ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Determinants of uncertainty in wildlife responses to human disturbance

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Outdoor recreation is increasing in intensity and space. Areas previously inaccessible are now being visited by ever-growing numbers of people, which increases human-wildlife encounters across habitats. This has raised concern among researchers and conservationists as, even in non-aggressive encounters, animals often perceive humans as predators and mount physiological and behavioural responses that can have negative consequences. However, despite all the research in recent decades, not many general patterns have emerged, especially at the level of populations, and many studies have yielded seemingly contradictory or inconclusive results. We argue that this is partly due to incomplete knowledge of the number and complexity of factors that may modulate the responses of animals. Thus, we aim to provide a conceptual approach intended to highlight the reasons that make it difficult to detect general patterns. We present a comprehensive compilation of factors modulating animal responses to humans at increasing levels (from sensory detection and immediate behavioural and physiological reactions, to changes in fitness and population trends), which may help understanding the uncertainty in the patterns. We observed that there are many modulating factors, which can be categorized as reflecting characteristics of the recreational activity itself (e.g. intensity of human presence), of the animals concerned (e.g. age or antipredatory strategy), and of the spatio-temporal context (e.g. habitat or timing of the encounter). Some factors appear to have non-linear and complex effects, which, if not considered, may lead to erroneous conclusions. Finally, we conclude that the difficulty in finding general patterns will be amplified at higher levels (i.e. at the level of populations), since as we proceed from one level to the next, the number of potential modulating factors accumulates, adding noise and obscuring direct associations between recreation and wildlife. More comprehensive knowledge about which (and how) factors affect animal responses across levels will certainly improve future research design and interpretation, and thus, our understanding of human recreational impacts on wildlife.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Biol. Rev. (2017), 92, pp. 216233. 216
doi: 10.1111/brv.12224
Determinants of uncertainty in wildlife
responses to human disturbance
Zulima Tabladoand Lukas Jenni
Swiss Ornithological Institute, Seerose 1, CH-6204, Sempach, Switzerland
ABSTRACT
Outdoor recreation is increasing in intensity and space. Areas previously inaccessible are now being visited by
ever-growing numbers of people, which increases humanwildlife encounters across habitats. This has raised concern
among researchers and conservationists as, even in non-aggressive encounters, animals often perceive humans as
predators and mount physiological and behavioural responses that can have negative consequences. However, despite
all the research in recent decades, not many general patterns have emerged, especially at the level of populations,
and many studies have yielded seemingly contradictory or inconclusive results. We argue that this is partly due to
incomplete knowledge of the number and complexity of factors that may modulate the responses of animals. Thus, we
aim to provide a conceptual approach intended to highlight the reasons that make it difficult to detect general patterns.
We present a comprehensive compilation of factors modulating animal responses to humans at increasing levels (from
sensory detection and immediate behavioural and physiological reactions, to changes in fitness and population trends),
which may help understanding the uncertainty in the patterns. We observed that there are many modulating factors,
which can be categorized as reflecting characteristics of the recreational activity itself (e.g. intensity of human presence),
of the animals concerned (e.g. age or antipredatory strategy), and of the spatio-temporal context (e.g. habitat or
timing of the encounter). Some factors appear to have non-linear and complex effects, which, if not considered, may
lead to erroneous conclusions. Finally, we conclude that the difficulty in finding general patterns will be amplified at
higher levels (i.e. at the level of populations), since as we proceed from one level to the next, the number of potential
modulating factors accumulates, adding noise and obscuring direct associations between recreation and wildlife. More
comprehensive knowledge about which (and how) factors affect animal responses across levels will certainly improve
future research design and interpretation, and thus, our understanding of human recreational impacts on wildlife.
Key words: human disturbance, recreational activities, vertebrates, adrenocortical response, flight initiation distance,
stress response, survival, reproduction, spatial use, population growth.
CONTENTS
I. Introduction .............................................................................................. 217
II. Level 1: Sensory detection of human approach .......................................................... 222
(1) Mechanisms .......................................................................................... 222
(2) Modulators ........................................................................................... 222
III. Level 2: Physiological and behavioural responses to human presence .................................... 223
(1) Mechanisms .......................................................................................... 223
(2) Modulators ........................................................................................... 223
IV. Level 3: Impacts of recreational activities on fitness and space use ....................................... 225
(1) Mechanisms .......................................................................................... 225
(2) Modulators ........................................................................................... 226
V. Level 4: Consequences of human disturbance for animal populations and species ....................... 227
(1) Mechanisms .......................................................................................... 227
(2) Modulators ........................................................................................... 227
VI. Conclusions .............................................................................................. 228
VII. Acknowlegdements ....................................................................................... 228
VIII. References ................................................................................................ 229
* Address for correspondence (Tel: +41 414629927; E-mail: Zulima.Tablado@vogelwarte.ch).
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
Modulators of wildlife response to recreation 217
I. INTRODUCTION
Outdoor recreational activities have increased dramatically
over the past decades (Boyle & Samson, 1985; Balmford
et al., 2009). Natural habitats and species that remained
relatively undisturbed in the past are now being visited by
an ever-increasing number of tourists (Balmford et al., 2009;
Buckley, 2009; Carney & Sydeman, 1999). The development
of technologies and transportation has allowed humans to
reach areas that were previously inaccessible to most people,
such as Antarctica, underwater habitats, or rainforests
(Sinclair & Jayawardena, 2010; Tisdell, 2010; Ríos-Jara
et al., 2013). This trend, together with the ever-decreasing
amount of ‘natural’ areas, calls for a deeper understanding
of the effects that humannature interactions may impose
on wildlife species around the world (Buckley, 2004b;Bejder,
2005; Arlettaz et al., 2007; Price, 2008). Many authors
have investigated various responses of animals to human
disturbance; however, most have focused on single species or
on single locations (van der Zande & Vos, 1984; Harper
& Eastman, 2000; Bolduc & Guillemette, 2003; Thiel
et al., 2008). Most studies measured changes in behaviour
or hormones as a direct response to human disturbance
without further investigating possible consequences for
demographic rates, population size or species resilience
(Gill, Norris & Sutherland, 2001; Stankowich & Blumstein,
2005; Breuner, Patterson & Hahn, 2008; Bonier
et al., 2009).
Despite the interest and concern directed towards this
subject in the last few decades, not many general patterns
have emerged. The findings to date are often inconclusive
or in disagreement, especially concerning consequences of
human disturbance for animal fitness parameters or popu-
lation trends (Burger, Gochfeld & Niles, 1995; Price, 2008;
Bonier et al., 2009). We suggest that this difficulty in finding
patterns is in part due to an incomplete understanding of the
mechanisms linking animal responses to human disturbance
and, perhaps more importantly, to insufficient knowledge
of the factors modulating the response mechanisms. We also
suggest that these factors sometimes modify animal responses
to disturbance in complex non-linear ways that have been
largely overlooked. Moreover, the combined effect of several
modulating factors could largely obscure the effects of human
activities. Therefore, to obtain a better insight into the effect
of human recreational activities on wildlife, a comprehensive
understanding of the modulating factors is necessary. Our
objective in this review is to present an overview of the
modulating factors and mechanisms that underlie responses
of wildlife to encounters with humans at multiple levels of
complexity.
To do so, we present a conceptual scheme (Fig. 1) that
integrates the different levels in humanwildlife interactions,
beginning with detection of recreationists by animals,
and followed by immediate physiological or behavioural
responses, consequences at the level of individual fitness
and habitat use, and subsequent impacts on populations or
species. It is not our intention to describe exhaustively the
Fig. 1. Conceptual model showing the different levels in the
process of interaction between recreationists and wildlife. The
resultant responses of animals to human disturbance can be
modulated by factors depending on the source of the disturbance
(i.e. characteristics of human activity), on properties of the
animal, and on the spatio-temporal context (e.g. habitat, climate,
timing). Decreases in the width of the green/black arrows,
as we go down the diagram, represent the dilution of the
association between human disturbance and animal response,
due to confounding effects of the accumulation of modulating
factors across levels.
mechanisms of response of disturbed animals (for that see,
for example, Sapolsky, Romero & Munck, 2000; Romero,
2004; Bejder, 2005; Breuner et al., 2008; Bejder et al., 2009;
Bonier, 2012), but rather to focus on factors that may alter
the expected patterns of response. At each level we first
present a synthesis of the mechanisms of response; we then
identify factors that may modulate those mechanisms, and
the type of effects they may cause (i.e. amplification or
attenuation, in a linear or non-linear manner), according
to what has been described, so far, in the literature (Table 1
and Fig. 2).
We classify the modulating factors into three different
categories: (i) factors dependent on the humans themselves
(e.g. number and frequency of people visiting an area
or the nature of the recreational activity), (ii) intrinsic
characteristics of the animals being disturbed, such as their
antipredatory strategy or sex, and (iii) modulators depending
on the spatio-temporal context in which the disturbance
takes place (e.g. season, climate or habitat). Although the
modulating factors and mechanisms act at specific levels,
these levels are not independent of each other (Fig. 1).
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
218 Zulima Tablado and Lukas Jenni
Table 1. Summary of factors modulating animal responses to human recreation activities at different levels
Levels Mechanisms Modulators
LEVEL 1a
Human Intrinsic Spatio-temporal context
Visual, hearing, olfactory
activation
Size (e.g. human
group size)
+[1] Sensory abilities (e.g.
larger versus smaller
species)
+[2– 4] Time of day (changes
in sensorial abilities
and vigilance
during the day)
A[57]
Colouration
(conspicuity)b
+[8– 10] Vigilance +[11– 13] Habitat characteristics
(vegetation density
and topography)
[14– 16]
Noise (volume) +[17,18] Previous experience
(i.e. learn to better
recognize and be
more vigilant)
+[19– 21] Climate (cloud, rain,
etc. reducing
disturbance
detection)
[22– 24]
Odour +[25– 28] Social aggregation +[13,29] Relative position of
disturbance to
animal (e.g.
perching height)
+[30,31]
Rate of movement +[32,33]
Proximity +[14,34]
LEVEL 2a
Human Intrinsic Spatio-temporal context
Behavioural changes (Behav),
physiological responses, i.e.
corticosteroid reactivity
(Cort), heart rate (HR)
Size (e.g. human
group size)
+[1,35] Antipredator strategy:
passive (hiding,
crypticity) versus
active (flushing,
fighting)
B [36– 38] Time of day
(circadian,
ultradian)
K[3942]
Colouration
(difference from
animal)b
+[10] Previous experience
(with humans and
natural predators)
C [37,43– 49] Time of year (e.g.
breeding season,
moulting)
L [40,50– 52]
Noise (volume, type) +[18,53,54] Cognitive aptitudes [55] Habitat characteristics
(vegetation density)
[14,16,56– 58]
Speed of movement +[59,60] Flushing performancecD [37,48,6166] Climate (i.e. direct
effect of
temperature)
[14,65]
Proximity +[37,67] Offspring value and
parental care (as
determined by, for
example, mating
system, breeding
season length and
survival rates)
E [68– 73] Relative position of
disturbance to
animal (e.g.
perching height)
[58,74– 76]
Directness of human
approach and gaze
+[59,60,77,78] Social aggregation F [14,37,79 81] Predation pressure +[82– 84]
Unpredictability +[34,85– 87] Mass-specific
metabolic rates
+[88] Concomitant stressful
events (e.g. extreme
weather agonistic
or predator
encounters)
+[89– 92]
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
Modulators of wildlife response to recreation 219
Table 1. Continued
Levels Mechanisms Modulators
Type (i.e.
aggressiveness or
similarity to
predators, hearing
versus seeing
humans)
+[25,93– 95] Sex and reproductive status (e.g.
effects through sexual
hormones)
G [64,75,96– 107]
Body condition (e.g. due to high
density of conspecifics, poor
habitat)
H [72,108– 110]
Coping style (due to selection,
neonatal development,
perinatal stress exposure, social
rank, etc.)
I [111– 114]
Age (developmental)dJ [72,75,84,115– 121]
LEVEL 3
Human Intrinsic Spatio-temporal context
Decrease in reproduction,
decrease in survival,
space-use changes,
chronic stress
Intensity (amount of
visitors)
+[122,123] Social aggregation M [124– 128] Timing of the disturbance (e.g.
periods of higher vulnerability
of nest abandonment, HPA
axis maturation)
+[129– 131]
Frequency or
continuity
+[126,132– 134] Cognitive aptitudes (smarter
animals will learn to recognize
faster non-consumptive
recreation activities, avoiding
chronic stress)
[55,135] Habitat quality (e.g. food
availability, offspring
concealment) and its value
relative to alternative areas.
[43,136– 138]
Energetic constraints (limited
compensation for
disturbance-induced energy
loss; e.g. migratory birds)
+[118– 120] Predation pressure +[130,139]
Body condition (body reserves to
buffer negative effects of
disturbance)
N [43,138,140,141] Climate (i.e. harsh weather and
climatic conditions)
+[129,142– 144]
Coping style (due to selection,
neonatal development,
perinatal stress exposure, social
rank, etc.)
O [43,90,145]
Previous investment in an area P [43,138]
LEVEL 4
Human Intrinsic Spatio-temporal context
Decreases in
demographic rates
leading to population
decreases, increased
extinction risk and
changes in distribution
Spatial and temporal
scale
+[146– 148] Life-history traits (e.g. a higher
number of reproductive
attempts, higher survival may
compensate for negative
medium-term responses)
[149– 151] Concomitant deterioration of
habitate
+[152– 154]
Ecological generality
of the disturbance
Q [155– 158] Site fidelity and philopatry (low
plasticity or learning ability to
avoid disturbed areas)
R [159– 162] Concomitant changes in
disturbance-independent
factors (e.g. climate change,
migration patterns, invasions
overriding the effects of human
disturbance)
T [163– 167]
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
220 Zulima Tablado and Lukas Jenni
Table 1. Continued
Levels Mechanisms Modulators
Social aggregation +[125,168]
Density (through release from density-dependent negative effects) [169– 171]
Meta-population dynamics (disturbed population is a source or a
sink)
S [172– 174]
Capacity to adapt (genetic/phenotypic variation, and phenotypic
plasticity)f
[175– 177]
Sign columns indicate the direction of the effect of a given factor. For the more complex effects a capital letter is assigned which corresponds to a graph in Fig. 2, depicting the shape of the relationship. We
only included mechanism, effects and trends which have been already described in, suggested in, or emerge through combining, previous studies.
References: 1, Geist et al. (2007); 2, Jones et al. (2007); 3, Kiltie (2000); 4, Blumstein et al. (2004); 5, Amo et al. (2011); 6, Gordon et al. (2010); 7, Elbaz et al. (2013); 8, Cuthill et al. (2005); 9, Gomez & Thery
(2007); 10, Gutzwiller & Marcum (1997); 11, Fernandez-Juricic & Schroeder (2003); 12, Blumstein (2006); 13, Beauchamp (2010); 14, Fernandez-Juricic et al. (2002); 15, Whittingham et al. (2004); 16, Lazarus
& Symonds (1992); 17, Margalida et al. (2011); 18, Karp & Root (2009); 19, Utne-Palm (2001); 20, Kelley & Magurran (2003); 21, Knight & Knight (1986); 22, Martin (2011); 23, Hilton et al. (1999); 24,
Carere et al. (2009); 25, Bates et al. (2007); 26, Fedosenko & Blank (2005); 27, Dupuch, Magnan & Dill (2004); 28, Roth et al. (2008); 29, Schaik et al. (1983); 30, Carlson (1985); 31, Moreno (1984); 32, Ewert
et al. (2001); 33, Siegel (1972); 34. Miller et al. (2001); 35, Burger & Gochfeld (1991); 36, Espmark & Langvatn (1985); 37, Stankowich & Blumstein (2005); 38, Steen et al. (1988); 39, Llewelyn et al. (2010); 40,
Romero & Remage-Healey (2000); 41, Sarabdjitsingh et al. (2012); 42, Tillmann (2009); 43, Bejder et al. (2009); 44, M ¨ullner et al. (2004); 45, Ikuta & Blumstein (2003); 46, R ¨
odl et al. (2007); 47, Bateman et al.
(2014); 48, Ydenberg & Dill (1986); 49, Helfman (1989); 50, Stankowich (2008); 51, Riou et al. (2010); 52, Romero (2002); 53, Burger & Gochfeld (1998); 54, Waynert et al. (1999); 55, Lendvai et al., 2013; 56,
Camp et al. (2012); 57, Dill & Houtman (1989); 58, Tadesse & Kotler (2012); 59, Cooper (1997); 60, Bateman & Fleming (2011); 61, Iriarte-Diaz (2002); 62, Bishop (2005); 63, Losos (1990); 64, Bra ˜
na (1993);
65, Cooper et al. (2009); 66, Møller et al. (2013); 67, Weston et al. (2012); 68, B ´
okony et al. (2009); 69, Breuner (2010); 70, Warner (1998); 71, Lendvai & Chastel, 2008; 72, Breuner et al. (2008); 73, Silverin
& Wingfield (1998); 74, MacArthur et al. (1982); 75, Steidl & Anthony (1996); 76, Thiel et al. (2007); 77, Bateman & Fleming (2014); 78, Stankowich & Coss (2006); 79, Ramp et al. (2005); 80, Kikusui et al.
