ArticlePDF Available

Fear and the Human Amygdala

Authors:

Abstract

We have previously reported that bilateral amygdala damage in humans compromises the recognition of fear in facial expressions while leaving intact recognition of face identity (Adolphs et al., 1994). The present study aims at examining questions motivated by this finding. We addressed the possibility that unilateral amygdala damage might be sufficient to impair recognition of emotional expressions. We also obtained further data on our subject with bilateral amygdala damage, in order to elucidate possible mechanisms that could account for the impaired recognition of expressions of fear. The results show that bilateral, but not unilateral, damage to the human amygdala impairs the processing of fearful facial expressions. This impairment appears to result from an insensitivity to the intensity of fear expressed by faces. We also confirmed a double dissociation between the recognition of facial expressions of fear, and the recognition of identity of a face: these two processes can be impaired independently, lending support to the idea that they are subserved in part by anatomically separate neural systems. Based on our data, and on what is known about the amygdala's connectivity, we propose that the amygdala is required to link visual representations of facial expressions, on the one hand, with representations that constitute the concept of fear, on the other. Preliminary data suggest the amygdala's role extends to both recognition and recall of fearful facial expressions.
The Journal of Neuroscience, September 1995, 75(g): 5879-5891
Fear and the Human Amygdala
Ralph Adolphs,’ Daniel Tranel,’ Hanna Damasio1s2 and Antonio R. Damasio1’2
‘Department of Neurology, Division of Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Iowa College of Medicine, Iowa City,
Iowa and ‘The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, California
We have previously reported that bilateral amygdala dam-
age in humans compromises the recognition of fear in fa-
cial expressions while leaving intact recognition of face
identity (Adolphs et al., 1994). The present study aims at
examining questions motivated by this finding. We ad-
dressed the possibility that unilateral amygdala damage
might be sufficient to impair recognition of emotional ex-
pressions. We also obtained further data on our subject
with bilateral amygdala damage, in order to elucidate pos-
sible mechanisms that could account for the impaired rec-
ognition of expressions of fear.
The results show that bilateral, but not unilateral, dam-
age to the human amygdala impairs the processing of fear-
ful facial expressions. This impairment appears to result
from an insensitivity to the intensity of fear expressed by
faces. We also confirmed a double dissociation between
the recognition of facial expressions of fear, and the rec-
ognition of identity of a face: these two processes can be
impaired independently, lending support to the idea that
they are subserved in part by anatomically separate neural
systems. Based on our data, and on what is known about
the amygdala’s connectivity, we propose that the amygdala
is required to link visual representations of facial expres-
sions, on the one hand, with representations that consti-
tute the concept of fear, on the other. Preliminary data sug-
gest the amygdala’s role extends to both recognition and
recall of fearful facial expressions.
[Key words: amygdala, emotion, affect, fear, facial ex-
pression, social cognition]
As borne out by two recent reports on human amygdala dam-
age (Adolphs et al., 1994; Young et al., 199.5) the amygdala
is ideally suited to participate in processing visual stimuli that
communicate emotional significance in social situations, such
as facial expressions (Darwin, 1872/1965; Stein et al., 1990;
Kling and Brothers, 1992). We previously reported that bilat-
eral damage to the human amygdala impairs the recognition of
fear in facial expressions (Adolphs et al., 1994). This finding
left open the possibility that unilateral amygdala damage could
produce the same deficit, a question we address here. An ad-
ditional purpose of the present study is to provide a more com-
plete characterization of the processing defect that results from
Received Feb. X, 1995; revised Apr. 25, 1995; accepted Apr. 28, 1995.
We thank all subjects for their participation. This study was supported by
NINDS Grant NS 19632. R.A. is a Burroughs-Wellcome Fund Fellow of the
Life Sciences Research Foundation.
Correspondence should be addressed to Ralph Adolphs, Department of Neu-
rology, 200 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA 52242.
Copyright 0 1995 Society for Neuroscience 0270.6474/95/155879-13$05.00/O
bilateral amygdala damage, in order to suggest mechanisms by
which the amygdala helps to process stimuli related to fear.
Since our study involves vision and emotion, we briefly re-
view the amygdala’s role in these two domains. Highly pro-
cessed visual information reaches the amygdala via temporal
cortices, including areas TE, TEO, and the temporal pole (Her-
zog and Van Hoesen, 1976; Iwai and Yukie, 1987; Amaral et
al., 1992). The inferotemporal cortex in the monkey contains
neurons that respond selectively to the sight of faces and bodies
of conspecifics. These responses can be selective for a particular
face, or a particular view of faces or bodies, for a particular
direction of gaze in the face, or for facial expressions (Perrett
et al., 1982; Hasselmo et al., 1989; Wachsmuth et al., 1994). In
humans, bilateral lesions of the temporal cortex are usually re-
quired to impair recognition of the identity of faces (Damasio
et al., 1990), but unilateral lesions of the right temporal cortex
can be sufficient to impair recognition of emotional expressions
(Bowers and Heilman, 1984; Rapcsak et al., 1989; Rapcsak et
al., 1993). While both the human (Heit et al., 1988; Allison et
al., 1994) and monkey (Leonard et al., 198.5; Rolls, 1992) amyg-
dala also contain neurons that respond selectively to faces, little
is known about the functional significance of this finding. Given
our previous finding that bilateral amygdala damage causes spe-
cific deficits in recognizing facial expressions, we wanted (1) to
investigate the unilateral contributions of the left and right
amygdala independently, and (2) to show how specific to facial
expressions of fear the deficits are.
Evidence suggesting that the amygdala participates in emo-
tion is found in the close links between the amygdala and the
autonomic nervous system (Chapman et al., 1954; LeDoux et
al., 1988; Lee et al., 1988a; Amaral et al., 1992), as well as in
the high densities of binding sites within the amygdala to sub-
stances that modulate fear and aggression, such as the benzo-
diazepines (Niehoff and Kuhar, 1983) and serotonin (Hensman
et al., 1991; Saudou et al., 1994). Some neurons in the monkey
amygdala respond to the affective significance (the rewarding
or punishing contingencies) of sensory stimuli (Nishijo et al.,
1988). Monkeys with lesions of the amygdala are insensitive
to stimuli that normally arouse intense fear (Weiskrantz, 1956;
Blanchard and Blanchard, 1972) and cannot be conditioned to
associate stimuli with fear (Kapp et al., 1982; Hitchcock and
Davis, 1986; LeDoux et al., 1990; Davis, 1992a,b). While
many of these findings may extrapolate to humans, we won-
dered if the amygdala would be equally involved in processing
components of the concept of fear that depend mostly on lan-
guage, as well as those that depend mostly on nonverbal im-
ages. We show here that while bilateral amygdala damage im-
pairs recognition of fearful facial expressions, and impairs their
5880 Adolphs et al. * Fear and the Human Amygdala
recall in imagery, it leaves relatively unaffected verbal knowl-
edge related to fear.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
We compared target subjects, who all had damage to the amygdala, to
a group of brain-damaged controls, who had no damage to the amyg-
dala. The target subjects were chosen on the basis of neuroanatomical
site of lesions, and consisted of one patient with bilateral damage to the
amygdala, three patients with unilateral damage to the left, and three
with unilateral damage to the right amygdala. Control subjects were 12
patients with brain damage, but intact amygdalae, and 7 normal indi-
viduals with no history of neurological or psychiatric disease. Both
target and control brain-damaged subjects were selected from the Pa-
tient Registry of the Division of Behavioral Neurology and Cognitive
Neuroscience at the University of Iowa, and had been fully character-
ized neuroanatomically and neuropsychologically according to the stan-
dard protocols of the Benton Neuropsychology Laboratory (Tranel,
1995) and the Laboratory of Neuroimaging and Human Neuroanatomy
(Damasio and Damasio, 1989; Damasio and Frank, 1992). For each
subject, MR and/or CT scan data were available, and additional neu-
roanatomical analyses were carried out as necessary. Three-dimensional
reconstructions of MR images were obtained for all target subjects in
order to allow a detailed analysis of their lesions (Damasio and Frank,
1992). We excluded subjects who were significantly depressed, as as-
sessed with the Beck Depression Inventory and the Minnesota Multi-
phasic Personality Inventory. All subjects used in our study had given
informed consent to participate in research.
Bilateral amygdalu lesions. Patient SM-046 had nearly complete bi-
lateral amygdala lesions. This subject suffers from a rare, autosomal
recessive, genetic disease, Urbach-Wiethe disease (Hofer, 1973), which
has resulted in bilateral calcification and atrophy of her amygdala (Tra-
nel and Hyman, 1990; Nahm et al., 1993; Adolphs et al., 1994).
Unilaterul umygdala lesions. The six subjects in this group had uni-
lateral damage to the amygdala and to other cortical and subcortical
regions in the anterior temporal lobe, caused by either temporal lobec-
tomy for the treatment of epilepsy, or by herpes simplex encephalitis.
Bruin-damaged controls. Twelve subjects with lesions that did not
include the amygdala were selected to address a number of possibly
confounding variables.
To conclude that a target subject might be impaired in processing
facial expressions of emotion, we needed to exclude the possibility that
defective task performance could be attributed solely to confounding
variables that could also affect performance on our tasks. These vari-
ables included IQ and visuoperceptual function. Our 12 brain-damaged
controls, consequently, included subjects with IQ similar to, or lower
than, that of subjects with amygdala damage. We addressed the possi-
bility of an effect due to the visuoperceptual demands of the task by
including brain-damaged controls who had poor visual acuity or visual
field defects, and who performed at or below the level of target subjects
on standard tests of visuoperceptual function. To control for the bila-
terality of brain damage of SM.046 we included four control subjects
with bilateral brain damage. All 12 subjects had participated as controls
in a previous study of the recognition of emotion in facial expressions
(Adolphs et al., 1994), and included patients with histories of stroke,
tumor, or anoxia.
