ArticlePDF Available

Meta-Analysis of Psychotherapy Outcome Studies

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Results of 375 controlled evaluations of psychotherapy and counseling were coded and integrated statistically. The findings provide convincing evidence of the efficacy of psychotherapy. On the average, the typical therapy client is better off than 75% of untreated individuals. Few important differences in effectiveness could be established among many quite different types of psychotherapy. More generally, virtually no difference in effectiveness was observed between the class of all behavioral therapies (e.g., systematic desensitization and behavior modification) and the nonbehavioral therapies (e.g., Rogerian, psychodynamic, rational-emotive, and transactional analysis). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).
Content may be subject to copyright.
Meta-Analysis
of
Psychotherapy
Outcome
Studies
MARY
LEE
SMITH
GENE
V
GLASSUniversity
of
ColoradoBoulder
University
of
ColoradoBoulder
ABSTRACT:
Results
of
nearly
400
controlled
evalua-
tions
of
psychotherapy
and
counseling
were coded
and
integrated
statistically.
The findings
provide
convincing
evidence
of the
efficacy
of
psychotherapy.
On the
average,
the
typical therapy
client
is
better
off
than
75% of
untreated
individuals.
Few
important
differ-
ences
in
effectiveness
could
be
established
among
many
quite
different
types
of
psychotherapy.
More
generally,
virtually
no
difference
in
effectiveness
was
observed
be-
tween
the
class
of all
behavioral
therapies
(systematic
desensitization,
behavior
modification)
and the
nonbe-
havioral
therapies
(Rogerian,
psychodynamic,
rational-
emotive,
transactional
analysis,
etc.).
Scholars
and
clinicians have argued bitterly
for
decades
about
the
efficacy
of
psychotherapy
and
counseling. Michael Scriven proposed
to the
Ameri-
can
Psychological Association's Ethics Committee
that
APA-member clinicians
be
required
to
present
a
card
to
prospective clients
on
which
it
would
be
explained
that
the
procedure
they
were about
to
undergo
had
never been proven superior
to a
placebo ("Psychotherapy
Caveat,"
1974). Most
academics have read little more
than
Eysenck's
(1952,
1965)
tendentious diatribes
in
which
he
claimed
to
prove
that
75%
of
neurotics
got
better
regardless
of
whether
or not
they were
in
therapy
a
conclusion based
on the
interpretation
of six
con-
trolled studies.
The
perception
that
research shows
the
ineffkacy
of
psychotherapy
has
become
part
of
conventional wisdom even within
the
profession.
The
following
testimony
was
recently presented
before
the
Colorado
State
Legislature:
Are
they [the legislators] also aware
of the
relatively
primitive
state
of
the art of
treatment outcome evaluation
which
is
still,
after
fifty
years,
in
kind
of a
virginal state?
About
all
we've been able
to
prove
is
that
a
third
of the
people
get
better,
a
third
of the
people stay
the
same,
and
a
third
of the
people
get
worse,
irregardless
of the
treat-
ment
to
which they
are
subjected. (Quoted
by
Ellis, 1977,
P. 3)
Only close followers
of the
issue have read
Bergin's
(1971)
astute dismantling
of the
Eysenck
myth
in his
review
of the findings of 23
controlled
evaluations
of
therapy. Bergin
found
evidence
that
therapy
is
effective.
Emrick (1975) reviewed
72
studies
of the
psychological
and
psychopharmaco-
logical treatment
of
alcoholism
and
concluded
that
evidence
existed
for the
efficacy
of
therapy. Lubor-
sky, Singer,
and
Luborsky
(1975)
reviewed about
40
controlled studies
and
found
more evidence.
Al-
though
these reviews were reassuring,
two
sources
of
doubt remained.
First,
the
number
of
studies
in
which
the
effects
of
counseling
and
psychotherapy
have been tested
is
closer
to 400
than
to 40. How
representative
the 40 are of the 400 is
unknown.
Second,
in
these reviews,
the
"voting method"
was
used;
that
is, the
number
of
studies with statisti-
cally significant results
in
favor
of one
treatment
or
another
was
tallied.
This
method
is too
weak
to
answer
many important questions
and is
biased
in
favor
of
large-sample studies.
The
purpose
of the
present research
has
three
parts:
(1)
to
identify
and
collect
all
studies
that
tested
the
effects
of
counseling
and
psychotherapy;
(2)
to
determine
the
magnitude
of
effect
of the
therapy
in
each study;
and
(3)
to
compare
the
effects
of
different
types
of
therapy
and
relate
the
size
of
effect
to the
characteristics
of the
therapy
(e.g.,
diagnosis
of
patient, training
of
therapist)
and of the
study.
Meta-analysis,
the
integration
of
research through statistical analysis
of the
analyses
of
individual studies (Glass, 1976),
was
used
to
investigate
the
problem.
Procedures
Standard search procedures were used
to
identify
1,000 documents: Psychological
Abstracts,
Disser-
tation
Abstracts,
and
branching
off
of
bibliographies
of
the
documents themselves.
Of
those documents
located, approximately
500
were selected
for
inclu-
sion
in the
study,
and
375
were
fully
analyzed.
To
be
selected,
a
study
had to
have
at
least
one
ther-
752
SEPTEMBER
1977
AMERICAN
PSYCHOLOGIST
apy
treatment
group compared
to an
untreated
group
or to a
different
therapy group.
The
rigor
of
the
research design
was not a
selection criterion
but was one of
several features
of the
individual
study
to be
related
to the
effect
of the
treatment
in
that
study.
The
definition
of
psychotherapy
used
to
select
the
studies
was
presented
by
Meltzoff
and
Kornreich
(1970):
Psychotherapy
is
taken
to
mean
the
informed
and
planful
application
of
techniques
derived
from
established
psycho-
logical
principles,
by
persons
qualified
through
training
and
experience
to
understand
these
principles
and to
apply these
techniques
with
the
intention
of
assisting
individuals
to
modify
such personal characteristics
as
feelings,
values,
attitudes,
and
behaviors
which
are
judged
by the
therapist
to be
maladaptive
or
maladjustive.
(p. 6)
Those
studies
in
which
the
treatment
was
labeled
"counseling"
but
whose methods
fit the
above
definition
were included. Drug
therapies,
hypno-
therapy,
bibliotherapy,
occupational therapy, milieu
therapy,
and
peer counseling were excluded. Sensi-
tivity training, marathon encounter groups, con-
sciousness-raising groups,
and
psychodrama were
also
excluded.
Those
studies
that
Bergin
and
Luborsky
eliminated because they used
"analogue"
therapy were retained
for the
present research.
Such
studies have been designated analogue studies
because
therapy lasted only
a few
hours
or the
therapists
were
relatively
untrained.
Rather
than
arbitrarily eliminating large numbers
of
studies
and
losing potentially valuable information,
it was
deemed
preferable
to
retain these studies
and in-
vestigate
the
relationship between length
of
ther-
apy,
training
of
therapists,
and
other characteristics
of
the
study
and
their measured
effects.