(2006); 81, Pride (2005); 82, St Clair et al. (2010); 83, Januchowski-Hartley et al. (2011); 84, Berger et al. (2007); 85, Desire et al. (2002); 86, Boissy (1995); 87, Taylor & Knight (2003); 88, Jessop et al. (2013);
89, Romero et al. (2000); 90, Romero (2004); 91, Thaker et al. (2009a); 92, Thaker et al. (2010); 93, Martinetto & Cugnasse (2001); 94, Gonz´
alez et al. (2006); 95, Gabrielsen & Smith (1995); 96, Cartledge &
Jones (2007); 97, Solomon (2009); 98, Tilbrook et al. (2006); 99, Peczely (1979); 100, Wingfield & Sapolsky (2003); 101, Jiang et al. (2013); 102, O’Reilly & Wingfield (2001); 103, Irschick et al. (2005); 104,
Torner & Neumann (2002); 105, Albrecht & Klvana (2004); 106, Malo et al. (2011); 107, Wynne-Edwards & Timonin (2007); 108, Beale & Monaghan (2004a); 109, Stillman & Goss-Custard (2002); 110,
Moore & Jessop (2003); 111, Thaker et al. (2009b); 112, Atwell et al. (2012); 113, Schoech et al. (2011); 114, Carere et al. (2010); 115, Reeder & Kramer (2005); 116, Jacobsen (1979); 117, Wilcoxen et al. (2011);
118, Ingram (2000); 119, Walker, Boersma & Wingfield (2005); 120, Goutte et al. (2010); 121, Møller (2014); 122, Thiel et al. (2008); 123, Niles & Clark (1989); 124, Buckley (2004b); 125, Carney & Sydeman
(1999); 126, Robert & Ralph (1975); 127, Burger (1988); 128, Serrano et al. (2004); 129, Buckley (2011); 130, Bolduc & Guillemette (2003); 131, Kapoor et al. (2006); 132, Wheeler et al. (2009); 133, Harper
& Eastman (2000); 134, French et al. (2011); 135, Carrete & Tella (2011); 136, Lambert & Kleindorfer (2006); 137, Weidinger (2002); 138, Gill et al. (2001); 139, Gutzwiller et al. (2002); 140, Weimerskirch
(1999); 141, Weimerskirch et al. (2002); 142, Martin & Wiebe (2004); 143, Cowles (1956); 144, Carr & Lima (2012); 145, Fowler (1999); 146, Finney et al. (2005); 147, Lima (1993); 148, Riffell et al. (1996);
149, Church et al. (2007); 150, Lima (2009); 151, Heppell et al. (2000); 152, Buckley (2004a); 153, Cole & Landres (1995); 154, Braunisch et al. (2010); 155, Møller (2012); 156, Hebblewhite et al. (2005); 157,
Price (2008); 158, Skagen et al. (1991); 159, Yohannes et al. (2007); 160, Igual et al. (2007); 161, Stamps & Krishnan (1999); 162, Schlaepfer et al. (2002); 163, Foden et al. (2013); 164, Ozgul et al. (2010); 165,
Schaefer, Jetz & B¨
ohning-Gaese (2008); 166, Dextrase & Mandrak (2006); 167, Tablado & Revilla (2012); 168, Burger (1981); 169, Mallord et al. (2007); 170, Sinclair & Pech (1996); 171, McGowan et al.
(2011); 172, Brawn & Robinson (1996); 173, Pulliam (1988); 174, Dias (1996); 175, Lande & Shannon (1996); 176, Carvalho (1993); 177, Dufty et al. (2002).
aIn many cases previous research does not allow us to distinguish between the effects of human recreation on detection (level 1) from the effects on short-term behavioural/physiological response (level 2; see
for example the human factor ‘size’ for which we cited the same reference). Nevertheless, when possible, we emphasized this difference. A good example is the intrinsic factor ‘social aggregation’. A larger
group size facilitates the detection of humans and other predators; however, the effect of group size on the decisions of the animals to flush away after detecting the disturbance is likely to be the opposite.
Some authors have found that animals in larger groups show a lower physiological response to stressors and tolerate for longer the approaching human before flushing, when comparing alert distances, which
we use as a proxy of detection, with flight initiation distance (FID). This could be due either to the dilution of risk in larger groups, or to group-dependent antipredator strategies, such as in some fish species.
In any case at too large aggregation densities the pattern can start to reverse and increases in arousal and general stress could increase again in some species.
bAnimals tend to see humans wearing bright/conspicuous colours earlier; however once detected the reaction of the animals towards those humans depends on their own coloration pattern. Some authors
have shown that animals tolerate humans better when dressed in colours similar to their own.
cAs the economics of fleeing predicts, animals should escape only when the cost of staying is greater than the cost of flushing away. In this line, animals with a lower ability to flush, such as gravid females
or newborn individuals with reduced mobility, rely on crypsis/hiding and allow closer approach. As performance increases, FID also increases; however, FID decreases again in individuals whose escaping
ability is much higher than the risk of staying (e.g. In some species, the better locomotor performance of adults, allows them to rely more on last-minute escaping strategies than in the case of juveniles of the
same species).
dWe chose to consider age using a developmental perspective and not directly as days after birth in order to take into account the differences in physiological maturation between altricial and precocial
species (e.g. varying timing of the hypothalamic— pituitary— adrenal axis hyporesponsive period).
eIt is important to understand the difference between direct effects of human presence (e.g. tourists visiting penguin colonies) and the additional anthropogenic effects through habitat deterioration (e.g.
construction of roads or ski resorts).
fAdaptation to humans will be positive for a species, provided that it does not entail a concomitant relaxation of the anti-predator response to other non-human/real predators. Otherwise, excessive boldness
may be counterproductive for the species.
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
Modulators of wildlife response to recreation 221
Day Night
Time of day
Nocturnal
animals
Level 1:
Detection
Diurnal
animals
(A)
Passive ----------------> Active
Anti-predator strategy
HR
Behav
(B)
Day Night
Time of day
Nocturnal
animals
Diurnal
animals
(K)
Body condition
Spatial change
(N)
Previous investment in the area
Demogr. process c hange
Chronic stress
Spatial change
(P)
Ecological extent of disturbance
Concomitant impact on
prey (or beneficial species)
Concomitant impact on
predators (or competitors )
(Q)
Source -------->Sink population
Meta-population dynamics
(S)
Concomitant changes in disturbance-
independent factors
Factors with positiv e impact on
the population/s
Factors with additional negative impact
(T)
Previous experience
Aggressive
(Natural predators, hunters)
Neutral or positive
(C)
Flushing per formanc e
(D)
Behav
Parental care per capita
Offspring value
(E)
Sex hormones (Masculine --> Feminine)
Parental care hormones
(G)
age (development)
Behav
Cort
(J)
Reactive/Shy --------> Proactive/Bold
Copying style
(I)
Body condition
Behav
Cort
(H)
Prebreed. Breeding Postbreed.
Time of year
(L)
(moult
in birds)
(R)
Site fidelity/philopatry
Change of
distribution
Population decrease and
extinction risk
Reactive/Shy --------> Proactive/Bold
Copying style
(O)
Social aggregation
Demogr. process c hange
Chronic stress
Spatial change
(M)
Social aggregation
(F)
Level 2: Physiologicala
nd
behaviouralresponses
Level 2: Physiological and
behavioural response
Level 2: Physiological and
behavioural responses
Level 2: Physiologicaland
behavioural responses
Level 2:Physiologicaland
behaviouralresp
onses
Level 2: Physiological and
behaviouralre
sponses
Level2:Physiologicala
nd
behavioural responses
Level 2: Physiological and
behavioural responses
Level2
:Physiologicaland
behavioural responses
Level 2:Physiologicala
nd
behavioural responses
Level 2: Physiological and
behavioural responses
Level 3: C hanges i n fitnes s
andspaceu
se
Demogr. process c hange
Chronic stress
Level 3:Changes in fitness
and space use
Level 3:Changes infitn
ess
andspace use
Level 3:Changes in fitness
andspaceu
se
Level4:Population and
species trends
Level 4: Populationand
speciest
rends
Level 4: Populationand
species trends
Level 4: Populationand
species trends
Fig. 2. Schematic representation of complex associations between detection or responses of animals to direct human presence
and some modulating factors. Letters correspond to the relationship marked with the same letter in Table 1, where supporting
references can be found. As in Table 1, we only included relationships that have been documented in, suggested in, or can be
inferred from combining, the existing literature. Black and grey lines summarize the trends when all mechanisms within a given
level respond similarly to a factor; coloured lines indicate mechanism-specific associations to a factor (e.g. specific physiological
responses or behavioural responses within level 2). Cort =adrenocortical response; HR =heart rate; Behav =flushing/escaping
distance; Demogr. =demographic.
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
222 Zulima Tablado and Lukas Jenni
That is, responses at a certain level will only happen if
the responses at previous levels have already taken place
(e.g. human disturbance can only have negative effects
on demographic rates if animals detect people and react
to them). This dependence on previous levels entails the
accumulation of modulating factors across levels, which
will generally lead to a weakening of the association
between disturbance and animal responses at higher levels
(see Fig. 1).
To support this conceptual model, we focus mainly on
studies of the effects of human recreation on vertebrate
species, especially birds, given the large representation of this
taxon in the disturbance literature (e.g. Keller, 1996; Carney
& Sydeman, 1999; Buckley, 2004a; Price, 2008; Steven,
Pickering & Castley, 2011). We also used complementary
literature from other fields, such as predation ecology,
urban wildlife, or laboratory-induced stress (e.g. Lima, 1993;
Møller, 2012; Elbaz et al., 2013). Our approach focuses
on disturbance caused by recreational activities with the
least alteration of the habitat (such as people walking or
cross-country skiing). In this way, we hope to extract the
direct responses of animals to the presence of humans.
Anthropogenic activities associated with substantial habitat
modifications, such as the construction of tourist resorts or
roads, will be assumed to amplify (i.e. modulate) the impact
of recreationists, and are included, therefore, as modulating
factors. We hope that the comprehensive overview given
here about the scope and influence of modulating factors
will serve to help future researchers identify variables
that need to be taken into account in the design and
interpretation of studies and, thereby, to provide a better
understanding of the effect of recreational activities on
wildlife.
II. LEVEL 1: SENSORY DETECTION OF HUMAN
APPROACH
(1) Mechanisms
As suggested by previous research, wild species may perceive
humans as predators (Frid & Dill, 2002; Beale & Monaghan,
2004b); however, for this to happen, animals must first detect
them. What are the cues and mechanisms used by vertebrate
animals to detect the presence of humans? Wildlife perceive
humans (as well as predators) visually, auditorially, and
olfactorially (Utne-Palm, 2001; Smith, Kane & Popper, 2004;
Whittingham et al., 2004; Bates et al., 2007; Hagelin & Jones,
2007; Fernandez-Juricic et al., 2012), although, as we will see
later, the type and threshold of the sensory response will
vary according to factors such as the intensity of the human
signal, the taxon, and the environment. Detection of human
presence is typically characterized by an immediate orienting
reflex (also called ‘alert’ position) in which the animals cease
their current activity (feeding, singing, etc.) and focus their
senses on the anthropogenic stimulus, increasing vigilance
to gather further information about the potential threat and
assess the danger (Sokolov et al., 1963; Gabrielsen, Blix &
Ursin, 1985; Knight & Gutzwiller, 1995).
(2) Modulators
Many factors may modify the ability of animals to detect
recreationists (Table 1 and Fig. 2). First, detection likelihood
depends on the obviousness of the anthropogenic presence
in itself, such as the number of people approaching, clothing
colour (Cuthill et al., 2005; Gomez & Thery, 2007), noise
level (Karp & Root, 2009; Margalida et al., 2011), odour
(e.g. presence or absence of pet species) (Miller, Knight &
Clinton, 2001; Bates et al., 2007; Roth, Cox & Lima, 2008),
distance (Miller et al., 2001; Fernandez-Juricic, Jimenez
& Lucas, 2002), and speed of movement (Siegel, 1972;
Ewert et al., 2001). That is, animals will more easily sense
humans if they are in larger groups, conspicuously dressed,
louder, smell stronger, and are moving fast or in close
proximity.
Second, the intrinsic properties of the species and
individuals being approached will also affect the likelihood of
detection. Among these intrinsic characteristics are variation
in sensory abilities (e.g. raptors have better vision than other
bird species, larger animals have a larger range of detection
than smaller ones) (Kiltie, 2000; Jones, Pierce & Ward,
2007), vigilance effort (which may vary with an individual’s
body condition or reproductive status) (Fernandez-Juricic
& Schroeder, 2003; Beauchamp, 2010), previous experience
(i.e. individuals may learn better to recognize human
presence) (Utne-Palm, 2001; Kelley & Magurran, 2003),
and degree of gregariousness (animals in larger social groups
usually have a structured sentinel system leading to a higher
overall vigilance level and probability of detection) (Schaik
et al., 1983; Beauchamp, 2010). The last category of factors
influencing detection is related to the spatio-temporal con-
text of humanwildlife interaction. For example, time of the
day, habitat characteristics, weather conditions and relative
position of the animal to the recreational activity will deter-
mine whether animals detect people or not. Time of day will
influence not only light levels and other environmental char-
acteristics that may influence the ability to detect humans,
but also the level of awareness and sensory ability of animals
(e.g. nocturnal animals have senses and activity patterns
better adapted for night life while it is the opposite for diurnal
species) (Gordon, Dickman & Thompson, 2010; Amo, Caro
& Visser, 2011; Elbaz et al., 2013). Characteristics of the
habitat (i.e. vegetation density and pronounced topography),
together with weather conditions, such as precipitation or
fog, will also limit the detection ability of animals, as they may
decrease visibility, odour and sound transmission (Lazarus
& Symonds, 1992; Hilton, Ruxton & Cresswell, 1999;
Fernandez-Juricic et al., 2002; Whittingham et al., 2004;
Carere et al., 2009; Martin, 2011). Finally, the position of
people relative to the animal may also affect detection, that is,
for instance animals resting/nesting high in trees may have a
larger range of visibility than ground dwellers (Moreno, 1984;
Carlson, 1985).
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
Modulators of wildlife response to recreation 223
III. LEVEL 2: PHYSIOLOGICAL AND
BEHAVIOURAL RESPONSES TO HUMAN
PRESENCE
(1) Mechanisms
When human presence is detected, the sensory response
(and the corresponding alert reflex) may be followed by
a behavioural and physiological anti-predatory response
(Gabrielsen & Smith, 1995; Berger et al., 2007; Blumstein,
2010), depending on the degree to which human presence
is perceived as a threat. Behaviourally, this reaction may be
passive or active. That is, upon perception of a threat, an
individual will evaluate the situation and respond accordingly
by relying on staying still and not being seen (i.e. hide or
freeze and remain cryptic) or by actively escaping from or
fighting the threat (Lima & Dill, 1990; Lima, 1993; Bracha,
2004).
At the physiological level, there are almost instantaneous
increases in the secretion of acetylcholine and catecholamines
from the autonomic nervous system (Fisher, 1990; Knight
& Gutzwiller, 1995; Sapolsky et al., 2000; Romero & Butler,
2007), and corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) from the
hypothalamus in the brain (Sapolsky et al., 2000; Reeder
& Kramer, 2005). These secreted substances contribute
importantly to passive or active behavioural reactions
by decreasing or increasing locomotion, cardiovascular
activity, oxygen consumption, glucose mobilization, and
body temperature (Fisher, 1990; Knight & Gutzwiller,
1995; Wingfield et al., 1997; Sapolsky et al., 2000). They also
produce temporary interruptions of reproductive behaviour
and appetite during stressful events (Sapolsky et al., 2000;
Wingfield & Sapolsky, 2003). Simultaneously, there is an
activation of the immune system through the release of
cytokines in preparation for potential injuries and infections
(Wingfield et al., 1997; Sapolsky et al., 2000; Wingfield &
Sapolsky, 2003; Romero, Dickens & Cyr, 2009).
In addition to its role in the immediate arousal reaction,
the release of CRH triggers the slower adrenocortical stress
response (Sapolsky et al., 2000; Romero, 2004) through the
activation of the hypothalamicpituitaryadrenal (HPA)
axis (or the homologous hypothalamicpituitaryinterrenal
axis, depending on the vertebrate taxon) (Sapolsky et al., 2000;
Rollins-Smith, 2001; Øverli et al., 2007; Gasser, Lowry &
Orchinik, 2009; Wingfield, 2013). Thus, hypothalamic CRH
promotes the secretion of adrenocorticotropic hormone
(ACTH) from the pituitary gland (also called hypophysis)
which in turn stimulates the adrenal cortex to increase
production of corticosteroids (CORT; mainly cortisol in fish
and most mammals and corticosterone in birds, reptiles,
amphibians, and many rodents) (Sapolsky et al., 2000;
Romero, 2004; Reeder & Kramer, 2005). The stress-induced
levels of CORT start to be detected in plasma a few minutes
after disturbance and exert numerous effects throughout
the body. These effects will depend on the abundance
and distribution of CORT receptors in different organs
(Bellingham, Sar & Cidlowski, 1992; Kapoor et al., 2006;
Crespi et al., 2013; Lattin et al., 2015) and the action of CORT
binding globulins (a plasma protein that binds CORT with
high affinity and can regulate CORT delivery and availability
at specific sites) (Breuner & Orchinik, 2002; Malisch &
Breuner, 2010; but see Schoech et al., 2013).
