ArticlePDF Available

Spectral bandwidth and ocular accommodation

Optica Publishing Group
Journal of the Optical Society of America A
Authors:
  • Ben Vision Research

Abstract and Figures

Previous studies have suggested that targets illuminated by monochromatic (narrow-band) light are less effective in stimulating the eye to change its focus than are black-white (broadband) targets. The present study investigates the influence of target spectral bandwidth on the dynamic accommodation response in eight subjects. The fixation target was a 3.5-cycle/deg square-wave grating illuminated by midspectral light of various bandwidths [10, 40, and 80 nm and white (CIE Illuminant B)]. The target was moved sinusoidally toward and away from the eye, and accommodation responses were recorded and Fourier analyzed. Accommodative gain increases, and phase lag decreases, with increasing spectral bandwidth. Thus the eye focuses more accurately on targets of wider spectral bandwidth. The visual system appears to have the ability to analyze polychromatic blur to determine the state of focus of the eye for the purpose of guiding the accommodation response.
Content may be subject to copyright.
450 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A /Vol. 12, No. 3/March 1995 Aggarwala et al.
Spectral bandwidth and ocular accommodation
Karan R. Aggarwala, Ekaterina S. Kruger, Steven Mathews, and Philip B. Kruger
Schnurmacher Institute for Vision Research, State College of Optometry,
State University of New York, 100 East 24th Street, New York, New York 10010
Received April 13, 1994; revised manuscript received October 4, 1994; accepted October 4, 1994
Previous studies have suggested that targets illuminated by monochromatic (narrow-band) light are less
effective in stimulating the eye to change its focus than are black white (broadband) targets. The present
study investigates the influence of target spectral bandwidth on the dynamic accommodation response in
eight subjects. The fixation target was a 3.5-cycleydeg square-wave grating illuminated by midspectral light
of various bandwidths [10, 40, and 80 nm and white (CIE Illuminant B)]. The target was moved sinusoidally
toward and away from the eye, and accommodation responses were recorded and Fourier analyzed. Ac-
commodative gain increases, and phase lag decreases, with increasing spectral bandwidth. Thus the eye
focuses more accurately on targets of wider spectral bandwidth. The visual system appears to have the
ability to analyze polychromatic blur to determine the state of focus of the eye for the purpose of guiding the
accommodation response.
Key words: Aberration, accommodation, bandwidth, blur, chromatic, focus, retinal image, spectral,
wavelength
1. INTRODUCTION
Accommodation is the process by which the eye focuses
objects in response to changes in viewing distance. Al-
though studies have shown that perceived distance,1,2 cog-
nitive demand,3and voluntary effort4,5 contribute to the
accommodation response, the eye accommodates with re-
markable accuracy even when these cues are eliminated.6
This implies that optical (dioptric) stimuli for accommo-
dation (e.g., blur produced by defocus) are important for
driving accommodation.
Blur has been regarded by several investigators as
the primary optical stimulus for accommodation.7–10 Yet
blur of a monochromatic (narrow-band) target is not an
effective stimulus for accommodation.11–14 This implies
that the visual system obtains certain information about
the state of focus of the eye from the blurred image of a
polychromatic target and that such information is absent
in monochromatic light. Crane15 proposed that, in the
presence of chromatic aberration, the three photoreceptor
mechanisms of the eye, with their individual spectral sen-
sitivity functions, sample the polychromatic retinal image
at three levels of focus. As a natural consequence of lon-
gitudinal chromatic aberration, contrast of the retinal im-
age is maximum for the wavelength in focus such that if
long-wave light is in focus, image contrast is maximum
at long wavelengths and is reduced for short-wavelength
light.16 It seems plausible that a comparison of image
contrast between two wavebands could yield information
(encoded as neural signals) that represents the state of
focus of the eye. In a computational model, Flitcroft17
suggests that spatially antagonistic, color-opponent cells
might form a substrate for comparing contrast in different
wavebands to monitor the focus of targets of intermediate
spatial frequency [28 cycles per degree (cycydeg)].
In the present study we illuminated a grating target
by light of four nominal spectral bandwidths [10, 40,
and 80 nm and broadband white (CIE Illuminant B)] to
determine whether targets of progressively wider spec-
tral bandwidth encourage more-accurate accommodation.
We analyzed the nature of the blur spread function for
targets of increasing spectral bandwidth after consider-
ing the effects of the photopic spectral sensitivity of the
eye and longitudinal chromatic aberration.18,19
2. METHODS
The eight subjects were young adults with normal color
vision (Nagel anomaloscope) and 20y20 corrected Snellen
acuity. A 3.5-cycydeg square-wave grating target was
presented to the subject’s eye in a Badal optical system20
so that changes in target distance altered focus without
affecting the size or illumination of the target.21 The
grating was a Ronchi ruling, illuminated by broadband
white light (4874-K CIE Illuminant B) or by bandpassed
light produced by the introduction of interference fil-
ters (10, 40, and 80 nm) in front of a tungsten– halogen
source. Target luminance was equalized by a neutral-
density wedge. An aerial image of the target moved
sinusoidally toward and away from the Badal lens to
stimulate the eye to change its focus. Accommoda-
tion of the eye was monitored by a high-speed infrared
optometer,22 and the data were analyzed by a fast Fourier
transform (FFT). Gain and phase lag of the response at
the temporal frequency of target motion (0.2 Hz) served
as an index of accommodative performance to the various
spectral targets.
A. Optical System
The optical system used for presenting targets and stimu-
lating accommodation is described in Fig. 1. The re-
cording optometer was described previously22 and is
represented in Fig. 1 as a rectangle (IR OPT).
1. Illumination Optics (Dashed Lines)
Light from a tungsten–halogen source (3200 K) was
filtered by a Kodak (80 B) color-compensating filter to
produce light of a higher color temperature (CIE Illumi-
0740-3232/95/030450-06$06.00 1995 Optical Society of America
Aggarwala et al. Vol. 12, No. 3/March 1995/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 451
Fig. 1. Schematic of the Badal optical system for stimulating
accommodation of the eye (E). Dashed lines show the optical
path from the source of illumination, and solid lines represent
target optics. Interference filters of three bandwidths (10, 40,
and 80 nm) could be introduced at F to alter the spectral composi-
tion of a square-wave grating target (T). The sinusoidal motion
of prism P2 moved an aerial image of target T0toward and away
from the Badal lens (L4) through a range of 1.0 D.
nant B, 4874 K),23 and the light source was imaged onto
an opal diffuser (D). Light from a circular patch on the
diffuser was collimated by lens L1, and interference fil-
ters of various half-peak bandwidths (10, 40, and 80 nm)
were introduced to alter the spectral bandwidth of the
source. The collimated beam was deflected by a mirror
(M) and illuminated a grating target (T) from behind.
Lens L2 formed an image of the source in the plane of a
12-mm aperture (A). Lenses L3 and L4 together imaged
the source in the plane of the subject’s pupil (Maxwellian
view). Aperture A was imaged by these lenses (L3 and
L4) as a 3-mm artificial pupil. Light rays of the illumi-
nation system remained collimated as they reflected off
the mirrored surfaces of prisms P1 and P2.
2. Target Optics (Solid Lines)
The target was a Ronchi ruling oriented vertically, pre-
sented in a 6-deg circular field with blurred margins.