The
arbi-
trary elimination
of
such analogue studies
was
based
on an
implicit assumption
that
they
differ
not
only
in
their methods
but
also
in
their
effects
and how
those
effects
are
achieved. Considering
methods,
analogue studies
fade
imperceptibly into
"real"
therapy,
since
the
latter
is
often
short
term,
or
practiced
by
relative novices, etc. Furthermore,
the
magnitude
of
effects
and
their relationships
with other variables
are
empirical questions,
not to
be
assumed
out of
existence. Dissertations
and
fugitive
documents were likewise
retained,
and the
The
research
reported here
was
supported
by a
grant
from
the
Spencer
Foundation,
Chicago,
Illinois.
This
paper
draws
in
part
from
the
presidential address
of the
second
author
to the
American Educational Research Asso-
ciation,
San
Francisco,
April
21,
1976.
Requests
for
:reprints
should
be
sent
to
Gene
V
Glass,
Laboratory
of
Educational Research, University
of
Colo-
rado,
Boulder,
Colorado
80302.
measured
effects
of the
studies
compared according
to the
source
of the
studies.
The
most important
feature
of an
outcome study
was the
magnitude
of the
effect
of
therapy.
The
definition
of the
magnitude
of
effect—or
"effect
size"—was
the
mean
difference
between
the
treated
and
control subjects divided
by the
standard devia-
tion
of the
control group,
that
is, ES =
(.XT
XC)/SG-
Thus,
an
"effect
size"
of +1
indicates
that
a
person
at the
mean
of the
control group
would
be
expected
to
rise
to the
84th percentile
of
the
control group
after
treatment.
The
effect
size
was
calculated
on
any-outcome
variable
the
researcher chose
to
measure.
In
many
cases,
one
study yielded more than
one
effect
size,
since
effects
might
be
measured
at
more
than
one
time
after
treatment
or on
more than
one
different
type
of
outcome
variable.
The
effect-size measures
represent
different
types
of
outcomes: self-esteem,
anxiety,
work/school achievement, physiological
stress, etc. Mixing
different
outcomes together
is
defensible.
First,
it is
clear
that
all
outcome mea-
sures
are
more
or
less
related
to
"well-being"
and
so
at a
general level
are
comparable. Second,
it is
easy
to
imagine
a
Senator conducting hearings
on
the
NIMH
appropriations
or a
college president
deciding
whether
to
continue
funding
the
counsel-
ing
center asking,
"What
kind
of
effect
does ther-
apy
produce—on
anything?" Third, each primary
researcher made value judgments concerning
the
definition
and
direction
of
positive therapeutic
ef-
fects
for the
particular clients
he or she
studied.
It is
reasonable
to
adopt these value judgments
and
aggregate them
in the
present study. Fourth, since
all
effect
sizes
are
identified
by
type
of
outcome,
the
magnitude
of
effect
can be
compared
across
type
of
outcome
to
determine whether therapy
has
greater
effect
on
anxiety,
for
example,
than
it
does
on
self-esteem.
Calculating
effect
sizes
was
straightforward when
means
and
standard deviations were reported.
Al-
though this
information
is
thought
to be
funda-
mental
in
reporting research,
it was
often
over-
looked
by
authors
and
editors. When means
and
standard
deviations
were
not
reported,
effect
sizes
were
obtained
by the
solution
of
equations
from
t
and
F
ratios
or
other inferential test
statistics.
Probit transformations were used
to
convert
to ef-
fect
sizes
the
percentages
of
patients
who
improved
(Glass,
in
press).
Original
data
were
requested
from
several authors when
effect
sizes could
not be
derived
from
any
reported information.
In two
instances,
effect
sizes were impossible
to
recon-
AMERICAN
PSYCHOLOGIST
SEPTEMBER
1977
7S3
struct:
(a)
nonparametric
statistics irretrievably
disguise
effect
sizes,
and (b) the
reporting
of no
data except
the
alpha level
at
which
a
mean
differ-
ence
was
significant gives
no
clue other than that
the
standardized mean
difference
must exceed some
known
value.
Eight hundred thirty-three
effect
sizes were
computed
from
375
studies, several studies yielding
effects
on
more than
one
type
of
outcome
or at
more
than
one
time
after
therapy. Including more
than
one
effect
size
for
each study perhaps intro-
duces
dependence
in the
errors
and
violates some
assumptions
of
inferential statistics. However,
the
loss
of
information that would have resulted
from
averaging
effects
across types
of
outcome
or at
different
follow-up
points
was too
great
a
price
to
pay for
statistical purity.
The
effect
sizes
of the
separate studies became
the
"dependent variable"
in the
meta-analysis.
The
"independent
variables" were
16
features
of the
study
described
or
measured
in the
following
ways:
1.
The
type
of
therapy
employed,
for
example,
psycho-
dynamic, client centered,
rational-emotive,
behavior modifi-
cation,
etc.
There
were
10
types
in
all;
each
will
be
men-
tioned
in the
Results
section.
2.
The
duration
of
therapy
in
hours.
3.
Whether
it was
group
or
individual
therapy.
4. The
number
of
years'
experience
of the
therapist.
5.
Whether clients
were
neurotics
or
psychotics.
6.
The age of the
clients.
7.
The
IQ
of the
clients.
8. The
source
of the
subjects—whether
solicited
for the
study,
committed
to an
institution,
or
sought
treatment
themselves.
9.
Whether
the
therapists
were
trained
in
education,
psychology,
or
psychiatry.
10.
The
social
and
ethnic
similarity
of
therapists
and
clients.
11. The
type
of
outcome measure
taken.
12.
The
number
of
months
after
therapy
that
the
out-
comes
were
measured.
13.
The
reactivity
or
"fakeability"
of the
outcome
measure.
14.
The
date
of
publication
of the
study.
15.
The
form
of
publication.
16.
The
internal
validity
of
'the
research
design.
Definitions
and
conventions were developed
to
increase
the
reliability
of
measurement
of the
fea-
tures
of the
studies
and to
assist
the
authors
in
estimating
the
data when
they
were
not
reported.
The
more important conventions appear
in
Table
1.
Variables
not
mentioned
in
Table
1
were measured
in
fairly
obvious ways.
The
reliability
of
measure-
ment
was
determined
by
comparing
the
codings
of
20
studies
by the two
authors
and
four
assistants.
Agreement
exceeded
90%
across
all
categories.1
Analysis
of the
data comprised
four
parts:
(1)
descriptive statistics
for the
body
of
data
as a
whole;
(2)
descriptive statistics
for the
comparison
of
therapy types
and
outcome types;
(3)
descrip-
tive statistics
for a
subset
of
studies
in
which
be-
havioral
and
nonbehavioral therapies were
com-
pared
in the
same
study;
and (4)
regression analy-
ses in
which
effect
sizes were regressed onto vari-
ables descriptive
of the
study.
Findings
DATA
FROM
ALL
EXPERIMENTS
Figure
1
contains
the findings at the
highest level
of
aggregation.
The two
curves depict
the
average
treated
and
untreated groups
of
clients across
375
studies,
833
effect-size
measures, representing
an
evaluation
of
approximately 25,000 control
and ex-
perimental
subjects each.