CORT produces its effect through both rapid and
delayed mechanisms (Sapolsky et al., 2000; Gasser et al.,
2009). In the former case, CORT acts through non-genomic
pathways and, in the course of minutes, induces changes
in concentrations of monoamines (e.g. catecholamines) in
some brain areas. It also alters brain cell excitability and
activation that result in autonomic output and rapid changes
in behaviour (Gasser et al., 2009; Groeneweg et al., 2011;
Sarabdjitsingh, Jo¨
els & De Kloet, 2012). The delayed effects,
on the other hand, entail genomic processes, such as gene
transcription and protein formation, and produce effects
hours and days after the disturbance (Sapolsky et al., 2000;
Romero, 2004; Wingfield, 2013). All these effects of CORT
serve to improve survival under threatening situations by
redirecting energy and attention away from non-essential
processes, to help the body to return to homeostasis after
the perturbation is over, and to prepare the animal for
subsequent stressors and energy expenditures (Wingfield
& Sapolsky, 2003; Tarlow & Blumstein, 2007; Wingfield,
2013). In this sense, stress-induced CORT contributes to
the inhibition of reproductive behaviour and physiology,
the facilitation of locomotor responses, the enhancement
of stress-related memory and learning, and the increase of
energy availability (e.g. by promoting gluconeogenesis, fat
and protein catabolism, and decreasing peripheral glucose
uptake by non-essential tissues). CORT also acts to suppress
stress-induced immune responses to prevent them from
overshooting and to down-regulate the HPA axis itself
through negative feedback (Sapolsky et al., 2000; Gasser et al.,
2009; Breuner, 2010).
(2) Modulators
The physiological and behavioural responses to human
presence will also be modulated by anthropogenic, intrinsic
and context-dependent factors (Table 1 and Fig. 2). In
regard to human-dependent factors, the perception of risk
increases when animals are approached by people: (i)in
large groups (Burger & Gochfeld, 1991; Geist et al., 2007);
(ii) wearing coloured clothing that differs markedly from
the animal’s colouration (Gutzwiller & Marcum, 1997);
(iii) involved in activities that generate considerable noise
(Burger & Gochfeld, 1998; Waynert et al., 1999; Karp &
Root, 2009); and (iv) engaged in activities characterized
by rapid movements (Cooper, 1997; Bateman & Fleming,
2011). Perceived risk also increases when humans get closer
to animals (Stankowich & Blumstein, 2005; Weston et al.,
2012), and move and look directly towards them (Cooper,
1997; Stankowich & Coss, 2006; Carter et al., 2008; Bateman
& Fleming, 2011, 2014). Predictability of the disturbance
is another key factor to take into account. Animals are
more distressed by recreationists appearing suddenly in an
unpredictable way (Boissy, 1995; Miller et al., 2001; Desire,
Boissy & Veissier, 2002; Taylor & Knight, 2003). Activities
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
224 Zulima Tablado and Lukas Jenni
that are more aggressive or similar to predation, such as
consumptive recreation (e.g. hunting, capture, and handling),
or presence of predator-like animals (e.g. dogs), will also
influence animal risk assessment (Gabrielsen & Smith, 1995;
Martinetto & Cugnasse, 2001; Gonz´
alez et al., 2006; Bates
et al., 2007; Bateman et al., 2014). Some research has also
shown that animals may be more reactive to the visual
presence of humans than to anthropogenic noise (Gabrielsen
& Smith, 1995).
Other modulating factors are the characteristics of the
animals being approached (Table 1 and Fig. 2). For example,
certain species have more passive antipredatory strategies,
such as hiding and crypticity, while others opt for active
strategies like flushing or fighting (Espmark & Langvatn,
1985; Steen, Gabrielsen & Kanwisher, 1988; Stankowich &
Blumstein, 2005). Factors such as previous experience (Ikuta
& Blumstein, 2003; M¨ullner, Eduard Linsenmair & Wikelski,
2004; R¨
odl et al., 2007) and cognitive ability (Lendvai et al.,
2013) will also influence the way in which animals distinguish
non-dangerous humans from real threats, affecting the
response exhibited. Prior interactions with human activities
or natural predation might increase stress responses in
further encounters (i.e. sensitization of individuals exposed
to hunting, noxious anthropogenic stimuli or high natural
predation pressure) (Bejder et al., 2009; Bateman et al., 2014).
However, human-induced stress response can be low when
animals are naïve to predation risk due to the lack of natural
predators or when individuals have been habituated to harm-
less anthropogenic stimuli (Ydenberg & Dill, 1986; Helfman,
1989; Bejder et al., 2009). The ability to escape (flushing
performance in Table 1 and Fig. 2D) may affect animal
reactivity (Ydenberg & Dill, 1986; Losos, 1990; Iriarte-Diaz,
2002; Bishop, 2005; Møller, V´
ag´
asi & Pap, 2013). That is,
even if they perceive a high risk, animals with extremely poor
flushing abilities (e.g. gravid females or newborn offspring)
will rely on hiding, thereby showing reduced behavioural
responses compared to animals better able to escape (Bra˜
na,
1993; Stankowich & Blumstein, 2005). However, animals
with better flushing performance and/or fitter may be
‘confident’ of their ability to escape and, therefore may
tolerate a closer approach, exhibiting a minimal behavioural
response (Ydenberg & Dill, 1986; Cooper, Hawlena &
P´
erez-Mellado, 2009; Blumstein, 2010; Møller et al., 2013).
The behavioural and physiological response of parents
with dependent offspring have been shown to be less strong
if the value of the offspring is high. Offspring value is
high if there is little chance for the individual to reproduce
again, and vice versa, which may depend, for example, on
sex, mating system, survival likelihood (i.e. individual age
and the chance of future reproduction), and environmental
conditions (Silverin & Wingfield, 1998; Warner, 1998;
Lendvai & Chastel, 2008; B´
okony et al., 2009; Breuner,
2010; Schmid et al., 2013). Antipredator responses will also
vary with social aggregation, which is expected to decrease
the perceived danger by single individuals (i.e. risk dilution
and coordinated defence strategies) at least up to a certain
group size. However, at even larger group sizes this trend
may switch, since the higher overall stress and lower arousal
thresholds (e.g. more likely alarm calls) of highly dense
groups may increase responsiveness (Fernandez-Juricic et al.,
2002; Pride, 2005; Ramp, Russell & Croft, 2005; Stankowich
& Blumstein, 2005; Kikusui, Winslow & Mori, 2006). Other
properties that may affect the physiological and psychological
state of individuals, and therefore their stress response, are
mass-specific metabolic rates (i.e. faster metabolism favours
stress reactivity) (Jessop, Woodford & Symonds, 2013), sex
and hormone profiles, body condition, coping style, and
developmental age (i.e. from foetus to adults). Sex hormones
and hormones involved in parental care play an important
role in modulating the physiological and behavioural stress
response. For example, oestrogens are related to higher
adrenocortical reactivity, while prolactin reduces parental
stress responses, thereby favouring offspring care and survival
(Peczely, 1979; Steidl & Anthony, 1996; Tilbrook et al., 2006;
Wynne-Edwards & Timonin, 2007; Solomon, 2009; Malo,
Acebes & Traba, 2011). Poor body condition (i.e. typical of
high anxiety levels, high densities, or poor habitats) favours
self-maintenance mechanisms, energy-saving behaviours
and a higher adrenocortical response (Moore & Jessop, 2003;
Beale & Monaghan, 2004a;Breuneret al., 2008). Individuals
may exhibit considerable variability in stress reactivity (i.e.
the intensity and duration of their response to a given
stressor). These variations in ‘coping style’ have both genetic
and environmental influences, such as perinatal exposure to
stressors and corticosteroids (Carere, Caramaschi & Fawcett,
2010; Schoech, Rensel & Heiss, 2011). The coping style will
be reflected both in the behavioural and the physiological
response to disturbance. That is, shyer individuals are
generally more fearful and, thus, are likely to mount a
higher adrenocortical response and flush (or hide) earlier
than bolder individuals (Thaker, Lima & Hews, 2009b;
Carere et al., 2010; Atwell et al., 2012). Physiological and
behavioural responses to disturbance will vary non-linearly
with developmental age. During early development, which
occurs mostly prenatally (or in ovo) in precocial species and
extends further into postnatal (or posthatch) life in altricial
species, there is a hypo-responsive period of the HPA axis
when CORT responses are low (Berger et al., 2007; Breuner
et al., 2008; Wada & Breuner, 2010). This has been suggested
to protect developing animals from the detrimental effects of
high CORT levels, which could cause permanent alterations
of the HPA axis, may cause damage to memory and cognitive
functions, and reduce growth rates (Kitaysky et al., 2003;
Hayward & Wingfield, 2004; Breuner et al., 2008; Rensel,
Boughton & Schoech, 2010; Wada & Breuner, 2010).
Later the HPA axis responsiveness increases and reaches
relatively high levels during juvenile and early-adulthood
periods. Some studies have shown that HPA reactivity
may, to a certain degree, decrease again during adulthood
(Table 1, Fig. 2J). That is, juvenile individuals and younger
adults would show a higher adrenocortical response than
experienced adults (Berger et al., 2007; Breuner et al., 2008;
Wilcoxen et al., 2011). With senescence; however, the
trendappearstoswitchoncemoreandtheresponsiveness
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
Modulators of wildlife response to recreation 225
of the HPA axis tends to increase (Goutte et al., 2010;
Wilcoxen et al., 2011). Behavioural responses also often
show a complex non-linear relationship with age (Table 1,
Fig. 2J). Behavioural responses may be lower (more passive)
in newborn and senescent individuals, due to locomotor and
energetic limitations (Knight & Gutzwiller, 1995; Berger
et al., 2007; Seltmann et al., 2012). At ‘intermediate’ ages,
flushing responses will show a U-shaped relationship, with
juvenile and older adults (but not senescent) age classes
tending to escape earlier than middle-aged adults (Berger
et al., 2007; Seltmann et al., 2012; Møller, 2014).
Animal responses to human activities can also vary
depending on the spatio-temporal context (Table 1, Fig. 2).
We observe that these responses often vary throughout
the day and year (Fig. 2J, K), i.e. depending on circadian
and circa-annual variations in physiology, activity and
motivation (e.g. nocturnality versus diurnality, territory
establishment versus moulting or migratory phase) (Romero &
Remage-Healey, 2000; Stankowich, 2008; Tillmann, 2009;
Llewelyn, Webb & Shine, 2010; Riou et al., 2010). In addition,
in recent years smaller-scale cycles have been described
for at least some species, in which within a given daily
cycle individuals experience ultradian HPA axis pulsatility
(Sarabdjitsingh et al., 2012). The stress response of a given
individual will, thus, be time-specific and depend on the
phase of the ultradian cycle when human disturbance
occurs. Habitat characteristics will also affect reactions
towards human presence, since apart from their effect on
food availability, and thus, body condition, they differ in
refuge availability for animals exposed to human recreation.
Wildlife having options to hide or retreat to refuges (e.g.
dense canopy, cavities or steep slopes), will ‘feel’ safer and
react differently from individuals or species found in more
open habitats and that have to traverse larger distances for
protection (Lazarus & Symonds, 1992; Fernandez-Juricic
et al., 2002; Camp et al., 2012; Tadesse & Kotler, 2012).
Weather can also directly affect animal behavioural
responses. For example, higher ambient temperatures tend to
increase an animal’s ‘tolerance’ to approaching people, due
to their faster capacity of reaction when warmer. In addition,
animals may want to limit their stress responses to, to some
extent, avoid excessive heat build-up due to stress-related
increases in metabolism and movement (Fernandez-Juricic
et al., 2002; Cooper et al., 2009). Other contextual factors that
determine the psychological and physiological excitability of
animals are the relative position of the animal to humans
(e.g. animals ‘feel’ safer when located higher or closer to
escape routes) (MacArthur, Geist & Johnston, 1982; Steidl &
Anthony, 1996; Thiel et al., 2007; Tadesse & Kotler, 2012),
predation pressure (Berger et al., 2007; St Clair et al., 2010;
Januchowski-Hartley et al., 2011), and simultaneous stressful
events, such as extreme weather or agonistic encounters with
conspecifics (Romero, Reed & Wingfield, 2000; Romero,
2004). Increases in the latter two usually lead to high levels
of awareness and baseline CORT that, in turn, may lower
response thresholds to disturbance (Thaker, Lima & Hews,
2009a; Thaker et al., 2010).
IV. LEVEL 3: IMPACTS OF RECREATIONAL
ACTIVITIES ON FITNESS AND SPACE USE
(1) Mechanisms
If individuals respond behaviourally or physiologically to the
presence of recreationists, their fitness (reproductive output
and survival as proxies) and habitat use may be compromised.
This can happen both through single disturbance events and
through more continuous or frequent disturbances (Bowles,
1995; Knight & Gutzwiller, 1995; Wingfield et al., 1997;
Frid & Dill, 2002; Buckley, 2011). Regarding the negative
effects of single disturbances on fitness, one of the most
common cases is when parents are flushed, leaving eggs or
newborn offspring unattended. The temporarily abandoned
offspring or eggs may then be depredated, starve, or die from
thermal stress (Ellison & Cleary, 1978; Anderson & Keith,
1980; Major, 1990; Buckley, 2011). Some predators may
even specialize in this type of opportunistic hunting (Kury
& Gochfeld, 1975; Strang, 1980; Knight & Cole, 1995).
Disturbance-related single panic reactions may cause acci-
dental self-injury or damage to conspecifics and progeny, thus
decreasing survival and reproductive output (Bowles, 1995;
Buckley, 2011). An understudied phenomenon is that single
disturbance events during settling periods (e.g. dispersal and
territory formation) might cause significant changes in habitat
use or even lead to abandonment (Buckley, 2004b, 2011).
If disturbances are frequent or continuous, they may
also cause negative effects through chronic stress (i.e.
continuous activation of HPA axis and disruption of negative
feedback, which impedes the return to basal CORT levels),
longer-term changes in activity budget patterns, behaviour
(e.g. movement patterns) and energy expenditure (Knight &
Gutzwiller, 1995; Wingfield et al., 1997; Frid & Dill, 2002;
Romero, 2004). Note that this might not apply in cases in
which habituation occurs [see Level 2 (Section III) for further
information on the modulating effect of previous experience
on animal responses to humans]. Chronic high levels of stress
hormones can lead to infertility, an offspring sex-ratio biased
towards the less costly sex, immunosuppression, weight
and muscle loss, growth inhibition, damage to the central
nervous system with accompanying impairment of cognitive
functions, among others (Sapolsky et al., 2000; Elenkov, 2004;
Wikelski & Cooke, 2006; Breuner, 2010). Regularly disturbed
areas may also be avoided or used only when humans are
not present, altering spatial use and/or activity patterns
(Tuite, Owen & Paynter, 1983; Pfister, Harrington & Lavine,
1992; Bejder et al., 2006, 2009). This can lead to a shift in
home range and displace wildlife into suboptimal habitats,
either permanently or temporarily, with all the possible
consequences for animal body condition and fitness (Olsson
et al., 2007; Paquet & Darimont, 2010; Kerley, Kowalczyk &
Cromsigt, 2012). Finally, waste of energy, as a consequence
of recurrent disturbance can lower body condition of
animals and, in turn, compromise survival and reproduction,
especially if it occurs during periods of high energetic needs,
such as migration, harsh winter conditions, breeding or moult
(Madsen, 1995; Buckley, 2004b;Barshepet al., 2013).
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
226 Zulima Tablado and Lukas Jenni
(2) Modulators
The impact of human intrusion on fitness and habitat use
will not be the same for all individuals and situations, but will
be modified by the characteristics of the disturbance itself,
the properties inherent to the species or individuals, and
the circumstances around the disturbance event. Human
recreation will tend to have a higher impact on breeding
success, survival, habitat selection and/or chronic stress
when it causes more frequent or intense behavioural or
physiological responses. This is the case for animals in
areas with frequent, continuous or intense human use (i.e.
higher number of people using an area at the same time)
and that fail to habituate to the disturbance (Robert &
Ralph, 1975; Niles & Clark, 1989; Harper & Eastman, 2000;
Thiel et al., 2008; Wheeler, Villiers & Majiedt, 2009; French
et al., 2011).
The outcome of disturbance at this intermediate level will
vary among species and individuals. Social aggregation may
modulate the effects of disturbance, since gregarious species
are more likely to suffer self-injury and injure conspecifics
during panic behaviour (Robert & Ralph, 1975; Carney &
Sydeman, 1999; Buckley, 2004a). The spatial response to
human recreation will likely differ between gregarious and
solitary taxa, since habitat selection of gregarious species
will depend not only on characteristics of the habitat (and
degree of disturbance) but also on group decisions and social
attraction that might increase the reluctance to abandon
a certain area (Burger, 1988; Serrano et al., 2004). Higher
cognitive ability will most likely also play an important role
in helping animals to recognise quickly and, thus, habituate
to non-dangerous human activities and reduce the likelihood
of suffering chronic stress and decreased fitness (Carrete &
Tella, 2011; Lendvai et al., 2013). In addition, individual
coping style may influence the speed of recovery after a
stressful perturbation, leading to differences in long-term
stress levels (Fowler, 1999; Romero, 2004; Bejder et al.,
2009). That is, animals with a more reactive HPA axis,
i.e. generally shyer, possess less efficient negative feedback
systems to facilitate the return of CORT to baseline levels
and, as a result, will be more prone to suffer chronic stress.
