (Shine, 1988). This study examines the
reproductive ecology of a resident manta ray (Manta alfredi) population off Maui,
Hawai‘i, and the relationship of body size and reproductive activities.
Two species of manta ray are currently recognized (AD Marshall et al., 2009): Manta
birostris, herein referred to as oceanic manta rays, and Manta alfredi, herein referred to
as resident manta rays (Deakos, 2010). The oceanic manta is the larger of the two species
with a maximum disc width (DW; measured from wing tip to wing tip) reported at 6.7 m
(Bigelow & Schroeder 1953) and is widely distributed, occurring in tropical, sub-tropical
and temperate waters around the globe. Members of this species spend the majority of
their time in deep waters but are commonly sighted along productive coastlines with
regular upwellings, oceanic island groups, and near offshore pinnacles, seamounts, and
submarine ridge systems (LJV Compagno & Last, 1999; AD Marshall et al., 2009;
Rubin, 2002).
Resident manta rays average smaller than oceanic manta rays with a maximum recorded
DW of 5.5 m (AD Marshall et al., 2009). This species is found in tropical and subtropical
regions of the Pacific, Atlantic, and Indian oceans within 30 degrees of latitude to the
north and south of the equator where they occur primarily along shallow coastal areas
that are often associated with coral reef habitats within a few kilometers of land (Homma
et al., 1999; A. D. Marshall, 2009).
Most female resident manta rays become pregnant on average every 2-3 yrs, though some
are capable of becoming pregnant in consecutive years (Homma et al., 1999; AD
Marshall & Bennett, 2010). Birthing has never been reported in free-ranging manta rays
and birthing areas are unknown. The only documented birthing event occurred at the
81
Okinawa Churaumi Aquarium in Japan, where a resident females produced a single,
precocial pup, following what was determined to be a twelve-month gestation period (S
Uchida, Toda, & Kamei, 1990). Parturition was immediate and the mother was seen
mating again within hours. The smallest free-swimming manta rays of either species have
been reported to be between 1-1.5 m DW (Bigelow & Schroeder, 1953; Homma et al.,
1999; AD Marshall et al., 2009). Parental care is absent beyond gestation and no
information exists about pup development or survival.
Pre-copulatory behavior of manta rays involves multiple, escorting males, pursuing a
single, fast-swimming female in what is known as a “mating train” (Yano et al., 1999).
Rapid swimming is interrupted by periods of quick turns and somersaults initiated by the
female and often mimicked by the pursuing males (AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010). When
copulation occurs, a male directly behind the female moves over her while biting her
pectoral fin, almost always on the left side, and twists his body so that his ventral side is
against hers and a clasper is inserted into her cloaca for insemination. Although mating
trains are commonly observed, reports of actual copulation in free-ranging manta rays are
rare (AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010; Yano et al., 1999).
Resident manta rays are sexually dimorphic with females as much as 16% larger than
males (Deakos, 2010; AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010). This size difference make it
unlikely that males are able to force an unwilling female to mate. Because females carry
all the burden of parental investment in a 12 month gestation, they are likely the choosier
sex (Trivers, 1972), and should select males that are the most fit (Fisher, 1930). Mating
trains are likely a way for reproductively available females to evaluate potential mates.
By moving rapidly through an area while advertising her willingness to mate, a female
may recruit male escorts in an attempt to find the best suitor. These “female recruitment
runs” have been observed in other species. For example, female bison (Bison bison) seek
to replace a lower-ranking, tending male by running away from him and towards higher-
ranking males, usually resulting in her tending male being replaced by one of higher rank
(Wolff, 1998). Humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) also engage in similar
behavior, in which two or more males, (sometimes as many as 20 or more males),
82
compete physically for the primary escort position (N1E) closest to the female (Baker &
Herman, 1984; Herman et al., 2007; Tyack & Whitehead, 1982). In humpback whales,
the N1E is typically the largest male in the group (S. Spitz, Herman, Pack, & Deakos,
2002).
Our limited understanding of elasmobranch reproduction is in large part due to the
difficulty in studying them in the wild. Their wide distribution in an aquatic environment
poses many challenges. However, two characteristics of resident manta rays facilitate our
ability to study their life history. First, each individual manta is born with a unique
pattern of spots on their ventral surface (Andrea D. Marshall et al., 2008), that appear to
remain unchanged over its lifetime and can be reliably identified (T. B. Clark, 2001;
Homma et al., 1999; Kitchen-Wheeler, 2010; A. D. Marshall, 2009; Yano et al., 1999).
Second, though the home range of individuals in this population is broad and extends
throughout Maui County waters, an area comprising over 3,210 km2 (Deakos, Baker, &
Bejder, submitted), resident manta rays are known to congregate at specific locations on
the reef known as cleaning stations. Cleaning stations are where individuals solicit host
cleaner fish to remove parasitic copepods from their body’s surface (Coté, 2000; Losey
Jr, 1972). Strong site fidelity to these cleaning stations allows for reliable encounters
(e.g., Homma et al., 1999), making these aggregation areas ideal for studying resident
manta rays (T. B. Clark, 2001; Deakos, 2010; Dewar et al., 2008; Homma et al., 1999; A.
D. Marshall, 2009).
This paper focuses on the reproductive cycles and role of body size in the reproductive
ecology of a resident manta ray population off Maui, Hawai‘i. Several aspects of their
reproductive ecology were investigated.
4.2.1 Reproductive Cycles
Most viviparous elasmobranchs follow annual reproductive cycles with somewhat
synchronous mating, gestation, and parturition (for review see Hamlett & Koob, 1999).
Seasonal reproduction generally occurs if it maximizes a female’s chance to successfully
83
produce offspring. This is often influenced by the seasonal availability of food so that
young are developing during a time of year when food is more plentiful. A reduction in
predation and improved weather conditions could also influence the occurrence of
seasonal breeding.
Reproductive seasonality in the Maui population of resident manta rays was investigated.
Females observed in mating trains were assumed to be ovulating. The hypothesis tested
was that a reproductive advantage to seasonal breeding should reveal mating trains and
late-term pregnancies (based on a 12 month gestation period) to occur more often during
certain times of the year.
4.2.2 Role of Body Size
The role of body size in the Maui population of resident manta rays was investigated by
the observations of the relationship between body size and reproductive activity. Based
on data collected by Deakos (2010), the estimated size of the largest female and male in
this population using paired-laser photogrammetry was 3.62 m DW and 3.05 m DW
respectively. The minimum size at sexual maturity was estimated at 3.37 m DW for
females, and 2.80 m DW for males. Assuming these maximum sizes are representative of
maximum growth, sexual maturity in both sexes is delayed until growth reaches greater
than 90% of maximum size. This suggests that the reproductive advantage of larger size
must strongly outweigh the cost of a reduced reproductive time period. Given that larger
body size should provide a reproductive advantage to both males and females in this
population, several hypotheses were tested:
1) Larger females should have higher pregnancy rates, and more consecutive year
pregnancies than smaller females. Larger females, being more fecund, should attract a
greater number of escorts to her mating train than do smaller females (cf. Pack et al.,
2009).
84
2) Larger males should be more likely to hold the position closest to the female (N1E), in
a mating train (cf. S. Spitz et al., 2002). These males should be larger, on average, than
all other males in the train, primarily those that have never been observed in the N1E
position. Larger males should also be more likely to choose larger females and therefore
should be associated with larger females (cf. Pack et al., 2009).
These hypotheses are based on the assumptions: a) males physically compete with other
males for access to limited females, and b) that an operational sex ratio (OSR), defined as
the average ratio of fertilizable females to sexually active males at any given time
(Emlen, 1976; Emlen & Oring, 1977), skewed towards males, should favor male
competition over limiting females.
The OSR for the Maui resident manta population was approximately two adult males for
every adult female (Deakos et al., submitted). Generally the sex with the lower parental
investment will be the sex towards which OSR is biased (Trivers, 1972). The level of bias
in the OSR will determine how intense the sex that is in excess will compete for access to
the other. The sex in shortage may afford to be selective if there are many potential mates
to choose among (e.g., Berglund, 1994) or they may be unselective and simply mate with
fitter mates as an outcome of contest competition (Cox & Le Boeuf, 1977).
4.3 METHODS
4.3.1 Data Collection
Surveys of the resident manta rays were conducted with SCUBA at an aggregation area
off the west coast of the island of Maui over 6-year period from 2005 through 2010. A
description of the study area and detailed methodology is presented in Chapter 3. A
survey consisted of a rectangular search pattern originating from the same location
covering an area approximately 200 m X 150 m. When manta rays were encountered,
attempts were made to collect the following information on each individual: (1) photo-
85
identification, (2) sex, (3) age class, (4) female pregnancy status, (5) DW measurements,
and (6) behavioral role when in a mating train.
Ventral markings of each individual were photographed for identification using either a
Canon Powershot S70 still camera in an underwater housing, or a Sony HDR-HC1 high
definition camcorder housed in a Sea & Sea VX-HC1 underwater housing. Sex was
determined by the presence (males) or absence (females) of claspers. Calcification of
claspers occurs rapidly over a relatively narrow range of growth with the majority of
calcification occurring once the claspers have extended beyond the length of the pelvic
fins (AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010; W. T. White et al., 2006). Since the onset of clasper
calcification in elasmobranchs coincides with a rapid rate of clasper growth and gonadal
maturation (e.g., L. Marshall, White, & Potter, 2007), claspers extending well beyond the
margins of the pelvic fins were used as a reliable indicator of sexual maturity. Females
were considered mature if they possessed mating scars (abrasions on the wing tip), or
were obviously pregnant based on the extreme distention of her abdomen (AD Marshall
& Bennett, 2010). The distention of the abdomen does not become apparent until at least
6.5 months into a female’s pregnancy (AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010). A female without
mating scars, or that was never observed pregnant was given an age class status of
“unknown.” DW measurements (measured from wing to tip to wing tip) were obtained
using paired-laser photogrammetry as described in Deakos (2010). A mating train
consisted of a single female being pursued by at least two adult males. Rarely a female
leading a mating train was seen to follow a second female for a brief period; this second
female was not considered part of the mating train. The female considered part of the
mating train was given the behavioral role of “nuclear female” (NF). The male closest to
the NF, usually directly behind, was called the “primary escort” (N1E). The male directly
behind the N1E was called the “secondary escort” (N2E). All additional males in the train
were call “escorts” (NE). Since the position of males often changed while observing a
mating train, males could receive multiple behavioral roles as part of any given train.
Mating train events were recorded using the high-definition, underwater, video camera.
86
4.3.2 Data Analysis
Photo-identification images of each individual from a survey were matched against a
catalog of all identified individuals from the study area to determine if that individual had
been previously seen (a resight) or was a first sighting. Size measurements were
processed as described in Chapter 2.
For a given time period, the number of surveys in which trains were observed was
divided by the total number of surveys to obtain a proportion of mating trains observed.
Proportions were calculated for each month and for each season. Seasons were
categorized as “winter” (November through April) and “summer” (May through October)
encompassing the coldest and wettest months and the warmest and driest months of the
year, respectively. The proportion of sightings an individual was observed in a particular
behavioral role was calculated for each individual, and for each behavioral role. If an
individual was observed in more than one behavioral role during a sighting, a proportion
was calculated for each.
4.3.3 Reproductive Cycles
Chi-square analyses were used to compare the proportion of surveys containing mating
trains, and the proportion of surveys containing a pregnant female (PF), by month and by
season. Mean train sizes were calculated by month and season and analyzed for
significant differences using a Kruskal-Wallis Test, and a Mann-Whitney U-Test
respectively.
Pregnancy rates were estimated by dividing the number of years in which a female was
determined to be visibly pregnant by the total number of years seen. Years with
insufficient sightings to determine if a female was pregnant during that year were
omitted. The overall mean pregnancy rate for the population was an average of all
individual pregnancy rates.
87
A successful consecutive year pregnancy was scored if a female was observed pregnant
during two consecutive years, with a minimum of 7 months occurring between the two
pregnancies. This minimum delay between pregnancies was to ensure that the same
pregnancy was not counted in both years. A failed consecutive year pregnancy was
scored if a female was not observed pregnant the year following a pregnancy. The rate of
consecutive year pregnancies for a female was calculated by dividing successful scores
by the sum of successful and failed scores. Only years with enough sightings to
determine whether or not a female was pregnant during that year, were used in the
analysis.
OSR was determined by calculating the total number of males prepared to mate (adult
males) divided by the total number of males and females prepared to mate (adult females
and reproductively available females) (Kvarnemo & Ahnesjo, 1996).
4.3.4 Role of Body Size
Minimum, maximum, and mean DWs, and standard deviations (SD) were quantified for
NFs, PFs, N1Es, N2Es, and NEs. A behavioral proportion was calculated for each
individual in each behavioral role as the number of sightings that individual was observed
in the behavioral role by the total number of sightings. The mean behavioral proportion
was calculated for all individuals observed in that role as a measure of the proportion an
individual may occupy a particular behavioral role. Thus, if the same individual were
observed in multiple behavioral roles during a single survey, a proportion was calculated
for each behavioral role.
The mean DW of NFs was compared to the mean DW of PFs using a Mann-Whitney U-
Test. Linear regression was used to determine if larger PFs were correlated with higher
pregnancy rates, and more consecutive year pregnancies. Linear regression was also used
to determine if larger NFs were positively correlated with more males in her train.
