ArticlePDF Available

Quantifying social semantics: An inclusive definition of socialness and ratings for 8388 English words

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

It has been proposed that social experience plays an important role in the grounding of concepts, and socialness has been proffered as a fundamental organisational principle underpinning semantic representation in the human brain. However, the empirical support for these hypotheses is limited by inconsistencies in the way socialness has been defined and measured. To further advance theory, the field must establish a clearer working definition, and research efforts could be facilitated by the availability of an extensive set of socialness ratings for individual concepts. Therefore, in the current work, we employed a novel and inclusive definition to test the extent to which socialness is reliably perceived as a broad construct, and we report socialness norms for over 8000 English words, including nouns, verbs, and adjectives. Our inclusive socialness measure shows good reliability and validity, and our analyses suggest that the socialness ratings capture aspects of word meaning which are distinct to those measured by other pertinent semantic constructs, including concreteness and emotional valence. Finally, in a series of regression analyses, we show for the first time that the socialness of a word's meaning explains unique variance in participant performance on lexical tasks. Our dataset of socialness norms has considerable item overlap with those used in both other lexical/semantic norms and in available behavioural mega-studies. They can help target testable predictions about brain and behaviour derived from multiple representation theories and neurobiological accounts of social semantics.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Behavior Research Methods
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-022-01810-x
Quantifying social semantics: Aninclusive definition ofsocialness
andratings for8388 English words
VeronicaDiveica1 · PennyM.Pexman2 · RichardJ.Binney1
Accepted: 4 February 2022
© The Author(s) 2022
Abstract
It has been proposed that social experience plays an important role in the grounding of concepts, and socialness has been
proffered as a fundamental organisational principle underpinning semantic representation in the human brain. However, the
empirical support for these hypotheses is limited by inconsistencies in the way socialness has been defined and measured. To
further advance theory, the field must establish a clearer working definition, and research efforts could be facilitated by the
availability of an extensive set of socialness ratings for individual concepts. Therefore, in the current work, we employed a
novel and inclusive definition to test the extent to which socialness is reliably perceived as a broad construct, and we report
socialness norms for over 8000 English words, including nouns, verbs, and adjectives. Our inclusive socialness measure
shows good reliability and validity, and our analyses suggest that the socialness ratings capture aspects of word meaning
which are distinct to those measured by other pertinent semantic constructs, including concreteness and emotional valence.
Finally, in a series of regression analyses, we show for the first time that the socialness of a word's meaning explains unique
variance in participant performance on lexical tasks. Our dataset of socialness norms has considerable item overlap with those
used in both other lexical/semantic norms and in available behavioural mega-studies. They can help target testable predictions
about brain and behaviour derived from multiple representation theories and neurobiological accounts of social semantics.
Keywords Word ratings· Lexical decisions· Semantic cognition· Social cognition· Grounded cognition
Introduction
Conceptual knowledge is the foundation of our complex
interactions with the environment, bringing meaning to the
objects, words, and social agents we encounter. A major
challenge for the cognitive sciences is therefore to charac-
terise how meaning is represented in the brain. Of particular
interest has been the issue of how the mental representations
of concepts become connected to their referents, termed the
symbol grounding problem (Harnad, 1990; Searle, 1980).
Within multiple representation accounts of semantic pro-
cessing, concepts are mapped to the world, or grounded,
by being directly represented within the neural systems
underpinning multiple experiential channels such as per-
ception, action, emotion, language and cognition (Borghi
etal., 2018; Kiefer & Harpaintner, 2020). Sensorimotor
systems are particularly important for grounding concrete
concepts such as festival and politician. In contrast, abstract
concepts like romance and democracy cannot, by defini-
tion, be directly experienced through the senses, and may
thus rely to a greater degree on other types of information,
such as affective (Fingerhut & Prinz, 2018; Kousta etal.,
2011), introspective (Shea, 2018) and linguistic experience
(Borghi etal., 2019; Dove, 2018). Further, there is growing
recognition that there are different types of abstract concepts
which depend to varying extents on these manifold sources
of information (Harpaintner etal., 2018; Villani etal., 2019)
and which elicit different patterns of behavioural responses
in lexical-semantic tasks (Muraki etal., 2020).
Recently, there has been a rise in interest concerning the
role that social experience plays in the acquisition and rep-
resentation of concepts. Indeed, there are proposals in which
social interaction and social context are pinpointed as a key
* Veronica Diveica
psuda2@bangor.ac.uk; veronicadiveica@gmail.com
* Richard J. Binney
R.Binney@bangor.ac.uk
1 School ofHuman andBehavioural Sciences, Bangor
University, Gwynedd, WalesLL572AS, UK
2 Department ofPsychology andHotchkiss Brain Institute,
University ofCalgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
source or mechanism for grounding that may be particu-
larly important for the representation of abstract concepts
(Barsalou, 2020; Borghi etal., 2019). For instance, Barsalou
(2020) proposed that the social environment (e.g., agents,
social interaction, culture) provides one form of grounding,
in addition to that afforded by perceptual modalities, both
of which are distinguished from the body, and the physical
environment. Likewise, Borghi etal. (2019) argued that both
social interactions and linguistic inputs are crucial for the
acquisition of abstract concepts (also see Borghi & Binkof-
ski, 2014). In Pexman etal. (2021), we have reviewed these
theoretical perspectives as well as two parallel sets of empir-
ical literature, which provide some evidence for socialness
being a key principle underpinning semantic representation.
For example, property generation and feature ratings studies
found that social semantic content, or socialness, helps dis-
tinguish concrete from abstract concepts (Barsalou & Wie-
mer-Hastings, 2005; Troche etal., 2014; Wiemer-Hastings &
Xu, 2005) and even different sub-types of abstract concepts
(Harpaintner etal., 2018; Villani etal., 2019). In parallel, a
set of neuroimaging studies have found that words high in
socialness are associated with differential patterns of brain
activation during semantic processing (e.g., Arioli etal.,
2021a, b; Binney etal., 2016; Mellem etal., 2016; Rice
etal., 2018; Wang etal., 2019; for another review, also see
Conca etal., 2021). Some authors have argued for a special
status of social concepts over other types of concept, and
have suggested that socialness may even be a fundamental
driver behind the functional organisation of the semantic
system (Lin etal., 2018; Ross & Olson, 2010; Simmons
etal., 2010; Zahn etal., 2007). These studies were all based
on limited word samples, but they provide some evidence
that social words might be a distinct type of concept, in
line with proposals of some multimodal (e.g., Borghi etal.,
2018) and neurobiological models (e.g., Olson etal., 2013)
of conceptual processing.
These theories are nascent and there are many outstand-
ing questions about the nature and extent of the contribu-
tion that socialness makes to semantic representation. One
fundamental question is whether socialness is a behaviour-
ally relevant principle as indexed, for example, by its ability
to account for variance in performance on lexical-semantic
tasks. However, the extant empirical support is limited by
the way socialness has been defined and measured. To our
knowledge, the largest source of openly available socialness
norms was compiled by Troche etal. (2017) and includes
social interaction ratings for 750 English nouns. Another
dataset collected by Binder etal. (2016) includes ratings
for 434 nouns, 62 verbs, and 39 adjectives on four socially
relevant dimensions labelled social, communication, human
and self. Thus, the scale and scope (i.e., the syntactic classes
of words) at which socialness has been explored has been
limited to date. Moreover, socialness as a construct has been
defined variably in terms of behavioural descriptiveness, and
there is no consensus on the criteria that differentiate social
from non-social concepts. The heterogeneity in definitions is
summarised by Pexman etal. (2021); some researchers have
measured socialness as, for example, the degree to which a
word’s meaning refers to relationships between people (Tro-
che etal., 2014, 2017), to social as opposed to individual
contexts (Arioli etal., 2021a), or to the relationship between
self and others (Crutch etal., 2012), and socialness has also
been defined as how well words describe social behaviour
(Zahn etal., 2007). This variability in the operationalisation
of socialness hinders our ability to compare findings across
studies and glean a broader understanding of the contribu-
tion made by socialness to conceptual representation in the
brain, and its behavioural consequences. Thus, we argue that
to further progress theory, the field must first establish a
clearer working definition of socialness.
Moreover, many of these past studies employed social-
ness definitions that emphasise specific aspects of social
experience (Pexman etal., 2021). These narrow definitions
might neglect important aspects of our highly complex inter-
actions with the social environment. Thus, taking a crucial
next step for understanding the construct of socialness,
we aimed to collect ratings using an inclusive definition
designed to capture all manner of features that are deemed
to be socially relevant. This allowed us to test the extent to
which socialness is reliably perceived as a broad construct.
Relatedly, our socialness definition can be equally applied
to a wide range of words, from nouns like those referring to
social roles (e.g., lawyer) or institutions (e.g., government),
to verbs like to befriend, and adjectives like trustworthy.
This broad and inclusive definition can be used as a starting
point for future studies exploring more fine-grained aspects
of the socialness construct.
In summary, the aims of the present study were as fol-
lows: 1) collect socialness ratings for a large set of Eng-
lish words to provide a useful resource for future research
endeavours; 2) use an inclusive definition to assess the extent
to which socialness is reliably perceived as a broad con-
struct; 3) explore to what extent these new socialness rat-
ings capture aspects of word meaning that are distinct from
those measured via other related semantic variables, such
as concreteness and emotional valence, and 4) test whether
socialness is a behaviourally relevant construct.
Method
Participants
Participants were recruited via the online platform Prolific
(https:// www. proli fic. co/). Responders were restricted to
those who self-reported being fluent in English and having
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
no language disorders. A total of 605 participants (359 male,
240 female, six unspecified, Mage = 29.44 years, SDage =
10.6) completed the study. Participants completed the rat-
ing task in 34 minutes on average and were compensated
with GBP £4. Following exclusions (see below), the final
sample consisted of 539 participants, with ages ranging
from 18 to 76 years (M = 29.7; SD = 10.67). Of the partici-
pants, 216 (40.07%) were female, 317 (58.81%) male and
six (1.11%) unspecified. English was the first language for
273 (50.65%) participants. Of the remaining 266 (49.35%)
participants, 111 self-reported as being proficient in English,
124 advanced and 31 beginner/intermediate. A total of 185
(34.32%) participants were monolingual, while the remain-
ing 354 (65.68%) reported speaking more than one language.