As mentioned above, human disturbance causing increases
in energy expenditure or decreases in food intake may lead to
decreases in animal survival and reproduction, especially in
periods of harsh weather conditions or during periods crucial
for reserve accumulation (e.g. during premigratory staging)
(Madsen, 1995; Buckley, 2004b;Barshepet al., 2013). This
will predominantly affect individuals and species that are
energetically constrained, reducing their limited capacity to
compensate for additional expenditures. An example of this
are species which are limited by the capacity of their digestive
tract (e.g. grouse species in winter feeding on coniferous nee-
dles) or performing costly activities (e.g. moulting, breeding)
(Smith, 1976; Millar, 1978; Koteja, 1996; Sedinger, 1997).
Other cases are, for instance, animals whose survival and
reproduction depend on especially high rates of energy accu-
mulation, such as long-distance migrating birds or animals
storing energy for wintering (MacKinnon, 1972; Piersma,
1990). If their fuel deposition rate is lowered through human
activities, their time schedule may be delayed, causing
reproduction and survival to be compromised (Madsen,
1995; Buckley, 2004b; Klaassen et al., 2006). Similarly, indi-
viduals in suboptimal condition (e.g. living in a suboptimal
habitat, harsh climatic conditions, low social rank, and/or
suffering from parasites or disease) will have a lower capacity
to buffer the deleterious effects of acute or prolonged
disturbances. These animals will not be able to compensate
with additional broods after stress-induced failures or will
produce smaller, more vulnerable offspring (Weimerskirch,
1999; Weimerskirch et al., 2002). Interestingly, poor body
condition may impede the abandonment of disturbed areas
as only individuals with enough energy reserves will be able
to search for new suitable habitat (Gill et al., 2001; Bejder
et al., 2009). The degree of psychological and energetic
investment in settling in a given area may also determine
the willingness and likelihood of abandoning it after being
disturbed by recreationists. The more an individual invests
in an area, the less prone will it be to leave, even though this
attachment may lead to chronic stress, since it will continue
to be exposed to human visitation (Gill et al., 2001; Bejder
et al., 2009).
Finally, timing and environmental characteristics will
also influence the fitness and spatial consequences of
humanwildlife encounters. For example, disturbance will
have a higher impact on survival, reproduction and spatial
use if it occurs during especially vulnerable moments (e.g.
during territory establishment or when nestlings are small and
more vulnerable to predation or to anxiety-induced HPA axis
alterations) (Bolduc & Guillemette, 2003; Kapoor et al., 2006;
Buckley, 2011). Additionally, apart from the effect of habitat
quality on body condition mentioned above, habitat quality
may induce spatial-use changes, namely a trade-off between
the quality of the current habitat and that of alternative
areas. If alternative habitats are poor or not available,
animals may be reluctant to abandon an area despite
human activities (Gill et al., 2001; Weidinger, 2002; Lambert
& Kleindorfer, 2006; Bejder et al., 2009). Relatively high
predation pressure can also aggravate the detrimental effects
of human activities (Gutzwiller, Riffell & Anderson, 2002;
Bolduc & Guillemette, 2003). For example, human-induced
momentary abandonment of offspring might have a higher
impact in cases in which the number of predators and
their probability of accessing the offspring are higher (e.g.
ground breeding location versus trees or cliffs). Bad weather
and harsh climate will also aggravate the impact of human
disturbance on animal fitness. Difficult weather conditions
will increase both the energetic expenditure associated with
escaping and the likelihood of mortality of unattended and
unprotected offspring (Cowles, 1956; Buckley, 2004b, 2011;
Carr & Lima, 2012). Similarly, harsh climatic conditions
will limit the capacity to compensate for the effects of
disturbance, given that such conditions are usually associated
with poorer body condition, lower resource availability,
and shorter breeding seasons (Wingfield, 1984; Martin &
Wiebe, 2004).
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
Modulators of wildlife response to recreation 227
V. LEVEL 4: CONSEQUENCES OF HUMAN
DISTURBANCE FOR ANIMAL POPULATIONS
AND SPECIES
(1) Mechanisms
If human activities reduce the fitness of at least some
individuals of a population and/or change their habitat use,
population demographic processes (rates of reproduction,
survival, immigration, and emigration) may be negatively
affected. Thus, population growth rate may decrease and
even reach negative values. This reduction in population
growth rates may, in turn, result in reductions in population
size, increased probability of local extirpation, and potential
changes in distribution (Tuite et al., 1983; Pfister et al., 1992;
Stevens & Boness, 2003; Bejder et al., 2006; Gill, 2007;
Lusseau & Bejder, 2007).
(2) Modulators
Even though the association between demographic processes
and population growth seems straightforward, the final
impact will vary depending on characteristics of the
recreational activity, of the animals and of the context
(Table 1, Fig. 2). Regarding recreational activity, both the
spatial and temporal scale, as well as the pervasiveness of
human disturbance will affect the fate of populations. That is,
if the effect of disturbance is widespread throughout a larger
portion of a population or species range, and/or occurs
over long periods of the year, then we would expect greater
decreases in population growth rate than if disturbance only
affects a small part of the population locally or occurs only
occasionally (Lima, 1993; Riffell, Gutzwiller & Anderson,
1996; Finney, Pearce-Higgins & Yalden, 2005). If human
presence has a large ecological effect, that is, if it affects
several taxa simultaneously, disturbance-induced decreases
of competitor or predator species might indirectly favour
species expected to be negatively affected by humans.
Conversely, the direct effect of recreation on a given
species may be further aggravated if human activities
concurrently reduce other species on which they depend
(e.g. human-induced reductions of prey availability will
indirectly affect predators) (Skagen, Knight & Orians, 1991;
Hebblewhite et al., 2005; Price, 2008; Møller, 2012).
The effect of human activities at the population level
will also vary according to the properties of the populations
and species themselves. Differences in life-history traits (e.g.
survival rates, number of reproductive attempts, generation
times) will determine how quickly the population changes
in the face of reductions in reproduction or survival caused
by human recreation. For example, some species will be
more sensitive to decreases in survival, while other will
be more responsive to declines in reproduction (Heppell,
Caswell & Crowder, 2000; Saether & Bakke, 2000; Oli &
Dobson, 2003; Church et al., 2007; Lima, 2009). That is,
some species dynamics will be able to buffer or compensate
losses due to disturbance better than others. Higher degrees
of philopatry and site fidelity of a species (Yohannes, Hobson
& Pearson, 2007) will further modulate the consequences of
disturbance. That is, the reluctance to abandon a disturbed
area due to site fidelity in some species may produce an
evolutionary or ecological trap (Stamps & Krishnan, 1999;
Schlaepfer, Runge & Sherman, 2002; Igual et al., 2007;
Yohannes et al., 2007), leading to negative population trends.
Human recreation can also have stronger effects on social
species than on solitary ones, since disturbances in areas
where animals aggregate (e.g. bird colonies) will affect most
individuals simultaneously, which is further aggravated by
the usually higher site fidelity in gregarious species (Burger,
1981; Carney & Sydeman, 1999).
Additionally, characteristics such as population density
or meta-population dynamics are expected to have an
important role in modulating the effect of disturbance
at the population and species level. For example, small
populations, which are more vulnerable to stochasticity, are
more likely to be affected by human disturbances, even by
single events, that result in reductions in reproductive output,
habitat-use changes, or increased mortality that affect only
a few individuals (Lande, 1993; Lacy, 2000; McGowan et al.,
2011). On the other hand, in populations with high densities
entailing negative density dependence, the detrimental effects
of human disturbance could go unnoticed via release of
density dependence (Sinclair & Pech, 1996; Mallord et al.,
2007). When two or more populations are connected as a
meta-population system, recreational activities affecting sink
populations are expected to have a smaller impact because
the population is maintained primarily by immigration
from other sources. Conversely, in those instances in
which disturbance affects source populations, the overall
meta-population system might suffer the consequences
(Pulliam, 1988; Brawn & Robinson, 1996; Dias, 1996).
An important modulator of the consequences of distur-
bance at this level is the capacity of a given species or
population to adapt to humans, as demonstrated by the many
species that thrive in urban environments. That is, popula-
tions and species with a greater genetic and phenotypic
variability (e.g. variability of coping styles), as well as pheno-
typic plasticity, will be able to adjust more easily to human
presence by replacement of sensitive individuals by less sen-
sitive (usually bolder and less fearful) individuals (Carvalho,
1993; Lande & Shannon, 1996; Dufty, Clobert & Moller,
2002). However, adaptation to humans may come at a cost.
For example, reduction of fear will only be positive at the
population level if it does not increase the probability of being
depredated by non-human predators or result in a reduction
in reproduction (Dahlgren, 1990; Smith & Blumstein, 2008).
Finally, regardless of the species or human activities,
there exist many other factors that may modify or obscure
the demographic consequences of human disturbance by
causing similar or opposing effects. First, we emphasize that,
in most cases, human recreation is associated with habitat
deterioration (e.g. construction of roads or ski resorts) and
it is often difficult to separate the influence of direct human
presence from that of habitat degradation. Thus, habitat
modification associated with some recreational activities
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
228 Zulima Tablado and Lukas Jenni
may negatively affect populations by decreases in both
habitat quality and foraging opportunities. Clearly, this
might obscure or even worsen the consequences of stress
due to direct interactions with people (Cole & Landres,
1995; Buckley, 2004b; Braunisch, Patthey & Arlettaz, 2010).
Environmental changes independent of human disturbance
may influence population-level responses to recreation.
Factors with potentially positive effects on populations, such
as, for example, increases in reproduction, carrying capacity,
or immigration rates due to climate change, might increase
population growth rate obscuring or compensating for any
negative effects of disturbance (Ozgul et al., 2010; Tablado
& Revilla, 2012). On the other hand, factors that negatively
impact populations or species, such as biological invasions,
changes in land use, or detrimental effects of climate
change on ecosystems and migration patterns, might further
exacerbate negative effects of disturbance on populations
or species (Dextrase & Mandrak, 2006; Foden et al.,
2013).
VI. CONCLUSIONS
(1) From this synthesis, we conclude that the difficulties
in finding general patterns in the field of recreational
disturbance are due to three main reasons. First, for all
levels of response there are many diverse factors likely to
modulate the impact of human outdoor activities on wildlife,
and the combined influence of these factors within each
level may obscure or confound the effects of disturbance.
That is, the impact of human disturbance not only depends
on the intensity and type of the recreational activity, but
also on the characteristics of the species, and the context in
which interactions occur. For instance, whether individuals
respond behaviourally, physiologically, or not will depend
on their assessment of the risk imposed by humans, their
options to hide or escape, and the potential consequences
on their offspring’s and their own welfare. The impact of
recreational activities will also vary among populations or
species depending on a combination of factors (e.g. extent of
human activities, species ecology and context characteristics)
which may or may not allow them to compensate for or adapt
to disturbance.
(2) Second, the difficulty in finding general patterns
is due to the fact that some modulators might affect
wildlife responses in complex non-linear ways, such as the
bell-shaped dependence of behavioural response on flushing
performance (see Fig. 2D) (Ydenberg & Dill, 1986; Losos,
1990; Iriarte-Diaz, 2002). Many such relationships may
be non-linear; however, they still need to be explored. In
other instances, factor complexity results from contrasting
effects at different levels of response. For instance, group
size and predator (human) detection are positively related
(Schaik et al., 1983; Beauchamp, 2010). However, at the
next level, this relationship may not be positive because
animals in groups may feel safer than single animals and,
therefore, exhibit a dampened behavioural or physiological
reaction to humans (Fernandez-Juricic et al., 2002; Kikusui
et al., 2006). Moreover, the same modulating factor may
have contrasting impacts even within the same level of
response, depending on the response mechanism. This
entails a lack of correlation between different measures
of stress response, such as differences in behavioural versus
physiological responses (Killen et al., 2013). This would be,
for example, the case for body condition. Better body
condition tends to increase active behavioural responses
(e.g. flushing distance), however, the physiological response
tends to be dampened (i.e. HPA axis reactivity) as body
condition improves (Beale & Monaghan, 2004a;Breuner
et al., 2008).
(3) Third, another complication in finding general
patterns of the impacts of human disturbance on wildlife
exists because of the accumulation of modulating factors
across different levels of response which may confound the
patterns, especially at higher levels (such as reproductive
success, survival, or population and species trends) (Burger
et al., 1995; Gill et al., 2001; Price, 2008; Bonier et al., 2009).
The more levels of response (i.e. from Level 1 to 2 to 3,
etc.), the more factors accumulate, adding uncertainty to
the probability of finding generalizable associations (Fig. 1).
That is, even if negative reactions to human disturbance
are clearly observed at lower levels, we cannot readily infer
detrimental consequences at the demographic level since
the influence of many factors (e.g. habitat quality, previous
experience, climate, or density dependence) may confound
the expected effect at the population level.
(4) All the modulators and relationships considered here,
as well as those yet to be considered may explain why few
studies have shown general patterns for the relationships
between direct human disturbance and wildlife. However,
this should not discourage researchers in the field of human
disturbance. We rather hope to have set, with this synthesis,
a comprehensive basis that may be used as a future approach
and reference for identifying the type of factors that may
affect animals’ responses to human disturbance and the
complexity of their effects. There is a need for more research
to validate across populations and taxa the generality of
some of the modulating effects presented herein. Inclusion,
or at least acknowledgement, of factors that may modulate
responses to disturbance in a given system, and consideration
of the complexity and likely interactions of such factors
will facilitate improved study designs and interpretation
of findings. We are confident that doing so will improve
our understanding of the complex nature by which human
recreation influences wildlife.
VII. ACKNOWLEGDEMENTS
We thank Susi Jenni-Eiermann, Bettina Almasi, Benjamin
Homberger, Yves B¨
otsch, Juanita Olano, Gilberto Pasinelli,
and Michael Schaub for their comments on previous ver-
sions of the manuscript. We also thank two reviewers for their
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
Modulators of wildlife response to recreation 229
input, which substantially improved the manuscript, in par-
ticular Steve Schoech for additionally improving the English.
VIII. REFERENCES
Albrecht,T.&Klvana,P.(2004). Nest crypsis, reproductive value of a clutch
and escape decisions in incubating female mallards Anas platyrhynchos.Ethology 110,
603– 613.
Amo,L.,Caro,S.P.&Visser,M.E.(
2011). Sleeping birds do not respond to
predator odour. PLoS One 6, e27576.
Anderson,D.W.&Keith,J.O.(
1980). The human influence on seabird nesting
success: conservation implications. Biological Conservation 18, 65– 80.
Arlettaz,R.,Patthey,P.,Baltic,M.,Leu,T.,Schaub,M.,Palme,R.&
Jenni-Eiermann,S.(
2007). Spreading free-riding snow sports represent a novel
serious threat for wildlife. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences
274, 1219– 1224.
Atwell,J.W.,Cardoso,G.C.,Whittaker,D.J.,Campbell-Nelson,S.,
Robertson,K.W.&Ketterson,E.D.(
2012). Boldness behavior and stress
physiology in a novel urban environment suggest rapid correlated evolutionary
adaptation. Behavioral Ecology 23, 960– 969.
Balmford,A.,Beresford,J.,Green,J.,Naidoo,R.,Walpole,M.&Manica,
A. (2009). A global perspective on trends in nature-based tourism. PLoS Biology 7,
e1000144.
Barshep,Y.,Minton,C.D.T.,Underhill,L.G.,Erni,B.&Tomkovich,P.
(2013). Flexibility and constraints in the molt schedule of long-distance migratory
shorebirds: causes and consequences. Ecology and Evolution 3, 1967– 1976.
Bateman,P.W.&Fleming,P.A.(
2011). Who are you looking at? Hadeda ibises
use direction of gaze, head orientation and approach speed in their risk assessment
of a potential predator. Journal of Zoology 285, 316– 323.
Bateman,P.W.&Fleming,P.A.(
2014). Does human pedestrian behaviour
influence risk assessment in a successful mammal urban adapter? Journal of Zoology
294(2), 93– 98.
Bateman,P.W.,Fleming,P.A.,Jones,B.C.&Rothermel,B.B.(
2014). Defensive
responses of gopher tortoises (Gopherus polyphemus) are influenced by risk assessment
and level of habituation to humans. Behaviour 151, 1267– 1280.
Bates,L.A.,Sayialel,K.N.,Njiraini,N.W.,Moss,C.J.,Poole,J.H.&Byrne,
R. W. (2007). Elephants classify human ethnic groups by odor and garment color.
Current Biology 17, 1938– 1942.
Beale,C.M.&Monaghan,P.(
2004a). Behavioural responses to human disturbance:
a matter of choice? Animal Behaviour 68, 1065 –1069.
Beale,C.M.&Monaghan,P.(
2004b). Human disturbance: people as predation-free
predators? Journal of Applied Ecology 41, 335– 343.
Beauchamp,G.(
2010). A comparative analysis of vigilance in birds. Evolutionary Ecology
24, 1267– 1276.
Bejder,L.(
2005). Linking short and long-term effects of nature-based tourism on cetaceans.PhD
Thesis: Dalhousie University, Halifax.
Bejder,L.,Samuels,A.,Whitehead,H.,Finn,H.&Allen,S.(
2009). Impact
assessment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in
describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Marine Ecology Progress Series
395, 177– 185.
Bejder,L.,Samuels,A.,Whitehead,H.,Gales,N.,Mann,J.,Connor,
R., Heithaus,M.R.,Watson-Capps,J.,Flaherty,C.&Kr¨
utzen,M.
