ArticlePDF Available

Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities: Orientation of the mental number line

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Various sources of information demonstrate a tight link between visuospatial and nu-merical disabilities. In the past, this link has been attributed to the involvement of visuospa-tial peripheral support systems like for instance visuospatial working memory in numerical cognition. However, it is also possible that the association of visuospatial and numerical abilities has a more basic origin. The basic mental representation of numerical magnitude has been shown to take the form of an oriented mental number line in normally developing chil-dren and adults, as evidenced by the SNARC effect (preference for left-hand responses to small numbers and right-hand responses to large numbers). To investigate the possibility of abnormal spatial number coding in children with combined visuospatial and numerical dis-abilities, we measured the SNARC effect in a visuospatial disability group (VSD) during a number comparison task (smaller or larger than 5) and compared it to a matched control group. A SNARC effect was obtained in the control group but not in the visuospatial disabil-ity group. This result is a first indication that the link between visuospatial and numerical disabilities may be mediated by a basic abnormality in representing numerical magnitudes on an oriented mental number line.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychology Science, Volume 47, 2005 (1), p. 172 - 183
Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities:
orientation of the mental number line
JAN BACHOT1,WIM GEVERS2,WIM FIAS2,HERBERT ROEYERS3
Abstract
Various sources of information demonstrate a tight link between visuospatial and nu-
merical disabilities. In the past, this link has been attributed to the involvement of visuospa-
tial peripheral support systems like for instance visuospatial working memory in numerical
cognition. However, it is also possible that the association of visuospatial and numerical
abilities has a more basic origin. The basic mental representation of numerical magnitude has
been shown to take the form of an oriented mental number line in normally developing chil-
dren and adults, as evidenced by the SNARC effect (preference for left-hand responses to
small numbers and right-hand responses to large numbers). To investigate the possibility of
abnormal spatial number coding in children with combined visuospatial and numerical dis-
abilities, we measured the SNARC effect in a visuospatial disability group (VSD) during a
number comparison task (smaller or larger than 5) and compared it to a matched control
group. A SNARC effect was obtained in the control group but not in the visuospatial disabil-
ity group. This result is a first indication that the link between visuospatial and numerical
disabilities may be mediated by a basic abnormality in representing numerical magnitudes on
an oriented mental number line.
Key words: SNARC, visuospatial disability
1 J. Bachot, Center of Mental Health Andante, Herculusstraat 17, B-2600 Antwerp, Belgium; Phone: +32 3-
2703773, Fax: +32 3-2703774; E-mail: jan.bachot@andante.be
2 Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
3 Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent, Belgium
Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities:
orientation of the mental number line
173
The relation between visuospatial problems and mathematical disability has been primar-
ily attributed to dysfunctions at the level of peripheral support systems, like working mem-
ory, visual imagery (Keeler & Swanson, 2001; Reuhkala, 2001) or visuospatial attention,
causing deficiencies in retrieval and/or procedural operations (for review see Geary &
Hoard, 2001). In this paper we do not concentrate on these visuospatial functions supporting
mental arithmetic, but we focus on the central representation of numerical meaning, being
the core component of numerical and mathematical knowledge (Dehaene, 1992; Wynn,
1998), and show that the spatial component of this central representation is affected in chil-
dren with visuospatial disabilities.
Recent studies show that animals (Nieder, Freedman, & Miller, 2002; Sawamura, Shima,
& Tanji, 2002) and human infants (Wynn, 1996) are able to represent numerosity and to
discriminate it from other numerosities. The most important variable determining quantifica-
tion performance is the numerical distance between the numerosities to be discriminated:
discriminability increases with increasing difference between the two sets of elements. Put
more simply: a set of 2 elements can be more easily discriminated from a set of 4 elements
than from a set of 3 elements. Moreover, this distance effect is modulated by numerical size.
For larger numerosities, the numerical distance between the to-be-compared sets needs to be
larger in order to be discriminable (Xu & Spelke, 2000).
With further development the capacity of the number line representation is extended and
its representational accuracy increased (Dehaene, 2001; Verguts & Fias, 2004). Mastery of
symbol systems like language, and particularly the number word list and its integration with
explicit counting procedures (Gallistel & Gelman, 1992) can be expected to be the primary
determinants of the development of the initial representation to a more efficient system.
Importantly, when healthy adults compare quantities, be they presented as collections of
items (Buckley & Gillman, 1974; Fias, Lammertyn, Reynvoet, Dupout, & Orban, 2003) or
as symbols expressed in Arabic digit format (e.g. Moyer & Landauer, 1967) or verbal mo-
dality (Dehaene, 1992), numerical distance remains the most important determinant of per-
formance. Even after extensive training the distance effect does not disappear (Dehaene,
1997). This strongly suggests that the representations underlying adult performance originate
from the initial representational ability available in early childhood.
Yet, increased capacity and accuracy is not the only development of the number line rep-
resentation. Recent research shows that the number line representation becomes spatially
coded: small numbers are associated with left and large numbers with right. This spatial
coding was first demonstrated by Dehaene, Bossini, and Giraux (1993) in normal adults as a
Spatial Numerical Association of Response Codes effect: when doing parity judgment with
key presses as response, subjects exhibited faster left hand than right hand responses to small
numbers, the reverse being true for large numbers. Further research showed that the effect is
not specific to parity judgment and is robustly observed in other tasks (e.g. Fias, Brysbaert,
Geypens, & d’Ydewalle, 1996; Fias, Lauwereyns, & Lammertyn, 2001). In a developmental
study, Berch, Foley, Hill, and Ryan (1999) showed that the SNARC effect in parity judg-
ment emerges from the third grade. However, it should be noted that parity judgment is a
task which requires relatively advanced mathematical knowledge. Therefore, the actual
spatial coding of magnitude representations may occur at a younger age (Fias & Fischer,
2005), although a critical age has not been determined. It has been hypothesized that the
SNARC effect depends on reading habits: Lebanese subjects who are used to read from right
to left tend to exhibit an inverse SNARC effect (large-left and small-right; Zebian, in press).
J. Bachot, W. Gevers, W. Fias, H. Roeyers
174
It is hard to tell, though, whether this reversal is a direct consequence of reading direction or
from a more general culturally-determined habit of ordering information from right to left
(Tversky, Kugelmass, & Winter, 1991).
