ArticlePublisher preview available

Conditioned Brain-Stimulation Reward Attenuates the Acoustic Startle Reflex in Rats

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

The acoustic startle reflex (ASR) in rats is attenuated by a light paired with food or, in humans, by "pleasant" pictures. Rats were trained to barpress for lateral hypothalamus (LH) stimulation. ASR amplitudes were then measured at 4 intensities, with or without a light. Control rats that did not receive brain-stimulation reward (BSR) showed initially lower ASR amplitudes than did rats exposed to BSR, but both groups responded similarly with or without light. Next, experimental rats were given BSR in the presence of light but not in its absence. After conditioning, ASR amplitudes were reduced, and ASR thresholds were raised by a mean of 2.6 dB in the light but remained at preconditioning levels without light. No such change was found for control rats or rats with placements outside the LH.
Behavioral
Neuroscience
2001,
Vol.
115,
No. 3,
710-717
Copyright
2001
by the
American
Psychological Association, Inc.
0735-7044/01/S5.00
DOI:
10.1037//0735-7044.115.3.710
Conditioned Brain-Stimulation Reward Attenuates
the
Acoustic
Startle Reflex
in
Rats
Stephan
Steidl
University
of
Toronto
Liang
Li
Peking
University
John
S.
Yeomans
University
of
Toronto
The
acoustic startle reflex (ASR)
in
rats
is
attenuated
by a
light paired with food
or, in
humans,
by
"pleasant"
pictures. Rats were trained
to
barpress
for
lateral hypothalamus (LH) stimulation.
ASR
amplitudes
were then measured
at 4
intensities,
with
or
without
a
light. Control rats that
did not
receive
brain-stimulation
reward (BSR) showed
initially
lower
ASR
amplitudes
than
did
rats exposed
to
BSR,
but
both groups responded
similarly
with
or
without light. Next, experimental rats were given
BSR in the
presence
of
light
but not in its
absence.
After
conditioning,
ASR
amplitudes were reduced,
and ASR
thresholds
were raised
by a
mean
of 2.6 dB in the
light
but
remained
at
preconditioning levels
without
light.
No
such change
was
found
for
control
rats
or
rats with placements outside
the LH.
The
acoustic startle reflex (ASR) involves
a
rapid contraction
of
skeletal
muscles throughout
the
body after
an
unexpected
and
intense
acoustic stimulus.
The
magnitude
of the ASR is
modulated
by
changes
in the
perceptual
and
emotional state
of the
animal.
In
the
fear-potentiated startle paradigm,
the ASR is
enhanced
in the
presence
of a cue
predicting
an
aversive footshock (Brown, Kalish,
&
Farber, 1951; Davis,
1989).
The
amplitude
of the
eyeblink
component
of ASR in
humans
is
enhanced
in the
presence
of
emotionally
aversive pictures compared with emotionally neutral
pictures
(Lang, Bradley,
&
Cuthbert,
1990). Humans with post-
traumatic stress disorder show elevated startle responses (Butler
et
al.,
1990).
Startle provides
a
sensitive measure
of
emotional states,
variously
called "fear,"
"panic,"
"stress,"
or
"anxiety,"
that
is
useful
in
evaluating
the
effects
of
drugs
and in
studying neural
circuits
for
these states (Davis, Falls, Campeau,
&
Kim, 1993;
Frankland,
Josselyn, Bradwejn,
Vaccarino,
&
Yeomans, 1997;
Walker
&
Davis, 1997).
The
amplitude
of the
eyeblink component
of ASR in
humans
is
attenuated
in the
presence
of
pictures
described
as
emotionally
positive
(Lang
et
al.,
1990).
Of
most relevance
to the
present study,
ASR in
rats
was
reduced
in the
presence
of a
light that
had
previously
been paired with food (Schmid, Koch,
&
Schnitzler,
Stephan
Steidl
and
John
S.
