ArticlePDF Available

Mutual and unrequited love in adolescence and young adulthood

Authors:

Abstract

The purpose of the study was to examine the frequency of experiencing passionate love during adolescence and young adulthood as recalled by young adults, and to examine gender differences in this frequency. Female and male US. introductory psychology students, several of whom were of nontraditional age, responded to a questionnaire that asked about the frequency of their mutual and unrequited love experiences. Respondents provided information for four age periods ranging from childhood to age 25 years. Although the frequency of recalled mutual love experiences increased for both genders from childhood through the 16-to-20-year-age period, men reported more episodes or unrequited love during the 16-to-20-year-age period compared to (a) other age periods, (b) their reports of mutual love during that age period, and (c) the reports of unrequited love for 16-to-20-year-old women.
Seediscussions,stats,andauthorprofilesforthispublicationat:
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/229984645
Mutualandunrequitedlovein
adolescenceandyoungadulthood
ARTICLEinPERSONALRELATIONSHIPS·MAY2005
ImpactFactor:1.41·DOI:10.1111/j.1475-6811.1997.tb00127.x
CITATIONS
4
READS
150
3AUTHORS,INCLUDING:
CraigA.Hill
IndianaUniversity-PurdueUnive…
26PUBLICATIONS652CITATIONS
SEEPROFILE
JudithE.OwenBlakemore
IndianaUniversity-PurdueUnive…
26PUBLICATIONS249CITATIONS
SEEPROFILE
Availablefrom:CraigA.Hill
Retrievedon:13February2016
Pi~rsonul
Relafion.ships,
4
(l997),
15-23. Printed in the LJnitcd
States
of
America.
Copyright
0
1007
Cambridge Ilnivcrsity Press.
1350-4126197 $730
+
.10
Mutual and unrequited love in
adolescence and young adulthood
CRAIG
A.
HILL,
JUDITH
E.
OWEN
BLAKEMORE,
AND
PATRICK
DRUMM
Indiuna
Univ~rsity-I’urdu~
Univevsily
ut
Fort
Wayne
Abstract
The
purpose
ol‘
the
study was
to
cxaminc
the
I‘rcqucncy
of
experiencing passionate
lovc
during
adolescence
and young adulthood
as
rccallcd by young
adults,
and
to
examine gender differences in this lrcqucncy. Female
and male
US.
introductory psychology students, several
01
whom were
of
nontraditional
age,
responded to a
questionnaire that asked
about
the
t’requcncy of their mutual and unrequited love experiences. Respondents
provided information for four age periods ranging from childhood
to
age
25 years. Although
the
frequency
of
recalled mutual love cxpcricnccs increased for
both
genders from childhood through
the
16-to-20-year-age
period, men rcportcd morc episodes
or
unrcquitcd
love
during
the
16-to-20-year-agc period compared
to
(a)
other
age
periods, (b) their reports ol mutual
love
during
tliiit
age
period,
and
(c) the reports
of
unrequited
love
for 16-to-20-year-old
women.
Some years ago Dorothy Tennov proposed
the concept
of
limerence
to
describe the ex-
perience
of
intcnse emotional and sexual
attraction for a desired romantic partner
(Tennov, 1979). Other theorists (e.g., Hat-
field
&
Rapson,
1993)
have called this same
experience “passionate love.” Although
Tennov (1979) explored the phenomenon
of passionate love with both interviews and
questionnaires, the main thrust of her evi-
dence took the form
of
personal stories of
people obsessed with passionate feelings
toward another person. Little formal statis-
tical analysis was presented
to
document
the experience of liinerence.
Tennov described the feeling
of
limer-
ence
as
having several components. These
included intrusive thinking about the per-
We
are
grateful to
Ken
Borciens for comments on an
earlier version
or
this article. We also thank Scott
Bradtmillcr
and
Nancy Fitch for assistance with data
collection.
Address reprint requests
lo
Craig Hill (c-mail ad-
dress:
HILLC<(?CVAX.IPFW.INDIANA.EDU),
Ju-
dith Blakcinore, or Patrick Drumm,
Department
of
Psychological Sciences, Indiana University-Purduc
University Fort Wayne, Fort Wayne, IN
46805-1499,
son, a strong wish for the person
to
recipro-
cate the feelings, the inability
to
have such
feelings for more than one person
at
once,
a
fear
ol
rejection, sensitivity to signs of
intcrest on the other’s part, and a tendency
to dwell on the other’s positive charac-
teristics and avoid the negative.
In
begin-
ning her work, Tennov believed that the
feeling
or
limerencc was universal. How-
ever, she eventually discovered people who
reported never having experienced such
feelings.
Since Tennov published her book, the
study
of
passionate love
has
grown enor-
mously. Elaine Hatfield and her colleagues
have accumulated considcrable evidence
concerning the nature
of
passionate love
(Hatfield, 1988; Hatfield, Brinton,
&
Cor-
nelius, 1989; Hatfield
&
Rapson, 1987a;
Hatfield, Schmitz, Cornelius,
&
Rapson,
1988; Hatfield
&
Sprecher,
l986),
a phe-
nomenon that is virtually identical to the
notion of limerence. Hatfield and her col-
leagues defined passionate love, also re-
ferred
to
as “puppy love,” “infatuation,” or
“lovesickness,”
as
a kind of infatuation that
obsessively consumes one’s thoughts. Re-
15
16
C.A.
Hill,
J.E.
Owen Blakemore, and
R
Drumm
scarch in this area (Hatfield
&
Rapson,
1987b) has focused on passionate love
as
an
emotional state with neurochemical under-
pinnings. Zt is described as a very primitive
cmotion that may first be experienced prior
to
puberty (Hatfield et al., 1988), and is
thought to be culturally and historically
universal. Hatficld and Rapson (1987b), as
well as Berscheid
(1988),
argue that
pas-
sionate love is heavily influenced by and
greatly overlaps with, sexual desire.
Arguing from
a
different theoretical
perspective, some anthropologists and evo-
lutionary psychologists construe the hu-
man experience
of
passionate love as the
product
of
natural and sexual selection
(Buss,
1988; Fisher,
1987,
1989, 1992). Buss
(I
988)
has argued that certain behavior
patterns associated with the subjective ex-
perience
of
love exist because they have
been selected through the process
of
evo-
lution. Feelings
of
passionate love result in
actions that have evolutionary significance,
like
the
choice of a mate, intercourse, con-
ception, and reproduction. Another evolu-
tionary account (Fisher, 1987, 1989, 1992)
proposes that repeatedly falling in love,
and the resulting series
of
mates, created
genetic variation in offspring that led to
their disproportionate survival. From this
account, we would expect to see people ex-
periencing repeated episodes of passionate
love.