In sum, the normal core mental representation of numerical magnitude takes the form of
an oriented number line, with two behavioral effects emanating from this kind of representa-
tion: distance and SNARC effect. The distance effect is present in the initial stages of devel-
opment whereas the spatial coding of the number line is acquired later.
In this study, we address the question to what extent the spatial coding of the mental
number line develops normally in children with visuospatial disabilities, with otherwise
normal verbal skills, who showed also problems on arithmetic, next to a much better level on
reading. To exclude differences in automaticity from our results, the SNARC effect was
measured in the context of a magnitude comparison task, which requires access to the mental
number line for correct task performance.
Method
Participants
The sample consists of 32 children in the range from 7 to 12 years. Half of them be-
longed to the group of children with visuospatial disability (VSD group), the other half was a
matched control group. The VSD group consisted of 16 children (mean age = 9,29;
SD=1,21), who were referred to a Centre of revalidation or a Mental Health Centre for chil-
dren and adolescents with complaints of emotional and/or learning disorders. They were
called upon post-hoc and were submitted to a series of neuropsychological tasks. They had
relatively low scores on Performance IQ compared to normal Verbal IQ of the WISC-R and
WISC-III going together with visuospatial disabilities and dyscalculia (see Table 1). They all
had low scores on the block design subtest.
They also had difficulties with visuospatial tasks such as the spatial task of the PMA
(Thurstone & Thurstone, 1962), the Judgement of Line Orientation (Benton, Hamsher,
Varney, & Spreen, 1983), the Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration (Beery,
1997). They performed the latter in a deficient way without having difficulties on the sup-
plemental developmental tests of Visual Perception and Motor Coordination. Children with
problems in motor coordination were excluded from the experimental group. Moreover, we
administrated a test for visuo-spatial working memory, the VSS (de Sonneville, 2001). They
also did an arithmetic task for complex addition and number concepts (KRT, Cracco, 1993),
a task on simple automatized number facts (TTR, De Vos, 1992), probably based on retrieval
from rote verbal memory (Dehaene, 1992) and a reading test on words (Brus, One-Minute
Test; Brus & Voeten, 1979) and nonsense words (Klepel-pseudo-word test; Van den Bos,
Spelberg, Scheepstra, & De Vries, 1994) at their school level. Comparing mean results of the
Arithmetic test and those of reading (see Table 1), the discrepancy is obvious.
The control group consisted of 16 children matched by gender and age (M=9.24,
SD=1.15). These children were recruited from local schools on the basis of both the teach-
ers’ referrals and the test scores on the WISC-R or WISC-III as a measure of general intelli-
gence (Spreen & Strauss, 1991) and on reading tests. Verbal intelligence was matched to the
Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities:
orientation of the mental number line
175
Table 1:
Means, SD and F-values of VSD and Control groups4 on the variables.
Test5VSD (n=16)
M(sd)
Control (n=16)
M(sd)
F (1)
WISC
VIQ 108.63 (9.28) 106.19 (5.81) 0.79
PIQ 88.56 (10.56) 107.19 (9.21) 28.28**
BP 7.88 (1.74) 10.06 (1.81) 12.13**
Visuospatial tasks (z-score)
PMA -0.36 (0.97) 0.66 (0.93) 9.20**
JLO -0.60 (0.62) 0.66 (0.84) 23.52**
VMI -0.36 (0.55) 0.68 (0.63) 24.60**
VMIVP 0.41 0.80 1.85
VMIMC 0.33 0.44 0.17
Visuospatial WM (z-score)
VSS -1.90 (1.20) -0.03 (1.04) 22.31**
Arithmetic (z-score)
KRT -1.67 (0.98) -0.04 (0.63) 31.00**
TTR -0.53 (1.45) 0.56(1.68) 3.82
Reading (z-score)
Brus 0.04 (1.49) 0.79 (1.20) 2.46
Klepel 0.06 (0.70) 0.17 (0.53) 0.23
* p<0.05, ** p<0.01
VSD group. (see Table 1). In order to be accepted in the control group, a child’s perform-
ance IQ should not differ from its verbal IQ (maximal difference of 10).
An ANOVA on the standard scores of the two groups revealed a significant difference
between the two groups on all the visuospatial and the complex arithmetic tasks but not on
the reading tasks, being average for both groups (F-values provided in Table 1).
Procedure
In a number comparison task, subjects had to decide whether a presented Arabic number
was larger or smaller than five. Responses were given on an AZERTY keyboard by means of
the letters Q (left) and M (right). Subjects were asked to respond as fast and as accurate as
possible. In the first block, subjects had to respond to small numbers with the left hand and
to large numbers with the right hand. During the second block this response assignment was
reversed.
4 M age : VSD 9.29 (1.21); control 9.24 (1.15); F(1) =0.02, p=0.89
5 BP: Block Design; PMA: Primary Mental Abilities, figure completion; JLO: Judgement of Line Orientation;
VMI: Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration; VMIVP: VMI Visual Perception; VMI MC: VMI
Motor Coordination; VSS: Visuospatial sequencing; KRT: Kortrijkse rekentest; TTR: Tempo test rekenen;
Brus: 1 minute reading test; Klepel: reading test with pseudo-verbs.
J. Bachot, W. Gevers, W. Fias, H. Roeyers
176
Arabic numbers ranging from one to nine (except five) were shown at the center of the
screen (height: 1.25°; width: .57°). Each target number was preceded by a fixation cross at
the center of the screen for 500ms. Immediately following offset, the target number appeared
and remained there until response or 7000ms elapsed. After target presentation the screen
remained blank for 500 ms; thereafter a new trial was initiated.
Each target number was presented 10 times, leading to a total of 80 target presentations
per block. Both blocks were preceded by a training list in which each target number was
presented twice. Target numbers were randomized in such manner that the same number
could never be repeated.
Data analysis
The data were analyzed using a regression method proposed by Lorch and Myers (1990,
Method 3). For each subject, for each number and for each side of response, median reaction
time (RT) was calculated. From these median scores, differences in RT (dRT) were calcu-
lated for each subject, subtracting left hand responses from right hand responses. For each
subject, dRTs are then regressed with number magnitude as predictor variable. A t-test de-
cides whether the regression slope differs significantly from zero. Because a clear prediction
is postulated about the direction of the slope (negative correlation between dRT and number
magnitude), all tests are one-sided. The distance effect was analysed using the same method.