Rays from target T (Fig. 1) were collimated by lens L2
and focused by lens L3 to form an aerial image at T0af-
ter reflection off mirrored prisms P1 and P2. The po-
sition of the aerial image (with regard to Badal lens L4)
could be altered by movement of prism P2 toward or away
from prism P1, as shown by the arrows. Prism motion
was controlled by computer and synchronized with the
data acquisition. The subject’s eye was positioned with
the pupil plane at the second principal focus of the Badal
lens sf10 cmdby viewing of the first Purkinje image of
the target in a telescope (not shown). Each centimeter
of target motion sT0dgenerated a 1.0-D change in optical
vergence at the eye.
B. Spectral Bandwidths
Two definitions of spectral bandwidth have been em-
ployed in the present study. For the interference filters
used, the manufacturer’s specifications were used, and
these are defined as the wavelength interval at half-peak
transmittance. For the analysis presented in the dis-
cussion (Section 4 below), bandwidth is defined as wave-
length interval at 1yeof peak normalized luminance.
Four bands of light were used for illuminating the tar-
get. Light from the source (4874 K) was passed through
interference filters (peak transmittance at 550 nm) to cre-
ate the spectral bands depicted in Fig. 2. Luminance of
the targets was measured through the Maxwellian-view
system21 by a Pritchard photometer and maintained at
80 cdym2by a neutral-density wedge.
C. Procedures
Subjects were positioned on a bite plate assembly to sta-
bilize the head and to facilitate alignment. The target
moved sinusoidally toward and away from the subject’s
eye at a temporal frequency of 0.2 Hz with a peak-to-peak
amplitude of 1.0 D, around a mean level of 2 D. Subjects
were instructed to look at the center of the grating and
to pay undivided attention to the target. A temporal fre-
quency of 0.2 Hz was used because at higher temporal fre-
quencies gain declines substantially,6,13 thereby reducing
the signal-to-noise ratio, whereas at lower temporal fre-
quencies voluntary accommodation is more likely to have
some influence on the response.
Each accommodation trial lasted 40.96 s, yielding an
array of 4096 (212) data points at a sampling rate of 100ys.
We chose 40.96 s (as opposed to 40 s) because of con-
straints posed by the FFT procedure, which required an
array size that is an integer power of 2. During each
trial eight sinusoidal cycles of target focus were presented
monocularly. Subjects were allowed to blink but were in-
structed not to use blinks in an attempt to improve the
perceptual clarity of the target. Most subjects did not
blink more than three or four times, and data with ex-
cessive blinks (e.g., produced by tearing) were rejected.
Blinks produced high-amplitude transient artifacts in the
data that were eliminated by filtering if their velocity ex-
ceeded 12 Dys. The four spectral conditions (10, 40, and
80 nm and white) were presented five times to each sub-
ject in random order. Gain and phase lag of accommoda-
tion for each trial were obtained by Fourier analysis (FFT)
and were vector averaged for each condition. Analysis-
of-variance and post hoc multiple comparison procedures
were applied to the mean gain and phase data sn8dfor
a within-subject experimental design.
Fig. 2. Spectral distributions of the four test conditions (10,
40, and 80 nm and white) normalized to their individual peaks.
The bandwidths specified here are nominal in that they refer to
the filter manufacturer’s specifications of bandwidth at half-peak
transmittance.
452 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A /Vol. 12, No. 3/March 1995 Aggarwala et al.
Fig. 3. Accommodation responses to the four target conditions
for two subjects. The uppermost trace (stimulus) shows sinu-
soidal target motion (0.2 Hz, 1-D amplitude) toward and away
from the Badal lens. The response traces represent accommo-
dation to a 3.5-cycydeg square-wave grating target illuminated
by light of a specified spectral distribution (Fig. 2). The two
subjects shown here are not typical but rather depict extremes
of the range of accommodative behaviors observed in the present
study.
3. RESULTS
Accommodation responses to the four target conditions
(10, 40, and 80 nm and white) are plotted for two
subjects (S1 and S2) in Fig. 3. These two subjects rep-
resent the extremes of the range of accommodative be-
haviors exhibited by the sample (eight subjects in all).
Notice that the high-frequency oscillations of accommo-
dation are more pronounced for subject S2 than for sub-
ject S1 for all conditions. Another notable difference is
that the response of subject S1 to the 10-nm condition
shows some time periods during which little or no accom-
modative tracking is evident, whereas subject S2 exhibits
reasonable tracking ability in this condition (10 nm), al-
beit of reduced amplitude and longer phase lag than for
the white target. For both subjects the amplitude of
the response increases progressively as the bandwidth of
light illuminating the target is increased, suggesting that
accommodation is facilitated for targets of wider spectral
bandwidth. It is apparent from these raw data (Fig. 3)
that the natural high-frequency oscillations of accommo-
dation make it somewhat difficult to judge the accuracy
of the accommodation response. To make a quantitative
assessment of the data, gain and phase lag of the re-
sponse at the temporal frequency of the stimulus (0.2 Hz)
were computed (FFT) and were vector averaged for the
five trials from each subject.
Gain and phase lag for two typical subjects are plotted
in Fig. 4. Error bars represent one standard error on ei-
ther side of the mean for five data trials per condition.
It is clear from these data that, despite individual differ-
ences, the gain of accommodation increases and the phase
lag decreases as the spectral bandwidth of the illumina-
tion is changed from narrow-band (10 nm) to broadband
(white).
Average gain and phase lag (Fig. 5) demonstrate the ef-
fect of spectral bandwidth on accommodative function in a
group of eight subjects. Univariate analysis of variance
shows that the gain of accommodation differs significantly
across spectral bandwidth fFs3, 21d42.3, p,0.001g,
as does the phase lag fFs3, 21d17.9, p,0.001g.A
conservative multiple comparison test (Tukey HSD) be-
tween means illustrates that gain increases significantly
between successive progressive increases in bandwidth
sp,0.05d, except for the 40- and 80-nm pair of condi-
tions. However, mean phase lags of accommodation for
Fig. 4. Gain and phase lag of accommodation as a function of
the spectral bandwidth of the target for two typical subjects,
determined by a vector average of five trials per condition. The
gain is an amplitude ratio (responseystimulus), and the phase lag
is a time lag of the response with regard to the stimulus. As
the target’s spectral bandwidth increases, accommodative gain
improves and phase lag declines.
Fig. 5. Average gain and phase data for eight subjects to each
of the four spectral conditions (10, 40, and 80 nm and white).
Accommodative gain increases and phase lag decreases with
increasing spectral bandwidth.
Aggarwala et al. Vol. 12, No. 3/March 1995 / J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 453
Fig. 6. Effective spectral distribution of the test conditions
computed by multiplication of the photopic spectral sensitivity
function of the eye by the functions depicted in Fig. 2. The
horizontal line is 1yeheight for these effective wavebands.
The points of intersection of the 1yeline with the wavebands
in the short-wave region (below 550 nm) are designated lS, and
those in the long-wave region are represented by lL. Numerical
values for lSand lLare given in Table 1.
these two conditions are significantly different sp,0.05d.
Average phase data also differ at the 0.05 level for all
pairs of conditions, excluding the 10- and 40-nm bands,
to which the gain data were significantly different.
4. DISCUSSION
Taken together, the present results indicate that a wider
spectral bandwidth of illumination allows the visual
system to focus more accurately. From an ecological
standpoint this does not come as a surprise because natu-
ral objects possess broad spectral reflectance functions,24
and the eye is seldom confronted with narrow-band light.
