ArticlePDF Available

EEG microstates as novel functional biomarkers for adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background Research on the electroencephalographic (EEG) signatures of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has historically concentrated on its frequency spectrum or event-related evoked potentials. In this work, we investigate EEG microstates, an alternative framework defined by the clustering of recurring topographical patterns, as a novel approach for examining large-scale cortical dynamics in ADHD. Methods Using kmeans clustering, we studied the spatio-temporal dynamics of ADHD during rest condition by comparing the microstate (MS) segmentations between adult ADHD patients and neurotypical controls, across 2 independent datasets: the first dataset consisted of 66 ADHD patients and 66 controls, while the second dataset comprised of 22 ADHD patients and 22 controls and was used for out-of-sample validation. Results Spatially, ADHD and control subjects displayed equivalent MS topographies (canonical maps), indicating preservation of prototypical EEG generators in ADHD. However, this concordance was accompanied by significant differences in temporal dynamics. At the group level, and across both datasets, ADHD diagnosis was associated with longer mean durations of a fronto-central topography (D), indicating its electrocortical generator(s) could be acting as pronounced “attractors” of global cortical dynamics. Lastly, in the first (larger) dataset, we also found evidence for decreased time coverage and mean duration of microstate A, which inversely correlated with ADHD scores, while microstate D metrics were correlated with sleep disturbance, the latter being known to have strong relation with ADHD. Conclusions Overall, our study underlines the value of EEG microstates as promising functional biomarkers for ADHD, offering an additional lens through which to examine its neurophysiological mechanisms.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal Pre-proof
EEG microstates as novel functional biomarkers for adult attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder
Victor Férat, Martijn Arns, Marie-Pierre Deiber, Roland Hasler, Nader Perroud,
Christoph M. Michel, Tomas Ros
PII: S2451-9022(21)00319-0
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.11.006
Reference: BPSC 875
To appear in: Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and
Neuroimaging
Received Date: 25 June 2021
Revised Date: 5 November 2021
Accepted Date: 6 November 2021
Please cite this article as: Férat V., Arns M., Deiber M.-P., Hasler R., Perroud N., Michel C.M. & Ros
T., EEG microstates as novel functional biomarkers for adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder,
Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging (2021), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.bpsc.2021.11.006.
This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition
of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of
record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published
in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that,
during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal
disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
© 2021 Published by Elsevier Inc on behalf of Society of Biological Psychiatry.
1
EEG microstates as novel functional biomarkers for adult
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
Victor Férat1, Martijn Arns2,3,4, Marie-Pierre Deiber5,6, Roland Hasler5,7, Nader Perroud5,6,
Christoph M. Michel1,8, Tomas Ros1,5,8
[1] Functional Brain Mapping Laboratory, Department of Basic Neurosciences, Campus
Biotech, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
[2] Research Institute Brainclinics, Brainclinics Foundation, Nijmegen, The Netherlands,
[3] Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Location AMC,
Amsterdam Neuroscience, The Netherlands,
[4] Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience,
Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
[5] Division of Psychiatric Specialties, Department of Psychiatry, University Hospitals of
Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
[6] Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva,
Switzerland,
[7] Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada
[8] Centre for Biomedical Imaging (CIBM) Lausanne-Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Journal Pre-proof
2
Corresponding author:
Victor Férat
Victor.ferat@unige.ch
+41 22 379 54 57
Functional Brain Mapping Lab, Campus Biotech
Chemin des mines, 9 1202 Genève
Short/running title:
EEG microstates as biomarkers for adult ADHD
Keywords:
EEG
Microstates
Resting state
ADHD
Attention
Sleep disorders
Journal Pre-proof
3
Abstract
Background
Research on the electroencephalographic (EEG) signatures of attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) has historically concentrated on its frequency spectrum or event-related
evoked potentials. In this work, we investigate EEG microstates, an alternative framework
defined by the clustering of recurring topographical patterns, as a novel approach for
examining large-scale cortical dynamics in ADHD.
Methods
Using kmeans clustering, we studied the spatio-temporal dynamics of ADHD during rest
condition by comparing the microstate (MS) segmentations between adult ADHD patients
and neurotypical controls, across 2 independent datasets: the first dataset consisted of 66
ADHD patients and 66 controls, while the second dataset comprised of 22 ADHD patients
and 22 controls and was used for out-of-sample validation.
Results
Spatially, ADHD and control subjects displayed equivalent MS topographies (canonical
maps), indicating preservation of prototypical EEG generators in ADHD. However, this
concordance was accompanied by significant differences in temporal dynamics. At the group
level, and across both datasets, ADHD diagnosis was associated with longer mean durations
of a fronto-central topography (D), indicating its electrocortical generator(s) could be acting
as pronounced “attractors” of global cortical dynamics. Lastly, in the first (larger) dataset,
we also found evidence for decreased time coverage and mean duration of microstate A,
which inversely correlated with ADHD scores, while microstate D metrics were correlated
with sleep disturbance, the latter being known to have strong relation with ADHD.
Conclusions
Overall, our study underlines the value of EEG microstates as promising functional
biomarkers for ADHD, offering an additional lens through which to examine its
neurophysiological mechanisms.
Journal Pre-proof
4
Main Text
Introduction
Attention-deficit / hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterized by developmentally
inappropriate levels of inattention, hyperactivity, or impulsivity, and is one of the most
common psychiatric disorders, with a prevalence of 1 out of every 20 adults (1,2). As a result,
there is a pressing need to understand its neural underpinnings in the hope of devising better
treatments.
Recent literature reviews point to abnormal resting (EEG) electroencephalogram
activities in ADHD patients (36). This is exemplified by a significant cluster of ADHD
patients with a high theta to beta power ratio (TBR) (5,7), a signature supportive of theories
that ADHD may be caused by a delay of brain maturation (8), seeing that the theta/beta ratio
is known to progressively attenuate during normal cortical development (9,10).
However more recent studies (11,12) have failed to replicate this finding of elevated TBR as
a diagnostic feature in ADHD, which was also confirmed in a meta-analysis (13).
These divergent results suggest that the high TBR group, which is strongly associated with
treatment response to methylphenidate (14) and neurofeedback (15,16), is only a subgroup
within a wider spectrum of abnormal electrocortical activities. These different subtypes can
also be found with the EEG signatures derived from adults with ADHD: which besides excess
power of lower-frequency rhythms (1719), also display opposing pattern(s) comprising of
reduced alpha power (20,21) and/or excess higher-frequency beta power (22,23).Based on
Journal Pre-proof
5
these findings, the emerging consensus is that ADHD is not only highly heterogeneous in
terms of behavior (24), but also electrophysiologically (25).
Although previous research on ADHD has concentrated on examining its EEG frequency
spectrum (25), and/or event-related potentials (ERPs) (26) in this work we propose resting
state EEG microstates (27) as an alternative analytical framework. Microstate analyses in
ADHD have so far been limited to ERP microstates (28) (29), hence the spontaneous resting-
state EEG still needs to be explored. By modelling the spontaneous EEG as a sequence of
recurring topographical patterns, microstate (MS) analysis considers both spatial and
temporal dynamics simultaneously. This could facilitate clearer spatio-temporal
dissociations to be made in ADHD, as any uncovered deviations in MS dynamics would imply
abnormal temporal activations of spatially distinct cortical generators. Although it is difficult
to the identify microstates’ precise anatomical generators through mere clustering of scalp
EEG data, their abnormal temporal signatures nevertheless point to significant departures
from typical cortical dynamics. This may be a valuable framework when considering the
brain as a large-scale dynamical system (27) . Previous work has identified significant links
between microstate map dynamics and behavioral dimensions in clinical populations. For
instance, the duration of microstate class D has been found to correlate negatively with
hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia (30). Interestingly, as MS topographies are
estimated on a timepoint-by-timepoint basis (i.e. instantaneously) using a broadband (e.g.
1-30 Hz) signal, MS measures may be able to capture cortical dynamics that are either
independent or common across EEG frequencies.
Journal Pre-proof
6
To validate these hypotheses, we apply below MS analysis to resting-state EEG recordings
of 88 adults with ADHD, divided across two independent datasets. The first dataset,
designated as the “test” sample, comprised of 66 ADHD patients and 66 neurotypical controls
from the Netherlands. The second dataset, designated as the “retest” sample, comprised of
22 ADHD patients and 22 neurotypical controls from Switzerland.
Methods
I. Datasets
i. Dataset 1
Participants
EEG recording of 66 ADHD Patients (31 female, mean age: 34.1, SD: 11.4) and 66 controls
(41 female, mean age: 36.5, SD: 12.4) were obtained from participants enrolled by Research
Institute Brainclinics and the neuroCare Group Nijmegen in the Netherlands between 2001
and May 2018. (31). Briefly patients were screened for inclusion and included in case of an
ADHD or ADD diagnosis (as confirmed by the MINI Diagnostic Interview or by a qualified
clinician), or when ADHD-RS scores on either scale (ATT or HI) (32) was equal to or higher
than 5, for this study only adults were included. Patients were also screened for sleep
disorders trough the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (33). Sample was composed of 3
ADHD subtypes including 40 patients of mixed subtype (inattentive and hyperactive), the
“inattentive” subtype composed of 23 patients, and the “hyperactive” subtype composed of
3 patients. All subjects signed an informed consent before treatment was initiated.
