ArticlePDF Available

Are More Similar than Different Meta-Analyses of Sex Differences in Responses to Sexual Versus Emotional Infidelity : Men and Women

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

From 54 articles, 172 effect sizes were meta-analyzed to determine whether men and women are differentially distressed by emotional versus sexual infidelity. Predictions were derived and tested from an evolutionary psychology (EP) perspective, a social–cognitive perspective, and the double-shot perspective. The data were not consistent with the EP predictions because men tended to respond in the predicted manner in only the U.S. student samples, whereas the rest of the data were largely consistent with the social–cognitive theory. Specifically, both sexes tended to be more upset by emotional than sexual infidelity when forced to choose which type of infidelity was more distressing. Both sexes indicated that sexual infidelity was more distressing than emotional when asked to rate their level of distress separately for each using continuous measures. The lesbian and gay samples were mostly consistent with the double-shot hypothesis because they tended to respond based on stereotyping grounded in the sex of their partner, paralleling heterosexuals in this regard. Analysis of the scenarios designed to test the double-shot hypothesis found somewhat smaller effects when the possibility of both types of infidelity was ruled out. These findings suggest that professionals seeking to address problems associated with jealousy in romantic relationships would profit from avoiding sex-linked assumptions about which aspect of infidelity is likely to be more upsetting.
Content may be subject to copyright.
http://pwq.sagepub.com/
Psychology of Women Quarterly
http://pwq.sagepub.com/content/36/1/25
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/0361684311414537
2012 36: 25 originally published online 23 November 2011Psychology of Women Quarterly Christopher J. Carpenter
Are More Similar than Different
Meta-Analyses of Sex Differences in Responses to Sexual Versus Emotional Infidelity : Men and Women
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Society for the Psychology of Women
can be found at:Psychology of Women QuarterlyAdditional services and information for
http://pwq.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://pwq.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
What is This?
- Nov 23, 2011OnlineFirst Version of Record
- Feb 28, 2012Version of Record >>
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Research Articles
Meta-Analyses of Sex Differences in
Responses to Sexual Versus Emotional
Infidelity: Men and Women Are More
Similar than Different
Christopher J. Carpenter
1
Abstract
From 54 articles, 172 effect sizes were meta-analyzed to determine whether men and women are differentially distressed by
emotional versus sexual infidelity. Predictions were derived and tested from an evolutionary psychology (EP) perspective, a
social–cognitive perspective, and the double-shot perspective. The data were not consistent with the EP predictions because
men tended to respond in the predicted manner in only the U.S. student samples, whereas the rest of the data were largely
consistent with the social–cognitive theory. Specifically, both sexes tended to be more upset by emotional than sexual infide-
lity when forced to choose which type of infidelity was more distressing. Both sexes indicated that sexual infidelity was more
distressing than emotional when asked to rate their level of distress separately for each using continuous measures. The les-
bian and gay samples were mostly consistent with the double-shot hypothesis because they tended to respond based on
stereotyping grounded in the sex of their partner, paralleling heterosexuals in this regard. Analysis of the scenarios designed
to test the double-shot hypothesis found somewhat smaller effects when the possibility of both types of infidelity was ruled
out. These findings suggest that professionals seeking to address problems associated with jealousy in romantic relationships
would profit from avoiding sex-linked assumptions about which aspect of infidelity is likely to be more upsetting.
Keywords
infidelity, theory of evolution, social cognition, social perception, sexual attitudes, sexual orientation, sexual partners, human
sex differences
A controversy has developed in the romantic jealousy litera-
ture concerning sex differences in response to different types
of infidelity (Buss, Larsen, Westen, & Semmelroth, 1992;
Harris, 2003a; Sagarin, 2005). Sexual infidelity occurs in
situations such that one partner has sexual relations outside
her or his primary romantic relationship. Emotional infidelity
occurs when one partner develops an intense emotional
attachment outside her or his primary romantic relationship.
Some evolutionary psychologists (Buss et al., 1992) have
argued that men are likely to be more upset if they discover
their partner has engaged in sexual infidelity than if they dis-
cover their partner has engaged in emotional. They also
argued that women are likely to be more upset if they dis-
cover that their partner has engaged in emotional infidelity
than sexual. Other researchers working from a social–cogni-
tive perspective have argued that such sex differences do not
exist (Harris, 2003a) or that they can be explained by prevail-
ing stereotypes about the co-occurrence of the two types of
infidelity at once (DeSteno & Salovey, 1996).
Resolving this issue is of importance to social scientists
for several reasons. First, Cano and O’Leary (1997) argued
that jealousy is an important emotion to address in couple’s
therapy. The different reactions that men and women have
to jealousy based on different types of infidelity can have
an important impact on how therapists should seek to address
jealousy in their sessions. Furthermore, this issue has become
an important area of controversy regarding whether or not
sex differences are rooted more in biology or socialization
(Harris, 2003a). Several researchers have singled this predic-
tion out as one of the most compelling demonstrations of the
heuristic value of theories derived from an evolutionary
psychology (EP) perspective (Goetz & Shackelford, 2006;
Sesardic, 2003). Before presenting the results of a series of
meta-analyses of the research in this area, I will review the
varying perspectives concerning this issue.
1
Department of Communication, Western Illinois University, Macomb, IL,
USA
Corresponding Author:
Christopher J. Carpenter, Department of Communication, Western Illinois
University, 318 Memorial Hall #1 University Circle, Macomb, IL 61455, USA
Email: cj-carpenter2@wiu.edu
Psychology of Women Quarterly
36(1) 25-37
ªThe Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0361684311414537
http://pwq.sagepub.com
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
EP Perspective
Buss (2000) explained that one of the fundamental postulates
of EP is that men and women have always faced different
challenges associated with reproduction. This theorizing
focuses on the reproductive challenges of heterosexual cou-
ples. These separate problems produced different solutions
as humans adapted to their environment. Specifically, men
were predicted to be more upset at the discovery of their
mate’s extradyadic sexuality than their mate’s extradyadic
emotional involvement. Women were predicted to be more
upset over the discovery of their mate’s extradyadic emo-
tional involvement than their mate’s extradyadic sexuality.
Buss, Larsen, Westen, and Semmelroth (1992) suggested
that this difference stems from evolutionary pressures. A
man, they pointed out, could never be certain if he is the bio-
logical father of his mate’s child during the long period of
human evolution. Clearly, a man can get genetic testing now,
but from this perspective, jealousy is thought to be based on
the human male’s biological characteristics that were devel-
oped long before genetic testing. If his mate does not birth his
children, his genes will not be passed on; therefore, any
genetic variation that helps men prevent other men from hav-
ing sex with his mate will be selected. Buss et al. claimed that
sexual jealousy evolved to fill that need.
According to Buss et al. (1992), women face a different
reproductive problem. Whenever a woman bears a child, she
is absolutely certain that her genetic material will be passed
on in that child so that the sexual faithfulness of her mate
is irrelevant. However, if her mate becomes emotionally
attached to another woman, he may decide to devote his
resources to the rearing of that woman’s child rather than her
own. The loss of her mate’s resources may significantly
reduce the survival of, and eventual reproduction by, her
genetic child. Because of this problem, Buss et al. suggested
that women were genetically predisposed to be motivated
to prevent their mate from becoming emotionally attached
to other women. They noted that ‘‘cues to the development
of a deep emotional attachment have been reliable leading
indicators to women of potential reduction or loss of their
mate’s investment’’ (p. 251). The EP perspective thus pro-
posed that men would be more upset if their partner engaged
in sexual infidelity than emotional infidelity whereas women
would be more upset if their partner engaged in emotional
infidelity than sexual.
Social–Cognitive Perspective
Harris’s (2003a) social–cognitive theory of jealousy offers
predictions that are distinct from those derived from the EP
hypothesis. Harris argued that evolving a mechanism that
focuses on either type of infidelity would not have been adap-
tive. She suggested that men would not want to focus on cues
to sexual infidelity because it would likely be too late for
them to prevent infidelity by the time overt sexual cues were
present. She also pointed out that in many cases the cues of
emotional and sexual infidelity overlap substantially and vary
from culture to culture. These problems would necessitate a
general mechanism for detecting infidelity rather than a
content-specific one.
Harris (2003a, p. 119) argued that ‘‘jealousy can be
induced when any important aspect of an interpersonal rela-
tionship is threatened.’’ Whenever a rival appears to be threa-
tening an individual’s relationship, that individual makes a
judgment about how threatening that behavior is and what
he or she should do about it. Harris noted that the cognitive
appraisal literature on jealousy finds that interpretation of the
partner’s behavior is a key part of jealousy arousal. For exam-
ple, when an individual sees her or his mate talking to a desir-
able potential rival, evaluations are made of how threatening
the rival is and what the meaning of their interaction is. If the
mate is acting in ways that can be interpreted as flirtatious in
one’s culture, then jealousy is aroused. This general detection
mechanism allows both cultural and cognitive elements to
play a role in jealousy arousal. Harris pointed out that an
overall drive to make appraisals about threats to one’s pri-
mary relationship could just as easily have evolved similarly
for both sexes and would have provided an adaptive advan-
tage for both. Thus, Harris proposed that there are no sex dif-
ferences in the types of jealousy that are upsetting to women
and men.
Double-Shot Perspective
Another critique of the EP position was provided by DeSteno
and Salovey (1996) who proposed what they called the
‘double-shot hypothesis.’’ In the original Buss et al. (1992)
studies, participants were shown two pairs of scenarios
(referred to in their article as Scenario Pairs 1 and 2). In each
pair of scenarios, one of them described emotional infidelity
and the other described sexual infidelity. The participants
were asked to choose which situation was more distressing.
The double-shot hypothesis argued that the participants do
not perceive each scenario as presenting only one type of infi-
delity; rather, because of common stereotypes about how
each sex becomes romantically attached, emotional and sex-
ual infidelities were perceived intertwined in a ‘‘double
shot.’
DeSteno and Salovey (1996) argued that when a man
learns that his female partner has had sex with another man,
he assumes that she is also in love with him because women
are stereotypically believed to have sex only with individuals
with whom they are also emotionally involved. Thus, sexual
infidelity by women is perceived by stereotyping men as both
sexual and emotional. In contrast, stereotyping men can envi-
sion emotional attachments by women devoid of sexuality. If
the double-shot interpretation then was an accurate descrip-
tion of how women’s sexual infidelity is perceived by men,
men would logically choose sexual infidelity as more distres-
sing because it probably involves both forms of infidelity.
