ArticlePDF Available

Mother–infant co‐regulation in dyadic and triadic contexts at 4 and 6 months of age

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study examined interpersonal co‐regulation in 80 mother–infant dyads when interacting face‐to‐face and with an object, at the infant ages of 4 and 6 months. We found that co‐regulation patterns differed between conditions and with development. Under the dyadic condition, the dyads were mostly unilateral, with only the mother attending to the infant's focus, while in the triadic context, they were mostly symmetrical, with both mother and infant mutually engaged. While confirming previous studies showing that interpersonal co‐ordination is a relatively rare event in face‐to‐face exchanges, this result shows that the contrary is instead true when objects are available. Also, unilateral co‐regulation significantly increased in the triadic context from 4 to 6 months, together with an increase in the active role of the infant during symmetrical exchanges. The development of manipulative skills that occurred during this age period might have promoted these co‐regulation changes, through improvement of the infant activities with objects, both independently and with the mother. Finally, involvement in triadic interactions appears to relate to temperamental aspects, being favoured by the infant's disposition to be soothed by the caregiver. Overall, our results support the view of early social development as a context‐based and multidimensional process. Highlights Investigation of how mothers and infants co‐regulate in face‐to‐face interactions and with toys in the first 6 months of life. Eighty mother–infant dyads are observed longitudinally in dyadic and triadic interactions at 4 and 6 months of age. Social‐mediated object exchanges favour symmetrical co‐regulation in early mother–infant interactions, whereas unilateral co‐regulation prevails in face‐to‐face contexts.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Motherinfant coregulation in dyadic and triadic
contexts at 4 and 6 months of age
Tiziana Aureli
1
|Fabio Presaghi
2
|Maria Concetta Garito
1
1
Department of Neuroscience and Imaging,
DNI, University of ChietiPescara, Pescara,
Italy
2
Department of Psychology of Development
and Socialization Processes, Roma, La
Sapienza, Rome, Italy
Correspondence
Tiziana Aureli, Department of Neuroscience
and Imaging, DNI, University of ChietiPescara,
Italy.
Email: t.aureli@unich.it
Abstract
This study examined interpersonal coregulation in 80 motherinfant
dyads when interacting facetoface and with an object, at the infant
ages of 4 and 6 months. We found that coregulation patterns
differed between conditions and with development. Under the
dyadic condition, the dyads were mostly unilateral, with only the
mother attending to the infant's focus, while in the triadic context,
they were mostly symmetrical, with both mother and infant mutually
engaged. While confirming previous studies showing that interper-
sonal coordination is a relatively rare event in facetoface
exchanges, this result shows that the contrary is instead true when
objects are available. Also, unilateral coregulation significantly
increased in the triadic context from 4 to 6 months, together with
an increase in the active role of the infant during symmetrical
exchanges. The development of manipulative skills that occurred
during this age period might have promoted these coregulation
changes, through improvement of the infant activities with objects,
both independently and with the mother. Finally, involvement in
triadic interactions appears to relate to temperamental aspects, being
favoured by the infant's disposition to be soothed by the caregiver.
Overall, our results support the view of early social development as
a contextbased and multidimensional process.
Highlights
Investigation of how mothersand infants coregulate in facetoface
interactions and with toys in the first 6 months of life.
Eighty motherinfant dyads are observed longitudinally in dyadic
and triadic interactions at 4 and 6 months of age.
Socialmediated object exchanges favour symmetrical coregulation
in early motherinfant interactions, whereas unilateral coregulation
prevails in facetoface contexts.
KEYWORDS
facetoface and with toys interactions, interpersonal coregulation
patterns, temperament and coregulation relationship
Received: 9 October 2016 Revised: 21 October 2017 Accepted: 29 October 2017
DOI: 10.1002/icd.2072
Inf Child Dev. 2017;e2072.
https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2072
Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/icd 1of14
1|INTRODUCTION
The transition of infants from dyadic to triadic interactions marks a pivotal change in early communicative development.
Dyadic interactionsas persontopersonemerge very soon in postnatal life, at around 2 to 3 months of age, and allow
mothers and infants to exchange facial, postural, and vocal expressions in the facetoface context (Cohn & Tronick,
1987; Van Egeren, Barrat, & Roach, 2001). Triadic interactionsas persontopersontoobjectengage the partners in
communication about something that is external to the purely interpersonal context,and these interactions are commonly
observed some months later, at about the end of the first year of life (Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998; Trevarthen
& Hubley, 1978). While dyadic interactions only convey emotional and affective content, triadic interactions allow
sharing of intentions and meanings, thus moving communication beyond the body boundaries to the use of symbols.
According to the social cognitive perspective (Tomasello, 1995, 1999; Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, & Moll,
2005), the ability to participate in triadic interactions emerges at about the end of the first year of life, when infants
are involved in joint attention episodes. However, infants appreciate the triadic structure of the interactive context
well before this age. As shown by a number of behavioural studies, infants can follow other people's visual attention
towards external objects by 3 months of age (D'Entremont, Hains, & Muir, 1997; Hains & Muir, 1996; Hood, Willen, &
Driver, 1998). This ability improves in the subsequent few months, with infants becoming capable of actively
coordinating their visual attention between people and objects (Flom & Pick, 2005; Striano & Bertin, 2005), and
perceiving the link between the two (Woodward, 1998). Moreover, infants can distinguish facetoface interactions
from personobject interactions by 3 months of age and can also discriminate between different kinds of
personobject interactions (Striano & Stahl, 2005).