Even the spectral distributions of colorful objects can be
relatively broadband.24 When sunlight reflects diffusely
from these objects, the retinal image is composed of light
of a spectrum of wavelengths. Thus it seems reasonable
to speculate that the visual system might have evolved
focusing mechanisms that operate best in the presence of
broadband illumination.
Spectrally broadband targets, when imaged by the op-
tics of the eye, produce a complex retinal image that can
be thought to consist of a series of image planes, one image
plane for each wavelength of light. These image planes
are displaced axially by an amount that depends on the
longitudinal chromatic aberration of the eye and on the
particular wavelengths in question. In addition, the ef-
fects of chromatic aberration are altered by the spectral
sensitivity of the eye,23,25 which declines substantially at
the extremes of the visible spectrum. To help to illus-
trate the effects of chromatic aberration, we multiplied
the spectral luminous efficiency function of the eye (CIE:
193125) by the normalized spectral radiance of the four
targets used in the experiment. As a result, the band-
widths of the stimuli are reduced, most notably for the
white target, which now appears as a bandpass function
(see Fig. 6).
Two extreme wavelengths (lSand lL) were chosen for
each of the four wavebands, based on the 1yeheight
of the functions shown in Fig. 6. Dioptric vergence at
the retina was computed for each wavelength17 and is
presented in Table 1. For the present experiment the
chromatic difference in focus between lSand lLcan be
regarded as the ocular longitudinal chromatic aberration
(LCA) present in each of the four targets. Figure 7 illus-
trates the effects of chromatic aberration on the retinal
image of a luminance border (edge) for each of the four
wavebands of light when the longer wavelength slLdis in
focus. Nominal values for bandwidth (10, 40, and 80 nm
and white) have been retained in the figure. Real values
Fig. 7. Effect of increasing spectral bandwidth on the blur-
spread function (of a luminance edge) for an eye with a 4-mm
pupil. The longer wavelength slLdof each spectral condition
is in focus (dotted curves), and the shorter wavelength slSdis
out of focus (dashed curves) by an amount dependent on the
longitudinal chromatic aberration produced by light of these two
wavelengths.
Table 1. Optical Vergence and LCA for Each of the Four Spectral Wavebands at Two Extreme
Wavelengths, lL
lL
lLand lS
lS
lS
lLlSlL2l
S
Bandwidth Wavelength Vergence Wavelength Vergence Wavelength LCA
(nominal) (nm) (D) (nm) (D) (nm) (D)
10 nm 556 20.123 544 20.199 12 0.076
40 nm 576 20.010 531 20.288 45 0.278
80 nm 594 10.081 526 20.324 68 0.405
White 619 10.191 505 20.490 114 0.681
454 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A /Vol. 12, No. 3/March 1995 Aggarwala et al.
for lSand lLand for 1yebandwidth slL2l
S
dare tabu-
lated (Table 1) along with the dioptric vergence and the
amount of chromatic aberration.
The dashed curves in Fig. 7 show the luminance distri-
bution of lSwhen lL(dotted curves) is in focus on the
retina. The defocus of lSwith regard to lL(Table 1,
rightmost column) was used to find the standard deviation
(in minutes of arc) of a Gaussian point-spread function,
by the methods of Fry,26 for a schematic eye with a 4-mm
pupil. The edge-spread functions of Fig. 7 represent the
definite integral of the point-spread function for the tabu-
lated amounts of LCA. The type of blur depicted in Fig. 7
is a natural consequence of longitudinal chromatic aber-
ration in an eye with a pupil of 4-mm diameter. All sub-
jects in the study had pupils larger than 3 mm, and the
choice of a 4-mm pupil (for Fig. 7) is arbitrary. Larger
pupils result in wider edge-spread functions, and smaller
pupils (e.g., 2 mm) reduce the blur produced by LCA.
Although the difference in ocular focus between the
ends of the visible spectrum is substantial18,19 (2D or
more), even the relatively small amounts of chromatic
aberration used for the present analysis produce a sig-
nificant decline in the slope (contrast) of the edge-spread
function. The width of the effective edge-spread function
(including Vl) for the 40-nm band of light is ,4arcmin,
and for white light it is approximately 10 arcmin. It is
important to note that Fig. 7 has been generated strictly
for the purpose of illustrating the fact that, even after
the severely band-limiting effects of Vlare included, the
chromatic aberration of the eye has a notable effect on the
blur profile of a luminance edge.
Results of the present experiment are in agreement
with studies indicating that the visual system has the ca-
pacity to detect blur produced by chromatic aberration at
luminance edges.11 14 The three cone types of the retina,
with their individual spectral sensitivity functions, effec-
tively sample the retinal image at three different levels
of defocus, corresponding to their wavelengths of peak
sensitivity15 or perhaps to a weighted average includ-
ing the radiance distribution of the image.17 It seems
plausible that a comparison of retinal image quality be-
tween cone types, possibly through spatially bandpass,
color-opponent pathways, could generate a neural signal
that varies in proportion to ocular defocus and that could
be used to direct accommodation. However, further re-
search is necessary to confirm the involvement of color-
opponent mechanisms in the control of accommodation to
defocus of polychromatic targets.
In the present investigation the accuracy of dynamic
accommodation was influenced significantly by incre-
mental changes in target spectral bandwidth. Our
findings agree with the results of studies done concur-
rently by other investigators who used different stimulus
parameters.12 Previous investigators10 seem to disagree
with the view that spectrally bandpass light (and thereby
reduced ocular LCA) impairs accommodation, and the
reasons for this discrepancy are not entirely clear. One
possible explanation is that previous investigators tested
this issue by using stationary targets (stimulusresponse
function), and they may have trained their observers to
accommodate voluntarily. Those authors reported on
one na¨
ıve subject (aged 20 years) who showed poor ac-
commodation to spectrally bandpass (red or blue) targets
(Ref. 10, Fig. 2f, p. 462); however, they disregarded these
data as an artifact of inadequate training. They noted
that “Training and motivation undoubtedly also play an
important role, as is illustrated by subject (f ), a secretary
chosen to typify effects found with untrained observers.
She evidently failed to respond at all to the lens-induced,
higher target vergences.” After reinstructing this subject
(“careful explanation of the nature of the experiment”),
the authors reported that she too could focus in monochro-
matic light. They reported a similar initial inaccuracy
of accommodation for other na¨
ıve subjects to red or blue
targets,10 but their conclusions were based on the results
obtained from trained observers. In the present experi-
ment two of the authors served as subjects, while the re-
maining six were na¨
ıve to the purpose of the experiment.
We find that trained observers respond in the same way
as na¨
ıve subjects to moving targets, and we have used
moving targets in our experiments to minimize the influ-
ence of voluntary accommodation. A systematic study of
the effect of training is in order, but it must be conducted
by use of both stationary and moving targets.
The effects of ocular chromatic aberration are usually
dismissed as being small and not significant enough to
influence visual mechanisms.27,28 It is generally argued
that, if a midspectral wavelength (say, 555 nm) is in fo-
cus, the spectral sensitivity of the eye reduces the effective
chromatic aberration to very small amounts (0.15 D),28
which may lie within the depth of focus of the eye. How-
ever, the oscillations of accommodation29,30 are constantly
changing the wavelength in focus, and, when a target
moves toward or away from the eye, once again a new
wavelength comes into focus. In fact the natural lag of
accommodation to near targets and the lead of accommo-
dation to far targets31 also change the wavelength in fo-
cus. In the present study the 40-nm condition produced
only 0.278 D of LCA; however, even such small amounts
of LCA produce substantial facilitation of dynamic accom-
modation (see Section 3).