Because the distance effect is independent from response side, the regression was performed
on average RT for each number separately. Subsequently, each number was regressed with
the distance towards the reference number five as predictor variable (e.g. distance 4 for tar-
gets 1 and 9, distance 3 for targets 2 and 8, etc…).
Results
Control subjects
Average error rate over subjects was 4.92% with a maximum of 13.75%. There was no
speed-accuracy trade off over the 16 cells of the design (r= .44 n = 16, p<.09), therefore
errors were not analyzed separately. Median RT over targets ranging from one to nine was
809, 792, 814, 844, 897, 833, 777 and 835ms, respectively.
Subsequently, the Lorch and Myers method was applied for revealing possible distance
effects. As a result, the following regression equation was obtained:
Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities:
orientation of the mental number line
177
RT = 871.27 – 18.47 (Distance)
1A) Distance ef fect, control group
700
750
800
850
900
950
D4 D3 D2 D1 D1 D2 D3 D4
Distance
RT
Obs erved
Fitt ed
with distance contributing significantly [t(15) = -2.03, SD = 36.45, p<.05]. Furthermore, the
distance effect contributed in a reliable manner up to a distance of 3 from the reference num-
ber five [F(1,15) = 5.63; p < . 05]. The analysis to evaluate the presence of a SNARC effect
revealed the following equation:
dRT = 44.76 – 13.97 (Magnitude)
1C) SNA RC ef fect, c ontrol group
-150
-50
50
150
12346789
Numb er
dRT
Obs erved
Fitt ed
with a significant magnitude slope [t(15) = -2.12, SD = 26.35, p < .05].
So, both distance and SNARC effects are present when the control subjects performed
the number comparison task.
VSD group
Average error rate over subjects was 11.33% (maximal: 31%). A positive correlation
over the 16 cells of the design indicated the absence of a speed-accuracy trade-off (r=.37,
n=16, p < .2). Median RT over subjects for each target number ranging from one to nine was
J. Bachot, W. Gevers, W. Fias, H. Roeyers
178
973, 980, 963, 1094, 1116, 997, 992 and 960ms, respectively. The slope value for distance,
obtained from the following regression equation differed significantly from zero, t(15) =
-3.82, SD = 42.87, p < .001.
RT = 1111.66 – 40.97(Distance)
1B) Distance effect, VSD group
900
950
1000
1050
1100
1150
D4 D3 D2 D1 D1 D2 D3 D4
Distance
RT
Observed
Fitted
As can be seen in the Figure, there was only a reliable distance effect between distance
one and two (F(1,15) = 5.19; p < .05). The other distances did not reach significance (all p >
.5).
Finally, the regression analysis was run on the dRTs in order to investigate the SNARC
effect. Although Figure 1d shows a positive slope indicative for a reversed SNARC effect,
the slope did not significantly differ from zero. t(15) =1.35, SD = 58.27, p > .08.
dRT = -90.41 + 19.65 (Magnitude)
1D) SNARC effect, VSD group
-150
-100
-50
0
50
100
150
12346789
Number
dRT
Observed
Fitted
However, in spite of this lack of significance, a significant positive correlation was pre-
sent between the difference in IQ (VIQ minus PIQ) and the magnitude of the slope (r = .52;
p < .05). Hence, the larger the difference between VIQ and PIQ, the more positive the slope
value becomes.
Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities:
orientation of the mental number line
179
Figure 1:
Upper figures: observed data and regression line representing RT responses as a function of
distance by a) the control group and b) the experimental group. Lower figures: observed data and
regression line representing RT differences between right-handed minus left-handed responses as
a function of magnitude by c) the control group and d) the experimental group.
Direct comparison of visuospatial disability and control group
The VSD group was both slower in RT (t(15) = -2.69; p < .01) and made more errors
(t(15) = -3.43; p < .01) than the control group. In order to find out whether the visuospatial
disability group and the matched control group were different from each other with regard to
the Distance effect and the SNARC effect, dependent sample t-tests on the slopes were per-
formed. For the distance effect, there was a marginal significant difference between both
groups, t(15) = 1.65; SD = 54.48; p <.08. As can be seen in Figure 1a and 1b the slope re-
flecting the distance effect is steeper in the experimental than in the control group. Notice
also that the distance effect is more fine tuned in the control group than in the experimental
group. While the control subjects showed a reliable distance effect up to a distance of three
(F(1,15) = 5.63; MSe = 8793; p < .05), the VSD group only showed a reliable distance effect
when a distance of 1 was compared to a distance of 2 (F(1,15) = 5.19; MSe = 96583; p <
.05).
Furthermore, the SNARC effect was found to reliably differ between the two groups
t(26) = -2.43, p < .05. In order to visualize the individual differences in slope in the two
groups, figure 2 provides a cumulative frequency distribution of the two groups.
J. Bachot, W. Gevers, W. Fias, H. Roeyers
180
Figure 2:
Cumulative frequency distribution representing both the control and the VSD group as a function
of the slope value.
Relative Cumulative Distribution
0
0,1
0,2
0,3
0,4
0,5
0,6
0,7
0,8
0,9
1
-140
-120
-100
-80
-60
-40
-20
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
Negative <- Slope -> Positive
Relative freq uency
VSD
Control
In order to decide whether the values from the VSD group are stochastically more posi-
tive than the values of the control group, a Kolmogorov-Smirnov two-sample one-tailed test
was applied. The largest difference between both groups was .5 [D(16,16) = 16*16*.5 = 128;
p < .05]. From this analysis it can be concluded that the VSD group shows proportionally
more positive values than the control group.
In order to examine the influence of multiple differences and to exclude that the group
differences are due to other variables than those of interest we performed the following mul-
tiple regression analyses. Group membership appeared to contribute significantly to the slope
values for the SNARC effect, t(30) = -2.10; p < .05 but not for the distance effect t(30) =
1.60; p > .1. With regard to the SNARC slopes, entering verbal IQ and age into the regres-
sion did not alter the results. Neither of these factors predicted the slope values whereas
group membership continued to maintain a significant influence t(28) = -1.91; p < .05 (one-
sided).