Further support for early triadic ability is provided by neuroimaging data. Studies on eventrelated potential have
shown that at 4 months of age, the brain activity related to attentional processes is higher when an infant looks at an
object that an adult did not look at, compared to an object that the adult did look at. This suggests that a cued object is
perceived as more familiar than an uncued object (Reid, Striano, Kaufman, & Johnson, 2004). Consistent with this,
attentionrelated brain activity at 5 months is greater under the joint attention condition where the experimenter
alternates their gaze from the object to the infant's eyes, than under the nonjointattention condition (Parise, Reid,
Stets, & Striano, 2008). Recent studies using nearinfrared spectroscopy (see LloydFox, Blasi, & Elwell, 2010, for
details) have complemented these data by showing that by the age of 4 months (as also seen with adults), a specific
area in the left dorsal prefrontal cortex is recruited when infants are presented with joint attention conditions, as com-
pared to conditions where there is no referent or eye contact involved (Grossmann & Johnson, 2010).
While experimental studies have revealed that infants are sensitive to key components of the triadic context very
early in their development, observational studies have consistently shown that they can also be involved in person
personobject exchanges this early. As shown by the pivotal study of Trevarthen and Hubley (1978), objects are
incorporated into motherinfant games by 4 months of age. Later, Lyra and RossettiFerreira (1994) consistently
observed that mothers introduce objects into facetoface play by 3 months of age. The recent review by Fogel,
Garvey, Hsu, and WestStroming (2006) of the few studies that have focused on the social involvement of objects
at an infant age of under 6 months also reported that object play intersperses motherinfant interactions by 5 months
of age. Even more recently, de Barbaro, Johnson, and Deák (2013) confirmed this picture. By collecting systematic
observations of the realtime activities in motherinfant free play from the infant age of 4 to 12 months, they detailed
the way in which the infants become progressively able to follow routines that involve both people and objects, and
the way in which the mothers adapt their interactive behaviour to the growing skills of the infant.
The beginning of infant involvement in early socialmediated object exchanges can relate to the concurrent
improvement of motor skills. In particular, two motor domains strongly improve in this age period: object manipulation
and postural control. For object manipulation, the strengthening of eyehand and handmouth coordination and the
emergence of systematic handling and grasping of objects (Rochat & Goubet, 1995) allow infants to contact objects
in a more active way than previously (Rochat, 1989), thus promoting their interactions with the physical reality
(Bushnell & Boudreau, 1993; Palmer, 1989). For postural control, experimental (Fogel, Dedo & McEwen, 1992) and
2of14 AURELI ET AL.
naturalistic (Fogel, Messinger, Dickson, & Hsu, 1999; Fogel, Nwokah, Hsu, Dedo, & Walker, 1993; Kaye & Fogel, 1980;
Savelsbergh & van der Kamp, 1993) studies have shown that the increasing ability to remain in an upright position
allows the infants to be more likely to gaze at the surroundings and to reach for available objects, compared to when
they are in reclined or supine positions. Both of these changes in the motor domain might influence the motherinfant
interactions. As consistently reported by a number of very careful, although older, observational studies (Adamson &
Bakeman, 1985; Bakeman & Adamson, 1984; Kaye, 1982; Kaye & Fogel, 1980; Trevarthen, 1984), infant attention to
the mother's face during dyadic interactions declines by 3 to 4 months of age, in favour of the attention to the environ-
ment, and the portion of the infant's time spent looking away from the mother is more often spent attending to objects
(Cohn & Tronick, 1987). This shift appears to contribute to the opening of the dyadic context to the external reality.
Based on the habituation paradigm, Libertus and Needham (2014) recently reported a clear visual preference for a
face compared to a toy in 5monthold infants, so apparently counteracting the above results. It might be that at this
age the human face represents a more salient stimulus than the object when the infant looks at the two in an exper-
imental paradigm based on the salience of a picture; however, when the objects are available for manipulation, they
could be preferred to the person for interacting. Indeed, infants at this age contact objects in a more efficient way
then previously, thus being able to use manipulation for exploring a reality that is still largely novel for them and
presumably more attractive compared to the quite familiar human face.
The parents have a relevant part in the opening of the dyadic context to triadic interactions. As noted by the
pivotal study of Kaye and Fogel (1980), the mothers appear to easily capture the shift in the infant's attention from
their face to the object, which occurs at around 4 months. Indeed, while they try to catch and hold the infant's atten-
tion towards themselves at 6 weeks, they try to incorporate the attended objects into play at 26 weeks. Accordingly, a
number of subsequent observational studies (de Barbaro et al., 2013; Fogel et al., 2006; Lyra & RossettiFerreira,
1994) reported that when mothers are interacting with their infants early in the first year of the infant life and in a
context where objects are available, they are likely to spent time in objectinfant interactions. The mothers bring
the toys nearer to the infants to favour manual contact with them, they move the toys in front of the infant to convey
the infant's attention to them, and they manipulate the toys to reveal their properties. Therefore, although dyadic
interactions are the most notable occasions for social exchanges at an early age, triadic interactions are in place as
well, and infants show to appreciate a triadic way of experiencing reality well before they become able to share atten-
tion and action in fully fledged episodes of joint activity.