Journal Pre-proof
7
Recordings
2-minute Eyes Open (EO) EEG recordings were performed thanks to a standardized reliable
and consistent (34,35) developed by Brain Resource Ltd (36,37). Signals were recorded
continuously using “Quickcap” a 26-electrode cap placed according to the 1020
international system, with a sampling rate of 500 Hz. The ground electrode was placed on
the scalp at AFz, and data was referenced to averaged mastoids. All electrode impedances
were kept below 5 kΩ. In addition to that, a low pass filter above 100 Hz was applied prior
to digitization and Horizontal and vertical eye movements were controlled for. EOG-
correction based on Gratton et al. (38) was applied to the data.
ii. Dataset 2
Participants
Resting state EEG recordings of 22 ADHD (12 female, mean age: 32.3, SD: 9.2) adult patients
and 22 healthy controls (14 female, mean age: 31.1, SD: 7.3) were obtained from (20). ADHD
Patients were recruited through the Adult ADHD Unit at Geneva University Hospitals. After
giving the written informed consent patient and controls underwent four clinical
questionnaires including the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS v1.1) evaluates in 18
questions current ADHD symptoms in adolescents and adults (39).
Clinician’s diagnostic was based on three structured questionnaires: the ADHD Child
Evaluation for Adults (ACE+), https://www.psychology-services.uk.com/adhd.htm), the
Journal Pre-proof
8
French version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders
(SCID-II,(40)) and the French version of the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (DIGS,
mood disorder parts only, (41)(see (20) for extend description). Sample was composed of 3
ADHD subtypes: the “mixed” one composed of 16 patients of mixed subtype the “inattentive”
subtype composed of 5 patients, and the “hyperactive” subtype composed of the last patient.
This study was approved by the Research Ethic Committee of the Republic and Canton of
Geneva [project number 2017-01029].
Recordings
Here, 3 min of EO rest was recorded continuously using a 64 Ag/AgCl electrode cap (ANT
Waveguard, Netherlands) placed according to the 1020 international system, with a
sampling rate of 500 Hz. The ground electrode was placed on the scalp at a site equidistant
between Fpz and Fz, and the reference electrode at CPz. Electrical signals were amplified
using the eego mylab system (ANT Neuro, Netherlands), and all electrode impedances were
kept below 5 kΩ.
II. Preprocessing
Both datasets underwent the same preprocessing pipeline: data was processed in Matlab
with EEGLAB (42), using the default settings of the Harvard Automated Processing Pipeline
for Electroencephalography (HAPPE) (43). Concisely, this involved first filtering between 1-
100 Hz, removing line noise with a notch filter (between 48-52 Hz), rejection of bad channels
(standard deviation cutoff of z=3), removal of non-cerebral artifacts such as eye-blinks and
Journal Pre-proof
9
muscle activity using independent component analysis (via the MARA plug-in (44)). Lastly,
rejection of “bad” 1-second EEG segments was carried out using amplitude-based and joint
probability artifact detection (standard deviation cutoff of z=3).
III. Fitting
The de-artifacted data (from Datasets 1 and 2) was band-passed filtered between 1-30 Hz
and re-referenced to common average reference. Microstate maps were estimated
separately for each dataset (Dataset 1 and 2) and group (ADHD and CTRL). Here, we used
Koenig’s Microstate toolbox for EEGLAB (available
at https://www.thomaskoenig.ch/index.php/software/microstates-in-eeglab). For each
subject’s resting-state recording, 2000 GFP (Global Field Power) peaks were selected
randomly and submitted to modified (i.e. polarity-independent) kmeans clustering with 100
repetitions. For each cluster number k=4 to k=7, microstate (MS) maps (i.e. cluster
centroids) were estimated firstly at the subject level, and then optimally re-ordered between
subjects by minimizing the average spatial correlation across maps. Finally, respective MS
maps were averaged across all subjects (within each dataset/group) to give the aggregate
map for each cluster. We found that k=5 provided the highest map reliability across subjects
and datasets, which was estimated as the mean spatial correlation of each subject’s map with
the group’s aggregate.
Journal Pre-proof
10
IV. Backfitting
The k=5 global dominant maps of both datasets were then fitted back to the original EEGs
using Cartool (45). During this procedure, each time point was assigned to a cluster label (i.e.
microstate map) by spatial correlation analysis: each time point was assigned to the map
with which it shared the highest absolute spatial correlation. If the spatial correlation was
below the r=0.5 correlation threshold, the time point was labelled as “non-assigned”. A
smoothing window of 7 samples (56.0 ms) was used to ensure temporal continuity of the
signal by adjusting correlation of the central time point with a smoothing factor of 10.
Identical label sequences which did not reach a duration of 3 samples (24.0 ms) were split
into two parts, each sharing the highest spatial correlation with its neighboring segment and
relabeled accordingly to the latest. At the end of this procedure, non-assigned timepoints
were removed and participants with z >= 3 of unlabeled timepoints were excluded of further
analysis. A label sequence was derived for each individual recording, which was used to
compute 3 metrics:
Global explain variance (Gev): the sum of variances weighted by the global field
power of all time points assigned to a label. This metric is expressed in percentage
(%).
Time coverage (TimeCov): the proportion of time during which a label is present in
the recording. This metric is expressed in percentage (%).
Mean duration (MeanDur): mean temporal duration during which a label is present
without interruption. This metric is expressed in milliseconds (ms).
Journal Pre-proof
11
After backfitting, outlier detection based on a high number of unlabeled timepoints (z score
>3, dataset 1 = 13% | dataset 2 = 18%) identified two control subjects from dataset 1 and
one control subject from dataset 2. These subjects were excluded from further analysis.
V. Power Spectrum Analysis
Absolute power spectral density (PSD) was computed using Welch’s method for frequencies
ranging from 2 to 30Hz. The window had an effective size of 2.048 seconds and no overlap.
To obtain a relative metric that could be used for between subject comparisons, all values
were divided by the sum of the full spectrum (2 - 30Hz). Obtained values were then added
up within each studied frequency band: delta (2 - 4Hz), theta (4 - 8 Hz), alpha (8 - 12 Hz),
low-beta (12 - 20 Hz) and high-beta (20 - 30 Hz) for further analysis.
VI. Clinical measures of inattention and hyperactivity
For each dataset, we selected the standardized clinical questionnaires that best reflected
current (i.e. adult) symptoms of ADHD.
For dataset 1, this was the ADHD Rating Scale (ADHD-RS, (37), which contained 23 questions
regarding the presence of symptoms on a 4-point scale (0 =rarely or never, 1 =sometimes, 2
=often, 3 =very often). The ADHD-RS contains two subscales for symptoms of inattention and
hyperactivity.
For dataset 2, this was the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS v1.1) which uses 18
questions on a 5-point scale (0 =never, 1=rarely, 2=sometimes, 3=often, 4=very often) to
Journal Pre-proof
12
evaluate current ADHD symptoms in adolescents and adults (44). The ASRS contains two
subscales that assess the dimensions of hyperactivity and inattention.
VI. Statistics
Group comparisons were conducted on the 3 spatiotemporal parameters thanks to unpaired
permutation test for equality of means. Due to the absence of pre-established hypothesis,
two-sided test was used for the first dataset. Results derived from this first analysis were
used to establish working hypotheses for the second dataset leading to the use of one-sided
tests. P-values were estimated by simulated random sampling with 10000 replications.
Cohen’s d (d) was used to report effect sizes as standardized difference of means. When
applicable, statistical results were corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni
method.
Correlations between microstates parameters and Clinical scores were computed using two-
sided permutation test (10000 permutations) on Pearson correlation coefficient.
Results
I. Dataset 1
i. Microstate topographies
In the first dataset, we examined two minutes resting-state EEG data of 66 patients with
ADHD and 66 controls. Neither mean age (p = 0.25) nor gender (fisher exact test, p=0.08)
between groups differed significantly.
Journal Pre-proof
13
We applied microstate (MS) segmentation to both groups independently to identify potential
topographies that might be specific to one population. We identified 5 equivalent maps
across both ADHD and CTRL groups (Figure 1), corresponding to traditional MS
topographies previously reported in the literature: a left-right diagonal orientation (A), a
right-left diagonal orientation (B), a fronto-posterior orientation (C), fronto-central
maximum (D) and a parieto-central maximum (F). Spatial correlation analysis revealed
negligible differences between group MS maps, with a minimum absolute correlation of 87%
for matched topographies.