Stimuli
The stimuli were 8948 words, including 5569 nouns, 1343
verbs, 2009 adjectives, and 26 other parts of speech (based
on the dominant part-of-speech norms in Brysbaert etal.,
2012) 1. We compiled our stimulus set from two sources:
the Calgary Semantic Decision Project (Pexman etal.,
2017) and Brysbaert etal. (2014)’s dataset of concreteness
ratings. Ratings on emotion dimensions (valence, arousal,
dominance) from Warriner etal. (2013) and on concreteness
from Brysbaert etal. (2014) are available for all of the words
included and the selected words span the entire continuum
of these dimensions. In addition, we specifically selected
these words so that there would be considerable overlap with
behavioural mega-studies and other theoretically important
psycholinguistic dimensions, some of which were used in
analyses reported below, whereas others might be of interest
in future research (e.g., Calgary Semantic Decision Project
(Pexman etal., 2017), the Lancaster Sensorimotor Norms
(Lynott etal., 2020), the Glasgow norms (Scott etal., 2019),
word association norms (De Deyne etal., 2019), word preva-
lence norms (Brysbaert etal., 2018)).
We used 30 of the 8948 words as a set of control items
which were to be presented to every participant and used
during the data cleaning process (see below). These words
were selected based on the ratings received in a pilot study
(N = 36 participants) that was run to obtain an initial assess-
ment of whether participants understand the task instructions
and, in particular, the description of the inclusive socialness
measure, and whether they provide reliable ratings (for a
detailed description, see Section S1 of Supplementary Mate-
rials). Control words were selected to vary in the mean pilot
socialness ratings, as well as in their concreteness (Brysbaert
etal., 2014) and valence ratings (Warriner etal., 2013).
In addition to the 8948 words, we selected 12 practice
words to be rated before the main ratings task so that partici-
pants could become familiar with the task requirements. We
selected practice words that vary in concreteness (Brysbaert
etal., 2014) and valence (Warriner etal., 2013), and that
span the whole range of the social interaction dimension
as measured by Troche etal. (2017) to ensure that partici-
pants practised both items with high and with low socialness
ratings.
We used Qualtrics software (Qualtrics, 2020) to cre-
ate two questionnaires for presentation to participants. To
facilitate efficient Qualtrics processing, we divided the 8918
words into two lists of 4459 words from which each par-
ticipant saw a random subset. These lists were equated for
letter length, frequency (log subtitle frequency; Brysbaert
& New, 2009), concreteness (Brysbaert etal., 2014) and
valence (Warriner etal., 2013) to control for the probability
of selecting words with different characteristics from each
list. The control words were then added to both lists, result-
ing in two questionnaires each with 4489 words.
Procedure
The word stimuli were presented using Qualtrics (2020) and
linked to the Prolific online recruitment platform (www. pr oli
fic. co). Following the consent form, a demographics survey
and instructions, participants rated the 12 practice words,
then proceeded to rate the main set of items. Each partici-
pant rated 370 words randomly selected from one of the two
item lists, plus the 30 control words. The control words were
randomly intermixed with other items. The full instructions
given to participants are presented in Section S2 of sup-
plementary materials. In short, the participants were asked
to rate the degree to which the words’ meaning has social
relevance by describing or referring to the following:
a social characteristic of a person or group of people, a
social behaviour or interaction, a social role, a social
space, a social institution or system, a social value or
ideology, or any other socially relevant concept.
Participants provided their answers using a seven-point
Likert scale presented horizontally below each word. In
addition, there was an “I don’t know the meaning of this
word” option. There were 25 words presented per page. We
collected data until we obtained at least 25 ratings per word.
Data cleaning
In total, we collected 241,575 observations. The data clean-
ing pipeline involved sequentially implementing several
techniques consistent with recommendations for identify-
ing careless or insufficient effort responders (Curran, 2016)
and computer-generated random responding (Dupuis etal.,
1 Note that part-of-speech information was not available for one
word: hip hop.
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
2019), as well as other data cleaning procedures used in
previous word norming studies (Brysbaert etal., 2014; Pex-
man etal., 2019; Warriner etal., 2013). First, we removed
data from participants if they completed less than 33% of
the ratings task (n = 0), responded with “I don’t know the
meaning of this word” for more than 25% of items (n = 8)
and provided the same rating for more than 25 words in
a row (n = 17). Next, we examined each participant’s rat-
ings of the 30 control words and generated correlations with
the mean ratings of those words obtained in the pilot study.
We removed data from 36 participants with a correlation
coefficient less than .20. We then computed the correlation
between each participant’s ratings and the mean ratings of
all other participants. We deleted data from five participants
with a correlation coefficient less than .10. Finally, if more
than 15% of raters reported not knowing a particular word,
we removed those words from the analyses reported below.
This led to the exclusion of 560 words.
The final dataset was comprised of 8388 words and
202,841 observations, of which 3542 were “I don’t know the
meaning of this word” responses. Not taking into account
the control words rated by all participants, each word in the
final dataset had 21.92 valid ratings on average (SD = 1.68),
ranging from 15 to 27 ratings. Overall, 7703 (91.83%) words
had at least 20 valid ratings.
Data analysis overview
Data pre-processing, analysis and visualisation was accom-
plished using RStudio version 3.6.1 (RStudio Team, 2020).
We first computed descriptive statistics for the socialness
ratings and assessed their reliability. Then, to begin to
explore the nature of the information captured by the social-
ness dimension and characterize its relationship with other
pertinent psycholinguistic constructs, we computed the zero-
order correlations between the mean socialness ratings and
a variety of lexical and semantic properties of the words.
Next, we conducted a series of hierarchical regression analy-
ses to examine whether the socialness measure is related
to behaviour in lexical tasks, using behavioural responses
from the English Lexicon Project (ELP) lexical decision task
(LDT; Balota etal., 2007) and the English Crowdsourcing
Project (ECP) word knowledge task (Mandera etal., 2020).
The LDT outcome variables quantify the speed and accuracy
with which participants could distinguish between words
and non-word letter strings. The ECP RT outcome variable
measures the speed with which participants could recognize
a word as known to them, while the percentage of partici-
pants reporting not knowing a word (henceforth proportion
unknown) is a measure of word prevalence. We selected
these tasks because they require only a fairly shallow level
of semantic access (Muraki etal., 2020) and thus provide
a conservative test of the relationship between this meas-
ure and lexical semantic processing. In addition, in both of
these tasks, all word stimuli received the same behavioural
response (“word” in the ELP LDT, or “I know that word” in
the ECP) unlike, for instance, semantic decision tasks (e.g.,
Pexman etal., 2017) which involve different responses for
different types of words. All predictor variables were mean-
centred and we used reaction times standardized as z-scores
because these reduce the influence of individual differences
on overall processing speed (Faust etal., 1999).
Results
Descriptive statistics
The raw data and resulting socialness ratings are provided
on the Open Science Framework (OSF) project page (avail-
able at: https:// osf. io/ 2dqnj/). The socialness ratings have
a unimodal distribution with a mean of 3.63 (SD = 1.24)
Fig. 1 Distribution of socialness ratings. A Histogram of socialness
ratings for 8388 words; the dotted line represents the mean. B Ker-
nel density plot of ratings as a function of syntactic class. C Standard
deviation of ratings plotted against their respective mean rating, along
with a loess line (in green) that highlights the functional relationship
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
(Fig.1a). More descriptive statistics for the mean ratings
are provided in Table1 and the distribution of ratings as a
function of part of speech is depicted in Fig.1b. The rat-
ings have an average standard deviation of 1.85 (SD = 0.35)
and participants provided more consistent responses at the
extremes of the scale (Fig.1c). Examples of words at the
extremes of the socialness dimension are given in Table2.
Words like friendship, people and sociable received high
socialness values, while words like avalanche, millimeter
and hemoglobin received low socialness ratings, suggesting
good face validity.
Reliability andvalidity
We first examined the reliability of the ratings by computing
the one-way intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) of all
ratings using variances estimated via a random effects model
with a global intercept and a random intercept per word
(Brysbaert, 2019; Stevens & Brysbaert, 2016). We found
an ICC of 0.9, which indicates good reliability of the mean
socialness ratings. We further computed the split-half reli-
ability for the 30 control words which were the only items in
our dataset rated by all participants. We found a mean Spear-
man–Brown corrected split-half reliability of 0.998 (SD =
0.16) across 100 random splits, suggesting high reliability
for the control items.
We then examined the validity of the ratings by comput-
ing the correlations between the ratings observed here and
the mean ratings collected in the pilot study (n = 60 words),
as well as two previous related sets of social interaction
norms collected by Binder etal. (2016) (n = 258 words),
and Troche etal. (2017) (n = 450 words). The current social-
ness ratings were strongly and positively correlated with the
ratings collected in the pilot study (r = 0.97) and with the
previous social interaction ratings collected by Binder etal.
(2016) (r = 0.76) and Troche etal. (2017) (r = 0.76), sug-
gesting good validity.
Correlations withlexical andsemantic properties
We examined the correlations between the socialness ratings
and various lexical and semantic properties of the words.
We included lexical dimensions in our analysis as previ-
ous work has shown that semantic content is not independ-
ent of the linguistic properties of words (Lewis & Frank,
2016; Reilly etal., 2012, 2017; Strik-Lievers etal., 2021).