Though rcsearchers and theorists have
described the nature of passionate love in
some dctail, both the prevalence and the
frequency
of
experiencing these feelings
have not yet been clearly established. Ten-
nov
(1
979) reported several instances of
people who had no limerent experiences, an
occurrence of unknown frequency.
Is
pas-
sionate love virtually universal, or are there
substantial individual differences in the
phenomenon‘? For example, cross-cultural
evidence suggests that reliable gender dif-
ferences exist in patterns
of
mate choice
(Buss, 1994). One
of
the predictions
of
Buss’s
(1
988) sexual strategies theory is
that males will desire a greater number
of
short-term partners than will females.
Based on this prediction, gender differ-
ences might
be
expected in the frequency
of
reported love experiences.
Few
researchers investigating passionate
love have distinguished between mutual
love and love that is not returned by the
object of one’s desire (for cxarnple,
Baumeister
&
Wotman, 1992; and Baumeis-
ter, Wotman,
&
Stillwell, 1993, discuss cxpe-
riences of unrequited love but make few
comparisons to the experience of mutual
love).
In
addition, while some researchers
(Hatfield et al., 1988) have reported that
the experience
of
romantic love occurs in
children, they have not examined how the
experience progresses developmentally
through adolescence and young adulthood.
The major purpose of the present study,
then, was to examine the frequency
of
expe-
riencing passionate love during adoles-
cence and young adulthood, as recalled by
young adults, and to examine gender differ-
ences in this frequency. Several researchers
who have studied passionate or romantic
love (e.g., Thompson
&
Borrello, 1992) be-
lieve that obsessive thought about the tar-
get
of
one’s feelings is one of the major
defining aspects of this experience. Conse-
quently, obsessive thought is the key expe-
rience we focused on in this research.
Be-
cause Tennov (1979) reported that the
mean duration of such experiences was
2
years, and because relationships may out-
live the obsessive feelings she called limer-
ence, we asked our respondents to limit
their descriptions to the first
2
years when
reporting on long-lasting relationships.
To
summarize, we asked college students
to recall their mutual and unrequited love
experiences
to
determine the hquency
of
these experiences at different ages.
In
addi-
tion, based
on
the predictions of evolution-
ary theory (Buss, 1994), we hypothesized
that males would report more of such expe-
riences than would females.
Method
Participants
One-hundred-forty introductory psychol-
ogy students
(86
females,
54
males) at a
Mutual and unrequited love
17
regional commuter campus
of
a state uni-
versity
in
the United States participated in
the study as partial fulfillment of a course
experimental requirement. Participants
ranged in age from 18 to 43 years
(M
=
22.31;
SD
=
5.92).
As
can be seen from the
range and standard deviation, several of
these students were
of
nontraditional age;
31.43%) were 22 years
of
age or older. Sev-
enty percent
of
the participants had never
been married, 7.86% were engaged to
be
married,
lS.OOo/o
were legally married, and
7.14% were separated or divorced. Most
participants (52.86%) were involved in a
serious romantic relationship, 19.28% were
dating or seeing someone, and 27.86% were
not involved in a romantic relationship. The
majority (76.43%)
of
the participants were
white, 2.86% were African-American, and
20.71
%
werc
of
some other racial/ethnic
background. Likewise, the majority were
Protestant (37.86%), 25.71
YO
were Catho-
lic, 28.57% were other religions, and 7.86%
were not identified with a religion. The ma-
jority labelled their primary occupation as
“student” (62.86%), 8.57% labelled their
occupation as “clerical or service worker,”
5.61
YO
labelled their occupation as “sales-
person,” and 22.96% employed some other
labcl.
Materials
and
procedure
Participants completed a questionnaire con-
cerning demographic information and love
experiences (both mutual and unrequited).
They were also asked about personality
characteristics and love styles; however,
these latter questions will not be discussed
in this paper. Written instructions were pro-
vided
for
each section of the questionnaire.
Demographic
in,forrnation.
The first sev-
eral questions concerned gender, age, ra-
ciallethnic background, marital and rela-
tionship status. We asked about both
relationship and marital status in two dif-
ferent questions.
Love
experience.
The second section
of
the
questionnaire focused on participants’ re-
ports
of
love
expericnccs. The
following
written instructions preccdcd the questions
about the cxperience
of
passionate love:
This study is concerned with the experience of
romantic love. This experience is sometimes
callcd “being in lovc,” “obsessive lovc,” “infatu-
ation,” “lovesickness,” “puppy love,” or “having
a crush.” When one is expcriencing this emotion,
it has been described as having one’s emotions
on
a roller coaster, finding it difficult
to
conccn-
trate, and thinking constantly about the person
with whom you arc in love. The person is said to
have the power to produce extreme highs and
lows of emotion in you, dcpcnding on how
he
or
she acts towards you.
Sometimes this feeling is reciprocated, and
the two people may be considered to
be
“in
love” with each other. Other times,
a
person may
feel this way toward someone who doesn’t re-
turn the feeling (unrequited love). In this casc,
the object
of
one’s love is sometimes highly un-
likely to return the feeling
(e.g.,
one’s physician,
teacher, professor
or
therapist, or
a
celebrity
such as a movie star or a rock musician). and
sometimes it is simply a member
oi
your social
group who simply doesn’t feel about you in the
manner you feel about them.
We would like to know about your cxpcri-
ences with this emotional state; however, we
want to separate the timcs when it was rccipro-
cated from those when you “loved” someone
who didn’t return your love.
When you complete the questionnaire, it will
be helpful to try to recollect the actual people
with whom you were “in love.” Try to remember
each
of
the experiences as vividly as you
can-exactly how you felt, the events that oc-
curred leading up
to
your feelings about the
person, and the specific ways they reacted to-
ward you, if you had contact with them. On the
scratch paper we have provided, you might
list
the people’s initials to assist you in remember-
ing. That will help you
to
be as accurate as
pos-
sible when recalling and reconstructing these ex-
periences.
You
may discard the scratch paper or
take it with you when you leave.
When you describe these experiences, only
consider those situations in which you felt this
way about someone for
at least a month’s dura-
tion.
Additionally, while the experience
of
ro-
mantic love often leads to permanent, long-term
relationships, we are
not
focusing
on
those kinds
of
relationships in this research. Therefore, when
answering the following questions, consider
only
18
C.A.
Hill,
J.E.
Owen
Blakemore, and
I?
