ArticlePublisher preview available

A method for monitoring the impact of sex-role stereotypes on the therapeutic behavior of beginning psychotherapists

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

A methodology for assessing the therapeutic behavior of trainees is described, and results of a pilot study are presented. Findings suggest that female therapists are likely at the outset to be less confident, directive, and interpretive and more inclined to conform to stereotypic feminine traits of passivity, warmth, and nurturance; males displayed no such clear pattern, and used both active and passive styles in therapy. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved)
Amer.
J.
0rthop.s~~c.hicit.
52(3).
July
I982
A
METHOD
FOR
MONITORING THE IMPACT
BEHAVIOR
OF
BEGINNING PSYCHOTHERAPISTS
OF
SEX-ROLE STEREOTYPES ON THE THERAPEUTIC
Sara
Yogev,
Ph.D., and
William
R.
Shadish,
Jr.,
Ph.D.
A
tnc~thodology jbr ussrssing the therapeutic behavior
of
truinws is
de-
sc-rihed,
mid
results
of
u
pilot study are presented. Findings suggest that
.fi.mcilc
therupists ure likely
at
the outset
to
be less c.onfidc.nt, directiw
,
and
interpretive and more inclined
to
conform
to
stereotypic feminine traits
qf
pussivity, Mlarmth, and nurturance; males displayed
no
such
clear pattern,
and used both active and passive styles in therapy.
herapists. irrespective of their
theoretical orientations and per-
sonality differences, share certain basic
conceptions. Being a psychotherapist
means, on one hand, being an empathic
listener, providing basic acceptance,
understanding, and trust. On the other
hand, being a psychotherapist means
being active, analytical, and in charge of
guiding the diemx The aspect of em-
pathic understanding is consonant with
the nurturant, expressive sex-role stereo-
type of females
in
our society; it re-
quires the therapist
to
be sensitive to
the client's feelings,
to
understand and
to
follow the client. The aspect of being
active, providing objective interpreta-
tions, is consonant
with
the authoritat-
ive instrumental sex-role stereotype of
males
in
our society:
it
requires the
therapist to be
in
charge and to show the
client the dynamics "below the sur-
face."2*
The purpose of this study is to de-
velop a methodology
to
monitor thera-
peutic behavior of beginning psycho-
therapists
in
relation to sex-role stereo-
typic behavior. The focus is to develop
a means to assess how sex-role stereo-
types affect therapeutic interventions
and influence clinical work along the
two aspects mentioned earlier.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... For instance, Warburton et al. (1989) found that male and female supervises produced similar outcomes with their clients, but that female supervises tended to underestimate their influence whereas male supervises tended to overestimate their performance. Similarly, Yogev and Shadish (1982) found that female supervises were warm and nurturing but significantly less confident, directive, and interpretive than the male supervises who were both nurturing and directive. ...
Article
Uses a systemic cognitive-developmental framework to critique integration of gender issues into marriage and family counseling and therapy (MFC/T) supervision. The MFC/T field constructs its gender perspectives by oscillating between system exploration (challenging the predominant frame) and system consolidation (verifying and disseminating concepts scientifically). To develop unified theories, MFC/T supervisors need to have collaborative dialogs promoting multiple conceptualizations and approaches to gender within alternative and expanded formulations of supervision which can be operationalized and investigated. A systems enhancement orientation will occur when integrative dialogs among subgroups with permeable boundaries promote integrative dialog generating multiple perspectives which enhance each subgroup's assumptions. This will lead to system transformation, entailing reexamination of basic assumptions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
... This theory fits with sex role stereotypes and in fact has found some support in the research literature. A study of counseling trainees, presumably students seeing individual clients, did find that female trainees were warm and nurturing though significantly less confident, directive, and interpretive than males and that male trainees were both directive and nurturing (Yogev & Shadish, 1982). In a family therapy study that looked at therapy process in relation to therapist gender, Mas, Alexander, and Barton (1985) found that therapist gender has a significant impact on family therapists' and family members' verbalizations in the first sessions of family therapy. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper reports a study of 63 initial interviews of structural-strategic therapy with families with child-focused problems. These interviews were analyzed to determine differences in the process of therapy between male and female therapists. We tested specifically for differences in joining and executive skills between male and female therapists. Transcripts made from audiotapes of the interviews were coded with the Therapeutic Interaction Coding System (TICS). Thirtyseven cases completed therapy, and 26 dropped out of therapy against therapists' advice. There were no significant differences in male and female therapists' case completion rates. The overall level of supportive statements was not significantly different between males and females. Family members made more structuring or directive statements toward male therapists, but stated more disagreement with other family members when their therapist was female. Male therapists made more explanation statements than female therapists in response to family members' structuring or disagreement statements. In addition, we tested for differences in activity level and found that male therapists made more statements overall than did female therapists.