The lexical variables included letter length, orthographic
Levenshtein distance (Yarkoni etal., 2008), phonological
Levenshtein distance and frequency (log subtitle frequency;
Brysbaert & New, 2009). To examine the proposed rela-
tionship between socialness and abstractness (Borghi etal.,
2019), we included the following semantic variables that
index sensorimotor experience: concreteness (the degree
to which the word’s referent can be experienced through
one of the five senses ; Brysbaert etal., 2014), imageabil-
ity (the ease with which the word arouses a mental image ;
Cortese & Fugett, 2004; Schock etal., 2012), body–object
interaction (BOI; the ease with which a human body can
physically interact with a word’s referent; Pexman etal.,
2019), and sensory experience ratings (the degree of sen-
sory experience evoked; Juhasz & Yap, 2012). To assess the
generalizability of the association between socialness and
Table 1 Descriptive statistics for socialness ratings for 8388 words
Descriptive statistic Value
Mean 3.63
Median 3.57
Standard Deviation 1.24
Minimum 1.05
Maximum 7.00
1st Quartile 2.62
3rd Quartile 4.58
Skewness 0.19
Kurtosis – 0.80
Table 2 List of words at the extremes of the socialness dimension
Highest-rated words Rating Lowest-rated words Rating
friendship 7.00 eucalyptus 1.05
socialize 7.00 horizontal 1.09
relationship 6.96 crocodile 1.09
people 6.90 sulfur 1.10
romance 6.78 sleeve 1.17
marriage 6.76 turbo 1.18
socialism 6.75 cranberry 1.18
political 6.73 dragonfly 1.18
family 6.72 hemoglobin 1.20
teamwork 6.72 shark 1.21
boyfriend 6.68 sunflower 1.21
friend 6.68 sandpaper 1.22
sociable 6.68 millimeter 1.22
sisterhood 6.67 avalanche 1.22
mother 6.67 spinach 1.22
democracy 6.65 airspeed 1.23
togetherness 6.65 button 1.23
sister 6.65 redwood 1.23
festival 6.64 pistachio 1.24
stepfather 6.64 birch 1.25
humankind 6.62 haystack 1.25
meeting 6.62 toothpaste 1.26
parental 6.62 paprika 1.27
befriend 6.61 cellophane 1.28
chatty 6.61 magnolia 1.28
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
affective information reported in previous studies (Troche
etal., 2014, 2017; Villani etal., 2019), we included in our
analysis valence extremity (the degree to which the word
evokes positive/negative feelings; this was measured as the
absolute difference between the valence rating and the neu-
tral point of the original valence scale by Warriner etal.,
2013), arousal (the degree to which the word evokes feel-
ings of arousal as opposed to calm; Warriner etal., 2013),
and dominance (the degree to which the word evokes feel-
ings of being controlled as opposed to in control; Warriner
etal., 2013). Finally, to assess the relationship between the
socialness ratings and linguistic experience, the semantic
variables included semantic diversity (the extent to which
a word appears in semantically diverse contexts; Hoffman
etal., 2013), rating-based age of acquisition (AoA) (Kuper-
man etal., 2012), and a test-based AoA measure derived
from (Dale & O’Rourke, 1981) and updated by (Brysbaert
& Biemiller, 2017).
These correlations revealed several interesting relation-
ships that provide insight as to the nature of the word social-
ness measure (Fig.2; see Fig.S1 for scatterplots). Socialness
was negatively correlated with concreteness (r = – 0.32),
imageability (r = – 0.18), and BOI (r = – 0.17), which sug-
gests that words with less social relevance are associated
with more embodied sensorimotor information. In contrast,
socialness ratings were positively correlated with valence
extremity (r = 0.22) and arousal (r = 0.22), suggesting that
social words tend to have more affective information.
Relationships withperformance onlexical tasks
Next, we examined whether the socialness ratings are related
to lexical-semantic processing using behavioural responses
from the ELP LDT (Balota etal., 2007) and the ECP word
knowledge task (Mandera etal., 2020). We conducted a
series of item-wise hierarchical regression analyses in which
we included other lexical and semantic predictors (that are
typically related to behaviour in lexical tasks) in order to
isolate the unique relationships of socialness to standardized
reaction times (RTs), ELP error rates and ECP proportion
unknown. In the first step, we entered the control predic-
tors letter length, frequency (Brysbaert & New, 2009) and
Fig. 2 Correlations between mean socialness ratings and lexical-
semantic dimensions. Only correlations significant at p < .01 are
shown. The strength and direction of the correlation coefficients are
indicated by the colour and the numerical values. For each variable of
interest, the numbers of items in common with our socialness ratings
are as follows: length, concreteness, valence, arousal, and dominance:
8388; log subtitle frequency: 8160; OLD and PLD: 8027; rating-
based AoA: 8348; test-based AoA: 7321; imageability: 2680; BOI:
4038; SER: 2645. SER = sensory experience rating; BOI = body-
object interaction; AoA = age of acquisition; PLD = phonologic Lev-
enshtein distance; OLD = orthographic Levenshtein distance
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
rating-based AoA (Kuperman etal., 2012). In the second
step, we entered the semantic predictors: socialness, con-
creteness (Brysbaert etal., 2014), valence extremity (War-
riner etal., 2013) and semantic diversity (Hoffman etal.,
2013). We selected these other semantic predictors on the
basis of multidimensional theories (e.g., Borghi etal., 2019)
that highlight the simultaneous contribution of semantic var-
iables derived from multiple sources, including linguistic
(semantic diversity), sensorimotor (concreteness) and affec-
tive experience (valence extremity).
There were 6926 items for which we had values for all
variables of interest in the analysis predicting LDT per-
formance. Descriptive statistics and zero-order correla-
tions between all variables of interest from this dataset are
reported in Supplementary TableS1. The statistical results
are reported in Table3 and the standardized coefficients are
illustrated in Fig.3a. In this analysis, the control variables
were all significant predictors of LDT latencies – RTs were
faster for words that are shorter, more frequent and acquired
earlier. There was significant improvement in model fit with
the addition of the semantic variables, which collectively
accounted for a further 0.61% of variance in LDT laten-
cies. Of the semantic variables, only socialness and seman-
tic diversity were significant predictors, with faster RTs for
words with increased social relevance and for those encoun-
tered in more semantically diverse contexts. A similar pat-
tern of results was observed when predicting LDT error
rates. The control variables were all significant predictors,
with fewer errors for words that are longer, more frequent
and acquired earlier. There was significant improvement
in model fit with the inclusion of the semantic variables,
which accounted for an additional 0.56% of variance in LDT
error rates. Socialness and semantic diversity were the only
significant semantic predictors – error rates were lower for
words with increased socialness and for those that are more
semantically diverse.
There were 7010 items for which we had values for all
variables of interest in the analysis predicting performance
in the ECP word knowledge task. Descriptive statistics and
zero-order correlations between all variables of interest from
this dataset are reported in Supplementary TableS2. The
statistical results are reported in Table4 and the standardized
coefficients and illustrated in Fig.3b. In this analysis, the
control variables were all significant predictors of response
latencies – RTs were faster for words that are shorter, more
frequent and acquired earlier. There was significant improve-
ment in model fit with the addition of the semantic variables,
which accounted for a further 0.78% of variance in recogni-
tion RTs. All semantic variables were significant predictors,
with faster RTs for words with increased socialness, con-
creteness and valence extremity and for those encountered
in more semantically diverse contexts. The control variables
were all significant predictors of the proportion of people
reporting not knowing a word, with words that are longer,
more frequent and acquired earlier being more prevalent.
There was significant improvement in model fit with the
inclusion of the semantic variables, which accounted for an
additional 0.83% of variance in ECP proportion unknown.
Valence and semantic diversity were the only significant
semantic predictors – words that are more valenced and
encountered in more semantically diverse contexts were
reported as known by more people.
Table 3 Regression coefficients from item-level analyses predicting lexical decision task latencies and error rates (N = 6926)
Note. b represents unstandardized regression weights. SE represents the standard error of the regression weights. sr2 represents the semi-partial
correlation squared. zRTs standardized reaction times. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001
Predictor zRTs Error rates
b SE t p sr2R2R2b SE t p sr2R2R2
Step 1 0.51 0.21
 Intercept – 0.25 0.003 – 94.49 *** 0.06 0.001 70.97 ***
 Length 0.05 0.001 35.6 *** 0.09 – 0.01 < .001 – 22.57 *** 0.058
 Frequency – 0.15 0.005 – 29.99 *** 0.064 – 0.03 0.002 – 19 *** 0.041
 Age of Acquisition 0.04 0.001 26.91 *** 0.051 0.01 < .001 22.99 *** 0.06
Step 2 0.52 0.006 0.22 0.006
 Intercept – 0.25 0.003 – 95.06 *** 0.06 0.001 71.21 ***
 Length 0.05 0.001 35.75 *** 0.089 – 0.01 < .001 – 21.5 *** 0.052
 Frequency – 0.13 0.005 – 23.9 *** 0.04 – 0.03 0.002 – 14.7 *** 0.024
 Age of Acquisition 0.04 0.001 25.78 *** 0.046 0.01 0.001 22.31 *** 0.056
 Socialness – 0.01 0.002 – 4.73 *** 0.002 – 0.003 0.001 – 3.57 *** 0.001
 Concreteness < .001 0.004 0.02 0.984 0 0.002 0.001 1.7 0.088 0
 Valence Extremity 0.01 0.004 1.83 0.067 0 – 0.001 0.001 – 0.64 0.525 0
 Semantic Diversity – 0.07 0.01 – 6.77 *** 0.003 – 0.01 0.003 – 3.54 *** 0.001
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
Discussion
Although some contemporary accounts (e.g., Barsalou,
2020;Borghi etal., 2019 ; Kiefer & Harpaintner, 2020) prof-
fer a role for socialness in the organization and grounding
of conceptual knowledge, many key questions remain about
the nature of its contribution and its neural underpinnings.
With the aim of facilitating future endeavours, in the pre-
sent work we sought to 1) collect socialness norms for a
large set of words; 2) test the extent to which socialness is
reliably perceived as a broad construct; 3) explore to what
extent socialness captures a distinct aspect of word meaning
compared to those measured by other lexical and semantic
variables, and 4) assess whether socialness can account for
variance in behavioural responses in lexical tasks. To this
end, we compiled the largest set of socialness norms avail-
able to date by collecting ratings for a set of 8388 English
words, including nouns, verbs and adjectives. The social-
ness ratings show high reliability, and this suggests that the
construct is meaningful to participants even at the broad
and inclusive level of description provided. Moreover, the
validity of the socialness construct was confirmed by a
strong correlation with ratings on two other social semantic
dimensions (Binder etal., 2016; Troche etal., 2017), despite
the distinct definitions employed. However, our socialness
measure shared around 58% of its variance with each of
these other ratings, possibly reflecting differences in par-
ticipant characteristics or perhaps methodological choices
such as our more inclusive definition which might capture
some additional aspects of social experience. Subsequent
research will be needed to more thoroughly explore the pre-
cise aspects of our interactions with the social environment
that are captured by this inclusive socialness measure, such
as those measured by more restricted definitions (for exam-
ples, see Pexman etal., 2021).