88
A Kruskal-Wallis Test was used to compare mean sizes between all adult males, NEs,
N1Es, N2Es, and NEs that have never been observed as a N1E. Linear regression was
used to determine if larger N1Es were positively correlated with larger NFs. A Mann-
Whitney U-Test was used to compare the mean DW of all N1Es to the mean DW of all
NEs that had never been observed as an N1E.
All linear regression tests were directional (one-tailed) since the hypothesis was that
larger predictor variables would correlate positively with larger body size. Significance
was determined at a 0.05 probability level. Statistical analyses were performed using
SPSS version 17.0 (SPSS & Inc., 2007).
4.4 RESULTS
Of the 309 individual manta rays identified, 159 (51%) were males and 150 (49%) were
females. Based on clasper length (Deakos, 2010), 112 (70%) males were adults and 57
(30%) were juveniles. Based on size at sexual maturity for this population (Deakos,
2010), of the 163 sized individuals, among males, 51 (60%) were of adult size with 34
(40%) of juvenile size, and among females, 37 (47%) were of adult size with 41 (53%) of
juvenile size. Applying these proportions to the total number of males and females in the
population, 95 adult males and 71 adult females were estimated to be available for
mating, producing an OSR of 1.34 adult males per adult female.
A total of 286 surveys were conducted between February 4, 2005 and July 14, 2010
(Table 6). Even though it was somewhat common to see a single male in pursuit of a
female, the interaction was generally brief lasting only a few seconds before the male
abandoned the female and therefore was not considered a mating train. Mating trains
were observed on 32 (11%) surveys and ranged in size from 3 to 18 individuals.
Generally, only one mating train was seen on a survey. Mating trains were observed each
year and in all months of the year except May and August.
89
Table 6. Summary of surveys conducted by month and season showing the total number and proportion of surveys observed
with mating trains and pregnant females. The proportions of surveys with trains and pregnant females were significantly higher
during the winter season.
Month
No.
Surveys
No. Surveys
with Trains
Proportion
with Trains
Mean
Train Size
Surveys
with PFs
Proportion
with PFs
NOV
22
1
0.05
6.00
5
0.23
DEC
24
7
0.29
8.29
6
0.25
JAN
16
3
0.19
6.33
1
0.06
FEB
15
5
0.33
6.20
4
0.27
MAR
16
3
0.19
4.67
5
0.31
Winter
APR
31
4
0.13
8.25
5
0.16
Winter Subtotal
124
23
0.19a
6.62
26
0.21b
MAY
25
0
0.00
-
1
0.04
JUN
19
2
0.11
9.00
3
0.16
JUL
30
4
0.13
5.00
0
0.00
AUG
23
0
0.00
-
1
0.04
SEP
34
2
0.06
14.50
4
0.12
Summer
OCT
31
1
0.03
4.00
6
0.19
Summer Subtotal
162
9
0.06a
8.13
15
0.09b
Grand Total
286
32
0.11
7.22
41
0.14
a, b Significantly different, p < 0.05
90
4.4.1 Reproductive Cycles
The proportion of surveys containing a mating train varied significantly by month (Chi-
Square Test: χ2 = 27.255, df = 11, n = 286, p = 0.004) and by season (Chi-Square Test: χ2
= 11.932, df = 1, n = 286, p = 0.001). Mating trains were three times more likely to be
observed during the winter months (Table 6). Mean mating train size was 7.22 animals
(SD = 4.10) with the smallest containing a female and two males (by definition) and the
largest containing a female and 17 males. The most common train size was 2 males
(25%); 9 trains (28%) had 10 or more males. Train size did not vary significantly by
month (Kruskal-Wallis Test: χ2 = 9.220, df =9, n = 32, p = 0.417) or by season (Mann-
Whitney U-Test: Ζ = -0.407, n = 32, p = 0.705). Of the 28 trains observed, 21 different
NFs were identified (Table 7). On average, these females were observed as NF’s 21% of
the time (4% - 50%). Most were seen only once as a NF (n = 16) and one was seen four
times. Five NFs (24%) were also pregnant while leading the mating train.
The mean proportion of sightings containing a pregnant female varied significantly by
month (Chi-Square Test: χ2 = 19.917, df = 11, n = 286, p = 0.046) and by season (Chi-
Square Test: χ2 = 7.841, df = 1, n = 286, p=0.006). Pregnant females were more than
twice as likely (21%) to be observed during the winter compared to the summer (9%).
During 51 (18%) surveys, at least one pregnant female was observed (Table 6). A total of
20 individual females were observed pregnant (Table 8). On average these females were
observed pregnant on 25% of the surveys (3% - 100%).
91
Table 7. The resight history of 21 nuclear females (NF) observed between years 2005 and
2010. Numbers indicate the month in which they were observed as a NF during that year
with train size indicated in brackets. Bolded IDs indicate females observed in a train
during a summer month.
Nuclear
Female
ID
DW
(m)
2005
2006
2007
2008
2009
2010
Total
Sightings
1021
n/a
4(18)
2
1029
n/a
2(9)
5
1062
n/a
1(10)
4
2002
n/a
12(7)
1(3)
5
3011
n/a
9(16)
4
3041
n/a
3(8)
4
3056
n/a
2(4)
2
5023
n/a
12(8)
5
5029
n/a
4(5)
3
13005
3.30
9(13)
12(10)
7(5)
15
7006
3.37
7(11)
25
3003
3.39
10(4)
3(3)
14
3019
3.39
1(6)
19
12010
3.41
2(9)
9
5001
3.42
1(10)
9
5003
3.44
7(3)
4(6), 6(7),
12(10)
21
7002
3.45
12(3)
13
8008
3.48
2(3)
15
6011
3.48
12(10)
7
3060
3.52
2(4)
4
3030
3.62
3(3)
10
mean
3.44
92
Table 8. The resight history and disc width (DW), if available, of 20 pregnant females
during the years 2005 through 2010. P indicates she was observed pregnant and her
pregnancy was new for that year; N indicates she was observed enough times during that
year to determine she was unlikely pregnant; U indicates the she was observed during
that year but not sufficiently often to determine if she was visibly pregnant; and a dash (-)
indicates she was not sighted during that entire year. Bolded IDs indicate females
observed pregnant during a summer month. Estimated Pregnancy Rates (EPR) based on a
minimum of 3 yrs with sufficient data, and Estimate Consecutive Pregnancy Rates
(ECPR) based on a minimum of 2 consecutive yrs with sufficient data are shown.
Pregnant
Female
ID
DW
(m)
2005
2006
2007
2008
2009
2010
EPR
ECPR
Total
Sightings
2036
n/a
-
-
P
-
-
-
-
-
1
3041
n/a
-
-
U
N
P
P
0.67
1.00
4
3056
n/a
-
-
-
U
P
-
-
-
2
12009
3.35
-
-
-
P
-
-
-
-
6
12011
3.39
-
-
-
P
-
-
-
-
4
3003
3.39
P
-
N
P
N
U
0.50
0.00
14
3019
3.39
-
N
P
N
N
-
0.25
0.00
19
5013
3.41
-
-
-
P
N
-
-
0.00
21
5008
3.44
-
U
-
P
-
-
-
-
6
5003
3.44
N
N
U
N
P
-
0.25
-
21
10002
3.45
N
N
P
N
-
N
0.20
0.00
31
8008
3.48
N
U
U
P
-
-
0.50
-
15
3001
3.51
U
P
U
N
N
-
0.33
-
6
10000
3.52
U
-
-
P
-
-
-
-
7
3060
3.52
-
-
-
U
P
-
-
-
4
7000
3.53
P
P
-
P
N
-
0.75
0.50
6
3008
3.55
P
U
U
U
P
P
1.00
1.00
17
12005
3.58
-
-
P
U
N
-
-
-
5
1007
3.59
U
U
U
P
P
P
1.00
1.00
15
3030
3.62
-
P
-
U
N
P
0.66
-
10
mean
3.48
0.56
93
One notable female (ID# 5003) was observed as a NF on 4 occasions over an eight-
month period (Figure 12). On 22 Apr 2008, she was an NF with 6 males, periodically
following a larger pregnant female (ID#5013), who was not considered part of the mating
train. On 23 Apr 2008 she was being pursued briefly by a single male (ID#8002) that had
not been present the day before, and again periodically following a larger, pregnant
female (ID#5008), one different from the day before. Fresh mating scars were visible on
her left pectoral fin indicating that mating had at least been recently attempted. On 24
Apr 2008, she was observed on her own. The same male (ID#8002) was pursuing her two
days later on 26 Apr 2008. On 15 May 2008, she was observed on her own with other
mating trains in the vicinity. On 12 Jun 2008 she was leading a train of 7 males. On 24
Jun 2008 she was on her own and was not seen again until 10 Dec 2008, when she was
leading a train of 9 males. On 8 Feb 2009 she was observed on her own, and on 21 Mar
2009 she was observed on her own while a separate eight animal train passed through the
area. During both of these latter sightings, she did not appear pregnant, but on 24 Aug
2009, she was visually confirmed to be pregnant.
4.4.2 Role of Body Size
The mean, minimum, and maximum body size of all females, NFs, PFs, all males,
juvenile males, transition males, adult males, all escorting males, N1Es, N2Es, and NEs
that have never been seen as an N1E are shown in Figure 13. The average female DW
was 3.18 m (SD = 0.31). A total of 21 NFs and 20 pregnant females were observed. NFs
averaged 16% larger than all males measured in mating trains, but NF DWs did not
significantly differ from PF DWs (Mann-Whitney U Test: Ζ = -1.196, n = 29, p = 0.232).
Larger PFs had significantly higher pregnancy rates than smaller PFs (Linear Regression:
R2 = 0.520, df = 8, F = 8.684, p = 0.009), and were significantly more likely to be
observed pregnant in consecutive years (Linear Regression: R2 = 0.882, df = 5, F =
37.470, p = 0.001). Mating trains with larger females did not contain significantly more
males (Linear Regression: R2 = 0.277, df = 11, F = 3.841, p = 0.078).
94
Figure 12. The sighting history of a notable female (ID#5003) observed multiple times as a NF. Numbers in brackets indicate
the number of animals in the mating train.
95
Figure 13. Distribution of manta ray disc widths. Heavy black lines = means, box boundaries = 25th and 75th percentiles,
whiskers = smallest and largest observed values that are not statistical outliers, circles = statistical outliers, numbers = sample
sizes. *not significantly different, p = 0.232, #not significantly different, p = 0.363.
96
Most pregnant females were observed to be pregnant only once during the 6 years of the study (n
= 15). Two were observed pregnant in two separate years, and three were observed pregnant in
three separate years. The latter three were among the nine largest females of the 77 measured.
Three females were confirmed pregnant in two consecutive years. One female, confirmed
pregnant in three consecutive years, was the second largest female measured in the population.
Based on 11 females observed pregnant at least once, with sufficient sightings to determine
pregnancy status for at least 3 separate years, the estimated mean pregnancy rate was 0.56 pups
per year (0.20 – 1.0; Table 8). Based on a biennial pregnancy rate, nearly half of these females
may not be available for mating, thereby inflating the OSR to 2.68 adult males per
reproductively available female.
The most likely months for giving birth based on the earliest confirmation that a pregnant female
was no longer pregnant were November through April. One half of the PFs were confirmed with
mating scars, and at least two PFs were confirmed without. Of the 41 females observed with
mating scars, all had scars on the dorsal side of their left wing tip, and two (5%) females had
visible mating scars on the dorsal surface of both the left and right wing tip.
The average male DW was 2.83 m (SD = 0.14). A total of 22 different males were identified
occupying the N1E position in a mating train (Table 9). On average these males were observed
as the N1E on 14% of the surveys (2% - 40%), and the majority (73%) were only seen once as
the N1E during the 6 years. A total of 19 different N2Es were observed. On average these males
were observed as N2E’s 17% of the time (7% - 50%). A total of 40 different NEs were observed.
On average these males were observed as NEs on 22% of the surveys (4% - 100%).
No significant differences were found between the mean sizes of all adult males, NEs, N1Es,
N2Es, and NEs never seen as N1Es (Kruskal-Wallis Test: χ2 = 4.328, df = 4, n = 112, p = 0.363).
Larger N1Es were not significantly correlated with larger NFs (Linear Regression: R2 = 0.001, df
= 14, F = 0.011, p = 0.918). Adult males were frequently observed following females briefly
97
Table 9. The resight history and disc width (DW), if available, of 22 individual nuclear primary
escorts (N1Es) during the years 2005 through 2010. N1E indicates he was the primary escort in a
mating train, and the bracketed numbers represent the month followed by the train size. S
indicates the male was sighted but not as a N1E during that year. A dash (-) signifies he was not
sighted during any surveys for that entire year. Bolded IDs indicate a male observed escorting in
a train during a summer month.