Normal controls. Seven normal controls, with no history of neuro-
logical and psychiatric disease, were also included in our study, in order
to provide an independent, normal group to which results from subjects
in other groups could be compared.
Experimental tasks
Recognition of facial expressions of emotion. Subjects were shown
black and white slides of facial expressions, and were asked to judge
the expressions with respect to several verbal labels (which were the
adjectives that corresponded to the emotions we showed), as reported
previously (Adolphs et al., 1994). We chose 39 facial expressions from
Ekman and Friesen (Ekman, 1976) that had all been shown to be iden-
tified reliably by normal subjects at greater than 80% success rate. Each
of the 39 expressions was presented six times in two blocks separated
by several hours. Six faces (both male and female) each of anger, fear,
happiness, surprise, sadness, disgust, and three neutral faces, were pro-
jected on a screen, one at a time, in randomized order. Subjects had in
front of them cards with verbal labels describing emotions typed in large
print, of which they were periodically reminded by the experimenter.
Prior to rating faces on any label, subjects were involved in a brief
discussion that clarified the meaning of that word through examples.
Subjects were asked to judge each face on a scale of O-5 (0 = not at
all; 5 = very much) on the following six labels: happy, sad, disgusted,
angry, afraid, surprised (one adjective per block of slides), in random
order. There was no time limit. Care was taken to ensure that all subjects
knew which label they were rating and used the scale correctly. All
subjects understood the labels, as assessed by their ability to describe
appropriately scenarios pertaining to that emotional label.
SM-046 repeated the experiment a total of four times, in two sessions
6 months apart. In both sessions, she was tested by different experi-
menters who were blind to the nature of the experiment, and by R.A.
We also replicated the experiments with six of the brain-damaged con-
trols (data not shown), all of whom gave very consistent intrasubject
ratings.
Data from the task above were analyzed to yield three measures: (1)
subjects’ ratings of a facial expression on the verbal label that corre-
sponded to the emotion expressed. This measure is an index of the
intensity with which subjects judge a prototypical emotion to be ex-
pressed. (2) Correlations between a subject’s ratings of an expression
and normal ratings. This is a measure of recognition of facial expres-
sions of emotion (Adolphs et al., 1994). A subject’s ratings of a facial
expression on the six adjectives were correlated with mean ratings of
that expression given by the seven normal controls. The correlations
were Z-transformed to normalize their distribution, averaged over faces
that expressed the same emotion, and inverse Z-transformed to give the
mean Pearson correlation to normal ratings for that emotion category.
(3) Profile of ratings on all six emotional labels for each of the 39
individual facial expressions. An analysis of the raw ratings given by
subjects allowed us to investigate further those aspects of task perfor-
mance that may have resulted in defective recognition. Details are pro-
vided in the legend to Figure 5.
Recognition of identity of a face. We also assessed subjects’ ability
to recognize and name familiar faces, to provide further evidence that
recognition of the identity and of the emotion expressed by a face are
dissociable (Damasio et al., 1990). Subiects were shown black and
white slides of faces that were cropped to mclude only the head. Stimuli
consisted of faces of famous public figures, or of acquaintances of the
patient. The scores from these tests are presented as “impaired,” “bor-
derline,” or “normal,”
as previously described (Tranel et al., in press).
Drawing facial expressions. The tasks described above do not ex-
haust in any way the characterization of the defect in recognizing fear
in facial expressions. To begin further inquiry on this issue, we carried
out additional, qualitative studies with patient SM-046.
To assess SM-046’s ability to retrieve nonverbal information pertinent
to fear, we asked her, as well as four of our subjects with unilateral
amygdala lesions and four control subjects, to draw pictures of facial
expressions from memory, given both the oral and written command.
She was given unlimited time and was repeatedly prompted by the
experimenter. SM.046 had taken a correspondence art class in drawing,
and enjoyed drawing.
The verbal concept of fear. Interviews were conducted to address
whether SM-046’s defect extends to defective conceptual knowledge of
fear. Her recognition of the meaning of words that denote emotional
states was assessed by asking her to sort printed labels of the words
into piles on the basis of the similarity of the emotion they denote.
Results
Neuroanatomical projiles
MR scans of the subjects with amygdala damage are shown in
Figures 1 and 2. Subject SM-046 had damage that was restricted
to the amygdala, and which may have included some of the very
anterior entorhinal cortex, sparing hippocampus and all neocor-
tical regions (Fig. 1). All of neocortex and the hippocampi are
spared, thus providing a particularly specific case study with
selective amygdala lesions. PET scans confirmed that the tissue
of the amygdala was hypometabolic, consistent with atrophy of
this structure (data not shown).
Our subjects with unilateral amygdala damage included tem-
poral lobectomy cases and two subjects with limitkd damage
from herpes simplex encephalitis (LDV-692 and FR-1465). The
subjects all had unilateral damage to the amygdala and to some
of the surrounding structures in the temporal lobe (Fig. 2).
Neuropsychological profdes
SM-046 is fully dextrous, has an IQ in the low average range,
and a high school education. She has intact visual fields and
normal acuity, can read and write fluently, and has normal color
perception. She has no impairments of memory, language, or
perception, with the possible exception of somewhat impaired
immediate nonverbal memory (Tranel and Hyman, 1990) and
borderline olfactory recognition.
The ability of SM-046 to match simple stimuli across different
sensory modalities, as evidenced by visual-tactile associations
of simple geometric shapes, is normal (Nahm et al., 1993). SM-
046 has an inadequate ability to make decisions in regard to
personal and social matters, and has somewhat inappropriate so-
cial behavior (Tranel and Hyman, 1990; Nahm et al., 1993).
Our subjects with unilateral amygdala damage are all fully
dextrous, and include patients with a large range of IQ. All target
subjects had normal, or corrected to normal, visual acuity, and
none had visual field defects. Table 1 summarizes these data.
Judgments of intensity of emotional facial expressions
We first examined the intensity of emotion that subjects judged
to be signalled by a face. For each face within an emotion
category, subjects rated that face on its prototypical verbal la-
bel (e.g., for happy faces,
“how happy does this person
look?“), on a 6-point scale. Brain-damaged and normal con-
trols, as well as subjects with unilateral amygdala lesions all
endorsed high ratings on this task across every emotion cate-
gory (Fig. 3a), indicating that the expressions were, indeed,
strong, typical expressions of their respective emotions. There
were no significant differences between the ratings given by
brain-damaged controls, normal controls, or subjects with uni-
lateral amygdala damage. While all subjects with left amygdala
lesions rated faces showing disgust or sadness as slightly less
intense than did subjects with right amygdala lesions (p = 0.05
for both emotions; Mann-Whitney U test), we do not think this
finding is significant for our study, since all six subjects’ rat-
ings fell within the range of ratings given by brain-damaged
controls. Future studies, with planned comparisons between
larger numbers of unilaterally lesioned subjects, might follow
up this finding in relation to possible hemispheric asymmetries
in processing emotion, as has been suggested for other brain
regions (e.g., Davidson, 1992).
Our subject with bilateral amygdala damage, SM-046, judged
faces showing fear to be less intense expressions of that emotion
than did any of the brain-damaged controls, or than did any of
the subjects with unilateral amygdala damage (Fig. 3b). SM-046
also differed from brain-damaged controls in her judgments of
faces expressing emotions normally judged to be similar to fear:
she judged surprised and angry faces to signal less surprise or
anger than did controls. Her average ratings of surprise and fear
were 5 SD below the mean of thecbrain-damaged controls’ rat-
ings; her ratings of anger were 3 SD below the control mean,
and her ratings of all other emotions were within 1 SD of the
control mean. These results suggest that bilateral, but not uni-
lateral, damage to the amygdala impairs judgments of the inten-
sity of expressions of fear, and of expressions normally judged
to be very similar to fear, such as surprise.
The Journal of Neuroscience, September 1995, k?(9)
5881
Figure
1. MR scans of the brain of SM-046. From
top to bottom:
coronal section at the level of the hippocampus; coronal section at the
level of the amygdala; horizontal section at the level of the amygdala.
SM-046 has extensive and bilateral damage to the amygdala
(arrows),
but not to other structures,
such
as the hippocampus
(top).
Recognition of emotional facial expressions
To assess recognition of facial expressions, we asked subjects to
rate the face not just on the verbal label that described it best,
but also on labels denoting other emotions. The rationale for this
5882 Adolphs et al.
l
Fear and the Human Amygdala
A.
Figure 2.
Sites of lesions of
subjects
with unilateral amygdala damage. A,
Subjects with left amygdala lesions. B,
Subjects with right amygdala lesions.
For each subject, coronal and horizon-
tal sections at the level of the amygdala
are shown at leji and right, respective-
ly. The intact amygdala on one side is
indicated with an asterisk. Note that the
left side of the image corresponds to
the right side of the brain, and vice ver-
sa.
method came from previous studies with normal subjects, which
typical facial expression of surprise, and yet also be judged to
showed that facial expressions signal more than one emotion,
exhibit some happiness, or some fear. Asking subjects to apply
and that emotion categories have fuzzy and overlapping bound-
a single label to a facial expression will thus not be a complete
aries (Russell and Bullock, 1986; Russell and Fehr, 1987; Rus-
index of all the information recognized, and is not a sensitive
sell, 1991). For example, a surprised person can have a proto-
test of recognition of emotion.