However, when the scores on each of the visuospatial tasks were taken into considera-
tion, the group difference no longer reached significance, t(26) = -0.83, p > .4, supporting
our conclusion that the missing SNARC slopes in the VSD group rely on deficient spatial
processing. When the arithmetic tasks were entered in the regression, the KRT explained
most of the variance t(28) = -1.84; p < .08 whereas the group differences again disappeared,
t(28) = -0.43; p > .6. Whether deficient visuospatial processing is caused by deficient arith-
metic processing or the other way round can not be determined on the basis of the present
correlational data. Whatever it is, the present data do point to the importance of spatial char-
acteristics in the SNARC effect.
Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities:
orientation of the mental number line
181
Discussion
In this study, we addressed the question to what extent the spatial coding of the mental
number line develops normally in children with visuospatial disabilities, with otherwise
normal verbal skills, who also showed problems on arithmetic, next to a much better level on
reading. To this end, both the SNARC effect and the distance effect were measured within
this VSD group and compared with a control group matched on age and verbal intelligence.
A first indication for a difficulty with the mapping of the numbers on an internal number
representation was the general finding that performance of the VSD group was both slower
and less accurate compared to the control group.
Additionally, it was shown that the control group and the VSD group differ from each
other with respect to the SNARC effect. In line with Berch et al. (1999), we found that the
control group showed a normal SNARC effect in that small numbers were responded to
faster with the left hand and large numbers with the right hand. In contrast, the VSD group
showed no evidence for this mapping from magnitude to response side. A number of alterna-
tive interpretations regarding the absence of a SNARC effect in the VSD group can be ruled
out.
First, it cannot be attributed to the fact that VSD children would have particular difficul-
ties with a change of response assignment halfway the experiment. Because the congruent
stimulus-response mapping (small left, large right) was administered first followed by the
incongruent mapping, one would then expect an enhancement of the SNARC effect. The fact
that the SNARC effect tends to reverse in the VSD group, can have two reasons. One reason
could be that the number line of the VSD children is misoriented, i.e. from right to left in-
stead of from left to right. The reverse SNARC effect, albeit non-significant, could also be a
reflection of a general training effect because the reversal of the SNARC effect means that
the VSD group was faster in the second half of the experiment than in the first half. If this
would be the case, this suggests that the VSD children are largely insensitive to the specific
S-R mappings. Results from the complementary order of s-r mapping (incongruent followed
by congruent) will allow to distinguish between the two interpretations.
A second possible cause for the absence of a SNARC effect is that the VSD group was
far too heterogeneous to obtain reliable measures. However, this does not seem plausible
because the subjects were carefully selected in order to obtain a homogeneous group. Fur-
thermore, the VSD group was, with the exception of visuospatial properties and arithmetical
abilities, matched with a control group that reliably showed the SNARC effect. Moreover,
two points of evidence suggest that the positive slope value is not attributable to noise. First,
a positive correlation was obtained between the difference in Verbal and Performal IQ (P-V
IQ) and the slope value indicating that the larger the P-V IQ, the more positive the slope
value becomes. Second, a frequency distribution showed that the VSD group yielded propor-
tionally more positive slope values than did the control group.
This link between visuospatial disabilities and an abnormal representation of numerical
magnitudes on an oriented mental number line does not necessarily reflect a direct causal
relationship but may be established via an intervening variable. For instance, the specific
role of peripheral support systems like working memory needs further investigation. In fact
the VSD group performed significantly worse than the control group on a visuospatial work-
ing memory task (VSS). From a developmental point of view it seems not contradictory that
J. Bachot, W. Gevers, W. Fias, H. Roeyers
182
deficits in spatial working memory might contribute to a delay or deficiency in building up
automatized spatial number representations during early school years.
The finding that the VSD group showed no SNARC effect provides us with first evi-
dence that these children exhibit problems in mapping the numbers on a mental number
representation. Converging evidence towards this conclusion were the results obtained for
the distance effect. Whereas the control group showed a distance effect, reliable up to a
distance of three, the VSD group showed only a reliable distance effect for the numbers 4
and 6.
At this point, the results can only be regarded as a first indication of a problem with the
mapping of the numbers on the ‘mental number line’. The reason why there is a tendency for
a reverse orientation of the mental number line (or the mapping towards the mental number
line) remains unclear and cannot be resolved on the basis of the present experiment.
References
Beery, K.E. (1997). The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration:
Administration, Scoring and Teaching Manual (4th Editon, Revised). New Jersey: Modern
Curriculum Press.
Benton, A.L., Hamsher K.D., Varney, N.R. & Spreen, O., (1983). Contributions to
Neuropsychological Assessment, A Clinical Manual, Second edition. Oxford: University
Press.
Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., & Ryan, P. M. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude
from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental
representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 4, 286-308.
Brus, B.T., & Voeten, M.J.M. (1979). Een Minuut Test (One minute test). Lisse, the Netherlands:
Swets & Zeitlinger.
Buckley, P.B., & Gillman, C.B (1974). Comparisons of digits and dot patterns. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 103), 1131-1136.
Cracco, J. (1993). Niet verbale leerproblemen: Een overzicht van Rourke’s model. Tijdschrift
Klinische Psychologie, 23, 25-46.
De Vos, T. (1992). Tempo-Test-Rekenen 1992. Test voor het vaststellen van het
rekenvaardigheidsniveau der elementaire bewerkingen (automatisering) voor het basis en
voortgezet onderwijs. Handleiding. Nijmegen: Berkhout.
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1-42.
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense. How the mind creates mathematics. NY: Oxford
University Press.
Dehaene, S. (2001). Precis of the number sense. Mind & Language, 16, 16-36.
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P.(1993). The mental representation of parity and number
magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371-396.
Fias, W., & Fischer, M. (2005). Spatial representation of number. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.),
Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp.43-54). New York: Psychology Press.
Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude
information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical
Cognition, 2, 95-110.
Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., Orban, G.A. (2003). Parietal representation of
symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 47-56.
Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities:
orientation of the mental number line
183
Fias, W., Lauwereyns, J., & Lammertyn, J. (2001). Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention
depending on the overlap of neural circuits. Cognitive Brain Research, 12, 3, 415-23.
Gallistel, C.R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition,
44, 43-74.
Geary, D.C., & Hoard, M.K. (2001). Numerical and arithmetical deficits in learning-disabled
children: Relation to dyscalculia and dyslexia. Aphasiology, 15, 635-647.