N1E
ID#
DW
(m)
2005
2006
2007
2008
2009
2010
Total
Sightings
1033
n/a
-
-
S
S
N1E(3:8)
-
4
2014
n/a
S
N1E
(4:18)
-
N1E (12:10)
-
S
5
3017
n/a
-
S
S
N1E (12:10)
S
-
14
3055
n/a
-
-
-
N1E (6:7)
-
-
5
6023
n/a
-
-
-
S
-
N1E(3:3)
3
8017
2.84
S
S
-
N1E
(6:7,6:11)
S
S
32
2005
2.89
N1E
(7:3)
S
S
S
S
S
16
1001
2.90
S
S
-
S
S
N1E(1:3)
12
3064
2.90
-
-
-
S
N1E(1:6)
4
2039
2.91
-
-
-
S
S
N1E(3:3)
8
4000
2.92
S
S
S
N1E (2:9)
N1E(1:10,2:4)
-
27
7010
2.92
-
-
-
N1E (12:10)
-
-
5
3007
2.94
S
-
N1E
(7:11)
S
S
-
12
13007
2.94
S
S
S
S
N1E(1:10,2:4)
S
45
5019
2.96
-
-
-
N1E(4:6)
S
-
12
8012
2.98
-
S
-
S
N1E(1:10)
-
10
3033
2.98
-
-
S
S
S
N1E(3:3)
6
8002
2.99
N1E
(9:16)
S
N1E
(7:11)
N1E(10:4)
N1E(7:3)
-
34
2037
3.00
S
-
-
N1E(2:9,12:7)
S
-
8
1003
3.02
S
S
-
S
N1E(2:4)
-
10
3023
3.02
-
S
S
S
N1E(4:5)
S
21
8009
3.03
S
S
S
N1E(12:3)
N1E(1:10)
-
31
mean
2.95
98
from behind, or occasionally turning abruptly in order to pass through an area where a female
recently defecated. One notable male (ID#13007) was the most frequently sighted individual in
the study. He was observed on 41 occasions over six years, and during seven of these sightings
he was observed pursuing a female (Figure 14). Except for encounters on 22 Apr 2008 and 24
Apr 2008, in which the female was the same, all other females were different. On 20 Jan 2009,
one of the few occasions when more than one mating train was in the area, he was observed
switching back and forth between the two NFs. His behavioral role within the train varied
frequently as did others but he often moved to the N1E position with what appeared to be little
effort and without any conflict from those males already holding that primary position, even if
that male was larger in size.
4.5 DISCUSSION
4.5.1 Reproductive Cycles
Although mating trains and late-term pregnant manta rays in this study were observed at all
times of the year, they were significantly more likely to be observed during the winter season
indicating some reproductive advantage. Seasonal breeding may help to concentrate adult males
during certain times of the year, thereby increasing a female’s access to more or better mate
choices. Seasonality may also coincide with improved food resources available to pups or
reduced predation. Without knowing the location of birthing areas, or what resources pups use
during their initial years of development, it is difficult to identify the benefits that may exist for
pups born during the winter.
The preponderance of mating trains observed in winter, combined with 2 of the 5 females in
summer trains were also seen in a train during the previous winter, suggest that females prefer to
mate in winter. Summer mating trains may function to allow females who were unsuccessful in
getting pregnant during winter, or ones that aborted, a chance of mating again during a less
favorable time of the year. This was further supported by a female who was not visibly pregnant
99
Figure 14. The sighting history of a notable male (ID#13007) with a DW of 2.94 m, observed pursuing a female on seven
occasions. The behavioral role(s) observed are indicated for each sighting as well as the train size in brackets.
100
until the summer of 2009, indicating her mating attempts 14 and 16 months earlier were
unsuccessful, or that her pregnancy was aborted.
The rarity of two mating trains at one time suggests that ovulations may be staggered,
perhaps reducing competition among females, and providing all reproductively available
females an opportunity to mate with higher-ranking males in the area. Staggered
ovulation would make it possible for a select subset of higher-ranking males to dominate
paternity, at least within a localized area. However, since the study area represents only a
small portion of the estimated home range, additional mating trains may have been
present in other areas.
Females ovulating outside the primary reproductive season should find less competition
for male mates if the population of adult males remains constant throughout the year. If
true, larger train sizes would be expected during the summer with fewer available
females, but train sizes did not differ significantly between seasons, a possible indication
that the OSR remains unchanged with fewer adult males available as well. Females
seeking mates during the summer may have access to fewer mate choices and possibly
fewer quality males.
The existing OSR of an aggregation area may reflect the habitat choices of different
individuals in the population, which could vary by season. A female’s lifetime
reproductive success is dependent on her ability to raise offspring to the age of
independence (Clutton-Brock, Guinness, & Albon, 1982), and the habitat she chooses is
often a trade-off between an area rich in food resources and the needs and security of her
offspring. Females that are preparing to give birth may choose a habitat that is near
sufficient food resources and cleaning stations, but also near a desired birthing area.
Therefore, aggregation sites in close proximity to birthing areas may be biased towards
pregnant females, while non-pregnant females and others members of the population take
advantage of better food resources at locations that may be distant from any birthing area.
This could explain a difference in sex ratios between the Maui aggregation site, where
101
females comprise 47% of individuals, and an aggregation site off Mozambique, in which
females comprise 75% of the individuals (AD Marshall & Bennett, 2010). The absence of
very small individuals (Deakos, 2010) and a nearly equal number of males and females
utilizing the Maui study site could reflect the absence of a nearby birthing area.
Elasmobranchs are known to have a very good sense of olfaction and taste (Hodgson &
Mathewson, 1978; Kleerekoper, 1978) and can use these senses to detect biochemical
products released by other organisms, including females trying to attract potential mates
(I. Gordon, 1993; Johnson & Nelson, 1978). Ari & Correia (2008) reported an acute
sense of smell from a captive oceanic manta ray. Brief investigations of females by adult
males were likely attempts by males to sense a female’s reproductive state through her
bodily excretions.
The low pregnancy frequencies for this resident population of manta rays is consistent
with reported biennial mating in many elasmobranch females, most likely to allow post-
partum recovery to rebuild reproductive reserves before mating again (Pratt & Carrier,
2001). Due to the small sample size in determining the mean pregnancy rate, and the
possibility that females classified as non-pregnant in earlier years of the study could have
been immature at that time, the mean pregnancy rate should be used with caution.
Additionally, if some pregnant females had less distended abdomens during late-term
pregnancy, they could have been misdiagnosed as non-pregnant.
4.5.2 Role of Body Size
Larger females had significantly higher pregnancy rates and were significantly more
likely to become pregnant in consecutive years, consistent with larger females benefiting
from greater reproductive success. Larger females did not have a greater number of male
escorts, contrary to what was expected if males were exhibiting mate choice and choosing
larger, more fecund females. However, given that mating trains could last several hours
and possibly several days, the number of males observed during the survey may consist
of only a fraction of the total.
102
Males holding the N1E position in a mating train were not significantly larger than any
other adult male, contrary to what was expected if males were competing physically for
access to the female (cf. S. Spitz et al., 2002). There was also no evidence that larger
males were choosing larger females to escort.
Occasionally males that were not part of the mating train inserted themselves into the
primary position for very brief periods. When this occurred, the N1E at the time simply
retreated to the N2E position without any confrontation. It is possible that dominance had
already been established among these males and risking injury by fighting a more
dominant male was not advantageous. The absence of male combats suggests that this is
not a preferred strategy of males or perhaps other mating opportunities are available that
reduce the need to engage in fighting for potential mates.
A variable OSR across seasons can strongly influence the success of male tactics and the
predominant mating system for that season (Madsen & Shine, 1993). When OSR is low
(female bias), body size seems to show little advantage in reproductive success since
many females are available, few combats occur, and smaller males receive mating
opportunities. Factors that may affect OSR include biased adult sex ratios, differences
between the sexes in age at maturity, reproductive longevity, migration schedules, spatial
distribution, mortality during the reproductive season (for review see Clutton-Brock &
Vincent, 1991), or momentary differences in the distribution of the sexes (e.g., Höglund,
Montgomerie, & Widemo, 1993).
Although the overall number of males and females in this population were nearly equal,
the sex ratio of adults was estimated at 1.34 males per female. Biennial mating would
reduce the number of reproductively available females by half, which will lead to an OSR
of 2.68 mating males for every mating female in this population. This male bias should
favor more intense competition between males but the absence of male combats in
mating trains indicates such competition may not be directly physical.
103
Some studies have found that in the absence of intense physical competition between
males, male reproductive success is influenced more by social factors than by
morphological traits associated with size (Bercovitch, 1989). In whitetip reef sharks
(Triaenodon obesus), group courtship has been observed where multiple males at the
same time bite, mount, and attempt to copulate with a single female (Whitney 2004).
Cooperation between males has also been suggested in order to achieve successful mating
with a female (Carrier, Pratt Jr, & Martin, 1994).
Competition between males may be occurring through sperm competition. Observations
of some female sharks copulating with multiple males during a mating bout suggest
sperm competition may be occurring in some elasmobranch species (Carrier et al., 1994;
Pratt & Carrier, 2001). Sperm competition may be an alternative mating strategy by
males, in which the male’s sperm compete for fertilization of the eggs during a single
fertile period (GA Parker, 1970). Yano (1999) reported two male oceanic manta rays
mating in succession with the same female, although this has been the only report of a
female manta mating with more than one male in the same day (AD Marshall & Bennett,
2010).
Among mammals, relative testes size is a good indicator of whether or not sperm
competition exists (Gomendio , Harcourt , & Roldan 1998). Right whales (Eubalaena
australis) for example, which have multiple males mating almost in unison with a single
female, have testes weighing over one ton each, more than 1% of their total body weight,
while those of sperm whales and humpback whales, which are known to fight
aggressively for mates, have testes weighing less than 0.5% of their total body weight
(Brownell & Balls 1986). The relative weight of manta ray testes in mature males should
be further examined to determine the likelihood of sperm competition as a mating
strategy. In birds and mammals, where frequency of copulation is high, testes tend to be
large, and where it is low, testes tend to be small (e.g., TR Birkhead, Briskie, & Miller,
1993; Harvey & Harcourt, 1984). The rarity of observed copulation acts suggests that
sperm competition is not likely a predominant male mating strategy in resident manta
rays.
104
Endurance rivalry, which can be defined as the ability to remain reproductively active
during a large part of the mating season (Andersson, 1994), can favor larger males for
reasons of energetics (Andersson & Iwasa, 1996). In manta rays, larger males may be
more able to endure a long lasting mating-train consisting of rapid swimming, abrupt
turns, and somersaulting. This study showed that mating trains could last for more than
one day (see Chapter 2). Females selecting males based on endurance would likely select
those males capable of remaining with the train over time, and not simply by their
proximity to her within the train.
The absence of any observations of copulation make it difficult to know which males are
mating more often. Future work with genetic sampling would be beneficial in
deciphering paternity and could help to identify which traits may be contributing to male
reproductive success.
4.5.3 Conclusion
A winter breeding and birthing season exists in a resident population of manta rays off
Maui, Hawai‘i. Late maturation by females suggests that food resources are likely readily
available and predictable and that large body size is advantageous. Females, primarily
larger females, have the ability to give birth in consecutive years, but the energy
requirements are likely so great that most females will rest for one or more years between
pregnancies. Late maturation by males for larger body size also suggests a reproductive
advantage in males but how larger males are benefiting is unclear. Since direct physical
combats with other males do not occur, understanding the benefits of larger size needs
further study. The Maui aggregation area appears to be an important breeding area due to
the recurrence of the same reproductively active individuals across years. Combining
long-term field studies with the use of genetics to identify paternity and reproductive
success among males is suggested for future work in helping to improve our
understanding of resident manta ray reproductive ecology.
105
5 SUMMARY AND GENERAL DISCUSSION
Manta rays are one of the most susceptible marine taxa to population depletion from
fisheries exploitation (Dulvy et al., 2008; Dulvy & Reynolds, 2002), primarily due to
their life history characteristics of slow growth, late maturation, and low fecundity. The
size and status of manta populations globally are unknown. Manta rays are currently
classified by the IUCN Red List as Near Threatened (A. D. Marshall et al., 2006), but this
list does not currently differentiate between the larger, pelagic species (M. birostris), and
the smaller, coastal species (M. alfredi).
For management purposes, differentiating between oceanic and resident manta rays is
extremely important. Each species occupies a very different habitat and therefore may be
vulnerable to very different anthropogenic impacts. M. alfredi, which appears to consist
of small, geographically isolated populations, with little or no exchange of individuals
between populations, is more vulnerable to nearshore anthropogenic impacts such as
coastal development, storm water runoff, pollutant loadings, boat strikes, entanglement
in fishing and mooring lines, and increased pressure from “swim-with manta” programs.
Existing information on resident manta ray life history and ecology is severely limited
and much needed for proper management decisions. Data presented in this study
contribute new information on: (1) an effective method for measuring sizes of free-
ranging manta rays, (2) size demographic for a resident manta population, (3) an estimate
of population size and home range, (4) temporal use of an aggregation area, (5) existing
natural and anthropomorphic threats, (6) reproductive seasonality, and (7) the role of
body size in the reproductive success of females and males. Findings presented in this
paper are based on 6 years of research collected from 2005 through 2010 and provide a
broader understanding of resident manta ecology and behavior to better assist with
management of this species.
106
5.1 Using paired-laser photogrammetry as a simple and accurate system
to measure the body size of free-ranging manta rays (Manta alfredi)
In Chapter 2, paired-laser photogrammetry was shown to be a simple, accurate, and
precise remote measuring tool, providing a single diver with the ability to obtain a large
number of manta ray sizes quickly, while concurrently gathering information about the
individual’s identification, sex, age class, and behavioral role. Paired-laser
photogrammetry proved to be as, or more, accurate and precise than other reported
photogrammetry systems (Bergeron, 2007; Cosens & Blouw, 2003; Cubbage &
Calambokidis, 1987; Perryman & Lynn, 1993; S. S. Spitz et al., 2000).