Drumm
the,
fi’nt
two
ymrs
of
love relationships
that
lastcd
longer
than
that
time.
Do
not continue
to
dcscribe
a
long-term
love
relationship
beyond
thc
lirst
two
years
of
that
relationship.
In
order to encourage participants to re-
call
as
accurately as possible, the instruc-
tions provided specific recommendations,
such as trying to remember each experi-
ence as vividly as they could and using
scratch paper provided with the question-
nairc to list
tbc
initials
of
those who were
loved. These instructions were followed by
questions on mutual (i.e., reciprocated) and
unrcquitcd (i.e., unreciprocated) love.
Al-
though we asked participants about the fre-
quency, duration, and intensity
of
these re-
lationships, only the frequency data will be
reported here because frequency
is
the
main focus
of
the study.]
A
nulyses
Rcported frequencies of mutual and unre-
quited love were adjusted for the age pe-
riod within which the participant’s age fell
at
thc time of the survey, if the participant
had not yet reached the maximum age for
that agc period. For example, if
a
partici-
pant at the time
of
the survey
was
22 years
old, then that individual would not have
completed the
21
-to-25-year-old age range
designated
as
a
reporting period
in
the
study. Consequently, the frequency of love
experience reported by that individual
would cover only one-fifth of the specified
age period. These data were therefore pro-
jected based
on
(a) participants’ actual
ages;
(b)
the proportion
of
the age period
participants had completed; and (c) the fre-
quency they reported.2 Projected frequen-
cies were included with the unmodified re-
ported frequencies in analyses examining
differences across age periods, depending
on the participant’s age.
Two sets of analyses were conducted:
one that included
all
participants and a sec-
ond analysis that comprised the subset
of
participants who were older than 20 years.
In the analysis that included all participants,
a
3
(age period)
X
2 (gender)
X
2 (type of
love) repeated measures analysis of vari-
ance (ANOVA) was used to determine
whether there were developmental differ-
ences in males’ and females’ reports of mu-
tual and unrequited love. Age period and
type of love were within-subject factors.
Gender was a between-subjects factor.
To
examine differences in frequencies
reported for the earlier age periods with the
21-to-25-year age period, a second repeated
measures analysis
was
conducted with all
four age periods included in the age-period
factor; however, the second analysis in-
volved only those participants who were
older than 20 years
(N
=
50). Comparisons
among means were conducted employing
Tukey’s honestly significant difference
method in order to examine the pattern
of
the means for the significant effects
(p’s
for
all comparisons
<
.05).
I.
lntcnsity was assessed
on
a
7-point Likcrt scale,
with
7
reprcsenting the greatest intensity.
The
incan
intcnsity
of
mutual lovc was 4.63
lor
men and
4.W
lor
wonicn,
t(
135)
I
.41,
p
>
.05.
Thc mean
intcnsity of unrequited love was 3.95 for men and
3.74 for women, t(lO6)
=
0.65,p
>
.05.
Duration
of
passionatc
lovc experiences was estimatcd within
thc
following intervals:
(a)
less than 3 months,
(13)
3
to
6
months, (c)
6
months to
1
year,
(d)
1
to
2
ycars,
and
(c)
2 or more years. These intervals were
codcd as
1
through
5,
respectively. Mean values for
duration of mutual love experiences were 2.99 for
men
and
3.5
I
for women,
t(
13.5)
=
2.86,
p
<
.01;
that is about
6
months to
1
year.
Mean
values for
duration
of
unrequited love experiences were 2.01
lor
men and 2.45
lor
women,
t(
112)
=
2.07,
p
<
.05;
that
is
betwecn 3
and
6
months.
Results
All respondents reported at least one love
experience, although nine individuals
2. Thus for the 16-20-year-agc pcriod:
at
age
18,
pro-
jected frequency
:
reported frcqucncy
X
1.67,
and
at
age 19, projected frequency
=
rcported fre-
quency
X
1.25. Similarly,
for
the 21-25-year-age
pcriod, at age 21, projected frequency
=
reported
frequency
X
5; at age
22,
projected frequency
=
re-
ported frequency
X
2.5; at age
23,
projected fre-
qucncy
=
reported frcqucncy
X
1.67; and at age
24, projected frequency
=
reporlcd frcqucncy
X
1.25.
Mutual and unrequited
love
I
0
(6.43%) reported only one episode, with
the sole experience for all nine rcspon-
dents involving mutual love. Additionally,
three respondents (2.14%) reported no
mutual love experiences evcr, although all
of them rcported more than one unre-
quited love expericnce. At the othcr ex-
trcme, 29 individuals (20.7
1
%)
reported
more than five mutual lovc expericnces
and more than five unrequited love expe-
riences. More generally, the average
number
of
rcported experiences was 10.6
1
(no differcnce was round between men
and women); the median numbcr was
X.00,
and the modal numbcr was
4.
The average
number of mutual love experiences was
5.35; the median number was 4.00, and the
modal numbcr was 1.00. The averagc
number of unrequited love cxperienccs
was 5.26; the median number was 3.00, and
the modal number was
0.00.
The corrcla-
tion between the overall frequency
of,
mu-
tual and unrcquited love was .21
(p
<
.(IS).
positive but certainly not large.
Frequency o/'mutual and unrequited love
across
four
uge periods
The adjusted mcan number
of
times that
respondcnts reported expcriencing mutual
and unrequited love for three of the four
age periods (because not all participants
were older than 20 years of age) can
be
hound in Table
1.
To
determine whether
passionate love occurs more frequently at
certain developmcntal periods than others,
the values were subjected to a repeated
measures analysis
of
variance, including
two within-subjects factors-uge
period
when the love was experienced (youngcr
than 10 years old, 10-15 years old, 16-20
years old) and
t.ype
of
love
(mutual or un-
requited)-and one between-subjects fac-
tor,
gender
of
respondent.
Threc effects
were statistically significant in this
ANOVA: (a) age period when the passion-
ate love was experienced, F(2, 276)
=
92.04,
p
<
.0001;
(b) the two-way interac-
tion
of
gender and typc
of
love (mutual
vs. unrequited),
F(1,
138)
=
6.71,
p
<
.011;
and (c) the three-way interaction
of
gen-
der,
age
period, and type
ol
love,
Ic(2,
276)
=
7.6X,
p
<
,001.
With rccpect
to
the
cllect
loi
age
period,
thc mean number
ol
tiincs
I
cspondcnls rc-
ported experiencing love before
1
he
dgc
of
10
was 0.74 times (collapsing acro5s gcncicr
and type ot love),
I.%
times
from
10
to
15
years old, and 2.29 times
1
rom
I6
to 20 yen
old. Compai
icons
among
the
mccins indi-
cated that
all
means
were
significantly
dil-
fercnt lrom the other
mcms.