Keeler, M.L., & Swanson, H.L. (2001). Does strategy knowledge influence working memory in
children with mathematical disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 418-434.
Lorch, R.F., Jr., & Myers, J.L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in
cognition research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition,
16, 149-157.
Moyer, R.S., & Landauer, T.K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequality.
Nature, 215, 1519-1520.
Nieder, A., Freedman, D.J., & Miller, E.K. (2002). Representation of the quantity of visual items
in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 297, 1708-1711.
Reuhkala M. (2001). Mathematical skills in ninth-graders: Relationship with visuo-spatial
abilities and working memory. Educational Psychology, 21, 387-399.
Sawamura, H., Shima, K., & Tanji, J. (2002). Numerical representation for action in the parietal
cortex of the monkey. Nature, 415, 918-922.
Sonneville de, L.J.M. (2001). Amsterdamse Neuropsychologische Taken (ANT 2.1). Amstelveen:
Sonar.
Spreen, O. & Strauss, E. (1991). A compendium of neuropsychological tests, administration,
norms and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.
Thurstone, LL. & Thurstone, T.G. (1962). Primary mental abilities (Rev.) Chicago: Science
Research Associates.
Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., & Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in
graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 515-557.
Van den Bos, K.P., Spelberg, H.C.L., Scheepstra, A.S.M., & De Vries J.R. (1994). De Klepel:
Pseudowoordentest. Nijmegen: Berkhout.
Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2004). Representation of number in animals and humans: a neural
model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1493-1504.
Wechsler, D. (1986). Adapted by Vander Steene, G., Van Haasen, P., De Bruyn, D., Coetsier, P.,
Pijl, Y., Poortinga, Y., Spilberg, N., & Stinissen, J. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children
Revised. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger
Wechsler, D. (1991). Adapted by Kort, W., Compaan, E.L., Bleichrodt, N., Schittekatte, M.,
Bosmans, M., Vermeir, G., Verhaeghe, P. Wechsler intelligence scale for children - Third
edition. New York: The Psychological Corporation, Amsterdan: Nederlands Instituut van
Psychologen.
Wynn, K. (1996). Infants’ individuation and enumeration of actions. Psychological Science, 7,
164-169.
Wynn, K. (1998). Psychological foundations of number: numerical competence in human infants.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 296-303.
Xu, F., & Spelke, E.S. (2000). Large number discrimination in 6-month old infants. Cognition,
74, B1-B11.
Zebian, S. (in press). Influences of cultural artefacts and social practices on number
conceptualisation in adults: experimental and ethnographic approaches to everyday numeric
cognition. In: Ongoing themes in psychology and culture. Kanisius, Yogyakarta, Indonesia.
... In addition, Cheng et al. (2021) found that children proficient in mental abacus (i.e., using the mental image of an abacus, which represents values by the place of columns, smaller values on the left and larger values on the right) showed better mental rotation ability, indirectly indicating the significant association between visual-spatial abilities and spatial-numerical associations. Bachot et al. (2005) have found a positive association between visual-spatial abilities and the symbolic SNARC effect. However, some researchers have not observed a significant correlation between mental rotation ability and the symbolic SNARC effect (Bernabini et al., 2021;Viarouge et al., 2014). ...
... Working memory is an individual's ability to temporarily store and process current information (Takeuchi et al., 2010), and the working memory account points out that the spatial coding of the position of the number sequence in working memory results in the SNARC effect. Studies have proven that working memory ability could predict the SNARC effect (Bachot et al., 2005;Herrera et al., 2008;van Dijck & Fias, 2011;Wu et al., 2020) despite controversy over the predictive direction. In addition, Formoso et al. (2017) found that visual-spatial working memory was correlated with the non-symbolic SNARC effects, but a recent study with adults showed that it is not an important predictor of non-symbolic SNARC effects (Nemeh et al., 2018). ...
... In addition, numerous studies have shown that inhibitory ability is related to the SNARC effect (Hoffmann et al., 2014;Wood et al., 2008;Wu et al., 2020). Thus, as shown in Fig. 1, we hypothesized that phonological processing and executive function would contribute to the symbolic SNARC effect of children and adults (Krajewski & Schneider, 2009;Meyer et al., 2010;Wu et al., 2020) and visual-spatial ability and executive function would contribute to the non-symbolic SNARC effect of children and adults (Bachot et al., 2005;Bernabini et al., 2021). ...
... These individual factors usually have complex influences. For instance, mathematical ability is found to be significantly NO SNARC EFFECT AMONG LEFT-TO-RIGHT READERS 5 influential on the SNARC effect, usually in groups with either very high or very low levels of maths skills (Bachot et al., 2005;Hoffmann et al., 2014a;Cipora et al., 2016) rather than those with more typical levels (Cipora & Nuerk, 2013). Furthermore, the direction of the relationship also changes according to whether the participants are adults or children; better maths skills are associated with a stronger SNARC in children (Bachot et al., 2005) but a weaker SNARC in adults (Hoffmann et al., 2014a) (for a review, see Cipora et al., 2020). ...
... For instance, mathematical ability is found to be significantly NO SNARC EFFECT AMONG LEFT-TO-RIGHT READERS 5 influential on the SNARC effect, usually in groups with either very high or very low levels of maths skills (Bachot et al., 2005;Hoffmann et al., 2014a;Cipora et al., 2016) rather than those with more typical levels (Cipora & Nuerk, 2013). Furthermore, the direction of the relationship also changes according to whether the participants are adults or children; better maths skills are associated with a stronger SNARC in children (Bachot et al., 2005) but a weaker SNARC in adults (Hoffmann et al., 2014a) (for a review, see Cipora et al., 2020). Therefore, in addition to its situational nature, the SNARC effect involves an inter-individual variance, probably influenced by the interaction of multiple factors. ...
Preprint
The spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) refers to the faster left-hand responses to smaller numbers and faster right-hand responses to larger numbers. Although easily replicable in Western cultures, the prevalence of the SNARC effect in other cultures has long been an issue. In the current study, we aimed to replicate the SNARC effect in a parity judgement task with Turkish participants (N = 66) whose reading habits are entirely left-to-right. The results revealed no SNARC effect. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first finding indicating the absence of regular SNARC effect among left-to-right readers in a classical parity judgement task. Based on these findings, we suggest that investigations of cultural influences on spatial-numerical associations should take a broader perspective rather than only focusing on reading habits. The study concludes by discussing the possible factors specific to Turkish culture, which may shed light on the present findings.