The data (Figs. 3 5) and the statistical analysis (Tukey
HSD test) indicate that successively wider bands of target
illumination produce an incremental improvement in ac-
commodative performance. The bandwidth at which ac-
commodative performance is the same as that for white
is difficult to identify from these data mainly because the
response to a white target (CIE Illuminant B) is signifi-
cantly better than accommodation to the 80-nm bandpass
target. The improvement in gain (and reduction in phase
lag) from the 80-nm to the broadband white condition
indicates the involvement of short-wavelength-sensitive
cones in the analysis of the blur-spread function. Al-
though the notion of accommodative control by individual
cone types15 (or by comparisons of image quality between
cone types mediated by color-opponent cells17) is attrac-
tive, more experimental evidence is required.
The present investigation supports the view that the
visual system has mechanisms for utilizing chromatic
aberration as a source of information about the state of
focus of the eye, and it uses this information to guide
the accommodation response. These mechanisms could
operate at levels close to the thresholds for chromaticity
discrimination32 34 and contrast-decrement sensitivity.35
Further research is needed to uncover the neural sub-
strate for the observed sensitivity of the eye–brain system
Aggarwala et al. Vol. 12, No. 3/March 1995 / J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 455
to blur produced by polychromatic targets in the presence
of ocular longitudinal chromatic aberration.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by grants from the National
Eye Institute (EYO7494, EYO8953, EYO5901) and by a
postdoctoral fellowship (F 32 EYO6403-02) awarded to
K. R. Aggarwala. We thank Dean Yager, Milton Katz,
and Jordan Pola for their helpful suggestions, John
Orzuchowski and Mathew Polasky for technical assis-
tance, and Jong Park for assistance with data collection.
REFERENCES
1. W. H. Ittleson and A. Ames, “Accommodation, convergence,
and their relation to apparent distance,” J. Psychol. 30,
43–62 (1950).
2. P. B. Kruger and J. Pola, “Changing target size is a stimulus
for accommodation,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 1832 1835 (1985).
3. P. B. Kruger, “The effect of cognitive demand on accommo-
dation,” Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Opt. 57, 440 445 (1980).
4. R. R. Provine and J. M. Enoch, “On voluntary ocular accom-
modation,” Percept. Psychophys. 17, 209212 (1975).
5. L. N. McLin and C. M. Schor, “Voluntary effort as a stimulus
to accommodation and vergence,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis.
Sci. 29, 1739–1746 (1988).
6. F. W. Campbell and G. Westheimer, “Dynamics of accom-
modation responses of the human eye,” J. Physiol. (London)
151, 285–295 (1960).
7. L. M. Smithline, “Accommodative responses to blur,” J. Opt.
Soc. Am. 64, 1512–1516 (1974).
8. A. Troelstra, B. L. Zuber, D. Miller, and L. Stark, “Accom-
modative tracking: a trial-and-error function,” Vision Res.
4, 585–594 (1964).
9. S. Phillips and L. Stark, “Blur: a sufficient accommodative
stimulus,” Doc. Ophthalmol. 43, 65–89 (1977).
10. W. N. Charman and J. Tucker, “Accommodation and color,”
J. Opt. Soc. Am. 68, 459–471 (1978).
11. D. I. Flitcroft and S. J. Judge, “The effect of stimulus chro-
maticity on ocular accommodation in the monkey,” J. Phys-
iol. (London) 398, 36 (1988).
12. J. C. Kotulak, S. E. Morse, and V. A. Billock, “Red–green
opponent channel mediates control of human ocular accom-
modation,” J. Physiol. (London) (to be published).
13. P. B. Kruger and J. Pola, “Stimuli for accommodation: blur,
chromatic aberration and size,” Vision Res. 26, 957 971
(1986).
14. P. B. Kruger, S. Mathews, K. R. Aggarwala, and N. Sanchez,
“Chromatic aberration and ocular focus: Fincham revis-
ited,” Vision Res. 33, 1397–1411 (1993).
15. H. D. Crane, “A theoretical analysis of the visual accommo-
dation system in humans,” Stanford Res. Inst. Proj. 5454,
NASA CR-606 (NASA, Washington, D.C., 1966).
16. A. Bradley, X. Zhang, and L. N. Thibos, “Failures of isolumi-
nance caused by ocular chromatic aberrations,” Appl. Opt.
31, 3657–3667 (1992).
17. D. I. Flitcroft, “A neural and computational model for the
chromatic control of accommodation,” Visual Neurosci. 5,
547–555 (1990).
18. R. E. Bedford and G. Wyszecki, “Axial chromatic aberration
of the eye,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 564565 (1957).
19. P. A. Howarth and A. Bradley, “The longitudinal chromatic
aberration of the eye and its correction,” Vision Res. 26,
361–366 (1986).
20. H. D. Crane and T. N. Cornsweet, “Ocular-focus stimulator,”
J. Opt. Soc. Am. 60, 577 (1970).
21. G. Westheimer, “The Maxwellian view,” Vision Res. 6,
669–682 (1966).
22. P. B. Kruger, “Infrared recording retinoscope for monotoring
accommodation,” Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Opt. 56, 116 123
(1979).
23. J. Pokorny and V. Smith, “Colorimetry and color discrimina-
tion,” in Handbook of Perception and Human Performance,
K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, and J. P. Thomas, eds. (Wiley, New
York, 1986), Vol. 1.
24. E. L. Krinov, “Spectral reflectance properties of natural
formations,” Tech. Translation TT-439 (National Research
Council of Canada, Ottawa, Canada, 1947).
25. G. Wyszecki and W. S. Stiles, Color Science: Concepts
and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulas (Wiley, New
York, 1967).
26. G. A. Fry, Blur of the Retinal Image (Ohio State U. Press,
Columbus, Ohio, 1955).
27. M. Millidot, “Effect of aberrations of the eye on visual percep-
tion,” in Visual Psychophysics and Physiology, J. Armington,
J. Krauskopf, and B. R. Wooten, eds. (Academic, New York,
1978), Chap. 35.
28. A. Bradley, X. Zhang, and L. N. Thibos, “Achromatizing the
human eye,” Optom. Vis. Sci. 68, 608–616 (1991).
29. F. W. Campbell, J. Robson, and G. Westheimer, “Fluctua-
tions of accommodation under steady viewing conditions,”
J. Physiol. (London) 145, 579–594 (1959).
30. W. N. Charman and G. Heron, “Fluctuations in accommoda-
tion: a review,” Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 8, 153 164 (1988).
31. G. Westheimer, “Focusing responses of the human eye,” Am.
J. Optom. Arch. Am. Acad. Optom. 43, 221–232 (1966).
32. C. Noorlander, M. J. G. Heuts, and J. J. Koenderink, “Sen-
sitivity to spatio-temporal combined luminance and chro-
maticity contrast,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 71, 453–459 (1981).
33. R. H. Hilz, G. Huppman, and C. R. Cavonius, “Influence of
luminance contrast on hue discrimination,” J. Opt. Soc. Am.
64, 763–766 (1974).
34. G. J. C. van der Horst and M. A. Bouman, “Spatiotem-
poral chromaticity discrimination,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 59,
1482–1488 (1969).