Article
Women who work with male perpetrators of violence against women such as batterers and sex offenders face unique challenges in their roles as counselors. This article explores some of the issues female counselors face in counseling male perpetrators of violence against women. The manner in which gender, gender role, therapist characteristics, ethnicity, counselor power, racism, countertransference reactions, and relationship with a male co-facilitator interact to impact on the counseling process and the female counselor is discussed. Recommendations are made to assist beginning counselors as well as women who practice in this area.
Article
Effects of the sex of the counselor and of gender role orientation on client ratings of counselors-in-training were examined in a group (N=65) of graduate counselor trainees. An interaction between sex and gender role orientation was found for differences in clients' willingness to refer a friend to the counselor. Implications for training and research are discussed. (Author/KW)
Article
Although family therapists are examining gender in families, there has been little exploration of gender dynamics between the therapist and family members. From a social constructivist position, this paper explores the hypothesis that certain repetitive patterns or impasses emerge between women family therapists and male clients, related to dominant cultural understandings about male and female nature. Special attention is paid to beliefs about differences between men and women; relational responsibility and the world of feelings; gender and anger; and power.
Article
Full-text available
22 female (mean age 31.8 yrs) and 16 male (mean age 27.5 yrs) practicum students in clinical psychology completed the Bem Sex-Role Inventory and a questionnaire assessing their attitudes toward clinical work. Females were more likely than males to work predominantly with children. Masculine and undifferentiated Ss attributed the source of their theoretical orientation to their clinical work, whereas feminine and androgynous Ss considered their own personal treatment as important a source of influence as clinical work. Masculine Ss viewed hearing about important changes in patients' lives as more gratifying than direct observation of change, while other Ss viewed the direct observation of change as most gratifying. (20 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Two studies investigated whether counselor androgyny is related to more flexibility, whether counselor androgyny is related to an enhanced counseling process, and whether flexible counseling style is related to enhanced counseling process. In Study 1, 41 counseling students completed the Bem Sex-Role Inventory (BSRI) and the Counselor Behavior Analysis Inventory. There was no relationship between androgyny and counseling flexibility. In Study 2, 42 counseling students (who counseled 42 Ss acting as clients) completed the BSRI and were videotaped in counseling sessions with a client. There was a significant relationship between counselor flexibility and client ratings of working alliance, but no relationship between counselor androgyny and counselor flexibility. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Presents a 4-quadrant model that differentiates between the effects of sex and gender roles on the therapy process. Theory and research findings in this area can be conceptualized in terms of statements regarding the sex or gender role of the client or the counselor. The literature on the sex and gender role pairings of clients and counselors is reviewed from this 4-quadrant perspective. It is concluded that while sex and gender roles exert at least a moderate effect on the therapy process, definitive conclusions on these effects have yet to be reached. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Since Freud's era, women have been counseling and psychotherapy's largest consumers. White Freud talked and wrote almost exclusively about men, his patients were primarily women. This practice of treating women as patients, while perceiving men as the prevailing standard for percept and practice has resulted in the largely androcentric or mate-determined psychology of human behavior which we have today (Denmark, 1980; Hare-Mustin, 1983). A number of studies commencing with the Broverman's work in the 1970(s) has demonstrated that psychotherapists, for the most part, share the same sex role biases toward women as the rest of society (Broverman et al., 1970; Fabrikant, 1973, 1974; Nickerson, 1978; Orlinsky & Howard, 1980). Most disconcerting, however, is a recent extensive replication of the Brovermans' study (O'Malley & Richardson, 1985), using 249 counselors, which found that they still described males and females in stereotypic ways, though they did attribute some female designated traits to healthy adults. The evidence regarding the measured effects of therapists' attitudes, whether reportedly biased or gender-fair, however, is mixed in nature and replete with methodological problems (Maffeo, 1979; Marachek & Johnson, 1980; Orlinsky & Howard, 1980; Smith, 1980; Whiteley, 1979); as is the evidence for effectiveness of therapy generally (Garfield, 1981). Some have argued that a counselor's values and expectations do not matter, as long as she or he is aware and does not impose them (Smith, 1980; Whiteley, 1979). It would seem equally probable, based on other evidence that choice of therapeutic goals and processes can be influenced by the therapist's expectations (Brovermans et al., 1970; Nickerson, 1978; O'Malley & Richardson, 1985). The crucial point is that in a sexist society (Such as the United States), it is improbable that anyone without special non-sexist, gender-fair training can adequately aid women in exercising their independence and selecting their goals. Hence, a format is presented for insuring the dream of gender-fair counseling through the inclusion of appropriate training experiences and materials.