(2006). Decline in relative abundance of bottlenose dolphins exposed to long-term
disturbance. Conservation Biology 20, 1791– 1798.
Bellingham,D.L.,Sar,M.&Cidlowski,J.A.(
1992). Ligand-dependent
down-regulation of stably transfected human glucocorticoid receptors is associated
with the loss of functional glucocorticoid responsiveness. Molecular Endocrinology 6,
2090– 2102.
Berger,S.,Wikelski,M.,Romero,L.M.,Kalko,E.K.V.&R¨
odl,T.(
2007).
Behavioral and physiological adjustments to new predators in an endemic island
species, the Galapagos marine iguana. Hormones and Behavior 52, 653 663.
Bishop,C.M.(
2005). Circulatory variables and the flight performance of birds. Journal
of Experimental Biology 208, 1695– 1708.
Blumstein,D.T.(
2006). Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: how life history
and natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Animal Behaviour 71,
389– 399.
Blumstein,D.T.(
2010). Flush early and avoid the rush: a general rule of antipredator
behavior? Behavioral Ecology 21, 440– 442.
Blumstein,D.T.,Fernandez-Juricic,E.,LeDee, O., Larsen,E.,
Rodriguez-Prieto,I.&Zugmeyer,C.(
2004). Avian risk assessment: effects
of perching height and detectability. Ethology 110, 273– 285.
Boissy,A.(
1995). Fear and fearfulness in animals. The Quarterly Review of Biology 70,
165– 191.
B´
okony,V.,Lendvai,A.Z.,Liker,A.,Angelier,F., Wingfield,J.C.&Chastel,
O. (2009). Stress response and the value of reproduction: are birds prudent parents?
American Naturalist 173, 589 598.
Bolduc,F.&Guillemette,M.(
2003). Human disturbance and nesting success
of common eiders: interaction between visitors and gulls. Biological Conservation 110,
77– 83.
Bonier,F.(
2012). Hormones in the city: endocrine ecology of urban birds. Hormones
and Behavior 61, 763 772.
Bonier, F., Martin,P.R.,Moore,I.T.&Wingfield,J.C.(
2009). Do baseline
glucocorticoids predict fitness? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 24, 634– 642.
Bowles,A.E.(
1995). Responses of wildlife to noise. In Wildlife and Recreationists:
Coexistence Through Management and Research, pp. 109– 156. Island Press, Washington.
Boyle,S.A.&Samson,F.B.(
1985). Effects of nonconsumptive recreation on wildlife:
areview.Wildlife Society Bulletin 13, 110– 116.
Bracha,D.(
2004). Freeze, flight, fight, fright, faint: adaptationist perspectives on the
acute stress response spectrum. CNS Spectrums 9, 679–685.
Bra˜
na,F.(
1993). Shifts in body temperature and escape behaviour of female, Podarcis
muralis, during pregnancy. Oikos 66, 216– 222.
Braunisch,V.,Patthey,P.&Arlettaz,R.(
2010). Spatially explicit modeling of
conflict zones between wildlife and snow sports: prioritizing areas for winter refuges.
Ecological Applications 21, 955–967.
Brawn,J.D.&Robinson,S.K.(
1996). Source-sink population dynamics may
complicate the interpretation of long-term census data. Ecology 77, 3– 12.
Breuner,C.W.(
2010). Stress and reproduction in birds. In Hormones and Reproduction
of Vertebrates (eds D. O. Norris andK.H.Lopez), pp. 129 151. Academic Press,
London.
Breuner,C.W.&Orchinik,M.(
2002). Plasma binding proteins as mediators of
corticosteroid action in vertebrates. Journal of Endocrinology 175, 99– 112.
Breuner,C.W.,Patterson,S.H.&Hahn,T.P.(
2008). In search of relationships
between the acute adrenocortical response and fitness. General and Comparative
Endocrinology 157, 288– 295.
Buckley,R.(
2004a). Impacts of ecotourism on birds. In Environmental Impacts of
Ecotourism (ed. R. Buckley), pp. 187– 209. CABI, London.
Buckley,R.(
2004b). Impacts of ecotourism on terrestrial wildlife. In Environmental
Impacts of Ecotourism (ed. R. Buckley), pp. 211– 228. CABI, London.
Buckley,R.(
2009). Parks and tourism. PLoS Biology 7(6), e1000143.
Buckley,R.(
2011). Tourism and environment. Annual Review of Environment and
Resources 36, 397– 416.
Burger,J.(
1981). Effects of human disturbance on colonial species, particularly gulls.
Colonial Waterbirds 4,28–36.
Burger,J.(
1988). Social attraction in nesting least terns: effects of numbers, spacing,
and pair bonds. Condor 90, 575– 582.
Burger,J.&Gochfeld,M.(
1991). Human distance and birds: tolerance and
response distances of resident and migrant species in India. Environmental Conservation
18, 158– 165.
Burger,J.&Gochfeld,M.(
1998). Effects of ecotourists on bird behaviour at
Loxahatchee National Wildlife Refuge, Florida. Environmental Conservation 25,13–21.
Burger,J.,Gochfeld,M.&Niles,L.J.(
1995). Ecotourism and birds in coastal
New Jersey: contrasting responses of birds, tourists, and managers. Environmental
Conservation 22, 56– 65.
Camp,M.J.,Rachlow,J.L.,Woods,B.A.,Johnson,T.R.&Shipley,L.A.
(2012). When to run and when to hide: the influence of concealment, visibility, and
proximity to refugia on perceptions of risk. Ethology 118, 1010– 1017.
Carere,C.,Caramaschi,D.&Fawcett,T.W.(
2010). Covariation between
personalities and individual differences in coping with stress: converging evidence
and hypotheses. Current Zoology 56, 728– 740.
Carere,C.,Montanino,S.,Moreschini, F., Zoratto, F., Chiarotti, F.,
Santucci,D.&Alleva,E.(
2009). Aerial flocking patterns of wintering starlings,
Sturnus vulgaris, under different predation risk. Animal Behaviour 77, 101 –107.
Carlson,A.(
1985). Prey detection in the red-backed shrike (Lanius collurio): an
experimental study. Animal Behaviour 33, 1243 –1249.
Carney,K.M.&Sydeman,W.J.(
1999). A review of human disturbance effects on
nesting colonial waterbirds. Waterbirds: The International Journal of Waterbird Biology 22,
68– 79.
Carr,J.M.&Lima,S.L.(
2012). Heat-conserving postures hinder escape: a
thermoregulation-predation trade-off in wintering birds. Behavioural Ecology 23,
434– 441.
Carrete,M.&Tella,J.L.(
2011). Inter-individual variability in fear of humans and
relative brain size of the species are related to contemporary urban invasion in birds.
PLoS One 6, e18859.
Carter,J.,Lyons,N.J.,Cole,H.L.&Goldsmith,A.R.(
2008). Subtle cues of
predation risk: starlings respond to a predator’s direction of eye-gaze. Proceedings of
the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 275(1644), 1709–1715.
Cartledge,V.A.&Jones,S.M.(
2007). Does adrenal responsiveness vary with sex
and reproductive status in Egernia whitii, a viviparous skink? General and Comparative
Endocrinology 150, 132– 139.
Carvalho,G.R.(
1993). Evolutionary aspects of fish distribution: genetic variability
and adaptation. Journal of Fish Biology 43, 53– 73.
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
230 Zulima Tablado and Lukas Jenni
Church,D.R.,Bailey,L.L.,Wilbur,H.M.,Kendall,W.L.&Hines,J.E.
(2007). Iteroparity in the variable environment of the salamander Ambystoma tigrinum.
Ecology 88, 891– 903.
Cole,D.N.&Landres,P.B.(
1995). Indirect effects of recreation on wildlife. In
Wildlife and Recreationists.Coexistence Through Management and Research (eds R. L. Knight
and K. J. Gutzwiller), pp. 183– 202. Island Press, Washington.
Cooper,W.E.(
1997). Factors affecting risk and cost of escape by the broad-headed
skink (Eumeces laticeps): predator speed, directness of approach, and female presence.
Herpetologica 53,464–474.
Cooper,W.E.,Hawlena,D.&P´
erez-Mellado,V.(
2009). Effects of predation
risk factors on escape behavior by Balearic lizards (Podarcis lilfordi) in relation to
optimal escape theory. Amphibia-Reptilia 30, 99– 110.
Cowles,R.B.(
1956). Sidewinding locomotion in snakes. Copeia 4, 211– 214.
Crespi,E.J.,Williams,T.D.,Jessop,T.S.&Delehanty,B.(
2013). Life history
and the ecology of stress: how do glucocorticoid hormones influence life-history
variation in animals? Functional Ecology 27, 93– 106.
Cuthill,I.C.,Stevens,M.,Sheppard,J.,Maddocks,T.,Parraga,C.A.&
Troscianko,T.S.(
2005). Disruptive coloration and background pattern matching.
Nature 434,7274.
Dahlgren,J.(
1990). Females choose vigilant males: an experiment with the
monogamous grey partridge Perdix perdix.Animal Behaviour 39, 646 –651.
Desire,L.,Boissy,A.&Veissier,I.(
2002). Emotions in farm animals: a new
approach to animal welfare in applied ethology. Behavioural Processes 60, 165 –180.
Dextrase,A.J.&Mandrak,N.E.(
2006). Impacts of alien invasive species on
freshwater fauna at risk in Canada. Biological Invasions 8,13–24.
Dias,P.C.(
1996). Sources and sinks in population biology. Trends in Ecology and
Evolution 11, 326– 330.
Dill,L.M.&Houtman,R.(
1989). The influence of distance to refuge on flight
initiation distance in the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis). Canadian Journal of Zoology
67, 233– 235.
Dufty,A.M.Jr.,Clobert,J.&Moller,A.P.(
2002). Hormones, developmental
plasticity and adaptation. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 17, 190– 196.
Dupuch,A.,Magnan,P.&Dill,L.M.(
2004). Sensitivity of northern redbelly dace,
Phoxinus eos, to chemical alarm cues. Canadian Journal of Zoology 82, 407–415.
Elbaz,I.,Foulkes,N.S.,Gothilf,Y.&Appelbaum,L.(
2013). Circadian clocks,
rhythmic synaptic plasticity and the sleep-wake cycle in zebrafish. Frontiers in Neural
Circuits 7,9.
Elenkov,I.J.(
2004). Glucocorticoids and the Th1/Th2 balance. Annals of the New
York Academy of Sciences 1024, 138–146.
Ellison,L.N.&Cleary,L.(
1978). Effects of human disturbance on breeding of
double-crested cormorants. Auk 95, 510– 517.
Espmark,Y.&Langvatn,R.(
1985). Development and habituation of cardiac and
behavioral responses in young red deer calves (Cervus elaphus) exposed to alarm
stimuli. Journal of Mammalogy 66, 702– 711.
Ewert,J.P.,Buxbaum-Conradi,H.,Dreisvogt, F., Glagow,M.,
Merkel-Harff,C.,R¨
ottgen,A.,Sch ¨
urg-Pfeiffer,E.&Schwippert,W.
W. (2001). Neural modulation of visuomotor functions underlying prey-catching
behaviour in anurans: perception, attention, motor performance, learning.
Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology 128,
417– 460.
Fedosenko,A.K.&Blank,D.A.(
2005). Ovis ammon. Mammalian Species 773, 1 –15.
Fernandez-Juricic,E.,Deisher,M.,Stark,A.C.&Randolet,J.(
2012). Predator
detection is limited in microhabitats with high light intensity: an experiment with
brown-headed cowbirds. Ethology 118, 341– 350.
Fernandez-Juricic,E.,Jimenez,M.D.&Lucas,E.(
2002). Factors affecting
intra-and inter-specific variations in the difference between alert distances and flight
distances for birds in forested habitats. Canadian Journal of Zoology 80, 1212– 1220.
Fernandez-Juricic,E.&Schroeder,N.(
2003). Do variations in scanning behavior
affect tolerance to human disturbance? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 84, 219– 234.
Finney,S.K.,Pearce-Higgins,J.W.&Yalden,D.W.(
2005). The effect of
recreational disturbance on an upland breeding bird, the golden plover Pluvialis
apricaria.Biological Conservation 121, 53– 63.
Fisher,L.(
1990). Stress and cardiovascular physiology in animals. In Stress: Neurobiology
and Neuroendocrinology (eds M. R. Brown,G.F.Koob and C. Rivier), pp. 463– 474.
Marcel Dekker, New York.
Foden,W.B.,Butchart,S.H.M.,Stuart,S.N.,Vie,J.C.,Akcakaya,H.R.,
Angulo,A.,DeVantier,L.M.,Gutsche,A.,Turak,E.,Cao,L.,Donner,S.
D., Katariya,V.,Bernard,R.,Holland,R.A.,Hughes, A. F., O’Hanlon,
S. E., Garnett,S.T.,Sekercioglu,C.H.&Ace,G.M.(
2013). Identifying the
world’s most climate change vulnerable species: a systematic trait-based assessment
of all birds, amphibians and corals. PLoS One 8, e65427.
Fowler,G.S.(
1999). Behavioral and hormonal responses of Magellanic penguins
(Spheniscus magellanicus) to tourism and nest site visitation. Biological Conservation 90,
143– 149.
French,S.S.,Gonz´
alez-Su´
arez,M.,Young,J.K.,Durham,S.&Gerber,L.
R. (2011). Human disturbance influences reproductive success and growth rate in
California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). PLoS One 6, e17686.
Frid,A.&Dill,L.M.(
2002). Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of
predation risk. Conservation Ecology 6, 11.
Gabrielsen,G.W.,Blix,A.S.&Ursin,H.(
1985). Orienting and freezing responses
in incubating ptarmigan hens. Physiology and Behavior 34, 925 –934.
Gabrielsen,G.W.&Smith,E.N.(
1995). Physiological responses of wildlife to
disturbance. In Wildlife and Recreationists.Coexistence Through Management and Research
(eds R. L. Knight and K. J. Gutzwiller), pp. 183– 202. Island Press, Washington.
Gasser,P.J.,Lowry,C.A.&Orchinik,M.(
2009). Rapid corticosteroid actions on
behavior: mechanisms and implications. Rapid corticosteroid actions on behavior:
mechanisms and implications. In Hormones, Brain and Behavior (eds D. W. Pfaff,A.
P. Arnold,S.E.Fahrbach,A.M.Etgen and R. T. Rubin), pp. 1365 1387.
Academic Press, New York.
Geist,C.,Liao,J.,Libby,S.&Blumstein,D.T.(
2007). Does intruder group
size and orientation affect flight initiation distance in birds? Animal Biodiversity and
Conservation 28, 69– 73.
Gill,J.A.(
2007). Approaches to measuring the effects of human disturbance on birds.
Ibis 149, 9–14.
Gill,J.A.,Norris,K.&Sutherland,W.J.(
2001). Why behavioural responses may
not reflect the population consequences of human disturbance. Biological Conservation
97, 265– 268.
Gomez,D.&Thery,M.(
2007). Simultaneous crypsis and conspicuousness in color
patterns: comparative analysis of a neotropical rainforest bird community. American
Naturalist 169, S42– S61.
Gonz´
alez,L.M.,Arroyo,B.E.,Margalida,A.,S´
anchez,R.&Oria,J.(
2006).
Effect of human activities on the behaviour of breeding Spanish imperial eagles
(Aquila adalberti): management implications for the conservation of a threatened
species. Animal Conservation 9, 85– 93.
Gordon,C.E.,Dickman,C.R.&Thompson,M.B.(
2010). What factors
allow opportunistic nocturnal activity in a primarily diurnal desert lizard (Ctenotus
pantherinus)? Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A: Molecular & Integrative
Physiology 156, 255– 261.
Goutte,A.,Antoine,E.,Weimerskirch,H.&Chastel,O.(
2010). Age and the
timing of breeding in a long-lived bird: a role for stress hormones? Functional Ecology
24, 1007– 1016.
Groeneweg,F.L.,Karst,H.,de Kloet,E.R.&Joels,M.(
2011). Rapid
non-genomic effects of corticosteroids and their role in the central stress response.
Journal of Endocrinology 209, 153– 167.
Gutzwiller,K.J.&Marcum,H.A.(
1997). Bird reactions to observer clothing color:
implications for distance-sampling techniques. The Journal of Wildlife Management 61,
935– 947.
Gutzwiller,K.J.,Riffell,S.K.&Anderson,S.H.(
2002). Repeated human
intrusion and the potential for nest predation by gray jays. Journal of Wildlife
Management 66, 372– 380.
Hagelin,J.C.&Jones,I.J.(
2007). Bird odors and other chemical substances: a
defense mechanism or overlooked mode of intraspecific communication? Auk 124,
741– 761.
Harper,W.L.&Eastman,D.S.(
2000). Wildlife and commercial backcountry
recreation in British Columbia: assessment of impacts and interim guidelines for
mitigation, Discussion Paper. Wildlife Branch Ministry of Environment, Lands and
Parks Victoria.
Hayward,L.S.&Wingfield,J.C.(
2004). Maternal corticosterone is transferred
to avian yolk and may alter offspring growth and adult phenotype. General and
Comparative Endocrinology 135(3), 365– 371.