35. S. M. Mathews, N. Kapoor, D. Yager, and P. B. Kruger, “Ac-
commodative fluctuations and contrast decrement sensitiv-
ity,” Invest. Ophthal. Vis. Sci. Suppl. 34, 2974 (1993).
... However, various textbackground colour combinations on electronic visual displays show no significant difference in the accommodative response, which may stem from the fact that different subjects use different focus strategies [23,24]. Some studies found that light with narrow spectral bandwidth can't drive accurate accommodation [25,26]. However, using colour flickers that can provide a Fig. 3 Effects of frequency of colour flickers on variability of accommodation response (VAR) in the emmetrope (EMM) and myope (MYO) groups under the different colour conditions. ...
... On the contrary, VAR for R/G flicker were larger than for B/G flicker at low-frequency conditions, although the wavelength difference for the R/G flicker was smaller than that for the B/G flicker. This unexpected result may be due to the relative insensitivity of the central fovea of the human eye to blue light and narrow spectral bandwidth of blue light in our study [25,27]. This implies that the VAR may not simply depend on the wavelength difference, and thus further studies of the relationship between wavelength differences and VAR should be performed in the future. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Myopia is hypothesized to be influenced by environmental light conditions. For example, it has been shown that colour and temporal frequency of flickering light affect emmetropisation in animals. Considering the omnipresence of flickering light in our daily life, we decided to analyze the effect of colour flickers on variability of the accommodation response (VAR) in emmetropes and myopes. Methods We measured the dynamic accommodative responses of 19 emmetropic and 22 myopic adults using a Grand Seiko WAM-5500 open-field autorefractor. The subjects focused for more than 20 s on a black Snellen E target against three different backgrounds made up of three colour flicker combinations (red/green, red/blue and blue/green) and under five frequency conditions (0.20 Hz, 0.50 Hz, 1.00 Hz, 1.67 Hz, and 5.00 Hz). Results Flicker frequency and colour both had a significant effect on VAR. Lower frequencies were associated with larger variability. Colour had an effect only at low frequencies, and red/blue colour flicker resulted in the largest variability. The variability in myopes were larger than those in emmetropes. Conclusions These findings support the hypothesis that further studies on the colour and temporal frequency of flickering light can lead to a better understanding of the development and progression of myopia.
... By inverse square law: radiant intensity of microwaves emanating from the cellular phone quadruples for 9 inch compared to 18 inch. Ecological design of the human visual apparatus [25] enables complete utilization of the natural ocular spectral dispersion disparity analysed as R-G and B-Y waveband comparisons [26][27][28] bringing equality of perceptual analytics to rhesus monkey, guinea pig, goldfish and poultry fowler: each and all enabled with clear motivation toward evolutionary niche explorations by exposure to chromatic extremes [29] yet in search for our very own [30] Madagascar paradise. But in this tortuous pathological ranting, did we completely ignore the jumping spider? ...
Article
This is not just another commentary on developmental biology of myopia. It is a call to justice. As responsible physicians we could restore credibility and reclaim the Oath attributed to Hippocrates. Our journey has never been more complicated. We may never again require anything more than the trust we betrayed. Ocular sciences here below lead into clinical practice with a smattering on technology. A sincere attempt is made to concisely convey to the reader: multiple interlinking subject area domains perhaps regarded as unrelated by traditional science. The debate has only just begun.
... It has been demonstrated that longitudinal chromatic aberrations provide directional information for accommodation in order to regain sharp focus in the retina, in such a way that the contrast of spectralwavebands of the retinal image mediate the signals that specify dioptric vergence (Kruger, Nowbotsing, & Aggarwala, 1995). Thus, the contrast of the retinal image would be maximum for the wavelength in focus and minimum for the farther wavelength of retina, and the three cone types would effectively sample this retinal contrast and therefore determinate the direction and magnitude of accommodative response (Aggarwala, Kruger, Mathews, & Kruger, 1995). In this sense, a greater long wavelength contrast than middle-wavelength contrast would lead to accommodative shift towards the visual far point (dis-accommodation), while a response towards near (accommodation) with a chromatic short-wavelength cone contrast would be expected (Rucker & Kruger, 2004. ...
Article
The purpose of the present study was to assess the accommodative response and pupillary dynamics while reading passages with different text-background color combinations on an LCD screen. Twenty healthy young adults read fourteen 2-min passages designed with fourteen different color combinations between text and background, while the accommodative and pupil responses were continuously measured with a binocular open-field autorefractometer. Our results revealed that the text-background color combination modulates the accommodative and pupillary dynamics during a 2-minutes reading task. The blue-red combination induced a heightened accommodative response, whereas positive polarities were associated with more variability of the accommodative response and smaller pupil sizes. Participants reported lower perceived ratings of legibility for text-background color combination with lower luminance contrast (white-yellow). The manipulation of text-background color did not have a significant effect on reading speed. These results may have important applications in the design of digital visual interfaces.
Article
Full-text available
The longitudinal chromatic aberration (LCA) of the eye creates a chromatic blur on the retina that is an important cue for accommodation. Although this mechanism can work optimally in broadband illuminants such as daylight, it is not clear how the system responds to the narrowband illuminants used by many modern displays. Here, we measured pupil and accommodative responses as well as visual acuity under narrowband light-emitting diode (LED) illuminants of different peak wavelengths. Observers were able to accommodate under narrowband light and compensate for the LCA of the eye, with no difference in the variability of the steady-state accommodation response between narrowband and broadband illuminants. Intriguingly, our subjects compensated more fully for LCA at nearer distances. That is, the difference in accommodation to different wavelengths became larger when the object was placed nearer the observer, causing the slope of the accommodation response curve to become shallower for shorter wavelengths and steeper for longer ones. Within the accommodative range of observers, accommodative errors were small and visual acuity normal. When comparing between illuminants, when accommodation was accurate, visual acuity was worst for blue narrowband light. This cannot be due to the sparser spacing for S-cones, as our stimuli had equal luminance and thus activated LM-cones roughly equally. It is likely because ocular LCA changes more rapidly at shorter wavelength and so the finite spectral bandwidth of LEDs corresponds to a greater dioptric range at shorter wavelengths. This effect disappears for larger accommodative errors, due to the increased depth of focus of the eye.