Our preliminary analyses provide some important ini-
tial insights into the nature of the socialness dimension.
First, while low socialness words tend to be concrete,
Fig. 3 Standardized coefficient weights and 95% CIs for the second
step of the hierarchical regression analyses predicting task outcome
variables. A Standardized beta coefficients for LDT RTs (blue) and
errors (red). B Standardized beta coefficients for ECP Word Knowl-
edge Task RTs (blue) and the proportion of people reporting not
knowing a word (red)
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
high socialness words span the entire concreteness con-
tinuum, from concrete concepts like mother, to more
abstract ones like political. In line with previous reports
of a negative association between a social interaction
measure and modality-specific perceptual ratings (Tro-
che etal., 2017), we found that words high in socialness
tend to be more abstract and to rely less on sensorimotor
information. However, the present findings further suggest
that socialness does not relate to concreteness in a simple
linear fashion. Although theories of conceptual represen-
tation have proposed that social concepts are a sub-type
of abstract concepts (Borghi etal., 2019; Kiefer & Har-
paintner, 2020), this finding highlights the need to better
understand the contribution made by socialness beyond
this extreme of the concreteness dimension. Second, we
found that words with increased socialness tend to be more
valenced and arousing. This is in line with findings that
social and affective dimensions reduce to the same latent
factor of a multidimensional semantic space (Troche etal.,
2014, 2017; Villani etal., 2019). Importantly, while the
socialness ratings are significantly correlated with all the
lexical and semantic variables explored here, the associ-
ated effect sizes are modest and suggest that the socialness
measure captures a distinct aspect of word meaning. This
is consistent with fMRI studies which found that the effect
of socialness on brain activation during lexical-semantic
processing is independent from that of a number of key
semantic variables, namely imageability, concreteness,
and valence, and suggest that socialness makes a unique
contribution to semantic representation (Lin etal., 2018;
Wang etal., 2019).
Using regression analyses, we have demonstrated for
the first time that socialness of word meaning is related to
performance in lexical tasks. This is true even at the broad
and inclusive level of description provided. Specifically,
we found a facilitatory effect on behavioural performance
in lexical decision and word knowledge tasks – increased
socialness was associated with faster decision latencies in
both tasks and with better accuracy in the LDT. Importantly,
this was true after controlling for other semantic variables
known to influence lexical-semantic processing, namely con-
creteness, valence and semantic diversity. Further, this was
true even in lexical tasks that involve only shallow semantic
processing, where there is a limited pool of variance to be
explained by semantic predictors. This unique contribution
of the socialness measure suggests that it captures important
information about semantic representation and processing
and is in line with previous research on semantic richness
effects. Semantic richness refers to the phenomenon whereby
responses to words that are associated with relatively more
semantic information tend to be facilitated in lexical and
semantic tasks by virtue of their richer representations that
allow faster and more accurate retrieval of meaning (for a
review, see Pexman, 2012). As such, increased socialness
might enrich a word’s conceptual representation and, con-
sequently, facilitate lexical decisions via stronger feedback
from semantic to orthographic representations (Hino etal.,
2002; Hino & Lupker, 1996). Furthermore, our results sug-
gest that socialness contributes to processing alongside
other meaning dimensions derived from multiple experien-
tial channels including linguistic (i.e., semantic diversity),
sensorimotor (i.e., concreteness) and affective experience
Table 4 Regression coefficients from item-level analyses predicting ECP word knowledge task latencies and proportion unknown (N = 7010)
Note. b represents unstandardized regression weights. SE represents the standard error of the regression weights. sr2 represents the semi-partial
correlation squared. zRTs standardized reaction times. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001
Predictor zRTs Proportion unknown
b SE t p sr2R2R2b SE t p sr2R2R2
Step 1 0.4 0.23
 Intercept – 0.53 0.001 – 495.33 *** 0.013 < .001 69.57 ***
 Length 0.01 0.001 19.37 *** 0.032 – 0.002 < .001 – 22.24 *** 0.055
 Frequency – 0.06 0.002 – 27.65 *** 0.065 – 0.007 < .001 – 19.99 *** 0.044
 Age of Acquisition 0.01 0.001 25.28 *** 0.054 0.002 < .001 24.02 *** 0.064
Step2 0.41 0.008 0.23 0.008
 Intercept – 0.53 0.001 – 498.44 *** 0.013 < .001 69.93 ***
 Length 0.01 0.001 20.21 *** 0.034 – 0.002 < .001 – 21.67 *** 0.051
 Frequency – 0.05 0.002 – 22.07 *** 0.041 – 0.006 < .001 – 15.74 *** 0.027
 Age of Acquisition 0.01 0.001 22.5 *** 0.043 0.002 < .001 22.38 *** 0.055
 Socialness – 0.003 0.001 – 3.6 *** 0.001 < .001 < .001 – 0.31 0.754 0
 Concreteness – 0.003 0.001 – 2.04 * < .001 < .001 < .001 1.46 0.145 0
 Valence Extremity – 0.01 0.001 – 6.09 *** 0.003 – 0.001 < .001 – 3.52 *** 0.001
 Semantic Diversity – 0.02 0.004 – 6.01 *** 0.003 – 0.004 0.001 – 5.89 *** 0.004
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
(i.e., valence). This is consistent with theories claiming that
conceptual representation is multidimensional in nature and
that social experience may be one of the underlying semantic
dimensions (e.g., Borghi etal., 2019).
The ability of the semantic dimensions to explain vari-
ance in behavioural responses varied depending on the
requirements of the task. While socialness and semantic
diversity had a facilitatory effect on RTs in both tasks, con-
creteness and valence contributed to the word knowledge
task, but not to the LDT. This is in line with research sug-
gesting that conceptual representations are not stable across
time and contexts; instead, the aspects of a word’s concep-
tual representation retrieved at any one point depend on the
specific task/context (Pexman, 2020; Yee & Thompson-
Schill, 2016). Our pattern of findings may be explained by
the fact that LDT only requires the retrieval of some indica-
tion that a word has meaning, such as that indexed by its
association with a multiplicity of meanings (i.e., semantic
diversity). In comparison, the word recognition task might
require access to additional features of a word’s meaning,
like those that tap into the richness of associated sensori-
motor (i.e., concreteness) and emotional experience (i.e.,
valence extremity). It might also suggest that socialness
does not contribute additional semantic features to enrich
a word’s conceptual representation, but is more indicative
of the general relevance or salience of its meaning. This
might be consistent with our finding that the socialness of a
word does not account for variance in the number of people
who know its meaning. Relatedly, it has been observed that
social stimuli are preferentially processed during free view-
ing of complex naturalistic scenes, to the extent that social-
ness competes with the physical saliency of stimuli (End
& Gamer, 2017, 2019). However, future research is needed
to better understand the nature of the contribution made by
socialness to the semantic richness of concepts (see Muraki
etal., 2019 for an example of how to approach examining
the factor structure of semantic richness). Moreover, it is
important to highlight that, while the words we encounter
are typically embedded in rich linguistic contexts (e.g., sen-
tences) that shape our understanding of individual words, the
socialness ratings were generated based on words presented
in isolation. Future research should address this limitation
by moving away from single word processing and consider-
ing the lexical-semantic properties of connected text/speech.
Conclusions
In the present study, we compiled the largest set of openly
available socialness norms to date. We used an inclusive defi-
nition, found that it produced reliable ratings and, thereby,
showed that socialness has meaning as a broad construct. An
important avenue for future research is identifying the specific
aspects of social experience that are most related to concep-
tual processing to refine our working definition of socialness.
Further, our explorations suggest that socialness captures an
aspect of word meaning that is distinct to those measured by
other key semantic variables and notably, an aspect of meaning
that is behaviourally relevant. Our study also provides some
initial insights into the information captured by the socialness
measure, but subsequent work will be needed on this mat-
ter, as well as its role and behavioural consequences across
the lifespan, including during acquisition, retrieval and when
the semantic system is impaired. Thus, the socialness norms
described here will enable future research into the organiza-
tion and grounding of conceptual knowledge, and can help
target testable predictions about brain and behaviour that can
be derived from multiple representation theories (e.g., Borghi
etal., 2019) and neurobiological accounts of social semantics
(for an extensive discussion, see Pexman etal., 2021; also Bin-
ney etal., 2016; Binney & Ramsey, 2020; Diveica etal., 2021).
Supplementary Information The online version contains supplemen-
tary material available at https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 022- 01810-x.
Open practices statement The datasets generated and/or analysed in
the current study and the analysis scripts are available via the Open Sci-
ence Framework (OSF) project: https:// osf. io/ 2dqnj/. Given its explora-
tory nature, the study was not pre-registered.
Author’s contribution The authors wish it to be known that PMP and
RJB contributed equally to this article. Veronica Diveica: Conceptual-
ization, Methodology, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Visualization,
Writing - Original Draft, Writing – Review and Editing. Penny Pex-
man: Conceptualization, Methodology; Writing – Review and Editing.
Richard Binney: Conceptualization, Methodology; Writing – Review
and Editing; Supervision.
Funding This work was supported by the Economic and Social
Research Council (ESRC) Wales Doctoral Training Partnership in the
form of a PhD studentship [ES/P00069X/1], a joint award from UK
Research and Innovation (UKRI) and Mitacs under the UK-Canada
Globalink Doctoral Exchange Scheme [NE/T014180/1] (both awarded
to VD and RJB; PhD student: VD), the Professor Beatrice Edgell Post-
graduate Grant from the British Psychological Society Welsh Branch
(awarded to VD) and a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Coun-
cil (SSHRC) of Canada Insight Grant (awarded to PMP).