Hebblewhite,M.,White,C.A.,Nietvelt,C.G.,McKenzie,J.A.,Hurd,T.E.,
Fryxell,J.M.,Bayley,S.E.&Paquet,P.C.(
2005). Human activity mediates
a trophic cascade caused by wolves. Ecology 86, 2135– 2144.
Helfman,G.S.(
1989). Threat-sensitive predator avoidance in damselfish-trumpetfish
interactions. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 24, 47– 58.
Heppell,S.S.,Caswell,H.&Crowder,L.B.(
2000). Life histories and elasticity
patterns: perturbation analysis for species with minimal demographic data. Ecology
81, 654– 665.
Hilton,G.M.,Ruxton,G.D.&Cresswell,W.(
1999). Choice of foraging area
with respect to predation risk in redshanks: the effects of weather and predator
activity. Oikos 87, 295– 302.
Igual,J.M.,Forero,M.G.,Gomez,T.&Oro,D.(
2007). Can an introduced
predator trigger an evolutionary trap in a colonial seabird? Biological Conservation 137,
189– 196.
Ikuta,L.A.&Blumstein,D.T.(
2003). Do fences protect birds from human
disturbance? Biological Conservation 112, 447– 452.
Ingram,D.K.(
2000). Age-related decline in physical activity: generalization to
nonhumans. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 32, 1623–1629.
Iriarte-Diaz,J.(
2002). Differential scaling of locomotor performance in small and
large terrestrial mammals. Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 2897– 2908.
Irschick,D.J.,Carlisle,E.,Elstrott,J.,Ramos,M.,Buckley,C.,
Vanhooydonck,B.,Meyers,J.&Herrel,A.(
2005). A comparison of habitat
use, morphology, clinging performance and escape behaviour among two divergent
green anole lizard (Anolis carolinensis) populations. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society
85, 223– 234.
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
Modulators of wildlife response to recreation 231
Jacobsen,N.K.(1979). Alarm bradycardia in white-tailed deer fawns (Odocoileus
virginianus). Journal of Mammalogy 60, 343–349.
Januchowski-Hartley,F.A.,Graham,N.A.J.,Feary,D.A.,Morove,T.&
Cinner,J.E.(
2011). Fear of fishers: human predation explains behavioral changes
in coral reef fishes. PLoS One 6, e22761.
Jessop,T.S.,Woodford,R.&Symonds,M.R.E.(
2013). Macrostress: do large-scale
ecological patterns exist in the glucocorticoid stress response of vertebrates? Functional
Ecology 27, 120– 130.
Jiang,T.,Wang,X.,Ding,Y.,Liu,Z.&Wang,Z.(
2013). Behavioral responses
of blue sheep (Pseudois nayaur) to nonlethal human recreational disturbance. Chinese
Science Bulletin 58, 1– 11.
Jones,M.P.,Pierce,K.E.Jr.&Ward,D.(
2007). Avian vision: a review of form
and function with special consideration to birds of prey. Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine
16, 69– 87.
Kapoor,A.,Dunn,E.,Kostaki,A.,Andrews,M.H.&Matthews,S.G.(
2006).
Fetal programming of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal function: prenatal stress and
glucocorticoids. Journal of Physiology 572, 31– 44.
Karp,D.&Root,T.(
2009). Sound the stressor: how Hoatzins (Opisthocomus hoazin)
react to ecotourist conversation. Biodiversity and Conservation 18, 3733– 3742.
Keller,V.(
1996). Effects and management of disturbance of waterbirds by human
recreational activities: a review. Gibier Faune Sauvage 13, 1039 –1047.
Kelley,J.L.&Magurran,A.E.(
2003). Learned predator recognition and
antipredator responses in fishes. Fish and Fisheries 4, 216 –226.
Kerley,G.I.H.,Kowalczyk,R.&Cromsigt,J.P.G.M.(
2012). Conservation
implications of the refugee species concept and the European bison: king of the
forest or refugee in a marginal habitat? Ecography 35, 519– 529.
Kikusui,T.,Winslow,J.T.&Mori,Y.(
2006). Social buffering: relief from stress
and anxiety. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 361,
2215– 2228.
Killen,S.S.,Marras,S.,Metcalfe,N.B.,McKenzie,D.J.&Domenici,P.(
2013).
Environmental stressors alter relationships between physiology and behaviour. Trends
in Ecology and Evolution 28, 651– 658.
Kiltie,R.A.(
2000). Scaling of visual acuity with body size in mammals and birds.
Functional Ecology 14, 226– 234.
Kitaysky,A.S.,Kitaiskaia,E.V.,Piatt,J.F.&Wingfield,J.C.(
2003). Benefits
and costs of increased levels of corticosterone in seabird chicks. Hormones and Behavior
43(1), 140– 149.
Klaassen,M.,Bauer,S.,Madsen,J.&Tombre,I.(
2006). Modelling behavioural
and fitness consequences of disturbance for geese along their spring flyway. Journal
of Applied Ecology 43, 92– 100.
Knight,R.L.&Cole,D.N.(
1995). Wildlife responses to recreationists. In Wildlife
and Recreationists: Coexistence Through Management and Research, pp. 51– 69. Island Press,
Washington.
Knight,R.L.&Gutzwiller,K.J.(
1995). Wildlife and Recreationists: Coexistence Through
Management and Research. Island Press, Washington.
Knight,S.K.&Knight,R.L.(
1986). Vigilance patterns of bald eagles feeding in
groups. The Auk 103, 263 –272.
Koteja,P.(
1996). Limits to the energy budget in a rodent, Peromyscus maniculatus: does
gut capacity set the limit? Physiological Zoology 69, 994– 1020.
Kury,C.R.&Gochfeld,M.(
1975). Human interference and gull predation in
cormorant colonies. Biological Conservation 8, 23– 34.
Lacy,R.C.(
2000). Considering threats to the viability of small populations using
individual-based models. Ecological Bulletins 48, 39–51.
Lambert,S.&Kleindorfer,S.(
2006). Nest concealment but not human visitation
predicts predation of New Holland honeyeater nests. Emu 106, 63– 68.
Lande,R.(
1993). Risks of population extinction from demographic and environmental
stochasticity and random catastrophes. American Naturalist 142(6), 911 927.
Lande,R.&Shannon,S.(
1996). The role of genetic variation in adaptation and
population persistence in a changing environment. Evolution 50, 434– 437.
Lattin,C.R.,Keniston,D.E.,Reed,J.M.&Romero,L.M.(
2015). Are
receptor concentrations correlated across tissues within individuals? A case study
examining glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptor binding. Endocrinology
156(4), 1354– 1361.
Lazarus,J.&Symonds,M.(
1992). Contrasting effects of protective and obstructive
cover on avian vigilance. Animal Behaviour 43, 519 521.
Lendvai,A.Z.,B´
okony,V.,Angelier, F., Chastel,O.&Sol,D.(
2013). Do smart
birds stress less? An interspecific relationship between brain size and corticosterone
levels. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 280, 20131734.
Lendvai,A.Z.&Chastel,O.(
2008). Experimental mate-removal increases the
stress response of female house sparrows: the effects of offspring value? Hormones and
Behavior 53,395–401.
Lima,S.L.(
1993). Ecological and evolutionary perspectives on escape from predatory
attack: a survey of North American birds. Wilson Bulletin 105, 1– 47.
Lima,S.L.(
2009). Predators and the breeding bird: behavioral and reproductive
flexibility under the risk of predation. Biological Reviews 84, 485 –513.
Lima,S.L.&Dill,L.M.(
1990). Behavioral decisions made under the risk of
predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68, 619– 640.
Llewelyn,J.,Webb,J.K.&Shine,R.(
2010). Flexible defense: context-dependent
antipredator responses of two species of Australian elapid snakes. Herpetologica 66,
1– 11.
Losos,J.B.(
1990). The evolution of form and function: morphology and locomotor
performance in West Indian Anolis Lizards. Evolution 44, 1189– 1203.
Lusseau,D.&Bejder,L.(
2007). The long-term consequences of short-term responses
to disturbance experiences from whalewatching impact assessment. International
Journal of Comparative Psychology 20, 228– 236.
MacArthur,R.A.,Geist,V.&Johnston,R.H.(
1982). Cardiac and behavioral
responses of mountain sheep to human disturbance. Journal of Wildlife Management
46, 351– 358.
MacKinnon,J.C.(
1972). Summer storage of energy and its use for winter metabolism
and gonad maturation in American Plaice (Hippoglossoides platessoides). Journal of the
Fisheries Research Board of Canada 29, 1749– 1759.
Madsen,J.(
1995). Impacts of disturbance on migratory waterfowl. Ibis 137, S67– S74.
Major,R.E.(
1990). The effect of human observers on the intensity of nest predation.
Ibis 132, 608– 612.
Malisch,J.L.&Breuner,C.W.(
2010). Steroid-binding proteins and free steroids
in birds. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology 316, 42– 52.
Mallord,J.W.,Dolman,P.M.,Brown,A.F.&Sutherland,W.J.(
2007).
Linking recreational disturbance to population size in a ground-nesting passerine.
Journal of Applied Ecology 44, 185– 195.
Malo,J.,Acebes,P.&Traba,J.(
2011). Measuring ungulate tolerance to human
with flight distance: a reliable visitor management tool? Biodiversity and Conservation
20, 3477– 3488.
Margalida,A.,Moreno-Opo,R.,Arroyo,B.E.&Arredondo,A.(
2011).
Reconciling the conservation of endangered species with economically important
anthropogenic activities: interactions between cork exploitation and the cinereous
vulture in Spain. Animal Conservation 14, 167– 174.
Martin,G.R.(
2011). Understanding bird collisions with man-made objects: a sensory
ecology approach. Ibis 153, 239– 254.
Martin,K.&Wiebe,K.L.(
2004). Coping mechanisms of alpine and arctic breeding
birds: extreme weather and limitations to reproductive resilience. Integrative and
Comparative Biology 44, 177– 185.
Martinetto,K.&Cugnasse,J.M.(
2001). Reaction distance in Mediterranean
Mouflon (Ovis gmelini musimon x Ovis sp.) in the presence of hikers with a dog on the
Caroux plateau (H´
erault, France). Revue d’Ecologie 56, 231–242.
McGowan,C.P.,Ryan,M.R.,Runge,M.C.,Millspaugh,J.J.&Cochrane,
J. F. (2011). The role of demographic compensation theory in incidental take
assessments for endangered species. Biological Conservation 144, 730– 737.
Millar,J.S.(
1978). Energetics of reproduction in Peromyscus leucopus:thecostof
lactation. Ecology 59, 1055– 1061.
Miller,S.G.,Knight,R.L.&Clinton,K.M.(
2001). Wildlife responses to
pedestrians and dogs. Wildlife Society Bulletin 29, 124– 132.
Møller,A.P.(
2012). Urban areas as refuges from predators and flight distance of
prey. Behavioral Ecology 23, 1030– 1035.
Møller,A.P.(
2014). Life history, predation and flight initiation distance in a
migratory bird. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 27(6), 1105– 1113.
Møller,A.P.,V´
ag´
asi,C.I.&Pap,P.L.(
2013). Risk-taking and the evolution
of mechanisms for rapid escape from predators. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 26(5),
1143– 1150.
Moore,I.T.&Jessop,T.S.(
2003). Stress, reproduction, and adrenocortical
modulation in amphibians and reptiles. Hormones and Behavior 43,39–47.
Moreno,J.(
1984). Search strategies of wheatears (Oenanthe oenanthe) and stonechats
(Saxicola torquata): adaptive variation in perch height, search time, sally distance and
inter-perch move length. Journal of Animal Ecology 53, 147– 159.
M¨
ullner,A.,Eduard Linsenmair,K.&Wikelski,M.(
2004). Exposure to
ecotourism reduces survival and affects stress response in hoatzin chicks (Opisthocomus
hoazin). Biological Conservation 118, 549– 558.
Niles,L.J.&Clark,K.E.(
1989). Prey management for migrating raptors. In
Proceedings of the Northeast Raptor Management Symposium and Workshop, pp. 154– 161.
National Wildlife Federation, Washington.
Oli,M.K.&Dobson,F.S.(
2003). The relative importance of lifehistory variables
to population growth rate in mammals: cole’s prediction revisited. American Naturalist
161, 422– 440.
Olsson, M. P. O., Cox,J.,Larkin,J.,Maehr,D.,Widen,P.&Wichrowski,M.
(2007). Movement and activity patterns of translocated elk (Cervus elaphus nelsoni)on
an active coal mine in Kentucky. Wildlife Biology in Practice 3,18.
O’Reilly,K.M.&Wingfield,J.C.(
2001). Ecological factors underlying the
adrenocortical response to capture stress in arctic-breeding shorebirds. General and
Comparative Endocrinology 124, 1– 11.
Øverli, Ø., Sørensen,C.,Pulman,K.G.T.,Pottinger,T.G.,Korzan,
W., Summers,C.H.&Nilsson,G.E.(
2007). Evolutionary background for
stress-coping styles: relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive
traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 31, 396 –412.
Ozgul,A.,Childs,D.Z.,Oli,M.K.,Armitage,K.B.,Blumstein,D.T.,
Olson,L.E.,Tuljapurkar,S.&Coulson,T.(
2010). Coupled dynamics of
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
232 Zulima Tablado and Lukas Jenni
body mass and population growth in response to environmental change. Nature 466,
482– 485.
Paquet,P.C.&Darimont,C.T.(
2010). Wildlife conservation and animal welfare:
two sides of the same coin? Animal Welfare 19(2), 177– 190.
Peczely,P.(
1979). Effect of testosterone and thyroxine on corticosterone and
transcortine plasma levels in different bird species. Acta Physiologica Academiae
Scientiarum Hungaricae 53, 9–15.
Pfister,C.,Harrington,B.A.&Lavine,M.(
1992). The impact of human
disturbance on shorebirds at a migration staging area. Biological Conservation 60,
115– 126.
Piersma,T.(
1990). Pre-migratory ‘‘fattening’’ usually involves more than the
deposition of fat alone. Ringing & Migration 11, 113– 115.
Price,M.(
2008). The impact of human disturbance on birds: a selective review. In
Too Close for Comfort: Conflicts in Human Wildlife Encounters (eds D. Lunney,A.Munn
and W. Meikle), pp. 163– 196. Royal Zoological Society of NSW, Sydney.
Pride,R.E.(
2005). Optimal group size and seasonal stress in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur
catta). Behavioral Ecology 16,550–560.
Pulliam,H.R.(
1988). Sources, sinks, and population regulation. American Naturalist
132,652–661.
Ramp,D.,Russell,B.G.&Croft,D.B.(
2005). Predator scent induces differing
responses in two sympatric macropodids. Australian Journal of Zoology 53, 73– 78.
Reeder,D.M.&Kramer,K.M.(
2005). Stress in free-ranging mammals: integrating
physiology, ecology, and natural history. Journal of Mammalogy 86, 225– 235.
Rensel,M.A.,Boughton,R.K.&Schoech,S.J.(
2010). Development of the
adrenal stress response in the Florida scrub-jay (Aphelocoma coerulescens). General and
Comparative Endocrinology 165(2), 255– 261.
Riffell,S.K.,Gutzwiller,K.J.&Anderson,S.H.(
1996). Does repeated human
intrusion cause cumulative declines in avian richness and abundance? Ecological
Applications 6, 492– 505.
Ríos-Jara,E.,Galv´
an-Villa,C.M.,Rodríguez-Zaragoza,F.A.,
L´
opez-Uriarte,E.&Mu ˜
noz-Fern´
andez,V.T.(
2013). The tourism carrying
capacity of underwater trails in Isabel Island National Park, Mexico. Environmental
Management 52, 1– 13.
Riou,S.,Chastel, O., Lacroix,A.&Hamer,K.C.(
2010). Stress and parental
care: prolactin responses to acute stress throughout the breeding cycle in a long-lived
bird. General and Comparative Endocrinology 168, 8–13.
Robert,H.C.&Ralph,C.J.(
1975). Effects of human disturbance on the breeding
success of gulls. Condor 77, 495– 499.
R¨
odl,T.,Berger,S.,Romero,L.M.&Wikelski,M.(
2007). Tameness and stress
physiology in a predator-naive island species confronted with novel predation threat.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 274, 577– 582.
Rollins-Smith,L.A.(
2001). Neuroendocrine-immune system interactions in
amphibians. Immunologic Research 23, 273– 280.
Romero,L.M.(
2002). Seasonal changes in plasma glucocorticoid concentrations in
free-living vertebrates. General and Comparative Endocrinology 128, 1– 24.
Romero,L.M.(
2004). Physiological stress in ecology: lessons from biomedical
research. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 19, 249– 255.
Romero,L.M.&Butler,L.K.(
2007). Endocrinology of stress. International Journal
of Comparative Psychology 20, 89– 95.
Romero,L.M.,Dickens,M.J.&Cyr,N.E.(
2009). The reactive scope model– a
new model integrating homeostasis, allostasis, and stress. Hormones and Behavior 55,
375– 389.
Romero,L.M.,Reed,J.M.&Wingfield,J.C.(
2000). Effects of weather on
corticosterone responses in wild free-living passerine birds. General and Comparative
Endocrinology 118, 113– 122.
Romero,L.M.&Remage-Healey,L.(
2000). Daily and seasonal variation in
response to stress in captive starlings (Sturnus vulgaris): corticosterone. General and
Comparative Endocrinology 119, 52– 59.