Consequently, we concatenated the EEGs of both ADHD and CTRL groups into a single
‘pooled’ kmeans analysis, to obtain a set of common maps for both groups. These latter maps
were used in the backfitting of all individual participant data.
ii. Microstate Segmentation
As seen in Figure 2, we firstly observed a reduced temporal prevalence of map A in the ADHD
group compared to CTRL: in other words, the relative amount of time subjects spend in this
configuration was significantly reduced (p ≤ 0.05, d= -0.43) in the ADHD group compared to
CTRL. Additionally, although non-significant, state durations of map A were on average
lower for the ADHD group (n.s., d= -0.-59) and the amount of global variance explained by
map A was also reduced on average (n.s., d= -0.32).
Interestingly, opposite effects were found for map D, which exhibited a relative increase in
prevalence in the ADHD group: the fronto central topography of map D explained on average
Journal Pre-proof
14
more global variance (GEV, p ≤ 0.01, d = 0.71), dominated an increased temporal proportion
(Time Coverage, p ≤ 0.05, d = 0.59) and had longer state durations (Mean Duration, p ≤ 0.05,
d = 0.53) in the ADHD population.
No significant results were found for other topographies
iii. Regression analysis between microstates parameters and clinical
measures
By focusing on the significant results of the group-wise analysis, we hypothesized that
microstate A and D dynamics might be related to differences in ADHD severity. We evaluated
the relationship between the parameters of these two microstates and individual scores on
the ADHD Rating Scale in ADHD patients. As show in in Figure 3, correlation analyses
revealed a negative correlation between the microstate A parameters and clinical ADHD
scores: significant negative correlations were found between map A Time coverage
and ADHD_total score (p 0.05 (F(x) = -0.2x + 15, R² = 7.7%), as well as ADHD_Hyperactivity
(p 0.05 F(x) = -0.1x + 7, R² = 7.4%). Similar results were found between map A global
explained variance (Gev) and ADHD_total score (p ≤ 0.05 F(x) = -0.3x + 14,= 7.7%) and
ADHD_Hyperactivity (p ≤ 0.05 F(x) = -0.2x + 7,= 7.1%). Mean duration of map A was also
correlated to ADHD_total score (p 0.05 F(x) = -0.1x + 22, = 9.3%) and ADHD_Inattention
(p ≤ 0.05 F(x) = -0.06x + 11, = 5.8%). In this dataset, no significant correlations were
found between clinical measures and map D parameters.
Journal Pre-proof
15
Microstate D dynamics were also associated with Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)
(Figure 4) in the ADHD group, where higher PSQI scores indicate greater sleep disturbance.
Here, positive correlations were found between PSQI total score and microstate D global
explained variance (p ≤ 0.05 F(x) = 0.3x + 5.8, R² = 7.8%) and time coverage (p 0.05 F(x)
= 0.2x + 4.8, R² = 8.4%).
II. Dataset 2
i. Microstate topographies
In this second ‘replication’ dataset, we applied the same MS analysis pipeline to 3 min
resting-state EEG data of 22 adult ADHD patients and 22 adult controls. Neither mean age
(p = 0.66) nor gender (fisher exact test, p=0.8) between groups differed significantly.
. We observed remarkably similar MS topographies to dataset 1 (Figure 5), with a minimal
inter-dataset spatial correlation of 0.89 (Figure S1). Both ADHD and CTRL groups exhibited
the 5 classical microstate topographies ABCDF. Spatial correlation analysis revealed minor
difference between ADHD and CTRL group topographies (Figure 4a), with a minimum
absolute correlation of 91% on the diagonal. Topographies were unchanged after
concatenation of the ADHD and CTRL data. Similarly, to dataset 1, we used the group
concatenated MS maps for backfitting and estimation of MS dynamics at the level of
individual subjects.
Journal Pre-proof
16
ii. Microstate Segmentation
Based on the independent, group-wise differences found in the first dataset, we
hypothesized that microstate D parameters would be elevated in the ADHD population while
those of map A would be reduced. To test this, we performed directional (i.e. one-sided)
permutation tests for equality of means on microstate A and D parameters only (Figure
6). Hence, in this section, statistical results were corrected for 6 comparisons.
We replicated the deviations for map D both in terms of effect size and statistical significance:
timepoints assigned to map D were significantly longer (p = 0.05, d = 0.77) in the ADHD
population, while noticeable (but non-significant) increases of global explained variance
(n.s., d = 0.49) and time coverage (n.s., d = 0.57) were also present. No significant differences
were found for map A, hence ADHD deviations in this microstate were not replicated (n.s.,
GEV: d = -0.14 | time coverage: d = -0.07 | mean duration: d = 0.42) in terms of statistical
significance.
iii. Clinical correlations
Based on group analyses led on both datasets, we tested the assumption that only microstate
A and D would have a significant relationship with clinical scores.
Analysis of ADHD patients alone did not reveal any significant correlations between ADHD
clinical scores and those MS parameters.
Journal Pre-proof
17
Spectral power analysis
None of the EEG bands demonstrated significant differences between ADHD and CTRL
groups after Bonferroni correction, either for the first or second dataset (Fig 7).
Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate EEG microstates (MS) as potentially novel functional
biomarkers for attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). By applying this
method to adult ADHD patients, we uncovered new electrophysiological characteristics of
this disorder. To this end, we applied spatial kmeans-clustering to two independent
datasets, each composed of adults with ADHD and a neurotypical control group. We firstly
observed a close correspondence between ADHD topographies (i.e. map clusters) and
classical MS maps (A, B, C, D, F) typical of the normal population, suggesting no major
deviations in the spatial organization of electrocortical generators. This equivalence enabled
us to estimate each MS map underlying temporal dynamics, while testing for any statistical
differences between ADHD and control samples. Here, we identified a longer mean temporal
duration of a fronto-central topography (microstate D), which was statistically significant
and had a medium-to-large effect size in both the first and second datasets (d=0.59 and
d=0.77, respectively). Secondly, in the first (larger) dataset, we found additional evidence
for decreased time coverage (d = -0.59) and mean duration (d = -0.43) of microstate A, which
inversely correlated with ADHD inattention scores.
Journal Pre-proof
18
Microstate D
Interestingly, microstate D has been reported to be more expressed during attentional tasks,
such mental arithmetic (46,47), so it is intriguing (and perhaps counterintuitive) that it is
also observed to be more prevalent in ADHD. However, a stronger temporal prevalence of
specifically microstate D has also been found to accompany periods of unresponsiveness to
stimuli during transitions to drowsiness (48). In contradistinction, a recent study reported
that microstate D duration was positively correlated with vigilance level (49). Microstate D
prevalence has also been observed to be altered during hypnosis (50), hallucinations (30),
sleep (46,51) and in patients with schizophrenia (52)In view of the larger prevalence and
duration of microstate D in both our datasets, this balance seems to be tipped towards the
upper end of the distribution in adult ADHD. As a result, we hypothesize that the
electrocortical generator(s) of map D may be acting as persistent “attractors” of cortical
dynamics, thereby reducing their global variability and/or complexity. This interpretation
would also be compatible with a recent review suggesting that microstate D may be
responsible for aspects of reflexive attention such as reorientation and switching of
attentional focus (27,53,54).
Anatomically, the fronto-central topography of map D has previously been associated with
activation of the right inferior parietal lobe, the right middle and superior frontal gyri, and
the right insula (46,55,56). These brain regions are known to be part of the Dorsal Attention
Network (57,58). Hence, our findings tentatively point to abnormal dynamics within this
network and are supported by functional MRI studies (59).
Journal Pre-proof
19
Relationship with sleep disturbance
Interestingly, we observed a significant correlation between microstate D prevalence and
poorer sleep quality in ADHD patients. Several relationships have previously been
established between sleep disorders and attentional deficits (see (60) for a review). This
result is even more intriguing considering a recent study by Ke and colleagues (61), who
reported increases in microstate D coverage (and a reduction in microstate A) in sleep
deprived individuals. These results, which overlap with those observed in the present study,
support pre-existing hypotheses of a trinity between sleep, hyperactivity disorder and
abnormal EEG signatures (62,63).
Microstate A
In the larger dataset,, we additionally observed significantly decreased time coverage of
microstate A, which was inversely correlated with clinical inattention scores in the ADHD
sample. A recent study has shown that states of increased vigilance/alertness were
associated with relatively less prevalence of microstate A (and longer durations of
microstate D) (49). Thus, the combined signature of lower microstate A coverage and
increased microstate D duration in our study would imply that ADHD could be characterized
as a condition of “hyper-vigilance”, consistent with its behavioral symptoms of physical and
emotional hyperactivity (65,66).