B.
The Journal of Net Jroscience, September 1995, 15(9)
5883
Figure 2.
Continued.
Brain-damaged controls and subjects with unilateral amygdala
domed ratings for afraid faces that correlated very poorly, or not
damage gave ratings of the faces that correlated well with nor- at all, with normal ratings, and which correlated more poorly
mal ratings (Fig. 4). Our subject with bilateral amygdala dam-
with normal ratings than did the ratings given by any brain-
age, by contrast, had a severe impairment in the ability to rec- damaged control (Fig. 4).
ognize some expressions, as assessed by this task. SM-046 en-
These data show that bilateral, but not unilateral, damage to
5884 Adolphs et al.
l
Fear and the Human Amygdala
Table 1. Background neuropsycbology of subjects
Visuoperceptual
function (%ile)
Subject Age/sex
VIQ PIQ
Lines Faces
4.5
Bilateral amygdala damage
SM-046 30/F 86
Left amygdala damage
LV-580 31/F
81
JS-1077 22&l
129
UB-1251 35/M 93
Right amygdala damage
LDV-692 3 l/F 87
FR-1465 64lM
100
AK- 1603 25/F 106
Brain-damaged controls
CE- 1023 68/M 117
m-1257 73/M
117
RM- 1362 68/M
96
RJ- 1374” 52/M 96
JR-1584 51/M 90
RJ- 1605 49/M
1146
DE- 1607 77/F
98
RS-165@ 49/M 101
BL- 1 722d 52/F 85
RM- 1740 63/M
94
MR- l79@ 62/F 97
90 11th 90th
112 40th 49th
126 74th 8th
99 22nd 49th
77 11th
4th
130 72nd 77th
133 56th 32nd
'r;
3
a
a 2.5
5
z 2
1.5
96
93
110
96
111
112b
90
82
85
101
80
115
40th
86th
86th
NT
99th
NT
22nd
4th
0th
NT
6th
97th
15th
7lst
49th
15th
49th
71st
49th
NT
77th
71st
happy surprise
sgusted sad
0
Brain-damaged controls
-- Normal controls
4 Left Amygdala damage
b Right Amygdala damage
0 SM-046
56th CF- 1899 64/M 120
VIQ. PIQ: verbal and performance IQs from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Test-Revised (WAIS-R) (Wechsler, 1981). Lines: ability to discriminate be-
tween orientation of lines (Benton et al., 1983). Faces: ability to discriminate
between similar faces (Benton et al., 1983). Underlined subjects have bilateral
lesions. NT: Not tested.
(’ Blind in one visual field.
h Estimated from the National Adult Test-Revised.
Perimetric mapping indicated complete blindness in the left visual field, and
macular sparing not extending past 10” eccentricity in the right visual field.
Nearly blind in left eye; achromatopsic.
5_
4.5-r,
cm
41
un
3.5-3;
Cl
El
Cl
cl
El
Cl
0.5:
0
happy
mprise afraid
Cl
Cl
cl
Cl
0
cl
m
cl
all
0
isguste sad
the amygdala impairs the processing of some facial expressions
of emotion. The recognition of fear appears to be most severely
impaired.
Recognition
of
multiple emotion categories
The impaired recognition of expressions of fear seen in SM-046
could have several causes at the level of task performance. She
might not know what fearful faces signal at all, or she might
misrecognize fear as another emotion. To address this issue, we
analyzed each facial expression’s membership in different emo-
tion categories. Figure 5 shows the ratings given by subjects for
all the faces, on all the different adjectives that describe emotions.
Controls show peaks in their rating profiles that coincide with the
facial expression most typical of that adjective: happy faces re-
ceived the highest ratings on “happf,” sad faces on “sad,” and
so on. Several important points emerge from the data of the con-
trols
(both
brain-damaged and normal controls): (1) there are pro-
totypical expressions that best exemplify an emotion, (2) most
faces belong largely to one emotion category, (3) most faces also
have some membership in other categories of emotions that are
judged to be similar, (4) most faces therefore signal multiple emo-
Figure
3. Ratings of the intensity of an emotion in facial expressions.
Rating scores (from O-5) on the emotional word for which the face was
a typical example are shown as the mean of all faces within an emotion
category.
A,
Data from 12 brain-damaged controls
(0).
3 subjects with
left (d), and 3 with right (b) amygdala damage. B, Mean data from
four experiments with SM-046 (0).
The Journal of Neuroscience, September 1995, 15(9) 5885
0 Controls
0
happy surprisec afraid
b Right
Amygdala
q Left
Amygdala
8
?
El
4
0
angry
isgustel
!
t
8
4
sad
0.5-
0
happy
El
0
;urprisc
a SM-046
El
q
afraic
Ei
awry
u
isgusl
0
El
sad
Emotion Category of Faces
Figure 4. Correlations of subjects’ ratings with normal ratings. Mean Pearson correlations are shown of mean normal control ratings (n = 7)
correlated with the ratings given by brain-damaged controls (0;)
II = 12, by subjects with left amygdala lesions (4; IZ = 3), and subjects with
right amygdala lesions (b; n = 3) (left), and correlated with the ratings given by SM-046 (0; four experiments) (right). All normal controls’ ratings
c&elatedwell with one another (Y 5 0.7 for every emotion category).
tions. These observations have been made in previous studies of
normal subjects (e.g., Russell and Bullock, 1986).
Normal controls, brain-damaged controls, and patients with
unilateral amygdala damage yielded similar data in this analysis.
However, SM-046 differed from all other subjects in the ratings
she assigned to some faces (Fig. 5). SM-046 failed to assign
high ratings of fear to faces that controls judge to depict fear.
She also failed to assign normal ratings of fear or of surprise to
faces that are judged similar to fear, such as surprised faces.
However, the highest ratings that SM-046 gave on the adjective
“afraid” do correspond to faces expressing fear or surprise. In
one session (January) she generally gave such faces higher
scores on adjectives denoting emotions other than fear, leading
to classification of the expression into an incorrect emotion cat-
egory. Consequently, SM-046 often described faces depicting
fear as surprised or angry. In another session (July), she did not
give fearful faces high ratings on any emotion label (Fig. 5).
We followed up this experiment by asking SM-046 explicitly
to comment in more detail about those fearful facial expressions
that she had been unable to recognize, and that she had rated as
not being afraid. Although she could recognize that an emotion
was being expressed (she did not think they were neutral), she
was unsure of what the emotion might be, supplying answers
that included surprise, anger, disgust, sadness, and, on some oc-
cassions, fear.
Given these data, and the further finding that SM-046 never
attributed a clearly absent emotion to faces expressing fear (she
never called them happy, for instance), we think that the primary
defect in SM-046 consists in a specific insensitivity to the emo-
tion of fear in facial expressions, with intact recognition of all
other emotions.
Double dissociation between recognition
of
identity
and
expression
SM-046 had no difficulty recognizing people by their faces. She
correctly identified 19 of 19 acquaintances (Adolphs et al.,
1994), and learned the identity of new faces with ease (Table
2). She also performed normally in discriminating similar faces
from one another (Tranel and Hyman, 1990; see Table 1).
Several of the patients with unilateral amygdala lesions had
difficulty in naming familiar faces, which we have previously
attributed to damage to anterior temporal cortices (Damasio et
al., 1990). The two patients with the most extensive lesions of
the temporal lobe (LDV-692 and FR-1465) were also signifi-
cantly impaired in recognizing the identity of faces (Table 2).
These data provide additional evidence for a double dissoci-
ation (cf. McCarthy and Warrington, 1990, pp 17-19) between
recognition of emotion and of identity in faces: while SM-046
is impaired at recognizing some emotional facial expressions,
but not facial identity, two of our other subjects, LDV-692 and
FR-1465, were impaired at recognizing facial identity (cf. Tranel
et al., 1988, patient #2), but not emotion in facial expressions.
These results are consistent with previous findings that recog-
nition of facial identity can be impaired while sparing recogni-
tion of the emotion signalled by a face (Tranel et al., 1988;
Damasio et al., 1990; Humphreys et al., 1993).
5888 Adobhs et al. * Fear and the Human Amvodala
DISGUSTED
S&l-046: January 9-l
Table 2. Recognition and naming of familiar faces
Subject
Recognition Naming
Bilateral amygdala damage
SM-046 normal normal
Left amygdala damage
LV-580 normal impaired
JS-1077 normal normal
UB-1251 normal impaired
Right amygdala damage
LDV-692 impaired impaired
FR-1465 impaired impaired
AK- 1603 normal normal
Brain-damaged controls
CE-1023 normal normal
WW-1257 normal normal
RM-1362 normal borderline
RJ-1374 normal normal
JR-1584 normal normal
RJ- 1605 normal normal
DE- 1607 normal normal
RS-1658 normal normal
BL-1722 normal normal
RM-1740 NT NT
MR-1790 normal normal
CF-1899 normal normal
Drawing ,facial expressions
We now describe the results of more qualitative experiments,
which are intended to supplement the above data. Might SM-
046’s impairment be limited to recognition, or might she have a
more general impairment in memory for stimuli with emotional
meaning? This question is especially important, as recent evi-
dence suggests that there are double dissociations between im-
pairments of visual perception and impairments of visual im-
agery (Behrmann et al., 1992; Guariglia et al., 1993).
To assess the ability of SM-046 to construct an image of an
afraid face, we asked her and eight other subjects to draw pic-
tures of facial expressions from memory. This task was admin-
istered immediately after subjects had been exposed to all the
pictures of facial expressions used in our other experiments.