26 Psychology of Women Quarterly 36(1)
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
At the same time, DeSteno and Salovey (1996) predicted
that gender stereotyping by women would follow an opposite
logic. They argued that stereotyping women believe that men
who form deep emotional attachments with women are prob-
ably also having sex with them whereas men often have sex
without emotional attachment. So for the women, emotional
infidelity should be rated as more distressing because women
assume that sexual infidelity is also occurring. Both forms of
gender stereotyping then would work in tandem to increase
the gap in women’s and men’s infidelity ratings, and indeed
this hypothesized similarity was what DeSteno and Salovey
found. Furthermore, the description of sexual infidelity they
showed to participants in Buss et al.’s (1992) study described
it as ‘‘passionate sexual intercourse’’ (see Table 1, Scenario
Pair 1), possibly further conflating sexual with emotional
infidelity.
In order to tease apart the influence of sexuality and
emotions, Buss et al. (1999) developed four additional pairs
of scenarios that directly addressed both sexual and emo-
tional infidelity. The first added scenario pair asked partici-
pants to imagine that both types of infidelity had occurred
and then asked which was more distressing (see Question 3
in Table 1). This forced-choice directly takes the differential
base rates of both occurring suggested by the double-shot into
account by forcing the participants to consider both types at
one time. The additional three scenarios specify one trans-
gression without the other (see Scenario Pairs 4–6 in Table
1). In the present meta-analyses, if the predicted sex differ-
ence emerges across studies using these scenario pairs, such
a finding would be inconsistent with the double-shot hypoth-
esis. Several researchers have examined the problem using
this methodology (e.g., Brase, Caprar, & Voracek, 2004;
Henline, Lamke, & Howard, 2007; Ward & Voracek,
2004), and these results will be included in the present
meta-analyses. These studies generally find the statistically
significant interactions predicted by EP between sex of the
participant and the type of infidelity chosen as more distres-
sing. Yet, it remains to be seen if the effect sizes associated
with responses to the double-shot scenarios are as large as
those found for the original scenarios.
Other researchers have argued that the double-shot
hypothesis can be tested by examining the responses of gay
men and lesbians. Dijkstra et al. (2001) suggested that gay
men and lesbians perceive infidelity similarly to heterosex-
uals of the other sex, basing their stereotyped expectations
on the sex of their partner rather than on their own sex.
According to this interpretation, stereotyping gay men, like
heterosexual women, would assume that if their male partner
had committed emotional infidelity, that partner was also
having sex; however, they would not necessarily equate sex-
ual involvement with emotional connection. Stereotyping les-
bians, like heterosexual men, would assume that the sexual
infidelity of their female partner implies emotional infidelity
as well, whereas they would not necessarily equate emotional
sharing with sexual transgression. Dijkstra et al. claimed that
consistently finding that gay men are more concerned about
emotional, and lesbians with sexual, infidelity would be con-
sistent with the double-shot hypothesis. This prediction sug-
gests that the sex difference in infidelity reactions is not a
product of innate differences in jealousy, but rather a stereo-
typic response to the expected behavior of one’s partner. If
gay and lesbian samples show a parallel pattern based on the
sex of their partner, then that finding would be consistent with
the double-shot hypothesis.
It is important to note that this hypothesis does not imply
that lesbians are more likely than heterosexual women to pos-
sess male characteristics or that gay men are more likely to
possess female characteristics than heterosexual men.
Instead, the hypothesis proposes that everyone has the same
gender-stereotypical assumptions about men and women and
that they apply those assumptions to their partner’s behavior.
Table 1. Buss et al.’s (1999) Items
Scenario Pair 1: Which of the following events would be more
distressing?
A. Imagining your partner forming a deep emotional attachment to
that other person (emotional infidelity), or
B. Imagining your partner enjoying passionate sexual intercourse
with that other person (sexual infidelity).
Scenario Pair 2: Which of the following events would be more
distressing?
A. Imagining your partner falling in love with that other person
(emotional infidelity), or
B. Imagining your partner trying different sexual positions with that
person (sexual infidelity).
Scenario Pair 3: Imagine your partner both formed an emotional
attachment to another person and had sexual intercourse with
that other person. Which aspect of your partner’s involvement
would upset you more?
A. The sexual intercourse withthat other person (sexual infidelity), or
B. The emotional attachment to that other person (emotional
infidelity).
Scenario Pair 4: Which would upset or distress you more?
A. Imagining your partner having sexual intercourse with that
person, but you are certain that they will not form a deep
emotional attachment (sexual, without emotional infidelity), or
B. Imagining your partner forming a deep emotional attachment to
that person, but you are certain that they will not have sexual
intercourse (emotional, without sexual, infidelity).
Scenario Pair 5: Which would upset or distress you more?
A. Imagining that your partner is still sexually interested in the
former lover, but is no longer in love with this person (sexual,
without emotional infidelity), or
B. Imagining that your partner is still emotionally involved with the
former lover, but is no longer sexually interested in this person
(emotional, without sexual, infidelity).
Scenario Pair 6: Which would upset or distress you more?
A. Imagining your partner having sexual intercourse for just one
night with another person, with no chance of any further
involvement (sexual, without emotional infidelity), or
B. Imagining your partner becoming emotionally involved with
another person, with no chance of any sexual involvement
(emotional, without sexual, infidelity).
Carpenter 27
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Theoretically, bisexual individuals of either sex would be
more upset by sexual infidelity when they happen to have a
female partner but would be more upset by emotional infide-
lity when they have a male partner. Unfortunately, no
research has been conducted on that hypothesis. Thus, sam-
ples of gay men and lesbians will be included to test the
double-shot hypothesis in the present meta-analyses.
Previous Meta-Analyses
Harris’s (2003a) meta-analysis of 32 studies found insubstan-
tial and inconsistent sex differences in response to the differ-
ent types of infidelity consistent with the social–cognitive
perspective Harris proposed. That meta-analysis also found
that sexual orientation moderated the relationship between
sex and response to infidelity such that the same partner-
focused pattern was found for samples of gay men and
lesbians. This finding was consistent with the double-shot
perspective. The Harris meta-analysis also found that the
effect sizes were larger when student samples were used than
nonstudent samples. Harris proposed that student samples
find a larger effect because young males have a more heigh-
tened sex drive than they do at other points in their life which
might cause them to have an increased focus on sex.
The current meta-analyses will expand on Harris’s (2003a)
excellent review in several ways. First, the 66 new studies
that have been conducted since then will triple the total size
of the sample of studies. Furthermore, Harris also argued that
continuous measures of jealousy do not find sex differences
in the amount of jealousy aroused by the two types of infide-
lity. At the time of Harris’s publication, there were not
enough studies employing continuous measures to test that
assertion meta-analytically. The current meta-analyses will
be able to test the claim that the effects are smaller when the
participants are rating each type of infidelity on continuous
response scales instead of the dichotomous forced-choice
items. Also, the current meta-analyses will examine the aver-
age effect size for the first time for the studies that employed
the four new pairs of scenarios (Buss et al., 1999) that were
designed to test the double-shot hypothesis.
Furthermore, Harris’s (2003a) meta-analysis examined the
issue of cross-cultural variability but did not have enough inter-
national samples to determine the effects of different cultures on
reactions to emotional and sexual infidelity. Although some
minor cultural variability is expected under the EP predictions
(Sagarin, 2005), as Buss et al. (1999) explained in their seminal
article:
... the underlying psychology of jealousy is proposed to con-
tain design features that reflect solutions to the recurrent
adaptive problems that each sex faced over the long expanse
of human evolutionary history. These design features are
hypothesized to be universal, and hence are predicted to be
found across cultures. This provides an additional arena for
empirical testing and potential falsification. (p. 126)
In order to test this hypothesis, samples from different cultures
will be compared in the present meta-analyses. If the predicted
set of findings emerges in some cultures and theopposite pattern
in others, Harris (2003a) argues that such differences would be
consistent with the social–cognitive perspective because the cir-
cumstances that indicate threats to the relationship vary from
culture to culture. These two perspectives thus offer competing
hypotheses. Although the current meta-analyses do not contain
enough international samples for a culture-by-culture compari-
son, there are sufficient data to test whether the United States
differs from the rest of the world. Ideally, cultural-specific
hypotheses would be derived and tested from the social–cogni-
tive theory based on culture specific triggers of jealousy; how-
ever, such detailed comparisons await further research.
Method
Literature Search
Several steps were taken in order to find the maximum num-
ber of studies. Two databases were searched using the term
‘infidelity’’ including Communication and Mass Media
Complete and PsychINFO.Web of Science was used in order
to find all the articles that cited the seminal work in this area
by Buss et al. (1992). The references sections of all obtained
articles, especially the research reviews by Harris (2003a)
and Buss (2000, 2007), were also combed to find additional
research articles. The possibility of my search missing an
unacceptable number of null unpublished findings is belied
by the presence of several articles that were published focus-
ing on null findings in this area (DeSteno, Bartlett, Braver-
man, & Salovey, 2002; Grice & Seely, 2000; Harris, 2000,
2003a, 2003b; Sheets & Wolfe, 2001).
Study Selection
There have been several other dependent variables examined
including using functional magenetic resonance imaging
scans (Takahashi et al., 2006), physiological measures (Buss
et al., 1992; Grice & Seely, 2000), and purely cognitive reac-
tions (Schu
¨tzwohl & Koch, 2004). Because there are not
enough articles using any of these methodologies to be
meta-analyzed, my review is limited to articles that report
studies using participant self-report of emotional reactions
to both types of infidelity. From this set of articles, 172 effect
sizes were coded. For Scenario Pair 1, there were 54 studies
(N¼10,632); for Scenario Pair 2, 29 (N¼5,035); for Sce-
nario Pair 3, 14 (N¼2,519); for Scenario Pair 4, 18 (N¼
4,115); for Scenario Pair 5, 7 (N¼1,561); and for Scenario
Pair 6, 8 (N¼1,561). There were 42 (N¼7,876) studies that
reported data from continuous measures.