Yeomans, Department
of
Psychology, Uni-
versity
of
Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Liang
Li,
Department
of
Psychology,
Peking
University,
Peking,
People's
Republic
of
China.
This
research
was
supported
by
National Sciences
and
Engineering
Research Council
of
Canada Grant
A
7077
and
Medical Research Council
of
Canada Grant MT14624.
We
thank David Bush
and
James Fulton
for
technical
assistance.
Correspondence concerning this article should
be
addressed
to
John
S.
Yeomans, Department
of
Psychology, University
of
Toronto,
100 St.
George Street, Toronto, Ontario
M5S
3G3, Canada. Electronic mail
may be
sent
to
yeomans@psych.utoronto.ca.
1995). These
effects
have been called "pleasure-attenuated star-
tle."
In the
latter study, baseline
ASR
amplitudes were obtained
without
a
light
cue
from food-
and
water-deprived rats. Rats
in the
conditioned
group were trained
to
associate
a
neutral stimulus
(conditioned
stimulus [CS], light) with food (unconditioned stim-
ulus).
Conditioning took place
in a
dark chamber
in
which food
was
presented during
a
4-min
period with
the
light turned
on.
Control rats spent
the
same amount
of
time
in
their home
cages,
receiving
the
same amount
of
food
at
irregular points
in
time.
No
trials without
a
light
cue
(CS—)
were presented after conditioning,
and
conditioning took place
in a
different
context than subsequent
testing.
ASR was
significantly attenuated
in the
presence
of
con-
stant
light
in the
conditioned rats compared with preconditioning
baselines, whereas controls showed
no
significant difference ver-
sus
baseline.
The
main goal
of the
present study
was to
extend
the
phenom-
enon
of
reward-attenuated startle from conditioned food reward
to
conditioned brain-stimulation reward (BSR). Rats prefer
BSR
over
food
in
some conditions, suggesting that
BSR can be
more reward-
ing
(Routtenberg
&
Lindy, 1965).
No
deprivation
is
required
in
BSR
experiments. Controls were added
to (a)
evaluate
the
uncon-
ditioned effects
of
light,
(b)
control
the
environment within
and
between groups,
and (c)
estimate
ASR
sensitivity changes result-
ing
from
the
conditioned reward
by the
shift
in
decibel
threshold.
Method
Subjects
Thirteen
male Wistar rats
(Rattus
norvegicus) weighing
a
mean
of 350 g,
obtained
from
Charles River Canada (St. Constant, Quebec), were individ-
ually
housed
on a
12-hr
light-dark
cycle.
Food
and
water were available
in
the
home cages throughout
the
experiment.
The
University
of
Toronto
Animal
Care Committee, following
the
guidelines
of the
Canadian Council
of
Animal
Care,
approved
the
care
of
rats used
in
this study.
710
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Conversely, startle magnitude is often attenuated in people exposed to pleasant pictures 27,29 , sounds 41 , or conditioned visual stimuli paired with the removal of a painful stimulus and hence 'relief ' 42 , and in rodents exposed to conditioned visual or olfactory stimuli associated with rewards such as food [43][44][45][46] and rewarding brain stimulation 47 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Affective states are key determinants of animal welfare. Assessing such states under field conditions is thus an important goal in animal welfare science. The rapid Defence Cascade (DC) response (startle, freeze) to sudden unexpected stimuli is a potential indicator of animal affect; humans and rodents in negative affective states often show potentiated startle magnitude and freeze duration. To be a practical field welfare indicator, quick and easy measurement is necessary. Here we evaluate whether DC responses can be quantified in pigs using computer vision. 280 video clips of induced DC responses made by 12 pigs were analysed by eye to provide ‘ground truth’ measures of startle magnitude and freeze duration which were also estimated by (i) sparse feature tracking computer vision image analysis of 200 Hz video, (ii) load platform, (iii) Kinect depth camera, and (iv) Kinematic data. Image analysis data strongly predicted ground truth measures and were strongly positively correlated with these and all other estimates of DC responses. Characteristics of the DC-inducing stimulus, pig orientation relative to it, and ‘relaxed-tense’ pig behaviour prior to it moderated DC responses. Computer vision image analysis thus offers a practical approach to measuring pig DC responses, and potentially pig affect and welfare, under field conditions.