On the
average, clients
22
years
of age
received
17
hours
of
therapy
from
therapists with about
3J
years
of
experience
and
were
measured
on the
outcome variables about
3f
months
after
the
therapy.
For
ease
of
representation,
the figure is
drawn
in
the
form
of two
normal distributions.
No
conclu-
sion
about
the
distributions
of the
scores within
studies
is
intended.
In
most studies,
no
informa-
tion
was
given about
the
shape
of an
individual's
scores within treated
and
untreated groups.
We
suspect that normality
has as
much justification
as
any
other
form.
The
average study showed
a .68
standard devia-
tion
superiority
of the
treated group over
the
con-
trol group. Thus,
the
average client receiving
therapy
was
better
off
than
75%
of the
untreated
controls.
Ironically,
the 75% figure
that Eysenck
used
repeatedly
to
embarrass psychotherapy
ap-
0.68
CONTROL
TREATED
50th
%-ILE/
OF
CONTROL
75th
%-ILE
OF
CONTROL
1
The
values assigned
to the
features
of the
studies,
the
effect
sizes,
and all
procedures
are
available
in
Glass,
Smith,
and
Miller
(Note
1).
AVE.
EFFECT SIZE:
0.68
<rx
STD.
DEV.
OF
EFFECT SIZE:
0.67
crx
Figure
1.
Effect
of
therapy
on any
outcome.
(Data based
on 375
studies;
833
data points.)
754
SEPTEMBER
1977
AMERICAN
PSYCHOLOGIST
TABLE
1:
Conventions
for
Measurement
of
the
Features
of
Studies
Study
feature
Value
Experience
of
therapist
(when
not
given)
Diagnosis
of
client
(neurotic
or
psychotic)
IQ
of
client (low, average,
high)
Source
of
subjects
Similarity
of
therapist
and
client ("very
similar"
to
"very
dissimilar")
Type
of
outcome measure
Lay
counselor
(0
years)
MA
candidate
(1
year)
MA
counselor
(2
years)
PhD
candidate
or
psychiatric
resident
(3
years)
PhD
therapist
(4
years)
Well-known
PhD or
psychiatrist
(5
years)
Neurotic unless symptoms
or
labels clearly indicate
otherwise.
Average
unless
identified
as
otherwise
by
diagnostic labels
(e.g., mentally retarded)
or
institutional
affiliation
(college
attendance).
Clients
solicited
for
purpose
of
the
study.
Clients committed
to
institu-
tion, hence
to
therapy.
Clients recognized
existence
of
problem
and
sought
treatment.
College
students:
very similar
Neurotic
adults:
moderately
similar
Juveniles,
minorities:
moderately dissimilar
Hospitalized, chronic
adults,
disturbed children, prisoners:
very dissimilar
Fear,
anxiety:
Spielberger
&
Cattell anxiety measures,
be-
havioral approach tests.
Self-esteem:
inventories,
self-
ideal correlations, ratings
by
self
and
others.
Adjustment:
adjustment scales,
improvement
ratings,
re-
hospitalization, time
out of
hospital, sobriety, symptoma-
tic
complaints, disruptive
behavior.
Study
feature
Value
Type
of
outcome measure
(continued)
Reactivity
of
measurement
Form
of
publication
Internal
validity (high,
medium, low)
Work/school
achievement:
grade point
average,
job
supervisor ratings,
promotions.
Personality
traits:
MMPI
or
other
trait
inventories,
pro-
jective
test
results.
Social
behavior:
dating, class-
room
discipline, public
speaking, information-seeking
behavior, sociometrics.
Emotional-somatic
disorder:
frigidity,
impotence.
Physiological
stress:
galvanic
skin
response,
Palmer
Sweat
Index, blood pressure,
heart
rate.
1
(low):
Physiological mea-
sures; grade point
average
2
Projective device
(blind);
discharge
from
hospital
(blind)
3
Standardized
measures,
of
traits
(MMPI,
Rotter)
4
Experimenter-con-
structed question-
naires; client's
self-
report
to
experi-
menter;
discharge
(nonblind);
be-
havior
in
presence
of
therapist
5
(high):
Therapist
rating;
projective device
(nonblind)
Journal
Book
Thesis
Unpublished document
High:
Randomization,
low
mortality
Medium:
More
than
one
threat
to
internal
validity
Low:
No
matching
of
pretest
information
to
equate
groups
pears
in a
slightly
different
context
as the
most
de-
fensible
figure on the
efficacy
of
therapy:
The
therapies
represented
by the
available
outcome
evaluations
move
the
average
client
from
the
50th
to the
75th
percentile.
The
standard
deviation
of the
effect
sizes
is
,67.
Their
skewness
is
+.99.
Only
12%
of the 833
effect-size
measures
from
the 375
studies
were
negative.
If
therapies
of any
type
were ineffective
and
design
and
measurement
flaws
were
immaterial,
one
would expect half
the
effect-size
measures
to
be
negative.
The 833
effect-size
measures
were classified
into
10
categories
descriptive
of the
type
of
outcome
AMERICAN
PSYCHOLOGIST
SEPTEMBER
1977
755
being
assessed,
for
example,
fear
and
anxiety reduc-
tion, self-esteem, adjustment
(freedom
from
de-
bilitating symptoms), achievement
in
school
or on
the
job, social relations, emotional-somatic prob-
lems, physiological
stress
measures, etc.
Effect-size
measures
for
four
outcome categories
are
presented
in
Table
2.
Two
hundred
sixty-one
effect
sizes
from
over
100
studies average about
1
standard deviation
on
measures
of
fear
and
anxiety reduction.
Thus,
the
average
treated
client
is
better
off
than
83%
of
those
untreated with respect
to the
alleviation
of
fear
and
anxiety.
The
improvement
in
self-esteem
is
nearly
as
large.
The
effect
sizes average
.9 of a
standard deviation. Improvement
on
variables
in
the
"adjustment"
outcome class averages consider-
ably less, roughly
.6 of a
standard deviation.
These
outcome
variables
are
measures
of
personal
func-
tioning
and
frequently involve indices
of
hospitali-
zation
or
incarceration
for
psychotic, alcoholic,
or
criminal
episodes.
The
average
effect
size
for
school
or
work
achievement—most
frequently
"grade
point
average"—is
smallest
of the
four
out-
come
classes.
The
studies
in the
four
outcome measure cate-
gories
are not
comparable
in
terms
of
type
of
therapy, duration, experience
of
therapists, number
of
months posttherapy
at
which outcomes were
measured,
etc. Nonetheless,
the findings in
Table
2
are
fairly
consistent with expectations
and
give
the
credible impression
that
fear
and
self-esteem
are
more susceptible
to
change
in
therapy than
are
the
relatively more serious behaviors grouped under
the
categories
"adjustment"
and
"achievement."