Journal Pre-proof
20
Spectral Power differences
Classical EEG spectral power analyses have frequently revealed slow-wave (e.g. theta)
abnormalities with a fronto-central topography in clinical cohorts with ADHD (e.g. (67,68)).
A plethora of studies have investigated spectral power differences in childhood and adult
ADHD (5,69), but ultimately systematic reviews report an absence of consistent resting EEG
abnormalities that could be characteristic of ADHD (6). This is in line with the data presented
here, for which no significant differences in relative spectral power were found between
ADHD and CTRL groups. Specifically, in the first dataset we observed relatively
decreased low-beta power in ADHD patients compared to controls, while the second dataset
appeared to have the opposite pattern. One may notice significance of this result different
from the original article (20) using dataset 2. In our view, the difference may be explained
by first a loss of statistical power owing to a smaller sample size necessary for balancing the
dataset during MS analysis, and second a change in filter settings, since in the
study broadband was defined as 1 - 30 Hz while original work used 0.5 - 40 Hz.
Consequently, it is possible that microstate measures, in particular microstate D, may prove
to be more generalizable auxiliary biomarkers for the diagnosis and/or prognosis of ADHD.
Conclusion
In conclusion, and to the best of our knowledge, we present the first study resting-state
microstate dynamics in adult with ADHD. We have confirmed across two datasets that
microstates D and/or A may be promising functional biomarkers of ADHD (or at least one
subtype of it). To date, although no biological markers have been successfully used to clearly
diagnose or guide ADHD treatment, the potential application of microstate analysis in this
Journal Pre-proof
21
population could prove to be an additional asset, to better understand its neurophysiological
mechanisms.
Limitations
Given the case-cohort design as well as correlational analyses of this cross-sectional study,
there was no way of being certain whether the observed MS differences were actually a
cause or a consequence of ADHD. It is important to note that the process of diagnosing
ADHD may have differed between and within our two datasets, given the involvement of
different clinicians and psychiatric scales, and that those diagnostic methods may differ for
current standard (70,71) especially for the second dataset which has not considered
symptom history (71). Hence, it is possible that the microstate biomarkers uncovered are
not specific to ADHD as a diagnosis per se but some of its behavioral subcomponent; for
example, sleep disturbance (72).
Journal Pre-proof
22
Acknowledgments
The study was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (NCCR Synapsy grant
No. 51NF40 185897 and grant No. 320030_184677) to CMM.
Journal Pre-proof
23
Disclosures
MA is unpaid chairman of the non-profit Brainclinics Foundation, a minority shareholder in
neuroCare Group (Munich, Germany), and a co-inventor on 4 patent applications related to
EEG, neuromodulation and psychophysiology, but receives no royalties related to these
patents. The other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of
interest.
Journal Pre-proof
24
Journal Pre-proof
25
1. Kessler R, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners C, Demler O, et al. (2006): The
prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the
National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry 163: 71623.
2. Estévez N, Eich-Höchli D, Dey M, Gmel G, Studer J, Mohler-Kuo M (2014): Prevalence of
and associated factors for adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young
Swiss men. PLoS One 9: e89298.
3. Adamou M, Fullen T, Jones S (2020): EEG for Diagnosis of Adult ADHD: A Systematic
Review With Narrative Analysis. Front Psychiatry 11: 871.
4. Alba G, Pereda E, Mañas S, Méndez L, González A, González J (2015):
Electroencephalography signatures of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
clinical utility. Neuropsychiatr Treat 11: 275569.
5. Arns M, Conners C, Kraemer H (2013): A decade of EEG Theta/Beta Ratio Research in
ADHD: a meta-analysis. J Atten Disord 17: 37483.
6. Lenartowicz A, Mazaheri A, Jensen O, Loo S (2018): Aberrant Modulation of Brain
Oscillatory Activity and Attentional Impairment in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 3: 1929.
7. Bussalb A, Collin S, Barthélemy Q, Ojeda D, Bioulac S, Blasco-Fontecilla H, et al. (2019): Is
there a cluster of high theta-beta ratio patients in attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder? Clin Neurophysiol 130: 13871396.
8. Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch J, Greenstein D, et al. (2007):
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical
maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U A 104: 1964954.
Journal Pre-proof
26
9. Donoghue T, Dominguez J, Voytek B (2020): Electrophysiological Frequency Band Ratio
Measures Conflate Periodic and Aperiodic Neural Activity. eNeuro 7.
10. Whitford T, Rennie C, Grieve S, Clark C, Gordon E, Williams L (2007): Brain maturation
in adolescence: concurrent changes in neuroanatomy and neurophysiology. Hum
Brain Mapp 28: 22837.
11. Arns M, Vollebregt MA, Palmer D, Spooner C, Gordon E, Kohn M, et al. (2018):
Electroencephalographic biomarkers as predictors of methylphenidate response in
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 28: 881891.
12. Drechsler R, Brem S, Brandeis D, Grünblatt E, Berger G, Walitza S (2020): ADHD:
Current Concepts and Treatments in Children and Adolescents. Neuropediatrics 51:
315335.
13. Arns M, Conners CK, Kraemer HC (2012): A Decade of EEG Theta/Beta Ratio Research
in ADHD. J Atten Disord 17: 374383.
14. Clarke AR, Barry RJ, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, Croft RJ (2002): EEG differences
between good and poor responders to methylphenidate in boys with the inattentive
type of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 113: 11911198.
15. Gevensleben H, Holl B, Albrecht B, Vogel C, Schlamp D, Kratz O, et al. (2009): Is
neurofeedback an efficacious treatment for ADHD? A randomised controlled clinical
trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50: 780789.
16. Janssen TWP, Bink M, Geladé K, Mourik R van, Maras A, Oosterlaan J (2016): A
randomized controlled trial into the effects of neurofeedback methylphenidate, and
physical activity on EEG power spectra in children with ADHD. J Child Psychol
Psychiatry 57: 633644.
Journal Pre-proof
27
17. Koehler S, Lauer P, Schreppel T, Jacob C, Heine M, Boreatti-Hümmer A, et al. (2009):
Increased EEG power density in alpha and theta bands in adult ADHD patients. J
Neural Transm Vienna 116: 97104.
18. Poil S, Bollmann S, Ghisleni C, O’Gorman R, Klaver P, Ball J, et al. (2014): Age dependent
electroencephalographic changes in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD). Clin Neurophysiol 125: 162638.
19. Woltering S, Jung J, Liu Z, Tannock R (2012): Resting state EEG oscillatory power
differences in ADHD college students and their peers. Behav Brain Funct 8: 60.
20. Deiber M, Hasler R, Colin J, Dayer A, Aubry J, Baggio S, et al. (2020): Linking alpha
oscillations, attention and inhibitory control in adult ADHD with EEG
neurofeedback. Neuroimage Clin 25: 102145.
21. Loo S, Hale T, Macion J, Hanada G, McGough J, McCracken J, Smalley S (2009): Cortical
activity patterns in ADHD during arousal, activation and sustained attention.
Neuropsychologia 47: 21149.
22. Arns M, Swatzyna RJ, Gunkelman J, Olbrich S (2015): Sleep maintenance spindling
excessive beta and impulse control: an RDoC arousal and regulatory systems
approach? Neuropsychiatr Electrophysiol 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40810-015-
0005-9
23. Meier N, Perrig W, Koenig T (2014): Is excessive electroencephalography beta activity
associated with delinquent behavior in men with attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder symptomatology? Neuropsychobiology 70: 2109.
Journal Pre-proof
28
24. Silk T, Malpas C, Beare R, Efron D, Anderson V, Hazell P, et al. (2019): A network
analysis approach to ADHD symptoms: More than the sum of its parts. PLoS One 14:
e0211053.
25. Loo S, McGough J, McCracken J, Smalley S (2018): Parsing heterogeneity in attention-
deficit hyperactivity disorder using EEG-based subgroups. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
59: 223231.
26. Johnstone SJ, Barry RJ, Clarke AR (2013): Ten years on: A follow-up review of ERP
research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 124: 644
657.
27. Michel C, Koenig T (2018): EEG microstates as a tool for studying the temporal
dynamics of whole-brain neuronal networks: A review. Neuroimage 180: 577593.
28. Meier NM, Perrig W, Koenig T (2012): Neurophysiological correlates of delinquent
behaviour in adult subjects with ADHD. Int J Psychophysiol 84: 116.
29. Doehnert M, Brandeis D, Schneider G, Drechsler R, Steinhausen H-C (2013): A
neurophysiological marker of impaired preparation in an 11-year follow-up study of
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Marker of impaired preparation in
ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54: 260270.