Thesis
Full-text available
(Title in Spanish: Estudio de la dinámica de la acomodación y las fluctuaciones del desenfoque en el ojo humano) The quality of the retinal image is the first, physical limiting factor of visual quality. Defocus is the most common source of blur leading to retinal image quality loss. It depends on the interrelationship between the eye's axial length, optical power, and distance to the object. Until it is lost with age, the eye has the ability to modify its optical power (i.e., to accommodate) to produce focused retinal images. Although this process is not instantaneous, accommodation is a fast and fairly accurate mechanism in most young subjects, that results in a clear vision. However, it has been suggested that myopia onset and/or progression may be related to alterations in the accommodative process that could upset the emmetropization process. On the other hand, even when steadily looking at an object at a fixed distance, the optical power of the eye fluctuates more or less randomly. It is unclear if this fluctuation is an undesired inability of the eye to keep a constant focus or may serve a purpose in the accommodative process. In any case, from an optical point of view, fast fluctuations of defocus would be expected to produce some kind of blurring in the retinal images. In those circumstances, a short integration time may allow the visual system to select the best focused position in the sequence to maximize visual quality. In this context, this thesis studies the effects of changes in focus, both discrete and progressive, aiming to discern how the visual system copes with them. Two separate experiments were carried out with an open-view Hartmann-Shack sensor measuring refraction and high-order aberrations in real time. First, the dynamics of the accommodative response was analyzed in realistic binocular viewing conditions, both for emmetropic subjects and myopes, when the fixation abruptly changed from far to near. In a second experiment, we studied the effect on contrast sensitivity of fast oscillations of defocus with different magnitudes and temporal frequencies, generated with a tunable lens attached to the system. During the accommodation mechanism, convergence of the eyeballs and miosis of the pupils accompany the change in optical power of the crystalline lens. There is extensive literature on these processes but relatively few studies simultaneously measuring all three of them in binocular vision. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study of their combined dynamics in real time under realistic viewing conditions. Furthermore, it was performed in both myopic and emmetropic young individuals. Eighteen young subjects participated in the first experiment, with an average refractive error of -2.3 D and a range from -7.5 D to 0 D. Cylinder was below 2 D in all cases. Excluding refractive errors, no subject had a history of visual problems and all of them reached 20/20 VA or better in both eyes. They were corrected during the measurements. The near stimulus, located at 2.8 D, and far target, at 0.36 D, were both black-on-white Maltese crosses with 1.3° width. Each subject underwent 3 cycles of 6 target switching (far-near-far-near-far-near). The data was analyzed with a threshold method consisting of calculating the initial and final states for each studied variable and considering the central 80% of the variation. Several far-to-near response parameters were calculated, including accommodation speed and amplitude, convergence speed and amplitude, pupil miosis speed, and amplitude, high-order aberration RMS, spherical aberration, lag of accommodation, and duration of accommodation, convergence, and pupil miosis. Correlation analysis between refractive error and accommodation speed and of these two variables with various far-to-near response parameters was performed. The correlation analysis of refraction (spherical equivalent, SE) with accommodation dynamics parameters suggests that myopia mildly affects or is affected by accommodation. The lag of accommodation was found to be linked to refractive error (R = -0.57, p = 0.01). Moreover, the correlation between miosis speed and refractive error also had a p-value below 0.05 (R = -0.49, p = 0.04). In other words, myopes may tend to have less precise accommodation and slower pupil constriction. The correlation coefficients between SE and the rest of accommodation-related parameters were small, with p-values well above 0.05. A substantial, low-p-value correlation was found between accommodation speed and convergence speed (R = 0.48, p = 0.04). To the best of our knowledge, this finding has not been previously reported. Furthermore, the correlation was stronger between accommodation speed and convergence duration (R = 0.57, p = 0.01), which may reflect the differences in the dynamics of these two processes. In addition, there may be a correlation between accommodation speed and miosis amplitude since the p-value was below 0.05 (R = 0.47, p = 0.049). These analyses showed that slower accommodation might be a function of slow convergence and more evident pupil miosis. For the second part of the thesis, a faster HS sensor with a refresh rate of 60 Hz and higher sensitivity to 1050 nm IR light was developed. This sensor was employed to characterize an optically tunable lens both in the typical static mode and, for the first time to our knowledge, in dynamic mode. After calibration, the tunable lens was used to apply defocus oscillations during contrast sensitivity measurements. Different amplitudes and frequencies were induced in 5 young emmetropes with 20/20 or better VA and no previous history of visual troubles. The visual stimulus was a 12 c/deg Gabor patch of 1º angular diameter located at 3 m. It was tilted 10 degrees left or right and a two-choice forced-choice protocol was used to determine the contrast threshold for each oscillation condition. The measurements were carried out in monocular mode, and the subjects viewed the stimulus through the tunable lens with their right eye. The sinusoidal waves induced included combinations of 3 temporal frequencies, 5, 15, and 25 Hz, and 8 peak-to-valley defocus values, ranging from 0.15 to 3 D, presented in fully random order. To the best of our knowledge, the effect of this kind of fast fluctuations of defocus on visual quality has not been previously studied. Visual performance, in the form of contrast threshold, was found resilient to induced defocus oscillations. The data showed that only for fast, large variations (25 Hz, ± 1.5 D), there was a noticeable reduction in contrast sensitivity. This indicates that for the eye to clearly perceive visual stimuli, the retinal image only needs to be in focus for a short time. A quantitative model was developed for predicting the deterioration in retinal image quality due to periodic defocus fluctuations. For the amplitudes and frequencies of oscillation used in the experiment, the average PSF was calculated for several integration times and the loss in the ensuing MTF was computed. Comparison between experimental results and simulated data suggests that the eye may be integrating defocus blur at 10 to 20 ms intervals. El proceso de la visión que finaliza con la sensación de ver algo, es decir, con la percepción de un estímulo visual, comienza con la formación de la imagen del mundo sobre la retina. Esto hace que, aunque pueden surgir múltiples complicaciones a distintos niveles que empeoren la calidad visual, la calidad de la imagen retiniana impone un primer límite físico a nuestra capacidad de distinguir detalles y extraer información de lo que vemos. El desenfoque es la causa más importante y a la vez más común de emborronamiento de la imagen en la retina. Esta borrosidad reduce la calidad de imagen, disminuyendo su resolución y contraste y haciendo que se pierdan detalles. La nitidez de la imagen depende de la interrelación entre la longitud axial del ojo, su potencia óptica y la distancia al objeto. Hasta que se vuelve rígido con la edad, el ojo es capaz de modificar la forma del cristalino para ajustar su potencia óptica, un proceso que se denomina acomodación. De esta forma el sistema visual puede enfocar sobre la retina las imágenes de objetos situados a distintas distancias. Aunque este proceso no es instantáneo, la acomodación es un mecanismo rápido y bastante preciso en la mayoría de los sujetos jóvenes, que da como resultado una visión clara del mundo tridimensional. Sin embargo, se ha sugerido que la aparición y/o progresión de la miopía podría estar relacionada con alteraciones en el proceso acomodativo que podrían alterar el proceso de emetropización. Por otro lado, incluso cuando se mira fijamente un objeto a una distancia determinada, la potencia óptica del ojo fluctúa de forma más o menos aleatoria. No