TABLE
2:
Effects
of
Therapy
on
Four
Types
of
Outcome
Measure
TABLE
3:
Effects
of Ten
Types
of
Therapy
on Any
Outcome
Measure
Type
of
outcome
Fear-anxiety
reduction
Self-esteem
Adjustment
School/work
achievement
Average
effect
size
.97
.90
.56
.31
No. of
effect
sizes
261
53
229
145
Standard
error
of
mean
effect
sizea
.15
.13
.05
.03
Mdn
treated
person's
percentile
status
in
control
group
83
82
71
62
Type
of
therapy
Psychodynamic
Adlerian
Eclectic
Transactional
analysis
Rational-emotive
Gestalt
Client-centered
Systematic
desensitization
Implosion
Behavior
modification
Average
effect
size
.59
.71
.48
.58
.77
.26
.63
.91
.64
.76
No. of
effect
sizes
96
16
70
25
35
8
94
223
45
132
Standard
error
of
mean
effect
size
.05
.19
.07
.19
.13
.09
.08
.05
.09
.06
Mdn
treated
person's
percentile
status
in
control
group
72
76
68
72
78
60
74
82
74
78
The
standard errors
of the
mean
are
calculated
by
dividing
the
standard
deviation
of the
effect
sizes
(not
reported)
by the
square
root
of the
number
of
them,
This
method,
based
on the
assumption
of
independence
known
to be
false,
gives
a
lower
bound
to the
standard
errors
(Tukey, Note
2).
Inferential
techniques
employing
Tukey's
jackknife
.method
which
take
the
nonindependence
into
account
are
examined
in
Glass
(in
press).
Table
3
presents
the
average
effect
sizes
for 10
types
of
therapy. Nearly
100
effect-size
measures
arising
from
evaluations
of
psychodynamic
therapy,
that
is,
Freudianlike therapy
but not
psychoanaly-
sis, average approximately
.6 of a
standard devia-
tion. Studies
of
Adlerian therapy show
an
average
of
.7
sigma,
but
only
16
effect
sizes
were
found.
Eclectic therapies,
that
is,
verbal, cognitive, non-
behavioral
therapies more similar
to
psychodynamic
therapies than
any
other type, gave
a
mean
effect
size
of
about
.5 of a
standard deviation. Although
the
number
of
controlled evaluations
of
Berne's
transactional analysis
was
rather small,
it
gave
a
respectable average
effect
size
of .6
sigma,
the
same
as
psychodynamic therapies. Albert
Ellis's
ra-
tional-emotive therapy, with
a
mean
effect
size
of
nearly
.8 of a
standard deviation,
finished
second
among
all 10
therapy types.
The
Gestalt
therapies
were
relatively untested,
but 8
studies showed
16
effect
sizes averaging only
.25
of a
standard devia-
tion. Rogerian client-centered therapy showed
a
.6
sigma
effect
size averaged across about
60
studies.
The
average
of
over
200
effect-size
mea-
sures
from
approximately
100
studies
of
systematic
desensitization therapy
was .9
sigma,
the
largest
average
effect
size
of all
therapy types. Implosive
therapy showed
a
mean
effect
size
of
.,64
of a
stan-
dard
deviation, about equal
to
that
for
Rogerian
and
psychodynamic therapies. Significantly,
the
average
effect
size
for
implosive therapy
is
mark-
edly
lower than that
for
systematic desensitization,
which
was
usually evaluated
in
studies using similar
kinds
of
clients with similar
problems—principally,
7S6
SEPTEMBER
1977
AMERICAN
PSYCHOLOGIST
simple phobias.
The final
therapy depicted
in
Table
3 is
Skinnerian behavior modification, which
showed
a
.75
sigma
effect
size.
Hay's
to2,
which relates
the
categorical variable
"type
of
therapy"
to the
quantitative variable
"effect
size,"
has the
value
of .10 for the
data
in
Table
3.
Thus, these
10
therapy types account
for
10%
of the
variance
in the
effect
size
that
studies
produce.
The
types
of
therapy depicted
in
Table
3
were
clearly
not
equated
for
duration,
severity
of
prob-
lem,
type
of
outcome, etc. Nonetheless,
the
dif-
ferences
in
average
effect
sizes
are
interesting
and
interpretable.
There
is
probably
a
tendency
for
researchers
to
evaluate
the
therapy they like best
and to
pick clients, circumstances,
and
outcome
measures
which show
that
therapy
in the
best
light.
Even
so,
major
differences
among
the
therapies
ap-
pear. Implosive therapy
is
demonstrably
inferior
to
systematic desensitization. Behavior
modifica-
tion
shows
the
same mean
effect
size
as
rational-
emotive
therapy.
EFFECTS
OF
CLASSES
OF
THERAPY
To
compare
the
effect
of
therapy
type
after
equat-
ing for
duration
of
therapy, diagnosis
of
client, type
of
outcome, etc.,
it was
necessary
to
move
to a
coarser
level
of
analysis
in
which
data
could
be
grouped
into more stable composites.
The
problem
was
to
group
the 10
types
of
therapy into classes,
so
that
effect
sizes could
be
compared among more
general types
of
therapy. Methods
of
multidimen-
sional scaling were used
to
derive
a
structure
from
the
perceptions
of
similarities
among
the 10
thera-
pies
by a
group
of
25
clinicians
and
counselors.
All
of the
judges
in
this scaling study were enrolled
in
a
graduate-level
seminar.
For five
weeks,
the
theory
and
techniques
of the 10
therapies were
studied
and
discussed. Then, each judge performed
a
multidimensional rank ordering
of the
therapies,
judging
similarity among them
on
whatever basis
he or she
chose, articulated
or
unarticulated, con-
scious
or
unconscious.
The
results
of the
Shepard-
Kruskal
multidimensional scaling analysis appear
as
Figure
2.
In
Figure
2 one
clearly
sees
four
classes
of
thera-
pies:
the ego
therapies
(transactional
analysis
and
rational-emotive therapy)
in
front;
the
three
dy-
namic
therapies
low,
in the
background;
the be-
havioral triad, upper right;
and the
pair
of
"hu-
manistic" therapies, Gestalt
and
Rogerian.
The
average
effect
sizes
among
the
four
classes
of
SYS.
OESENS.
'.
MOD.
PSYCHOANAL.
PSYCHOTHERP.
Figure
2.
Multidimensional scaling
of 10
therapies
by 25
clinicians
and
counselors.
therapies have been compared,
but the findings are
not
reported here. Instead,
a
higher level
of ag-
gregation
of the
therapies, called
"superclasses,"
was
studied.
The first
superclass
was
formed from
those therapies above
the
horizontal plane
in
Figure
2,
with
the
exception
of
Gestalt therapy
for
which
there
was an
inadequate
number
of
studies.
This
superclass
was
then identical with
the
group
of be-
havioral
therapies: implosion, systematic desensi-
tization,
and
behavior modification.
The
second
superclass comprises
the six
therapies below
the
horizontal plane
in
Figure
2 and is
termed
the
nonbehavioral
superclass,
a
composite
of
psycho-
analytic psychotherapy, Adlerian, Rogerian,
ra-
tional-emotive, eclectic therapy,
and
transactional
analysis.
Figure
3
represents
the
mean
effect
sizes
for
studies classified
by the two
superclasses.
On the
average,
approximately
200
evaluations
of
behav-
ioral
therapies showed
a
mean
effect
of
about
.8<rx,
standard error
of
.03, over
the
control group.