Disc width (DW), the standard metric for measuring the size of rays, was not always
reliable for measuring free-ranging manta rays. Disc length measurements were much
more reliable and could be converted to DW using an empirically derived disc ratio (DR)
function for standardized comparisons with other studies.
Measurements on 154 different individual manta rays provided information about
maximum size and size at sexual maturity. Data collected indicate that resident manta
rays are sexually dimorphic in size, with females larger on average (mean = 3.18 m DW,
SD = 0.31) than males (mean = 2.83 m DW, SD = 0.14), and that size varies
geographically. The largest female in this Maui population was estimated at 3.64 m DW,
much smaller than 5.5 m DW for the largest female reported in southern Mozambique
(AD Marshall et al., 2009), or 4.3 m DW reported in Japan (Kashiwagi et al., 2008). The
largest male in this Maui population was estimated at 3.03 m DW, much smaller than the
largest male reported in Japan at 3.6 m DW (Kashiwagi et al., 2008).
Using pregnancy as an indicator of sexual maturity in females, a DW of 3.37 m was a
conservative estimate of the size at sexual maturity. Males appeared to achieve sexual
maturity between 2.75 and 2.80 m DW, at the time when their claspers grow rapidly and
extend beyond the margins of their pelvic fins.
107
Very small manta rays were not observed in the Maui aggregation area providing the first
evidence of age class segregation in resident manta rays. Newborn manta rays have been
reported between 1.1 – 1.5 m DW (Homma et al., 1999; AD Marshall et al., 2009). At the
Maui study site, no manta rays less than 2.5 m DW were observed. Manta rays have
never been observed giving birth in the wild, and birthing areas are unknown. Female
manta rays may retreat to more protected habitats to give birth, where pups may reside,
without parental care, until they reach a certain age or size. Age class segregation is
commonly reported in many shark species (Klimley, 1985; Pratt & Carrier, 2001) and
some mobulids (G. Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, 1988), and female sharks of several species
are known to move into protected, nursery areas to give birth (Bass, 1978; Springer,
1967). Pups remain in these areas during early development, presumably for protection
against predation.
By visiting areas where manta rays are known to aggregate, length measurements using
paired-laser photogrammetry can be obtained from a large part of the population in a
relatively short period of time. The ability to integrate individual identities and life
histories with morphometrics can be beneficial in longitudinal studies of growth. The
presence of fewer older and larger animals in the population can help to identify stock
depletion (Cubbage & Calambokidis, 1987). Population parameters such as growth and
survival rates, and age at first and last pregnancy can be obtained by measuring captive
and free-ranging manta rays of known age over time.
5.2 Characteristics of a manta ray (Manta alfredi) population off Maui,
Hawaii, and implications for management
Chapter 3 describes how photo-identification and active tracking were used to determine
population age and sex structure, abundance, home range, and temporal use of the Maui
aggregation area.
Findings indicated that more than 290 manta rays, consisting of nearly equal numbers of
males and females, occurred within the study area but that individuals moved in and out
108
of the area such that only a portion of the population was resident at any given time. Sizes
of resident manta ray populations in other parts of the world appear to vary widely with
reports ranging from 54 individuals in Yap Island, Micronesia, to 890 in Southern
Mozambique, to more than 2,000 reported in the Republic of the Maldives (C. R.
Anderson et al., 2008; T. B. Clark, 2001; Homma et al., 1999; A. D. Marshall, 2009).
Population sizes may reflect food availability of the various regions.
The study area appears to be an important staging area where individuals make routine,
year-round visits to rid themselves of parasites or find available mates. The
predominance of adult males compared to juvenile males and the high frequency of
mating trains observed indicate the study area is likely a significant mating area,
primarily between the months of December through April.
High resight rates within and across years provided strong evidence of site fidelity to the
study area. Evidence from two actively tracked individuals and photo-identifications
revealed that individuals in this population range throughout waters of the Maui County
region (Maui, Molokai, Lanai, and Kahoolawe), and appear to be geographically distinct
from a neighboring island population off Kona, Hawai‘i. Additional active tracking of
individuals combined with genetic sampling is needed to determine the extent of overlap
in these populations.
If island-associated resident manta ray populations are geographically independent, with
little genetic transfer occuring between populations, local management is needed to
address potential threats that may be unique to each region. Small, isolated populations
can be at serious risk of rapid and unrecoverable decline (Musick, 1999), and the frequent
occurrence of large aggregations of manta rays in a small area makes them more
vulnerable to localized anthropogenic impacts.
One of the greatest immediate concerns to this population is entanglement in
monofilament fishing line, which can result in disabling or dismembering the manta’s
cephalic fin. Alarmingly, 10% of individuals in this population have lost the use of one of
109
their cephalic fins. Such an injury is likely to impact an individual’s feeding efficiency
but to what extent requires further investigation. Additional potential threats facing this
population include: (1) unregulated swim with manta ray programs adding increased
pressure on animals utilizing this natural aggregation area, (2) increased boat strikes on
manta rays that frequently travel just below the surface, and (3) entanglement in proposed
mooring lines.
5.3 The reproductive ecology of manta rays (Manta alfredi) off Maui,
Hawai‘i, with an emphasis on body size
In Chapter 4, photo-identification and paired-laser photogrammetry were used to
investigate reproductive seasonality, and the role of body size in the reproductive success
of male and female resident manta rays off Maui, Hawai‘i.
This study revealed a reproductive season with mating trains and late-term pregnant
females observed more often during winter. The time of year when mating is observed in
other populations globally seems to vary according to region (C. R. Anderson et al.,
2008; T. B. Clark, 2001; Homma et al., 1999), and may reflect differences in temporal
food sources, and weather systems. Breeding seasons generally occur if it improves a
female’s chance for successful offspring, and is usually guided by a greater availability of
food resources, a reduction in predation, improved weather conditions, or a combination
of these. Most viviparous elasmobranchs follow annual reproductive cycles with
somewhat synchronous mating, gestation, and parturition (for review see Hamlett &
Koob, 1999).
Though mating activities were mostly observed during winter, females in this population
appear capable of ovulating any time of year and multiple times within a year. This was
supported by observations of the same females seen in mating trains in winter and
summer during the same year. If initial mating attempts were unsuccessful or aborted,
females may mate again outside the primary reproductive season. Although fewer
110
competing females may occur during the summer, the number of male mate choices may
also be reduced.
Males seem capable of detecting a female’s reproductive state through her bodily
excretions. This was supported by observations of males positioning themselves directly
behind females for brief periods, or turning abruptly to pass through her bodily
excretions. Elasmobranchs are known to have a very good sense of olfaction and taste
(Hodgson & Mathewson, 1978; Kleerekoper, 1978) and can use these senses to detect
biochemical products released by other organisms, including females trying to attract
potential mates (I. Gordon, 1993; Johnson & Nelson, 1978).
A nearly biennial pregnancy frequency estimated for female manta rays in this population
is consistent with information for many elasmobranchs, most likely to allow for post-
partum recovery and the rebuilding of reproductive reserves before mating again (Pratt &
Carrier, 2001). Based on a biennial mating cycle, the operational sex ratio (OSR) in this
population, appears skewed towards males with an estimated 2.7 adult males per
available female.
The demographics of a population affect the OSR, which can ultimately impact the
predominant mating system and the success of male tactics (Madsen & Shine, 1993). For
example when the OSR is skewed toward females, larger male body size seems to show
little advantage in reproductive success. Since many females are available, few male-to-
male combats occur, and smaller males receive mating opportunities. The males-biased
OSR found in the Maui population combined with females providing the only parental
care in the form of a 12-month gestation, would predict that males should compete for
access to limited numbers of reproductively available females as was observed within the
mating trains consisting of a single female followed by multiple males.
Delayed sexual maturity can indicate that the benefits of large body size outweigh the
cost of a reduced reproductive lifespan (Shine, 1988). Larger females tend to be more
fecund and produce larger, healthier offspring, while larger males, who compete
111
physically for access to mates, generally outcompete smaller males due to their size and
strength (Ralls & Mesnick, 2002). In this manta population, sexual maturity is delayed in
both males and females until their body size exceeds 90% of their maximum size
(Deakos, 2010), an indicator that large body size provides a reproductive advantage.
Larger females had a higher frequency of pregnancies and a greater likelihood of
reproducing in successive years. Among males, the reproductive advantage of large body
size was not clear, but the absence of observed combats in mating trains suggests that
males use strategies other than direct physical competition to gain access to mates.
Observations of escorts changing their position frequently in a mating train, sometimes
moving into the position directly behind the female, with no apparent conflict from other
males, suggest that position in the train may not be an indicator of male fitness. If females
select males based on their endurance, demonstrated by their ability to stay with her
mating train over long-durations, larger males may benefit from greater energy reserves
(Andersson & Iwasa, 1996), especially if males are unable to feed while in a mating train.
Sperm competition has been documented in some species of sharks (Carrier et al., 1994;
Pratt & Carrier, 2001), but does not seem to be an important mating strategy in this
population of manta rays since sperm competition is generally correlated with increasing
bouts of copulation (e.g., T Birkhead, 2000), and no copulations were observed during
the six years of this study.
5.4 SUMMARY
The information presented in this study broadens our understanding of the social behavior
and ecology of M. alfredi. These data describe a resident population of ranging
throughout Maui County waters, with most individuals showing residency to the main
study area off Maui, Hawai‘i, for cleaning and reproductive behaviors. Size data provided
evidence of sexual dimorphism, geographical variation in size, and reproductive benefits
of late maturation in M. alfredi. Reproductive data provided evidence of a mating season,
and a nearly biennial female reproductive cycle. Larger females were more fecund and
larger males may benefit more from endurance rivalry rather than direct physical
112
competition between males for limited reproductively available females. The location
where females are birthing is unknown but the absence of very small individuals at the
study area suggests newborns are segregated, possibly in shallow, protected areas where
they reside during early development. Tracking and photo-identification results suggest
that resident manta rays in Hawai‘i may consist of independent, island-associated stocks.
In many parts of the world, measures have been taken to reduce anthropogenic threats on
local manta ray populations. Some of these measures include: laws making it illegal to
kill or capture manta rays, the establishment of marine protected areas in certain parts of
the world where manta rays are known to aggregate, and the establishment of codes of
conduct for interacting with manta rays underwater. In areas subject to direct removal of
manta rays, limiting take numbers to the Potential Biological Removal (PBR) is highly
recommended in order to allow small, geographically isolated stocks to reach or maintain
their optimum sustainable population (Taylor et al., 2000). This requires knowledge of
the minimum population size and maximum population growth rate.
Future research should include active and passive tracking to better describe individual
home ranges, to locate birthing areas, to identify areas frequented that pose a high risk of
entanglement in monofilament line, and to determine the extent of overlap between
neighboring island populations. Genetic sampling of resident populations throughout the
Hawaiian Islands could help to identify which males have the greatest reproductive
success, and could further determine the extent of mixing occurring between populations.
113
LITERATURE CITED
Adams, J., Speakman, T., Zolman, E., & Schwacke, L. (2006). Automating image
matching, cataloging, and analysis for photo-identification research. Aquatic
Mammals, 32(3), 374-384.
Alava, M. N. R., Dolumbaló, E. R. Z., Yaptinchay, A. A., & Trono, R. B. (2002). Fishery
and trade of whale sharks and manta rays in the Bohol Sea, Philippines. Paper
presented at the Elasmobranch biodiversity, conservation and management:
proceedings of the international seminar and workshop, Sabah, Malaysia.
Anderson, C. R., Shiham, M. A., Joaquim, I. G., Kitchen-Wheeler, A., & Stevens, M. W.
(2008). Manta rays, Manta birostris, in the Maldives: seasonal distribution and
economic value Paper presented at the 2008 Joint Meeting of Ichthyologists and
Herpetologists, Montreal, Canada.
Anderson, R., Adam, M., Kitchen-Wheeler, A., & Stevens, G. (2011). Extent and
Economic Value of Manta Ray Watching in Maldives. Tourism in Marine
Environments, 7(1), 15-27.
Andersson, M. (1994). Sexual Selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Andersson, M., & Iwasa, Y. (1996). Sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution,
11(2), 53-58.
Andrews, K., Karczmarski, L., Au, W., Rickards, S., Vanderlip, C., & Toonen, R. (2006).
Patterns of genetic diversity of the Hawaiian spinner dolphin (Stenella
longirostris). Atoll Research Bulletin, 543, 65-73.
Anon. (1997). Fisheries Conservation Crisis in Indonesia: massive destruction of marine
mammals, sea turtles and fish reported from trap nets in pelagic migratory
channel.
Ari, C., & Correia, J. (2008). Role of sensory cues on food searching behavior of a
captive Manta birostris (Chondrichtyes, Mobulidae). Zoo Biology, 27(4).
Baird, R., Gorgone, A., McSweeney, D., Ligon, A., Deakos, M., Webster, D., . . .
Mahaffy, S. (2009). Population structure of island-associated dolphins: Evidence
114
from photo-identification of common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in
the main Hawaiian Islands. Marine Mammal Science, 25(2), 251-274.
Baker, C., & Herman, L. (1984). Aggressive behavior between humpback whales
(Megaptera novaeangliae) wintering in Hawaiian waters. Canadian Journal of
Zoology, 62(10), 1922-1937.
Bass, A. (1978). Problems in studies of sharks in the southwest Indian Ocean. Sensory
Biology of Sharks, Skates and Rays, 545-594.