... The ASR is modulated by a number of factors, including the individual's arousal and affective state [1][2][3][4] . In rodents, ASR can be modified in response to emotional situations, with the startle magnitude being increased by fearful/threatening stimuli (also referred to as fear-potentiated startle (FPS)) 3,5-8 and attenuated by threat-reducing or joyful/rewarding stimuli 3,7,[9][10][11] . Because these ASR modulations can be observed in both humans and rodents, the ASR provides an excellent translational research tool to investigate symptoms characterizing neuropsychiatric disorders 6,7,12,13 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Accurate discrimination between danger and safety cues is essential for survival. Recent findings in humans indicate that patients suffering from anxiety disorders cannot reliably use safety cues in order to inhibit fear responses. However, the neuroanatomical pathways of conditioned safety are still unclear. Aim of the present study was to investigate whether chronic inhibition of GABA synthesis in the infralimbic (IL) cortex, a critical region for fear inhibition, would lead to enhanced conditioned safety memory. Male Sprague Dawley rats were equipped with osmotic mini-pumps attached to an infusion cannula aimed at the IL. Mini-pumps were either filled with the glutamate decarboxylase (GAD) inhibitor l-allylglycine (l-AG) or the inactive enantiomer d-allylglycine (d-AG). Previous studies demonstrated that chronic infusions of l-AG lead to lower GABA levels and overall enhanced neural activity. The effect of IL disinhibition on conditioned safety was investigated utilizing the acoustic startle response. Chronic disinhibition of the IL facilitated conditioned safety memory, along with reduced contextual fear and lower corticosterone levels. The present findings suggest that the IL is a key brain region for conditioned safety memory. Because anxiety disorder patients are often not capable to use safety cues to inhibit unnecessary fear responses, the present findings are of clinical relevance and could potentially contribute to therapy optimization.
... As in our previous studies focused on neural mechanisms of relief learning (Andreatta et al., 2012;Bruning et al., 2016;Kahl and Fendt, 2016;Mohammadi et al., 2014;Mohammadi and Fendt, 2015), we used the acoustic startle response to measure conditioned relief. The startle response is a translational paradigm that is sensitive to the emotional state of the test subject (Koch, 1999), i.e. the startle magnitude is robustly potentiated by unconditioned and conditioned aversive stimuli (Davis et al., 1993;Fendt and Fanselow, 1999) and attenuated by unconditioned and conditioned rewarding stimuli (Schmid et al., 1995;Steidl et al., 2001). As a to-be-learned stimulus, a visual cue was used. ...
... It is particularly important in the current context that the startle response has a nonzero baseline; that is, both increases and decreases in startle amplitude can be measured! Indeed, after associating a cue with a reward such as food or rewarding brain stimulation, the amplitude of the startle response is decreased in the presence of the cue (Schmid et al. 1995;Koch et al. 1996;Steidl et al. 2001;Schneider and Spanagel 2008). Thus, as in the case already discussed for flies, the paradigm of startle modulation is bivalent: Under neutral conditions startle is normal, while when expecting something bad startle is potentiated, and when expecting something good startle is attenuated. ...