Declarations
Competing interests The authors declare no potential conflicts of inter-
est.
Ethics approval This study was performed in line with the principles
of the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethics approval was granted by Bangor
University School of Psychology Ethics Board (Approval Number:
2017-16108).
Consent to participate Informed consent was obtained from all indi-
vidual participants included in the study.
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attri-
bution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adapta-
tion, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long
as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes
were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in
the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
References
Arioli, M., Basso, G., Poggi, P., & Canessa, N. (2021a). Fronto-
temporal brain activity and connectivity track implicit attention
to positive and negative social words in a novel socio-emo-
tional Stroop task. NeuroImage, 226, 117580. https:// doi. org/
10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2020. 117580
Arioli, M., Gianelli, C., & Canessa, N. (2021b). Neural represen-
tation of social concepts: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of
fMRI studies. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 15(4), 1912–1921.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ S11682- 020- 00384-6
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kes-
sler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B.,
& Treiman, R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior
Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/
BF031 93014
Barsalou, Lawrence W. (2020). Challenges and Opportunities for
Grounding Cognition. Journal of Cognition, 3(1), 1–24. https://
doi. org/ 10. 5334/ JOC. 116
Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating Abstract
Concepts. In D. Pecher & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding Cogni-
tion: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language,
and Thinking (pp. 129–164). Cambridge University Press.
Binder, J. R., Conant, L. L., Humphries, C. J., Fernandino, L., Simons,
S. B., Aguilar, M., & Desai, R. H. (2016). Toward a brain-based
componential semantic representation. Cognitive Neuropsychol-
ogy, 33(3–4), 130–174. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 02643 294. 2016.
11474 26
Binney, R. J., & Ramsey, R. (2020). Social Semantics: The role of
conceptual knowledge and cognitive control in a neurobiologi-
cal model of the social brain. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Reviews, 112, 28–38. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neubi orev. 2020.
01. 030
Binney, R. J., Hoffman, P., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2016). Mapping
the Multiple Graded Contributions of the Anterior Temporal Lobe
Representational Hub to Abstract and Social Concepts: Evidence
from Distortion-corrected fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 4227–4241.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ cercor/ bhw260
Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2014). Words as social tools: an
embodied view on abstract concepts. Springer. https:// doi. org/
10. 1007/ 978-1- 4614- 9539-0
Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., & Tummolini, L. (2018). Vari-
eties of abstract concepts: development, use and representation
in the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:
Biological Sciences, 373(1752). https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/ RSTB.
2017. 0121
Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo,
G., & Tummolini, L. (2019). Words as social tools: Language,
sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts. Physics of Life
Reviews, 29, 120–153. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/J. PLREV. 2018. 12.
001
Brysbaert, M. (2019). How Many Participants Do We Have to Include
in Properly Powered Experiments? A Tutorial of Power Analysis
with Reference Tables. Journal of Cognition, 2(1), 1–38. https://
doi. org/ 10. 5334/ joc. 72
Brysbaert, M., & Biemiller, A. (2017). Test-based age-of-acquisi-
tion norms for 44 thousand English word meanings. Behavior
Research Methods, 49(4), 1520–1523. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/
s13428- 016- 0811-4
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis:
A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the
introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for
American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ BRM. 41.4. 977
Brysbaert, M., New, B., & Keuleers, E. (2012). Adding part-of-speech
information to the SUBTLEX-US word frequencies. Behavior
Research Methods 2012 44:4, 44(4), 991–997. https:// doi. org/ 10.
3758/ S13428- 012- 0190-4
Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness
ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas.
Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 904–911. https:// doi. org/ 10.
3758/ s13428- 013- 0403-5
Brysbaert, M., Mandera, P., McCormick, S. F., & Keuleers, E. (2018).
Word prevalence norms for 62,000 English lemmas. Behavior
Research Methods 2018 51:2, 51(2), 467–479. https:// doi. org/ 10.
3758/ S13428- 018- 1077-9
Conca, F., Borsa, V. M., Cappa, S. F., & Catricalà, E. (2021). The
multidimensionality of abstract concepts: A systematic review.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 127, 474–491. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/J. NEUBI OREV. 2021. 05. 004
Cortese, M. J., & Fugett, A. (2004). Imageability ratings for 3,000
monosyllabic words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments,
& Computers 2004 36:3, 36(3), 384–387. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/
BF031 95585
Crutch, S. J., Williams, P., Ridgway, G. R., & Borgenicht, L. (2012).
The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptual knowl-
edge: Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimensional ratings
of abstract words. Neuropsychologia, 50(11), 2636–2644. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/J. NEURO PSYCH OLOGIA. 2012. 07. 015
Curran, P. G. (2016). Methods for the detection of carelessly invalid
responses in survey data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
ogy, 66, 4–19. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jesp. 2015. 07. 006
Dale, E., & O’Rourke, J. (1981). The living word vocabulary, the words
we know: a national vocabulary inventory. World Book.
De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., Perfors, A., Brysbaert, M., & Storms, G.
(2019). The “Small World of Words” English word association
norms for over 12,000 cue words. Behavior Research Methods,
51(3), 987–1006. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 018- 1115-7
Diveica, V., Koldewyn, K., & Binney, R. J. (2021). Establishing a role
of the semantic control network in social cognitive processing: A
meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage,
245, 118702. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/J. NEURO IMAGE. 2021.
118702
Dove, G. (2018). Language as a disruptive technology: abstract con-
cepts, embodiment and the flexible mind. Philosophical Trans-
actions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752).
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/ RSTB. 2017. 0135
Dupuis, M., Meier, E., & Cuneo, F. (2019). Detecting computer-
generated random responding in questionnaire-based data: A
comparison of seven indices. Behavior Research Methods, 51(5),
2228–2237. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 018- 1103-y
End, A., & Gamer, M. (2017). Preferential processing of social features
and their interplay with physical saliency in complex naturalistic
scenes. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 418. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/
FPSYG. 2017. 00418/ BIBTEX
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
End, A., & Gamer, M. (2019). Task instructions can accelerate the
early preference for social features in naturalistic scenes. Royal
Society Open Science, 6(3). https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/ RSOS. 180596
Faust, M. E., Ferraro, F. R., Balota, D. A., & Spieler, D. H. (1999).
Individual differences in information-processing rate and amount:
Implications for group differences in response latency. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 125(6), 777–799. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0033-
2909. 125.6. 777
Fingerhut, J., & Prinz, J. J. (2018). Grounding evaluative concepts.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological
Sciences, 373(1752), 20170142. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/ RSTB.
2017. 0142
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Non-
linear Phenomena, 42(1–3), 335–346. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/
0167- 2789(90) 90087-6
Harpaintner, M., Trumpp, N. M., & Kiefer, M. (2018). The Semantic
Content of Abstract Concepts: A Property Listing Study of 296
Abstract Words. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1748. https:// doi. org/
10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2018. 01748
Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J. (1996). Effects of polysemy in lexical decision
and naming: An alternative to lexical access accounts. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
22(6), 1331. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0096- 1523. 22.6. 1331
Hino, Y., Lupker, S. J., & Pexman, P. M. (2002). Ambiguity and
synonymy effects in lexical decision, naming, and semantic cat-
egorization tasks: Interactions between orthography, phonology,
and semantics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning
Memory and Cognition, 28(4), 686–713. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/
0278- 7393. 28.4. 686
Hoffman, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2013). Semantic
diversity: A measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in
the contextual usage of words. Behavior Research Methods, 45(3),
718–730. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 012- 0278-x
Juhasz, B. J., & Yap, M. J. (2012). Sensory experience ratings for
over 5,000 mono- and disyllabic words. Behavior Research
Methods 2012 45:1, 45(1), 160–168. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/
S13428- 012- 0242-9
Kiefer, M., & Harpaintner, M. (2020). Varieties of abstract concepts
and their grounding in perception or action. Open Psychology,
2(1), 119–137. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1515/ PSYCH- 2020- 0104
Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del
Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: Why
emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
140(1), 14–34. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ a0021 446
Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012).
Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior
Research Methods, 44(4), 978–990. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/
s13428- 012- 0210-4
Lewis, M. L., & Frank, M. C. (2016). The length of words reflects their
conceptual complexity. Cognition, 153, 182–195. https:// doi. org/
10. 1016/J. COGNI TION. 2016. 04. 003
Lin, N., Wang, X., Xu, Y., Wang, X., Hua, H., Zhao, Y., & Li, X.
(2018). Fine subdivisions of the semantic network supporting
social and sensory–motor semantic processing. Cerebral Cortex,
28(8), 2699–2710. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ CERCOR/ BHX148
Lynott, D., Connell, L., Brysbaert, M., Brand, J., & Carney, J. (2020).
The Lancaster Sensorimotor Norms: multidimensional meas-
ures of perceptual and action strength for 40,000 English words.
Behavior Research Methods, 52, 1271–1291. https:// doi. org/ 10.
3758/ s13428- 019- 01316-z
Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2020). Recognition times
for 62 thousand English words: Data from the English Crowd-
sourcing Project. Behavior Research Methods, 52(2), 741–760.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 019- 01272-8
Mellem, M. S., Jasmin, K. M., Peng, C., & Martin, A. (2016). Sen-
tence processing in anterior superior temporal cortex shows a
social-emotional bias. Neuropsychologia, 89, 217–224. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/J. NEURO PSYCH OLOGIA. 2016. 06. 019
Muraki, E. J., Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2019). Mapping seman-
tic space: property norms and semantic richness. Cognitive Pro-
cessing. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10339- 019- 00933-y
Muraki, E. J., Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2020). Heterogenous
abstract concepts: is “ponder” different from “dissolve”? Psycho-
logical Research. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s00426- 020- 01398-x
Olson, I. R., Mccoy, D., Klobusicky, E., & Ross, L. A. (2013). Social
cognition and the anterior temporal lobes: a review and theoreti-
cal framework. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8,
123–133. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ scan/ nss119
Pexman, P. M. (2012). Meaning-level influences on visual word recog-
nition. In J. Adelman (Ed.), Visual Word Recognition: Meaning
and context, individuals and development (pp. 24–43). Psychol-
ogy Press.