They thus had significant exposure to facial expressions just pri-
or to the drawing session.
Subjects vary widely in their ability to draw facial expressions.
However, all subjects that were able to draw disgusted or angry
faces, or able to draw faces in the detail that SM-046 did, were
able to draw clear facial expressions of fear. All patients with
unilateral amygdala damage that we tested on this task (n = 4)
were able to draw facial expressions of fear, although many sub-
t
The Journal of Neuroscience, September 1995, 15(9) 5887
jects found fear, anger, and disgust more difficult expressions to
draw.
SM-046’s drawings are reproduced in Figure 6. SM-046 was
able to produce without difficulty skillful renditions of all facial
expressions except fear. She often provided additional context,
and enjoyed drawing. However, she was entirely unable to draw
expressions of fear. She explained that she did not know what
an afraid face would look like, and that she was unable to draw
any depiction of it. The experimenter had to prompt repeatedly
to elicit any kind of drawing of fear at all; she produced her
drawing only after several failed attempts. She knew, however,
that people who are afraid will tend to cower, and are typically
portrayed with their hair standing on end, two features that did
find their way into her drawing (Fig. 6). But she could not bring
to mind any image of a facial expression to go with fear, and,
accordingly, this was the only one of her six drawings that did
not include a frontal view of the face.
The
concept of fear
The above results might suggest that SM-046 simply lacks the
concept of fear, and thus cannot evoke this concept when she
encounters a facial expression that denotes it. However, our in-
terviews with her revealed that she does know (a) what sorts of
situations would evoke fear in people (being threatened, being
alone in a dark alley at night, and so on), and (b) how frightened
people tend to behave (their heart pounds, they may scream, and
they try to run away). During the course of extensive interviews,
SM-046 used the words, “fear,” “afraid,” “frightened,” appro-
priately.
We further tested SM-046’s ability to recognize the meaning
of words denoting emotions by her ability to sort a set of 28
adjective labels into various numbers of piles on the basis of the
similarity of the emotion denoted by an adjective. She performed
normally on this task, and grouped together the words, “afraid,”
“scared,” “worried,” “terrified,” and “alarmed,” indicating
that she found them to be closely related.
We have no way of testing for all components of the concept
of fear in SM-046. It does appear that she can access many im-
portant components of the concept, provided that they admit of
verbal description. We think it likely, however, that SM-046 does
not experience fear in a normal way, as we have found no con-
vincing evidence that she feels frightened given the appropriate
stimulus, and have preliminary evidence that she fails to exhibit
the full range of psychophysiological responses to fear (A. Be-
chara, R. Adolphs, D. Tranel, personal observation). This issue
requires additional studies, some of which are now underway.
Discussion
Based on a series of quantitative tasks, we have shown that
bilateral amygdala damage (1) impairs judgments of the intensity
of faces depicting fear, (2) impairs judgments of faces depicting
fear with respect to several emotional labels, (3) impairs the
ability to detect fear in faces predominantly depicting other ex-
category
is denoted on each plot by the colored bar at the very top of the graph. The y-axis represents the mean rating (from O-5) given to a facial
expression. Each of the differently colored lines corresponds to the ratings endorsed on a particular emotional label. For ease of visualization, all
plots were smoothed as follows. We triplicated the summed raw data for each plot, producing three plots strung together, in order to embed the
center plot in continuous data, and applied a Lowess smoothing filter with a 5% window. The center plot of the three plots was used, and numbers
were divided by the number of datasets to produce the final plot shown on the figure. Careful comparisons with the raw data confirmed that the
smoothing procedure conservatively captured the relevant trends in the data without producing artifacts. Top: data from brain-damaged (n = 12)
and normal controls (n = 7). Middle:
data
from subjects with left (n = 3) and right (n = 3) unilateral amygdala damage. Bottom: data from subject
with bilateral amygdala damage. Each of the two plots for SM-046 correspond to the mean of two experiments.
5888 Adolphs et al. * Fear and the Human Amygdala
HAPPY
SUiiPRISED
ANG-i3Y
SAD
tJ/ /.’
DISGUSTED
AFRAID
Figure 6. SM-046’s drawings of facial expressions from memory. SM.046 tended to provide context to her drawings, in all cases appropriate.
She insisted that she did not know how to draw an afraid face, because she did not know what such an expression would look like. Her drawing
was produced only after repeated prompting, and she was not satisfied with it.
pressions (such as surprise), and (4) leaves intact the ability to
recognize the identity of faces. In addition, based on supple-
mentary, qualitative tasks, we suggest that bilateral amygdala
damage compromises the ability to reconstruct visual images of
faces that depict fear, while leaving intact many aspects of the
concept of fear that can be verbally described. None of the sub-
jects with
unilateral
amygdala damage were impaired at rec-
ognizing any facial expressions.
Taken as a whole, these results suggest that bilateral amygdala
damage impairs the processing of facial expressions of fear both
in recognition and in recall. However, the full scope of the im-
pairments that follow bilateral damage to the human amygdala
will be broader than what we have reported here. For example,
it is likely that behaviors that depend on recognition of facial
expressions, such as components of social behavior, are also
compromised in some way.
One important issue is the age of subjects at the onset of their
amygdala damage: all patients with unilateral amygdala damage
sustained damage to this structure in late adolescence or in adult-
hood. They would thus have had many years with bilaterally
The Journal of Neuroscience, September 1995, 15(9) 5889
intact amygdalae prior to sustaining their lesion. It is somewhat
less clear at what point in time SM-046 acquired her amygdala
lesions. We cannot reconstruct the time course of SM-046’s le-
sions with any certainty. However, the literature on Urbach-Wie-
the disease (Hofer, 1973) together with reports of SM-046’s
childhood, suggest that the neurological symptoms that can re-
sult from this disease are progressively acquired throughout late
childhood and adolescence, as calcified material continues to be
deposited in the tissue and blood vessels of mesial temporal
structures. We thus consider it likely that all our patients with
amygdala lesions acquired such lesions several years into their
life, and had occasion to develop an initially normal concept of
fear prior to the onset of their lesion.
Studies of the amygdala in humans
Studies of the amygdala in humans have come largely from two
sources: surgical lesions and electrical stimulation. Although
surgical lesions of the human amygdala suffer from the draw-
back that they may be incomplete, and that the subjects may not
be normal prior to the surgery (Narabayashi, 1972; Jacobson,
1986; Lee et al., 1988a,b), a review of these cases (Aggleton,
1992) strongly supports the role of the amygdala in processing
fear and aggression and in social behavior. Electrical stimulation
studies of the amygdala in epileptic patients also suffer from
comparable drawbacks, but are again suggestive of functions
related to fear, aggression, and social behavior (Chapman et al.,
19.54; Heath et al., 1955; Halgren et al., 1978; Gloor et al.,
1982).
Our subject SM-046 provides a rare opportunity to examine
selective and bilateral lesions of the amygdala in humans. A
recent report of two other patients with Urbach-Wiethe disease
suggested that they were impaired in processing information that
had emotional significance (Markowitsch et al., 1994). Func-
tional imaging studies with PET in these patients (Markowitsch
et al., 1994), as well as in SM-046, confirmed that the amygdala
is severely hypometabolic, as would be expected of atrophic
tissue.
The amygdala is an essential link in the response to stimuli
with emotional value
In primates, the amygdala projects back to all visual cortices,
including the striate cortex (Amaral and Price, 1984; Iwai and
Yukie, 1987; Amaral et al., 1992). It is thus possible that high-
level feedback from the amygdala directly participates in the
activity of those regions, during both perception (Rolls, 1992)
and visual imagery (Damasio et al., 1993; Kosslyn et al., 1993).
We envision the function of the human amygdala as a high-order
“convergence” zone for the social, homeostatic, and survival-
related meaning of a class of complex stimuli, such as facial
expressions of some emotions (Damasio, 1989; Damasio and
Damasio, 1993; Damasio, 1994). In this view, to recognize
someone as afraid, the amygdala must make the sight of the
fearful face activate myriad cortical and subcortical regions,
whose ensemble, temporally coordinated activity constitutes the
concept of fear. Important components of such a distributed,
macroscopic neural representation would result from the amyg-
dala’s engagement of limbic and somatic activity during danger
or threat, and from the engagement of sensory cortices that rep-
resent entities and scenarios related to fear. The amygdala may
thus orchestrate patterns of neural activation in disparate sectors
of the brain that would encode both the intrinsic, physical fea-
tures of stimuli (e.g., shape, position in space, and so on), and
the value that certain stimuli have to the organism (e.g., their
emotional significance).
We propose that SM-046 can perceive the visual features of
a face normally; i.e., she constructs normal percepts of the phys-
ical features of a face. She can also connect such percepts with
the class of knowledge necessary to identify a familiar person:
the sight of a familiar face activates internal responses that sub-
sume knowledge pertinent and specific to that unique face. How-
ever, she lacks the ability to link the perception of certain con-
stellations of facial features to the class of knowledge related to
emotions, specifically to the knowledge of fear: the perception
of an expression of fear fails to activate responses that subsume
the concept of fear (these responses would include autonomic
changes, changes in posture, and facial expression of SM-046,
mental images describing scenarios related to fear, verbal reports
pertaining to the fearful face, etc.). The broken link in SM-046
affects a bidirectional process: she cannot retrieve knowledge
about fearful faces when confronted with the face, and she also
cannot generate constellations of the facial features of fear in
imagery when given the verbal label.