Article
Full-text available
Memories relating to a painful, negative event are adaptive and can be stored for a lifetime to support preemptive avoidance, escape, or attack behavior. However, under unfavorable circumstances such memories can become overwhelmingly powerful. They may trigger excessively negative psychological states and uncontrollable avoidance of locations, objects, or social interactions. It is therefore obvious that any process to counteract such effects will be of value. In this context, we stress from a basic-research perspective that painful, negative events are "Janus-faced" in the sense that there are actually two aspects about them that are worth remembering: What made them happen and what made them cease. We review published findings from fruit flies, rats, and man showing that both aspects, respectively related to the onset and the offset of the negative event, induce distinct and oppositely valenced memories: Stimuli experienced before an electric shock acquire negative valence as they signal upcoming punishment, whereas stimuli experienced after an electric shock acquire positive valence because of their association with the relieving cessation of pain. We discuss how memories for such punishment- and relief-learning are organized, how this organization fits into the threat-imminence model of defensive behavior, and what perspectives these considerations offer for applied psychology in the context of trauma, panic, and nonsuicidal self-injury.
... Habituation and sensitization of the ASR are useful for the investigation of the neuronal basis of non-associative learning (Davis and File 1984). Reward expectation has been shown to attenuate the ASR in rats (Koch et al. 1996;Schneider and Spanagel 2008;Steidl et al. 2001), which can be used to model the hedonic reduction of the ASR magnitude in humans (Lang 1995). ...
Article
The startle is a relatively simple ubiquitous reflex. Interestingly, it has a "non-zero baseline", i.e., its magnitude can be reduced or enhanced. We reflect here on the translational value of prepulse inhibition and fear-related potentiation as endophenotypes that can be used for the investigation of complex psychiatric diseases such as schizophrenia and anxiety-related disorders. Our main conclusions are that both phenomena of startle modulation are useful tools for basic research in investigating the genetic and/or neurobiological basis of certain aspects of these disorders. Because of their stable and robust nature, however, both biomarkers are of limited use for predicting the occurrence of diseases in high-risk people or for predicting the course of an illness.
... Estos resultados, que sugieren que las mujeres con bulimia nerviosa experimentan menos placer y control sobre la comida y los impulsos sexuales que las mujeres sanas, podrían explicarse si tenemos en cuenta que tanto la comida como la sexualidad son actividades controladas por el mismo circuito cerebral. Según han propuesto algunos autores, hay dos circuitos motivacionales primarios en el cerebro: el apetitivo y el defensivo (Lang, Davis, y Öhman, 2000;Steidl y Yeomans, 2001). El circuito apetitivo está controlado por el sistema mesolímbico dopaminérgico (núcleo acumbens) y su activación afecta a conductas de aproximación y de ingesta. ...
Article
Full-text available
Lack of self-control is an essential fea-ture of bulimia nervosa; so much so that impulsiveness has been associated with neuropsychological and physiological alterations in these patients. In this paper we review articles on this subject that confirm that the existence of a decrease of inhibitory control explains the lack of self-control in behaviors con-cerning food or sexual activity. Patients with bulimia nervosa succumb easily to an intense desire for food and experience frequent gluttonous behav-ior. Similarly, sexual activity increases and becomes uncontrollable. Both food and sexual behaviors seem to have as a main function the momentary relief of negative emotions. The literature, in addition, shows that these highly destructive behaviors reflect the exis-tence of a very low control of impulsive-IMPULSIVIDAD EN
... Interestingly, in line with aforementioned findings of disrupted affective startle modulation in depressed patients, a recent study showed blunted light-enhanced startle in female rats subjected to early maternal deprivation, a paradigm proposed to model early life adversity [134]. Next to paradigms measuring emotional responses to aversive stimuli, some attempts have been made to develop startle paradigms measuring the emotional response to pleasant stimuli, the so-called pleasure-attenuated startle paradigm [135][136][137][138]. In a paradigm developed by Schmid and Koch, rats were conditioned to associate a house light with the availability of palatable food [135,136]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Major depression and anxiety disorders are the most prevalent psychiatric disorders. The high co-morbidity and strong overlap in symptoms suggest that neurobiological mechanisms may also overlap. Two