Roth,T.C.,Cox,J.G.&Lima,S.L.(
2008). Can foraging birds assess predation risk
by scent? Animal Behaviour 76, 2021 2027.
Saether,B.-E.&Bakke.(
2000). Avian life history variation and contribution of
demographic traits to the population growth rate. Ecology 81, 642– 653.
Sapolsky,R.M.,Romero,L.M.&Munck,A.U.(
2000). How do glucocorticoids
influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and
preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews 21, 55–89.
Sarabdjitsingh,R.A.,Jo¨
els,M.&De Kloet,E.R.(
2012). Glucocorticoid
pulsatility and rapid corticosteroid actions in the central stress response. Physiology
and Behavior 106, 73 80.
Schaefer,H.C.,Jetz,W.&B¨
ohning-Gaese,K.(
2008). Impact of climate change
on migratory birds: community reassembly versus adaptation. Global Ecology and
Biogeography 17, 38– 49.
Schaik,C.,Noordwijk,M.,Warsono,B.&Sutriono,E.(
1983). Party size and
early detection of predators in Sumatran forest primates. Primates 24, 211– 221.
Schlaepfer,M.A.,Runge,M.C.&Sherman,P.W.(
2002). Ecological and
evolutionary traps. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 17, 474– 480.
Schmid,B.,Tam-Dafond,L.,Jenni-Eiermann,S.,Arlettaz,R.,Schaub,M.
&Jenni,L.(
2013). Modulation of the adrenocortical response to acute stress with
respect to brood value, reproductive success and survival in the Eurasian hoopoe.
Oecologia 173, 33– 44.
Schoech,S.J.,Rensel,M.A.&Heiss,R.S.(
2011). Short and long-term effects of
developmental corticosterone exposure on avian physiology, behavioral phenotype,
cognition, and fitness: a review. Current Zoology 57, 514– 530.
Schoech,S.J.,Romero,L.M.,Moore,I.T.&Bonier,F.(
2013). Constraints,
concerns and considerations about the necessity of estimating free glucocorticoid
concentrations for field endocrine studies. Functional Ecology 27, 1100– 1106.
Sedinger,J.S.(
1997). Adaptations to and consequences of an herbivorous diet in
grouse and waterfowl. Condor 99, 314– 326.
Seltmann,M.W., ¨
Ost,M.,Jaatinen,K.,Atkinson,S.,Mashburn,K.&
Hollm´
en,T.(
2012). Stress responsiveness, age and body condition interactively
affect flight initiation distance in breeding female eiders. Animal Behaviour 84(4),
889– 896.
Serrano,D.,Forero,M.G.,Don´
azar,J.A.&Tella,J.L.(
2004). Dispersal and
social attraction affect colony selection and dynamics of lesser kestrels. Ecology 85,
3438– 3447.
Siegel,I.M.(
1972). Optics and visual physiology. Archives of Ophthalmology 88,212–227.
Silverin,B.&Wingfield,J.C.(
1998). Adrenocortical responses to stress in breeding
pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca: relation to latitude, sex and mating status. Journal
of Avian Biology 29, 228– 234.
Sinclair,D.&Jayawardena,C.C.(
2010). Tourism in the Amazon: identifying
challenges and finding solutions. Worldwide Hospitality and Tourism Themes 2,124–135.
Sinclair,A.R.E.&Pech,R.P.(
1996). Density dependence, dtochasticity,
compensation and predator regulation. Oikos 75, 164– 173.
Skagen,S.K.,Knight,R.L.&Orians,G.H.(
1991). Human disturbance of an
avian scavenging guild. Ecological Applications 1, 215–225.
Smith,G.C.(
1976). Ecological energetics of three species of ectothermic vertebrates.
Ecology 57, 252– 264.
Smith,B.R.&Blumstein,D.T.(
2008). Fitness consequences of personality: a
meta-analysis. Behavioral Ecology 19, 448– 455.
Smith,M.E.,Kane,A.S.&Popper,A.N.(
2004). Noise-induced stress response and
hearing loss in goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal of Experimental Biology 207, 427 –435.
Sokolov,E.N.,Waydenfeld,S.W.,Worters,R.&Clarke,A.D.B.(
1963).
Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. Pergamon Press, Oxford.
Solomon,M.B.(
2009). Sex differences in HPA-axis regulation: the role of gonadal
hormones. In Hormones, Brain and Behavior. Second Edition (D. W. Pfaff,A.P.
Arnold,S.E.Fahrbach,A.M.Etgen,R.T.Rubin, eds)., pp. 2291 2306.
Academic Press, New York.
St Clair,J.J.H.,García-Pe ˜
na,G.E.,Woods,R.W.&Sz ´
ekely,T.(
2010).
Presence of mammalian predators decreases tolerance to human disturbance in a
breeding shorebird. Behavioral Ecology 21, 1285– 1292.
Stamps,J.A.&Krishnan,V.V.(
1999). A learning-based model of territory
establishment. Quarterly Review of Biology 74, 291– 318.
Stankowich,T.(
2008). Ungulate flight responses to human disturbance: a review
and meta-analysis. Biological Conservation 141, 2159– 2173.
Stankowich,T.&Blumstein,D.T.(
2005). Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and
review of risk assessment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences
272, 2627– 2634.
Stankowich,T.&Coss,R.G.(
2006). Effects of predator behavior and
proximity on risk assessment by Columbian black-tailed deer. Behavioral Ecology
17, 246– 254.
Steen,J.B.,Gabrielsen,G.W.&Kanwisher,J.W.(
1988). Physiological aspects
of freezing behaviour in willow ptarmigan hens. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 134,
299– 304.
Steidl,R.J.&Anthony,R.G.(
1996). Responses of bald eagles to human activity
during the summer in interior Alaska. Ecological Applications 6, 482 –491.
Steven,R.,Pickering,C.&Castley,J.G.(
2011).Areviewofthe
impacts of nature based recreation on birds. Journal of Environmental Management
92, 2287.
Stevens,M.A.&Boness,D.J.(
2003). Influences of habitat features and human
disturbance on use of breeding sites by a declining population of southern fur seals
(Arctocephalus australis). Journal of Zoology 260, 145–152.
Stillman,R.A.&Goss-Custard,J.D.(
2002). Seasonal changes in the response of
oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus to human disturbance. Journal of Avian Biology 33,
358– 365.
Strang,C.A.(
1980). Incidence of avian predators near people searching for waterfowl
nests. Journal of Wildlife Management 44, 220 222.
Tablado,Z.&Revilla,E.(
2012). Contrasting effects of climate change on rabbit
populations through reproduction. PLoS One 7, e48988.
Tadesse,S.A.&Kotler,B.P.(
2012). Impact of tourism on Nubian Ibex (Capra
nubiana) revealed through assessment of behavioral indicators. Behavioral Ecology 23(6),
1257– 1262.
Tarlow,E.M.&Blumstein,D.T.(
2007). Evaluating methods to quantify
anthropogenic stressors on wild animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102,
429– 451.
Taylor,A.R.&Knight,R.L.(
2003). Wildlife responses to recreation and associated
visitor perceptions. Ecological Applications 13, 951 –963.
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
Modulators of wildlife response to recreation 233
Thaker,M.,Lima,S.L.&Hews,D.K.(2009a). Acute corticosterone elevation
enhances antipredator behaviors in male tree lizard morphs. Hormones and Behavior
56, 51– 57.
Thaker,M.,Lima,S.L.&Hews,D.K.(
2009b). Alternative antipredator tactics in
tree lizard morphs: hormonal and behavioural responses to a predator encounter.
Animal Behaviour 77, 395 –401.
Thaker,M.,Vanak,A.T.,Lima,S.L.&Hews,D.K.(
2010). Stress and aversive
learning in a wild vertebrate: the role of corticosterone in mediating escape from a
novel stressor. American Naturalist 175, 50 60.
Thiel,D.,Jenni-Eiermann,S.,Braunisch,V.,Palme,R.&Jenni,L.(
2008). Ski
tourism affects habitat use and evokes a physiological stress response in capercaillie,
Tetrao urogallus: a new methodological approach. Journal of Applied Ecology 45, 845 –853.
Thiel,D.,Menoni,E.,Brenot,J.&Jenni,L.(
2007). Effects of recreation and
hunting on flushing distance of capercaillie. Journal of Wildlife Management 71,
1784– 1792.
Tilbrook,A.J.,Turner,A.I.,Ibbott,M.D.&Clarke,I.J.(
2006). Activation
of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis by isolation and restraint stress during
lactation in ewes: effect of the presence of the lamb and suckling. Endocrinology 147,
3501– 3509.
Tillmann,J.E.(
2009). Fear of the dark: night-time roosting and anti-predation
behaviour in the grey partridge (Perdix perdix L.). Behaviour 146, 999–1023.
Tisdell,C.A.(
2010). Antarctic tourism: environmental concerns and the importance
of Antarctica’s natural attractions for tourists. School of Economics, University of
Queensland, Brisbane.
Torner,L.&Neumann,I.D.(
2002). The brain prolactin system: involvement in
stress response adaptations in lactation. Stress: The International Journal on the Biology of
Stress 5, 249– 257.
Tuite,C.H.,Owen,M.&Paynter,D.(
1983). Interaction between wildfowl and
recreation at Llangorse lake and Talybont Reservoir, South Wales. Wildfowl 34,
48–63.
Utne-Palm,A.C.(
2001). Response of naive two-spotted gobies Gobiusculus flavescens
to visual and chemical stimuli of their natural predator, cod Gadus morhua.Marine
Ecology Progress Series 218,267–274.
Wada,H.&Breuner,C.W.(
2010). Developmental changes in neural corticosteroid
receptor binding capacity in altricial nestlings. Developmental Neurobiology 70, 853– 861.
Walker,B.G.,Boersma,P.D.&Wingfield,J.C.(
2005). Field endocrinology and
conservation biology. Integrative and Comparative Biology 45, 12– 18.
Warner,R.R.(
1998). The role of extreme iteroparity and risk avoidance in the
evolution of mating systems. Journal of Fish Biology 53, 82– 93.
Waynert,D.F.,Stookey,J.M.,Schwartzkopf-Genswein,K.S.,Watts,J.M.
&Waltz,C.S.(
1999). The response of beef cattle to noise during handling. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science 62, 27–42.
Weidinger,K.(
2002). Interactive effects of concealment, parental behaviour and
predators on the survival of open passerine nests. Journal of Animal Ecology 71,
424– 437.
Weimerskirch,H.(
1999). The role of body condition in breeding and foraging
decision in albatrosses and petrels. In Proceedings of the 22th International Ornithological
Congress (eds N. J. Adams and R. H. Slotow), pp. 1178– 1189. BirdLife,
Johannesburg, South Africa.
Weimerskirch,H.,Shaffer,S.A.,Mabille,G.,Martin,J.,Boutard,O.&
Rouanet,J.L.(
2002). Heart rate and energy expenditure of incubating wandering
albatrosses: basal levels, natural variation, and the effects of human disturbance.
Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 475– 483.
Weston,M.A.,McLeod,E.M.,Blumstein,D.T.&Guay,P.J.(
2012). A
review of flight-initiation distances and their application to managing disturbance
to Australian birds. Emu 112, 269– 286.
Wheeler,M.,Villiers,M.&Majiedt,P.(
2009). The effect of frequency and nature
of pedestrian approaches on the behaviour of wandering albatrosses at sub-Antarctic
Marion Island. Polar Biology 32, 197– 205.
Whittingham,M.J.,Butler,S.J.,Quinn,J.L.&Cresswell,W.(
2004). The
effect of limited visibility on vigilance behaviour and speed of predator detection:
implications for the conservation of granivorous passerines. Oikos 106,377–385.
Wikelski,M.&Cooke,S.J.(
2006). Conservation physiology. Trends in Ecology and
Evolution 21, 38– 46.
Wilcoxen,T.E.,Boughton,R.K.,Bridge,E.S.,Rensel,M.A.&Schoech,
S. J. (2011). Age-related differences in baseline and stress-induced corticosterone in
Florida scrub-jays. General and Comparative Endocrinology 173, 461– 466.
Wingfield,J.C.(
1984). Influence of weather on reproduction. Journal of Experimental
Zoology 232, 589– 594.
Wingfield,J.C.(
2013). Ecological processes and the ecology of stress: the impacts of
abiotic environmental factors. Functional Ecology 27, 37– 44.
Wingfield,J.C.,Hunt,K.,Breuner,C.,Dunlap,K.,Fowler,G.S.,
Freed,L.&Lepson,J.(
1997). Environmental stress, field endocrinology, and
conservation biology. In Behavioral Approaches to Conservation in the Wild (eds J.
R. Clemmons and R. Buchholz), pp. 95– 131. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Wingfield,J.C.&Sapolsky,R.M.(
2003). Reproduction and resistance to stress:
when and how. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 15, 711– 724.
Wynne-Edwards,K.E.&Timonin,M.E.(
2007). Paternal care in rodents:
weakening support for hormonal regulation of the transition to behavioral fatherhood
in rodent animal models of biparental care. Hormones and Behavior 52, 114 –121.
Ydenberg,R.C.&Dill,L.M.(
1986). The economics of fleeing from predators.
Advances in the Study of Behavior 16, 229 –249.
Yohannes,E.,Hobson,K.A.&Pearson,D.J.(
2007). Feather stable-isotope
profiles reveal stopover habitat selection and site fidelity in nine migratory
species moving through sub-Saharan Africa. Journal of Avian Biology 38,
347– 355.
van der Zande,A.N.&Vos,P.(
1984). Impact of a semi-experimental increase in
recreation intensity on the densities of birds in groves and hedges on a lake shore in
The Netherlands. Biological Conservation 30, 237– 259.
(Received 14 July 2014; revised 7 September 2015; accepted 9 September 2015; published online 14 October 2015)
Biological Reviews 92 (2017) 216– 233 ©2015 Cambridge Philosophical Society
... The development of human civilization has a significant impact on the distribution and activity of wildlife species (Dirzo et al. 2014, Tablado & Jenni 2017, Gaynor et al. 2018, Tucker et al. 2018). Increased urbanisation and land-use change (Hansen et al. 2005) are among the main threats to wildlife, not only through the destruction or fragmentation of species' habitats but also because human presence creates "anthropogenic disturbance" among species that perceive it as a threat and causing anti-predatory behaviour in them (Frid & Dill 2002). ...
... Different species of animals respond to human activities in different ways. Changes in the behaviour, activity or distribution of species have been observed, which may depend on the type, intensity and frequency of disturbance (Larson et al. 2016, Tablado & Jenni 2017, Gaynor et al. 2018, Tucker et al. 2018. For many wild mammals, humans are one of the main mortality factors (Darimont et al. 2015) and several studies have shown that species exhibit anti-predator behaviour in the presence of humans (Frid & Dill 2002, Clinchy et al. 2016, Smith et al. 2017. ...
... The influence of anthropogenic disturbance on the behaviour and activity of animals is the subject of several studies (Packard et al. 1999, Frid & Dill 2002, Kerley et al. 2002, Moreira-Arce et al. 2015, Larson et al. 2016, Tablado & Jenni 2017, Cruz et al. 2018. Various forms of tourism are also of anthropogenic concern and this has become an increasing factor influencing the processes in wild nature (Cole & Landres 1995, Larson et al. 2016. ...
Article
Full-text available
The circadian activity of selected mammal species and the temporal overlap with human presence or between species were analysed in 2013-2014 on the territory of the Sinite Kamani Nature Park, Eastern Stara Planina. The purpose of this study was to determine how the human presence affected the circadian activity of mammal species in this protected area. The wild boar (n = 68) showed 22% overlap with the humans (n = 54) in its circadian activity (Δ = 0.22, CI 0.02-0.21) while the roe deer (n = 144) activity overlapped in 49% with the human activity (Δ = 0.49, CI 0.29-0.51). The European brown hare (n=26) activity had a 23% overlap (Δ = 0.23, CI 0.07-0.26), the golden jackal (n = 42)-36% (Δ = 0.36, CI 0.22-0.49) and the red fox (n = 131)-24% (Δ = 0.24, CI 0.06-0.25). The overlap between the red fox and European brown hare activity was 81% (Δ = 0.81, CI 0.71-0.93), between red fox and golden jackal-71% (Δ = 0.71, CI0.49-0.77) and between red fox and Martes sp.-81% (Δ = 0.81, CI 0.72-0.93). The target species avoided the range of time when humans were active. This forced the species to use a narrower temporal niche when sharing the same space. These results provided insights for the better management of the species in the protected areas.
... Animal vigilance as a form of antipredator behaviour involves loss of energy and should therefore be analysed in relation to the speci c threat (Lima and Dill 1990). Humans are often perceived as potential predators, so the best way to avoid this threat is simply to escape (Møller et al. 2015; Tablado and Jenni 2015). However, anthropogenic habitats like crop elds and meadows are attractive to animals as they offer plentiful food resources, but even rubbish dumps are adopted and exploited by animals in their search for food, as this may increase their tness (Kruszyk and Ciach 2010; Gilbert et al. 2016). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Human activities often negatively affect the time birds spend on activities such as parental care, foraging and resting. Forms of antipredator behaviour among birds such as vigilance can be an adaptation to human disturbances which can enhance their fitness in human‐managed habitats. We studied the flight initiation distance (FID) of White Storks Ciconia ciconia foraging on hay meadows during their breeding season. Our study showed that farm work, the type of meadows and starting distance (the distance between the bird’s position and the observer at the start of his walk) all had an influence on FID. Conversely, the numbers of storks in a particular foraging flock had no effect on an individual bird’s FID. The lower FID could have been due to the presence of machinery operating in the meadows, because storks then have opportunities to catch energy-rich prey that has been scared off by the machines. Unlike meadows cleared of hay, mown meadows with cut grass are more attractive to storks, and the FID there is shorter. The starting distance positively affected stork responses. These findings suggest that the FID of White Storks, treated as a measure of the risk of predation, depends largely on the degree of attractiveness of the feeding grounds.