When SM-046 fails to recognize fear in facial expressions,
what does she think those expressions are? SM-046 sometimes
described faces denoting fear as looking surprised or angry, be-
havior exhibited also by normal subjects to some degree. Rarely,
she would even describe a face as looking afraid, but always
insisted that it was not very afraid. In several experiments, she
appeared to have no idea what emotion was being signalled by
an afraid face, and gave the face the lowest possible ratings on
all emotion labels. Our interpretation is that SM-046 did not
think afraid faces looked as afraid as the controls thought they
did. However, since she was able to recognize other emotional
expressions, she also did not think that afraid faces exhibited an
emotion that was not at all present (for instance, she never
thought afraid faces looked happy).
Interestingly, SM-046 did give the highest mean ratings on
the adjective “afraid” to faces that depicted fear, just as controls
did (Fig. 5), but she was quite abnormal in the low magnitude
of fear she ascribed to those faces. These results are consistent
with our proposal: SM-046 can decide to some extent whether
a person may be expressing fear or not, possibly by reasoning
from prototypical features she knows to accompany fear (e.g.,
wide eyes), and possibly reasoning from exclusion of other emo-
tions that she can recognize normally. However, she lacks the
ability to link the perception of the expression with knowledge
that would allow her to judge the intensity of the emotion. Our
proposal, then, is that the amygdala is essential to retrieve com-
prehensive knowledge related to the concept of fear.
Her impaired ability to produce drawings of expressions of
fear lend further support to this interpretation. Although SM-046
was able to portray some of the stereotypical features normally
associated with the expression of fear (cowering posture, hair
standing on end), she was unable to construct an image of a
facial expression. This task requires the generation of an image
on the basis of its link to a verbal label denoting a concept. We
propose that the ability to link representations of facial expres-
sions of fear to other components of the concept of fear is bi-
directionally defective in SM-046.
The amygdala and social behavior
The behavioral impairments of amygdalectomized monkeys are
most severe in the wild, resulting in the death of the animal if
left unattended in its troop (Kling and Brothers, 1992). Neurons
5890 Adolphs et al. * Fear and the Human Amygdala
within the amygdala are active during social interactions (Kling
et al., 1979) and social communication (Jurgens, 1982). The pro-
found social dysfunction of amygdalectomized animals (Rosvold
et al., 1954) can be understood in the context of the findings
described above. The integration of complex sensory informa-
tion, such as recognition of faces, with the motivational valence
of the stimulus, is especially important in order to recognize
social intentions, social status, and to guide behavior on the basis
of social cues (threats, warnings, submissive gestures; Kling and
Brothers, 1992).
It is noteworthy that the behavior of SM-046, even in real
life, is not nearly as impaired as that of monkeys with experi-
mental amygdala lesions. She has a history of inadequate social
decision making, somewhat inappropriate behavior, failure to
maintain employment or marital relations, and she depends on
welfare. Nonetheless, SM-046 is not a social outcast. One reason
for the discrepancy between social behavior in amygdalectomi-
zed monkeys and in SM-046 might be that the monkeys have
more complete lesions, or lesions in structures other than the
amygdala. Another reason for the difference may be that the
amygdala is less important to social behavior in the case of
humans than in the case of monkeys. Humans can engage de-
clarative knowledge and reasoning in a purely verbal mode,
which largely parallels the nonverbal processes directly guided
by circuits that include the amygdala. Nonetheless, we believe
complex social and aesthetic judgments in humans do require
all the neural machinery used to process emotions, and certainly
do include the amygdala (Damasio, 1994). It is only that the
deficits produced by dysfunction in these basic systems may be
partially masked by the intact propositional competence of hu-
mans, thus making the behavioral sequalae of bilateral human
amygdala damage somewhat more subtle.
References
Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio A (1994) Impaired rec-
ognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage
to the human amygdala. Nature 372:669-672.
Aggleton JP (1992) The functional effects of amygdala lesions in hu-
mans: a comparison with findings from monkeys. In: The amygdala:
neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction
(Aggleton JP, ed), pp 485-504. New York: Wiley-Liss.
Allison T, McCarthy G, Nobre A, Puce A, Belger A (1994) Human
extrastriate visual cortex and the perception of faces, words, numbers,
and colors. Cereb Cortex 4:544-554.
Amaral DG, Price JL (1984) Amygdalo-cortical connections in the
monkey (Macaca fascicularis). J Comp Neurol 230:465496.
Amaral DG, Price JL, Pitkanen A, Carmichael ST (1992) Anatomical
organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In: The amygdala:
neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction
(Aggleton JP, ed), pp l-66. New York: Wiley-Liss.
Behrmann M, Winocur G, Moscovitch M (1992) Dissociation between
mental imagery and object recognition in a brain-damaged patient.
Nature 359:636-637.
Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ (1972) Innate and conditioned reactions
to threat in rats with amygdaloid lesions. J Comp Physiol Psycho1
81:281-290.
Bowers D, Heilman KM (1984) Dissociation between the processing
of affective and nonaffective faces: a case study. J Clin Neuropsychol
6:367-379.
Chapman WP, Schroeder HR, Geyer G, Brazier MAB, Fager C, Poppen
JL, Solomon HC, Yakovlev PI (1954) Physiological evidence con-
cerning importance of the amygdaloid nuclear region in the integra-
tion of circulatory function and emotion in man. Science 120:949-
950.
Damasio AR (1989) Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a sys-
tems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition.
Cognition 33:25-62.
Damasio AR (1994) Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human
brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.
Damasio AR, Damasio H (1993) Cortical systems for retrieval of con-
crete knowledge: the convergence zone framework. In: Large-scale
neuronal theories of the brain (Koch C, ed). London: MIT Press.
Damasio AR, Tranel D, Damasio H (1990) Face agnosia and the neural
substrates of memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 13:89-109.
Damasio H, Damasio AR (1989) Lesion analysis in neuropsychology.
New York: Oxford UP
Damasio H, Frank R (1992) Three-dimensional in viva mapping of
brain lesions in humans. Arch Neurol 49:137-143.
Damasio H, Grabowski TJ, Damasio A, Tranel D, Boles-Ponto L, Wat-
kins GL, Hichwa RD (1993) Visual recall with eyes closed and
covered activates early visual cortices. Sot Neurosci Abstr 19:1603.
Darwin C (1872/1965) The expression of the emotions in man and
animals. Chicago: University of Chicago.
Davidson RJ (1992) Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of
emotion. Brain Cognit 6:245-268.
Davis M (1992a) The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. In: The
amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental
dysfunction (Aggleton JP, ed), pp 255-306. New York: Wiley-Liss.
Davis M (1992b) The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu
Rev Neurosci 15:353-375.
Ekman P (1976) Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists.
Gloor P, Olivier A, Quesney LF, Andermann F, Horowitz S (1982) The
role of the limbic system in experiential phenomena of temporal lobe
epilepsy. Ann Neurol 12: 129-144.
Guariglia C, Padovani A, Pantano P, Pizzamiglio L (1993) Unilateral
neglect restricted to visual imagery. Nature 364:235-237.
Halgren E, Walter RD, Cherlow DG, Crandall PH (1978) Mental phe-
nomena evoked by electrical stimulation of the human hippocampal
formation and amygdala. Brain 101:83-l 17.
Hasselmo ME, Rolls ET, Baylis GC (1989) The role of expression and
identity in the face-selective responses of neurons in the temporal
visual cortex of the monkey. Behav Brain Res 32:203-218.
Heath RG, Russell RM, Mickle WA (1955) Stimulation of the amyg-
daloid nucleus in a schizophrenic patient. Am J Psychiatry 111:862-
863.
Heit G, Smith ME, Halgren E (1988) Neural encoding of individual
words and faces by the human hippocampus and amygdala. Nature
3331773-775.
Hensman R, Guimaraes FS, Wang M, Deakin JFW (1991) Effects of
ritanserin on aversive classical conditioning in humans. Psychophar-
macology (Berlin) 104:220-224.)
Herzog AG, Van Hoesen GW (1976) Temporal neocortical afferent
connections to the amygdala in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 115:
57-69.
Hitchcock JM, Davis M (1986) Lesions of the amygdala, but not of
the cerebellum or red nucleus, block conditioned fear as measured
with the potentiated startle paradigm. Behav Neurosci 1OO:l l-22.
Hofer P-A (1973) Urbach-Wiethe disease: a review. Acta Derm Ve-
nerol 53:5-52.
Humphreys GW, Donnelly N, Riddoch MJ (1993) Expression is com-
puted separately from facial identity, and it is computed separately
for moving and static faces: neuropsychological evidence. Neurop-
sychologia 31:173-181.
Iwai E, Yukie M (1987) Amygdalofugal and amygdalopetal connec-
tions with modality-specific visual cortical areas in Macaques. J
Comp Neurol 261:362-387.
Jacobson R (1986) Disorders of facial recognition, social behavior and
affect after combined bilateral amygdalotomy and subcaudate trac-
totomy-a clinical and experimental study. Psycho1 Med 16:439-450.
Jurgens U (1982) Amygdalar vocalization pathways in the squirrel
monkey. Brain Res 241:189-l 96.
Kapp BS, Gallagher M, Applegate CD, Frysinger RC (1982) The
amygdala central nucleus: contributions to conditioned cardiovascular
responding during aversive Pavlovian conditioning in the rabbit. In:
Conditioning: representation of involved neural functions (Woody
CD, ed), pp 581-599. New York: Plenum.
Kling A, Steklis HD, Deutsch S (1979) Radiotelemetered activity from
the amygdala during social interactions in the monkey. Exp Neurol
66:88-96.