Coding
The coding and effect sizes for each study can be obtained by
contacting the author. Aspects of the sample that were coded
28 Psychology of Women Quarterly 36(1)
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
include whether or not it used a student sample, the region
from which the sample was drawn (United States or interna-
tional), the sexual orientation of the sample (heterosexual or
lesbian and gay), and the size of the sample including the
breakdown by participants’ sex. Next, the methodology of the
studies was coded for whether the study used the forced-
choice format or a continuous measure as well as the infide-
lity scenarios presented to the participants. Reliabilities for
the continuous measures were also recorded when they were
reported.
In addition, several different effect sizes were calculated.
For the forced-choice measures, the man:woman odds ratio
was computed by calculating the odds of a man choosing sex-
ual infidelity as more distressing than emotional infidelity
divided by the odds of a woman choosing sexual as more dis-
tressing than emotional. The natural log of each odds ratio
was used to calculate the weighted mean odds ratio in order
to place a null effect at zero and allow more accurate statis-
tical analysis (Haddock, Rindskopf, & Shadish, 1998). From
the odds ratio, Digby’s (1983) tetrachoric correlation approx-
imation was calculated. Positive correlations would indicate
that men are more likely than women to choose sexual infide-
lity as more distressing than emotional; negative correlations,
that women are more likely. The correlation coefficient was
not transformed to Fisher’s zbecause recent simulation stud-
ies have shown that this transformation tends to reduce accu-
racy in meta-analyses (Schulze, 2007).
Correlations were also calculated using the continuous
measure data. The correlation was computed by calculating
the dstatistic and converting it to the correlation coefficient.
Both the within-subject comparisons and the between-subject
comparisons were calculated this way as per the recommen-
dations in Dunlap, Cortina, Vaslow, and Burke (1996). There
were four correlations calculated for each study. First, the dif-
ference between mens scores in response to sexual versus
emotional infidelity was created. A positive correlation indi-
cates that the men reported that sexual infidelity would cause
stronger emotional reactions than emotional infidelity. The
second correlation calculated was the same difference for
women, with positive scores indicating that the women in that
sample thought sexual infidelity would cause them to feel
more distress than emotional. The third and fourth were when
men and women were then compared within type of infide-
lity, and scores were calculated to show if men thought each
kind of infidelity would produce stronger emotions than those
reported by the women in their sample (as indicated by pos-
itive correlations).
Controversies have arisen concerning which statistical
comparison is the proper way to test the EP predictions (Bul-
ler, 2005; Buss & Haselton, 2005; Harris, 2005, Sagarin,
2005). Buller (2005) explained that when one conducts a
study measuring which type of infidelity is more upsetting
to which sex, there are a number of ways to analyze the result-
ing data set. Buller suggested that each sex should be exam-
ined separately, and the size of the preference for that sex’s
predicted type of infidelity should be measured. Essentially,
one looks at how much more men are distressed by sexual
infidelity than emotional and then one looks at how much
more women are distressed by emotional infidelity than sex-
ual. This choice preserves the original EP predictions of Buss
et al. (1992) that men would be more likely to be upset by
sexual infidelity than emotional and women would be more
likely to be upset by emotional than sexual (Lishner, Nguyen,
Stocks, & Zillmer, 2008). Buller’s recommendations will be
used here to analyze my data.
Data Analysis
The effect sizes were averaged using the sample size weight-
ing method outlined in Hunter and Schmidt (2004). There
were several pairs of scenarios commonly used in the litera-
ture that were based on Buss et al.’s (1992) and Buss et al.’s
(1999) scenarios. Many of the studies using Buss et al.’s
(1999) forced-choice items asked participants to respond to
more than one of the six pairs of scenarios. Effect sizes for
the forced-choice data were grouped and averaged based on
which of Buss et al.’s (1992) and Buss et al., (1999) pairs
of scenarios they tested in order to determine if the different
pairs of scenarios are substantially different from each other
and to avoid nonindependence of data. The scenarios in the
studies were coded based on which of the Buss et al. pairs
of scenarios the authors used (see Table 1 for the scenarios).
Some of the studies that used the forced-choice items
asked the participants to indicate which type of infidelity
would produce a stronger emotional reaction for several
different emotions. In such cases, participants’ ratings of
‘distress’’ were used in keeping with the seminal Buss
et al. (1992) study. If distress was not measured, upset (fol-
lowed by jealousy) was used. For those studies using contin-
uous measures, the effect sizes were corrected for attenuation
due to error of measurement using Hunter and Schmidt’s
(2004) artifact distribution method.
The correlation coefficient was used to perform the statis-
tical tests to determine whether further moderator searches
were necessary. If 75%or more of the obtained variance can
be explained by sampling error and artifacts, the remainder
can be assumed to be accounted for by uncorrected artifacts
so that further moderator analysis is deemed unnecessary
(Hunter & Schmidt, 2004). Accounting for more than 75%
of the obtained variance also indicates that the estimate of the
population correlation probably represents one population of
studies (a fixed-effects estimate, also called a homogeneous
effect), whereas if less than 75%of the variance can be
explained, then the estimate is the mean of a range of popu-
lation correlations (a random effects estimate, also called a
heterogeneous effect).
Credibility intervals indicate how wide the population
effect sizes are expected to vary because of the undetected
moderators and uncorrected artifacts (Hunter & Schmidt,
2000). They represent an index of how heterogeneous the
Carpenter 29
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
findings are. The wider the credibility interval, the more
heterogeneous the body of studies is. If 100%of the variance
can be attributed to sampling error and artifacts, then there is
no credibility interval, and the effect is thought to be an accu-
rate point estimate of the population parameter. Confidence
intervals were not calculated because they are often biased
in the presence of heterogeneity (Hunter & Schmidt, 2000).
Hunter and Schmidt (2000) noted that the same problem
applies to calculating statistical significance tests of effect
size estimates. Because of these considerations, credibility
intervals were reported, and neither confidence intervals nor
statistical significance tests were calculated.
Results
Summary
In general, the results were not consistent with the EP predic-
tions. For the most part, both men and women were more
likely to report that emotional infidelity would be more
distressingthansexualinfidelitywhenforcedtochoose.
For the continuous data, both men and women indicated
that sexual infidelity would be more upsetting. These data
were consistent with the social–cognitive predictions
because there were no consistent sex effects, especially for
the continuous data. These data were also consistent with
the double-shot hypothesis prediction concerning gay men
and lesbians such that the effect was parallel according to
the sex of the raters’ partner. Below, I first will examine
the data tables, followed by the data relevant to each
perspective.
Data Tables
Table 2 contains the forced-choice data such that participants
were asked to indicate which type of infidelity would
be more distressing. It is divided according to which of
the six pairs of scenarios participants were responding
and further subdivided by moderator (students, region,
and sexual orientation). Table 3 presents the summary of
the meta-analyses of the studies that used continuous
measures of emotional reaction to each type of infidelity
separately. The effect sizes are broken down by the com-
parisons of sexual with emotional infidelity and of men
and women.
EP Perspective’s Predictions
Recall that according to the EP predictions men would con-
sider sexual infidelity to be more distressing than emotional.
Examination of Column 6 in Table 2 shows the extent to
which men consider sexual infidelity to be more distressing
than emotional. For the first and fourth pairs of scenarios, het-
erosexual men tended to be slightly more likely to be more
distressed by sexual infidelity than emotional as indicated
by the odds being higher than 1.0. For the other four pairs
of scenarios, men were consistently more likely to
consider emotional infidelity to be more distressing as
indicated by the odds of men choosing sexual infidelity
to be more distressing than emotional being consistently
below 1.0. Table 3 shows that men’s ratings of how dis-
tressing sexual infidelity would be are higher than their
ratings of how distressing emotional infidelity would be.
For none of these estimates did sampling error explain
enough variance to indicate a homogeneous set of effect
sizes. This heterogeneity suggests the presence of modera-
tors. The forced-choice data are not consistent with the
predictionthatmenwouldfindsexual infidelity more dis-
tressing than emotional but the continuous data were con-
sistent with that prediction.
According to the predictions derived from EP, it was also
predicted that women would consider emotional infidelity to
be more distressing than sexual. Table 2 shows that the odds
of a heterosexual woman choosing sexual infidelity as more
distressing than emotional are consistently below 1.0 for
every pair of scenarios. This finding is consistent with the
EP hypothesis. On the other hand, Table 3 shows that women
who rated how distressing each type of infidelity was sepa-
rately consistently rated sexual infidelity as more distressing
than emotional. This finding is inconsistent with the EP
hypothesis. Essentially, the forced-choice data show that both
sexes tended to regard emotional infidelity as more distres-
sing but the continuous data showed that both sexes regarded
sexual infidelity as more distressing. Although the two
measurement techniques produce different results, both
sexes generally chose the same type of infidelity as more
distressing.
According to Buss et al. (1999), EP would predict that
although the effect size may vary, men in every culture would
perceive sexual infidelity as more distressing than emotional
whereas women would consistently perceive emotional infi-
delity as more distressing than sexual. Examination of the
odds of heterosexual men reporting that sexual infidelity
would be more distressing than emotional found in Column
6 of Table 2 shows that the odds are in the predicted direction
(greater than 1.0) for the U.S. samples for Scenario Pairs 1, 3,
4, 5, and 6, but not 2. The odds for the international samples
are in the predicted direction only for Scenario Pair 4. In
every other case, heterosexual men living in countries outside
the United States were more likely to indicate that emotional
infidelity by their partner would be more distressing than sex-
ual. Given that U.S. men do tend to find sexual infidelity to be
more distressing than emotional, but the men from the rest of
the world do not, the data are not consistent with the EP-
derived prediction of cross-cultural consistency. Heterosex-
ual women were consistently more likely to indicate that
emotional infidelity would be more distressing than sex-
ual—every moderator subdivision for heterosexual women
contained odds of less than 1.0. This finding was consistent
with the EP hypothesis of cross-cultural consistency for
women.