1171s
piltcrn
ol
ditfcrcnces
suggc5ts
thd
thc rrcclucncy
01
cxpcriencing love increased dcro4s
c,ich
4y
period, regardless
of
gender.
Although thc repented mccisui
cs
ANOVA revealed a 5ignilicant
cllcct
lor
the two-way interaction
bctwccn
gender
and type
of
love, comparisons
mwng
1
he
mean trcquencics tor each lcvcl
ol
the
con-
bined factors revealed no signilic<int dillcr-
enccs. Consqucntly,
this
ctlcct
will
not
be
discussed further.
With respect to
the
three-way rntcrac-
tion
among gender, age period, dnd type
ol
love, thc mean frequencies lor
each
levcl
of the thrcc factors arc presented
in
Idble
I.
The pattern
of
dittercnccs
suggests
thot,
although the frequency
of
tnulud love
in
creased tor both men and women across
the three age periods, the pattcrii was dil-
fcrent tor unrequited love For
mcilcs,
the
mean numbcr
of
reported unrcyuiicd
cx-
pcriences
increa4ed s~gnilic~iiitly
t
rom
youngcr
lo
older age
peiiod5,
with unrc
quited expcnences outnum bcring mutual
cxperienccs in the 10-1
5
md 16-20
,igc
pc-
riods and pcaking between
I6
and 20 years
ot agc. For women, the mean iiumbcr
of
reported unrequited
experiences
incrc,ised
significantly, but moderatcly, between the
ages of
10
and 15 ycars,
and
rcm,iincd
cs-
sentially the samc between
16
mcl
20 yc'irs
The most pronounced
eflcct,
thcrclorc, 'tp-
pears to be the markedly higher lrcqucncy
of unrequited lovc reported by
men
in
late
adolescence (16
to
20 ycarc
old),
(a) com-
pared
to
other agc periods, (b) compared
to the experience of mutual love reported
during the 16-to-20-year pcrtod,
and
(c)
compared to the experience ot unrequited
love reported by women in the
lalc
adolcs-
20
C.A.
Hill,
J.
E.
Owen
Blakemcm,
and
P
Drumm
l’able
1.
Means and
,Ytun(iard
deviations
of
the frequency
of
mutual
and
unrequited
rotnatalic
lovr
rrculled
lor
three age
periods
Jbr
all
respondents
Men
Womcn
Var
ia
b
Ic
M
S
n
M
sn
Mutual
Lovc
Youngci
than
10
ycars
old
0.6P
1
.l6
0.5
1
A
0.89
10-15
yedrs
old
1.13” 1.23 1.3
1‘3
1
.hO
I6
20
years
old
2.26” 2.23 2.50‘ 2.21
Younger
than
10
ycai
b
old
1.04A
I
.72
0.80*
1.62
10-15
yca1s
old
I
.80‘%
2.18 1.36” 2.05
16-20
ycn1s
old 3.06‘
2.80 1.621’
2.02
lim
cquitetl
Lovc
Norca:
l+xliicncy
Iclcrs
to
the nunihcr
ol’
times
respondents
experienced romantic
love
or
inlatuation.
“Youngcl-
th;rn
10
years
old”
1-
lore
the
age
of
10:
“10-15
yea
to
the
nuinhcr
of
times
respondents experienced romantic
love
hc-
Id”
rcfcrs
to
the number
of
times they cxpcricnccd romantic love
igcs
of
10
and
IS,
ctc.
Means
within gender
and
type
ol‘
love
that
arc
marked with
dil-
ript
Ictlcrs
arc
significantly dillcrcnt
at
p
<
.05.
Means within
age
period and gender
(across
type
01
love)
ni;irkcd with asterisks
(:h)
arc
significantly different
at
p
<
.OS.
The significant
dilfci.cncc
hctwccn
iiicn
;ind
women
lor
unrcquited
love
froin
16-20
years
old
is denoted
with
a
iliifigcr
(I)
symbol
(p
<
.05).
N
=
54
incn and
86
women.
cent period.
Iii
short, these data indicate
that, although both men and women re-
ported the highest frequencies
of
mutual
love during
latc
adolescence,
inen
tended
to report even higher levels
of
unrequited
lovc during
this
age period, whereas
women reported lower levels compared
to
iiicii
and
compared to their experience of
mutual love.
7’hc
frequency
of
both types
of
love was
also
analy/ed separatcly
for
those who
were
older than
20
years
(N
=
50)
at the
time
of
the study to examine the reports
ot
pa4sionatc
love during the 21
-to-25-
year-old
agc
period.
As
before, a repeated
mea4urcs analysis was conducted, cniploy-
ing
two
within-subjects factors-age
~?erzod
when the passionate love was experienced
(younger than
10
years old, 10-15 years
old,
16-20
years
old,
21-25 years old) and
gyw
of
lovr (iiiutual or unrequited)-and
one hctwecn-sul7jects lactor, gender
of
re-
spondent. Again, a main ef€ect was ob-
tained for agc period when the passionate
love was experienccd,
F(3,
144)
=
17.21,
p
<
.OOOl.
The two-way interaction of gen-
der and type
of
love (mutual vs. unre-
quited) was also significant,
F(1,
48)
=
4.99,
p
<
.03,
although no differences were
significant in post hoc comparisons
of
means. While the three-way interaction of
gender, age period, and type of love was
not significant,? the two-way interaction
between age period and type
of
love was
significant,
F(3,
144)
=
2.67,
p
<
.05.
With respect to the effect for age period
for participants 21 years and older, the
mean number
of
times respondents re-
ported experiencing love before the age of
10
was 0.76 times (collapsing across gender
and type of love; see Table 2), 1.20 times
from
10
to
15
years old, 2.00 times from 16
to
20
years old, and 1.43 times from
21
to
25
years old. The only significant difference
was between the mean frequency of experi-
encing love before the age
of
10
(M
=
0.76)
and the mean frequency for the 16-20-year
3.
Another analysis was also conducted with only the
three carliest age groups,
as
was done
Tor
the cntii-e
sample.
In
this analysis, the thrcc-way interaction
for gender, age period, and
type
of
love was signifi-
cant,
F(2,
96)
=
3.41,
p
<
.OS,
as
was found
for
the
entire sample.
Mutual and unrequited love
21
Table
2.