30. Kindler J, Hubl D, Strik WK, Dierks T, Koenig T (2011): Resting-state EEG in
schizophrenia: Auditory verbal hallucinations are related to shortening of specific
microstates. Clin Neurophysiol 122: 11791182.
31. Krepel N, Egtberts T, Sack AT, Heinrich H, Ryan M, Arns M (2020): A multicenter
effectiveness trial of QEEG-informed neurofeedback in ADHD: Replication and
treatment prediction. NeuroImage Clin 28: 102399.
Journal Pre-proof
29
32. Kooij JJS, Boonstra AM, Swinkels SHN, Bekker EM, Noord I de, Buitelaar JK (2008):
Reliability Validity, and Utility of Instruments for Self-Report and Informant Report
Concerning Symptoms of ADHD in Adult Patients. J Atten Disord 11: 445458.
33. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ (1989): The Pittsburgh sleep
quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res
28: 193213.
34. PAUL RH, GUNSTAD J, COOPER N, WILLIAMS LM, CLARK CR, COHEN RA, et al. (2007):
CROSS-CULTURAL ASSESSMENT OF NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PERFORMANCE AND
ELECTRICAL BRAIN FUNCTION MEASURES: ADDITIONAL VALIDATION OF AN
INTERNATIONAL BRAIN DATABASE. Int J Neurosci 117: 549568.
35. WILLIAMS LM, SIMMS E, CLARK CR, PAUL RH, ROWE D, GORDON E (2005): THE TEST-
RETEST RELIABILITY OF A STANDARDIZED NEUROCOGNITIVE AND
NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL TEST BATTERY: “NEUROMARKER.” Int J Neurosci 115:
16051630.
36. ARNS M, GUNKELMAN J, BREER M, SPRONK D (2008): EEG PHENOTYPES PREDICT
TREATMENT OUTCOME TO STIMULANTS IN CHILDREN WITH ADHD. J Integr
Neurosci 07: 421438.
37. Williams LM, Rush AJ, Koslow SH, Wisniewski SR, Cooper NJ, Nemeroff CB, et al. (2011):
International Study to Predict Optimized Treatment for Depression (iSPOT-D) a
randomized clinical trial: rationale and protocol. Trials 12.
https://doi.org/10.1186/1745-6215-12-4
38. Gratton G, Coles MGH, Donchin E (1983): A new method for off-line removal of ocular
artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 55: 468484.
Journal Pre-proof
30
39. Kessler RC, ADLER L, AMES M, DEMLER O, FARAONE S, HIRIPI E, et al. (2005): The
World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening
scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med 35: 245256.
40. Gorgens KA (2011): Structured Clinical Interview For DSM-IV (SCID-I/SCID-II). In:
Kreutzer JS, DeLuca J, Caplan B, editors. Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology.
New York, NY: Springer New York, pp 24102417.
41. Preisig M, Fenton BT, Matthey M-L, Berney A, Ferrero F (1999): Diagnostic interview
for genetic studies (DIGS): inter-rater and test-retest reliability of the French
version. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 249: 174179.
42. Delorme A, Makeig S (2004): EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial
EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:
921.
43. Gabard-Durnam L, Mendez LA, Wilkinson C, Levin A (2018): The Harvard Automated
Processing Pipeline for Electroencephalography (HAPPE): Standardized Processing
Software for Developmental and High-Artifact Data. Front Neurosci 12: 97.
44. Winkler I, Haufe S, Tangermann M (2011): Automatic classification of artifactual ICA-
components for artifact removal in EEG signals. Behav Brain Funct 7: 30.
45. Brunet D, Murray M, Michel C (2011): Spatiotemporal analysis of multichannel EEG:
CARTOOL. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011: 813870.
46. Bréchet L, Brunet D, Birot G, Gruetter R, Michel CM, Jorge J (2019): Capturing the
spatiotemporal dynamics of self-generated task-initiated thoughts with EEG and
fMRI. NeuroImage 194: 8292.
Journal Pre-proof
31
47. Seitzman BA, Abell M, Bartley SC, Erickson MA, Bolbecker AR, Hetrick WP (2017):
Cognitive manipulation of brain electric microstates. NeuroImage 146: 533543.
48. Comsa I, Bekinschtein T, Chennu S (2019): Transient Topographical Dynamics of the
Electroencephalogram Predict Brain Connectivity and Behavioural Responsiveness
During Drowsiness. Brain Topogr 32: 315331.
49. Krylova M, Alizadeh S, Izyurov I, Teckentrup V, Chang C, Meer J van der, et al. (2021):
Evidence for modulation of EEG microstate sequence by vigilance level. NeuroImage
224: 117393.
50. Katayama H, Gianotti LRR, Isotani T, Faber PL, Sasada K, Kinoshita T, Lehmann D
(2007): Classes of Multichannel EEG Microstates in Light and Deep Hypnotic
Conditions. Brain Topogr 20: 714.
51. Brodbeck V, Kuhn A, Wegner F von, Morzelewski A, Tagliazucchi E, Borisov S, et al.
(2012): EEG microstates of wakefulness and NREM sleep. NeuroImage 62: 2129
2139.
52. da Cruz J, Favrod O, Roinishvili M, Chkonia E, Brand A, Mohr C, et al. (2020): EEG
microstates are a candidate endophenotype for schizophrenia. Nat Commun 11:
3089.
53. D’Croz-Baron DF, Bréchet L, Baker M, Karp T (2020): Auditory and Visual Tasks
Influence the Temporal Dynamics of EEG Microstates During Post-encoding Rest.
Brain Topogr 34: 1928.
54. Milz P, Faber PL, Lehmann D, Koenig T, Kochi K, Pascual-Marqui RD (2016): The
functional significance of EEG microstatesAssociations with modalities of
thinking. NeuroImage 125: 643656.
Journal Pre-proof
32
55. Britz J, Ville DVD, Michel CM (2010): BOLD correlates of EEG topography reveal rapid
resting-state network dynamics. NeuroImage 52: 11621170.
56. Custo A, Ville DVD, Wells WM, Tomescu MI, Brunet D, Michel CM (2017):
Electroencephalographic Resting-State Networks: Source Localization of
Microstates. Brain Connect 7: 671682.
57. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, Beckmann
CF (2006): Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad
Sci 103: 1384813853.
58. Mantini D, Perrucci MG, Gratta CD, Romani GL, Corbetta M (2007): Electrophysiological
signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104:
1317013175.
59. McCarthy H, Skokauskas N, Mulligan A, Donohoe G, Mullins D, Kelly J, et al. (2013):
Attention Network Hypoconnectivity With Default and Affective Network
Hyperconnectivity in Adults Diagnosed With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder in Childhood. JAMA Psychiatry 70: 1329.
60. Scarpelli S, Gorgoni M, D’Atri A, Reda F, Gennaro LD (2019): Advances in Understanding
the Relationship between Sleep and Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder
(ADHD). J Clin Med 8: 1737.
61. Ke M, Li J, Wang L (2021): Alteration in Resting-State EEG Microstates Following 24
Hours of Total Sleep Deprivation in Healthy Young Male Subjects. Front Hum
Neurosci 15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.636252
62. Arns M, Gordon E (2014): Quantitative EEG (QEEG) in psychiatry: diagnostic or
prognostic use? Clin Neurophysiol 125: 15046.
Journal Pre-proof
33
63. Bijlenga D, Vollebregt M, Kooij J, Arns M (2019): The role of the circadian system in the
etiology and pathophysiology of ADHD: time to redefine ADHD? Atten Defic Hyperact
Disord 11: 519.
64. Drissi NM, Szakács A, Witt ST, Wretman A, Ulander M, St\aahlbrandt H, et al. (2016):
Altered Brain Microstate Dynamics in Adolescents with Narcolepsy. Front Hum
Neurosci 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00369
65. LJ I (2016): Questions about Adult ADHD Patients: Dimensional Diagnosis Emotion
Dysregulation, Competences and Empathy. Acta Psychopathol 02.
https://doi.org/10.4172/2469-6676.100069
66. Nobukawa S, Shirama A, Takahashi T, Takeda T, Ohta H, Kikuchi M, et al. (2021):
Identification of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder based on the complexity
and symmetricity of pupil diameter. Sci Rep 11: 8439.
67. Clarke A, Barry R, Baker I, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M (2017): An Investigation of
Stimulant Effects on the EEG of Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder. Clin EEG Neurosci 48: 235242.
68. Sohn H, Kim I, Lee W, Peterson B, Hong H, Chae J, et al. (2010): Linear and non-linear
EEG analysis of adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during a
cognitive task. Clin Neurophysiol 121: 186370.
69. van Dijk H, deBeus R, Kerson C, Roley-Roberts M, Monastra V, Arnold L, et al. (2020):
Different Spectral Analysis Methods for the Theta/Beta Ratio Calculate Different
Ratios But Do Not Distinguish ADHD from Controls. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback
45: 165173.