30 Psychology of Women Quarterly 36(1)
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Table 2. Results of the Forced-Choice Measures Separated by Scenario and Moderator
Column 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Scenario and Moderator
Number
of
Studies
Weighted Mean rfor
the Overall Sex
Difference
a
80% Credibility
Interval
b
% of Variance
Explained by Sam-
pling Error
Weighted
Mean Odds
Ratio
c
Weighted Mean Odds of
Men Choosing Sexual Over
Emotional
Weighted Mean Odds of
Women Choosing Sexual
Over Emotional
Scenario 1: Emotional versus sexual 54 0.40 .15 < r< .65 8.60 3.23 1.12 0.35
U.S. students 37 0.45 .32 < r< .58 24.26 3.88 1.51 0.37
U.S. nonstudents 4 0.31 .18 < r< .44 25.83 2.42 0.82 0.34
International students 8 0.40 .23 < r< .58 13.51 3.22 0.74 0.25
International nonstudents 3 0.23 .12 < r< .35 32.88 1.91 0.56 0.29
Lesbian and gay 2 0.23 .29 < r<.18 74.21 0.53 0.51 0.97
Scenario 2: Love versus sexual positions 29 0.49 .25 < r< .72 9.01 4.25 0.64 0.14
U.S. students 17 0.32 .10 < r< .54 28.97 5.74 0.91 0.15
International students 8 0.43 .06 < r< .92 4.89 3.60 0.52 0.13
Nonstudents 2 0.49 100.00 4.27 0.41 0.09
Lesbian and gay 2 0.09 .48 < r< .66 2.93 1.31 0.26 0.18
Scenario 3: Both occurred 14 0.27 .06 < r< .59 6.95 2.21 0.78 0.37
United States 7 0.44 .05 < r< .91 6.42 3.72 1.45 0.39
International 6 0.22 .07 < r< .52 17.06 1.85 0.52 0.29
Scenario 4: One not the other 18 0.33 .05 < r< .60 7.05 2.62 1.30 0.41
United States 8 0.44 .31 < r< .57 19.93 3.61 1.14 0.29
International 7 0.28 .04 < r< .54 9.05 2.32 1.60 0.58
Lesbian and gay 2 .18 100.00 0.62 0.87 1.40
Scenario 5: Former lover 7 0.22 .01 < r< .45 11.36 1.83 0.88 0.48
United States 2 0.42 100.00 3.27 1.24 0.38
International 4 0.24 .18 < r< .30 61.27 1.93 0.77 0.42
Scenario 6: One but no chance of other 8 0.13 .01 < r< .50 10.76 2.15 0.57 0.59
United States 2 0.65 100.00 8.03 1.64 0.20
International 5 0.13 .03 < r< .30 23.25 2.72 0.57 0.33
Note. The subdivisions by world region and student status do not include the samples of gay men and lesbians.
a
A positive correlation indicates that men are more likely than women to report that sexual infidelity is more distressing than emotional infidelity; a negative correlation, that women are more likely than men to report
that sexual infidelity is more distressing than emotional infidelity.
b
Shows how widely the effect size is expected to vary based on the influence of moderators.
c
Calculated by taking the odds of a man indicating that sexual infidelity was more distressing than emotional and dividing it by the odds of a woman indicating that sexual infidelity was more distressing than emotional.
31
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Social–Cognitive Perspective’s Predictions
Social–cognitive theory predicted that there would be no sub-
stantial sex differences concerning which type of infidelity
participants would rate as more distressing. As stated above,
for most subsamples responding to the force-choice items,
both sexes tended to report that emotional infidelity would
be more distressing than sexual. Also, both sexes indicated
that sexual infidelity would be more distressing than
emotional on the continuous measures data displayed in
Table 2. This result is consistent with the social–cognitive
hypothesis.
In the explication of the social–cognitive hypothesis,
Harris (2003a) also included the prediction that student status
would increase the size of the sex differences because young
men tend to be more focused on sexuality than older men.
There were only enough nonstudent samples to subdivide the
results for Scenario Pairs 1 and 2. The only subsample of het-
erosexual men who responded to Scenario Pair 1 who were
likely to indicate that sexual infidelity would be more
distressing than emotional were the group of samples from
U.S. students. American nonstudents and the international
samples of men were consistently more likely to choose emo-
tional infidelity as more distressing than sexual. This finding
is consistent with the social–cognitive perspective. Every
subsample of male heterosexual participants who responded
to Scenario Pair 2 was more likely to indicate that emotional
infidelity would be more distressing than sexual. Although
the meta-analysis of Scenario Pair 2 did suggest that students
responded in the same way as nonstudents, none of the sam-
ples who responded to this scenario produced results consis-
tent with the EP perspective.
Double-Shot Perspective’s Predictions
According to the double-shot prediction, when the possibility
of one type of infidelity implying the presence of both was
controlled for, the sex differences were predicted to be insub-
stantial. There is some support for this hypothesis in that the
point estimates of the overall effect sizes found in Column 2
of Table 2 are somewhat larger for the first two scenario pairs
than the effect sizes for the remaining four double-shot sce-
nario pairs. This finding should be considered with caution
because the credibility intervals are so wide given the hetero-
geneity of variance that there appear to be moderators that
could make the effect sizes equivalent.
The double-shot hypothesis also predicted that gay men
would consider emotional infidelity to be more distressing
than sexual infidelity. Table 2 shows that gay men considered
emotional infidelity to be more distressing for every scenario
pair for which there were enough samples to calculate an
average effect. (For scenario pairs for which there is not a set
with lesbians and gay men listed in Table 2, it is because there
was only one study with lesbians and gay men in the set of
studies available for that scenario pair.) This finding is con-
sistent with the double-shot hypothesis.
The double-shot hypothesis also predicted that lesbians
would consider sexual infidelity to be more distressing than
emotional. The data were consistent with this prediction such
that lesbians were much more likely to consider sexual infi-
delity as more distressing than emotional. For Scenario Pair
1, the odds of a lesbian choosing sexual infidelity as more dis-
tressing were nearly equal to her chances of choosing emo-
tional. For Scenario Pair 2, lesbians were more likely to
report that emotional infidelity would be more distressing
than sexual. Despite this inconsistency, for Scenario Pairs 1
and 4, lesbians were more likely than gay men to choose sex-
ual infidelity as more distressing than emotional. This finding
is the exact opposite of the standard EP prediction for
women; instead, the data from lesbians were largely consis-
tent with the double-shot hypothesis. Overall, the support for
the double-shot hypotheses was somewhat equivocal, but the
data did tend toward consistency with the double-shot
predictions.
Discussion
The EP hypotheses predicted that men would be more likely to
indicate that sexual infidelity would be more distressing than
emotional. In general, the data were not consistent with this
hypothesis because most samples ofraters who responded to the
forced-choice questions indicated that emotional infidelity was
Table 3. Results of the Continuous Measures
Comparison
Weighted and
Corrected Mean r
80% Credibility
Interval
% of Variance Explained by
Sampling Error and Corrected Artifacts
By Type of Infidelity
Men: Sexual—Emotional
a
0.24 .06 < r< .42 41.69
Women: Sexual—Emotional
b
0.09 .18 < r< .35 21.93
Of Men and Women
Sexual: Men—Women
c
0.06 .20 < r< .08 36.34
Emotional: Men—Women
d
0.21 .33 < r<.10 43.60
Note.
a
The extent to which men reported that sexual infidelity was more distressing than emotional.
b
The extent to which women reported that sexual infidelity was more distressing than emotional.
c
How much more distressing men considered sexual infidelity to be than women did.
d
How much more distressing men considered emotional infidelity to be than women did.
32 Psychology of Women Quarterly 36(1)
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
more distressing than sexual, regardless of the raters’ sex.
Furthermore, the samples who responded to the continuous
questions consistently indicated that sexual infidelity was more
distressing than emotional, again regardless of the raters’ biolo-
gical sex.
The EP hypothesis found the strongest support from U.S. stu-
dents. Most nonstudentmen and men fromother countries were
more likely to indicate that emotional infidelity would be more
distressing than sexual. These data suggest that the EP predic-
tions are only able to explain the choices of American hetero-
sexual male students. If, however, the EP hypothesis can only
explain the behavior of these U.S. students, it seems unlikely
that an evolutionary hypothesis is an appropriate explanatory
mechanism. Perhaps, these data showa case where student sam-
ples produce the distorted view of human nature that Sears
(1986) predicted. Harris (2003a) suggested this difference was
due to young men’s heightened interest in sexuality.
The EP hypothesis also predicted that women would find
emotional infidelity to be more upsetting than sexual. The
data were largely consistent with this hypothesis, although
this finding may simply be a reflection of the more general
pattern that both men and women tend to choose emotional
infidelity as more distressing than sexual. On the other hand,
even though most people chose emotional infidelity as more
distressing, heterosexual women were more likely to choose
emotional infidelity as more distressing than heterosexual
men. This result is consistent with EP predictions if one uses
the more liberal criteria of Sagarin (2005) that only required
an interaction between participants’ sex and the type of infi-
delity that distresses them more.
The findings were largely consistent with Harris’s (2003a)
social–cognitive approach. She argued that men and women
did not evolve separate, content-specific mechanisms for
detecting relational cheating. Instead, she argued that they
both evolved a general vigilance against indicators of infide-
lity. This general vigilance causes people to look for cues of
infidelity and to use their specific cultural lens to interpret
them. From this perspective, both types of infidelity would
probably be perceived as threatening. Thus, she predicted
no sex differences and cultural variability in the direction
of the effects. The forced-choice data found that for the most
part, men and women are likely to consider emotional infide-
lity to be more distressing than sexual. The finding that
American male students tended to consider sexual infidelity
more distressing whereas most other samples of men did not
is consistent with her prediction of cultural variability.
The continuous data were the most inconsistent with the
EP predictions and most consistent with Harris’s (2003a)
social–cognitive prediction of no sex differences. Harris pre-
dicted that both sexes would simply make an assessment of a
given behavior by their partner and use their cultural lens to
determine if there is a threat to their relationship. There was
no substantial interaction between sex and type of infidelity.
Both sexes rated sexual infidelity as more distressing than
emotional and women in general rated both types of infidelity
as more distressing than men. All the findings in the contin-
uous response scale data, even after correcting for measure-
ment error, were still quite small.