Ap-
proximately
170
evaluations
of
nonbehavioral
studies
gave
a
mean
effect
size
of
.6<rx,
standard
error
of
.04. This small
difference
(,2<rx)
between
the
outcomes
of
behavioral
and
nonbehavioral
therapies must
be
considered
in
light
of the
circum-
stances under which these studies were conducted.
The
evaluators
of
behavioral
superclass
therapies
waited
an
average
of 2
months
after
the
therapy
to
measure
its
effects,
whereas
the
postassessment
of
the
nonbehavioral
therapies
was
made
in the vi-
cinity
of 5
months,
on the
average. Furthermore,
the
reactivity
or
susceptibility
to
bias
of the
out-
come
measures
was
higher
for the
behavioral
super-
AMERICAN
PSYCHOLOGIST
SEPTEMBER
1977
757
TREATMENT
DESCRIPTION
SUPERCLASS
#1
#2
2.0I
mos.
4.70 mos.
3.44
3.I8
AVE.
FOLLOW-UP
AVE.
REACTIVITY
0.83
Ox
0.59
erx
CONTROL
SUPERCLASS
#1
n
= 403
SUPERCLASS
#2
n = 344
72nd %-ILE
OF
CONTROL
\
X
80th
%-ILE
Figure
3.
Effect
of
Superclass
#1
(behavioral)
and
Superclass
#2
(nonbehavioral).
class than
for the
nonbehavioral
superclass;
that
is,
the
behavioral
researchers
showed
a
slightly
greater
tendency
to
rely
on
more subjective outcome mea-
sures. These
differences
lead
one to
suspect
that
the
.2o-x
difference
between
the
behavioral
and
non-
behavioral superclasses
is
somewhat exaggerated
in
favor
of the
behavioral superclass. Exactly
how
much
the
difference
ought
to be
reduced
is a
ques-
tion
that
can be
approached
in at
least
two
ways:
(a)
examine
the
behavioral versus nonbehavioral
difference
for
only those studies
in
which
one
ther-
apy
from
each superclass
was
represented, since
for
those studies
the
experimental circumstances will
be
equivalent;
(2)
regress
"effect
size"
onto vari-
ables descriptive
of the
study
and
correct statisti-
cally
for
differences
in
circumstances between
be-
havioral
and
nonbehavioral studies.
Figure
4
represents
120
effect-size
measures
de-
rived
from
those studies, approximately
SO in
num-
ber,
in
which
a
behavioral therapy
and
nonbehav-
ioral
therapy were compared simultaneously with
an
untreated control. Hence,
for
these studies,
the
collective
behavioral
and
nonbehavioral therapies
are
equivalent with respect
to all
important
features
of
the
experimental setting, namely, experience
of
the
therapists, nature
of the
clients' problems, dura-
tion
of
therapy, type
of
outcome measure, months
after
therapy
for
measuring
the
outcomes, etc.
The
results
are
provocative.
The
.2o-x
"uncon-
trolled"
difference
in
Figure
3 has
shrunk
to a
.07o-x
difference
in
average
effect
size.
The
standard
error
of the
mean
of the
119
different
scores (be-
havioral
effect
size minus nonbehavioral
effect
size
CONTROL
SUPERCLASS
#\
n=
119
SUPERCLASS
#2
n
=119
73rd
%-ILE
OF
CONTROL
75th
%-ILE
Figure
4.
Effect
of
Superclass
#1
(behavioral)
and
Superclass
#2
(nonbehavioral).
(Data
drawn
only
from
experiments
in
which Superclass
#1 and
Superclass
#2
were simultaneously compared with
control.)
in
each study)
is
.66/VH9
=
.06.
The
behavioral
and
nonbehavioral therapies show about
the
same
average
effect.
The
second approach
to
correcting
for
mea-
surable
differences
between behavioral
and
non-
behavioral
therapies
is
statistical adjustment
by
regression
analysis.
By
this method,
it is
possible
to
quantify
and
study
the
natural covariation
among
the
principal outcome variable
of
studies
and the
many
variables
descriptive
of the
context
of
the
studies.
Eleven features
of
each study were correlated
with
the
effect
size
the
study produced
(Table
4).
For
example,
the
correlation between
the
duration
TABLE
4:
Correlations
of
Several
Descriptive
Variables
with
Effect
Size
Variable
Correlation
with
effect
size
Organization
(1 =
individual;
2 =
group)
—.07
Duration
of
therapy
(in
hours)
.02
Years'
experience
of
therapists
.01
Diagnosis
of
clients
(1
=
psychotic;
2 =
neurotic)
.02
IQ
of
clients
(1
=
bw;
2 =
medium;
3 =
high)
.15**
Age of
clients
.02
Similarity
of
therapists
and
clients
(1
=
very
similar;
...
; 4 =
very
dissimilar)
—.19**
Internal
validity
of
study
(1
=
high;
2 =
medium;
3 =
low) -.09*
Date
of
publication
.09*
"Reactivity"
of
outcome measure
(1
=
low;
...
;5 =
high)
.30**
No. of
months
posttherapy
for
follow-up
—.10*
* P <
.05.
**P
<
.01.
758
SEPTEMBER
1977
AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST
TABLE
5:
Regression Analyses Within
Therapies
Unstandardized
regression
coefficients
Independent variable
Diagnosis
(1 =
psychotic;
2 =
neurotic)
Intelligence
(1 =
low;
...
; 3 =
high)
Transformed
age"
Experience
of
Therapist
X
Neurotic
Experience
of
Therapist
X
Psychotic
Clients self-presented
Clients
solicited
Organization
(1 =
individual;
2 =
group)
Transformed months
posttherapyb
Transformed reactivity
of
measure"
Additive
constant
Multiple
R
00
Psychodynamic
(«
= 94)
.174
-.114
.002
-.011
-.015
-.111
.182
.108
-.031
.003
.757
.423
.173
Systematic
desensitization
(n
=
212)
-.193
.201
-.002
-.034
.004
.287
.088
-.086
-.047
.025
.489
.512
.386
Behavior
modification
(n
=
129)
.041
.201
.002
-.018
-.033
-.015
-.163
-.276
.007
.021
.453
.509
.340
»
Transformed
age =
(Age
-
25)
(|
Age
- 25
|)*.
b
Transformed months
posttherapy
=
(No.
months)*.
0
Transformed
reactivity
of
measure
=
(Reactivity)"'.
of
the
therapy
in
hours
and the
effect
size
of the
study
is
nearly
zero,
—.02.
The
correlations
are
generally low, although several
are
reliably non-
zero.
Some
of the
more interesting correlations
show
a
positive relationship between
an
estimate
of
the
intelligence
of the
group
of
clients
and the ef-
fect
of
therapy,
and a
somewhat larger correlation
indicating
that
therapists
who
resemble
their
clients
in
ethnic group, age,
and
social level
get
better
results.
The
effect
sizes diminish across time
after
therapy
as
shown
by the
last correlation
in
Table
4, a
correlation
of
—.10
which
is
closer
to
—.20
when
the
curvilinearity
of the
relationship
is
taken
into account.