neuromodulators have received much attention. First, serotonin: Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, which target the serotonin system, are currently pharmacological treatment of choice for both depression and anxiety. Second, corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF), which is a key regulator in the neuroendocrine and behavioural responses to stress., is suggested to play a role in the development of psychiatric illness following chronic stress This thesis focused on one characteristic, which is evident in both depression and anxiety, namely disturbed affect regulation. The acoustic startle response was used to study affect regulation, as this response can be modulated by emotional stimuli: pleasant stimuli attenuate the startle response, whereas aversive stimuli potentiate the startle response. In psychiatric patients, changes in affective startle modulation have already been shown. However, how changes in affective state influence affective startle modulation in rodents has hardly been studied. Therefore, the aims of this thesis were to 1. Evaluate how changes in affective startle influence affective startle modulation in rodents. And 2. Further study the involvement of corticotropin-releasing factor and serotonin in affect regulation. The data presented in this thesis shows that affective startle modulation shows high translational value. Similar to what is seen in depressed patients, emotional numbing is reflected in blunted affective startle modulation in rodent models for affective disorders. In addition, a negative affective state results in exacerbation of specific forms of aversive startle modulation, depending on the manipulation used to induce this negative affective state. A positive affective state, on the other hand, results in attenuation of aversive startle modulation. In this thesis, both a pharmacological and genetic approach were used to modulate CRF and serotonin signalling. The data further underlined the involvement of both neuromodulators in affect regulation first of all, repeated local corticotropin-releasing factor infusion resulted in exacerbation of different types of negative affect (fear or anxiety) later in life, depending on the brain area targeted. This suggests that individual differences in sensitivity of certain brain areas to the effects of CRF, may determine which systems are affected by chronic stress and, consequently, which psychiatric symptoms develop. In addition, deletion of the serotonin transporter resulted in maladaptive fear responses, similar to those seen in panic disorder patients., suggesting that the serotonin transporter may be important in symptom exacerbation in these patients. The high translational value of affective startle models makes these models very useful in studying the brain systems involved in the regulation of affective state. In addition, the studies described in this thesis show that corticotropin-releasing factor and serotonin play an important role in affect regulation
Chapter
This chapter will present evidence to suggest that emotion is best defined, not as a single reaction, but as a process: Emotion involves multiple responses, organized according to temporal and spatial parameters. Thus, events that are positive/appetitive or aversive/threatening engage attention. They prompt information gathering, and do so more than other less motivationally relevant stimuli. Motive cues also occasion metabolic arousal, anticipatory responses that are oriented towards the engaging event, a mobilization of the organism that can lead to some action. Emotional processing can be compressed into fractions of a second or considerably extended in time. We suggest that neural networks underlying emotion include direct connections to the brain's primary motivational systems, appetitive and defensive. These neural circuits were laid down early in our evolutionary history, in primitive cortex, subcortex, and midbrain, to mediate behaviors basic to the survival of individuals and species. Unconditioned and conditioned appetitive and aversive stimuli activate these motivational circuits. They determine the general mobilization of the organism, the deployment of reflexive attentional, approach, and defensive behaviors, and mediate the formation of conditioned associations originally based on primary reinforcement.
Chapter
Full-text available
In this chapter, we discuss a new method for assessing attention and affect that relies on the measurement of a simple reflex that occurs in all mammalian species — the startle reflex. The abrupt presentation of a sensory stimulus— for example, a burst of noise or a flash of light — elicits a reflexive startle response. The gross features of startle in humans include a forward thrusting of the head and a descending flexor wave reaction that extends through the trunk to the knees. The first, fastest, and most stable element in the startle sequence is the reflexive eyeblink, occurring within 30–80 ms after stimulus onset, which is a reliable motor response that is easy both to measure and to quantify.