Pexman, P. M. (2020). How Does Meaning Come to Mind? Four Broad
Principles of Semantic Processing. Canadian Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology. https:// doi. org/ 10. 31234/ osf. io/ scwau
Pexman, P. M., Heard, A., Lloyd, E., & Yap, M. J. (2017). The Cal-
gary semantic decision project: concrete/abstract decision data
for 10,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 49(2),
407–417. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 016- 0720-6
Pexman, P. M., Muraki, E., Sidhu, D. M., Siakaluk, P. D., & Yap,
M. J. (2019). Quantifying sensorimotor experience: Body–object
interaction ratings for more than 9,000 English words. Behav-
ior Research Methods, 51(2), 453–466. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/
s13428- 018- 1171-z
Pexman, P. M., Diveica, V., & Binney, R. J. (2021). Social Semantics:
The Organisation and Grounding of Abstract Concepts. PsyArXiv.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 31234/ OSF. IO/ WRBGP
Qualtrics. (2020). Qualtrics (June, 2021). Qualtrics. https:// www. qualt
rics. com
Reilly, J., Westbury, C., Kean, J., & Peelle, J. E. (2012). Arbitrary
symbolism in natural language revisited: When word forms carry
meaning. PLOS ONE, 7(8), e42286. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/
JOURN AL. PONE. 00422 86
Reilly, J., Hung, J., & Westbury, C. (2017). Non-Arbitrariness in Map-
ping Word Form to Meaning: Cross-Linguistic Formal Markers of
Word Concreteness. Cognitive Science, 41(4), 1071–1089. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1111/ COGS. 12361
Rice, G. E., Hoffman, P., Binney, R. J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2018).
Concrete versus abstract forms of social concept: An fMRI com-
parison of knowledge about people versus social terms. Philo-
sophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
373(1752), 20170136. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/ rstb. 2017. 0136
Ross, L. A., & Olson, I. R. (2010). Social cognition and the anterior
temporal lobes. NeuroImage, 49(4), 3452–3462. https:// doi. org/
10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2009. 11. 012
RStudio Team. (2020). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStu-
dio. https:// www. rstud io. com/
Schock, J., Cortese, M. J., & Khanna, M. M. (2012). Imageability esti-
mates for 3,000 disyllabic words. Behavior Research Methods,
44(2), 374–379. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 011- 0162-0
Scott, G. G., Keitel, A., Becirspahic, M., Yao, B., & Sereno, S. C.
(2019). The Glasgow Norms: Ratings of 5,500 words on nine
scales. Behavior Research Methods, 51(3), 1258–1270. https://
doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 018- 1099-3
Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 3(3), 417–424. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ S0140
525X0 00057 56
Shea, N. (2018). Metacognition and abstract concepts. Philosophi-
cal Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
373(1752), 20170133. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/ rstb. 2017. 0133
Simmons, W. K., Reddish, M., Bellgowan, P. S. F., & Martin, A.
(2010). The Selectivity and Functional Connectivity of the
Behavior Research Methods
1 3
Anterior Temporal Lobes. Cerebral Cortex, 20(4), 813–825.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ CERCOR/ BHP149
Stevens, M., & Brysbaert, M. (2016). A simple solution for missing
observations based on random effects models. Informally pub-
lished manuscript available at: http:// crr. ugent. be/ membe rs/ marc-
brysb aert
Strik-Lievers, F., Bolognesi, M., & Winter, B. (2021). The linguistic
dimensions of concrete and abstract concepts: Lexical category,
morphological structure, countability, and etymology. Cognitive
Linguistics, 32(4), 641–670. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1515/ COG- 2021-
0007/ ASSET/ GRAPH IC/J_ COG- 2021- 0007_ FIG_ 005. JPG
Troche, J., Crutch, S., & Reilly, J. (2014). Clustering, hierarchical
organization, and the topography of abstract and concrete nouns.
Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 360. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ FPSYG.
2014. 00360
Troche, J., Crutch, S. J., & Reilly, J. (2017). Defining a conceptual
topography of word concreteness: Clustering properties of emo-
tion, sensation, and magnitude among 750 English words. Fron-
tiers in Psychology, 8, 1787. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2017.
01787
Villani, C., Lugli, L., Liuzza, M., & Borghi, A. M. (2019). Varieties of
abstract concepts and their multiple dimensions. Language and
Cognition, 11(3), 403–430. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ LANGC OG.
2019. 23
Wang, X., Wang, B., & Bi, Y. (2019). Close yet independent: Dissocia-
tion of social from valence and abstract semantic dimensions in
the left anterior temporal lobe. Human Brain Mapping, 40(16),
4759–4776. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1002/ HBM. 24735
Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of
valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas.
Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1191–1207. https:// doi. org/
10. 3758/ s13428- 012- 0314-x
Wiemer-Hastings, K. K., & Xu, X. (2005). Content Differences for
Abstract and Concrete Concepts. Cognitive Science, 29(5), 719–
736. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1207/ S1551 6709C OG0000_ 33
Yarkoni, T., Balota, D., & Yap, M. (2008). Moving beyond Coltheart’s
N: A new measure of orthographic similarity. Psychonomic Bul-
letin & Review 2008 15:5, 15(5), 971–979. https:// doi. org/ 10.
3758/ PBR. 15.5. 971
Yee, E., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2016). Putting concepts into con-
text. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2015 23:4, 23(4), 1015–
1027. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ S13423- 015- 0948-7
Zahn, R., Moll, J., Krueger, F., Huey, E. D., Garrido, G., & Grafman,
J. (2007). Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior
temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
of the United States of America, 104(15), 6430–6435. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1073/ pnas. 06070 61104
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
... In this method, participants are asked to rate how relevant/important a specific dimension is to the meaning of a target word. Available studies differ for the number of stimuli (e.g., from 400 in Troche et al., 2014, to 17940 in Wang et al., 2023 and the dimensions rated (e.g., from 1 in Diveica et al., 2023, to 65 in Binder et al., 2016. Overall, dimension rating studies have offered crucial support for the multidimensionality account of abstract concepts, stressing the importance of introspection, affective, social, and magnitude dimensions in their representation (Conca et al., 2021a). ...
... Overall, dimension rating studies have offered crucial support for the multidimensionality account of abstract concepts, stressing the importance of introspection, affective, social, and magnitude dimensions in their representation (Conca et al., 2021a). However, this type of study is only available in a small selection of languages, specifically English (Troche et al., 2014;Binder et al., 2016;Diveica et al., 2023), Italian (Villani et al., 2019;Borsa et al., submitted), and Chinese (Wang et al., 2023), and not in others, such as Turkish. ...
Article
Full-text available
The organization of abstract concepts reflects different dimensions, grounded in the brain regions coding for the corresponding experience. Normative measures of linguistic stimuli offer noteworthy insights into the organization of conceptual knowledge, but studies differ in the dimensions and classes of concepts considered. Additionally, most of the available information has been collected in English, without considering possible linguistic and cultural differences. Here, we aimed to create a comprehensive Turkish database for abstract concepts (TACO), including rarely investigated classes such as political concepts. We included 503 words-78 concrete (fruits, animals, tools) and 425 abstract (emotions, social, mental states, theoretical, quantity, space, political)-rated by 134 Turkish speakers for familiarity, imageability, age of acquisition, valence, arousal, quantity, space, theoretical, social, mental state, and political dimensions. We calculated dominance and exclusivity, indicating the dimension receiving the highest mean score for each word, and the position of the word along the unidimensional–multidimensional continuum, respectively. A principal component analysis (PCA) was conducted on the semantic dimensions. The results showed that mental state was the dominant dimension for most concepts. Moderate to low levels of exclusivity indicated that the concepts were multidimensional. PCA revealed three components: Component 1 captured the juxtaposition between social/mental state and magnitude polarities, Component 2 highlighted affective components, and Component 3 grouped together political and theoretical dimensions. The introduction of political concepts provided insights into the multidimensional nature of this unexplored class, closely intertwined with the theoretical dimension. TACO constitutes the first comprehensive Turkish database covering several abstract dimensions, paving the way for cross-linguistic and cross-cultural studies of semantic representations.
... 22,23 One explanation for a shared contribution to semantic and social processing is that the ATLs store social knowledge as part of semantic memory more generally. 10,13 A challenge of the current literature in providing clear answers to these questions is that there is no general consensus on what makes a concept 'social' 13,33,34 and thus it is unclear what types of concept are critical to social behaviours. Past investigations have each tended to focus on one type of "social" concept, which collectively span a very diverse range of concrete-to-abstract concepts, including people, 14 social behaviours 12,16,35 and emotions. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Degraded semantic memory is a prominent feature of frontotemporal dementia (FTD). It is classically associated with semantic dementia and anterior temporal lobe (ATL) atrophy, but semantic knowledge can also be compromised in behavioural-variant FTD (bvFTD). Motivated by understanding behavioural change in FTD, recent research has focused selectively on social-semantic knowledge, with proposals that the right ATL is specialised for social concepts. Previous studies have assessed very different types of social concepts and have not compared performance to that on matched non-social concepts. Consequently, it remains unclear to what extent various social concepts are (i) concurrently impaired in FTD, (ii) distinct from general semantic memory and (iii) differentially supported by the left and right ATL. This study assessed multiple aspects of social-semantic knowledge and general conceptual knowledge across cohorts with ATL-damage arising from either neurodegeneration or resection. We assembled a test battery measuring knowledge of multiple types of social concept. Performance was compared to non-social general conceptual knowledge, measured using the Cambridge Semantic Memory Test Battery and other matched non-social-semantic tests. Our transdiagnostic approach included bvFTD, semantic dementia and mixed intermediate cases to capture the FTD clinical spectrum, as well as age-matched healthy controls. People with unilateral left or right ATL resection for temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) were also recruited to assess how selective damage to the left or right ATL impacts social- and non-social-semantic knowledge. Social- and non-social-semantic deficits were severe and highly correlated in FTD. Much milder impairments were found after unilateral ATL resection, with no left vs. right differences in social-semantic knowledge or general semantic processing, and with only naming showing a greater deficit following left vs. right damage. A principal component analysis of all behavioural measures in the FTD cohort extracted three components, interpreted as capturing: (1) FTD severity, (2) semantic memory and (3) executive function. Social and non-social measures both loaded heavily on the same semantic memory component, and scores on this factor were uniquely associated with bilateral ATL grey matter volume but not with the degree of ATL asymmetry. Together, these findings demonstrate that both social- and non-social-semantic knowledge degrade in FTD (semantic dementia and bvFTD) following bilateral ATL atrophy. We propose that social-semantic knowledge is part of a broader conceptual system underpinned by a bilaterally-implemented, functionally-unitary semantic hub in the ATLs. Our results also highlight the value of a transdiagnostic approach for investigating the neuroanatomical underpinnings of cognitive deficits in FTD.