The
largest correlation
is
with
the
"reactivity"
or
subjectivity
of the
outcome
measure.
The
multiple correlation
of
these variables with
effect
size
is
about
.50.
Thus,
25%
of the
variance
in the
results
of
studies
can be
reduced
by
specifi-
cation
of
independent
variable
values.
In
several
important
subsets
of the
data
not
reported here,
the
multiple correlations
are
over .70, which indi-
cates that
in
some instances
it is
possible
to
reduce
more
than
half
of the
variability
in
study
findings
by
regressing
the
outcome
effect
onto contextual
variables
of the
study.
The
results
of
three separate multiple regression
analyses
appear
in
Table
5.
Multiple
regressions
were
performed
within each
of
three types
of
ther-
apy: psychodynamic, systematic desensitization,
and
behavior
modification.
Relatively complex
forms
of the
independent variables were used
to
account
for
interactions
and
nonlinear relationships.
For
example,
years'
experience
of the
therapist
bore
a
slight curvilinear relationship with outcome, prob-
ably because more experienced therapists worked
with
more seriously
ill
clients.
This
situation
was
accommodated
by
entering,
as an
independent vari-
able,
"therapist
experience"
in
interaction with
"diagnosis
of the
client."
Age of
client
and
fol-
low-up
date
were slightly curvilinearly
related
to
outcome
in
ways most directly handled
by
changing
exponents. These regression equations allow
esti-
mation
of the
effect
size
a
study shows when under-
taken with
a
certain type
of
client, with
a
therapist
of
a
certain level
of
experience, etc.
By
setting
the
independent
variables
at a
particular
set of
values,
one
can
estimate
what
a
study
of
that
type
would
reveal under each
of the
three types
of
therapy.
Thus,
a
statistically controlled comparison
of the
effects
of
psychodynamic, systematic desensitiza-
tion,
and
behavior
modification
therapies
can be
obtained
in
this
case.
The
three
regression
equa-
tions
are
clearly
not
homogeneous; hence,
one
ther-
apy
might
be
superior under
one set of
circum-
stances
and a
different
therapy superior under
others.
A
full
description
of the
nature
of
this
interaction
is
elusive, though
one can
illustrate
it
at
various particularly interesting points.
In
Figure
5,
estimates
are
made
of the
effect
sizes
that
would
be
shown
for
studies
in
which
simple
phobias
of
high-intelligence subjects,
20
years
of
age,
are
treated
by a
therapist with
2
years' experience
and
evaluated immediately after
therapy
with highly subjective outcome measures.
AMERICAN
PSYCHOLOGIST
SEPTEMBER
1977
759
ESTIMATED
EFFECT
SIZES
PSYCHODYNAMIC
0.919
SYSTEMATIC
DESENSITIZATION
1.049
BEHAVIORAL
MODIFICATION
1.119
CONTROL
Figure
5.
Three
within-therapy
regression equa-
tions
set to
describe
a
prototypic therapy client
(phobic)
and
therapy situation.
This
verbal description
of
circumstances
can be
translated into quantitative values
for the
inde-
pendent variables
in
Table
S and
substituted into
each
of the
three
regression
equations.
In
this
in-
stance,
the two
behavioral therapies show
effects
superior
to the
psychodynamic
therapy.
In
Figure
6, a
second prototypical psychotherapy
client
and
situation
are
captured
in the
independent
variable
values,
and the
effects
of the
three types
of
therapy
are
estimated.
For the
typical 30-year-
old
neurotic
of
average
IQ
seen
in
circumstances
like
those that prevail
in
mental health clinics (in-
dividual
therapy
by a
therapist with
S
years'
ex-
perience),
behavior
modification
is
estimated
to be
superior
to
psychodynamic therapy, which
is in
turn
superior
to
systematic desensitization
at the 6-
month
follow-up
point.
Besides
illuminating
the
relationships
in the
data,
the
quantitative techniques described here
can
give
direction
to
future
research.
By fitting
regression
equations
to the
relationship between
effect
size
and
the
independent variables descriptive
of the
studies
ESTIMATED-EFFECT
SIZES
PSYCHODYNAMIC
0.643
SYSTEMATIC
DESENSITIZATION
0.516
BEHAVIORAL
MODIFICATION
0.847
CONTROL
Figure
6.
Three
within-therapy
regression equa-
tions
set to
describe
a
prototypic therapy client
(neurotic)
and
therapy
situation.
and
then
by
placing confidence regions around these
hyperplanes,
the
regions where
the
input-output
relationships
are
most poorly determined
can be
identified.
By
concentrating
new
studies
in
these
regions,
one can
avoid
the
accumulation
of
redun-
dant studies
of
convenience that overelaborate
small
areas.
Conclusions
The
results
of
research demonstrate
the
beneficial
effects
of
counseling
and
psychotherapy. Despite
volumes
devoted
to the
theoretical
differences
among
different
schools
of
psychotherapy,
the re-
sults
of
research demonstrate negligible
differences
in
the
effects
produced
by
different
therapy types.
Unconditional
judgments
of
superiority
of one
type
or
another
of
psychotherapy,
and all
that
these
claims
imply about treatment
and
training policy,
are
unjustified.
Scholars
and
clinicians
are in the
rather embarrassing position
of
knowing less than
has
been proven, because knowledge, atomized
and
sprayed across
a
vast landscape
of
journals, books,
and
reports,
has not
been accessible. Extracting
knowledge
from
accumulated studies
is a
complex
and
important methodological problem which
de-
serves
further
attention.
REFERENCE
NOTES
1.
Glass,
G. V,
Smith,
M.
L.,
&
Miller,
T. I. The
benefits
of
psychotherapy.
Book
in
preparation, 1977.
2.
Tukey,
J. W.
Personal
communication,
November
IS,
1976.
REFERENCES
Bergin,
A. E. The
evaluation
of
therapeutic outcomes.
In A. E.
Bergin
& S. L.
Garfield
(Eds.),
Handbook
of
psychotherapy
and
behavior
change.
New
York:
Wiley,
1971.
Ellis,
R. H.
Letters.
Colorado Psychological
Association
Newsletter,
April 1977,
p. 3.
Emrick,
C. D. A
review
of
psychologically oriented
treat-
ment
of
alcoholism. Journal
of
Studies
on
Alcohol,
1975,
36,
88-108.
Eysenck,
H. J. The
effects
of
psychotherapy:
An
evalua-
tion.
Journal
of
Consulting
Psychology,
1952,
16,
319-
324.
Eysenck,
H. J. The
effects
of
psychotherapy.
Journal
of
Psychology,
1965,1,
97-118.
Glass,
G. V.
Primary,
secondary,
and
meta-analysis
of re-
search.
The
Educational
Researcher,
1976,10,
3-8.
Glass,
G. V.
Integrating findings:
The
meta-analysis
of
research.
Review
of
Research
in
Education,
in
press.
Luborsky,
L.,
Singer,
B., &
Luborsky,
L.
Comparative
studies
of
psychotherapies.
Archives
of
General
Psy-
chiatry,
1975,
32,
995-1008.
Meltzoff,
J.,
&
Kornreich,
M.
Research
in
psychotherapy.