Kling AS, Brothers LA (1992) The amygdala and social behavior. In:
The amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and
The Journal of Neuroscience, September 1995, 75(9) 5891
mental dysfunction (Aggleton JP, ed), pp 353-378. New York: Wiley-
Liss.
Kosslyn SM, Alpert NM, Thompson WL, Maljkovic V, Weise SB, Cha-
bris CE Hamilton SE. Rauch SL. Buonanno FS (1993) Visual mental
imagery activates topographically organized visual cortex: PET in-
vestigations. J Cogn Neurosci 5:263-287.
LeDoux JE, Iwata J, Cicchetti P Reis DJ (1988) Different projections
of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral
correlates of conditioned fear. J Neurosci 8:25 17-2529.
LeDoux JE, Cicchetti P, Xagoraris A, Romanski LM (1990) The lateral
amygdaloid nucleus: sensory interface of the amygdala in fear con-
ditioning. J Neurosci 10:1062-1069.
Lee GP, Arena JG, Meador KJ, Smith JR, Loring DW, Flanigin HF
(1988a) Changes in autonomic responsiveness following bilateral
amygdalotomy in humans. Neuropsychiatr Neuropsychol Behav Neu-
rol 1:119-129.
Lee GP, Meador KJ, Smith JR, Loring DW, Flanigin HF (1988b) Pre-
served crossmodal association following bilateral amygdalotomy in
man. Int J Neurosci 40:47-55.
Leonard CM, Rolls ET, Wilson FAW, Baylis GC (1985) Neurons in
the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. Behav
Brain Res 15: 1599176.
Markowitsch HJ, Calabrese P, Wuerker M, Durwen HE Kessler J, Ba-
binsky R, Brechtelsbauer D, Heuser L, Gehlen W (1994) The amyg-
dala’s contribution to memory-a study on two patients with Urbach-
Wiethe disease. Neuroreport 5: 1349-1352.
McCarthy RA, Warrington EK (1990) Cognitive neuropsychology.
New York: Academic.
Nahm FKD, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1993) Cross-modal
associations and the human amygdala. Neuropsychologia 3 1:727-
744.
Narabayashi H (1972) Stereotaxic amygdalotomy. In: The neurobiol-
ogy of the amygdala (Eleftheriou BE, ed), pp 459483. New York:
Plenum.
Niehoff DL, Kuhar MJ (1983) Benzodiazepine receptors: localization
in rat amygdala. J Neurosci 3:2091-2097.
Nishijo H, Ono T, Nishino H (1988) Single neuron responses in amyg-
dala of alert monkey during complex sensory stimulation with affec-
tive significance. J Neurosci 8:3570-3583.
Perrett DI, Rolls ET, Caan W (1982) Visual neurons responsive to faces
in the monkey temporal cortex. Exp Brain Res 47:329-342.
Rapcsak SZ, Kaszniak AW, Rubens AB (1989) Anomia for facial ex-
pressions: evidence for a category specific visual-verbal disconnec-
tion syndrome. Neuropsychologia 27: 1031-1041.
Rapcsak SZ, Comer JE Rubens AB (1993) Anomia for facial expres-
sions: neuropsychological mechanisms and anatomical correlates.
Brain Lang 45:233-252.
Rolls ET (1992) Neurophysiology and functions of the primate amyg-
dala. In: The amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory,
and mental dysfunction (Aggleton JP, ed), pp 143-167. New York:
Wiley-Liss.
Rosvold HE, Mirksy AE Pribram K (1954) Influence of amygdalec-
tomy on social behavior in monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psycho1 47:
173-178.
Russell JA (1991) In defense of a prototype approach to emotion con-
cepts. J Pers Sot Psycho1 60:37+7.
Russell JA, Bullock M (1986) Fuzzy concepts and the perception of
emotion in facial expressions. 4:309-341.
Russell JA, Fehr B (1987) Relativity in the perception of emotion in
facial expressions. J Exp Psycho1 116:223-237.
Saudou E Amara DA, Dierich A, LeMeur M, Ramboz S, Segu L, Buhot
M-C, Hen R (1994) Enhanced aggressive behavior in mice lacking
5-HTlB receptor. Science 265:1875-1878.
Stein NL, Leventhal B, Trabasso T (1990) Psychological and biological
approaches to emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
Tranel D (1995) The Iowa-Benton school of neuropsychological as-
sessment. In: Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric dis-
orders (Grant I. Adams KM. eds). New York: Oxford UP
Tranel D, ‘Hyman’BT (1990) Neurdpsychological correlates of bilateral
amygdala damage. Arch Neurol 47:349-355.
Tranel D, Damasio AR, Damasio H (1988) Intact recognition of facial
expression, gender, and age in patients with impaired recognition of
face identity. Neurology 38:690-696.
Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (in press) Double dissociation be-
tween overt and covert face recognition. J Cogn Neurosci, in press.
Wachsmuth E, Oram MW, Perrett DI (1994) Recognition of objects
and their component parts: responses of single units in the temporal
cortex of the macaque. Cereb Cortex 4:509-522.
Weiskrantz L (1956) Behavioral changes associated with ablation of
the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psycho1 49:
381-391.
Young AW, Aggleton JP, Hellawell DJ, Johnson M, Broks P Hanley JR
(1995) Face processing impairments after amygdalotomy. Brain 118:
15-24.
... The amygdala is a subcortical region well-established for its central role in fear acquisition and extinction. The amygdala belongs to a wider fear network which includes the hippocampus, insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) [19][20][21][22]. Nuclei within the amygdala support specific aspects of fear. ...
... In this randomized, sham-controlled study, active amygdala tFUS changed BOLD activation in several fear network regions, the amygdala, hippocampus, and dACC during a fear task compared to sham tFUS (Fig. 3). Our fear network activation results are mostly consistent with a vast literature in fear learning and extinction [19][20][21][22] and previous noninvasive studies targeting cortical structures in the fear network [33][34][35][36][37], which suggest that modulating one fear network region can lead to changes in other fear network regions as well as anxiety. However, at baseline (pre tFUS), the left amygdala did not show greater activation during the high risk trials compared to low risk trials in our task. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Current noninvasive brain stimulation methods are incapable of directly modulating subcortical brain regions critically involved in psychiatric disorders. Transcranial Focused Ultrasound (tFUS) is a newer form of noninvasive stimulation that could modulate the amygdala, a subcortical region implicated in fear. Objective: We investigated the effects of active and sham tFUS of the amygdala on fear circuit activation, skin conductance responses (SCR), and self-reported anxiety during a fear-inducing task. We also investigated amygdala tFUS’ effects on amygdala-fear circuit resting-state functional connectivity. Methods: Thirty healthy individuals were randomized in this double-blinded study to active or sham tFUS of the left amygdala. We collected fMRI scans, SCR, and self-reported anxiety during a fear-inducing task (participants viewed red or green circles which indicated the risk of receiving an aversive stimulus), as well as resting-state scans, before and after tFUS. Results: Compared to sham tFUS, active tFUS was associated with decreased (pre to post tFUS) blood-oxygen-level-dependent fMRI activation in the amygdala (F(1,25)=4.86, p=0.04, η2=0.16) during the fear task, and lower hippocampal (F(1,27)=4.41, p=0.05, η2=0.14), and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (F(1,27)=6.26, p=0.02; η2=0.19) activation during the post tFUS fear task. The decrease in amygdala activation was correlated with decreased subjective anxiety (r=0.62, p=0.03). There was no group effect in SCR changes from pre to post tFUS (F(1,23)=0.85, p=0.37). The active tFUS group also showed decreased amygdala-insula (F(1,28)=4.98, p=0.03) and amygdala-hippocampal (F(1,28)=7.14, p=0.01) rsFC, and increased amygdala-ventromedial prefrontal cortex (F(1,28)=3.52, p=0.05) resting-state functional connectivity. Conclusions: tFUS can change functional connectivity and brain region activation associated with decreased anxiety. Future studies should investigate tFUS’ therapeutic potential for individuals with clinical levels of anxiety.
... Patients with amygdala damage have been con rmed to have di culty recognizing fearful emotions mainly because they nd it di cult to discern clues related to fear conveyed through the eyes [23][24][25]. This evidence suggests that there is a close relationship between the eyes and the fearful brain and that the eyes are important clues for recognizing fear. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: Fearful facial recognition deficits are common predictors of Parkinson's disease, schizophrenia, autism, etc., and are closely related to disease severity. Training in fearful facial recognition has been shown to promote the recovery of social function in patients. The eye region plays a crucial role in recognizing fear, and Asians are more likely to use the eye region to express and recognize facial emotions. The aim of this study was to 1) define eye characteristic indicators that can be used to recognize fearful faces, 2) construct a fear emotion recognition model based on eye features using machine learning, and evaluate the accuracy and performance of these indicators for fearful face recognition to clarify the application value of eye indicators in fearful face recognition. Methods: In this research, images of fearful and nonfearful expressions from databases such as the Radboud Faces Database (RF), Facial Expression Recognition 2013 dataset (FER2013), Japanese Female Facial Expressions (JAFFE), and Extended Cohn-Kanade (CK+) were utilized, and 2573 emotional facial images were included (1264 fearful and 1309 nonfearful). OpenFace is employed to extract eye characteristics from facial images. Six algorithms, including the random forest, support vector machine, K-nearest neighbor, logistic regression, naive Bayes and neural network algorithms, are used for modelling. The ROC curve (AUC), precision, recall, F1 score, and specificity were used to evaluate the performance of the six machine learning models in fear emotion recognition. The optimal fear emotion recognition model based on eye characteristics is selected based on these evaluations. Results: Our findings indicate that eye opening degree, inner eye distance, pupil size, eyelid distance, and iris size are important indicators of fearful emotions. The modelling method utilizing eye characteristics based on the random forest algorithm achieved the highest accuracy in fear emotion recognition, at 91.58%, with superior model performance. Conclusions: Eye characteristics serve as important external clues for recognizing fearful emotions. Modelling eye characteristics based on the random forest algorithm can be effectively applied in the field of fear emotion recognition. Facial expression recognition training based on eye opening degree, inner eye distance, pupil size, eyelid distance, and iris size may aid in the recovery of social function in patients with Parkinson's disease, schizophrenia, autism, etc.