Means and standard deviations
of
the frequency ofmutuul
and unrequited romantic love recalled for
four
age perioh for
respondents 21 years
of
age and older
Variable
M
s
I1
Mutual
Low
Younger than
10
years
old
10-1
5
ycars
old
16-20
ycars
old
21-25
years
old
Unrequited
Love
Younger than
10
years
old
1
&I
5
years
old
16-20
years
old
21-25
years
old
0.42" 0.79
1.24"9" 1.52
2.12': 1.72
1.78Rq': 1.60
0.68"
1.32
1.34A.B 2.21
2.06H 2.42
I
.09"
2.48
Note:
Frcquency refcrs to
the
number
oC
timcs respondents expcricnwd romantic
lovc
or infatuation. "Younger than
10
ycars
old" refcrs
lo
the number
ol'
timcs
re-
spondcnts cxperienced romantic love
hcforc
thc agc of
10;
"10-15
ycars old"
rcfcrs
to
thc number of timcs they expericnccd romantic love bctwccn thc ages
of
10
and
15,
etc. Mcans within type of love that are markcd with different supcrscripl letters
arc significantly diffcrent at
p
<
.05.
N
=
20
men and
30
women.
period
(M
=
2.09). Although the pattern of
differcnces in general was similar to that
obtaincd for all participants, the results ad-
ditionally suggest that during the 21-to-25-
year age period the experience of passion-
ate love returns to levels similar to that
occurring in the 10-to-15-year age period.
As
in the previous set of analyscs, no signifi-
cant differences were found in comparisons
among means based on the two-way inter-
action between gender and type of love.
Regarding the two-way interaction be-
tween age and type of love, the frequency of
both types
of
love increased from child-
hood through the 26-to-20-year age period,
while the pattern was different with respect
to the 21-to-2s-year age period. Specifically,
the frequency of unrequited love in the 21
-
to-25-year age period returned to lower
levels equivalent to the periods before age
16. In contrast, mutual love remained at
relatively higher levels
(M
=
1.78) in the
21-to-25-year age period as indicated by the
fact that the mean value was not signifi-
cantly different from that for the 16-to-20-
year age period.
Discussion
The purpose of the current study was to
document the frequency
of
mutual and un-
requited love across difrercnt age periods,
as reported retrospectively.
A
second pur-
pose was to examine gender differcnccs
in
these reports.
All
participants in the study rcported
having at lcast one experience of passion-
ate love. Most people we surveycd rcported
such experiences several timcs over the
course of adolescence and young adult-
hood. Because participants
of
the current
study were relatively young individuals
(mcan age
=
22.31), that
rnakcs
it
impossi-
ble to establish exactly
the
typical number
of
times that passionate love is expericnced
across an average lifetime. Future research
involving older participants is needed to
provide a more complete picture
of
thc cu-
mulative frequency of passionate love that
is commonly experienced. Despite this limi-
tation, the present data provide valuable
information on the occurrencc of passion-
ate love during the period
of
life in which
individuals typically begin to lcarn about
22
C.A.
Hill,
J.
E.
Owen
Blukernore,
and
I-1
Drumrn
cni
o
t
ion
a
I
i
n
volvcmcn
t
with potential ro-
in
antic
part
ncrs.
In
addition
to
documenting the overall
frcclucncy with which passionate love was
reported,
wc
also
examined the pattern of
love
rcportcd
;\cross
different dcvclopmcn-
t;il
periods.
‘l’hc
exixricnce
of
passionate
love
was
found to
bc
relatively
rare
before
the
age
ol’
I0
compircd to its experience
Isctwccn
the
ages
of
I6
and
20 years, lor
hoth
m:ilcs
and
fcinalcs,
and
regardless
of
whether thc lccling
was
mutual
or
not. The
cxpcricncc
of
mutual love wits similar
for
Imth
men ;tnd women iicross
all
the age
pcriocls nicasurcd.
Whilc
we hypothcsizcd that men might
have
inorc
lovc
cxpcricnccs, this was found
lor
unrcquitcd experiences only, and only
during
the
period
from
16
to 20 years
of
age.
Men
reported
;I
markedly higher frequency
of
11
nrcq
u
i
tcd
I
ovc during late
adol
cscence
(
I0
to
20
years)
compared to other age
pc-
riods.
conip;rrcd
to
the frequency
of
mutual
love during the
1
6-to-20--ycar-old period,
and
compared
to
the
frequency
of
uiirc-
cluitcci
love
cxpericnced by women during
the
late
;idolcsccncc
period. l’his finding is
consistent with the rcscarch
of
Baumeister
ct
;it.
(I
093)
examining unrcyuitcd lovc
with
p;irticip;uits
in
roughly the same agc
range. ‘I’hcy
reported
that men
more
often
had
“would-be lover” experiences, whereas
wonicn more often had “rejector” expcri-
cnccs.
(’crtainly this issue
nceds
further
st
ucty.
A
ddifioiilil
c~otz.siticni/io~zs
‘I’hc
data
prcscntcd in the current study
were ohtuincd employing rctrospcctivc
self-rcports.
As
with
all
research based on
tlic
recall
01’
past events, the results may
have been affcctcd by crrors and distor-
tions
that
arc known
to
occur in human
memory proccsscs. However, the proce-
dure
cmploycd
in
the current design was
constructed to facilitate more accurate re-
call
of
cpisodcs
of
passionate
love.
Instruc-
tions
in
the
sclf-report instrumcnt re-
quested
that
respondents try to recollect
as
vividly
as
possible the actual people
with whom they were “in
10~~”
and to re-
member events that occurred during the
course
of
their infatuation with each pcr-
son.
Respondents were also provided with
a scratch paper
on
which they could
list
their romantic partners and makc notes
about them, if they desired. Finally, they
were asked to be as accurate as possible
in recalling their experiences.
One
indication that problems with inem-
ory did not systematically affect the recall
of
love wits the lack of influence
of
partici-
pant age on the results.
If
memory were
influencing recall of romantic episodcs,
older participants would presumably have
had greater difficulty remembering epi-
sodes in earlier age periods, resulting in sys-
tematic distortion for those periods. How-
ever, controlling for
age
of
participant
had
no effect
on
any of the repeated measures
analyses.
Conclusions
Passionate love was experienced by every
participant in the current study.
In
fact,
the
typical individual reported more than one
experience of love over the course of ado-
lescence and young adulthood. The fre-
quency
of
these experiences tended
to
in-
crease from childhood to late adolesccnce
and early adulthood. Gender differences
did not occur in the mean number
of
over-
all passionate love experiences, nor were
gender differences found in the pattern
of
mutual love across developmental periods.
However, the pattern
of
frequency was dif-
fercnt with rcspect
to
unrequited love.