Journal Pre-proof
34
70. Sibley MH, Pelham WE, Molina BSG, Gnagy EM, Waxmonsky JG, Waschbusch DA, et al.
(2012): When diagnosing ADHD in young adults emphasize informant reports, DSM
items, and impairment. J Consult Clin Psychol 80: 10521061.
71. Sibley MH, Rohde LA, Swanson JM, Hechtman LT, Molina BSG, Mitchell JT, et al. (2018):
Late-Onset ADHD Reconsidered With Comprehensive Repeated Assessments
Between Ages 10 and 25. Am J Psychiatry 175: 140149.
72. Asherson P, Agnew‐Blais J (2019): Annual Research Review: Does late‐onset attention‐
deficit/hyperactivity disorder exist? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 60: 333352.
Journal Pre-proof
35
Legends for tables and figures
Figure 1
Dataset 1: EEG microstate topographies in ADHD adults (n=66) vs. controls (CTRL,
n=66). A) The five EEG resting-state topographies for the 3 conditions: ADHD, CTRL and ALL
(ADHD + CTRL). B) Spatial correlation coefficients of the 5 resting-state topographies
between ADHD and CTRL.
Figure 2
Dataset 1: Measures of EEG microstate dynamics in ADHD adults (n=66) vs. controls
(CTRL, n=64). A) The five EEG microstates for the 3 conditions: ADHD, CTRL and ALL (ADHD
+ CTRL). B) global explained variance (GEV) of each microstate. C) time coverage of each
microstate. D) mean duration of each microstate. (**p ≤ 0.001, *p ≤ 0.05, Bonferroni
corrected for 15 comparisons). Boxplots consist of median (Q2), first quartile (Q1), third
quartile (Q3), maximum (Q3 + 1.5*(Q3 - Q1)), minimum (Q1 -1.5*((Q3 - Q1).
Figure 3
Dataset 1: Correlation between EEG microstate parameters and ADHD clinical scores
(ADHD patients only, n=66). Scatterplots: A) between ADHD clinical score (ADHD_total)
and microstate A global explained variance (Gev, %). B) between ADHD clinical score
(ADHD_total) and microstate A Time Coverage (%). C) between ADHD clinical score
(ADHD_total) and microstate A Mean Duration (ms). ADHD patients only (n= 66), all
univariate regressions are significant.
Figure 4
Dataset 1: Correlation between EEG microstate parameters and ADHD sleep quality
(ADHD patients only, n=66). Scatterplots: A) between ADHD PSQI total score
(PQSI_total_pre) and microstate D global explained variance (Gev, %). B) between ADHD
PSQI total score (PQSI_total_pre) and microstate D Time Coverage (%). ADHD patients only
(n= 66), all univariate regressions are significant.
Figure 5
Dataset 2: EEG topographies in ADHD adults (n=22) vs. controls (CTRL, n=22). A) The
five EEG resting-state topographies for the 3 conditions: ADHD, CTRL and ALL (ADHD +
CTRL). B) Spatial correlation coefficients of the 5 resting-state topographies between ADHD
and CTRL.
Figure 6
Journal Pre-proof
36
Dataset 2: EEG microstates in ADHD adults (n=22) vs. controls (CTRL, n=21). A) The five
EEG microstates for the 3 conditions: ADHD, CTRL and ALL (ADHD + CTRL). B) global
explained variance (GEV) of each microstate. C) time coverage of each microstate. D) mean
duration of each microstate(*p ≤ 0.05, Bonferroni corrected for 6 a priori comparisons).
Boxplots consist of median (Q2), first quartile (Q1), third quartile (Q3), maximum (Q3 +
1.5*(Q3 - Q1)), minimum (Q1 -1.5*((Q3 - Q1).
Figure 7
EEG relative power spectrum differences between ADHD and CTRL groups. For dataset 1
(left panel, ADHD=66, CTRL=66) and dataset 2 (right panel, ADHD=22, CTRL=22): relative
band-power values over all electrodes. Solid lines represent mean value across subjects;
shaded areas represent 95% confidence intervals. Traditional frequency bands: delta (orange,
2 - 4Hz), theta (green, 4 - 8 Hz), alpha (blue, 8 - 12 Hz), low-beta (red, 12 - 20 Hz) and high-
beta (purple, 15 - 30 Hz) are highlighted on the x-axis.
Journal Pre-proof
Journal Pre-proof
Journal Pre-proof
Journal Pre-proof
Journal Pre-proof
Journal Pre-proof
Journal Pre-proof
Journal Pre-proof
... A growing body of research indicates significant MS alterations in a variety of neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia and depression (Khanna et al. 2015 for review). Our own group recently reported on abnormalities in EEG MS dynamics in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), finding a positive association between microstate D duration and sleep disturbance (Ferat et al. 2021). In the current study, we applied resting-state MS analysis for the first time in patients with BPD, in the hope of providing new neurophysiological insights of this disorder and/or identifying potential targets for future treatments. ...
... The pipeline for estimating the MS topographies has been described elsewhere (Ferat et al. 2021). MS topographies were estimated separately for the BPD and control groups using Thomas Koenig's Microstate toolbox v1 for EEGLAB. ...
... Group comparisons were conducted on the three MS spatiotemporal metrics using unpaired permutation test for equality of means (Ferat et al. 2021). In absence of preestablished hypothesis, the two-sided test was used. ...
Article
Full-text available
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a debilitating psychiatric condition characterized by emotional dysregulation, unstable sense of self, and impulsive, potentially self-harming behavior. In order to provide new neurophysiological insights on BPD, we complemented resting-state EEG frequency spectrum analysis with EEG microstates (MS) analysis to capture the spatiotemporal dynamics of large-scale neural networks. High-density EEG was recorded at rest in 16 BPD patients and 16 age-matched neurotypical controls. The relative power spectrum and broadband MS spatiotemporal parameters were compared between groups and their inter-correlations were examined. Compared to controls, BPD patients showed similar global spectral power, but exploratory univariate analyses on single channels indicated reduced relative alpha power and enhanced relative delta power at parietal electrodes. In terms of EEG MS, BPD patients displayed similar MS topographies as controls, indicating comparable neural generators. However, the MS temporal dynamics were significantly altered in BPD patients, who demonstrated opposite prevalence of MS C (lower than controls) and MS E (higher than controls). Interestingly, MS C prevalence correlated positively with global alpha power and negatively with global delta power, while MS E did not correlate with any measures of spectral power. Taken together, these observations suggest that BPD patients exhibit a state of cortical hyperactivation, represented by decreased posterior alpha power, together with an elevated presence of MS E, consistent with symptoms of elevated arousal and/or vigilance. This is the first study to investigate resting-state MS patterns in BPD, with findings of elevated MS E and the suggestion of reduced posterior alpha power indicating a disorder-specific neurophysiological signature previously unreported in a psychiatric population.
... In adult ADHD research, most studies have focused on eventrelated potential microstates (Mauriello et al., 2022;Meier et al., 2012), and few have explored microstate features of resting-state EEG. One previous work found that adult patients with ADHD exhibited anomalous dynamics of microstate A and D, which were related to ADHD symptoms and sleep quality, respectively (Férat et al., 2022). Yet, less is known about the microstate indices in ADHD outcomes discrimination. ...
... Interestingly, individuals diagnosed with Attention-De cit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) also experience challenges related to attention focus and short duration of sustained focus. A study have revealed that ADHD patients exhibit prolonged average durations of microstate D compared to individuals without ADHD [43]. Consequently, microstate D has emerged as a potential biomarker for ADHD. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Objective This study aims to investigate the characteristics of resting-state electroencephalogram (EEG) microstate in elderly postoperative delirium patients and non-delirium patients, to increase understanding of the pathophysiology and phenomenology of postoperative delirium. Methods Resting-state EEG data and clinically relevant information were collected from 10 postoperative delirium patients and 18 postoperative non-delirium patients. The EEG microstate characteristic parameters of the two groups were compared, and Pearson analysis was used to analyze the correlation between the microstate characteristic parameters of the delirium group and the maximal concentration of intraoperative blood glucose. Results Intergroup comparisons of microstate characterization parameters found that microstate D duration was significantly higher in the delirium group than in the non-delirium group (P< 0.05), whereas frequency of occurrence and temporal coverage were significantly lower than in the non-delirium group (P< 0.05). Within-group comparisons of microstate feature parameters found that microstate D duration was prolonged and frequency of occurrence and temporal coverage decreased in the delirium group. A comparison of microstate transition characteristics found significant differences between the two groups for transitions from microstate B to microstate D, from microstate C to microstate B, and from microstate D to microstate B (P < 0.05). Correlation analysis found a significant positive correlation between intraoperative maximal blood glucose and the frequency of occurrence (P = 0.01) and temporal coverage (P = 0.006) of microstate C. Conclusions Our results suggest that postoperative delirium has an impact on the EEG microstates during the resting-state. Changes in these microstates may be associated with altered cognition and consciousness in individuals experiencing delirium. Therefore, EEG microstate analysis holds potential clinical value for predicting and aiding in the diagnosis of postoperative delirium.