está claro si esta inestabilidad es un resultado no deseado de la incapacidad del ojo para mantener un enfoque constante o puede ser parte integrante del proceso de acomodación con un propósito concreto, por ejemplo, aumentando la velocidad de respuesta. Sea como fuere, considerando el fenómeno desde un punto de vista óptico, sería de esperar que la imagen retiniana sufriera algún tipo de degradación al verse sometida a una fluctuación de foco, perdiendo nitidez. En esas circunstancias, un tiempo de integración corto podría permitir que el sistema visual percibiera una secuencia instantáneas con distintos niveles de emborronamiento, y que empleara la más nítida para extraer información sobre el objeto, maximizando la calidad visual. En este contexto, esta tesis estudia los efectos de distintos tipos de variaciones de enfoque, tanto discretas como progresivas, con el objetivo de analizar cómo son manejados por el sistema visual. Para ello se llevaron a cabo dos experimentos separados empleando un sensor Hartmann-Shack (HS) de campo abierto, que mide tanto la refracción como las aberraciones de alto orden de ambos ojos en tiempo real. En primer lugar, se analizó la dinámica de la respuesta acomodativa en condiciones realistas de visión binocular, tanto en sujetos emétropes como miopes, cuando cambiaban su fijación abruptamente de un objeto lejano a otro cercano. Posteriormente, en un segundo experimento estudiamos el efecto sobre la sensibilidad al contraste de oscilaciones rápidas de desenfoque con diferentes amplitudes y frecuencias temporales, generadas con una lente sintonizable acoplada al sistema para este propósito. En el primer experimento, se midió la dinámica en tiempo real de las tres componentes de la respuesta acomodativa binocular (acomodación, convergencia y miosis pupilar) en emétropes y sujetos con distintos grados de miopía. El mecanismo de acomodación no solo conlleva el cambio de potencia óptica del cristalino sino que además incluye una rotación coordinada de los globos oculares para hacer que las líneas de mirada converjan sobre el objeto observado y una reducción del tamaño (miosis) de las pupilas. Existe una extensa literatura sobre estos procesos y sus combinaciones, pero relativamente pocos estudios los miden simultáneamente en visión binocular. Hasta donde sabemos, este es el primer estudio de la dinámica combinada de las tres componentes de la respuesta acomodativa se mide de forma precisa, en tiempo real y en condiciones realistas de observación. Además, se realizaron medidas tanto en miopes como emétropes, con el objeto de analizar las posibles diferencias de comportamiento entre ellos. En el estudio participaron 18 sujetos jóvenes, con un error refractivo promedio de -2.3 D en un rango de 0 D a -7.5 D, todos ellos con valores de cilindro por debajo de 2 D. Con su mejor corrección, que portaron durante las medidas en caso necesario, todos los sujetos alcanzaron una agudeza visual decimal por encima de la unidad y ninguno presentó antecedentes de enfermedades oculares o problemas visuales. Tanto el estímulo cercano, ubicado a 2.8 D, como el lejano, a 0.36 D, consistieron en cruces de Malta negras sobre un fondo blanco, con una anchura angular de 1.3° en ambos casos. Cada sujeto se sometió a 3 ciclos de 6 cambios de distancia de fijación (lejos-cerca-lejos-cerca-lejos-cerca). Para el análisis de datos se empleó un método de umbralización consistente determinar los instantes en los que se alcanza el 10% y se supera el 90% del rango de variación entre los estados inicial y final para cada variable estudiada. A partir de los datos experimentales se calcularon varios parámetros relacionados con el cambio de fijación de lejos a cerca, incluyendo las amplitudes de acomodación, convergencia y miosis pupilar, las duraciones de estos tres procesos y sus velocidades medias, el retraso acomodativo, el RMS total de las aberraciones de alto orden y la magnitud de la aberración esférica, y se analizaron los coeficientes de correlación de todas estas variables con el error refractivo y con la velocidad de acomodación. El análisis de correlación de la refracción (en forma de equivalente esférico) con los parámetros dinámicos de la respuesta acomodativa sugiere que la miopía afecta levemente o se ve afectada por la acomodación. Se encontró una relación de proporcionalidad entre el retraso acomodativo y el error refractivo (R = -0.57, p = 0.01), así como una correlación apreciable entre la velocidad de constricción pupilar y la refracción del sujeto (R = -0.49, p = 0.04), en ambos casos con valores de p por debajo de 0.05. En otras palabras, la acomodación en los miopes parece tender a ser menos precisa y la contracción de su pupila a ser más lenta. Los coeficientes de correlación entre el equivalente esférico y el resto de parámetros relacionados con la respuesta acomodativa fueron en general pequeños, con valores de p muy por encima de 0.05. Por otro lado, se encontró una correlación sustancial, con valor de p bajo, entre la velocidad de acomodación y la velocidad de convergencia (R = 0.48, p = 0.04), una asociación que no hemos encontrado mencionada en la literatura previa. Y todavía más fuerte fue la correlación la velocidad de acomodación y la duración de la convergencia (R = 0.57, p = 0.01), lo que puede reflejar las diferencias entre las dinámicas de estos dos procesos. Además, parece haber una correlación entre la velocidad de acomodación y la amplitud de la miosis pupilar, ya que el valor de p fue inferior a 0.05 (R = 0.47, p = 0.049). En resumen, el análisis de correlación de la velocidad de acomodación sugiere que una acomodación lenta puede estar asociada a una convergencia lente y a una constricción pupilar más evidente. Para la segunda parte de esta tesis se construyó un sensor HS más rápido, con una frecuencia de actualización de 60 Hz, y con mayor sensibilidad a la luz infrarroja de 1050 nm de longitud de onda. Este sensor se empleó para caracterizar una lente sintonizable, no solo para la producción en modo estático de valores fijos de desenfoque sino también, por primera vez que sepamos, en modo dinámico para la generación de variaciones sinusoidales de desenfoque. Después de la calibración, la lente sintonizable se utilizó para provocar oscilaciones periódicas de desenfoque, de distintas amplitudes y frecuencias, a 5 jóvenes emétropes mientras realizaban una tarea de sensibilidad al contraste. Todos los sujetos alcanzaron agudeza visual unidad o mejor y carecían de antecedentes de problemas visuales. El estímulo visual fue un test de Gabor de 1º de diámetro angular y una frecuencia espacial de 12 c/grado, ubicado a 3 m de distancia del observador. Las franjas se inclinaron aleatoriamente 10º hacia la izquierda o la derecha de la vertical y se utilizó un protocolo de elección forzada de dos opciones para determinar el umbral de contraste para cada condición de oscilación. Las medidas se tomaron monocularmente con el ojo derecho. En total se estudiaron 24 casos de fluctuación sinusoidal de desenfoque presentados en orden aleatorio, correspondientes a las combinaciones de 3 frecuencias temporales (5, 15 y 25 Hz) y 8 valores de pico-valle de desenfoque entre un mínimo de 0,15 D y 3 D de máximo. No se ha encontrado literatura previa analizando el efecto de este tipo de fluctuaciones rápidas de desenfoque en la calidad visual. Los resultados de este estudio indican que la calidad visual, cuantificada mediante el umbral de contraste, es resistente a las oscilaciones de desenfoque inducidas. Solo se encontró una reducción notable de la sensibilidad al contraste para las variaciones más grandes y rápidas (25 Hz, ± 1,5 D) consideradas. Este hecho se puede tomar como una indicación de que el ojo humano solo necesita que la imagen retiniana esté enfocada durante un periodo corto de tiempo para poder percibir claramente los estímulos visuales. Como complemento a las medidas experimentales, en la última sección de esta tesis se desarrolló un modelo cuantitativo para predecir el deterioro en la calidad de imagen retiniana que puede producir una fluctuación periódica de desenfoque. Para las amplitudes y frecuencias de oscilación utilizadas en el experimento, se calculó la PSF promedio para varios tiempos de integración. A partir de ellas se evaluó la MTF compuesta para 12 c/grado y se comparó con la MTF limitada por difracción a dicha frecuencia, con el objeto de determinar la pérdida de modulación inducida por las variaciones de enfoque. La comparación entre los resultados experimentales y los datos simulados sugiere que el ojo puede estar integrando el emborronamiento causado por desenfoque en intervalos de 10 a 20 ms, y que una instantánea más o menos enfocada en una secuencia muy emborronada resulta suficiente para que el sistema visual extraiga información relevante para percibir el estímulo.