Men reported increasing numbers of un-
rcyuited love experiences that surpassed
the number of mutual experiences
in
both
of the adolescent age periods (10-15 years
old and 16-20 years
old).
Unrequited love
culminated for men during the 16-20-year-
old period and dropped only slightly in the
early adult period.
Women, in contrast, reported a signifi-
cant increase in unrequited love after
10
years
of
age,
a
level that remained constant
through the 16-20-year period and that was
significantly lower than the number of
mu-
Mutual and unrequited love
23
tual experiences they reported,
as
well
as
being significantly lower than that for
males. The highest frequency
of
passionate
love in general occurred in late adolescence
for both men and women, although the
more frequent type
of
love for males was
unrequited, whereas the more frequent
type for females was mutual love.
Future research must include individuals
older than
25
years
of
age in order to estab-
References
Baumeister, R. F.,
&
Wotman,
S.
R.
(1992).
Breaking
hearts: The two
sides
of
unrequited love.
New York:
Guilford
Press.
Baumeistcr, R.
F.,
Wolnian,
S.
R.,
&
Stillwcll,
A.
M.
(1993). Unrequited love: On heartbreak, anger,
guilt, scriptlessness, and humiliation.
Journal
of
Per.sonatity
und
Social Psychology,
64,
377-394.
Berscheid, E. (1988). Some comments on love’s anat-
omy: Or, whatever happened to old-fashioned lust?
In R.
J.
Sternberg
&
M.
L.
Barnes (Eds.),
The psy-
chology
of
love
(pp. 359-374). New Haven, CT
Yale [Jniversity Press.
Borrello,
G.
M.,
&
Thompson,B. (1990).
A
note regard-
ing the validity of Lee’s typology
of
love.
The Juur-
nu1 of KTychology,
124,
639-644.
Buss, D.
M.
(1988).
Love acts:
The
evolutionary hiology
of
lovc. ln
R.
J.
Sternberg
&
M.
L.
Barnes (Eds.),
‘/’he
psychology
of
love
(pp.
100-1
18).
Ncw Haven,
CT
Yale University Press.
Buss,
D.
M.
(1994). The strategies of human mating.
American Scientist,
82, 238-249.
Fisher, H.
E.
(1987, October). The four-year itch.
Natu-
rul History,
22-33.
Fisher, H.
E.
(1989). Evolution of human serial pair-
bonding.
American Journal
of
Physical Anthropol-
Fisher, H.
E.
(1992).
Anatomy
of
love: The natural
his-
tory
of
monogamy, udultery, and divorce.
New
York:
Norton.
ogy,
78,
33
1-354.
lish definitively the pattern
of
passionate
love across later developmental periods.
It
is
possible that the frequency of this kind
of
love may decline
as
individuals become in-
volved in committed relationships and may
eventually return
to
higher levels
as
the
emotional needs
of
individuals change in
later years of life-for example, bccausc of
dissatisfaction with current relationships,
divorce, or the
death
of
romantic partners.
Hatfield,
E.
(I
988). Passionate and companionate love.
In R.
J.
Sternberg
&
M.
L.
Barnes
(Eds.),
The psy-
chology
of
love
(pp. 191-217).
New
Haven,
CT
Yale University Press.
Hatfield,
E.,
Brinton,
C.,
Kc
Cornelius,
J.
(1989).
Pas-
sionate love and anxiety in young adolcscents.
Mo-
tivation und Emotion,
I.?,
27 I-2X9.
Hatfield,
E.,
&
Rapson,
R.
L.
(198721). Passionate lovc:
New directions in rcscarch.
In
W.
H.
Jones
&
D.
Perlman (Eds.),
A
dvances
in
prr.sonal re/ation.ships
(pp. 109-139). Greenwich,
CT:
JAI
Press.
Hatfield, E.,
&
Kapson, R.
L.
(1987b). Passionatc
loveisexual desire: Can the samc paradigm explain
both‘?
Archives oJSexuul Behavior,
16,
259-278.
Hatfield,
E.,
&
Rapson, R.
L.
(1993).
/,eve,
sex,
and
intimacy.
New York: HarperCollins.
Hatfield, E.,Schmitz,E.,
Cornclius,J.,&
Kapson,
R.
L.
(1988).
Passionate lovc:
How
early docs
it
begin?
Journal
of
P.sy:ychology
&
Human Sexuulity,
I,
35-52.
Hatfield,
E.,
&
Sprecher,
S.
(1986).
Measuring passion-
ate love in intimate rclationships.
Journal
oj
Ado-
lescence,
9, 38341 0.
Tennov,
D.
(1979).
Love
und
limewnce: The txperience
of
being
in
love.
Chelsea,
MI:
Scarborough House.
Thompson,
B.,
&
Borrcllo,
G.
M.
(1992).
Dilfcrent
views
of
love: Deductive and inductive lines
of
inquiry.
Current t)irc:cfions
in
Psychological
Sci-
ence,
1,
154-1
55.
... As to UL, previous research has demonstrated both quantitative and qualitative differences between men and women. Hill et al. (1997) compared men and women in terms of the frequency of mutual love and UL in different age periods, and men during the 16-to-20-year age period reported more episodes of UL than women in the same age period. Baumeister et al.'s (1993) study showed that men and women experienced UL in different ways. ...
... To distinguish between different types of UL, we could have asked more questions such as whether the participants knew the beloved ones in person, whether they had made efforts in pursuing the beloved ones, and the length of the relationships. For instance, in an aforementioned study comparing UL and mutual love, Hill et al. (1997) asked the participants to report their UL that had lasted at least a month, and this approach ruled out those experiences of short-term "crush." Had we asked these questions and included them as the covariates in the data analyses of this study, we could have lessened the confounding effects caused by the possible mixture of different types of UL. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined people’s expected reactions to their beloved ones’ romantic relationship announcements in parasocial love (PSL) and unrequited love (UL). In a between-subjects design study, 330 college students in China were asked to name a media figure with whom they had PSL or someone they knew in person who had been their UL object. Then they reported their expected negative emotional reactions, closeness reduction, and coping strategies if the media figure or the love object announces romantic relationship involvement. PSL was found to be lower than UL in strength. People with stronger PSL and UL are expected to have more negative emotional reactions. People in PSL reported less negative emotional reactions and less closeness reduction in contrast to their counterparts in UL. The likeliness to adopt different coping strategies varied by the type of relationship and gender.