... Developmental studies have shown that the SNARC effect seems to become automatic at around 9 years of age (4 th grade) and decreases over the following years until at least 14 years (e.g., Berch et al., 1999; see also Bachot et al., 2005;Van Galen & Reitsma;. If this were the case for our cohort, then the effects of the space-number association would be stronger for our 4 th , 5 th and 6 th graders, and would explain the stronger congruity effect on the right vs. ...
... Kinder lernen in den ersten beiden Schuljahren Rechenaufgaben durch effiziente Rechenstrategien, wie Zerlegungen, Ableitungen von auswendig gelernten Aufgaben oder durch einen direkten Faktenabruf zu lösen(Gaidoschik, 2010;Padberg & Benz, 2011). Grundlage dafür ist der Aufbau mentaler Vorstellungen von Zahlen und Zahlbeziehungen, welcher unter anderem vom visuell-räumlichen Arbeitsgedächtnis abzuhängen scheint(Bachot et al., 2005;Chan & Wong, 2019). Durch den Einsatz von Anschauungsmaterialien soll dieser Prozess unterstützt werden ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Thema der vorliegenden Dissertation ist die Entwicklung mathematischer Kompetenzen vom Kindergartenalter bis zum Ende der zweiten Klassenstufe und die Frage, ob diese Entwicklung bei Mädchen und Jungen unterschiedlich verläuft. Anhand zweier Messzeitpunkte einer Längsschnittstudie wurde zunächst untersucht, inwieweit sich Mädchen (N = 105) und Jungen (N = 119) im letzten Kindergartenjahr hinsichtlich ihrer mathematischen Basiskompetenzen (erhoben mit dem MBK-0; Krajewski, 2018) unterscheiden. Dies könnte Hinweise auf eine mögliche geschlechtsspezifische Sozialisation in Bezug auf den frühen Erwerb mathematischer Kompetenzen geben. Ein Dreivierteljahr vor der Einschulung (MZP 1) unterschieden sich Mädchen und Jungen nicht signifikant bezüglich ihrer mathematischen Basiskompetenzen. Ein Vierteljahr vor der Einschulung (MZP 2) wiesen Jungen signifikant höhere mathematische Basiskompetenzen auf als Mädchen. Dieser Geschlechtsunterschied war insbesondere auf einen signifikanten Vorteil der Jungen hinsichtlich der numerischen Basisfertigkeiten (Ebene 1 des Modells der Zahl-Größen-Verknüpfung (Krajewski, 2013); Zahlenfolge und Ziffernkenntnis) zurückzuführen. Die Effekte sind jedoch als gering einzustufen. Auffallend war, dass der Geschlechtsunterschied hinsichtlich des Gesamtwertes der mathematischen Basiskompetenzen im unteren Leistungsbereich kaum vorhanden war und zum oberen Leistungsbereich hin anstieg. Eine Tendenz diesbezüglich war bereits zum ersten Messzeitpunkt erkennbar. Da teilweise deutliche Deckeneffekte vorlagen, waren die Kompetenzen der Kinder im oberen Leistungsbereich jedoch nicht genau charakterisierbar. Aufgrund theoretischer Annahmen zu verschiedenen Denkweisen, die bei Mädchen und Jungen unterschiedlich häufig vorkommen könnten, wurde darüber hinaus untersucht, ob das visuell-räumliche Arbeitsgedächtnis beim Erwerb mathematischer Basiskompetenzen im Vorschulalter bei Mädchen eine geringere Rolle spielt als bei Jungen, beim Kompetenzerwerb im Schulalter dann jedoch bei Mädchen eine größere Rolle spielt als bei Jungen. Um dies zu überprüfen wurden Pfadmodelle für Mädchen (N = 85) und Jungen (N = 105) spezifiziert. Gruppenvergleiche ergaben jedoch keine signifikanten Geschlechtsunterschiede in Bezug auf die vermuteten Prozesse. Auch wurde untersucht, ob die Prognose einer Rechenschwäche aufgrund der vermuteten Entwicklungsunterschiede bei Mädchen ungenauer ausfällt als bei Jungen. Die zur Bewertung der Prognose herangezogenen Gütekriterien unterschieden sich nicht signifikant zwischen Mädchen und Jungen. Die Prävalenz einer Rechenschwäche fiel bei Mädchen tendenziell höher aus (23 Prozent) als bei Jungen (12 Prozent). Ein Geschlechtsunterschied hinsichtlich mathematischer Kompetenzen scheint also zunächst vorrangig im oberen und mittleren Leistungsbereich aufzutreten und sich innerhalb der ersten Grundschuljahre auf die gesamte Verteilung auszubreiten. Um den vorhandenen Geschlechtsunterschied im Kindergartenalter auszugleichen, müssten Mädchen bereits vor Schulbeginn stärker ermuntert werden, sich mit höheren (mindestens zweistelligen) Zahlen (auch in Ziffernform) auseinanderzusetzen. Eine Förderung des einfachen (ZGV-Modell: Ebene 2) und tiefen Zahlverständnisses (ZGV-Modell: Ebene 3) sollte dabei jedoch nicht vernachlässigt werden, da diese Kompetenzen eine wichtige Grundlage für die Erarbeitung erfolgreicher Rechenstrategien bilden.
... Moreover, empirical evidence suggests an interrelation of more general visuospatial deficits and numerical performance. In this context, Bachot et al. (2005) found that the prevalence of mathematical difficulties was significantly higher for children with deficits in the general processing of visuo-spatial information (see also Rourke and Finlayson 1978 for a corresponding subtype of developmental dyscalculia). ...