... The resulting ratings data have been used to examine how these various dimensions account for variability in speed and accuracy of lexical semantic processing (Kumar, 2021;Osgood, 1952; for an example see Balota et al., 2004) and to examine a multidimensional semantic space using dimension reduction strategies (Diveica et al., 2023;Muraki et al., 2020). Several semantic dimensions have been quantified for English words, including body-object interaction (Pexman et al., 2019), concreteness (Brysbaert et al., 2014), embodiment (Sidhu et al., 2014), imageability (Schock et al., 2012), effector-and modalityspecific sensory and motor experience (Lynott et al., 2020), socialness (Diveica et al., 2022), and valence and arousal (Warriner et al., 2013). ...
Preprint
Semantic ratings studies have resulted in significant methodological advances towards understanding the importance of experiential information to lexical-semantic processing. Yet, the existing norms are biased towards nouns, with fewer ratings available for verbs. In the present study we collected new semantic rating norms for 2,937 verbs on the dimensions of concreteness, embodiment, imageability, valence, and arousal. The resulting ratings provide the largest database of verb-specific rating norms across multiple semantic dimensions. They show good reliability and validity on four of five dimensions, with some evident challenges rating arousal for the verb stimuli. We demonstrate that the norms account for variance in response latencies and accuracy in a lexical decision task, word recognition task, and recognition memory, above and beyond dimensions such as length, frequency, orthographic Levenshtein distance, and age of acquisition, and suggests that semantic richness effects are observed in verb processing. The norms described here should be a useful resource for researchers interested in verb lexical-semantic processing.
... A growing number of studies are working to unravel other experiential dimensions that might significantly contribute to language processing. Among these dimensions are emotional valence and dominance (Kousta et al., 2011), modality of acquisition (Della Rosa et al., 2010), body-object interaction (Tillotson et al., 2008), interoception (Connell and Lynott, 2012;Villani et al., 2021a), socialness (Diveica et al., 2023; see also Fini and Borghi, 2019;Borghi, 2022), time to perceive a concept (Davis and Yee, 2023), and personal relevance (Westbury and Wurm, 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Abstraction processes involve two variables that are often confused with one another: concreteness ( banana versus belief ) and specificity ( chair versus furniture or Buddhism versus religion ). Researchers are investigating the relationship between them, but many questions remain open, such as: What type of semantics characterizes words with varying degrees of concreteness and specificity? We tackle this topic through an in-depth semantic analysis of 1049 Italian words for which human-generated concreteness and specificity ratings are available. Our findings show that (as expected) the semantics of concrete and abstract concepts differs, but most interestingly when specificity is considered, the variance in concreteness ratings explained by semantic types increases substantially, suggesting the need to carefully control word specificity in future research. For instance, mathematical concepts ( phase ) are on average abstract and generic, while behavioral qualities ( arrogant ) are on average abstract but specific. Moreover, through cluster analyses based on concreteness and specificity ratings, we observe the bottom-up emergence of four subgroups of semantically coherent words. Overall, this study provides empirical evidence and theoretical insight into the interplay of concreteness and specificity in shaping semantic categorization.
Article
Full-text available
Key unanswered questions for cognitive neuroscience include whether social cognition is underpinned by specialised brain regions and to what extent it simultaneously depends on more domain-general systems. Until we glean a better understanding of the full set of contributions made by various systems, theories of social cognition will remain fundamentally limited. In the present study, we evaluate a recent proposal that semantic cognition plays a crucial role in supporting social cognition. While previous brain-based investigations have focused on dissociating these two systems, our primary aim was to assess the degree to which the neural correlates are overlapping, particularly within two key regions, the anterior temporal lobe (ATL) and the temporoparietal junction (TPJ). We focus on activation associated with theory of mind (ToM) and adopt a meta-analytic activation likelihood approach to synthesise a large set of functional neuroimaging studies and compare their results with studies of semantic cognition. As a key consideration, we sought to account for methodological differences across the two sets of studies, including the fact that ToM studies tend to use nonverbal stimuli while the semantics literature is dominated by language-based tasks. Overall, we observed consistent overlap between the two sets of brain regions, especially in the ATL and TPJ. This supports the claim that tasks involving ToM draw upon more general semantic retrieval processes. We also identified activation specific to ToM in the right TPJ, bilateral anterior mPFC, and right precuneus. This is consistent with the view that, nested amongst more domain-general systems, there is specialised circuitry that is tuned to social processes.
Article
Working memory facilitates cognitive activities. Of the important cognitive activities within our daily lives, social activity has recently attracted attention in terms of the specific type of working memory; namely, social working memory. This paper reviews various findings from prior studies that emphasize both the neurological and behavioral differences between social and cognitive (canonical) working memory; findings that underscore the significance of the social working-memory construct. However, it is also evident that there has been a dearth of comprehensive perspectives to account for the findings from previous studies. More specifically, the review identifies four topics that have not been sufficiently addressed: (a) correlations among social working memory tasks; (b) theories concerning the relationship between social working memory and social cognition; (c) distinctions and/or similarities between affective versus social working memory; and (d) the nature of the representations within social working memory. Accordingly, this paper recommends tackling these issues as directions for future research. In focusing on these aspects, it is possible that future studies might elaborate the concept of social working memory and provide a perspective capable of integrating the diverse findings.
Article
Full-text available
Decoding social environments and engaging meaningfully with other people are critical aspects of human cognition. Multiple cognitive systems, including social and semantic cognition, work alongside each other to support these processes. This study investigated shared processing between social and semantic systems using neuroimaging data collected during movie-viewing, which captures the multimodal environment in which social knowledge is exchanged. Semantic and social content from movie events (event-level) and movie transcripts (word-level) were used in parametric modulation analyses to test (1) the degree to which semantic and social information is processed within each respective network and (2) engagement of the same cross-network regions or the same domain-general hub located within the semantic network during semantic and social processing. Semantic word and event-level content engaged the same fronto-temporo-parietal network and a portion of the semantic hub in the anterior temporal lobe (ATL). Social word and event-level content engaged the supplementary motor area and right angular gyrus within the social network, but only social words engaged the domain-general semantic hub in left ATL. There was evidence of shared processing between the social and semantic systems in the dorsolateral portion of right ATL which was engaged by word and event-level semantic and social content. Overlap between the semantic and social word and event results was highly variable within and across participants, with the most consistent loci of overlap occurring in left inferior frontal, bilateral precentral and supramarginal gyri for social and semantic words and in bilateral superior temporal gyrus extending from ATL posteriorly into supramarginal gyri for social and semantic events. These results indicate a complex pattern of shared and distinct regions for social and semantic cognition during naturalistic processing. Protocol registration The stage 1 protocol for this Registered Report was accepted in principle on October 11, 2022. The protocol, as accepted by the journal, can be found at: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/ACWQY.
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary theories of semantic representation posit that social experience is an important source of information for deriving meaning. However, there is a lack of behavioral evidence in support of this proposal. The aim of the present work was to test whether words’ degree of social relevance, or socialness, influences lexical–semantic processing. In Study 1, across a series of item-level regression analyses, we found that (a) socialness can facilitate responses in lexical, semantic, and memory tasks, and (b) limited evidence for an interaction of socialness with concreteness. In Studies 2–3, we tested the preregistered hypothesis that social words, compared to nonsocial words, will be associated with faster and more accurate responses during a syntactic classification task. We found that socialness has a facilitatory effect on noun decisions (Study 3), but not verb decisions (Study 2). Overall, our results suggest that the socialness of a word affects lexical–semantic processing but also that this is task-dependent. These findings constitute novel evidence in support of proposals that social information is an important dimension of semantic representation.
Article
Impaired social cognition is a core deficit in frontotemporal dementia (FTD). It is most commonly associated with the behavioural-variant of FTD, with atrophy of the orbitofrontal and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social cognitive changes are also common in semantic dementia, with atrophy centred on the anterior temporal lobes. The impairment of social behaviour in FTD has typically been attributed to damage to the orbitofrontal cortex and/or temporal poles and/or the uncinate fasciculus that connects them. However, the relative contributions of each region are unresolved. In this review, we present a unified neurocognitive model of controlled social behaviour that not only explains the observed impairment of social behaviours in FTD, but also assimilates both consistent and potentially contradictory findings from other patient groups, comparative neurology and normative cognitive neuroscience. We propose that impaired social behaviour results from damage to two cognitively- and anatomically-distinct components. The first component is social-semantic knowledge, a part of the general semantic-conceptual system supported by the anterior temporal lobes bilaterally. The second component is social control, supported by the orbitofrontal cortex, medial frontal cortex and ventrolateral frontal cortex, which interacts with social-semantic knowledge to guide and shape social behaviour.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract concepts, like justice and friendship, are central features of our daily lives. Traditionally, abstract concepts are distinguished from other concepts in that they cannot be directly experienced through the senses. As such, they pose a challenge for strongly embodied models of semantic representation that assume a central role for sensorimotor information. There is growing recognition, however, that it is possible for meaning to be ‘grounded’ via cognitive systems, including those involved in processing language and emotion. In this article, we focus on the specific proposal that social significance is a key feature in the representation of some concepts. We begin by reviewing recent evidence in favour of this proposal from the fields of psycholinguistics and neuroimaging. We then discuss the limited extent to which there is consensus about the definition of ‘socialness’ and propose essential next steps for research in this domain. Taking one such step, we describe preliminary data from an unprecedented large-scale rating study that can help determine how socialness is distinct from other facets of word meaning. We provide a backdrop of contemporary theories regarding semantic representation and social cognition and highlight important predictions for both brain and behaviour. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Concepts in interaction: social engagement and inner experiences’.