The present evidence was more consistent than inconsis-
tent with the double-shot predictions. The double-shot sce-
narios did find smaller sex differences than the original
scenarios, but the heterogeneity of variance in those added
sets of effect sizes makes this conclusion tenuous. When
comparing gay men’s and lesbians’ responses to those of het-
erosexuals, they were similar based on the sex of the rater’s
partner. These results are consistent with the double-shot
hypothesis (Dijkstra et al., 2001) in that (a) most people
whose partner is a man (i.e., gay men and heterosexual
women) will consider a report of emotional infidelity to likely
imply the co-occurrence of sexual infidelity whereas sexual
infidelity does not imply emotional and (b) most people whose
partner is a woman (i.e., lesbians and heterosexual men) will
believe that if their partner is having extradyadic sex, there
is also an emotional attachment whereas an emotional attach-
ment does not imply a sexual one. This line of research sug-
gests that the effects found in previous studies were not
rooted in the nature of their participants’ sexual orientation,
but rather in their perceptions of the likely pattern of behavior
appropriate to stereotyping of their partner’s gender.
Interestingly enough, this pattern emerged for the studies
using samples of gay men and lesbians who responded to Sce-
nario 4 which stated clearly that only one type of infidelity
was occurring and that the other was not. The scenarios
designed to test the double-shot hypothesis may have failed
to convince people that the presence of one type of infidelity
did not imply that the other was also occurring. Dijkstra et al.
(2001) suggested that even if the scenario specifies that only
one type of infidelity is taking place, the devastating effect of
the co-occurrence of both warrants increased vigilance even
if only one type appears to be present. It is also possible that
an explanation other then the double-shot is required to
explain the results from the samples of gay men and lesbians.
Methodology Differences
The results from the forced-choice meta-analyses consistently
found that heterosexualwomen were more likely to indicate that
emotional infidelity would be more distressing than sexual. On
the other hand, the results from the meta-analyses of the contin-
uous scale studies found that women were consistently more
likely to be distressed by sexual infidelity than emotional. DeS-
teno et al. (2002) argued that when faced with the forced-choice
methodology, participants put forth more cognitive effort than
when rating emotional responses on Likert-type scales. They
demonstrated that when their participants were under cognitive
load, the sex differencewas substantially diminished.They sug-
gested that continuous measures are a better indicator of jea-
lousy because they tap a less thoughtful and more emotional
reaction. Shackelford, Buss, and Bennett (2002) argued that
continuous measures are problematic because they allow
Carpenter 33
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
respondents to indicate that both types of infidelity would be
equally distressing.This possibility did not appear to be a prob-
lem in the continuous measure studies reviewed here because
differences were reliably detected. These differences, however,
were not in the direction specified by the hypotheses derived
from EP.
Comparison to the Harris Meta-Analysis
The current analysis replicated Harris’s (2003a) findings con-
cerning the effects of sexual orientation and student status. That
meta-analysis did not find that culture moderated the size of the
sex difference, but Harr is noted there may not have been enough
international studies to detect differences. The current meta-
analyses included more international samples and were able
to detect a difference between United States and international
samples. These findings greatly expanded Harris’s initial work
on the nature of jealousy. Also, the current meta-analyses of the
continuous responsescale data provided the strongest rebuttal to
the EP hypothesis. By examining these additional data (includ-
ing dozens of additional studies), the current meta-analyses
were able to strengthen Harris’s critique of the EP explanation
of jealousy. Future work should expand Harris’s argument by
empirically demonstrating the particular features of cultures
that moderate the types of behaviors that arouse jealousy. This
expansion would make Harris’s position more falsifiable than
the current broad prediction of cultural heterogeneity.
Limitations
First, the lack of more studies conducted outside the United
States prevented these meta-analyses from conducting a
stronger test of the competing hypotheses concerning cross-
cultural consistency of the direction of the predicted sex
difference. Until more research is conducted in more coun-
tries, especially based on aspects of the particular cultures
in question, the support for the social–cognitive perspective
must remain tentative. Given the small sample of non-U.S.
countries, my sample may also be biased toward particular
regions of the world. As it stands, American students seem
to be the only group in which men are consistently more
likely to perceive sexual infidelity as more distressing than
emotional in response to a forced-choice question. Future
work is needed to identify the moderators of this effect.
Harris (2003a) suggested it was young men’s elevated focus
on sex. Future work might find cultures with a heightened
focus on sex to determine if one or both sexes are more or less
distressed by sexual infidelity in that culture. Additionally,
the meta-analyses reported are only related to self-reports of
distress in reaction to hypothetical scenarios. More recent work
has been conducted on recall of cues to each type of infidelity
(Schu
¨tzwohl & Koch, 2004) and asking if cognitive load
can change how individuals react to each type of infidelity
(DeSteno et al., 2002). Research that goes beyond the simple
self-report of distress will continue to enrich this literature.
Conclusion
The evidence was not consistent with the EP predictions. My
meta-analyses failed to consistently find that men are more
likely to perceive sexual infidelity as more distressing than
emotional, and this finding weakens the explanatory power
of the EP hypothesis about jealousy. The continuous response
scale data suggested that both sexes perceive sexual infidelity
as more distressing than emotional. The data were largely
consistent with Harris’s (2003a) social–cognitive perspective
and the double-shot perspective (DeSteno & Salovey, 1996).
Rather than conducting further replications of the same six
forced-choice scenarios, future work would profit from using
the various perspectives to test different cognitive and
behavioral predictions. For example, Schu
¨tzwohl (2008a) has
conducted some intriguing studies demonstrating that men
are more distracted by cues to sexual infidelity and women
are more distracted by emotional cues. Schu
¨tzwohl (2008b)
has also expanded the research in this area beyond jealousy
and into relief—such that men are more relieved when sexual
jealousy is disconfirmed and women are more relieved when
emotional jealousy is disconfirmed. Although the traditional
self-report measures in this area are not consistent with the
EP predictions, more subtle means for investigating the pro-
posed sex differences (such as Schu
¨tzwohl’s program of
research) may find additional evidence consistent with the
EP predictions. Also, research with bisexual samples would
provide an interesting test of the double-shot hypothesis
given that these individuals would be predicted to respond
based on their current partner rather than their own sex.
Broadly speaking, these meta-analyses suggest that men
and women do not differ in the types of infidelity that they
find distressing. Instead, men and women seem to become
distressed to the extent that their partner’s behavior threatens
their relationship rather than affects their long-term reproduc-
tive ability. This finding is fitting given that jealousy was
thought to evolve in order to help individuals maintain their
close relationships in a hostile environment that required help
from others for survival.
Acknowledgments
The author would like to thank Fred Oswald and Brent Donnellan
for their helpful suggestions.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author received no financial support for the research, author-
ship, and/or publication of this article.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the
meta-analyses.
34 Psychology of Women Quarterly 36(1)
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
*Abraham, W. T., Cramer, R. E., Fernandez, A. M., & Mahler, E.
(2001). Infidelity, race, and gender: An evolutionary perspective
on asymmetries in subjective distress to violations-of-trust. Cur-
rent Psychology, 20, 337–348.
*Ahrndt, S. M., & Dindia, K. (2006, November). Distress in
response to infidelity: An examination of the evolutionary per-
spective. Paper presented at the meeting of the National Commu-
nication Association, San Antonio, TX.
*Becker, D. V., Sagarin, B. J., Guadagno, R. E., Millevoi, A., &
Nicastle, L. D. (2004). When the sexes need not differ: Emo-
tional responses to the sexual and emotional aspects of infidelity.
Personal Relationships, 11, 529–538.
*Berman, M. I., & Frazier, P. A. (2005). Relationship power and
betrayal experience as predictors of reactions to infidelity.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1617–1627.
*Bohner, G., & Wanke, M. (2004). Priming of AIDS and reactions
to infidelity: Are sex differences in jealousy context-dependent?
Zeitschrift fur Sozialpsychologie, 35, 107–114.
*Brase,G.L.,Caprar,D.V.,&Voracek,M.(2004).Sexdiffer-
ences in response to relationship threats in England and
Romania. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21,
763–778.
Buller, D. J. (2005). Evolutionary psychology: The emperor’s new
paradigm. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 277–283.
Buss, D. M. (2000). The dangerous passion. New York: The Free Press.
Buss, D. M. (2007). The evolution of human mating. Acta psycholo-
gical Sinica, 39, 502–512.
Buss, D. M., & Haselton, M. (2005). The evolution of jealousy.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 506–507.
*Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D., & Semmelroth, J. (1992).
Sex differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychol-
ogy. Psychological Science, 3, 251–255.
*Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Kirkpatrick, L. A., Choe, J. C.,
Lim, H. K., Hasegawa, M., ... Bennett, K. (1999). Jealousy and
the nature of beliefs about infidelity: Tests of competing hypoth-
eses about sex differences in the United States, Korea, and Japan.
Personal Relationships, 6, 25–150.
*Buunk, B. P., Angleitner, A., Oubaid, V., & Buss, D. M. (1996).
Sex differences in jealousy in evolutionary and cultural perspec-
tive: Tests from the Netherlands, German, and the United States.
Psychological Science, 7, 359–363.
*Cann, A., & Baucom, T. R. (2004). Former partners and new rivals
as threats to a relationship: Infidelity type, gender, and commit-
ment as factors related to distress and forgiveness. Personal
Relationships, 11, 305–318.
*Cann, A., Mangum, J. L., & Wells, M. (2001). Distress in response
to relationship infidelity: The roles of gender and attitudes about
relationships. Journal of Sex Research, 38, 185–190.
Cano, A., & O’Leary, K. D. (1997). Romantic jealousy and affairs:
Research and implications for couple therapy. Journal of Sex &
Marital Therapy, 23, 249–275.
*Cramer, R. E., Abraham, W. T., Johnson, L. M., & Manning-Ryan,
B. (2001). Gender differences in subjective distress to emotional
and sexual infidelity: Evolutionary or logical inference explana-
tion? Current Psychology, 20, 327–336.
*Cramer, R. E., Lipinski, R. E., Bowman, A., & Carollo, T. (2009).
Subjective distress to violations of trust in Mexican American
close relationships conforms to evolutionary principles. Current
Psychology, 28, 1–11.
*Cramer, R. E., Lipinski, R. E., Meteer, J. D., & Houska, J. A.