Article
Full-text available
A new procedure for determining the optimal number of interpretable factors to extract from a correlation matrix is introduced and compared to more conventional procedures. The new method evaluates the magnitude of the Very Simple Structure index of goodness of fit for factor solutions of increas- ing rank. The number of factors which maximizes the VSS criterion is taken as being the optimal number of factors to extract. Thirty-two artificial and two real data sets are used in order to oompare this procedure with wch methods as maximum likelihood, the eigenvalue greater than 1.0 rule, and comparison of the observed eigenvalues with those expected from random data. A frequent point of concern in measurement is the proper number of constructs to measure. Give11 a particular domain of items or of tests, what is the best way to describe the domain? Is it better to have a few, broad factors, or many, narrow ones? This trade off between parsimony and completeness, or between simplicity and complexity is debated frequently. In psychome1;rics we call this the number of factors problem. As would be expected, there is a wide variety of proposed solutions to this problem which may be grouped into three major approaches: the use of theoretical arguments, psychometric rules af thumb, and statistical estimates of goodness of fit. We would like to introduce a procedure which is a conglomerate of all three approaches and to compare this pro- cedure to a variety of other decision )rules for determining the number of factors to extract from a given data set. To do this we first will outline our procedure and then make comparisons on thirty-two artificial data sets with known structure and two real data sets with inferred structures. As we have already noted, there are t:hree major wajrs to determine how many factors to extract to summarize and describe a particular data set. Of these three, theoretical principles are probably the simplest; extract and rotate factors only as long as
Article
Full-text available
Gave a sex-role stereotype questionnaire consisting of 122 bipolar items to 79 actively functioning clinicians with 1 of 3 sets of instructions: to describe a healthy, mature, socially competent (a) adult, sex unspecified, (b) a man, or (c) a woman. It was hypothesized that clinical judgments about the characteristics of healthy individuals would differ as a function of sex of person judged, and that these differences would parallel sterotypic sex-role differences. A 2nd hypothesis predicted that behaviors and characteristics judged healthy for an adult, sex unspecified, which are presumed to reflect an ideal standard of health, will resemble behaviors judged healthy for men, but not for women. Both hypotheses were confirmed. (21 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
A common problem in the social sciences is to form a set of relatively independent and internally consistent scales from a large pool of items. Frequently, these scales are formed by simply summing the responses to keyed items. The problem, then, is to determine how best to partition the initial set of items into subsets or scales that are highly internally consistent and relatively independent. A common alternative is to factor analyze the interitem correlation matrix and then to select items on the basis of factor loadings. Those items with a high loading on a particular factor are combined into a scale by applying unit weights to the items. This method, although probably the most common scale construction procedure, has several drawbacks: Inter­ item correlations are usually small (average interitem correlations ~.3) and the sample sizes are usually not much larger than the number of items. These problems tend to lead to overfactoring (extracting too many factors), unstable rotations, and generally nonsensical solutions. In fact, because of the problems encountered in factoring items, many experienced factor analysts recommend against such procedures (Cattell, 1973; Comrey, 1961; Nunnally, 1967). However, a sampling of journals in the social sciences suggests that this advice is rarely followed. When the item pool is large (greater than 10-20 items), when the item intercorrelations are small (between 0.0 and .5), or when the sample sizes are small, an alterna­ tive method that is particularly appropriate is cluster analysis. Cluster analysis is a loosely defined set of procedures associated with the partitioning of a set of objects into nonoverlapping groups or clusters (Everitt, 1974; Hartigan, 1975). Although normally used to group objects, occasionally cluster analysis has been applied to the problem of grouping variables and, as such, is similar to procedures of group factor analysis (Loevinger, Gieser, & Dubois, 1953; Revelle, in press; Tryon & Bailey, 1970). A disadvantage for scale construction of many clustering procedures is that they do not include basic psychometric decision rules to evaluate either the quality or the number of clusters to extract. It is possible, though, to combine psychometric principles with clustering procedures. This combination results in a simple but useful approach to scale construction, and, for forming scales from items, may be compared
Article
Full-text available
Describes the development of a new sex-role inventory that treats masculinity and femininity as 2 independent dimensions, thereby making it possible to characterize a person as masculine, feminine, or "androgynous" as a function of the difference between his or her endorsement of masculine and feminine personality characteristics. Normative data, provided by 561 male and 356 female college and junior college students, are presented, as well as the results of various psychometric analyses. Findings indicate that: (a) The dimensions of masculinity and femininity are empirically and logically independent. (b) The concept of psychological androgyny is a reliable one. (c) Highly sex-typed scores do not reflect a general tendency to respond in a socially desirable direction, but rather a specific tendency to describe oneself in accordance with sex-typed standards of desirable behavior for men and women. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Sex role stereotypes in clinical judgements of mental health
  • C E I R R O V E H M A N
  • Ai
R R O V E H M A N. c. E.I. AI. 1970. Sex role stereotypes in clinical judgements of mental health. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 34:l-7.
Semantic Differential Technique
  • Osgood
OSGOOD. c, 1969. Semantic Differential Technique. Aldine, Chicago.
Very simple structure: an alternative procedure for estimating the optimal numher of interpretable factors
  • I R T V E L L H , W . A N D Rocki In
R t v E L L h, w. A N D ROCKI IN. I. 1979. Very simple structure: an alternative procedure for estimating the optimal numher of interpretable factors. Multivar. Behav. Res. 14:403-414.
Psychotherapy: Clinical Research and Theoretical Issues
  • H S I R U P P
S I R U P P, H. 1973. Psychotherapy: Clinical Research and Theoretical Issues. Aronson. New York.
  • Strupp