... regulation. Therefore, reasons, symptoms and course of atypical development of numerical cognition are manifold and diverse (Bachot et al., 2005;von Aster and Shalev, 2007;von Aster et al., 2021). As DMD and SMA are diseases with similar motor but quite different cognitive and social-emotional outcomes, it will be of interest how children of both groups differ according to their embodied cognitive numerical representations and developmental patterns of domain-specific and domain-general intellectual functioning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Spinal muscular atrophy (SMA) and Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD) both are rare genetic neuromuscular diseases with progressive loss of motor ability. The neuromotor developmental course of those diseases is well documented. In contrast, there is only little evidence about characteristics of general and specific cognitive development. In both conditions the final motor outcome is characterized by an inability to move autonomously: children with SMA never accomplish independent motoric exploration of their environment, while children with DMD do but later lose this ability again. These profound differences in developmental pathways might affect cognitive development of SMA vs. DMD children, as cognition is shaped by individual motor experiences. DMD patients show impaired executive functions, working memory, and verbal IQ, whereas only motor ability seems to be impaired in SMA. Advanced cognitive capacity in SMA may serve as a compensatory mechanism for achieving in education, career progression, and social satisfaction. This study aimed to relate differences in basic numerical concepts and arithmetic achievement in SMA and DMD patients to differences in their motor development and resulting sensorimotor and environmental experiences. Horizontal and vertical spatial-numerical associations were explored in SMA/DMD children ranging between 6 and 12 years through the random number generation task. Furthermore, arithmetic skills as well as general cognitive ability were assessed. Groups differed in spatial number processing as well as in arithmetic and domain-general cognitive functions. Children with SMA showed no horizontal and even reversed vertical spatial-numerical associations. Children with DMD on the other hand revealed patterns in spatial numerical associations comparable to healthy developing children. From the embodied Cognition perspective, early sensorimotor experience does play a role in development of mental number representations. However, it remains open whether and how this becomes relevant for the acquisition of higher order cognitive and arithmetic skills.
Chapter
Full-text available
The chapter aims to synthesize the experimental studies testing training programs for supporting cognitive and mathematical abilities of children with dyscalculia and mathematical difficulties. Developmental dyscalculia is a specific learning disability that manifests with persistent difficulties in comprehending basic numeric and arithmetic concepts, despite within average-level intelligence quotient and schooling opportunities. Given the predominant use of numbers in modern society, this condition can pose major challenges to students’ personal and academic development. Analyzing the cognitive profiles and the characteristics of training programs, we reviewed studies reporting experimental or quasi-experimental studies, testing a training program directed to mathematical or cognitive abilities, written in English, with a sample of children between 6 and 18 years who meet criteria for dyscalculia or mathematical difficulty, published in the last 10 years. After the search strategy process, 11 studies were judged eligible. The total sample consisted of n = 338 participants (M = 198, 59%) aged between 6 and 13 years. Eight studies implemented a cognitive program, four of which implemented a specific program focused on executive functions, while two studies used mathematical training and one employed motivational training. Studies with cognitive, mathematical, or motivational training positively influenced children’s cognitive and mathematical abilities. Moreover, the interventions appear more effective if delivered through a technological device (such as laptop or head-mounted display) as it appears to enhance child motivation.
Book
Written for pre-service and in-service educators, as well as parents of children in preschool through grade five, this book connects research in cognitive development and math education to offer an accessibly written and practical introduction to the science of elementary math learning. Structured according to children's mathematical development, How Children Learn Math systematically reviews and synthesizes the latest developmental research on mathematical cognition into accessible sections that explain both the scientific evidence available and its practical classroom application. Written by an author team with decades of collective experience in cognitive learning research, clinical learning evaluations, and classroom experience working with both teachers and children, this amply illustrated text offers a powerful resource for understanding children's mathematical development, from quantitative intuition to word problems, and helps readers understand and identify math learning difficulties that may emerge in later grades. Aimed at pre-service and in-service teachers and educators with little background in cognitive development, the book distills important findings in cognitive development into clear, accessible language and practical suggestions. The book therefore serves as an ideal text for pre-service early childhood, elementary, and special education teachers, as well as early career researchers, or as a professional development resource for in-service teachers, supervisors and administrators, school psychologists, homeschool parents, and other educators.
Article
Background Extant eye-tracking studies suggest that foreign-language learners tend to read the native language captions while watching foreign-language videos. However, it remains unclear how the captions affect the learners' eye movements when watching Math videos. Purpose While watching teaching videos, we seek to determine how the lesson type (English or Math), cognitive load (high or low), and caption type (meaningful, no captions, or meaningless) affect the dwell times and fixation counts on the captions. Methods One hundred and eighty undergraduate students were randomly and equally assigned to six (2 lesson type × 3 caption type) conditions. Each participant watched two short teaching videos (one low load and one high load). After watching each video, a comprehension test and three self-reported items (fatigue, effort, and difficulty) regarding this particular video were given. Results We reported more dwell times and fixation counts on the meaningful captions, compared to the meaningless captions and no captions. In the high-load condition, viewers watching an English lesson relied more on the meaningful captions than they did when watching a Math lesson. In the low-load condition, the dwell times and fixation counts on the captions were similar between the English and Math lessons. Finally, the captions did not affect the comprehension test performances after ruling out individual differences in the prior performances of English and Math. Conclusions English language learning may rely more on the captions than is the case in learning Math. This study provides the direction for designing multimedia teaching materials in the current trend of multimedia teaching.