... Recent publications demonstrate the potential of microstate research to contribute to a more sophisticated diagnosis, monitoring, prognosis, and prevention of mental disorders in clinical psychology and psychiatry. Microstate characteristics may serve as biomarkers of schizophrenia (da Cruz et al. 2020;de Bock et al. 2020), affective disorders (Al Zoubi et al. 2019;Damborská et al. 2019b;Murphy et al. 2020), anxiety disorders (Al Zoubi et al. 2019), ADHD (Férat et al. 2022a), and autism (D'Croz- Baron et al. 2019;Bochet et al. 2021). Facilitating the usage of microstate analysis in clinical settings, EEG systems are relatively cheap and easy to use (e.g., compared to fMRI) and increasingly mobile and quick to implement (e.g., Gargiulo et al. 2008;Askamp and van Putten 2014;Lau-Zhu et al. 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
EEG microstates represent functional brain networks observable in resting EEG recordings that remain stable for 40–120ms before rapidly switching into another network. It is assumed that microstate characteristics (i.e., durations, occurrences, percentage coverage, and transitions) may serve as neural markers of mental and neurological disorders and psychosocial traits. However, robust data on their retest-reliability are needed to provide the basis for this assumption. Furthermore, researchers currently use different methodological approaches that need to be compared regarding their consistency and suitability to produce reliable results. Based on an extensive dataset largely representative of western societies (2 days with two resting EEG measures each; day one: n = 583; day two: n = 542) we found good to excellent short-term retest-reliability of microstate durations, occurrences, and coverages (average ICCs = 0.874-0.920). There was good overall long-term retest-reliability of these microstate characteristics (average ICCs = 0.671-0.852), even when the interval between measures was longer than half a year, supporting the longstanding notion that microstate durations, occurrences, and coverages represent stable neural traits. Findings were robust across different EEG systems (64 vs. 30 electrodes), recording lengths (3 vs. 2 min), and cognitive states (before vs. after experiment). However, we found poor retest-reliability of transitions. There was good to excellent consistency of microstate characteristics across clustering procedures (except for transitions), and both procedures produced reliable results. Grand-mean fitting yielded more reliable results compared to individual fitting. Overall, these findings provide robust evidence for the reliability of the microstate approach.
Article
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is the initial phase of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). The cognitive decline is linked to abnormal connectivity between different regions of the brain. Most brain network studies fail to consider the changes in brain patterns and do not reflect the dynamic pathological characteristics of patients. Therefore, this paper proposes a method for constructing brain networks based on microstate sequences. It also analyzes the microstate temporal parameters and introduces a new feature, the brain homeostasis coefficient (Bhc), to quantify the stability of patient brain connections. The results showed that microstate class B parameters were higher in the MCI than in the HC group. Additionally, the Bhc values in most channels of the MCI and AD groups were lower than those of the HC group, with the most significant differences observed in the right frontal lobe. These differences were statistically significant (P < 0.05). The findings indicate that connectivity in the right frontal lobe may be most severely disrupted in patients with cognitive impairment. Furthermore, the Montreal Cognitive Assessment score showed a strong positive correlation with Bhc. This suggests that Bhc could be a novel biomarker for evaluating cognitive function in patients with cognitive impairment.
Article
The diagnosis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is based on the health history and on the evaluation of questionnaires to identify symptoms. This evaluation can be subjective and lengthy, especially in children. Therefore, a biomarker would be of great value to assist mental health professionals in the process of diagnosing ADHD. Event-related potential (ERP) is one of the most informative and dynamic methods of monitoring cognitive processes. Previous works suggested that specific sets of ERP-microstates are selectively affected by ADHD. This paper proposes a new methodology for the ERP-microstate analysis and identification of ADHD patients based on complex networks to model the microstate topographic maps. The analysis of global and local features of ERP-microstate networks revealed topological differences between ADHD and healthy control. The classification using a neural network with a single hidden layer resulted in an average accuracy of 99.72% in binary classification and 99.31% in the classification of ADHD subtypes. The results were compared to the power band spectral densities and the energy of wavelet coefficients. The temporal features of ERP-microstates, such as frequency of occurrence, duration, coverage, and transition probabilities, were also evaluated for comparison proposes. Overall, the selected topological features of ERP-microstate networks derived from the proposed method performed significantly better classification results. The results suggest that topological features of ERP-microstate networks are promising to identify ADHD and its subtypes with a neural network model compared to power band spectrum density, wavelet transform, and temporal features of ERP-microstates.
Preprint
The neural manifold in state space represents the mass neural dynamics of a biological system. A challenging modern approach treats the brain as a whole in terms of the interaction between the agent and the world. Therefore, we need to develop a method for this global neural workspace. The current study aimed to visualize spontaneous neural trajectories regardless of their measuring modalities (electroencephalography [EEG], functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI], and magnetoencephalography [MEG]). First, we examined the possible visualization of EEG manifolds. These results suggest that a spherical surface can be clearly observed within the spatial similarity space. Once valid (e.g., differentiable) and useful (e.g., low-dimensional) manifolds are obtained, the nature of the sphere, such as shape and size, becomes a possible target of interest. Because these should be practically useful, we suggest advantages of the EEG manifold (essentially continuous) or the state transition matrix (coarse-grained discrete). Finally, because our basic procedure is modality-independent, MEG and fMRI manifolds were also compared. These results strongly suggest the need to update our understanding of neural mass representations to include robust "global" dynamics.
Article
Full-text available
Adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) frequently leads to psychological/social dysfunction if unaddressed. Identifying a reliable biomarker would assist the diagnosis of adult ADHD and ensure that adults with ADHD receive treatment. Pupil diameter can reflect inherent neural activity and deficits of attention or arousal characteristic of ADHD. Furthermore, distinct profiles of the complexity and symmetricity of neural activity are associated with some psychiatric disorders. We hypothesized that analysing the relationship between the size, complexity of temporal patterns, and asymmetricity of pupil diameters will help characterize the nervous systems of adults with ADHD and that an identification method combining these features would ease the diagnosis of adult ADHD. To validate this hypothesis, we evaluated the resting state hippus in adult participants with or without ADHD by examining the pupil diameter and its temporal complexity using sample entropy and the asymmetricity of the left and right pupils using transfer entropy. We found that large pupil diameters and low temporal complexity and symmetry were associated with ADHD. Moreover, the combination of these factors by the classifier enhanced the accuracy of ADHD identification. These findings may contribute to the development of tools to diagnose adult ADHD.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: The cognitive effects of total sleep deprivation (TSD) on the brain remain poorly understood. Electroencephalography (EEG) is a very useful tool for detecting spontaneous brain activity in the resting state. Quasi-stable electrical distributions, known as microstates, carry useful information about the dynamics of large-scale brain networks. In this study, microstate analysis was used to study changes in brain activity after 24 h of total sleep deprivation. Participants and Methods: Twenty-seven healthy volunteers were recruited and underwent EEG scans before and after 24 h of TSD. Microstate analysis was applied, and six microstate classes (A–F) were identified. Topographies and temporal parameters of the microstates were compared between the rested wakefulness (RW) and TSD conditions. Results: Microstate class A (a right-anterior to left-posterior orientation of the mapped field) showed lower global explained variance (GEV), frequency of occurrence, and time coverage in TSD than RW, whereas microstate class D (a fronto-central extreme location of the mapped field) displayed higher GEV, frequency of occurrence, and time coverage in TSD compared to RW. Moreover, subjective sleepiness was significantly negatively correlated with the microstate parameters of class A and positively correlated with the microstate parameters of class D. Transition analysis revealed that class B exhibited a higher probability of transition than did classes D and F in TSD compared to RW. Conclusion: The observation suggests alterations of the dynamic brain-state properties of TSD in healthy young male subjects, which may serve as system-level neural underpinnings for cognitive declines in sleep-deprived subjects.
Article
Full-text available
Re-activations of task-dependent patterns of neural activity take place during post-encoding periods of wakeful rest and sleep. However, it is still unclear how the temporal dynamics of brain states change during these periods, which are shaped by prior conscious experiences. Here, we examined the very brief periods of wakeful rest immediately after encoding and recognition of auditory and visual stimuli, by applying the EEG microstate analysis, in which the global variance of the EEG is explained by only a few prototypical topographies. We identified the dominant brain states of sub-second duration during the tasks-dependent periods of rest, finding that the temporal dynamics-represented here by two temporal parameters: the frequency of occurrence and the fraction of time coverage-of three task-related microstate classes changed compared to wakeful rest. This study provides evidence of experience-dependent temporal changes in post-encoding periods of resting brain activity, which can be captured using the EEG microstates approach.