New
York: Atherton, 1970.
Psychotherapy
caveat.
APA
Monitor,
December 1974,
p. 7.
760
SEPTEMBER
1977
AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST
... Meta-analyses suggest that certain psychological interventions are effective for treating psychiatric disorders including depression 9 , obsessive-compulsive disorder 10 and post-traumatic stress disorder 11 . These syntheses have played a pivotal part in some of the greatest successes 12,13 and controversies in psychological science [14][15][16][17] . ...
... For example, expanded definitions of impact mean that studies will probably have more than one inclusion-worthy effect, thereby requiring methods for analysing multiple intervention effects simultaneously [43][44][45][46] . Prior meta-analytic modelling techniques 12,47 can accommodate only one 'independent' effect size per study. Researchers have therefore used a variety of strategies (such as choosing only one effect size per study or averaging effect sizes within a study) to avoid violating this statistical assumption. ...
... (but might remain open to a small effect for it, not because of therapeutic skill accrual per se, but rather because of a deepening of emotion regulation properties relevant to clinical work with particular conditions and phenomena). Past work, taken together, converges on the finding of a nil-tosmall effect for therapist experience, it should be noted (e.g., Huppert et al., 2001;Smith & Glass, 1977;Uhl et al., 2022;cf. Speers et al., 2022). ...
... Specifically, in the current case, there was no evidence that inexperienced therapists' patients had worse outcomes in general, including if the patients' initial presentations were clinically severe. The present results align reasonably well with the larger literature on therapist experience effects, going back to the classic work of Smith and Glass (1977), with effect sizes being variable and generally small (cf. Uhl et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective A goal of this effort is to explicate two differing views on training models in clinical psychology and allied fields, namely, the gradualist view—which assumes that trainees’ exposure to patients with severe clinical presentations should be restricted until clinical experience accrues—and the anti-fragilist view—which, under certain conditions, encourages trainees’ exposure to the full range of severity regardless of experience. A related aim is to empirically test a prediction arising from the tension between these two perspectives. Methods Patient and therapist data were available from a low-cost population-facing treatment center (N = 399; 69.8% women; 73.6% Non-Hispanic White), which collected data on patients’ severity of clinical presentation at intake and over time and on therapist experience, and which does not restrict assignment based on therapist experience. The statistical interaction between severity of clinical presentation at intake and therapist experience predicting clinical outcomes was tested. Results There was no evidence that inexperienced therapists’ patients had worse outcomes in general, including if the patients’ initial presentations were clinically severe. Conclusions There are reasons to favor an anti-fragilizing training model, including that it better respects and serves patients and trainees, and that the current results do not support a core assumption of the gradualist model.
... Philip S. Gallo, Jr. Elizabeth Lynn San Diego State University Willson (May 1980) has taken the first author rather severely to task for miscalculating the strength of the effect of psychotherapy as originally reported by Smith and Glass (1977) and for misinterpreting the results of that study. Unfortunately for the readership of the American Psychologist, Willson fails to provide any calculations or data to support his contention that the value of omega square was miscalculated (it was not) or that the figures presented in his Table 1 represent the correct calculations (which they do not). ...
Article
Full-text available
Replies to V. L. Willson's (1980) criticism of P. S. Gallo (1978) for miscalculating the strength of the effect of psychotherapy as originally reported by M. L. Smith and G. V. Glass and misinterpreting the results of Smith and Glass's study. Willson fails to provide any calculations or data to support his contentions. The original calculations that were employed are presented, and Willson's figures are shown to be incorrect.
... The origins of this design are in 1950s scientific literature, but it was not until 1980 that such designs were required by the FDA for drug approval (Shapiro & Shapiro, 1997). Important to such evaluation was the advent of meta-analysis methods (Smith & Glass, 1977) that allowed for the computation of effect sizes. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Details the growth, roles, and applications of this new specialty aiming to protect American national and public well-being in the face of increasing and novel threats both inside and outside the United States. In this age of asymmetric warfare, increasing home-grown terrorism, and continuing threats from abroad, a new specialty has emerged and expanded—operational psychology. Operational psychology plays a unique role in supporting issues of national security, national defense, and public safety. In this book, authors Mark A. Staal and Sally C. Harvey, both operational psychologists and retired military colonels, lead a team of experts explaining the field, its many roles, and how it is expanding. Topics include its application in intelligence, counterintelligence, and counterterrorism activities, consultation in high-risk training, criminal investigations including those of internet crimes against children, threat assessment, interrogations, aviation, personnel selection, and leadership development. The text addresses the ethical questions and controversies that surround some of these roles, such as those associated with interrogation techniques. It also describes the role of operational psychologists in activities ranging from assessing and training people for maximum resiliency and hardiness to profiling people and groups of concern in national security investigations.
... Almost half a century ago, Smith and Glass (1977) started a movement to determine which version of psychotherapy to use by conducting the first meta-analysis that systematically and empirically summarized the psychotherapy outcome literature. Mihura et al. (2013) conducted the first large-scale psychological test validity meta-analyses on Rorschach construct validity. ...
Article
In this brief article, we update the training of newer versions of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) and Rorschach and compare to a 2015 assessment training survey of American Psychological Association accredited clinical psychology doctoral programs. The survey sample sizes for 2015, 2021, and 2022 were 83, 81, and 88, respectively. By 2015, of the programs teaching any adult MMPI version, almost all (94%) were still teaching the MMPI-2, and 68% had started teaching the MMPI-2-RF. In 2021 and 2022, respectively, almost all programs (96% and 94%) had started teaching the MMPI-2-RF or MMPI-3, although most were still teaching the MMPI-2 (77% and 66%). By 2015, of the programs teaching the Rorschach, 85% were still teaching the Comprehensive System (CS) and 60% had started teaching the Rorschach Performance Assessment System (R-PAS). In 2021 and 2022, respectively, most programs had started teaching R-PAS (77% and 77%) although many (65% and 50%) were still teaching the CS. Therefore, doctoral programs are indeed switching to newer versions of the MMPI and Rorschach, although more slowly than one might expect. We recommend that APA provide more guidance in selecting test versions for training programs, practitioners, and researchers.
... 111;Miller, 1985, blz. 98-100;Smelser, 1962, blz. 85. 128 Vasterman, 1999Vasterman, 2004, blz. ...
Book
Full-text available
In 'Van kwaad tot erger' wordt het debat over satanisch ritueel misbruik in de Verenigde Staten en Nederland beschreven, geanalyseerd en met elkaar vergeleken. Na een introductie van de maatschappelijke terreinen waarop de discussie over satanisch ritueel misbruik is en wordt gevoerd - de psychotherapie, de media en de strafrechtspraktijk - wordt het verloop van de sociale constructie van satanisch ritueel misbruik periodegewijs besproken aan de hand van relevante ontwikkelingen binnen die gebieden en van enkele geruchtmakende (straf)zaken, die grote invloed hebben gehad op het verloop van de discussie. Daarbij wordt diepgaand ingegaan op de overeenkomsten en verschillen in de maatschappelijke, religieuze en politieke structuur van de Verenigde Staten en Nederland. Tevens wordt in 'Van kwaad tot erger' een verklaring gegeven voor het feit dat het debat over satanisch ritueel misbruik in de Verenigde Staten is ontaard in een morele paniek, terwijl zoiets in Nederland is uitgebleven.