Article
In order to evaluate the influence of changes in affective state on light-enhanced startle, the effects of positive affect, induced by acute cocaine administration, and the effect of negative affect, induced by spontaneous cocaine withdrawal-induced anxiety, were studied. Acute cocaine administration decreased LES, whereas withdrawal from chronic cocaine administration exacerbated LES 24h after withdrawal, an effect indicative of increased anxiety. This exacerbated LES was reduced, but not back to normal, 4 days after withdrawal. The finding that both cocaine-induced positive and negative affect can be detected in LES, suggests that this may be a valuable tool in studying affect regulation in rodents.
Article
Full-text available
Prepulse inhibition (PPI) of startle is impaired in schizophrenics, which suggests they have disturbances in circuitry that controls PPI. How activity in forebrain circuitry is communicated to the primary startle circuit to modulate PPI was explored. Subpallidal cells innervate the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPTg). Infusion of the γ-aminobutyric acid antagonist picrotoxin into the subpallidum impaired PPI. In other rats, electrolytic PPTg lesions decreased or eliminated PPI, potentiated startle amplitude, and did not alter habituation. The disruption of PPI correlated significantly with the extent of PPTg damage. PPTg lesions reduced PPI when startle stimuli were weak or intense (104 or 120 dB) and when prepulse stimuli ranged from 2 to 17 dB above background but were most profound with prepulses 5–8 dB above background. The PPTg modulates sensorimotor gating and may process and transmit information from forebrain structures to the primary startle circuit.
Article
Full-text available
The startle threshold of the albino Sprague-Dawley rat runs parallel to the curve of the hearing threshold. The difference between the startle and hearing threshold is 87 dB (SPL) at a background noise level of 75 dB (SPL). At 110 dB (SPL), the threshold has a range from 2 kHz to 50 kHz with a minimum at 10 kHz and a second minimum at 40 kHz. Amplitude and latency of the startle response are not only dependent on the sensation level of the acoustic stimulus but also on the frequency. At threshold, only the head movement component of the startle response is elicited.
Article
Cholinergic neurons of the pedunculopontine nucleus (Ch5) are believed to monosynaptically excite ventral tegmental dopamine neurons. Muscarinic blockers injected near dopamine cells block the rewarding effect of hypothalamic or dorsal tegmental rewarding brain stimulation (RBS) in rats. Because Ch5 cells are inhibited by muscarinic agonists, we injected muscarinic drugs unilaterally near Ch5 neurons to inhibit or disinhibit them. Carbachol raised thresholds for hypothalamic self-stimulation bilaterally by over 400%, whereas scopolamine reduced thresholds by 20%-80%. Pretreatment with either carbachol or scopolamine blocked the effect of the other drug, which suggests that both acted through the same receptors near Ch5 cells
Article
Article
Bilateral microinjections of the neuroleptic, cis-flupenthixol, were made into 56 forebrain targets distributed across various dopamine (DA) terminal fields in the forebrain. Drug effects on medial forebrain bundle (MFB) stimulation-produced reward were assessed with a rate-frequency procedure implemented in a runway paradigm in a discrete-trial fashion. This method generated independent measures of drug-induced changes in the MFB stimulation reward and operant motor/performance capacity. Control experiments were run with the inactive isomer, trans-flupenthixol. Results indicated a major role for accumbens DA in MFB reward, but not for the DA in caudate and medial frontal cortex. Few drug-induced motor/performance deficits were found at any site. In 14 selected subjects, 6-OHDA-induced chronic DA lesions were made at the same site as neuroleptic microinjection. These results confirmed the reward effects of acute DA receptor blockade, but produced a greater associated motor/performance impairment. Both behavioral effects of the lesion recovered within 2 weeks in many, but not all subjects.
Article
The startle response (response to a loud noise, for instance) is a reflex that is wired into the brain at a very basic level. Although everybody has such a reflex, the strength and quickness of the startle response is modified by a subject's underlying psychoneurological state. The nature of this modification, therefore, is now seen as an accurate, objective measure of very deep neurological processes. This book is a comprehensive volume devoted to startle modification and offers a unique overview of the methods, measurement, physiology and psychology of the phenomenon, particularly modification of the human startle eyeblink. Chapters are written by many of the world's leading investigators in the field and include coverage of elicitation and recording of startle blink, issues in measurement and quantification, the neurophysiological basis of the basic startle response and its modification by attentional and affective processes, psychological processes underlying short and long lead interval modification (including prepulse inhibition), applications of startle modification to the study of psychopathology, including schizophrenia, affective disorders, and psychopathy, developmental processes and relationships with ERPs and behavioural measures of information processing.