How motherinfant interactions unfold at an early age under both dyadic and triadic conditions is far from being
fully analysed. We know a lot about the dyadic interactions that occur in the first few months of an infants' life, and a
lot about the triadic interactions that occur later, at around the end of the first year. However, we know much less
about what happens inbetween. Here, the infant involvement in triadic interactions begins when the dyadic are also
present, and, as the structure and the content of the two are different, it is reasonable to presume that they develop in
somehow different ways. Therefore, what we know about motherinfant interactions in dyadic situations might not
be equally true in triadic situations.
To add to our knowledge of how infant social experience is at an early age, the present study analysed longitu-
dinally motherinfant interactions in facetoface (i.e., dyadic) and with objects (i.e., triadic) contexts at 4 and 6 months
of infant age. With reference to the view (Fogel, 1993, 2000) of communication as a relational event that unfolds
through the partners continuously adjusting to each other, we selected motherinfant coregulation as the key
feature of their interactive experience. Accordingly, the data were analysed with the Relational Coding System (Fogel,
1994), which distinguishes different coregulation patterns based on the quality of the interpersonal adjustment. In
particular, there are three patterns that are of particular note: unilateral (one partner pays attention to the other, while
the other attends to something else); asymmetrical (only one partner is involved in the activity, while the other simply
observes); and symmetrical (both partners are mutually engaged in a common activity). To observe these processes in
more detail, we used the revised version of this system (Fogel et al., 2003), which adds subcategories to the main codes.
The main aim of the study was to compare coregulation in dyadic interactions and in triadic interactions. As
previous studies on facetoface exchanges have consistently shown that unilateral coregulation is the most frequent
AURELI ET AL.3of14
pattern in the age period under study (Evans & Porter, 2009; Hsu & Fogel, 2003; Silvén, 2001), we expected that this
pattern would prevail over the asymmetrical and symmetrical under dyadic conditions. Due to the emerging attractive
power of objects, we would expect the same result under the triadic condition too. However, due to the mother's role
in favouring the infant's contacts with objects, the opposite could occur, with a higher presence of asymmetrical and
symmetrical coregulation patterns compared to unilateral ones.
Based on the longitudinal design of the study, an additional aim was to investigate the coregulation stability in
both contexts. According to previous studies, coregulation is little affected by the longterm history of the
motherinfant communicative experience (Hsu & Fogel, 2003); therefore, we expected to find no significant correla-
tions between 4 and 6 months of age, independent of context. Finally, as the infant temperament influences early
interactions (Putnam, Sanson, & Rothbart, 2002), since easier children might be more likely to have supportive parents
and more difficult children might have more rejecting parents, we expected that the infants who were higher in
regulation would be more able to engage in coordinated, symmetrical interactions than the lower infants, irrespective
of the social context (dyadic or triadic) in which they were involved.
2|METHOD
2.1 |Participants
One hundred mothers were recruited from a public hospital in an urban area located along the centraleastern coast of
Italy. All of the mothers were approached within 2 days of the birth of their baby, and asked to fill in a questionnaire
that included demographic information. They were then informed about the project and asked for their agreement to
participate in the present study. Eighty motherinfant dyads (80% of the mothers contacted) agreed and gave their
informed consent. No differences with respect to the educational level, age, language, and social status were found
between the mothers who agreed to participate and those who declined to participate. For the mothers who entered
the study, their mean age was 34.67 years (SD, 4.56 years; range, 2444 years), their mean length of education was
14.93 years (SD, 2.99 years; range, 818 years), and 75% (N= 45) of them had a stable job (the remaining mothers
either had temporary jobs or were not employed). Also, 84% (n= 67) had medium to high income, with fewer on a
low income (n= 13).
For the infants, 51% (N= 41) were male and 58% (N= 48) were firstborn. All of the infants were born fullterm,
and none had medical complications at birth, had experienced hospitalization, or had been diagnosed with medical or
psychological delays/ disorders. All of the infants belonged to biparental Italian families, where the mothers were
their primary caregivers, and the fathers were actively engaged in the childcare.
2.2 |Procedures
The motherinfant dyads were observed longitudinally when the infants were 4 months old (mean age, 3;9 months,
SD, 0;5; girls: mean age, 4;0 months, SD, 0;5; boys: mean age, 3;8 months, SD, 0;5) and 6 months old (mean age,
5;9 months, SD, 0;4; girls: mean age, 5;9 months, SD, 0;8; boys: mean age, 5;7 months, SD, 0;4).
The mothers were contacted by telephone twice: when their infants were about 4 months of age and 2 months
later. On both occasions, they were invited to the baby laboratory to take part in a videotaped interaction session. The
mothers were asked to play with the infant as they normally would at home. The infants were placed in an infant seat
positioned on a table, facing their mother. During the first 5 min, the mothers engaged in facetoface interactions,
and for the following 5 min, they were provided with three ageappropriate toys (i.e., a soft toy, a maracas, and a
rattle), which they could use as they wanted during the interactions. Three cameras were used to film each play
session. The first camera was focused on the face of the mother, the second on the face of the infant, and the third
recorded the general situation. The outputs from the three cameras were assembled through a special effects
generator to produce a splitscreen image with a timer superimposed on the screen.