(2008). Sex differences in subjective distress to unfaithfulness:
Testing competing evolutionary and violation of infidelity
expectations hypotheses. The Journal of Social Psychology,
148, 389–405.
*Cramer, R. E., Manning-Ryan, B., Johnson, L. M., & Barbo, E.
(2000). Sex differences in subjective distress to violations of
trust: Extending an evolutionary perspective. Basic and Applied
Social Psychology, 22, 101–109.
*De Souza, A. A. L., Verderane, M. P., Taira, J. T., & Otta, E.
(2006). Emotional and sexual jealousy as a function of sex and
sexual orientation in a Brazilian sample. Psychological Reports,
98, 529–535.
*DeSteno, D., Bartlett, M. Y., Braverman J., & Salovey, P. (2002).
Sex differences in jealousy: Evolutionary mechanism or artifact
of measurement? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
83, 1103–1116.
*DeSteno, D. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Evolutionary origins of sex
differences in jealousy? Questioning the ‘‘fitness’’ of the model.
Psychological Science, 7, 367–372.
Digby, P. G. N. (1983). Approximating the tetrachoric correlation
coeffiecient. Biometrics, 39, 753–757.
*Dijkstra, P., Groothof, H., Poel, G., Laverman, T., Schrier, M., &
Buunk, B. (2001). Sex differences in the events that elicit jea-
lousy among homosexuals. Personal Relationships, 8, 41–54.
Dunlap, W. P., Cortina, J. M., Vaslow, J. B., & Burke, M. J. (1996).
Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated
measures designs. Psychological Methods, 1, 170–177.
*Edlund, J. E., Heider, J. D., Scherer, C. R., Farc, M. M., & Sagarin,
B. J. (2006). Sex differences in jealousy in response to actual
infidelity. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 462–470.
*Edlund, J. E., & Sagarin, B. J. (2009). Sex differences in jealousy:
Misinterpretation of nonsignificant results as refuting the theory.
Personal Relationships, 16, 67–78.
*Fernandez, A. M., Vera-Villarroel, P., Sierra, J. C., & Zubeidat, I.
(2007). Distress in response to emotional and sexual infidelity:
Evidence of evolved gender differences in Spanish students.
Journal of Psychology, 141, 17–24.
*Gaulin, S., Silverman, I., Phillips, K., & Reiber, C. (1997). Activa-
tional hormonal influences on abilities and attitudes. Evolution
and Cognition, 3, 191–199.
*Geary, D. C., DeSoto, M. C., Hoard, M. K., Shildon. M. S., &
Cooper, L. (2001). Estrogens and relationship jealousy. Human
Nature, 12, 299–320.
*Geary, D. C., Rumsey, M., Bow-Thomas, C. C., & Hoard, M. K.
(1995). Sexual jealousy as a facultative trait: Evidence from the
pattern of sex differences in adults from China and the United
States. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 355–383.
Goetz, A. T., & Shackelford, T. K. (2006). Modern application of
evolutionary theory to psychology: Key concepts and clarifica-
tions. American Journal of Psychology, 119, 567–584.
Carpenter 35
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
*Goldenberg, J. L., Landau, M. J., Pyszczynski, T., Cox, C. R.,
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Dunnam, H. (2003). Gender-
typical responses to sexual and emotional infidelity as a unction
of mortality salience induced self-esteem striving. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1585–1595.
*Green, M. C., & Sabini, J. (2006). Gender, socioeconomic status,
age, and jealousy: Emotional responses to infidelity in a national
sample. Emotion, 6, 330–334.
Grice, J. W., & Seely, E. (2000). The evolution of sex differences in
jealousy: Failure to replicate previous results. Journal of
Research in Personality, 34, 348–356.
Haddock, C. K., Rindskopf, D., & Shadish, W. R. (1998). Using
odds ratios as effect sizes for meta-analysis of dichotomous
data: A primer on methods and issues. Psychological Methods,
3, 339–353.
Harris, C. (2000). Psychophysiological responses to imagined
infidelity: The specific innate modular view of jealousy recon-
sidered. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
1082–1091.
*Harris, C. (2002). Sexual and romantic jealousy in heterosexual
and homosexual adults. Psychological Science, 13, 7–12.
Harris, C. (2003a). A review of sex differences in sexual jealousy,
including self-report data, psychophysiological responses, inter-
personal violence, and morbid jealousy. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 7, 102–128.
*Harris, C. (2003b). Factors associated with jealousy over real and
imagined infidelity: An examination of the social-cognitive and
evolutionary perspectives. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27,
319–329.
Harris, C. (2005). Male and female jealousy, still more similar than
different: Reply to Sagarin (2005). Personality and Social Psy-
chology Review, 9, 76–86.
*Harris, C., & Christenfeld, N. (1996). Gender, jealousy, and reason.
Psychological Science, 7, 364–366.
*Henline,B.H.,Lamke,L.K.,&Howard,M.D.(2007).Exploring
perceptions of online infidelity. Personal Relationships, 14,
113–128.
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2000). Fixed effects vs. random
effects meta-analysis models: Implications for cumulative
research knowledge. International Journal of Selection and
Assessment, 8, 275–292.
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis:
Correcting for error and bias in research findings. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
*Hupka, R., & Bank, A. (1996). Sex differences in jealousy:
Evolution or social construction? Cross-Cultural Research,
30,2459.
*Landolfi, J. F., Geher, G., & Andrews, A. (2007). The role of sti-
mulus specificity on infidelity reactions: Seeing is disturbing.
Current Psychology, 26, 46–59.
*Levy, K. N., & Kelly, K. M. (2010). Sex differences in jealousy: A
contribution from attachment theory. Psychological Science, 21,
168–173.
*Lishner, D. A., Nguyen, S., Stocks, E. L., & Zillmer, E. J. (2008).
Are sexual and emotional infidelity equally upsetting to men and
women? Making sense of forced-choice responses. Evolutionary
Psychology, 6, 667–675.
*Mathes, E. W. (2003). Are sex differences in sexual vs emotional
jealousy explained better by differences in sexual strategies or
uncertainty of paternity? Psychological Reports, 93, 895–906.
*Mathes, E. W. (2005). Men’s desire for children carrying their
genes and sexual jealousy: A test of paternity uncertainty as an
exploitation of male sexual jealousy. Psychological Reports,
96, 791–798.
*Murphy, S. M., Vallacher, R. R., Shackelford, T. K., Bjorklund, &
Yunger, J. L. (2006). Relationship experience as a predictor of
romantic jealousy. Personality and Individual Differences, 40,
761–769.
*Miller, J. K., & Maner, S. L. (2009). Sex differences in response to
sexual versus emotional infidelity: The moderating role of indi-
vidual differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 46,
287–291.
*Penke, L., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2008). Evidence for conditional sex
difference in emotional but not in sexual jealousy at the auto-
matic level of cognitive processing. European Journal of Per-
sonality, 22,3–30.
*Pietrzak, R. H., Laird, J. D., Stevens, D. A., & Thompson, N. S.
(2002). Sex differences in human jealousy: A coordinated study
of forced-choice, continuous rating-scale, and physiological
responses on the same subjects. Evolution and Human Behavior,
23, 83–94.
*Pines, A. M., & Friedman, A. (1998). Gender differences in roman-
tic jealousy. The Journal of Social Psychology, 138, 54–71.
*Russel, E. B., & Harton, H. C. (2005). The ‘‘other factors’’: Using
individual and relationship characteristics to predict sexual and
emotional jealousy. Current Psychology, 24, 242–257.
*Sabini, J., & Green, M. C. (2004). Emotional responses to sexual
and emotional infidelity: Constants and differences across gen-
ders, samples, and methods. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 30, 1375–1388.
Sagarin, B. J. (2005). Reconsidering evolved sex differences in jea-
lousy: Comment on Harris (2003). Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 9, 62–75.
*Sagarin, B. J., Becker, D. V., Guadagno, R. E., Nicastle, L. D., &
Millevoi, A. (2003). Sex differences (and similarities) in jea-
lousy: The moderating influence of infidelity experience and
sexual orientation of the infidelity. Evolution and Human Beha-
vior, 24, 17–23.
*Sagarin, B. J., & Guadagno, R. E. (2004). Sex differences in
the contexts of extreme jealousy. Personal Relationships, 11,
319–328.
Schulze, R. (2007). Current methods for meta-analysis:
Approaches, issues, and developments. Journal of Psychol-
ogy, 215,90103.
*Schu
¨tzwohl, A. (2004). Which infidelity type makes you more jea-
lousy? Decision strategies in a forced-choice between sexual and
emotional infidelity. Evolutionary Psychology, 2, 121–128.
*Schu
¨tzwohl, A. (2007). Decision strategies in continuous ratings of
jealousy feelings elicited by sexual and emotional infidelity.
Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 815–828.
36 Psychology of Women Quarterly 36(1)
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Schu
¨tzwohl, A. (2008a). The disengagement of attentive resources
from task-irrelevant cues to sexual and emotional infidelity. Per-
sonality and Individual Differences, 44, 633–644.
Schu
¨tzwohl, A. (2008b). Relief over the disconfirmation of the pros-
pect of sexual and emotional infidelity. Personality and Individ-
ual Differences, 44, 668–678.
Schu
¨tzwohl, A., & Koch, S. (2004). Sex differences in jealousy: The
recall of cues to sexual and emotional infidelity in personally
more and less threatening context conditions. Evolution and
Human Behavior, 25, 249–257.
Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences
of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515–530.
Sesardic, N. (2003). Evolution of human jealousy: A just-so story or a
just-so criticism? Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 33, 427–443.
*Shackelford, T. K., Buss, D. M., & Bennett, K. (2002). Forgiveness
or breakup: Sex differences in responses to a partner’s infidelity.
Cognition and Emotion, 16, 299–307.
Sheets, V. L., & Wolfe, M. D. (2001). Sexual jealousy in heterosex-
uals, lesbians, and gays. Sex Roles, 44, 255–276.
*Takahashi, H., Matsuura, M., Yahata, N., Koeda, T. S., Suhara, T.,
& Okubo, Y. (2006). Men and women show distinct brain activa-
tions during imagery of sexual and emotional infidelity. Neuro-
Image, 32, 1299–1307.