... It was even observed that random assignment did not make any difference in effectiveness. 143 Furthermore, one study showed that inexperienced therapists obtained the same results as professional therapists. 144 Tinnitus is still an emergent field and not enough studies have been done to reach these same kinds of conclusions. ...
Article
Full-text available
The Veterans Affairs (VA) Rehabilitation Research & Development (RR&D) National Center for Rehabilitative Auditory Research (NCRAR) was first funded by the RR&D Service in 1997 and has been funded continuously since that time. The overall purpose of the NCRAR is to “improve the quality of life of Veterans and others with hearing and balance problems through clinical research, technology development, and education that leads to better patient care” ( www.ncrar.research.va.gov ). An important component of the research conducted at the NCRAR has been a focus on clinical and rehabilitative aspects of tinnitus. Multiple investigators have received grants to conduct tinnitus research and the present article provides an overview of this research from the NCRAR's inception through 2021.
Article
Zunächst wird ein kurzer Abriß der Geschichte der Psychotherapieforschung und eine Darstellung der aktuellen Problematik der Psychotherapieforschung in und für die Praxis gegeben. An Hand eines Beispiels wird die Möglichkeit einer Selbstevaluation in der Praxis skizziert.
Article
L'articolo si rivolge ai clinici in formazione e ai professionisti esperti proponendo il metodo della ricerca empirica in psicoterapia come bussola per orientarsi di fronte ai sani dubbi che accompagnano il mestiere dello psicoterapeuta. L'autore accompagna il lettore a scoprire i vantaggi e i limiti dell'approccio em- pirico attraverso alcune tappe della sua storia. Prendendo in esame i fondamentali studi scientifici in questo ambito si propongono delle risposte ad alcuni quesiti che concernono la clinica. La psicoterapia è efficace e per quali pazienti? Quanto tempo è necessario perché vi siano dei cambiamenti? Quali sono le terapie più efficaci? Come funzionano? Quale è il ruolo del paziente, del terapeuta, della relazione e del metodo nel percorso terapeutico? Quali strumenti sviluppati nella ricerca empirica possono aiutare il clinico a implementare la qualità e l'efficacia dell'intervento?
Article
Identifying active ingredients of psychological interventions is a major goal of psychotherapy researchers that is often justified by the promise that it will lead to improved patient outcomes. Much of this "active ingredients" research is conducted within randomized controlled trials (RCTs) with patient populations, putting it in Phase T2 of the clinical-translational spectrum. I argue that RCTs in patient populations are very "messy laboratories" in which to conduct active ingredient work and that T0 and T1 research provide more controlled contexts. However, I call attention to the long road from identifying active ingredients of CBTs, whether in T0, T1, or T2 research, to improving outcomes. Dissemination and implementation research (T3 and T4 approaches) may be conceptually closer to improving outcomes. Given how common and disabling mental health symptoms are, I argue that if researchers want to improve patient outcomes, these research programs must receive more attention including work on the uptake of psychological interventions as well as work on optimal ordering of existing interventions.
Article
Tallies were made of outcomes of all reasonably controlled comparisons of psychotherapies with each other and with other treatments. For comparisons of psychotherapy with each other, most studies found insignificant differences in proportions of patients who improved (though most patients benefited). This "tie score effect" did not apply to psychotherapies vs psychopharmacotherapies compared singly—psychopharmacotherapies did better. Combined treatments often did better than single treatments. Among the comparisons, only two specially beneficial matches between type of patient and type of treatment were found. Our explanations for the usual tie score effect emphasize the common components among psychotherapies, especially the helping relationship with a therapist. However, we believe the research does not justify the conclusion that we should randomly assign patients to treatments—research results are usually based on amount of improvement; "amount" may not disclose differences in quality of improvement from each treatment.
Article
Michael J. Lambert of the Institute for Studies in Values and Human Behavior, Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, wrote this review of studies on the results of psychological treatment. This is a first volume in a series put out by Annual Research Reviews published by Eden Press.The work surveys the effectiveness of psychotherapy and the research methods for studying this. It skillfully clarifies many of the professional controversies and concludes that there is a definite, but modest, positive result from therapy when compared with no treatment, waiting lists, and placebos. There is a brief, incompletely explained summary of the deterioration in patients, which may be caused by therapy itself.The volume attempts to look primarily at individual psychotherapy, but it includes some material on group treatment, behavior therapy, and family therapy and covers nonmedical as well as medical therapists. (Reference is made throughout to clients rather than to
Article
The content of this volume represents the proceedings of the Second Conference on Research in Psychotherapy held in 1961. The book is organized according to three conference topics: 1. Research problems relating to measuring personality change in psychotherapy. The emphasis here rested on a thorough discussion of relevant variables in patients so as to allow significant assessments of change as a result of therapy; techniques for measuring personality change; selection of patients in terms of predictor variables. 2. Research problems relating to the psychotherapist's contribution to the treatment process. Questions to be dealt with under this heading included: ways of evaluating the contribution of the therapist's personality and attitudes upon progress and outcome of therapy; effects of variations in therapist behavior upon the process of therapy. 3. Research problems relating to the definition, measurement, and analysis of significant variables in psychotherapy, such as transference, resistance, etc. The purpose here was to bridge the gap between dynamic events observed in the clinical situation and their assessment and measurement by objective means. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
A survey was made of reports on the improvement of neurotic patients after psychotherapy, and the results compared with the best available estimates of recovery without benefit of such therapy. The figures fail to support the hypothesis that psychotherapy facilitates recovery from neurotic disorder. In view of the many difficulties attending such actuarial comparisons, no further conclusions could be derived from the data whose shortcomings highlight the necessity of properly planned and executed experimental studies into this important field." 40 references.
Article
Integrating findings: The meta-analysis of research Review of Research in Education, in press studies of psychotherapies. Archives of General Psy-chiatry Research in psychotherapy The evaluation of therapeutic outcomes Comparative 760 • SEPTEMBER
  • G V Glass
  • L Luborsky
  • B Singer
  • L Luborsky
Glass, G. V. Integrating findings: The meta-analysis of research. Review of Research in Education, in press. Luborsky, L., Singer, B., & Luborsky, L. studies of psychotherapies. Archives of General Psy-chiatry, 1975, 32, 995-1008. Meltzoff, J., & Kornreich, M. Research in psychotherapy. New York: Atherton, 1970. Psychotherapy caveat. APA Monitor, December 1974, p. 7. The evaluation of therapeutic outcomes. Comparative 760 • SEPTEMBER 1977 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST
A review of psychologically oriented treatment of alcoholism
  • C D Emrick
Emrick, C. D. A review of psychologically oriented treatment of alcoholism. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1975, 36, 88-108.
The benefits of psychotherapy
  • G V Glass
  • M L Smith
  • T I Miller
Glass, G. V, Smith, M. L., & Miller, T. I. The benefits of psychotherapy. Book in preparation, 1977.