4of14 AURELI ET AL.
2.3 |Motherinfant coregulation
Motherinfant interactions were coded using the revised Relational Coding System, which was developed by Fogel
et al. (2003) to capture the quality of the interactive involvement between mothers and infants over time (Fogel &
Lyra, 1997; Hsu & Fogel, 2003). This ranges from the absence of orientation of one partner to the other, to the mutual
and continuous adjustment of their respective actions.
The first edition of the coding system (Fogel, 1994) included five global categories of communicative interactions:
unilateral, asymmetrical, symmetrical, disruptive, and unengaged (see Table 1). This has been subsequently revised to
include subcategories, such as following, initiating, and demanding for unilateral coregulation; demonstrating and
expecting for asymmetrical; sequential and resonant for symmetrical (Fogel et al., 2003).
In analysing these data, we recognized that symmetrical sequential coregulation (continuous participation of the
two partners, with each engaging in actions that are contingent to the other's actions) occurred in two main ways: with
the infant simply accepting the mother's offer (e.g., grasping the object presented by the mother), or with the infant
adding something new (e.g., shaking the presented object, once accepted). While shared focus is achieved in both cases,
in the former case (grasping) the infant limits the interaction to receiving the mother's communicative bid, whereas in
the latter case (shaking) the infant also elaborates upon it. Therefore, although both behaviours fall under the same
category (i.e., symmetrical), only the second one fully meets the main criterion for coding a coregulation pattern as
symmetrical (i.e., the innovative quality of the partner contribution). As the difference in the quality of symmetrical
exchanges revealed by our data might be due to the infant's age and ability, we followed the recommendation of Fogel
et al. (2003) to take into account these aspects when coding the behaviours. We therefore decided to recode symmet-
rical exchanges by distinguishing the roles of the infant as accepting or innovating, respectively.
According to the coding manual (Fogel et al., 2003), episodes of coregulation lasted at least 2 s and were iden-
tified from the action of one partner followed by the opportunity for the other partner to participate. The resulting co
TABLE 1 Coregulation coding system (adapted from The Revised Relational Coding System, Fogel et al., 1993)
Codes Description
Symmetrical Both partners adjust their communicative actions to the continuously changing actions of the partner and
engage in active, mutual engagement, and shared experience via vocal and non vocal behaviours:
(a) symmetricalresonant is characterized by a high level of spontaneity and simultaneity of the actions,
emotions, and attention (e.g., I makes a funny face; M bursts out into laughter and I smiles at the same
time);
(b) symmetricalaccepting has a sequential way of proceeding, with the infant responding to the mother's
communicative signals (e.g., M offers an object to I; I takes the offered object and holds it);
(c) symmetricalinnovating has a sequential way of proceeding, with the infant responding to the mother's
communicative bids by adding something new (e.g., M offers an object to I; I takes the offered object and
smiles looking to the M).
Asymmetrical One partner is merely attending to the other without responding to the other's moves:
(a) asymmetricaldemonstrating: one partner is innovative (e.g., demonstrating a toy). The other partner
observes the first, but is otherwise not actively engaged;
(b) asymmetricalexpecting: one partner is innovative by creatively initiating a theme (e.g., vocalizing). The
other partner observes the first but does not actively participate (e.g., does not respond).
Unilateral Only one partner tries to engage the other, who is absorbed in the own activity and does not pay attention
to the partner or respond to the partner's initiations:
(a) unilateralfollowing, when the active partner is available to notice and support the other (e.g., I looks to an
object and acts upon it; M says: Yes, it is nice! isn't it?
(b) unilateralinitiating, with the active partner initiating an interaction, without receiving any response from
the other (e.g. M says Looks at the teddy bear! You can take it; I continues to be engaged with a ring)
(c) unilateraldemanding, with the mother initiating an interaction by using repetitive requests, and/or a
compelling tone of the voice, and/or an intrusive quality of touch.
Disruptive One partner disturbs the ongoing flow of action of the other partner in such a way that their interaction
becomes disrupted (e.g., I puts a toy in their mouth and M forcefully takes it out, after which I becomes
distressed).
Unengaged Partners are simultaneously engaged in acting with respect to self and not with respect to each other.
No code Missing information required for coding.
AURELI ET AL.5of14
regulation patterns were defined as in Table 1. The patterns were coded every second from the videotapes by a
trained coder, using the Mangold Interact 8 software (version 8.1.3). An independently trained coder processed
25% of the sessions to compute interobserver reliability. The Kappa values were calculated for each code under each
condition at each age and ranged from .86 to .94. The low frequency of disruptive episodesonly one occurrence (2 s)
and Unengaged episodes10 occurrences, for 158 s in totalresulted in their exclusion from the analysis.