Chapter
An understanding of equine vision allows the clinician to provide a more accurate prognosis prior to initiating therapy and to more intelligently select from a range of potential therapeutic options for a particular disorder to maximize the probability of optimally preserving the eye's visual capabilities. This chapter provides the clinician with a review of the normal visual abilities of the horse and describes how select ocular abnormalities may alter the animal's vision and behavior. The most critical aspect of vision, however, is the ability to identify an object (a wolf, for example) as separate from its surroundings (dense vegetation). Because this distinction is so important for survival, animals (including humans) with normal vision can “see” an object if it differs sufficiently from its surroundings in any one of six different aspects: luminance (brightness), motion, depth, texture (which is related to visual acuity), orientation, or color.
Article
Accommodative responses of humans operate seamlessly to ensure clear vision of targets at different viewing distances, up until the onset of presbyopia. To achieve this, the visual system must correctly decipher the polarity and magnitude of retinal defocus in real-time, and often under very challenging viewing conditions. The past seven decades of research in this area has identified several retinotopic cues that may potentially provide the desired odd- and even-error information to the visual system for solving these challenges. These studies have used a variety of technology, experimental paradigms and outcome measures to determine the putative contribution of a given cue, or set of cues, in solving this problem. A variety of results, some offering consensus and others conflicting, have been observed in these studies. The present review distils this large volume of literature into specific, take-away points for the early reader of this topic, acknowledging that the problem is non-trivial and far from being solved. The review also reveals that many of these studies may not have used appropriate/sensitive methodology or outcome measures to tease apart the relative contribution of a cue in solving the direction and magnitude challenge. The review concludes with the proposal that, since a multitude of cues may be used by the visual system for solving these problems, future studies could employ a Bayesian statistical cue-combination approach to address this problem. Such approaches have yielded very meaningful insights in other areas of human decision-making involving multiple inter- and intra-modal combination of cues.
Article
In broadband light, longitudinal chromatic aberration (LCA) provides emmetropization signals from both wavelength defocus and the resulting chromatic cues. Indoor illuminants vary in their spectral output, potentially limiting the signals from LCA. Our aim is to investigate the effect that artificial illuminants with different spectral outputs have on chick emmetropization with and without low temporal frequency modulation. In Experiment 1, two-week-old chicks were exposed to 0.2 Hz, square-wave luminance modulation for 3 days. There were 4 spectral conditions: LED strips that simulated General Electric (GE) LED “Soft” (n = 13), GE LED “Daylight” (n = 12), a novel “Equal” condition (n = 12), and a novel “High S” condition (n = 10). These conditions were all tested at a mean level of 985 lux. In Experiment 2, the effect of intensity on the “Equal” condition was tested at two other light levels (70 lux: n = 10; 680 lux: n = 7). In Experiment 3, the effect of temporal modulation on the “Equal” condition was tested by comparing the 0.2 Hz condition with 0 Hz (steady). Significant differences were found in axial growth across lighting conditions. At 985 lux, birds exposed to the “Equal” condition at showed a greater reduction in axial growth (both p < 0.01) and a greater hyperopic shift compared to “Soft” and “Daylight” (both p < 0.01). The “High S” birds experienced more axial growth compared to “Equal” (p < 0.01) but less than in “Soft” and “Daylight” (p < 0.01). Axial changes in “Equal” were only observed at 985 lux with 0.2 Hz temporal modulation, and not with lower light levels or steady light. We conclude that axial growth and refraction were dependent on the lighting condition in a manner predicted by wavelength defocus signals arising from LCA.
Article
Full-text available
The focusing response of the human eye - accommodation - exhibits errors known as lags and leads. Lags occur when the stimulus is near and the eye appears to focus farther than the stimulus. Leads occur with far stimuli where the eye appears to focus nearer than the stimulus. We used objective and subjective measures simultaneously to determine where the eye is best focused. The objective measures were made with a wavefront sensor and an autorefractor, both of which analyze light reflected from the retina. These measures exhibited typical accommodative errors, mostly lags. The subjective measure was visual acuity, which of course depends not only on the eye's optics but also on photoreception and neural processing of the retinal image. The subjective measure revealed much smaller errors. Acuity was maximized at or very close to the distance of the accommodative stimulus. Thus, accommodation is accurate in terms of maximizing visual performance.
Article
Full-text available
Young observers were challenged to induce a marked monocular accommodative response to a relatively weak accommodative stimulus by placing a-9 diopter contact lens on the eye. At first, observers could not produce the desired response, but with training, three of four subjects achieved criterion. Both a voluntary accommodative response and a response to an adequate accommodative stimulus were apparently involved. The voluntary component of the response could be demonstrated by having the observers repeat the task in total darkness.
Book
The present volume provides a concise up-to-date survey of the whole field of solar radio physics. It is intended to represent the elementary knowledge about solar radio astronomy and to cover a wide range of interests for scientists from various fields. The book is divided into five chapters. Chapter I contains a brief general introduction to the history of solar radio astronomy and some fundamentals of astronomy and solar physics necessary for an understanding of solar radio physics. Chapter II presents some topics of the instrumental background of solar radio astronomy, and Chapter III gives the main results of observations. Chapter IV comprises the elements of a theoretical interpretation of solar radio observations. Chapter V outlines a synthesis of both observation and theory contributing to a general picture of solar and solar-terrestrial physics.
Article
An optometer (infrared recording retinoscope) that provides high resolution measurements of accommodative changes of the eye has been developed. The instrument employs the principle of retinoscopy to monitor changes in the refractive state of the eye. Infrared radiation is scanned across the eye to provide a moving retinal source as in retinoscopy. An electro-optical system replaces the observer in determining the direction and amount of apparent motion in the pupil of the patient. The optometer is sensitive to accommodative changes of 0.12 D over a range of 12 D. The response of the instrument is faster than the most rapid changes of accommodation, and measurements are unaffected by pupil changes or by small eye movements.
Article
Modern angular-momentum techniques are used to study a few refined aspects of the conventional Raman effect—aspects not easily accessible in the old days of Placzek, who used mainly direction cosines and circular coordinates. The intensity at an arbitrary direction of observation (specified by three instead of one Euler angles, øs, θs, ψs), of scattering by random molecular systems, has been derived. In the limit of øs=ψs=0, θs=π/2-Θs=Φs, it reduces to that of Placzek. The intensity of the Rayleigh and Raman scattering by uniformly oriented molecules (in solid matrices or in molecular crystals) has also been derived. It is illustrated for oriented molecules with cylindrical symmetry. The Raman scattering by rotating linear molecules in arbitrary electronic states has been investigated. The general formulas derived are usable also in electronic Raman effects.
Article
Article
Using a simple model eye with a wavelength-dependent diffraction, a wavelength-dependent refractive error (chromatic difference in refractive error), and a wavelength-dependent displacement of the foveal images (transverse chromatic aberration), we have evaluated the luminance modulations in retinal images of isoluminant color gratings. In cases where the chromatic difference in refractive error has been corrected, the retinal image suffers from chromatic parallax, which creates wavelength-dependent displacements of the retinal image that are similar to those caused by transverse chromatic aberration. Our calculations show that all three chromatic aberrations can introduce luminance modulations in the retinal images of isoluminant gratings. These luminance artifacts generally, but not always, increase with increasing spatial frequency. The contrast in the luminance artifact depends critically on the exact refractive error in the uncorrected eye and the precise position of the eye in the corrected case. Wavelength-dependent diffraction has little effect for large pupils (e.g., 5 mm) but can become a significant factor with small pupils. Luminance artifacts created by chromatic aberrations can be more detectable than the original color contrasts at spatial frequencies above 3 cycles/deg.