Beebe, W., & Tee-Van, J. (1941). Manta hamiltoni (Newman). Zoological Journal of the
Linnean Society, 26, 274-278.
Bejder, L., Fletcher, D., & Bräger, S. (1998). A method for testing association patterns of
social animals. Animal Behaviour, 56(3), 719-725.
Bell, C., Hindell, M., & Burton, H. (1997). Estimation of body mass in the southern
elephant seal, Mirounga leonina, by photogrammetry and morphometrics. Marine
Mammal Science, 13(4), 669-682.
Bercovitch, F. (1989). Body size, sperm competition, and determinants of reproductive
success in male savanna baboons. Evolution, 43(7), 1507-1521.
Bergeron, P. (2007). Parallel lasers for remote measurements of morphological traits.
Journal of Wildlife Management, 71(1), 289-292.
Berglund, A. (1994). The operational sex ratio influences choosiness in a pipefish.
Behavioral Ecology, 5(3), 254.
Best, P., & Ruther, H. (1992). Aerial photogrammetry of southern right whales,
Eubalaena australis. Journal of Zoology, 228(4), 595-614.
Bigelow, H., & Schroeder, W. C. (1953). Fishes of the western North Atlantic.
Sawfishes, guitarfishes, skates, rays, and chimaeroids. Mem. Sears Found. Mar.
Res., 1, 500-514.
Birkhead, T. (2000). Promiscuity: An evolutionary history of sperm competition: Harvard
Univ Pr.
Birkhead, T., Briskie, J., & Miller, A. (1993). Male sperm reserves and copulation
frequency in birds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 32(2), 85-93.
115
Bleckmann, H., & Hofmann, M. (1999). Special senses. In W. C. Hamlett (Ed.), Sharks,
skates, and rays: The biology of elasmobranch fishes (pp. 300-328). Baltimore,
MD: The Johns Hopkins Univeristy Press.
Bratton, B., & Ayers, J. (1987). Observations on the electric organ discharge of two skate
species (Chondrichthyes: Rajidae) and its relationship to behaviour.
Environmental Biology Of Fishes, 20(4), 241-254.
Brault, S., & Caswell, H. (1993). Pod-specific demography of killer whales (Orcinus
orca). Ecology, 74(5), 1444-1454.
Bres, M. (1993). The behaviour of sharks. Reviews In Fish Biology And Fisheries, 3(2),
133-159.
Breuer, T., Robbins, M., & Boesch, C. (2007). Using photogrammetry and color scoring
to assess sexual dimorphism in wild western gorillas. American Journal of
Physical Anthropology, 134, 369-382.
Brownell , R. L., & Balls , K. (1986). Potential for sperm competition in baleen whales.
Report for the International Whaling Commmission, 8 (Special issue), 97 – 112.
Cadenat, J. (1958). Les diables de mer. Notes Africaines, 80, 116-120.
Calambokidis, J., Steiger, G., Evenson, J., Flynn, K., Balcomb, K., Claridge, D., . . .
Ziegesar, O. (1996). Interchange and isolation of humpback whales off California
and other North Pacific feeding grounds. Marine Mammal Science, 12(2), 215-
226.
Calambokidis, J., Steiger, G., Evenson, J., Flynn, K., Balcomb, K., Claridge, D., . . .
Ziegesar, O. (2006). Interchange and isolation of humpback whales off California
and other North Pacific feeding grounds. Marine Mammal Science, 12(2), 215-
226.
Carrier, J., Pratt Jr, H., & Martin, L. (1994). Group reproductive behaviors in free-living
nurse sharks, Ginglymostoma cirratum. Copeia, 646-656.
Castro, J. (1993). The shark nursery of Bulls Bay, South Carolina, with a review of the
shark nurseries of the southeastern coast of the United States. Environmental
Biology Of Fishes, 38(1), 37-48.
Clark, E. (1969). The lady and the sharks: Haper & Row Publishers.
116
Clark, S., Odell, D., & Lacinak, C. (2000). Aspects of growth in captive killer whales
(Orcinus orca). Marine Mammal Science, 16(1), 110-123.
Clark, T. B. (2001). Population Structure of Manta birostris (Chondrichthyes:
mobulidae) from the Pacific and Atlantic Oceans. MS thesis, Texas A&M
University, Galveston, Texas.
Clutton-Brock, T., Guinness, F., & Albon, S. (1982). Behaviour and ecology of two
sexes. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh University Press.
Clutton-Brock, T., & Vincent, A. (1991). Sexual selection and the potential reproductive
rates of males and females. Nature, 351(6321), 58-60.
Coles, R. J. (1916). Natural history notes on the devil fish, Manta birostris (Walbaum)
and Mobula ofersi (Muller). Bull. Amer. Mus. Nat. Hist., 35, 649-657.
Compagno, L. (1999). Systematics and body form. In W. C. Hamlett (Ed.), Sharks,
Skates and Rays: The Biology of Elasmobranch Fishes (pp. 1-42). Baltimore,
MD: John Hopkins University Press.
Compagno, L., Dando, M., & Fowler, S. (2005). Sharks of the world. Princeton, NJ and
Oxford, UK: Princeton University Press.
Compagno, L., & Last, P. (1999). Mobulidae. In K. E. Carpenter & V. H. Niem (Eds.),
FAO species identification guide for fishery purposes. The living marine
resources of the western Central Pacific. (Vol. 3. Batoid Fishes, Chimaeras and
Bony Fishes. Part 1 (Elopidae to Linophyrnidae)). Rome: FAO.
Compagno, L. J. V. (1984). FAO Species Catalogue. Vol. 4. Sharks of the world. An
annotated and Illustrated catalogue of shark species known to date. Part 2.
Carcharhiniformes. FAO Fish. Syn. 125: pp 251–655.
Compagno, L. J. V. (1999). Systematics and body form. In W. C. Hamlett (Ed.), Sharks,
skates, and rays: the biology of elasmobranch fishes (pp. 1-42). Baltimore: John
Hopkins University Press.
Compagno, L. J. V., Marshall, A. D., Kashiwagi, T., & Bennett, M. (2008). Manta
Systematics and Nomenclature Paper presented at the 2008 Joint Meeting of
Ichthyologists and Herpetologists, Montreal, Canada.
Corkeron, P., Morris, R., & Bryden, M. (1987). Interactions between bottlenose dolphins
and sharks in Moreton Bay, Queensland. Aquatic Mammals, 13(3), 109-113.
117
Cosens, S., & Blouw, A. (2003). Size-and age-class segregation of bowhead whales
summering in northern Foxe Basin: A photogrammetric analysis. Marine
Mammal Science, 19(2), 284-296.
Coté, I. (2000). Evolution and ecology of cleaning symbioses in the sea. Oceanography
and Marine Biology, 38, 311-355.
Cox, C., & Le Boeuf, B. (1977). Female incitation of male competition: a mechanism in
sexual selection. American Naturalist, 111(978), 317-335.
Cubbage, J., & Calambokidis, J. (1987). Size-class segregation of bowhead whales
discerned through aerial stereophotogrammetry. Marine Mammal Science, 3(2),
179-185.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (Vol. 1 & 2). New
York: Appleton.
Daw, B., & McGregor, F. (2008). Management of Manta Ray (M.birostris) Interactive
Tours in the Shallow Lagoonal Waters of Ningaloo Reef, Western Australia – A
Global Benchmark for Tourism Interactions Paper presented at the 2008 Joint
Meeting of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists, Montreal, Canada.
Dawson, S., Chessum, C., Hunt, P., & Slooten, E. (1995). An inexpensive,
stereophotographic technique to measure sperm whales from small boats. Report
of the International Whaling Commission, 45(43), 1-436.
de Rosemont, M. (2008). Observation and Sighting Description of the Manta Birostris, in
BoraBora Island (French Polynesia – South Pacific) Paper presented at the 2008
Joint Meeting of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists, Montreal, Canada.
Deakos, M. (2010). Paired-laser photogrammetry as a simple and accurate system for
measuring the body size of free-ranging manta rays Manta alfredi. Aquatic
Biology, 10(1), 1-10. doi: 10.3354/ab00258
Deakos, M., Baker, J., & Bejder, L. (submitted). Characteristics of a resident manta ray
(Manta alfredi) population off Maui, Hawaii, and Implications for management.
Marine Ecology Progress Series.
Dewar, H. (2002). Preliminary Report: Manta Harvest in Lamakera (p. 3): Report from
the Pfleger Institute of Environmental Research and the Nature Conservancy.
118
Dewar, H., Mous, P., Domeier, M., Muljadi, A., Pet, J., & Whitty, J. (2008). Movements
and site fidelity of the giant manta ray, Manta birostris, in the Komodo Marine
Park, Indonesia. Marine Biology, 155(2), 121-133.
Dulvy, N., Baum, J., Clarke, S., Compagno, L., Cortés, E., Domingo, A., . . . Gibson, C.
(2008). You can swim but you can't hide: the global status and conservation of
oceanic pelagic sharks and rays. Aquatic Conservation: Marine and Freshwater
Ecosystems, 18(5), 459-482.
Dulvy, N., & Reynolds, J. (2002). Predicting extinction vulnerability in skates.
Conservation Biology, 16(2), 440-450.
Durban, J., & Parsons, K. (2006). Laser-metrics of free-ranging killer whales. Marine
Mammal Science, 22(3), 735-743.
Emlen, S. (1976). Lek organization and mating strategies in the bullfrog. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology, 1(3), 283-313.
Emlen, S., & Oring, L. (1977). Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating
systems. Science, 197(4300), 215-223.
Fairbairn, D. (1997). Allometry for sexual size dimorphism: pattern and process in the
coevolution of body size in males and females. Annual Review of Ecology and
Systematics, 28(1), 659-687.
Fisher, R. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford, UK: Clarendon
Press.
Fowler, H. (1941). The George Vanderbilt Oahu Survey: The Fishes. Paper presented at
the Proceedings of the Academy of Natural Sciences of Philadelphia.
Francis, M. (2006). Morphometric minefields - towards a measurement standard for
chondrichthyan fishes. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 77(3), 407-421.
Frisk, M., Miller, T., & Fogarty, M. (2001). Estimation and analysis of biological
parameters in elasmobranch fishes: a comparative life history study. Canadian
Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences, 58(5), 969-981.
Ginis, L. (2002). Manta Ray. Australian Geographic, 40-64.
Gomendio , M., Harcourt , A. H., & Roldan , E. R. S. (1998). Sperm competition in
mammals. In T. H. Birkhead & A. P. Moller (Eds.), Sperm competition and sexual
selection (pp. 467–755).
119
Gordon, I. (1993). Pre-copulatory behaviour of captive sandtiger sharks, Carcharias
taurus. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 38(1), 159-164.
Gordon, J. (1990). A simple photographic technique for measuring the length of whales
from boats at sea. Report of the International Whaling Commission, 40, 581-588.
Graham, R., & Roberts, C. (2007). Assessing the size, growth rate and structure of a
seasonal population of whale sharks (Rhincodon typus Smith 1828) using
conventional tagging and photo identification. Fisheries Research, 84(1), 71-80.
Greene, C., Wiebe, P., Pelkie, C., Benfield, M., & Popp, J. (1998). Three-dimensional
acoustic visualization of zooplankton patchiness. Deep-Sea Research Part II,
45(7), 1201-1217.
Gruber, S., & Cohen, J. (1978). Visual system of the elasmobranchs: state of the art
1960-1975.
Hamlett, W., & Koob, T. (1999). Female reproductive system. In W. C. Hamlett (Ed.),
Sharks, Skates, and Rays: The Biology of Elasmobranch Fishes (pp. 398-443).
Baltimore, MD: The John Hopkins University Press.
Hart, N., Lisney, T., & Collin, S. (2006). Visual communication in elasmobranchs.
Communication in Fishes (Ladich F, Collin SP, Moller P, Kapoor BG, eds), 337-
392.
Harvey, P., & Harcourt, A. (1984). Sperm competition, testes size, and breeding systems
in primates. Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating systems, 18,
589-600.
Hays, G. (2003). A review of the adaptive significance and ecosystem consequences of
zooplankton diel vertical migrations. Hydrobiologia, 503(1), 163-170.
Heithaus, M. (2001). Shark attacks on bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) in Shark
Bay, Western Australia: attack rate, bite scar frequencies, and attack seasonality.
Marine Mammal Science, 17(3), 526-539.
Heithaus, M., Frid, A., & Dill, L. (2002). Shark-inflicted injury frequencies, escape
ability, and habitat use of green and loggerhead turtles. Marine Biology, 140(2),
229-236.
Herman, E., Herman, L., Pack, A., Marshall, G., Shepard, M., & Bakhtiari, M. (2007).
When Whales Collide: Crittercam Offers Insight into the Competitive Behavior of
120
Humpback Whales on Their Hawaiian Wintering Grounds. Marine Technology
Society Journal, 41(4), 35-43.
Hodgson, E., & Mathewson, R. (1978). Sensory biology of sharks, skates, and rays:
Office of Naval Research, Dept. of the Navy: for sale by the Supt. of Docs., US
Govt. Print. Off.
Hoenig, J., & Gruber, S. (1990). Life-history patterns in the elasmobranchs: implications
for fisheries management. NOAA Technical Report NMFS, 90, 1-16.
Hofmann, M. (1999). Nervous system. In W. C. Hamlett (Ed.), Sharks, Skates, and Rays:
The Biology of Elasmobranch Fishes (pp. 273-299). Baltimore and London: The
Johns Hopkins Univeristy Press.