2.4 |Infant temperament
Infant temperament was measured using the Italian short version [blinded for review] of the IBQR (Gartstein &
Rothbart, 2003). This is a rating questionnaire for which the parents of an infant are asked to report the frequency
of infant behaviours that occur over a 2week period prior to the first laboratory visit, using a 7point scale ranging
from 1 (never)to7(always). All of the mothers involved in the study were asked to fill in this questionnaire when their
infants were 4 months old (with 2 weeks tolerance), and 90% of them did so. Our research interest was in the possible
relationships between interpersonal coregulation and the infant's regulatory functioning, in terms of the ability of the
infant to easily face external and internal stimuli. Therefore, only the IBQR measures that defined this ability were
considered. According to the factor analysis conducted by the authors of both the original (Gartstein & Rothbart,
2003) and Italian [blinded for review] versions, the Regulatory factor emerged as one of the three dimensions that
defined the IBQ scores, with Surgency and Negative affectivity being the other two. Therefore, we considered only
the scales included in this factor. They are the following: Cuddliness, as the infant's expression of enjoyment and
moulding of the body when held by the caregiver; Soothability, as the reduction in the infant's fussing, crying, or dis-
tress when soothing techniques were used by the caregiver; Perceptual Sensitivity, as the infant's detection of slight,
low intensity stimuli from the external environment; Low Intensity Pleasure, as the infant's amount of pleasure or
enjoyment related to low stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, novelty, and incongruity; and Duration of Orienting,as
the infant's attention to and/or interaction with a single object for extended periods of time.
2.5 |Analysis strategy
The proportional durations of each coregulation pattern at each age under each condition were used as dependent
variables. Therefore, the total number of seconds spent by the dyad in a given category in each session was divided
by the total number of seconds in the session. Preliminary analysis for the normality of the data (investigated through
ShapiroWilk tests of normality) indicated a high degree of deviation from a normal distribution. More specifically, the
SW statistics ranged from .868 for the asymmetrical pattern at 6 months under the triadic condition to .981 for the
unilateral pattern at 4 months under the dyadic condition. Therefore, arcsin transformation was applied to all of the
variables (Sokal & Rohlf, 1981). After this transformation, only one variable still showed a significant ShapiroWilk
test: the Total Unilateral proportional duration at 6 months. Unilateral, asymmetrical and symmetrical coregulation
codes and subcodes of each of the three coregulation patterns were considered for the analyses. ANOVA was per-
formed to define the effects of coregulation, Condition (Dyadic vs. Triadic), and Time (4 months vs. 6 months) on the
proportional durations. For posthoc tests, the nominal alpha was corrected by the Bonferroni method, by controlling
for the number of pairwise comparisons performed. We also analysed the relationships between the coregulation
patterns and between coregulation and temperament. In this case, Spearman correlation coefficients were computed
and their significance was also tested by applying Bonferroni correction for multiple correlations.
3|RESULTS
Preliminary analyses showed no significant differences in any of the observed variables with respect to infant gender;
therefore, no further analysis was carried out according to gender. We first analysed coregulation, and then the
relationships between coregulation and temperament.
6of14 AURELI ET AL.
3.1 |Motherinfant coregulation
Three communication patterns accounted for most of the occurrences: unilateral, asymmetrical, and symmetrical (see
Table 2 for descriptive statistics of the main codes and subcodes). We first analysed these codes, and then their
subcodes.
3.1.1 |Main codes
As the research design involve a 2 (age points) × 2 (conditions) × 3 (coregulation) repeated measures ANOVA, we first
present the main effects, followed by the twoway interaction effects, and finally the threeway interaction effects.
The main effect of coregulation was significant (F(2, 78) = 36.82, p< .01, η
2partial
= .49), as well as the context (F(1,
79) = 84.63, p< .01, η
2partial
= .52). However, the main effect of age was nonsignificant (F(1, 79) = 1.78, p= .19,
η
2partial
= .02).
Turning to the twoway interaction effects, the coregulation pattern by context interaction was significant (F(2,
78) = 20.09, p< .01, η
2partial
= .34), as was the interaction between coregulation pattern and age (F(2, 78) = 29.37,
p< .01, η
2partial
= .43). However, the interaction between context and age was not significant (F(2, 78) = 3.51,
p= .07, η
2partial
= .04). Finally, the threeway interaction effect was significant (F(2,78) = 4.62, p= .013, η
2partial
= .11;
see Figure 1). To better investigate this interaction, we decomposed it into its simple effects (Keppel, 1991), thus
considering the coregulation patterns by condition at each of the two ages (4 vs. 6 months). At 4 months there were
significant differences between the coregulation in the dyadic context (F(2, 78) = 3.60, p= .03, η
2partial
= .09), with
unilateral coregulation being significantly higher (p< .01) than both asymmetrical and symmetrical (M
unilateral
= .34;
M
asymmetrical
= .20; M
symmetrical
= .19, respectively,). There were also significant differences between the coregulation
categories in the triadic context (F(2, 78) = 10.07, p< .01, η
2partial
= .23). Specifically, symmetrical was significantly
(p< .01) higher (M
symmetrical
= .36) than unilateral (M
unilateral
= .25), with no significant differences (p> .05) with respect
to asymmetrical (M
asymmetrical
= .30), and no significant differences between asymmetrical and unilateral.