*Ward, J., & Voracek, M. (2004). Evolutionary and social cognitive
explanations of sex differences in romantic jealousy. Australian
Journal of Psychology, 56, 165–171.
*Whitty, M. T., & Quigley, L. L. (2008). Emotional and sexual
infidelity offline and in cyberspace. Journal of Marital and Fam-
ily Therapy, 34, 461–468.
*Wiederman, M. W., & Allgeier, E. R. (1993). Gender differences
in sexual jealousy: Adaptationist or social learning explanation?
Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 115–140.
*Wiederman, M. W., & Kendall, E. (1999). Evolution, sex, and
jealousy: Investigation with a sample from Sweeden. Evolution
and Human Behavior, 20, 121–128.
Carpenter 37
at WESTERN ILLINOIS UNIV on March 15, 2012pwq.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... The theory has not, however, been universally accepted. Criticisms have been advanced of the theory's framing, the methods used to test it, and the interpretations of the evidence [35,[44][45][46][47]. ...
... A further possibility considers the possibility that socialcognitive variation explains most within-person and withinsex variation in jealousy, as well as average sex differences [35,46,52]. Such beliefs include beliefs about the nature of commitment, and context-dependent cues as to what threatens a person's primary relationship [35,46]. ...
... A further possibility considers the possibility that socialcognitive variation explains most within-person and withinsex variation in jealousy, as well as average sex differences [35,46,52]. Such beliefs include beliefs about the nature of commitment, and context-dependent cues as to what threatens a person's primary relationship [35,46]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Technologies that stimulate human social and sexual impulses could affect users and societies. Here, we report on two experiments designed to test participant responses to (1) “virtual friend” chatbots that vary in capacity to engage users socially and emotionally (i.e., emotional sophistication) and (2) “digital lover” technologies—in the form of sex toys, sex robots, or virtual reality entities—that vary in capacity to physically stimulate users (i.e., physical sophistication). Participants (173 female, 176 male) read vignettes that each described a particular technology and then answered whether, if their romantic partner were to use the described technology, they would anticipate jealousy or anger, and whether they would prefer to see the technology banned. Participant anticipations of jealousy and anger were so similar that we combined them in a single composite measure. In experiment 1, both the anticipation of jealousy-anger and the inclination to ban chatbots increased with emotional sophistication, particularly in female participants. In experiment 2, both sexes anticipated greater jealousy-anger and were more inclined to ban more physically sophisticated digital lovers. Female participants expressed higher levels of both responses across the range of sophistication. Experiment 2 participants were more likely to anticipate jealousy-anger and more inclined to ban sex robots than sex toys or virtual reality lovers. Our results show only limited consistency with evolutionary theories concerning sex differences in jealousy. Generally, the anticipated levels of jealousy-anger and inclination to ban the described technologies were low, suggesting low levels of resistance to the idea of the technologies.
... Harris [11,12] argued that evolutionary theory and sexual dimorphism have several limitations when explaining jealousy and proposed a social-cognitive theory of jealousy based on the principles of social learning and social cognition. According to this view, jealousy occurs when an individual evaluates that a rival threatens any important aspect of a romantic relationship or poses a threat to the representation of the self [11][12][13][14]. The evaluation process of the threat to the relationship includes both how threatening the rival is and the meaning of the interaction between the partner and the rival. ...
... The evaluation process of the threat to the relationship includes both how threatening the rival is and the meaning of the interaction between the partner and the rival. As jealousy is evoked when the rival is perceived as a threat and the interaction is regarded as flirtatious in one's culture, this is a general mechanism for detecting cues that evoke a jealousy response that considers the cultural and cognitive aspects of the situation [13]. Furthermore, Harris and Dolby [14] argued that the appraisal and attribution of a partner's behavior elicit a jealousy response, not the partner's behavior itself. ...
... Another critique of sexual dimorphism is the interpretation of the results of studies that reveal gender differences in jealousy considering the specific type of infidelity. According to the social cognitive theory, men will automatically react more strongly to a partner's sexual infidelity because there is a widely held stereotypical belief that a woman engaging in a sexual relationship with another person also feels emotional closeness to that individual [13]. On the other hand, it is commonly believed that a man can have a sexual relationship without emotional attachment to another person. ...
Article
Full-text available
Jealousy and mate retention have received attention in research over the last few decades. Despite this, most of the research has examined male jealousy and male mate retention, emphasizing cost-inflicting behavior due to its role in relationships and domestic violence. The aim of this study was to investigate the relationship between jealousy and all mate retention strategies in romantic relationships among women during the COVID-19 pandemic. The sample consisted of 772 Croatian women aged 19 to 40 who were in a heterosexual relationship at the time. This study was conducted online, and the participants completed the Multidimensional Jealousy Scale and Mate Retention Inventory. The results showed that cognitive, emotional, and behavioral jealousy were positively correlated with all mate retention strategies, which indicates that a stronger experience of jealousy can be expected to result in more frequent use of all partner retention strategies. We also found that all three dimensions of jealousy and relationship length positively predicted both cost-inflicting and benefit-provisioning mate retention behavior, whereas age was a negative predictor of benefit-provisioning behavior only. The findings of this study suggest that, although jealousy can substantially explain interpersonally risky and damaging behavior in relationships, it can also explain affectionate and attentive behavior, to some extent.
... Most cross-cultural studies look for differences between men and women according to the type of infidelity (emotional or sexual), as a cross-cultural comparison is important to support or refute the evolutionary hypothesis (Buunk et al., 1996). However, the results are not conclusive, and although the evolutionary theory has received great support (Bendixen et al., 2015;Edlund & Sagarin, 2017;Sagarin et al., 2012), there are also strong arguments in favour of the sociocultural perspective (Carpenter, 2012;Harris, 2003). These two mechanisms always interact. ...
... the individual's interpretation the situation, which may be shaped by the social environment, contributing to the extensive body of research on gender differences in jealousy (Carpenter, 2012;Edlund & Sagarin, 2017;Scelza et al., 2020). The significant differences in romantic jealousy scores between the three countries align with ethnographic research conducted by Hupka and Ryan (1990), which suggests that culture explains approximately 29% of the variance in jealousy situations. ...
... However, the EJM hypothesis has been criticised by some scholars (for review, Carpenter, 2012;Harris, 2003;Kato, 2022b). This is because the EJM hypothesis was not supported, when using other measurement methods (e.g. ...
... Some meta-analyses (e.g. Carpenter, 2012;Kato, 2022b) have supported this hypothesis. Considering that it might have been difficult for some participants in this study to imagine partners' sexual and emotional infidelity separately using hypothetical scenarios regarding partners' infidelity, the participants reported the associations between romantic imagination and distress over partners' sexual infidelity as well as between sexual imagination and distress over partners' emotional infidelity. ...
... Future work could continue to examine whether men's ambivalent sexism could predict their sexual and emotional infidelity to the same extent or not. In addition, research has indicated that gender differences in infidelity have been shrinking in recent decades (e.g., Carpenter, 2012;Fincham & May, 2017;Maddox Shaw et al., 2013;Scheeren et al., 2018). This trend of closing gender gap could be derived from several factors, such as women's financial independence due to presence in the workforce (Abzug, 2016), liberation of women from tight corsets of sexual restrictions with the gradual shift toward gender equality (Wróblewska-Skrzek, 2021), or simply the increasing likelihood of admitting their infidelity as women have gained greater independence and agency in their lives (e.g., Lammers et al., 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Infidelity has destructive effects on romantic relationships. Several idiographic characteristics or experiences in an intimate relationship have been linked to unfaithfulness. Yet, relatively little research has been paid to investigate how sexist beliefs might sabotage relationships by incurring infidelity. The present research examined the association between men’s ambivalent sexism – hostile sexism and benevolent sexism – and men’s infidelity as well as women’s perception of the likelihood of men’s infidelity. The results showed that men’s hostile sexism and benevolent sexism predicted their increased infidelity (Studies 1 and 2). In addition, the indirect association between ambivalent sexism (both hostile sexism and benevolent sexism) and infidelity was through the importance placed on power in one’s intimate relationship in general (Study 2). Importantly, women were unaware of benevolently sexist men’s increased infidelity, such that women rated benevolently sexist men as having a lower likelihood of engaging in infidelity than hostilely sexist men and believed benevolently sexist men’s infidelity level was similar to nonsexist men (Study 3). Therefore, these findings contribute to the psychology of infidelity by revealing that ambivalent sexism, both hostile sexism and benevolent sexism, are significant predictors. Implications of the findings are discussed.
... Concerning gender, there is research that argues that women and men experience differences in forgiveness in the characteristics of infidelity. One study stated that both genders are exposed to the same distress on emotional infidelity rather than sexual infidelity (Carpenter, 2012;Kato, 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Infidelity in relationships is increasingly common in marriage and dating relationships. Its strong effect on mental health makes this topic worthy of further research, especially in perceptions of dating infidelity relationships. Perceptions of dating infidelity have three dimensions: ambiguous, deceptive, and explicit infidelity. This study uses a quantitative design with a total of 77 women and using convenience sampling. We analysed using simple regression analysis. Data collection used The Forgiveness Scale and Perception of Dating Infidelity Scale. The results of this study indicate a significant negative effect between the perception of dating infidelity and forgiveness behaviour in the women population who are victims of infidelity value F (1.75) = 5.114, p= 0.027, adj. R 2 = 0.051, and regression coefficient (β=-0.091). Ambiguous behaviour (F (1.75) = 4.582, p= 0.036, adj. R 2 = 0.045, and regression coefficient (β=-0.077)) and deceptive behaviour (F (1.75) = 4.035, p= 0.048, adj. R 2 = 0.038, and regression coefficient (β=-0.054)) dimensions have a significant relationship with forgiveness. Whereas explicit behaviour is not significant (F (1.75) = 1.425, p= 0.236, adj. R 2 = 0.006, and regression coefficient (β=-0.028)). This research is an initial study on the perception of dating infidelity in Indonesia. It can be expected to be helpful as a study material in designing educational activities such as preventing infidelity in marriage preparation.