... Romantic love may be more common among adolescents than young adults. In one study (Hill et al., 1997), American university psychology students reported a greater occurrence of mutual and unrequited love experiences when they were 16-20 years old compared to when they were 21-25 years old. However, romantic love exists at all ages of adulthood in both sexes (Wang and Nguyen, 1995). ...
... Romantic love serves a mate choice and courtship function, and as a result, a large proportion of people experiencing romantic love are not in a relationship with their loved one (e.g., Bringle et al., 2013). A small number of studies have directly investigated unrequited love (e.g., Tennov, 1979;Baumeister et al., 1993;Hill et al., 1997;Aron et al., 1998;Bringle et al., 2013), but none of these investigated the mechanisms that cause romantic love. Studying such people might identify the specific contributions of particular mechanisms to particular functions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Romantic love is a phenomenon of immense interest to the general public as well as to scholars in several disciplines. It is known to be present in almost all human societies and has been studied from a number of perspectives. In this integrative review, we bring together what is known about romantic love using Tinbergen’s “four questions” framework originating from evolutionary biology. Under the first question, related to mechanisms, we show that it is caused by social, psychological mate choice, genetic, neural, and endocrine mechanisms. The mechanisms regulating psychopathology, cognitive biases, and animal models provide further insights into the mechanisms that regulate romantic love. Under the second question, related to development, we show that romantic love exists across the human lifespan in both sexes. We summarize what is known about its development and the internal and external factors that influence it. We consider cross-cultural perspectives and raise the issue of evolutionary mismatch. Under the third question, related to function, we discuss the fitness-relevant benefits and costs of romantic love with reference to mate choice, courtship, sex, and pair-bonding. We outline three possible selective pressures and contend that romantic love is a suite of adaptions and by-products. Under the fourth question, related to phylogeny, we summarize theories of romantic love’s evolutionary history and show that romantic love probably evolved in concert with pair-bonds in our recent ancestors. We describe the mammalian antecedents to romantic love and the contribution of genes and culture to the expression of modern romantic love. We advance four potential scenarios for the evolution of romantic love. We conclude by summarizing what Tinbergen’s four questions tell us, highlighting outstanding questions as avenues of potential future research, and suggesting a novel ethologically informed working definition to accommodate the multi-faceted understanding of romantic love advanced in this review.
... In comparison, the friendzone also embraces the mismatch in disinterest, while also trying to (re)negotiate friendship or romantic boundaries (see on-again/off-again relationships). Unrequited love (unreciprocated love) is usually compared to mutual love (reciprocated; see Hill et al., 1997)-this broad overlapping encapsulation covers three types that are not distinguished with the operationalization in previous scholarship only the conceptualization of unrequited love. This study offers further exploration of early states of romantic development and friendship rejection, or unreciprocal feelings. ...
Article
This study explores the friendzone, or the lexicon surrounding the nexus between platonic and romantic relationships. The friendzone situates layperson and scholarly depictions of platonic and romantic relationships. Minimal scholarship explores this common vernacular from either perspective—those that communicate romantic attraction or those that react. Specifically, this study investigates how initiators (those who communicate interest) and respondents (those who react to declarations) participate in the communicative and relational process of friendzoning. Participants ( N = 787) completed open-ended questions about their friendzone experiences, communication from initiators, reaction of respondents, and changes to friendships. This study utilizes analytic induction to provide further conceptualization and delineation of the friendzone through identifying friendzoning types, initiator communication strategies, respondent reactions, and relationship changes. Findings allowed for: the conceptualizing of the friendzone, highlighting obstacles for navigating non-normative relationship scripts, and identifying risks involved with relationship change.
... Male gullibility may thus be reflected in remaining emotionally attached to a woman beyond the point that is good for the man. Further supporting this notion is evidence that men report more experiences of unrequited love in young adulthood relative to experiences of mutual love, and more unrequited love than women (Hill, Blakemore & Drumm, 1997; see also Baumeister et al., 1993); again suggesting men may naively remain attached to unreciprocating women. ...
... Perhaps, the strongest "tip off" that one is in love are the obsessive thoughts that passionate lovers have towards their object of their desire. Such thoughts might almost define this experience (Hill, Blakemore & Drumm, 1997). When asked about how much time one thinks about the beloved, passionate lovers will probably say that they spend 90%, or all the time, thinking about the loved one . ...
Thesis
The main goal of this thesis is to explore the romantic feelings of passionate love widely defined as a state of longing with another. This construct is generally considered to be a universal experience strongly associated with sexual arousal and capable of having a strong effect in emotional, cognitive and behavioral dimensions. The main goal of this project is to provide further evidence about the contention that although subjective experiences of passionate love are culturally and contextually determinate, people all over the world present the same symptoms of passionate love with the same intensity when they consider being in love. Plus, the influences of passionate love on cognitive processes were tested in other studies. A total of 1000 college students participated in 4 different studies. The Passionate Love Scale (PLS) was administrated on Brazilian and French subjects in order to explore their evaluation of passionate love through cognitive, emotional and behavioral components. The social representations of these same groups about passionate love were explored with a structural analysis of word associations. Cognitive processes were tested through one study about the relationship between passionate love and sensory experience and another one about the effect of passionate love in creative productions. The results found with the PLS indicated the same psychometric properties in France and in Brazil. In both cases, the factorial analysis indicated one stronger dimension with high internal consistencies. Subjects in love seemed to love with equal passion in both cultures but gender differences were found in Brazil. The analysis of the word association revealed contextual, cultural and gender differences. Passionate love had a positive effect in low cognitive processes (physical attraction and sensory experience) but no effect in high cognitive tasks (divergent and convergent thinking). The results of these different studies are presented and discussed in the light of cross-cultural, neuropsychological and evolutionary perspectives on romantic love. Passionate love might be experienced in a number of ways but its manifestation is universally the same. It is concluded that passionate love might be mainly a biological phenomenon with minor cultural variations directed to insure reproductive success in our species.
Preprint
Romantic love is a psychobehavioral motivational state that facilitates pair-bonding in humans. Evolutionarily, it is thought to help establish and maintain long-term pair-bonds that enhance a heterosexual couple’s reproductive fitness. Little is known about romantic love in sexual minority groups. This study investigated romantic love in a cross-cultural sample of heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual young adults (N = 783) experiencing romantic love from the Romantic Love Survey 2022. We analyzed differences between sexual orientation groups on six variables pertaining to different aspects of romantic love. These variables included number of times ever in love; the timing of falling in love relative to starting a romantic relationship; intensity of romantic love; obsessive thinking about a loved one; commitment; and frequency of sex per week. Homosexual females reported a significantly lower frequency of sex per week than heterosexual and bisexual females. Bisexual males reported a greater number of times ever in love than heterosexual males. Other variables revealed small or very small differences between sexual orientation groups. This study provides initial findings indicating that romantic love manifests in somewhat different ways across sexual orientation groups.