Höglund, J., Montgomerie, R., & Widemo, F. (1993). Costs and consequences of
variation in the size of ruff leks. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 32(1), 31-
39.
Holden, M. J. (1974). Problems in the rational exploitation of elasmobranch populations
and some suggested solutions. In F. R. H. Jones (Ed.), Sea fisheries research (pp.
117-137). New york: Wiley and Sons.
Holmgren, N. (1940). Studies on the head in fishes. Acta Zoologica, 21, 51-267.
Homma, K., Maruyama, T., Itoh, T., Ishihara, H., & Uchida, S. (1999). Biology of the
manta ray, Manta birostris Walbaum, in the Indo-Pacific. Paper presented at the
Indo-Pacific fish biology: proceedings of the fifth international conference on
Indo-Pacific fishes, Noumea, 1997, France.
Johnson, R., & Nelson, D. (1978). Copulation and possible olfaction-mediated pair
formation in two species of carcharhinid sharks. Copeia, 1978(3), 539-542.
Jones, A., Hall, N., & Potter, I. (2008). Size compositions and reproductive biology of an
important bycatch shark species (Heterodontus portusjacksoni) in south-western
Australian waters. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the UK, 88(1),
189-197.
Kashiwagi, T., Ito, T., Ovenden, J., & Bennett, M. (2008). Population Characteristics of
Manta birostris Observed in Yaeyama, Okinawa, Japan, 1987-2006. Paper
presented at the 2008 Joint Meeting of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists,
Montreal, Canada.
121
Kitchen-Wheeler, A. (2008). Manta Rays: Research in the Maldives. Shark Focus, 31, 4.
Kitchen-Wheeler, A. (2010). Visual identification of individual manta ray (Manta alfredi)
in the Maldives Islands, Western Indian Ocean. Marine Biology Research, 6(4),
351-363.
Kitchen-Wheeler, A.-M. (2008). Migration Behaviour of the Giant Manta (Manta
birostris) in the Central Maldives Atolls Paper presented at the 2008 Joint
Meeting of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists, Montreal, Canada.
Kleerekoper, H. (1978). Chemoreception and its interaction with flow and light
perception in the locomotion and orientation of some elasmobranchs. Sensory
Biology of Sharks, Skates and Rays, 269-329.
Klimley, A. (1985). Schooling in Sphyrna lewini, a species with low risk of predation: a
non-egalitarian state. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 70(4), 297-319.
Klimley, A., & Brown, S. (1983). Stereophotography for the field biologist: measurement
of lengths and three-dimensional positions of free-swimming sharks. Marine
Biology, 74(2), 175-185.
Kvarnemo, C., & Ahnesjo, I. (1996). The dynamics of operational sex ratios and
competition for mates. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 11(10), 404-408.
Lalli, C., & Parsons, T. (1993). Biological oceanography: Pergamon Press.
Lamont, A. (1824). Notice of the colossal ray or skate. The Edinburgh Philosophical
Journal, 6(21), 113-118.
Langtimm, C., Beck, C., Edwards, H., Fick-Child, K., Ackerman, B., Barton, S., &
Hartley, W. (2004). Survival estimates for Florida manatees from the photo-
identification of individuals. Marine Mammal Science, 20(3), 438-463.
Lannoo, M. (1987). Neuromast topography in urodele amphibians. Journal Of
Morphology, 191(3), 247-263.
Last, P., & Stevens, J. (1994). Sharks and rays of Australia (2nd ed.). Melbourne,
Australia: CSIRO Publishing.
Lesueur, C. (1824). Description of Several Species of the Linnean Genus Raia, of North
America.
Losey Jr, G. (1972). The ecological importance of cleaning symbiosis. Copeia, 1972(4),
820-833.
122
Madsen, T., & Shine, R. (1993). Temporal variability in sexual selection acting on
reproductive tactics and body size in male snakes. American Naturalist, 141(1),
167-171.
Marshall, A., & Bennett, M. (2008). Cleaning Behaviour of a Photographically Identified
Population of Manta Rays in Southern Mozambique. Paper presented at the 2008
Joint Meeting of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists, Montreal, Canada.
Marshall, A., & Bennett, M. (2010). Reproductive ecology of the reef manta ray Manta
alfredi in southern Mozambique. Journal of Fish Biology, 77(1), 169-190.
Marshall, A., Compagno, L., & Bennett, M. (2009). Redescription of the genus Manta
with resurrection of Manta alfredi (Krefft,1868) (Chondrichthyes; Myliobatoidei;
Mobulidae). Zootaxa, 2301, 1–28.
Marshall, A. D. (2009). Biology and population ecology of Manta birostris in southern
Mozambique. PhD Thesis, University of Queensland.
Marshall, A. D., Ishihara, H., Dudley, S. F. J., Clark, T. B., Jorgensen, S., Smith, W. D.,
& Bizzarro, J. J. (2006). Manta birostris. In: IUCN 2006. from
http://www.iucnredlist.org
Marshall, A. D., Pierce, S. J., & Bennett, M. B. (2008). Morphological measurements of
manta rays (Manta birostris) with a description of a foetus from the east coast of
Southern Africa. Zootaxa, 1717, 24-30.
Marshall, L., White, W., & Potter, I. (2007). Reproductive biology and diet of the
southern fiddler ray, Trygonorrhina fasciata (Batoidea: Rhinobatidae), an
important trawl bycatch species. Marine and freshwater research, 58(1), 104-115.
McGregor, F., Van Keulen, M., Waite, A., & Meekan, M. (2008). Foraging Ecology and
Population Dynamics of the Manta Ray, Manta birostris in Lagoonal Waters of
Ningaloo Reef, Western Australia Paper presented at the 2008 Joint Meeting of
Ichthyologists and Herpetologists, Montreal, Canada.
McMahon, C., Burton, H., & Bester, M. (2000). Weaning mass and the future survival of
juvenile southern elephant seals, Mirounga leonina, at Macquarie Island.
Antarctic Science, 72(2), 749-753.
McSweeney, D., Baird, R., & Mahaffy, S. (2007). Site fidelity, associations, and
movements of Cuvier's (Ziphius cavirostris) and Blainville's (Mesoplodon
123
densirostris) beaked whales off the island of Hawai'i. Marine Mammal Science,
23(3), 666-687.
Mobley, J., Mazzuca, L., Craig, A., Newcomer, M., & Spitz, S. (2001). Killer whales
(Orcinus orca) sighted west of Niihau, Hawaii. Pacific Science, 55(3), 301-304.
Montgomery, J., & Skipworth, E. (1997). Detection of weak water jets by the short-tailed
stingray Dasyatis brevicaudata (Pisces: Dasyatidae). Copeia, 1997(4), 881-883.
Mueller, R., Brown, R., Hop, H., & Moulton, L. (2006). Video and acoustic camera
techniques for studying fish under ice: a review and comparison. Reviews in Fish
Biology and Fisheries, 16(2), 213-226.
Musick, J. (1999). Ecology and conservation of long-lived marine animals. American
Fisheries Society Symposium, 23, 1-10.
Myrberg, A. A. J. (1981). Sound communication and interception in fishes. In W. N.
Tavolgo, A. N. Popper & R. N. Fay (Eds.), Hearing and sound communication in
fishes. (pp. 397-425). New York, Heidelberg, and Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Nelson, D. (1967). Hearing thresholds, frequency discrimination, and acoustic orientation
in the lemon shark, Negaprion brevirostris (Poey). Bulletin of Marine Science,
17(3), 741-768.
Nelson, D., & Gruber, S. (1963). Sharks: attraction by low-frequency sounds. Science,
142, 975-977.
Nelson, J. (1984). Fishes of the world (Second ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Nishida, K. (1990). Phylogeny of the suborder Myliobatoidei. Memoirs of the Faculty of
Fisheries, 37, 1-108.
Northcutt, R. (1977). Elasmobranch central nervous system organization and its possible
evolutionary significance. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 17(2), 411.
Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, G. (1987). A revisionary study of the genus Mobula Rafinesque,
1810 (Chondrichthyes: Mobulidae) with the description of a new species.
Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society, 91, 1-91.
Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, G. (1988). Natural history notes of the rays of the genus Mobula
in the Gulf of California. Fisheries Bulletin, 86, 45-66.
Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, G. (1995). What future for manta rays? Shark News, 5, 1-2.
124
Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, G., & Hillyer, E. V. (1989). Mobulid rays off Eastern Venezuela
(Chondrichthyes, Mobulidae). Copeia(3), 607-614.
Pack, A., Herman, L., Spitz, S., Hakala, S., Deakos, M., & Herman, E. (2009). Male
humpback whales in the Hawaiian breeding grounds preferentially associate with
larger females. Animal Behaviour, 77(3), 653-662.
Parker, G. (1914). The directive influence of the sense of smell in the dogfish. Bull. US
Bur. Fish, 33, 61-68.
Parker, G. (1970). Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects.
Biol. Rev, 45, 525-567.
Paulin, C., Habib, G., Carey, C., Swanson, P., & Voss, G. (1982). New records of
Mobula japanica and Masturus lanceolatus, and further records of Luvaris
imperialis (Pisces: Mobulidae, Molidae, Louvaridae) from New Zealand. New
Zealand Journal of Marine and Freshwater Research, 16, 11-17.
Perryman, W., & Lynn, M. (1993). Identification of geographic forms of common
dolphin (Delphinus delphis) from aerial photogrammetry. Marine Mammal
Science, 9(2), 119-137.
Pillar, S., Armstrong, D., & Hutchings, L. (1989). Vertical migration, dispersal and
transport of Euphausia lucens in the southern Benguela Current. Marine Ecology
Progress Series, 53(2), 179-190.
Pratt, H., & Carrier, J. (2001). A review of elasmobranch reproductive behavior with a
case study on the nurse shark, Ginglymostoma cirratum. Environmental Biology
of Fishes, 60(1), 157-188.
Pratt, H., & Casey, J. (1990). Shark reproductive strategies as a limiting factor in directed
fisheries, with a review of Holdenís method of estimating growth parameters.
NOAA Technical Report, 90, 97-109.
Ralls, K., & Mesnick, S. (2002). Sexual dimorphism. In W. Perrin, B. Würsig & J.
Thewissen (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals (pp. 1071-1078). San Diego,
California: Academic Press.
Romanov, E. (2002). Bycatch in the tuna purse-seine fisheries of the western Indian
Ocean. Fishery Bulletin, 100(1), 90-105.
125
Rosenberger, L. (2001). Pectoral fin locomotion in batoid fishes: undulation versus
oscillation. Journal of Experimental Biology, 204(2), 379.
Rowat, D., Meekan, M., Engelhardt, U., Pardigon, B., & Vely, M. (2007). Aggregations
of juvenile whale sharks (Rhincodon typus) in the Gulf of Tadjoura, Djibouti.
Environmental Biology Of Fishes, 80(4), 465-472.
Rowe, L., & Dawson, S. (2008). Laser photogrammetry to determine dorsal fin size in a
population of bottlenose dolphins from Doubtful Sound, New Zealand. Australian
Journal of Zoology, 56(4), 239-248.
Rowe, S., & Hutchings, J. (2003). Mating systems and the conservation of commercially
exploited marine fish. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 18(11), 567-572.
Rubin, R. D. (2002). Manta Rays: not all black and white. Shark Focus, 15, 4-5.
Russell, K. (2008). Acquisition, Husbandry and Release Techniques for the Giant Manta
Ray (Manta birostris) At Atlantis Resort, Bahamas 2008 Joint Meeting of
Ichthyologists and Herpetologists, 123.
Rutherford, S., D'Hondt, S., & Prell, W. (1999). Environmental controls on the
geographic distribution of zooplankton diversity. Nature, 400(6746), 749-753.
Semeniuk, C., Bourgeon, S., Smith, S., & Rothley, K. (2009). Hematological differences
between stingrays at tourist and non-visited sites suggest physiological costs of
wildlife tourism. Biological Conservation, 142, 1818-1829.
Shen, X., Jia, F., & Zhou, J. (2001). Anti-tumor effect of the preparation extracted from
sea fish Manta birostris. Chinese Journal of Marine Drugs/Zhongguo Haiyang
Yaowu, 20(6), 35-39.
Shine, R. (1988). The evolution of large body size in females: a critique of Darwin's
"fecundity advantage" model. American Naturalist, 131(1), 124-131.
Simpfendorfer, C., & Milward, N. (1993). Utilisation of a tropical bay as a nursery area
by sharks of the families Carcharhinidae and Sphyrnidae. Environmental Biology
of Fishes, 37(4), 337-345.
Sims, D., Southall, E., Tarling, G., & Metcalfe, J. (2005). Habitat specific normal and
reverse diel vertical migration in the plankton feeding basking shark. Journal of
Animal Ecology, 74(4), 755-761.
126
Spitz, S., Herman, L., Pack, A., & Deakos, M. (2002). The relation of body size of male
humpback whales to their social roles on the Hawaiian winter grounds. Canadian
Journal of Zoology, 80(11), 1938-1947.
Spitz, S. S., Herman, L. M., & Pack, A. A. (2000). Measuring the size of humpback
whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) by underwater videogrammetry. Marine
Mammal Science, 16(3), 664-676.
Springer, S. (1967). Social organization of shark populations. In P. Gilbert, R.
Mathewson & D. Rall (Eds.), Sharks, Skates and Rays (pp. 149-174). Baltimore:
John Hopkins Press.