Article
Full-text available
Band ratio measures, computed as the ratio of power between two frequency bands, are a common analysis measure in neuroelectrophysiological recordings. Band ratio measures are typically interpreted as reflecting quantitative measures of periodic, or oscillatory, activity. This assumes that the measure reflects relative powers of distinct periodic components that are well captured by predefined frequency ranges. However, electrophysiological signals contain periodic components and a 1/f-like aperiodic component, the latter of which contributes power across all frequencies. Here, we investigate whether band ratio measures truly reflect oscillatory power differences, and/or to what extent ratios may instead reflect other periodic changes, such as in center frequency or bandwidth, and/or aperiodic activity. In simulation, we investigate how band ratio measures relate to changes in multiple spectral features, and show how multiple periodic and aperiodic features influence band ratio measures. We validate these findings in human electroencephalography (EEG) data, comparing band ratio measures to parameterizations of power spectral features and find that multiple disparate features influence ratio measures. For example, the commonly applied θ/β ratio is most reflective of differences in aperiodic activity, and not oscillatory θ or β power. Collectively, we show that periodic and aperiodic features can create the same observed changes in band ratio measures, and that this is inconsistent with their typical interpretations as measures of periodic power. We conclude that band ratio measures are a non-specific measure, conflating multiple possible underlying spectral changes, and recommend explicit parameterization of neural power spectra as a more specific approach.
Article
Full-text available
The momentary global functional state of the brain is reflected in its electric field configuration and cluster analytical approaches have consistently shown four configurations, referred to as EEG microstate classes A to D. Changes in microstate parameters are associated with a number of neuropsychiatric disorders, task performance, and mental state establishing their relevance for cognition. However, the common practice to use eye-closed resting state data to assess the temporal dynamics of microstate parameters might induce systematic confounds related to vigilance levels. Here, we studied the dynamics of microstate parameters in two independent data sets and showed that the parameters of microstates are strongly associated with vigilance level assessed both by EEG power analysis and fMRI global signal. We found that the duration and contribution of microstate class C, as well as transition probabilities towards microstate class C were positively associated with vigilance, whereas the sign was reversed for microstate classes A and B. Furthermore, in looking for the origins of the correspondence between microstates and vigilance level, we found Granger-causal effects of vigilance levels on microstate sequence parameters. Collectively, our findings suggest that duration and occurrence of microstates have a different origin and possibly reflect different physiological processes. Finally, our findings indicate the need for taking vigilance levels into consideration in resting-sate EEG investigations.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Quantitative Electroencephalogram- (QEEG-)informed neurofeedback is a method in which standard neurofeedback protocols are assigned, based on individual EEG characteristics in order to enhance effectiveness. Thus far clinical effectiveness data have only been published in a small sample of 21 ADHD patients. Therefore, this manuscript aims to replicate this effectiveness in a new sample of 114 patients treated with QEEG-informed neurofeedback, from a large multicentric dataset and to investigate potential predictors of neurofeedback response. Methods A sample of 114 patients were included as a replication sample. Patients were treated with standard neurofeedback protocols (Sensori-Motor-Rhythm (SMR), Theta-Beta (TBR), or Slow Cortical Potential (SCP) neurofeedback), in combination with coaching and sleep hygiene advice. The ADHD Rating Scale (ADHD-RS) and Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) were assessed at baseline, every 10th session, and at outtake. Holland Sleep Disorder Questionnaire (HSDQ) was assessed at baseline and outtake. Response was defined as ≥25% reduction (R25), ≥50% reduction (R50), and remission. Predictive analyses were focused on predicting remission status. Results In the current sample, response rates were 85% (R25), 70% (R50), and remission was 55% and clinical effectiveness was not significantly different from the original 2012 sample. Non-remitters exhibited significantly higher baseline hyperactivity ratings. Women who remitted had significantly shorter P300 latencies and boys who remitted had significantly lower iAPF’s. Discussion In the current sample, clinical effectiveness was replicated, suggesting it is possible to assign patients to a protocol based on their individual baseline QEEG to enhance signal-to-noise ratio. Furthermore, remitters had lower baseline hyperactivity scores. Likewise, female remitters had shorter P300 latencies, whereas boys who remitted have a lower iAPF. Our data suggests initial specificity in treatment allocation, yet further studies are needed to replicate the predictors of neurofeedback remission.
Article
Full-text available
Background Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is a common neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and or impulsivity. Since the development of the concept, a reliable biomarker to aid diagnosis has been sought. One potential method is the use of electroencephalogram to measure neuronal activity. The aim of this review is to provide an up to date synthesis of the literature surrounding the potential use of electroencephalogram for diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adulthood. Methods A search of PsycINFO, PubMed, and EMBASE was undertaken in February 2019 for peer-reviewed articles exploring electroencephalogram patterns in adults (18 years with no upper limit) diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Results Differences in electroencephalogram activity are potentially unique to adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder populations. Strongest support was derived for elevated levels of both absolute and relative theta power, alongside the observation that alpha activity is able to typically differentiate between adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and normative populations. Conclusions Electroencephalogram can have a use in clinical settings to aid adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder diagnosis, but areas of inconsistency are apparent.
Article
Full-text available
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is among the most frequent disorders within child and adolescent psychiatry, with a prevalence of over 5%. Nosological systems, such as the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5) and the International Classification of Diseases, editions 10 and 11 (ICD-10/11) continue to define ADHD according to behavioral criteria, based on observation and on informant reports. Despite an overwhelming body of research on ADHD over the last 10 to 20 years, valid neurobiological markers or other objective criteria that may lead to unequivocal diagnostic classification are still lacking. On the contrary, the concept of ADHD seems to have become broader and more heterogeneous. Thus, the diagnosis and treatment of ADHD are still challenging for clinicians, necessitating increased reliance on their expertise and experience. The first part of this review presents an overview of the current definitions of the disorder (DSM-5, ICD-10/11). Furthermore, it discusses more controversial aspects of the construct of ADHD, including the dimensional versus categorical approach, alternative ADHD constructs, and aspects pertaining to epidemiology and prevalence. The second part focuses on comorbidities, on the difficulty of distinguishing between “primary” and “secondary” ADHD for purposes of differential diagnosis, and on clinical diagnostic procedures. In the third and most prominent part, an overview of current neurobiological concepts of ADHD is given, including neuropsychological and neurophysiological researches and summaries of current neuroimaging and genetic studies. Finally, treatment options are reviewed, including a discussion of multimodal, pharmacological, and nonpharmacological interventions and their evidence base.
Article
Full-text available
Electroencephalogram microstates are recurrent scalp potential configurations that remain stable for around 90 ms. The dynamics of two of the four canonical classes of microstates, commonly labeled as C and D, have been suggested as a potential endophenotype for schizophrenia. For endophenotypes, unaffected relatives of patients must show abnormalities compared to controls. Here, we examined microstate dynamics in resting-state recordings of unaffected siblings of patients with schizophrenia, patients with schizophrenia, healthy controls, and patients with first episodes of psychosis (FEP). Patients with schizophrenia and their siblings showed increased presence of microstate class C and decreased presence of microstate class D compared to controls. No difference was found between FEP and chronic patients. Our findings suggest that the dynamics of microstate classes C and D are a candidate endophenotype for schizophrenia. EEG microstate abnormalities have been reported in patients with schizophrenia. Here the authors demonstrate that patients and their siblings show similar microstate abnormalities compared to healthy controls.
Article
Full-text available
There has been ongoing research on the ratio of theta to beta power (Theta/Beta Ratio, TBR) as an EEG-based test in the diagnosis of ADHD. Earlier studies reported significant TBR differences between patients with ADHD and controls. However, a recent meta-analysis revealed a marked decline of effect size for the difference in TBR between ADHD and controls for studies published in the past decade. Here, we test if differences in EEG processing explain the heterogeneity of findings. We analyzed EEG data from two multi-center clinical studies. Five different EEG signal processing algorithms were applied to calculate the TBR. Differences between resulting TBRs were subsequently assessed for clinical usability in the iSPOT-A dataset. Although there were significant differences in the resulting TBRs, none distinguished between children with and without ADHD, and no consistent associations with ADHD symptoms arose. Different methods for EEG signal processing result in significantly different TBRs. However, none of the methods significantly distinguished between ADHD and healthy controls in our sample. The secular effect size decline for the TBR is most likely explained by factors other than differences in EEG signal processing, e.g. fewer hours of sleep in participants and differences in inclusion criteria for healthy controls.