Preprint
Common conceptions of romantic love suggest that romantic love is associated with increased sexual activity with more frequent sex in the earlier stages of a romantic relationship. To our knowledge, no studies have investigated individual-level factors and sexual frequency using a validated measure of romantic love. This study tested a number of hypotheses about the factors associated with sexual frequency among 720 sexually active young adults experiencing romantic love from the Romantic Love Survey 2022. We hypothesized that relationship duration, the intensity of romantic love and obsessive thinking about a loved one, commitment, and elevated mood would be associated with sexual frequency. Using hierarchical linear regression, controlling for known covariates, we found no significant associations between any of our variables of interest and the frequency of sexual activity. The findings are surprising and highlight how little is currently known about sexual activity and sexual desire among individuals experiencing romantic love.
Article
The present study examined the roles of people's romantic beliefs and imagined interactions in unrequited love. Two hundred and twenty five Chinese college students participated in a survey study. More idealized romantic beliefs were associated with stronger unrequited love. Men's unrequited love was stronger than women's. Would-be lovers who had imagined interactions with beloved ones had stronger unrequited love in contrast to those who didn’t. Unrequited love and romantic beliefs were both positively related to the imagined interaction attributes of frequency and valence and the functions of compensation and relationship maintenance. Frequency of imagined interactions positively mediated the relationship between romantic beliefs and unrequited love.
Article
Full-text available
Cambridge Core - Social Psychology - The New Psychology of Love - edited by Robert J. Sternberg
Article
Full-text available
Unreciprocated romantic attraction was explored by comparing narrative accounts. Unrequited love emerged as a bilaterally distressing experience marked by mutual incomprehension and emotional interdependence. Would-be lovers looked back with both positive and intensely negative emotions, whereas rejectors were more uniformly negative in their accounts. Unlike rejectors, would-be lovers believed that the attraction had been mutual, that they had been led on, and that the rejection had never been communicated definitely. Rejectors depicted themselves as morally innocent but still felt guilty about hurting someone; many rejectors depicted the would-be lover's persistent efforts as intrusive and annoying. Rejectors constructed accounts to reduce guilt, whereas disappointed lovers constructed them to rebuild self-esteem. Rejectors saw would-be lovers as self-deceptive and unreasonable; would-be lovers saw rejectors as inconsistent and mysterious. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Modern humans have inherited the mating strategies that led to the success of their ancestors. These strategies include long-term mating, short-term mating, extra-pair mating, mate poaching, and mate guarding. This article presents empirical evidence supporting evolution-based hypotheses about the complexities of these mating strategies. Since men and women historically confronted different adaptive problems in the mating domain, the sexes differ profoundly in evolved strategic solutions. These differences include possessing different mate preferences, different desires for short-term mating, and differences in the triggers that evoke sexual jealousy. The study of human mating is one of the “success stories” of evolutionary psychology.
Chapter
In the 1960s and 1970s, experimental social psychologists who began to investigate intimacy soon found that such research was still taboo. Intimacy researchers were denounced by irate politicians, religious leaders, people-on-the-street, and even their own colleagues (Berscheid & Walster, 1978; Wexler, 1979). In the last few years, however, intimacy research has become not only respectable but fashionable. Social psychologists have begun to theorize about intimacy (Berscheid & Walster, 1978; Cook & McHenry, 1979; Hatfield, Traupmann, Spreacher, Utne, & Hay, 1982; Kelley, 1979; Stoller, 1979; Walster & Walster, 1977). Feminists agree that it is a profoundly important concern (Firestone, 1970). Marxists have discovered it (Foucault, 1978; Poster, 1978; Zaretsky, 1976). Even gerontologists have begun to recognize its importance (Huyck, 1977; Reedy & Birren, 1978; Stinnett, Carter, & Montgomery, 1972).
Article
In the present study, we investigated how well Lee's (1973/1976) typology of love characterized response data that adult subjects provided on selected items written by Hendrick and Hendrick. Results suggest that the model represents response data reasonably well and that Hendrick and Hendrick's (1986) measure ought to be valid.
Article
compares Stendhal's view of passionate love with the writings of the Christian mystics several major literary works are mined for their potential ability to enrich our understanding of modern passionate love (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Two studies tested the hypothesis that young adolescents who experience a great deal of anxiety will be more likely to experience passionate love as well. In the first study, 24 boys and 17 girls, ranging in age from 12 to 14, were given the Child Anxiety Scale (which measures trait anxiety) and the Juvenile Love Scale (JLS). In the second study, 32 adolescent girls and 32 adolescent boys, ranging in age from 13 to 16, were given the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (which measures both trait and state anxiety) and the JLS. The studies found significant correlations between anxiety (both state and trait) and JLS scores in these young adolescents.
Article
Data on divorce taken for all available years between 1947 and 1981 from the Demographic Yearbooks of the United Nations on 58 peoples illustrate that divorce has a consistent pattern. Divorces exhibit a skewed distribution, characterized by the occurrence of the mode early in marriage (with a divorce peak on or around the fourth year) and a gradual, long-tailed decline following this peak. Divorce risk peaks in age category 25–29 for males and age categories 20–24 and 25–29 for females, the height of reproductive and parenting years, and divorce counts peak among couples with two or fewer children. These properties of divorce are unrelated to divorce rate; they occur in societies with both high and low divorce rates. Data on available horticultural and gathering/hunting societies illustrate that divorce also peaks among young couples early in marriage. Remarriage by divorced and widowed individuals of reproductive age is also common cross-culturally. It is proposed that the above four-year modal marriage duration among couples of reproductive age who divorce reflects a hominid reproductive strategy that probably evolved some time after the appearance of Homo in response to increased female “reproductive burden” and functioned to ensure the survival of the hominid infant through weaning. Serial pairbonding during the female's reproductive years had ancestral adaptive advantages, producing the modern cross-cultural pattern of serial pairbonding.
Article
Two decades ago, experimental social psychologists became interested in the emotion of passionate love, "the desire for union with another." Recently, sex researchers have begun to focus on sexual desire, "the desire for sexual union with another," or the loss thereof. In this paper we review what experimental social psychologists have learned about the nature of passionate love in the last two decades and contrast their view of passion with that of sex researchers, especially with regard to the role that anxiety plays in the intensification/diminution of passion. Finally, we suggest that researchers might probably use the same paradigm to study these heretofore separate phenomena.