Article
In vivo microdialysis with HPLC-ED was used to measure dopamine (DA),3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC), homovanillic acid (HVA) and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) in the nucleus accumbens of the rat, prior to, during, and after 15-min periods of electrical brain-stimulation at sites in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) that supported intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS). In the first experiment, both ICSS and yoked-stimulation of the VTA evoked significant increases in extracellular concentrations of DA, its metabolites, and 5-HIAA.Comparable results from ICSS and yoked groups were interpreted as evidence that the rewarding properties of VTA stimulation were a causal factor in the elevated DA transmission in the nucleus accumbens, rather than intense operant behavior. Further evidence for this hypothesis came from a second set of data in which changes in extracellular DA levels during the measurement of rate/intensity functions for ICSS were positively correlated. 5-HIAA concentrations also increased during ICSS but these changes were not correlated with either ICSS rate or current intensity, suggesting that changes in serotonin metabolism were unlikely to subserve brain-stimulation reward in the VTA. These results provide further evidence that stimulated mesolimbic DA release is a sufficient condition for reward.
Article
The function relating bar-pressing rate to the frequency of cathodal pulses was obtained in rats self-stimulating with amygdaloid (AMY) and lateral hypothalamic (LH) electrodes. The maximum self-stimulation (SS) rates in the AMY was found to be very low, compared to the LH. Concurrent stimulation with pairs of AMY-LH pulses did not shift the rate-frequency functional laterally, indicating the absence of summation of the two rewarding effects. In a second experiment, concurrent AMY-LH stimulation (using sub-threshold intensity LH pulses) facilitated bar-pressing for AMY stimulation (it increased the slope of the AMY rate-frequency function) without shifting this function laterally. In a third experiment, subjects were given a choice between a pulse frequency yielding maximal AMY rate and a series of higher pulse frequencies. Subjects consistently preferred the higher frequency values, attesting that the maximum AMY rates were not constrained by a saturating reinforcing effect. In a fourth experiment, subjects were given a choice between AMY stimulation and concurrent AMY-LH stimulation, using low intensity LH pulses. Subjects showed no preference for either stimulation condition, although rates were higher for the latter condition. These findings suggest that the maximum rate for AMY stimulation was constrained by factors interfering with bar-pressing and that the effect of these factors was attenuated by co-activation of the LH. In a fifth experiment, pre-treatment with phenobarbital mimicked the rate-enhancing effect of concurrent AMY-LH stimulation for 2 of the 4 subjects tested. This finding suggests that the LH pulses contributed to attenuate seizure activity accompanying AMY SS. In a final experiment, AMY SS rates were also increased by co-activation of rewarding sites in the rostral MFB but not the dorsal raphe, suggesting an anatomical specificity of this effect.
Article
This theoretical model of emotion is based on research using the startle-probe methodology. It explains inconsistencies in probe studies of attention and fear conditioning and provides a new approach to emotional perception, imagery, and memory. Emotions are organized biphasically, as appetitive or aversive (defensive). Reflexes with the same valence as an ongoing emotional state are augmented; mismatched reflexes are inhibited. Thus, the startle response (an aversive reflex) is enhanced during a fear state and is diminished in a pleasant emotional context. This affect-startle effect is not determined by general arousal, simple attention, or probe modality. The effect is found when affects are prompted by pictures or memory images, changes appropriately with aversive conditioning, and may be dependent on right-hemisphere processing. Implications for clinical, neurophysiological, and basic research in emotion are outlined.