Then at 6 months, there were also significant differences in the coregulation patterns in the dyadic context (F(2,
78) = 48.72, p< .01, η
2partial
= .55), with unilateral coregulation (M
unilateral
= .40) significantly (p< .01) higher than both
TABLE 2 Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations) for all coregulation codes in dyadic and triadic condition
at 4 and 6 months of age
Dyadic Triadic
4th 6th 4th 6th
MSDMSDMSDMSD
Unilateral .34 .19 .40 .19 .25 .19 .37 .19
Following .12 .10 .15 .11 .19 .18 .30 .16
Initiating .19 .13 .22 .13 .06 .07 .07 .08
Demanding .02 .09 .03 .12 .00 .00 .00 .01
Symmetrical .19 .15 .23 .15 .35 .22 .39 .19
Accepting .01 .03 .02 .03 .27 .23 .23 .15
Innovating .18 .15 .21 .14 .08 .12 .16 .16
Resonant .00 .00 .00 .02 .00 .01 .00 .02
Asymmetrical .20 .15 .13 .10 .30 .22 .12 .10
Demonstrating .20 .15 .13 .09 .28 .21 .11 .09
Expecting .00 .01 .00 .01 .01 .02 .01 .02
Disruption .01 .02 .00 .01 .02 .03 .03 .05
Unengaged .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00
No code .26 .13 .24 .13 .08 .10 .09 .08
AURELI ET AL.7of14
asymmetrical (M
asymmetrical
= .13) and symmetrical (M
symmetrical
= .23), and symmetrical coregulation significantly
(p< .01) higher than asymmetrical. Finally, in the triadic context, there were also significant differences among the
coregulation categories (F(2, 78) = 106.02, p< .01, η
2partial
= .73), although in this case they were characterized by
the following pattern: the symmetrical and unilateral proportional durations were statistically not different (p> .05)
from each other (M
symmetrical
= .39; M
unilateral
= .38, respectively), but both categories were significantly (p< .01) higher
than asymmetrical (M
asymmetrical
= .12).
As expected, there were no significant correlations between ages for the proportional durations of any of the
coregulation patterns.
3.1.2 |Subcodes
We first analysed the unilateral pattern. As in a previous study (Hsu & Fogel, 2003), the active agent in all of the
instances of unilateral communication was the mother, so the initiating and following subcodes refer to the maternal
role. We performed a 2 (age points) × 2 (conditions) × 2 (coregulation) repeated measures ANOVA. The main effect of
coregulation was significant (F(1,79) = 26.65, p< .01, η
2partial
= .25) as well as that of conditions (F(1,79) = 9.89,
p< .01, η
2partial
= .11) and time (F(1,79) = 22.19, p< .01, η
2partial
= .22).
Turning to the twoway interaction effects, the time by condition interaction effect was significant (F(1,79) = 4.37,
p= .04, η
2partial
= .05), and also the interaction between time by coregulation (F(1,79) = 8.68, p< .01, η
2partial
= .10) and
the interaction between conditions and coregulation (F(1,79) = 171.68, p< .01, η
2partial
= .68). Finally also the threeway
interaction was significant (F(1,79) = 6.99, p= .01, η
2partial
= .08; see Figure 2). By decomposing the threeway interac-
tion using simple effects analysis, at 4 months the two unilateral subcodes differed significantly in the dyadic context
(F(1, 79) = 21.43, p< .01, η
2partial
= 0.21), with the proportional duration of episodes in which the mother followed the
infant's attention showing significantly lower proportional durations (p< .01) than that of episodes in which the mother
attempted to initiate a communicative exchange by redirecting the infant's attention (M
following
= .13, M
initiating
= .19,
respectively). In the triadic context, the contrary was true, with the mother following significantly higher than the
mother initiating (M
following
= .19, M
initiating
= .06, respectively), F(1, 79) = 36.72, p< .01, η
2partial
= .317.
The same pattern of data was found at 6 months, with significant differences between unilateral subcodes in the
dyadic context (F(1, 79) = 16.35, p< .01, η
2partial
= .17). Here, mother following showed a lower (p< .01) proportional
duration than mother initiating (M
following
= .15, M
initiating
= .22), while in the triadic context this pattern was reversed
(F(1, 79) = 161.64, p< .01, η
2partial
= .67), with proportional durations higher for mother following than for mother
initiating (M
following
= .30, M
initiating
= .07).
For symmetrical coregulation, Infant Accepting was absent in the dyadic context at 4 months for about 68.8% of
the participants (N= 55), and at 6 months for about 57.5% (N= 46). Therefore, comparisons between the two
subcodes were computed in a 2 (Time) × 2 (Coregulation) ANOVA design only for the triadic context. Both main
FIGURE 1 Unilateral, asymmetrical, and symmetrical coregulation in dyadic and triadic contexts at 4 and 6 months of age
8of14 AURELI ET AL.
effects of Time (F(1,79) = 8.25, p< .01, η
2partial
= .09) and coregulation (F(1,79) = 48.70, p< .01, η
2partial
= .38) were
significant. The twoway interaction was significant, F(1, 79) = 8.02, p< .01, η
2partial
= .09 (see Figure 3). In particular,
simple effects analysis showed that the proportional duration of episodes in which the infants accepted their mother's
offer was significantly higher than that of episodes in which the infants also innovated (M
accepting
= .27, M
innovating
= .08),
both at 4 months (F(1, 79) = 40.10, p< .01, η
2partial
= .34) and at 6 months (M
accepting
= .23, M
innovating
= .16; F(1,
79) = 10.46, p< .01, η
2partial
= .12). However, infant accepting was similar between the two ages (F(1, 79) = .67,
p= .41, η
2partial
= .01), whereas infant innovating was significantly higher at 6 months than 4 months (F(1,
79) = 14.81, p< .01, η
2partial
= .16).