Article
Full-text available
The contribution and neural basis of cognitive control is under-specified in many prominent models of socio-cognitive processing. Important outstanding questions include whether there are multiple, distinguishable systems underpinning control and whether control is ubiquitously or selectively engaged across different social behaviours and task demands. Recently, it has been proposed that the regulation of social behaviours could rely on brain regions specialised in the controlled retrieval of semantic information, namely the anterior inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and posterior middle temporal gyrus. Accordingly, we investigated for the first time whether the neural activation commonly found in social functional neuroimaging studies extends to these ‘semantic control’ regions. We conducted five coordinate-based meta-analyses to combine results of 499 fMRI/PET experiments and identified the brain regions consistently involved in semantic control, as well as four social abilities: theory of mind, trait inference, empathy and moral reasoning. This allowed an unprecedented parallel review of the neural networks associated with each of these cognitive domains. The results confirmed that the anterior left IFG region involved in semantic control is reliably engaged in all four social domains. This supports the hypothesis that social cognition is partly regulated by the neurocognitive system underpinning semantic control.
Article
Full-text available
The distinction between abstract and concrete concepts is fundamental to cognitive linguistics and cognitive science. This distinction is commonly operationalized through concreteness ratings based on the aggregated judgments of many people. What is often overlooked in experimental studies using this operationalization is that ratings are attributed to words , not to concepts directly. In this paper we explore the relationship between the linguistic properties of English words and conceptual abstractness/concreteness. Based on hypotheses stated in the existing linguistic literature we select a set of variables (part of speech, morphological structure, countability, etymology) and verify whether they are statistically associated with concreteness ratings. We show that English nouns are rated as more concrete compared to other parts of speech, but mass nouns are rated as less concrete than count nouns. Furthermore, a more complex morphological structure is associated with abstractness, and as for etymology, French- and Latin-derived words are more abstract than words of other origin. This shows that linguistic properties of words are indeed associated with the degree of concreteness that we attribute to the underlying concepts, and we discuss the implications that these findings have for linguistic theory and for empirical investigations in the cognitive sciences.
Article
Full-text available
Previous inconsistencies on the effects of implicitly processing positively - vs. negatively - connotated emotional words might reflect the influence of uncontrolled psycholinguistic dimensions, and/or social facets inherent in putative “emotional” stimuli. Based on the relevance of social features in semantic cognition, we developed a socio-emotional Stroop task to assess the influence of social vs. individual (non-social) emotional content, besides negative vs. positive valence, on implicit word processing. The effect of these variables was evaluated in terms of performance and RTs, alongside associated brain activity/connectivity. We matched conditions for several psycholinguistic variables, and assessed a modulation of brain activity/connectivity by trial-wise RT, to characterize the maximum of condition- and subject-specific variability. RTs were tracked by insular and anterior cingulate activations likely reflecting implicit attention to stimuli, interfering with task-performance based on condition-specific processing of their subjective salience. Slower performance for negative than neutral/positive words was tracked by left-hemispheric structures processing negative stimuli and emotions, such as fronto-insular cortex, while the lack of specific activations for positively-connotated words supported their marginal facilitatory effect. The speeding/slowing effects of processing positive/negative individual emotional stimuli were enhanced by social words, reflecting in specific activations of the right anterior temporal and orbitofrontal cortex, respectively. RTs to social positive and negative words modulated connectivity from these regions to fronto-striatal and sensorimotor structures, respectively, likely promoting approach vs. avoidance dispositions shaping their facilitatory vs. inhibitory effect. These results might help assessing the neural correlates of impaired social cognition and emotional regulation, and the effects of rehabilitative interventions.
Article
Full-text available
According to the grounded perspective, cognition emerges from the interaction of classic cognitive processes with the modalities, the body, and the environment. Rather than being an autonomous impenetrable module, cognition incorporates these other domains intrinsically into its operation. The Situated Action Cycle offers one way of understanding how the modalities, the body, and the environment become integrated to ground cognition. Seven challenges and opportunities are raised for this perspective: (1) How does cognition emerge from the Situated Action Cycle and in turn support it? (2) How can we move beyond simply equating embodiment with action, additionally establishing how embodiment arises in the autonomic, neuroendocrine, immune, cardiovascular, respiratory, digestive, and integumentary systems? (3) How can we better understand the mechanisms underlying multimodal simulation, its functions across the Situated Action Cycle, and its integration with other representational systems? (4) How can we develop and assess theoretical accounts of symbolic processing from the grounded perspective (perhaps using the construct of simulators)? (5) How can we move beyond the simplistic distinction between concrete and abstract concepts, instead addressing how concepts about the external and internal worlds pattern to support the Situated Action Cycle? (6) How do individual differences emerge from different populations of situational memories as the Situated Action Cycle manifests itself differently across individuals? (7) How can constructs from grounded cognition provide insight into the replication and generalization crises, perhaps from a quantum perspective on mechanisms (as exemplified by simulators).
Article
Full-text available
The possible uniqueness of social stimuli constitutes a key topic for cognitive neuroscience. Growing evidence highlights graded contributions to their semantic processing by the anterior temporal lobe (ATL), where the omni-category response displayed by its ventrolateral sector might reflect the integration of information relayed from other regions. Among these, the superior polar ATL was specifically associated with representing social concepts. However, most previous studies neglected the close relationship between social and emotional semantic features, which might confound interpreting the degree of overlap vs. specificity of social and emotional conceptual processing. We addressed this issue via two activation-likelihood-estimation meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies reporting brain structures associated with processing social or emotional concepts. Alongside a common involvement of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, we found social and emotional concepts to be specifically associated with lateral temporal areas (including the superior polar ATL) and the amygdala, respectively. These results support the specialization of distinct sectors of the fronto-temporo-limbic circuitry for processing social vs. emotional concepts, and the integration of their output in medial prefrontal regions underlying the regulation of social behavior. These results pave the way for further studies addressing the neural bases of conceptual knowledge, its impairment after fronto-temporal brain damage, and the effect of rehabilitative interventions targeting its main functional modules.
Preprint
Full-text available
When we see or hear a word, we can rapidly bring its meaning to mind. The process that underlies this ability is quite complex. Over the past two decades, considerable progress has been made towards understanding this process. In this paper, I offer four broad principles of semantic processing derived from lexical-semantic research. The first principle is that the relationship between form and meaning is not so arbitrary and I explore that by describing efforts to understand the relationship between form and meaning, highlighting advances from my own lab on the topics of sound symbolism and iconicity. The second principle is that more is better and I summarize previous research on semantic richness effects, and how those effects reveal the nature of semantic representation. The third principle is the many and various properties of abstract concepts. I point to abstract meaning as a challenge for some theories of semantic representation. In response to that challenge, I outline what has been learned about how those meanings are acquired and represented. The fourth principle is that experience matters, and I summarize research on the dynamic and experience-driven nature of semantic processing, detailing ways in which processing is modified by both immediate and long-term context. Finally, I describe some next steps for lexical-semantic research.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract words have usually been treated as a homogenous group, with limited investigation of the influence of different underlying representational systems for these words. In the present study we examined lexical–semantic processing of abstract verbs, separating them into mental state, emotional state and nonembodied state types. We used a syntactic classification task and a memory task to investigate behavioural differences amongst the abstract verb types. Semantic richness effects of each of the verbs’ associates were then investigated to determine the relationship of linguistic associations to semantic processing response times for abstract verbs. We found a modest effect of abstract verb type, with mental state abstract verbs processed more quickly than nonembodied abstract verbs in the syntactic classification task; however, this effect was task dependent. We also found that memory was less accurate for the mental state abstract verbs. The semantic richness analysis of abstract verb associates revealed (1) that the concreteness of an abstract verb’s associates has a positive relationship to the verb’s response time and (2) a negative relationship between response time and age of acquisition for associates of nonembodied verbs. The results provide support for the proposal that abstract concepts engage complex representations in modal and linguistic systems.
Article
Full-text available
For a very long time, theorizing in the cognitive sciences was dominated by the assumption that abstract concepts, which lack a perceivable referent, can only be handled by amodal or verbal linguistic representations. In the last years, however, refined grounded cognition theories emphasizing the importance of emotional and introspective information for abstract concepts, in addition to verbal associations and sensorimotor information, have received increasing support. Here, we review theoretical accounts of the structure and neural basis of conceptual memory and evaluate them in light of recent empirical evidence with regard to the processing of concrete and abstract concepts. Based on this literature review, we argue that abstract concepts should not be treated as a homogenous conceptual category, whose meaning is established by one single specific type of representation. Instead, depending on the feature composition, there are different subgroups of abstract concepts, including those with strong relations to vision or action, which are represented in the visual and motor brain systems similar to concrete concepts. The reviewed findings with regard to concrete and abstract concepts can be accommodated best by hybrid theories of conceptual representation assuming an interaction between modality-specific, multimodal and amodal hub areas.
Article
The neuroscientific study of conceptual representation has largely focused on categories of concrete entities (biological entities, tools…), while abstract knowledge has been less extensively investigated. The possible presence of a categorical organization of abstract knowledge is a debated issue. An embodied cognition framework predicts an organization of the abstract domain into different dimensions, grounded in the brain regions engaged by the corresponding experience. Here we review the types of experience that have been proposed to characterize different categories of abstract concepts, and the evidence supporting a corresponding organization derived from behavioural, neuroimaging (i.e., fMRI, MRI, PET, SPECT), EEG, and neurostimulation (i.e., TMS) studies in healthy and clinical populations. The available data provide substantial converging evidence in favour of the presence of distinct neural representations of social and emotional knowledge, mental states and magnitude concepts, engaging brain systems involved in the corresponding experiences. This evidence is supporting an extension of embodied models of semantic memory organization to several types of abstract knowledge.