... Despite several attempts to explain these sex differences as methodological artefacts or influences by social factors and experience (e.g., Carpenter, 2012;Harris, 2003a), sex differences in jealousy are found to be moderate to strong across measurement paradigms (forced choice vs. continuous measures) and are unaffected by having relationship experience, having experienced infidelity, and having children (Bendixen et al., 2015a(Bendixen et al., , 2015bFrederick & Fales, 2016). However, studies suggest men's and women's jealousy responses to become more similar as they grow older, and sex differences often not found in samples of sexual minority people or the effects are reversed (Dijkstra et al., 2001;Frederick & Fales, 2016;Harris, 2003b;Sheets & Wolfe, 2001). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines what beliefs people hold about other men’s and women’s reaction to infidelity and how related these beliefs are to one’s own jealousy response and to various socio-cultural influences. This novel approach was examined in a Facebook snowball sample (N = 1213) who responded to three infidelity scenarios regarding what aspect of infidelity (emotional or sexual) they believed would make men and women more jealous and then what aspect would make themselves more jealous. The results suggest that both men and women believed men would be more upset by the sexual aspect of infidelity and that women would be more upset by the emotional aspect (i.e., falling in love). Own jealousy responses in men and women were strongly associated with beliefs about same-sex responses to infidelity and showed moderate association with beliefs about opposite-sex responses. Self-reported perceptions of cues to infidelity and knowledge from various sources about what (1) may be cues to infidelity and (2) may be typical reactions to infidelity were unrelated to beliefs about men’s and women’s jealousy responses and to own jealousy responses. We discuss whether beliefs about men’s and women’s jealousy responses may be culturally transmitted or more likely involve a dual model consisting of (a) reflection of own jealousy responses with (b) some cross-sex insights into jealousy reactions in men and women. The findings suggest that there may be evolved psychological adaptations for jealousy beliefs that extend to others of same and opposite sex.
... In these, the participants are told of a rival for their partner who is either actively having sex with their partner or with whom their partner is falling in love and forming an emotional attachment. While the focus of the jealousy literature in evolutionary psychology has focused on the question of sex differences (Buss et al., 1996;Carpenter, 2012;DeSteno & Salovey, 1996;Harris, 2003;Harris & Christenfeld, 1996), few have argued that they do not generate significant arousal and upset (Buss et al., 1999;Pietzak, 2002;Sagarin et al., 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
Priming has become a common method for studying intrasexual competition, with various methods, such as vignettes and unbalanced sex ratios used in the literature. However, there have been few independent measures of the effectiveness of this priming, all of which rely on conscious responding. Here, we investigate the ability of one intrasexual competition prime, vignettes that imply an individual is attempting to infiltrate an ongoing relationship, to generate arousal. Participants read four vignettes while four psychophysical measurements of arousal were assessed, left and right pupil dilation, heart rate, and electrodermal activity. Results indicated that while reading the vignettes and reflecting on them, three of the measures, both right and left pupil dilation and heart rate, did in fact increase, indicating arousal. We argue that this arousal may be due to one of two underlying emotional states: jealousy related to intrasexual competition or anxiety associated with the anticipation of the competition itself.
Article
Full-text available
Introducción. Las actitudes y creencias en conducta sexual son constructos todavía abordados con clasificaciones no unificadas, encontrando propuestas teóricas muy diversas. Por ello, el objetivo de este estudio es revisar la literatura en la búsqueda de patrones comunes para generar una clasificación desambiguada. Métodos. Se llevó a cabo una revisión sistemática de cuestionarios y revisiones. Tras ello, siguiendo los principios del análisis temático, los códigos encontrados fueron revisados y categorizados. Resultados. De los 1126 artículos revisados, se evaluaron en profundidad 89 escalas y 62 revisiones. De ellos, se incluyeron 28 (15 cuestionarios y 13 revisiones). Se obtuvieron 144 elementos de información válida según criterios, que se clasificaron en 17 grupos según su contenido. Discusión. Este trabajo resultó en la clasificación referenciada de los esquemas cognitivos autorreferentes sobre conducta sexual (ECA-CS). Se trata de un primer paso en el desarrollo de clasificaciones unificadas para la evaluación de actitudes sexuales.
Chapter
The interface of sexual behavior and evolutionary psychology is a rapidly growing domain, rich in psychological theories and data as well as controversies and applications. With nearly eighty chapters by leading researchers from around the world, and combining theoretical and empirical perspectives, The Cambridge Handbook of Evolutionary Perspectives on Sexual Psychology is the most comprehensive and up-to-date reference work in the field. Providing a broad yet in-depth overview of the various evolutionary principles that influence all types of sexual behaviors, the handbook takes an inclusive approach that draws on a number of disciplines and covers nonhuman and human psychology. It is an essential resource for both established researchers and students in psychology, biology, anthropology, medicine, and criminology, among other fields. Volume 3: Female Sexual Adaptations addresses theory and research focused on sexual adaptations in human females.
Article
Full-text available
Two studies are presented that challenge the evidentiary basis for the existence of evolved sex differences in jealousy. In opposition to the evolutionary view. Study 1 demonstrated that a sex difference in jealousy resulting from sexual versus emotional infidelity is observed only when judgments are recorded using a forced-choice response format. On all other measures, no sex differences were found; both men and women reported greater jealousy in response to sexual infidelity. A second study revealed that the sex difference on the forced-choice measure disappeared under conditions of cognitive constraint. These findings suggest that the sex difference used to support the evolutionary view of jealousy (e.g., D. M. Buss, R. Larsen, D. Westen, & J. Semmelroth, 1992; D. M. Buss et al., 1999) likely represents a measurement artifact resulting from a format-induced effortful decision strategy and not an automatic, sex-specific response shaped by evolution.
Article
Full-text available
This study tested the prediction derived from the evolutionary psychological analysis of jealousy that men and women selecting the adaptively primary infidelity type (i.e., female sexual and male emotional infidelity, respectively) in a forced-choice response format need to engage in less elaborate decision strategies than men and women selecting the adaptively secondary infidelity type (i.e., male sexual and female emotional infidelity, respectively). Unknown to the participants, decision times were registered as an index of the elaborateness of their decision strategies. The results clearly support the prediction. Implications and limitations of the present findings are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Article
Full-text available
Two studies (total N = 689) tested the assumption of DeSteno, Bartlett, Braverman, and Salovey (2002) that sex differences in jealousy predicted by the evolutionary view are an artifact of measurement because they are restricted to a forced-choice response format and do not emerge when using continuous jealousy ratings. In Study 1, men and women rated how much a mate's emotional and sexual infidelity contributed to their jealousy feeling. In Study 2, men and women rated the intensity of their jealousy feeling elicited by a mate's emotional and sexual infidelity. In one condition they were asked to make their ratings spontaneously whereas in the other condition they were instructed to make their ratings only after careful consideration. The results of both studies lend no support for the artifact-of-measurement assumption. The implications of the present finding for the assumption of DeSteno et al. (2002) are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Research conclusions in the social sciences are increasingly based on meta-analysis, making questions of the accuracy of meta-analysis critical to the integrity of the base of cumulative knowledge. Both fixed effects (FE) and random effects (RE) meta-analysis models have been used widely in published meta-analyses. This article shows that FE models typically manifest a substantial Type I bias in significance tests for mean effect sizes and for moderator variables (interactions), while RE models do not. Likewise, FE models, but not RE models, yield confidence intervals for mean effect sizes that are narrower than their nominal width, thereby overstating the degree of precision in meta-analysis findings. This article demonstrates analytically that these biases in FE procedures are large enough to create serious distortions in conclusions about cumulative knowledge in the research literature. We therefore recommend that RE methods routinely be employed in meta-analysis in preference to FE methods.
Article
Full-text available
Prior research on sex differences in relationship jealousy and on reactions to third-party rivals has been conducted primarily within the U.S., Central Europe, and Asia. As these effects vary in magnitude across cultures, it is important to investigate both how these patterns differ across a wider range of cultures and the key mediating cultural variables. One hundred and fourteen English and 202 Romanian participants provided their reactions to hypothetical relationship infidelity situations and to various traits that a mate rival could possess. In both samples, males were more upset than females by sexualinfidelity, as compared with emotional infidelity, but this effect was much smaller for the Romanian sample. In line with evolutionary predictions, men were more upset by a rival who had better financial prospects, greater status and prestige, and was physically stronger, whereas women were more upset by a rival with a more attractive body and face. Additional, unpredicted, sex differences in the evaluation of rival traits were also found. Finally, different correlations were found (both across cultures and across sexes) between reported reactions to infidelities and to specific mate rival traits.
Article
According to the evolutionary model of psychology, biological influences may be a force behind many gender differences in relationship strategies and responses to relationship issues. For example, prior research has shown that males indicated more distress in relation to sexual infidelity, whereas females indicated more distress at the emotional infidelity of their partner. The current study attempted to replicate and extend this previous research by also examining relevant attitudinal factors possibly related to responses to infidelity. Participants (N = 156) were surveyed regarding probable responses to relationship infidelity (sexual vs. emotional infidelity), and measures of participants’ sexual attitudes and romantic beliefs were obtained. Results indicate a clear gender difference: Women were more likely to choose emotional infidelity as most distressing, while more men chose sexual infidelity as most distressing. For the men and women who did choose the same type of infidelity as most distressing, their ratings of the intensity of the distress did not differ. Sexual attitudes were found to be significant in predicting distress caused by either type of infidelity. Romantic beliefs were only significant in predicting distress due to emotional infidelity.
Article
This communication describes two approximating functions for the tetrachoric correlation coefficient. The values of both functions can be calculated easily and should be sufficiently accurate for many applications.
Article
We tested the prediction derived from the evolutionary view of jealousy that men preferentially recall cues to sexual infidelity, whereas women preferentially recall cues to emotional infidelity. This preferential recall was predicted to be more pronounced in a personally more threatening than in a personally less threatening context condition. In the personally less threatening context condition, the participants listened to a story about an anonymous couple spending an evening together; in the personally more threatening context condition, the same story referred to one's own romantic relationship. Integrated in this story were five ambiguous cues each to sexual and emotional infidelity. As predicted, in a surprise recall test, men preferentially recalled cues to sexual infidelity, whereas women preferentially recalled cues to emotional infidelity. This preferential recall was significant for both men and women only in the personally more threatening context condition.