3.2 |Temperament and coregulation
With the controlling of the nominal alpha for the number of correlations (Bonferroni corrections), there was only one
significant value: in the triadic context at 6 months, Soothability was positively correlated with Symmetrical coregu-
lation, rho = .36, p< .002 (see Table 3 for all correlations).
4|DISCUSSION
The present study examined motherinfant coregulation in facetoface and with objects interactions. The dyads
were observed longitudinally at 4 and 6 months of age, in a developmental period where the infant involvement in
purely dyadic exchanges with the mother decreases in favour of interest in engaging with the external reality. As
FIGURE 2 Mother initiating and mother following in dyadic and triadic contexts at 4 and 6 months of age
FIGURE 3 Infant accepting and infant innovating in triadic contexts at 4 and 6 months of age
AURELI ET AL.9of14
previous studies on coregulation in this age period have only focused on the facetoface context, the present study
aimed to fill the gap by presenting the dyads also with a context that included objects. We asked whether and how
coregulation patterns would be influenced by the interactive context. As the infant's individual dispositions proved
to be important in the early communicative episodes, the temperament dimensions were also considered.
Mothers and infants interacting in facetoface (dyadic condition) and with toys (triadic condition) spent their time
in the three coregulation patterns that have already been identified as the most recurrent ones during the first
6 months of age (Evans & Porter, 2009; Hsu & Fogel, 2003; Silvén, 2001): unilateral, with the mothers actively
monitoring their inattentive infants; asymmetrical, with the infants paying attention to the mother's actions without
actively participating; and symmetrical, with both the mother and infant mutually engaging in a common activity.
The proportional durations of each pattern differed between the two conditions and over time. We found that in
the dyadic condition at 4 months, the unilateral pattern largely prevailed over both the asymmetrical and symmetrical
patterns, whereas in the triadic condition, the unilateral pattern was lower than the other two. This result is consistent
with Hsu and Fogel (2003) and Silvén (2001) who previously showed that unilateral coregulation takes up most of the
time in facetoface interactions in the first few months of life. As a new result, we found that the predominance of
the unilateral pattern was limited to the dyadic condition, while in the triadic condition, symmetrical communication
prevailed. Therefore, the opportunity at 4 months to have the mother and the object available in the same context
helped the dyads to spend more time in interactions than the facetoface situation allows.
This opportunity continues to favour mutual engagement in triadic interactions at the later age, as symmetrical
coregulation at 6 months was still higher in the triadic than the dyadic context, where unilateral continued to be
the prevalent pattern. However, the significant predominance of the symmetrical over unilateral in the triadic context
that had appeared 2 months before disappeared at 6 months, due to the upsurge of the unilateral, which increased
sharply at the expense of the asymmetrical. To recall, in the asymmetrical state, the two partners attend to a common
focus, with the infant watching the mother's activity without acting; while in the unilateral state, only the mother is
attending to the infant, who attends to only its own activity. Both the increase in the unilateral pattern and the
decrease in the asymmetrical pattern might relate to the concurrent changes in the motor domain. The infant manip-
ulative skills also improve strongly in this developmental period (Soska & Adolph, 2014), due to the infant mastering
reaching and grasping abilities, together with the gains in postural control. Therefore, the infant engagement with the
physical environment develops in more intensive and various ways at 6 months than previously. Due to this change,
we can suppose that, whereas the poor manipulative skills prevented the 4monthold infants from being actively
engaged with the objects, the improvement in these skills in the following 2 months allowed the 6monthold infants
to be more able to act on the objects. This advance might favour the infant transition from being a passive onlooker of
the mother's activity to being an active agent, thus increasing the time spent in manipulating objects independently, as
in unilateral coregulation. Conversely, the time spent in only observing the object manipulated by the mother, as in
TABLE 3 Spearman correlations between temperament dimensions at 4 months of age and coregulation patterns in
dyadic and triadic contexts at 4 and 6 months
Dyadic Triadic
Unilateral Symmetrical Asymmetrical Unilateral Symmetrical Asymmetrical
4th 6th 4th 6th 4th 6th 4th 6th 4th 6th 4th 6th
Duration of orienting .02 .05 .03 .19 .08 .04 .00 .03 .04 .10 .06 .09
Low intensity Pleasure .01 .07 .10 .04 .23 .04 .04 .16 .08 .15 .06 .12
Soothability .13 .18 .18 .14 .27 .02 .01 .30 .06 .36* .02 .03
Cuddling .07 .13 .15 .05 .16 .04 .09 .24 .16 .18 .03 .05
Perceptive sensibility .05 .02 .03 .03 .18 .06 .18 .11 .12 .09 .20 .01
Note. N = 80.
*Bonferroni corrected alphap< .002.
10 of 14 AURELI ET AL.
asymmetrical coregulation, decreased. Interestingly, symmetrical coregulation did not decrease, as it was as high as
unilateral coregulation, which suggests that the infant interest in acting on the object independently accompanies the
interest in acting with the mother.