Book
Neuropsychologische Studien zeigen, dass die vorschulische Förderung von mathematischen Kompetenzen sowie von kognitiven und sozio-emotionalen Basisfähigkeiten gewöhnlich positiv auf den späteren Schulerfolg, die sozio-emotionale Entwicklung und das Selbstkonzept von Kindern einwirkt. Mit dem vorliegenden Trainingsmanual stellen wir ein wissenschaftlich evaluiertes Förderinstrument für den Elementarbereich vor, das als zweiwöchiges neuropsychologisches Gruppentraining für den Einsatz in inklusiven Lernumgebungen konzipiert wurde. Das STARK-Training berücksichtigt in besonderer Weise die Lernausgangslage von Kindern mit Migrationshintergrund und/oder besonderem Förderbedarf, die ein halbes Jahr vor Ihrer Einschulung stehen. In insgesamt 27 spielerisch und kooperativ gestalteten Übungen erlernen die Kinder mit Hilfe von nicht-sprachlichen Repräsentationsformen, d.h. auch ohne umfassende Kenntnis der deutschen Sprache, grundlegende Fähigkeiten der Raumwahrnehmung und Raumkognition, auf denen die weitere Entwicklung des mathematischen Verständnisses und des mathematisch-technischen Fähigkeitsselbstkonzeptes aufbauen kann. Die unterschiedlichen Übungen sind didaktisch so aufgebaut, dass sie mit ihren gestalterischen Elementen sowie Geschicklichkeits- und Bewegungselementen in besonderer Weise auf die körperlich-sensorische Lebens- und Erfahrungswelt der Kinder Bezug nehmen. Hierdurch können, wie auch die Evaluationsergebnisse des STARK-Trainings belegen, besonders die Aufmerksamkeit und das Arbeitsgedächtnis, aber auch die Selbst- und Fremdwahrnehmung, Selbstkompetenz, sozio-emotionale Kompetenz und Kreativität der Kinder inklusiv vermittelnd gefördert werden. Das vorliegende Trainingsmanual richtet sich primär an Erziehende im Elementarbereich, die Kinder in inklusiven Kleingruppen gezielt auf die bevorstehende Einschulung vorbereiten möchten. In diesem Praxisfeld wurde das STARK-Training wissenschaftlich evaluiert. Es kann sehr leicht auch an Lernkontexte im Gesundheitsbereich (z.B. im Rahmen einer psychologischen und neuropsychologischen Förderung) adaptiert werden. Die Konzeption und Evaluation des STARK-Trainings erfolgte im Rahmen eines Verbundprojektes zum Thema „Inklusive Bildungsforschung der frühen Kindheit als multidisziplinäre Herausforderung“ und in Kooperation von Praxispartnern sowie Wissenschaftlerinnen und Wissenschaftlern der Arbeitsgruppen „Neurodidaktik“ sowie „Didaktik der Mathematik“ an der Stiftung Universität Hildesheim. Das Verbundprojekt und die vorliegende Publikation wurden im Niedersächsischen Vorab durch die Volkswagenstiftung und das Niedersächsische Ministerium für Wissenschaft und Kultur gefördert.
Article
In the present study, the relationship between working memory (WM ) capacity, particularly visuo-spatial working memory (VSW M), the ability to mentally rotate three-dimensional objects and mathematical skills was investigated. In Experiment 1, the two VSW M components, viz. static visuo-spatial storage component and dynamic visuo-spatial storage component, were examined separately. The ability to retain gradually increasing square patterns (static VSW M capacity), the ability to retain movement sequences (dynamic VSWM capacity) and the ability to mentally rotate abstract figures were related to mathematical skills (n = 0.44 - 0.57). In Experiment 2, the contribution of other W M components to mathematical skills was examined. The results suggest that performances in the static visuo-spatial task (visual matrix pattern task) and in the mental rotation task are related to a mathematics test score (n = 0.42 - 0.58). Other WM components, viz. the central executive and phonological WM , did not relate to mathematical skills in the present study.
Article
Two experiments examined 6-month-old infants' ability to individuate and enumerate physical actions—the sequential jumps of a puppet In both experiments, which employed a habituation paradigm, infants successfully discriminated two-jump from three-jump sequences The sequences of activity in the two experiments provided for an initial exploration of the cues infants use to individuate actions Results show that (a) infants can individuate and enumerate actions in a sequence, indicating that their enumeration mechanism is quite general in the kinds of entities over which it will operate, (b) actions whose temporal boundaries are characterized by a contrast between motion and absence of motion are especially easy to individuate and enumerate, but nonetheless (c) infants can individuate and enumerate actions embedded in a sequence of continuous motion, indicating that infants possess procedures for parsing an ongoing motionful scene into distinct portions of activity
Article
How does space come to be used to represent nonspatial relations, as in graphs? Approximately 1200 children and adults from three language cultures, English, Hebrew, and Arabic, produced graphic representations of spatial, temporal, quantitative, and preference relations. Children placed stickers on square pieces of paper to represent, for example, a disliked food, a liked food, and a favorite food. Two major analyses of these data were performed. The analysis of directionality of the represented relation showed effects of direction of written language only for representations of temporal concepts, where left-to-right was dominant for speakers of English and right-to-left for speakers of Arabic, with Hebrew speakers in between. For quantity and preference, all canonical directions except top-to-bottom were used approximately equally by all cultures and ages. The analysis of information represented in the graphic representations showed an age trend; more of the older children represented ordinal and some interval information in their mappings. There was a small effect of abstractness of concept on information represented, with more interval information represented by children for the more concrete concepts, space, time, quantity, and preference in that order. Directionality findings were related to language-specific left-to-right or right-to-left directionality and to universal association of more or better with upward. The difficulties in externally representing interval information were related to prevalent difficulties in expressing comparative information. Children's graphic productions were compared to other invented notation systems, by children and by cultures, particularly for numbers and language.
Article
Ages 11-17. 1 form. 26 (44) min. Manual, profile sheet. (See J. consult. Psychol., 1947, 11, 340.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
‘Number sense’ is a short-hand for our ability to quickly understand, approximate, and manipulate numerical quantities. My hypothesis is that number sense rests on cerebral circuits that have evolved specifically for the purpose of representing basic arithmetic knowledge. Four lines of evidence suggesting that number sense constitutes a domain-specific, biologically-determined ability are reviewed: the presence of evolutionary precursors of arithmetic in animals; the early emergence of arithmetic competence in infants independently of other abilities, including language; the existence of a homology between the animal, infant, and human adult abilities for number processing; and the existence of a dedicated cerebral substrate. In adults of all cultures, lesions to the inferior parietal region can specifically impair number sense while leaving the knowledge of other cognitive domains intact. Furthermore, this region is demonstrably activated during number processing. I postulate that higher–level cultural devel-opments in arithmetic emerge through the establishment of linkages between this core analogical representation (the ‘number line’ ) and other verbal and visual representations of number notations. The neural and cognitive organization of those representations can explain why some mathematical concepts are intuitive, while others are so difficult to grasp. Thus, the ultimate foundations of mathematics rests on core representations that have been internalized in our brains through evolution.
Article
An enduring question in philosophy and psychology is that of how we come to possess knowledge of number. Here I review research suggesting that the capacity to represent and reason about number is part of the inherent structure of the human mind. In the first few months of life, human infants can enumerate sets of entities and perform numerical computations. One proposal is that these abilities arise from general cognitive capacities not specific to number. I argue that the body of data supports a very different proposal: humans possess a specialized mental mechanism for number, one which we share with other species and which has evolved through natural selection. This mechanism is inherently restricted in the kinds of numerical knowledge it can support, leading to some striking limitations to early competence.