SPSS, & Inc. (2007). SPSS Statistics Base 17.0 User’s Guide. Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc.
Stevens, G., & Rubin, R. D. (2008). Reproductive Behaviour, Mating and Male
Competition in Manta Rays (Manta birostris) in the Indian Ocean Paper
presented at the 2008 Joint Meeting of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists,
Montreal, Canada.
Swaminathan, R., & Nayar, S. (1999). Polycameras: camera clusters for wide angle
imaging CUCS-013-99. Columbia University, New York, New York.
Taylor, B., Wade, P., De Master, D., & Barlow, J. (2000). Incorporating uncertainty into
management models for marine mammals. Conservation Biology, 14(5), 1243.
Thornhill, R., & Alcock, J. (1983). The evolution of insect mating systems: Harvard
University Press.
Tricas, T., Michael, S., & Sisneros, J. (1995). Electrosensory optimization to conspecific
phasic signals for mating. Neuroscience letters, 202(1-2), 129-132.
Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. Sexual selection and the
descent of man: the Darwinian pivot, 136.
Tyack, P., & Whitehead, H. (1982). Male competition in large groups of wintering
humpback whales. Behaviour, 83(1), 132-154.
Uchida, S. (1994). Manta Ray. Basic data for the Japanese threatened wild water
organisms (pp. 152-159). Tokyo, Japan: Fishery Agency of Japan.
Uchida, S., Toda, M., & Kamei, Y. (1990). Reproduction of elasmobranchs in captivity.
Elasmobranchs as Living Resources: Advances in the Biology, Ecology,
127
Systematics, and the Status of the Fisheries. Seatle, NOAA Technical Report
NMFS, 90, 211-237.
Uchida, S., Toda, M., & Matsumoto, Y. (2008). Captive Records of Manta Rays in
Okinawa Churaumi Aquarium Paper presented at the 2008 Joint Meeting of
Ichthyologists and Herpetologists, Montreal, Canada.
Visser, I. N., & Bonoccorso, F. J. (2003). New observations and a review of killer whale
(Orcinus orca) sightings in Papua New Guinea waters. Aquatic Mammals, 29(1),
150-172.
Wade, P. (1998). Calculating limits to the allowable human-caused mortality of cetaceans
and pinnipeds. Marine Mammal Science, 14(1), 1-37.
Waters, S., & Whitehead, H. (1990). Population and growth parameters of Galapagos
sperm whales estimated from length distributions. Reports of the International
Whaling Commission, 40, 225-235.
White, W., & Kyne, P. (2010). The status of chondrichthyan conservation in the Indo-
Australasian region. Journal of fish biology, 76(9), 2090-2117.
White, W. T., Giles, J., Dharmadi, & Potter, I. C. (2006). Data on the bycatch fishery and
reproductive biology of mobulid rays (Myliobatiformes) in Indonesia. Fisheries
Research, 82(1-3), 65-73.
Whitley, G. P. (1936). The Australian devil ray, Daemomanta alfredi (Krefft), with
remarks on the superfamily Mobuloidea (Order Batoidei). Australian zoologist, 8,
164-184.
Wiklund, C., & Karlsson, B. (1988). Sexual size dimorphism in relation to fecundity in
some Swedish satyrid butterflies. The American Naturalist, 131(1), 132-138.
Winemiller, K., & Rose, K. (1992). Patterns of life-history diversification in North
American fishes: implications for population regulation. Canadian Journal of
Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences, 49, 2196-2218.
Wolff, J. (1998). Breeding strategies, mate choice, and reproductive success in American
bison. Oikos, 83(3), 529-544.
Würsig, B., & Jefferson, T. (1990). Methods of photo-identification for small cetaceans.
Reports of the International Whaling Commission, 12, 43-52.
128
Yanagisawa, F. (1994). Record of the devli fish Manta birostris (Dondorff), caught in
Kumanonada, Japan. Nankiseibutsu (Nanki Biol.Sci.), 1(36), 73-74.
Yano, K., Sato, F., & Takahashi, T. (1999). Observations of mating behavior of the
manta ray, Manta birostris, at the Ogasawara Islands, Japan. Ichthyological
Research, 46(3), 289-296.
Yoshihara, K. (1997). A fish body length measuring method using an underwater video
camera in combination with laser discharge equipment. Fisheries Science
(Japan), 63(5), 676-680.
... When sampled from a mixed-age group and overlapping generations, as in this study, N e is an approximate estimate of the harmonic mean of the number of breeders (N b ) in the population over several generations [87,88]. There are not yet robust abundance estimates of reef manta ray populations in the Hawaiian Islands (see [9,89]), however minimum population size can be approximated using the photo-identification catalog sizes of 318 and 600 unique individuals for Hawaiʻi Island and Maui Nui, respectively. These minimum population sizes do not consider individuals that have died. ...
... Together with small population size, restricted gene flow, low genetic diversity, and conservative life history traits, this leaves reef manta rays at extremely high risk to human-induced perturbations. Reef manta rays have low intrinsic growth rates due to their delayed age-at-maturity (8-17 years for females) and low fecundity [9,28,89]. These extremely conservative life history traits are expected to severely restrict the potential for recovery from any potential population reductions in the future. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Reef manta rays ( Mobula alfredi ) are globally distributed in tropical and subtropical seas. Their life history traits (slow growth, late maturity, low reproductive output) make them vulnerable to perturbations and therefore require informed management strategies. Previous studies have reported wide-spread genetic connectivity along continental shelves suggesting high gene flow along continuous habitats spanning hundreds of kilometers. However, in the Hawaiian Islands, tagging and photo-identification evidence suggest island populations are isolated despite proximity, a hypothesis that has not yet been evaluated with genetic data. Results This island-resident hypothesis was tested by analyzing whole mitogenome haplotypes and 2048 nuclear single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) between M. alfredi (n = 38) on Hawaiʻi Island and Maui Nui (the 4-island complex of Maui, Molokaʻi, Lānaʻi and Kahoʻolawe). Strong divergence in the mitogenome ( Φ ST = 0.488) relative to nuclear genome-wide SNPs (neutral F ST = 0.003; outlier F ST = 0.186), and clustering of mitochondrial haplotypes among islands provides robust evidence that female reef manta rays are strongly philopatric and do not migrate between these two island groups. Combined with restricted male-mediated migration, equivalent to a single male moving between islands every 2.2 generations (~ 64 years), we provide evidence these populations are significantly demographically isolated. Estimates of contemporary effective population size ( N e ) are 104 (95% CI: 99–110) in Hawaiʻi Island and 129 (95% CI: 122–136) in Maui Nui. Conclusions Concordant with evidence from photo identification and tagging studies, these genetic results indicate reef manta rays in Hawaiʻi have small, genetically-isolated resident island populations. We hypothesize that due to the Island Mass Effect, large islands provide sufficient resources to support resident populations, thereby making crossing deep channels separating island groups unnecessary. Small effective population size, low genetic diversity, and k-selected life history traits make these isolated populations vulnerable to region-specific anthropogenic threats, which include entanglement, boat strikes, and habitat degradation. The long-term persistence of reef manta rays in the Hawaiian Islands will require island-specific management strategies.
... Social recognition may be important in mate choice (Marshall and Bennett 2010). Manta rays perform group-based behaviors including collective foraging, following, breaching, copying, play, and curiosity towards humans (Marshall 2008;Deakos 2010;Gadig and Neto 2014;RJYP pers. obs.), which are associated with social functions and reminiscent of highly social marine mammals (Bradbury 1986). ...
... Though several studies have provided anecdotal evidence of social behavior in manta rays (Deakos 2010;Stevens 2016;Stewart et al. 2016bStewart et al. , 2018, this is the first study to provide a quantitative description of their social organization. Our aims were to describe the temporal and spatial structure of social relations and determine whether manta rays had genuine social preferences (caused by active choice of individuals to interact) by controlling for non-social structural factors, including location, time, phenotype, and individual gregariousness. ...
Article
Full-text available
Understanding how individual behavior shapes the structure and ecology of populations is key to species conservation and management. Like many elasmobranchs, manta rays are highly mobile and wide-ranging species threatened by anthropogenic impacts. In shallow water environments, these pelagic rays often form groups and perform several apparently socially mediated behaviors. Group structures may result from active choices of individual rays to interact or passive processes. Social behavior is known to affect spatial ecology in other elasmobranchs, but this is the first study providing quantitative evidence for structured social relationships in manta rays. To construct social networks, we collected data from more than 500 groups of reef manta rays (Mobula alfredi) over 5 years in the Raja Ampat Regency of West Papua. We used generalized affiliation indices to isolate social preferences from non-social associations, the first study on elasmobranchs to use this method. Longer lasting social preferences were detected mostly between female rays. We detected assortment of social relations by phenotype and variation in social strategies, with the overall social network divided into two main communities. Overall network structure was characteristic of a dynamic fission-fusion society, with differentiated relationships linked to strong fidelity to cleaning station sites. Our results suggest that fine-scale conservation measures will be useful in protecting social groups of M. alfredi in their natural habitats and that a more complete understanding of the social nature of manta rays will help predict population responses to anthropogenic pressures, such as increasing disturbance from dive tourism. Significance statement In social animals, relationships between individuals have important implications for species conservation. Like many other sharks and rays, manta rays are threatened species, and little is known about their natural behavior or how their populations are structured. This study provides evidence of social structure in a wild, free-ranging population of reef manta rays. We show for the first time that individual manta rays have preferred relationships with others that are maintained over time, and structured societies. This study extends our knowledge of elasmobranch ecology and population structuring. Results suggest that understanding social relationships in manta rays will be important in protecting populations from human impacts and developing sustainable, localized conservation and management initiatives.
... Our understanding of the reproductive behaviors of manta rays in their natural habitats remains considerably constrained. Courtship behaviors, a crucial aspect of their reproductive strategies, have been regularly observed in various regions, with the majority of observations involving M. alfredi and sparingly for M. birostris [4,[6][7][8][9][10][11][12]. While studies have reported the presence of fresh mating scars (caused by the male manta ray biting the left wing tip of the female manta ray to gain leverage before initiating copulation [11]) as indicative evidence of mating events, documented observations of mating events, including copulation, remain extremely rare. ...
Article
Full-text available
Though a previous study in Sudan confirmed the existence of a living hybrid of an oceanic manta ray (Mobula birostris) and a reef manta ray (M. alfredi), courtship behaviors between the two closely related species have never been documented. Here we report the first observation of courtship behaviors between two male M. birostris and a female M. alfredi at a manta ray cleaning station on a shallow coral reef seamount in Misool, Raja Ampat, Indonesia. The observed courtship event lasted for approximately 45 min and involved one male oceanic manta actively chasing the mature reef manta female around the cleaning station and repeatedly bumping her on the left wing tip with his right cephalic lobe, while the second male continuously flanked the female to seemingly prevent her retreat from the cleaning station and potential evasion of the pursuing male. Notably, the female exhibited a rapid coloration change, likely indicative of courtship-related stress, transitioning from the striking pale hue she exhibited throughout the courtship activity to a normal chevron pattern once the males departed.
... Reef manta rays (Mobula alfredi) are mobile pelagic rays that aggregate in surface waters of coastal regions during diel activities such as feeding, cleaning and courtship (Deakos 2010;Stevens 2016, Perryman et al. 2019. This makes them highly amenable to aerial monitoring but also vulnerable to anthropogenic impacts such as illegal fishing (Stewart et al. 2018) and unregulated dive tourism (Venables et al. 2016). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Research on free-ranging species of marine megafauna is required to understand their behavioural ecology, including how groups respond to environmental and anthropogenic pressures. New technologies are opening up potential for detailed observational research on these species in the wild, especially on group-based and collective behaviours. Reef manta rays (Mobula alfredi) are socially interactive elasmobranchs that form groups in coastal reef habitats. Collective behaviours are likely important to their fitness, but may be disturbed by humans. Using small, remotely-piloted drones, we performed aerial observations of manta ray groups in Raja Ampat, West Papua. We empirically quantified patterns of collective movement including relative spatial positions, alignment, speed and leadership positions of conspecifics. We found unique patterns of spatial positioning, alignment and leadership, including differences between sexes, and high levels of local attraction, which were suggestive of distinct collective behaviour states. We suggest that rules of interaction in manta rays vary at the individual level and can shift depending on local environmental and social conditions. Leader-follower behaviour likely has broad importance to cohesive movement and social behaviour in M. alfredi. We suggest that further studies on manta ray movement should consider utilising models of collective motion that capture group-level behavioural processes.
... The behavior is rarely observed and remains poorly studied, but the prevalence of breaches in Mobulidae indicates that it has an adaptive benefit (Rayner 1986, Homma et al. 1999. Various hypotheses have proposed that the breaches may be connected to courtship (Marshall & Bennett 2010, Stevens et al. 2018, parturition, removal of remoras and/or parasites, play, feeding, and/or communication (Rayner 1986, Homma et al. 1999, Deakos 2010, Medeiros et al. 2015, Stevens 2016, while it is possible that this behavior may have multiple functions. ...