... This study confirms a relation between the attachment system and possible dysregulation of these neural systems due to a premature loss experience. OT may exert its effects by directly inhibiting the amygdala (Davis, 1992;Adolphs et al., 1995). Further, recent contributions show that long-term anxiety and depressive states associated with development are associated with greater amygdala volume (van Elst et al., 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
Developing a sense of internal safety and security depends mainly on others: numerous neuromodulators play a significant role in the homeostatic process, regulating the importance of proximity to a caregiver and experiencing feelings that enable us to regulate our interdependence with our conspecifics since birth. This array of neurofunctional structures have been called the SEPARATION DISTRESS system (now more commonly known as the PANIC/ GRIEF system). This emotional system is mainly involved in the production of depressive symptoms. The disruption of this essential emotional balance leads to the onset of feelings of panic followed by depression. We will focus on the neuropeptides that play a crucial role in social approach behavior in mammals, which enhance prosocial behavior and facilitate the consolidation of social bonds. We propose that most prosocial behaviors are regulated through the specific neuromodulators acting on salient intersubjective stimuli, reflecting an increased sense of inner confidence (safety) in social relationships. This review considers the neurofunctional link between the feelings that may ultimately be at the base of a sense of inner safety and the central neuromodulatory systems. This link may shed light on the clinical implications for the development of early mother-infant bonding and the depressive clinical consequences when this bond is disrupted, such as in post-partum depression, depressive feelings connected to, addiction, neurofunctional disorders, and psychological trauma.
... The amygdala theory of autism proposes that difficulties in recognizing negative emotions may be linked to dysfunction in the amygdala [33]. The amygdala plays a crucial role in processing social stimuli and it is highly sensitive to detecting and responding to threat-related information, including facial expressions of fear [36,37]. Individuals with ASC exhibit deficits in amygdala activity when performing tasks related to processing facial expressions [38][39][40]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective Most previous studies have examined emotion recognition in autism spectrum condition (ASC) without intellectual disability (ID). However, ASC and ID co-occur to a high degree. The main aims of the study were to examine emotion recognition in individuals with ASC and co-occurring intellectual disability (ASC-ID) as compared to individuals with ID alone, and to investigate the relationship between emotion recognition and social functioning. Methods The sample consisted of 30 adult participants with ASC-ID and a comparison group of 29 participants with ID. Emotion recognition was assessed by the facial emotions test, while. social functioning was assessed by the social responsiveness scale–second edition (SRS-2). Results The accuracy of emotion recognition was significantly lower in individuals with ASC-ID compared to the control group with ID, especially when it came to identifying angry and fearful emotions. Participants with ASC-ID exhibited more pronounced difficulties in social functioning compared to those with ID, and there was a significant negative correlation between emotion recognition and social functioning. However, emotion recognition accounted for only 8% of the variability observed in social functioning. Conclusion Our data indicate severe difficulties in the social-perceptual domain and in everyday social functioning in individuals with ASC-ID.
... E.g., local stimulation of the amygdala modulates the activity of the ACC and insular ROIs of the SN [69], reinforcing the potential of amygdala modulation of this network through its connections. Brain imaging and lesion studies in humans also highlighted the relevance of the amygdala and its connections in processing emotionally salient stimuli [70][71][72][73][74]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background fMRI neurofeedback targeting the amygdala is a promising therapeutical tool in psychiatry. It induces resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) changes between the amygdala and regions of the salience and default mode networks (SN and DMN, respectively). We hypothesize these rsFC changes happen on the amygdala’s underlying anatomical circuits. Methods We used the coordinates from regions of interest (ROIs) from studies showing pre-to-post-neurofeedback changes in rsFC with the left amygdala. Using a cross-species brain parcellation, we identified the homologous locations in non-human primates. We injected bidirectional tracers in the amygdala of adult macaques and used bright- and dark-field microscopy to identify cells and axon terminals in each ROI. We also performed additional injections in specific ROIs to validate the results following amygdala injections and delineate potential disynaptic pathways. Finally, we used high-resolution diffusion MRI data from four post-mortem macaque brains and one in vivo human brain to translate our findings to the neuroimaging domain. Results The amygdala had significant monosynaptic connections with all the SN and DMN ipsilateral ROIs. Amygdala connections with the DMN contralateral ROIs are disynaptic through the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus. Diffusion MRI in both species benefitted from using the ground-truth tracer data to validate its findings, as we identified false-negative ipsilateral and false-positive contralateral connectivity results. Conclusions Amygdala neurofeedback modulates the SN and DMN through monosynaptic connections and disynaptic pathways - including hippocampal structures involved in the neurofeedback task. Neurofeedback may be a tool for rapid modulation and reinforcement of these anatomical connections, leading to clinical improvement.
... Perhaps the most notable finding was the lack of amygdala sensitivity not only to threats, but also to rewards, that is, the amygdala did not appear to respond to salient events or to imminence-dependent salient features ( Figure 5). This finding is contradictory to the classical understanding of the important role the amygdala plays in salience processing, particularly Pavlovian fear conditioning (Adolphs et al., 1995;Davis, 1992;Davis & Whalen, 2001;LeDoux, 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
Studies of affective neuroscience have typically employed highly controlled, static experimental paradigms to investigate the neural underpinnings of threat and reward processing in the brain. Yet our knowledge of affective processing in more naturalistic settings remains limited. Specifically, affective studies generally examine threat and reward features separately and under brief time periods, despite the fact that in nature organisms are often exposed to the simultaneous presence of threat and reward features for extended periods. To study the neural mechanisms of threat and reward processing under distinct temporal profiles, we created a modified version of the PACMAN game that included these environmental features. We also conducted two automated meta‐analyses to compare the findings from our semi‐naturalistic paradigm to those from more constrained experiments. Overall, our results revealed a distributed system of regions sensitive to threat imminence and a less distributed system related to reward imminence, both of which exhibited overlap yet neither of which involved the amygdala. Additionally, these systems broadly overlapped with corresponding meta‐analyses, with the notable absence of the amygdala in our findings. Together, these findings suggest a shared system for salience processing that reveals a heightened sensitivity toward environmental threats compared to rewards when both are simultaneously present in an environment. The broad correspondence of our findings to meta‐analyses, consisting of more tightly controlled paradigms, illustrates how semi‐naturalistic studies can corroborate previous findings in the literature while also potentially uncovering novel mechanisms resulting from the nuances and contexts that manifest in such dynamic environments.
... Likewise, in a PET study, fear processing in healthy controls induced a decreased activation in the posterior insula (Damasio et al., 2000). While broad literature and early studies reported the amygdala to be crucial in the processing and recognition of fearful faces (Adolphs et al., 1995;McFadyen et al., 2019;Morris et al., 1996), we did not identify such a lesion-symptom association. This lack of an association might be because our lesion coverage in the sample was too little to assess the potential effects of an amygdala lesion on facial recognition abilities (van den Berg et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Emotion processing deficits are known to accompany depressive symptoms and are often seen in stroke patients. Little is known about the influence of post-stroke depressive (PSD) symptoms and specific brain lesions on altered emotion processing abilities and how these phenomena develop over time. This potential relationship may impact post-stroke rehabilitation of neurological and psychosocial function. To address this scientific gap, we investigated the relationship between PSD symptoms and emotion processing abilities in a longitudinal study design from the first days post-stroke into the early chronic phase. Methods Twenty-six ischemic stroke patients performed an emotion processing task on videos with emotional faces ('happy,' 'sad,' 'anger,' 'fear,' and 'neutral') at different intensity levels (20%, 40%, 60%, 80%, 100%). Recognition accuracies and response times were measured, as well as scores of depressive symptoms (Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale). Twenty-eight healthy participants matched in age and sex were included as a control group. Whole-brain support-vector regression lesion-symptom mapping (SVR-LSM) analyses were performed to investigate whether specific lesion locations were associated with the recognition accuracy of specific emotion categories. Results Stroke patients performed worse in overall recognition accuracy compared to controls, specifically in the recognition of happy, sad, and fearful faces. Notably, more depressed stroke patients showed an increased processing towards specific negative emotions, as they responded significantly faster to angry faces and recognized sad faces of low intensities significantly more accurately. These effects obtained for the first days after stroke partly persisted to follow-up assessment several months later. SVR-LSM analyses revealed that inferior and middle frontal regions (IFG/MFG) and insula and putamen were associated with emotion-recognition deficits in stroke. Specifically, recognizing happy facial expressions was influenced by lesions affecting the anterior insula, putamen, IFG, MFG, orbitofrontal cortex, and rolandic operculum. Lesions in the posterior insula, rolandic operculum, and MFG were also related to reduced recognition accuracy of fearful facial expressions, whereas recognition deficits of sad faces were associated with frontal pole, IFG, and MFG damage. Conclusion PSD symptoms facilitate processing negative emotional stimuli, specifically angry and sad facial expressions. The recognition accuracy of different emotional categories was linked to brain lesions in emotion-related processing circuits, including insula, basal ganglia, IFG, and MFG. In summary, our study provides support for psychosocial and neural factors underlying emotional processing after stroke, contributing to the pathophysiology of PSD.