... That is often the case for non-extractive recreational activities yet there are also a growing number of examples of where superficially benign activities (e.g., bird watching, hiking) can have negative impacts on wildlife (Boyle and Samson 1985). A common feature of these studies whether in terrestrial or aquatic systems is that impacts are context specific (Tablado and Jenni 2017). What may be a threat for one location, season, or life-stage may not be a threat for another combination of contexts. ...
Article
Atlantic salmon populations face a number of significant, human-driven threats such as overfishing and thermal stress from anthropogenically-accelerated climate change. A considerable body of research has been devoted to such large-scale threats as well as catch-and-release fishing, while the potential impacts of other recreational activities on Atlantic salmon while in rivers have been largely overlooked. Here, we undertook a systematic literature review of the effects that recreational activities (excluding direct impacts of catch-and-release angling) might have on the welfare and survival of Atlantic salmon in riverine systems at all relevant life history stages. Examples of relevant activities examined here include swimming, all-terrain vehicle (ATV) use, and underwater photography. We also performed a relative risk assessment of such activities based on the likelihoods of their occurrence and the severities of their potential impacts. For the most part, the impacts of non-angling recreational activities on Atlantic salmon are likely widespread but largely temporary. Redds, eggs, and juveniles were generally found to be more susceptible to most threats than smolts and adults. However, some activities have significant destructive potential such as ATV use in or around spawning habitats. Significant risks also remain concerning pathogen and invasive species transfer via angling gear, waders, canoes, and other equipment that may be moved across systems without proper cleaning. Although we focused primarily on risks to native Atlantic salmon populations in eastern Canada, the risk assessment framework developed here is broadly applicable and easily adaptable for management in other contexts and jurisdictions with populations of riverine Atlantic salmon and potentially other migratory salmonids too.
... Demoiselle Cranes appear to prefer areas for resting, breeding, and foraging that have minimal human presence and disturbance [154]. Distance from the food resources [155][156][157][158][159][160], human disturbance, and associated predation risk determine a species' habitat selection [159,161]. Human presence also affects the foraging grounds of a species both spatially and temporally [161][162][163]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The inevitable impacts of climate change have reverberated across ecosystems and caused substantial global biodiversity loss. Climate-induced habitat loss has contributed to range shifts at both species and community levels. Given the importance of identifying suitable habitats for at-risk species, it is imperative to assess potential current and future distributions, and to understand influential environmental factors. Like many species, the Demoiselle crane is not immune to climatic pressures. Khyber Pakhtunkhwa and Balochistan provinces in Pakistan are known wintering grounds for this species. Given that Pakistan is among the top five countries facing devastating effects of climate change, this study sought to conduct species distribution modeling under climate change using data collected during 4 years of field surveys. We developed a Maximum Entropy distribution model to predict the current and projected future distribution of the species across the study area. Future habitat projections for 2050 and 2070 were carried out using two representative concentration pathways (RCP 4.5 and RCP 8.5) under three global circulation models, including HADGEM2-AO, BCC-CSM1-1, and CCSM4. The most influential factors shaping Demoiselle Crane habitat suitability included the temperature seasonality, annual mean temperature, terrain ruggedness index, and human population density, all of which contributed significantly to the suitability (81.3%). The model identified 35% of the study area as moderately suitable (134,068 km2) and highly suitable (27,911 km2) habitat for the species under current climatic conditions. Under changing climate scenarios, our model predicted a major loss of the species’ current suitable habitat, with shrinkage and shift towards western–central areas along the Pakistan–Afghanistan boarder. The RCP 8.5, which is the extreme climate change scenario, portrays particularly severe consequences, with habitat losses reaching 65% in 2050 and 85% in 2070. This comprehensive study provides useful insights into the Demoiselle Crane habitat’s current and future dynamics in Pakistan.
... Indeed, under the habituation hypothesis, animals reduce their responses to the stimuli by a learning process in which the stimuli cease to be regarded as dangerous after repeated exposures to it with no reinforcing consequences (Thompson and Spencer 1966;Alcock 1993). This is especially true in urban context, where individuals have been habituated to low and non-threatening stimuli, such as walkers (Eason et al. 2006;Weston and Elgar 2007;Tablado and Jenni 2017;Bötsch et al. 2017). On the other hand, in areas where human presence is associated with threatening or discouraging behaviour, such as hunting, animals respond with longer FIDs (Laursen et al. 2005;Thiel et al. 2007;Clucas and Marzluff 2012;Braimoh et al. 2018). ...
Article
Human activities can impact avian populations leading to impaired fitness. It is, therefore, important to monitor their response to direct disturbance. Flight initiation distance (FID) is considered a measure of tolerance to humans and can be affected by age and gregariousness, yet few systematic data are available across species in similar environments. We measured FID in eight species of waterbirds in two coastal lagoon environments with different protection regimes, taking into account age classes and whether individuals were grouped or alone. Species markedly differed in FID: average distance ranged between 50 (little egret, Egretta garzetta, singletons) and 188m (spoonbill, Platalea leucorodia, flocks). Overall, adults were more cautious than immatures, likely due to learning and experience, while flocks flew up sooner than singletons likely due to the “many eyes” effect. In areas strictly protected, where only low and non-threatening human activities are allowed, birds flew at a shorter distance than in areas with a less strict regime of protection. Interestingly, large-sized species flew at longer distance than small-sized species. These findings have important implications for management to reduce disturbance to avian wildlife by human activities and to assess the effectiveness of protected areas.
... In this study, several species (i.e., badger, bobcat, cottontail rabbit, kit fox, mule deer) decreased their use of canal overpasses as human development increased at a site during one or more seasons. Urbanized areas can lack habitat for many species (e.g., dense vegetation, open spaces, unrestricted movement), and in addition development and activities from humans can be perceived as a risk of predation (Frid and Dill, 2002;Tablado and Jenni, 2015). Most overpasses along the CAP canal occurred in fairly remote areas without human residences. ...
Article
Anthropogenic linear infrastructures can reduce landscape connectivity for wildlife, and crossing structures are a mitigation strategy to facilitate animal movement across potential barriers. However, the spatial and temporal factors promoting crossing structure use by the wildlife community across scales are not fully understood, especially for major water canals. We tested multiple hypotheses and predictions to evaluate how wildlife use of crossing structures was influenced by (1) landscape features (i.e., fine landscape-scale vegetation cover and broad landscape-scale plant productivity, topography, and human development), (2) season, and (3) time of day. We used remote cameras to monitor 43 overpasses and 13 siphons (i.e., natural crossing areas where the canal traveled underground) along the Central Arizona Project canal across three seasons (i.e., hot-dry, hot-wet, cool-wet) during one year. We detected 17 small- to large-sized herbivore and carnivore species using overpasses and siphons. When using crossing structures, each wildlife species exhibited unique habitat relationships in relation to vegetation cover, plant productivity, and topography. Several species decreased use of overpasses as the amount of human development increased. Crossing frequency increased in the hot-dry and cool-wet seasons for many species at overpasses (11 species) and siphons (9 species). Daily activity patterns of individual species were similar across seasons, and between overpasses and siphons. Ultimately, this study suggests that to promote movement across canals for the wildlife community, it is important to provide a variety of crossing types that occur away from human development and across a range of vegetation and topographic characteristics.
Article
Full-text available
Anthropogenic disturbance elicits adaptive responses in wildlife, generally aimed at risk-avoidance, ultimately imposing constraints on their spatial and temporal niches. Previous studies have largely focused on long-term adaptive responses to stable human pressure, but rapid adjustments in wildlife’s diel and habitat use patterns in response to fine-scale variations in human presence have so far been overlooked. In this study we estimate short-term spatio-temporal deviations in local habitat use and diel activity of medium and large mammal species in response to rapid variations in human disturbance. We employed a year-long camera-trapping within a small private reserve, and recorded spatio-temporal information on all sources of anthropogenic disturbance in the area. By controlling for the average habitat use and diel activity, we explored fine-scale spatiotemporal adjustments in seven mammal species. We found evidence of spatial and/or temporal avoidance across all species, except wild boar, with variations in magnitude and direction coherent with species traits and expected levels of human-tolerance. Most species exhibited temporal avoidance of human activities, with porcupine and roe deer eliciting particularly strong responses. Notably, foxes concurrently displayed temporal avoidance and spatial attraction, likely driven by the presence of anthropogenic trophic resources. Our study underscores the role of behavioral plasticity in enabling wildlife to adjust daily habitat use and activity patterns to varying levels of human pressure across space and time. Understanding these nuanced behavioral strategies can help to promote wildlife-human coexistence and mitigating the adverse impacts of human presence on wildlife fitness.
Article
Full-text available
Capture and manipulation are an integral part of wildlife research and management. These practices, however, can affect animals either directly or indirectly, and studies should generally evaluate the consequences of captures to ensure animal welfare and reduce sampling bias. Here, we investigated the indirect, behavioural effects of live‐capture on escape response to humans in Alpine marmot Marmota marmota within the Stelvio National Park (central Italian Alps) over three seasons (2021–2023). We used flight initiation distance (FID) as a measure of escape response and tested it in relation to capture status using linear mixed modelling. Captures did not have any detectable effect on escape response, and FID was best explained by covariates such as starting distance, distance to nearest burrow, current behaviour during the observation and year of observation. It might be that, in marmots, escape response to humans is a rather inert behaviour. As such, although we cannot rule out unmeasured effects, capture may not represent an excessively traumatic experience which could trigger immediate behavioural modification. In turn, capture is unlikely to compromise animal welfare or cause scientific bias in studies investigating escape response in this species, at least over the short term.
Preprint
Capture and manipulation are an integral part of wildlife research and management. These practices, however, can affect animals either directly or indirectly, and studies should generally evaluate the consequences of captures to ensure animal welfare and reduce sampling bias. Here, we investigated the indirect, behavioural effects of live-capture on escape response to humans in Alpine marmot Marmota marmota within the Stelvio National Park (central Italian Alps) over three seasons (2021- 2023). We used flight initiation distance (FID) as a measure of escape response and tested it in relation to capture status using linear mixed modelling. Captures did not have any detectable effect on escape response, and FID was best explained by covariates such as starting distance, distance to nearest burrow, current behaviour during the observation and year of observation. It might be that, in marmots, escape response to humans is a rather inert behaviour, and as such, although we cannot rule out unmeasured effects, capture may not represent an excessively traumatic experience which could trigger immediate behavioural modification. In turn, capture is unlikely to compromise animal welfare or cause scientific bias in studies investigating escape response in this species, at least over the short term.
Preprint
Full-text available
Understanding wildlife behavioral responses is crucial for assessing the effects of anthropogenic disturbance. We used camera traps to investigate the behavioral responses of two ungulate species, bearded pigs (Sus barbatus) and sambar deer (Rusa unicolor), to anthropogenic disturbance in three protected areas in Sabah, Malaysia, that have varying levels of human activity. We found that human activities generally influence the activity patterns of both ungulates, albeit with variations among the sites. The temporal activity pattern of bearded pigs was affected by anthropogenic disturbance, especially in the area targeted by poachers. While the core activity pattern of sambar deer remained consistent across sites, poaching pressure appeared to impact their behavior within specific environments. Bearded pigs approached plantations at times of low human activity, presumably to forage, indicating that they adjust spatiotemporal activity patterns to minimize human contact. We observed a reduction in active times for both species at sites of high anthropogenic disturbance. Despite these challenges, both species demonstrated behavioral adaptability to anthropogenic disturbance by utilizing artificial environments such as roads and oil palm plantations as foraging places, thereby potentially compensating for reduced feeding times. Our study underscores the negative impact of human activities on the activity patterns of the two ungulate species. Nevertheless, it also highlights their behavioral plasticity in response to anthropogenic disturbance, suggesting their ability to efficiently utilize alternative food resources. Our methodology provides insights into wildlife management strategies. We recommend urgent long-term monitoring of wildlife population dynamics, including behavioral responses, especially in Southeast Asia.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter draws on the results of surveys of tourists who undertook an Antarctic journey in January 2003 aboard the Antarctic cruise ship, Akademik Ioffe. These surveys were designed to determine the socio­economic profile of these travellers, to evaluate the importance to them of Antarctic wildlife for their travel, and to discover their attitudes to Antarctic wildlife conservation as well as to environmental issues involving Antarctica, both prior to and following their visit to Antarctica. This chapter reports on the socio-economic profile of respondents, their willingness to pay for their Antarctic trip, and their knowledge of Antarctica. The comparative importance of Antarctic wildlife as a factor motivating respondents to undertake their journey is assessed and the evaluation by travellers of the features of their journey following their visit is considered. The relative importance of different Antarctic wildlife species is taken into account as well as attractions other than wildlife. The attitudes of respondents to several environmental issues involving Antarctica (for example, the commercial use of its natural resources and global environmental change impacting on Antarctica) are canvassed and summarised. In the concluding section, the relevance of the survey results for Antarctic conservation is discussed. Particular attention is given to the question of whether Antarctic tourism favours or threatens Antarctic nature conservation.
Article
We studied the changes in heart rate (HR) associated with metabolic rate of incubating and resting adult wandering albatrosses (Diomedea exulans) on the Crozet Islands. Metabolic rates of resting albatrosses fitted with external HR recorders were measured in a metabolic chamber to calibrate the relationship between HR and oxygen consumption (V̇O2) (V̇O2=0.074×HR+0.019, r2=0.567, P<0.001, where V̇O2 is in ml kg–1 min–1 and HR is in beats min–1). Incubating albatrosses were then fitted with HR recorders to estimate energy expenditure of albatrosses within natural field conditions. We also examined the natural variation in HR and the effects of human disturbance on nesting birds by monitoring the changes in HR. Basal HR was positively related to the mass of the individual. The HR of incubating birds corresponded to a metabolic rate that was 1.5-fold (males) and 1.8-fold (females) lower than basal metabolic rate (BMR) measured in this and a previous study. The difference was probably attributable to birds being stressed while they were held in the metabolic chamber or wearing a mask. Thus, previous measurements of metabolic rate under basal conditions or for incubating wandering albatrosses are likely to be overestimates. Combining the relationship between HR and metabolic rate for both sexes, we estimate that wandering albatrosses expend 147 kJ kg–1 day–1 to incubate their eggs. In addition, the cost of incubation was assumed to vary because (i) HR was higher during the day than at night, and (ii) there was an effect of wind chill (<0°C) on basal HR. The presence of humans in the vicinity of the nest or after a band control was shown to increase HR for extended periods (2–3 h), suggesting that energy expenditure was increased as a result of the disturbance. Lastly, males and females reacted differently to handling in terms of HR response: males reacted more strongly than females before handling, whereas females took longer to recover after being handled.
Article
I tested biomechanical predictions that morphological proportions (snout-vent length, forelimb length, hindlimb length, tail length, and mass) and maximal sprinting and jumping ability have evolved concordantly among 15 species of Anolis lizards from Jamaica and Puerto Rico. Based on a phylogenetic hypothesis for these species, the ancestor reconstruction and contrast approaches were used to test hypotheses that variables coevolved. Evolutionary change in all morphological and performance variables scales positively with evolution of body size (represented by snout-vent length); size evolution accounts for greater than 50% of the variance in sprinting and jumping evolution. With the effect of the evolution of body size removed, increases in hindlimb length are associated with increases in sprinting and jumping capability. When further variables are removed, evolution in forelimb and tail length exhibits a negative relationship with evolution of both performance measures. The success of the biomechanical predictions indicates that the assumption that evolution in other variables (e.g., muscle mass and composition) did not affect performance evolution is probably correct; evolution of the morphological variables accounts for approximately 80% of the evolutionary change in performance ability. In this case, however, such assumptions are clade-specific; extrapolation to taxa outside the clade is thus unwarranted. The results have implications concerning ecomorphological evolution. The observed relationship between forelimb and tail length and ecology probably is a spurious result of the correlation between these variables and hindlimb length. Further, because the evolution of jumping and sprinting ability are closely linked, the ability to adapt to certain microhabitats may be limited.
Article
The effect of glucocorticoids on the regulation of stably transfected human glucocorticoid receptors has been examined. Exposure of a Chinese hamster ovary-derived cell line containing stably transfected human glucocorticoid receptor genes and glucocorticoid-responsive dihydrofolate reductase genes to 5 nM dexamethasone resulted in a rapid, time-dependent reduction in the level of glucocorticoid receptor protein to 50% of control levels within 5 h of steroid treatment. This decrease in receptor protein was persistent, with a maximal 70% reduction observed even after 4 weeks of dexamethasone treatment. Immunocytochemical analysis of the influence of dexamethasone on stably transfected glucocorticoid receptors revealed efficient translocation of receptors to the nucleus within 1 h of hormone treatment. However, upon longer exposure to dexamethasone (5 h), immunoreactive glucocorticoid receptors were localized primarily to the cytoplasm. By 24 h of treatment, glucocorticoid receptors were absent from the cyt...