Article
ABSTRACT: Improving our knowledge on the behavior of threatened species is essential for developing effective conservation actions. The Paranaguá Estuarine Complex (PEC), southern Brazil, is the only estuary in the world where manta rays have been observed performing breaches seasonally. The exact role of this breaching behavior and the environmental factors connected to it are unknown. Our goals were to determine the spatial distribution, and the temporal and environmental factors that influence the breaching behavior of this endangered group in a dynamic estuarine habitat for the first time. Manta rays were observed breaching in the PEC during austral summer and early autumn, when the sea surface temperature (SST) and precipitation were high. Generalized additive models revealed that the presence and frequency of the breaches were both influenced by the SST and hours of daily effort, while the breaching frequency was also influenced by the wind direction and speed, percentage of moon illumination, and year. The breaches were mainly concentrated near the mouth of a river. Likely these factors influenced not only the occurrence and behavior of manta rays, but also the distribution of their food source, potentially providing optimal conditions for foraging and reproduction. Based on the coloration pattern, it is possible that the observations were of Mobula cf. birostris. These results provide valuable insights into the breaching behavior of manta rays in estuarine waters that will assist future conservation initiatives and research on their behavioral ecology, to optimize fishery management and contribute to developing sustainable ecotourism in the PEC.
... According [9,13], they live for about 50-100 years and reach sexual maturity at an age of 10-25 years. Figure 1 shows Mobula alfredi, which grows up to an average wingspan of 3 m to 4.5 m with a weight of around 1.4 tones [7]. Mobula birostris is larger with a wingspan of up to 7 m and weight of up to 2 tonnes [10,14]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Manta rays are flagship species for marine conservation because of a number of threats including anthropogenic, overfishing, plastics (microplastics), over tourism, commercial trade (gills for medicine), and chaotic shipping lines where they often injured or killed. Because of these reasons, manta ray face risk of extinction and listed on the Red List of IUCN. A number of studies present the value of this fish estimated millions of dollars per year from tourism which show much greater valuable alive than dead. Responsible manta ray tourism encourages stakeholders to protect the species by generating incentives from tourism while develop conservations initiatives to protect the species. Desk study on current literatures were reviewed to identify the role of stakeholders in supporting the sustainable management of manta ray tourism. This chapter explored the operations of manta ray tourism in Indonesia as the study areas. In summary, to reach the positive contributions from manta ray tourism, there is an important role of co-management between stakeholders to ensure the sustainable operations and conservation of the ecology, economy, and socio-culture.
... Unlike most spindle-shaped fish, the body of a manta ray is more similar to a giant marine kite. The most typical features of the manta ray's body are the two large triangular pectoral fins, the wide but short mouth with two head fins, and the tail, which has a smaller triangular-shaped caudal fin but no dorsal fins [1][2][3][4]. In addition, the distance between the two pectoral fins of the manta ray is greater than its body length. ...
Article
Full-text available
A manta ray biomimetic glider is designed and studied with both laboratory experiments and numerical simulations with a new dynamic update method called the motion-based zonal mesh update method (MBZMU method) to reveal its hydrodynamic performance. Regarding the experimental study, an ejection gliding experiment is conducted for qualitative verification, and a hydrostatic free-fall experiment is conducted to quantitatively verify the reliability of the corresponding numerical simulation. Regarding the numerical simulation, to reduce the trend of nose-up movement and to obtain a long lasting and stable gliding motion, a series of cases with the center of mass offset forward by different distances and different initial angles of attack have been calculated. The results show that the glider will show the optimal gliding performance when the center of mass is 20mm in front of the center of geometry and the initial attack angle range lies between A 0 = -5° to A 0 = -2.5° at the same time. The optimal gliding distance can reach six times its body length under these circumstances. Furthermore, the stability of the glider is explained from the perspective of Blended-Wing-Body (BWB) configuration.
... Approaches from above were also significantly more likely to cause cessation of feeding entirely. Manta rays are unable to see directly above and behind, while their downward, lateral and forward vision is good (Deakos, 2010). Therefore, they are more likely to feel threatened and react adversely to an unidentified object approaching from these directions, hence it is recommended to approach from the side to avoid startling the animal. ...
Article
Full-text available
Manta ray tourism is estimated to contribute US$ 140 million annually to the global economy. The multitudes of tourists potentially disturb the animals, yet the effect of human behaviour on feeding manta rays has not been quantified. Using videos collected at feeding sites in the Maldives, we found that only 44% of observed human-manta interactions complied with existing guidelines. Human behaviours; accidental obstruction, diving too near/in front, chasing, and approaching from the front, all had a statistically significant negative effect on behaviour, resulting in feeding cessation, while passive interactions resulted in significantly less disturbance and cessation reactions. Interactions within three metres caused significantly increased avoidance behaviours. These findings support the guidelines provided by the Manta Trusts’ code of conduct, which aims to develop legislation to minimise disturbance by tourism. Our key recommendations aim to ensure that the manta ray tourism industry remains sustainable and non-detrimental to the animals’ natural behaviour. Visit https://swimwithmantas.org/ for video abstract.
Article
Full-text available
The Bird's Head Seascape (BHS) in West Papua, Indonesia, is widely recognized as the global epicenter of coral reef biodiversity and is protected by an extensive network of 20 marine protected areas (MPAs) totaling over 4.7 million ha. It is home to large populations of both the reef manta ray Mobula alfredi (Krefft, 1868) and the oceanic manta Mobula birostris (Walbaum, 1792). We document the natural history of manta rays in the BHS and describe the demographics and spatial ecology of Mobula alfredi using underwater and aerial observations, a comprehensive photo-ID database, and passive acoustic telemetry. Manta rays were recorded from 127 sites across the BHS, including 70 aggregation sites (cleaning stations and routine feeding aggregations), with the largest feeding aggregation recorded consisting of 112 M. alfredi in the Dampier Strait in the Raja Ampat archipelago. We recorded 4,052 photographically identified M. alfredi sightings of 1,375 individuals between November 2004 and December 2019, with a biased female-to-male sex ratio of 1.58 to 1.0 and 67.4% exhibiting the chevron color morph vs. 32.6% melanistic. Over 85% of sightings came from the two large MPAs (>330,000 ha) of South East Misool and Dampier Strait. Importantly, 16 photo-IDs of somersault-feeding individuals were obtained using a drone, apparently the first report of UAVs used for manta photo-IDs. We resighted 642 individuals (46.7%) at least once during the period, with the two most-resighted individuals registering 67 and 66 resightings over periods of about 12 years. We observed 217 females pregnant at least once, with one having 4 consecutive pregnancies from 2013-16 (and a total of 5 pregnancies in 7 years) and 15 with at least two consecutive-year pregnancies. Four nursery sites were identified with a consistent presence of numerous young-of-the-year (YoY; i.e. ≤2 m disc width) over 3-14 years of observations: we recorded 65 YoYfrom Raja Ampat. The Raja Ampat population is best described as a metapopulation composed of 4-7 subpopulations inhabiting island groups separated by over-water distances of only 20-40 km, but which nonetheless exhibit limited exchange of individuals. We recorded 309 movement events among 7 hypothesized manta subpopulations in Raja Ampat based on photo-IDs between 2004 and 2019 and passive acoustic telemetry between 2013 and 2019, with the longest movement we recorded 296 km minimum distance through water. Importantly, 115 of the identified manta ray sites (90.5%) are distributed within 13 of the 20 BHS MPAs, and 95.9% of sightings (3,887 of 4,052), 89.5% of individuals (1,231 of 1,375) and all 4 identified nursery areas were from within MPAs in Raja Ampat, indicating the Raja Ampat MPA network, and the broader BHS MPA network within which it is nested, are critical for the conservation of manta rays in West Papua.
Article
Full-text available
In French Polynesia, both currently recognized manta ray species, Mobula alfredi and M. birostris, are observed. Despite being an important cultural asset and generating significant economic benefits through manta ray watching tourism, published data on the ecology and threats to these species in the region are scarce. Based on an 18-year dataset of sighting records collected by citizen scientists and during two scientific expeditions, this study provides the first insights into the population characteristics and regional distribution of the two manta ray species in French Polynesia. A total of 1347 manta ray photographs (1337 for M. alfredi and 10 for M. birostris) were examined for the period January 2001–December 2017, with photo-identification techniques leading to the successful identification of 317 individual M. alfredi and 10 individual M. birostris throughout the Society, Tuamotu and Marquesas Islands. We provide the first confirmation of sympatric distribution of both species in the Society Islands. Our results highlight strong and long-term site fidelity of M. alfredi individuals to certain aggregation sites (> 9 years for 16 individuals) and reveal some degree of connectivity between populations, with 10 individuals recorded moving between islands located up to 50 km apart. Analysis of photographs of individuals bearing sub-lethal injuries (n = 68) suggests that M. alfredi are more likely to be injured at inhabited islands (Maupiti or Bora Bora; 75% of all injured individuals) than at uninhabited islands, with 75% of injuries related to boat propeller strikes and fishing gear entanglements. Our findings emphasize the need for further research to allow for a comprehensive evaluation of population structure, size and threats to manta rays in this region.
Article
Full-text available
ABSTRACT An historical examination of shark fisheries characterizes them as "boom and bust" enterprises. The reproductive strategies of elasmobranchs -e.g., a small number (2-135) of large young (100-1000 mm) are born after a gestation period of 0.5 to 2.0 years, and first maturity follows at a late age-require a long time for stocks to recover from the effects of directed fisheries. The rational exploitation of shark populations can be accomplished only through careful management. A review of the reproductive and growth parameters of shark species is undertaken which can be used to indicate species vulnerability to directed fisheries. A comparison of historical predictions of K values with recent empirically derived K values indicate that past methods cannot be applied universally and may not work at all. Future research to assess the potential of elasmobranch fisheries should address early life-history requirements, minimum reproductive biomass, and should examine the adaptations (if any) of elasmobranch fecundity and natural mortality to fluctuations in stock abundance and food supply.
Article
Full-text available
Killer whales live in stable social groups, called @'pods.@' It has been suggested that the structure of such groups may influence the vital rates, and hence the fitness, of their members. To test this suggestion we used data from a long-term study of killer whales in the Pacific Northwest (Bigg et al. 1990). We constructed stage-classified matrix population models for the entire population, two sub-populations, and individuals pods. The population growth rate for the entire population is @l = 1.0254, with 90% bootstrap confidence interval from 1.0178 to 1.0322. The mean female population stage distribution is not significantly different from the predicted stable stage distribution. Population growth rate is most sensitive to changes in adult and juvenile survival, followed by fertility Factors that cause even small changes in survival will thus have a large impact on population growth. Pod-specific growth rates range from @l = 0.9949 to @l = 1.0498. Most of the interpod variance in growth rate is due to variance in adult reproductive output. Randomization tests show that this variance is not significantly greater than expected on the basis of variation in individual life histories within the population. We conclude that there is no evidence for an effect of social structure on pod-specific population growth rate. The restriction of population growth rates to such a narrow range suggests, but does not prove, a possible role of density-dependent processes.
Article
The author has developed a device and a method to measure the body length of fish from played back images that reflect laser beams recorded on videotape by an underwater video camera with laser discharge equipment attached, and examined the usefulness of this measuring method through experimentation with dummies in a large water tank on land and in the field. The distances at which the laser spots could be recognized on various colors were, in ascending order, blue, as the lowest, followed by gray, yellow, white, red, and filefish skin; when a distance from the camera exceeds 2.5m, significant errors were observed. In the field experimentation, the measurement was made using a device for the body length of kokanee salmons, Onchoryncus nerka, coming upstream in an artificial river, and it was compared with that of body length of salmons caught by a trap. Because the mode of both body length distribution coincided, it was confirmed that the usefulness of the measuring method of the body length by the underwater video camera with a laser beam discharge equipment attached. However, its use was limited to conditions of clear water and short distance to fish.
Article
Mobula thurstoni was the most abundant (58% of the catch), followed by M. japanica (30%), M. munkiana (9%), and M. tarapacana (3%). The study area served as a nursery ground for M. thurstoni, a summer feeding and mating ground for M. thurstoni and M. japanica, and a wintering ground for M. munkiana and young M. thurstoni. Data on size, weight, sex ratio, life history, seasonality, feeding habits, behavior, habitat, and symbionts are presented. Size segregation was a common feature of M. thurstoni, M. japanica and M. munkiana. Summer prey were almost exclusively the euphausiid Nyctiphanes simplex; the mysid Mysidium sp. dominated in winter. -from Author
Article
One component of sexual selection is sperm competition. It has been reasoned that the intensity of sperm competition may be reflected in the relative testicular sizes of animals. Among males residing in multimale breeding systems, testicular size is relatively larger than among males residing in unimale mating systems. Information on whether differences in testicular size within a species can account for differences in male reproductive success is unavailable for natural populations of primates. A population of six troops of savanna baboons in Kenya was surveyed for morphometric analysis, and one of these troops was the subject of extensive behavioral observations afterwards. Testicular weights could not be obtained, but measurements of linear dimensions were transformed into volumetric estimates. Male weight accounted for 30% of the variance in testicular volume. Neither body size nor testicular volume was associated with differences in male reproductive activity. The outcome of fights over access to females could not be related to male body size, and ejaculatory patterns of males were independent of testicle size. Both sperm competition and aggressive competition intensified during the four-day optimum conception period, but fights over access to consort females were infrequent. Among savanna baboons, the probability of an ejaculation resulting in a conception is fairly low, which may account for the infrequency of injurious fights. Although testicle size influences sperm production, it does not influence either the timing of mating or the fertilizing capacity of spermatozoa, and both of these factors probably account for a substantial fraction of the variance in male baboon paternity. Sperm competition is an adjunct to agonistic competition as a mechanism affecting male baboon reproductive success. It is concluded that male reproductive success in baboons is affected more by social factors than by morphological traits associated with size.