... The traditional account of the human amygdala as center of fear was born from the holistic view that damage to the amygdala results in reductions of fear responsivity, experience and recognition (Adolphs et al., 1994(Adolphs et al., , 1995Feinstein et al., 2011). Driven by animal research and more focal amygdala damage studies in humans, the amygdala is now seen as a heterogenous structure of which the subregions have different, sometimes even opposing, functions . ...
Article
In this paper I discuss the main models that have tried to explain brain asymmetries for emotions. The first models, based on clinical observations, proposed either a general right hemisphere dominance for emotions (the’right hemisphere’) model or a different specialization of the right hemisphere for negative and of the left hemisphere for positive emotions (the‘valence’ model). In more recent times new models, based on partly modified versions of the previous ones have been proposed. The revised version of the ‘valence’ model, labeled the ‘approach-avoidance’ model maintained that hemispheric asymmetries are not related to the valence of the emotional stimulus but to the motivational (approach vs avoidance) system that is engaged by that stimulus. On the contrary, revised versions of the ‘right hemisphere’ hypothesis proposed graded versions of this model, maintaining that only some kinds or some levels of emotions are clearly right lateralized. One version of these models (the ’emotion type hypothesis’) assumed that only elementary basic emotions should be subsumed by the right hemisphere, wheres more complex social emotions should be subtended by the left hemisphere. The other version (the ‘schematic level of emotion hypothesis’) assumed that the right hemisphere should subsume only the basic ‘schematic’ level of emotions, characterized by an automatic and unconscious processing, whereas the more propositional and conscious ‘conceptual’ level could be less lateralized or subsumed by the left hemisphere. This last model is supported by the obsevation that the right hemisphere reveals a modus operandi (i.e. a prevalence of the ‘automatic’ over the ‘intentional’ and of the ‘unconscious’ over the ‘conscious’ functional processing) that is typical of the ‘schematic level of emotions
Article
Full-text available
The amygdala nuclei modulate distributed neural circuits that most likely evolved to respond to environmental threats and opportunities. So far, the specific role of unique amygdala nuclei in the context processing of salient environmental cues lacks adequate characterization across neural systems and over time. Here, we present amygdala nuclei morphometry and behavioral findings from longitudinal population data (>1400 subjects, age range 40-69 years, sampled 2-3 years apart): the UK Biobank offers exceptionally rich phenotyping along with brain morphology scans. This allows us to quantify how 18 microanatomical amygdala subregions undergo plastic changes in tandem with coupled neural systems and delineating their associated phenome-wide profiles. In the context of population change, the basal, lateral, accessory basal, and paralaminar nuclei change in lockstep with the prefrontal cortex, a region that subserves planning and decision-making. The central, medial and cortical nuclei are structurally coupled with the insular and anterior-cingulate nodes of the salience network, in addition to the MT/V5, basal ganglia, and putamen, areas proposed to represent internal bodily states and mediate attention to environmental cues. The central nucleus and anterior amygdaloid area are longitudinally tied with the inferior parietal lobule, known for a role in bodily awareness and social attention. These population-level amygdala-brain plasticity regimes in turn are linked with unique collections of phenotypes, ranging from social status and employment to sleep habits and risk taking. The obtained structural plasticity findings motivate hypotheses about the specific functions of distinct amygdala nuclei in humans.
Article
Full-text available
Previous work has implicated projections from the acoustic thalamus to the amygdala in the classical conditioning of emotional responses to auditory stimuli. The purpose of the present studies was to determine whether the lateral amygdaloid nucleus (AL), which is a major subcortical target of projections from the acoustic thalamus, might be the sensory interface of the amygdala in emotional conditioning. Lesions were placed in AL of rats and the effects on emotional conditioning were examined. Lesions of AL, but not lesions of the striatum above or the cortex adjacent to the AL, interfered with emotional conditioning. Lesions that only partially destroyed AL or lesions placed too ventrally that completely missed AL had no effect. AL lesions did not affect the responses elicited following nonassociative (random) training. AL is thus an essential link in the circuitry through which auditory stimuli are endowed with affective properties and may function as the sensory interface of the amygdala during emotional learning.
Article
The neuromodulator serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) has been associated with mood disorders such as depression, anxiety, and impulsive violence. To define the contribution of 5-HT receptor subtypes to behavior, mutant mice lacking the 5-HT1B receptor were generated by homologous recombination. These mice did not exhibit any obvious developmental or behavioral defects. However, the hyperlocomotor effect of the 5-HT1A/1B agonist RU24969 was absent in mutant mice, indicating that this effect is mediated by 5-HT1B receptors. Moreover, when confronted with an intruder, mutant mice attacked the intruder faster and more intensely than did wild-type mice, suggesting the participation of 5-HT1B receptors in aggressive behavior.
Chapter
This chapter presents an introduction to cognitive neuropsychology. The term cognitive neuropsychology is applied to the analysis of those handicaps in human cognitive function that result from brain injury. Cognitive neuropsychology is essentially interdisciplinary, drawing both on neurology and on cognitive psychology for insights into the cerebral organization of cognitive skills and abilities. Cognitive function is the ability to use and integrate basic capacities such as perception, language, actions, memory, and thought. The focus of clinical cognitive neuropsychology is on the many different types of highly selective impairments of cognitive function that are observed in individual patients following brain damage. The functional analysis of patients with selective deficits provides a very clear window through which one can observe the organization and procedures of normal cognition. Clinical cognitive neuropsychology has been successful in demonstrating a large number of dissociations between the subcomponents of cognitive skills. This enables to conclude that such components are dependent on distinct neural systems.
Chapter
The research described represents an attempt to determine the exact amygdala components which contribute to the acquisition of conditioned responding during aversive conditioning. Our initial analysis focuses on the amygdala central nucleus and its contribution to the acquisition of conditioned bradycardia during Pavlovian fear conditioning in the rabbit. The results demonstrate that (a) lesions of the central nucleus attenuate the magnitude of the conditioned bradycardia response, (b) the administration of ß-adrenergic antagonists and opiate agonists into the region of the central nucleus also attenuate the magnitude of the conditioned bradycardia response, (c) the medial component of the central nucleus projects directly to cardioregulatory nuclei in the dorsal medulla including the site of origin of cardioinhibitory neurons in the rabbit, (d) electrical stimulation of the central nucleus in rabbit produces profound, short-latency bradycardia and depressor responses, with maximum bradycardia elicited from the site of origin of the central nucleus-dorsal medulla projection, and (e) during the course of the conditioning procedure increases in central nucleus neuronal activity develop to the conditioned stimulus (CS) at the time when the conditioned bradycardia response develops. In some cases the magnitude of the increase in neuronal activity to the CS was significantly correlated with the magnitude of the bradycardia response to the CS over the course of the conditioning session. The results are consistent with the hypothesis that at least one function of the amygdala central nucleus in the acquisition of conditioned bradycardia may be in the motoric expression of the conditioned response to the CS by modulation of vagal preganglionic cardioinhibitory neurons within the dorsal medulla.
Article
Abstract Cerebral blood flow was measured using positron emission tomography (PET) in three experiments while subjects performed mental imagery or analogous perceptual tasks. In Experiment 1, the subjects either visualized letters in grids and decided whether an X mark would have fallen on each letter if it were actually in the grid, or they saw letters in grids and decided whether an X mark fell on each letter. A region identified as part of area 17 by the Talairach and Tournoux (1988) atlas, in addition to other areas involved in vision, was activated more in the mental imagery task than in the perception task. In Experiment 2, the identical stimuli were presented in imagery and baseline conditions, but subjects were asked to form images only in the imagery condition; the portion of area 17 that was more active in the imagery condition of Experiment 1 was also more activated in imagery than in the baseline condition, as was part of area 18. Subjects also were tested with degraded perceptual stimuli, which caused visual cortex to be activated to the same degree in imagery and perception. In both Experiments 1 and 2, however, imagery selectively activated the extreme anterior part of what was identified as area 17, which is inconsistent with the relatively small size of the imaged stimuli. These results, then, suggest that imagery may have activated another region just anterior to area 17. In Experiment 3, subjects were instructed to close their eyes and evaluate visual mental images of upper case letters that were formed at a small size or large size. The small mental images engendered more activation in the posterior portion of visual cortex, and the large mental images engendered more activation in anterior portions of visual cortex. This finding is strong evidence that imagery activates topographically mapped cortex. The activated regions were also consistent with their being localized in area 17. Finally, additional results were consistent with the existence of two types of imagery, one that rests on allocating attention to form a pattern and one that rests on activating stored visual memories.
Article
Some patients with face agnosia (prosopagnosia) caused by occipitotemporal damage produce discriminatory covert responses to the familiar faces that they fail to identify overtly. For example, their average skin conductance responses (SCRs) to familiar faces are significantly larger than average SCRs to unfamiliar faces. In this study we describe the opposite dissociation in four patients with bilateral ventromedial frontal damage: The patients recognized the identity of familiar faces normally, yet failed to generate discriminatory SCRs to those same familiar faces. Taken together, the two sets of results constitute a double dissociation: bilateral occipitotemporal damage impairs recognition but allows SCR discrimination, whereas bilateral ventromedial damage causes the opposite. The findings suggest that the neural systems that process the somatic-based valence of stimuli are separate from and parallel to the neural systems that process the factual, nonsomatic information associated with the same stimuli.