This result is in accordance with previous studies, which showed some relationships between motor and social
domains at an early age. As in Clearfield (2011), advances in social interactions in the age period immediately followed
the onset of upright locomotion and, according to Sommerville, Woodward, and Needham (2005), 3monthold
infants who were able to act on objects exhibited an altered perception of the mother's actions. More recently and
of particular interest for the present study, Libertus and Needham (2011) found that the improvement in the infant's
attention to the mother's face accompanied the onset of reaching, which suggests that the infant's advances in dealing
with objects does not preclude an interest in other people. Indeed, as observed by de Barbaro et al. (2013), the two
domains begin to integrate into daily life by virtue of the infant's involvement in triadic contexts.
Results from the analysis of the subcodes further detailed the coregulation dynamics. For unilateral episodes,
the initiatives of the mother to attract and redirect the attention of the infant towards her face prevailed under the
dyadic condition. Maybe the mothers recognized the lower interest of the infant in being engaged with them, and
actively intervened in the facetoface context to make the interaction occur. When the toys were available, the fol-
lowing role prevailed, with the mothers simply monitoring and supporting the infant's activity. It might be that the
mothers took the infant's increasing interest in the objects into account, and refrained from intervening, to leave their
infants free to play with the toys. If so, the mothers behaved in opposite ways to adapt to the infant's current
resources in each context. As the proportional duration of mother following behaviour was significantly higher at
6 months compared to 2 months before, the mothers showed to adapt to their infant's increasing developmental level,
by increasing the opportunity for the infant to act on the objects independently, according to the infant's growing
skills. Altogether, these results confirm the mother's sensitivity to infant functioning as the key feature of the parental
attitude (Kochanska & Aksan, 2004).
With respect to the subcodes of symmetrical pattern, the infant role as an innovating partnerwho brings
something new to the interactionwas higher in the dyadic than triadic context. This is consistent with a huge
amount of research (as an example, see Als, Tronick, & Brazelton, 1979; Cohn & Tronick, 1987) that has shown
that infants participate actively in dyadic exchanges by reciprocating the other's moves with a wide repertoire
of expressive signals at facial, vocal, and body levels. Under the triadic condition, the passive role prevailed at both
4 and 6 months, with the infant responses to the mother's bids being limited to accepting her offers instead of
elaborating on them. However, a process leading the infant to have a more active role in that condition can be
detected, as the proportional duration of episodes in which the infants were capable of innovating the content
of social exchanges significantly increased at 6 months with respect to 2 months before. The improvement in
manipulative skills in this period would explain these results, as it might allow the infants to manage objectmedi-
ated social exchanges in a more creative manner than previously. If so, the influence of the motor domain on the
communicative domain we saw beforeunilateral coregulation was higher at 6 months compared to 4 months
would extend to symmetrical coregulation, by favouring in this case a higher quality of infant participation to
the triadic interaction as a creative partner.
With respect to the second aim, stability in coregulation was not seen, as the proportional durations of all of the
patterns did not correlate significantly between the two ages. This evidence is consistent with Hsu and Fogel (2003),
who showed that coregulation is affected by the immediately preceding pattern more than by the previous experi-
ence. It also suggests that the quality of the coregulation cannot be considered as a particular and enduring style
of a given dyad, but as a result of the current interaction. If so, the view of Fogel (1993, 2000) on coregulation as
a selforganizing phenomenon, which cannot be understood beyond the situational specificity of its occurrence,
would be supported.
Finally, according to the literature on relationships between infant temperament and early social interactions, we
found that the more regulated infants were also higher in the symmetrical pattern. In the correlation of the infant
scores at 4 months with coregulation patterns at 4 and 6 months, only one significant value emerged: the
AURELI ET AL.11 of 14
symmetrical pattern positively correlated with the Soothability dimension under the triadic condition at 6 months. The
high specificity of the relation between the regulatory dimension of temperament and coregulation at this age makes
sense, because this dimension refers to a reduction in infant fussing, crying, or distress when soothing techniques are
used by the caregiver. As being actively involved in triadic interactions might be a new and perhaps challenging expe-
rience for infants aged 6 months, being easier to overcome distress would help the more soothable infants to join this
experience better than their less soothable peers.
5|CONCLUSIONS
To the best of our knowledge, the present study is the first to investigate motherinfant coregulation in facetoface
and with toys interactions at 4 and 6 months. Studies to date have focused on either the former or the latter situation,
depending on the age period that was chosen for the observing: under facetoface conditions when the infants were
in the first half of their first year of life, or with toys when the infants were in the second half.
The limitations of this study are indicated by the lack of an intensive longitudinal design, which prevents the
study from detailing the passage from 4 to 6 months of age as a continuous process. Moreover, independent
measures related to the mother, such as sensitivity or mindmindedness, and to the infant, such as motor abilities,
would have been useful to better understand the complexity of the coregulation as a multidimensional process.
These limitations notwithstanding, the results we have obtained add to our understanding of early communicative
development. In particular, they shed light on a process (i.e., involvement of the infant in triadic interactions at an
early age) that has been poorly investigated to date. In general, through the comparison of coregulation in dyadic
and triadic interactions, this study has contributed to support a contextbased and relational view of infant
development.
ORCID
Tiziana Aureli http://orcid.org/0000-0002-8584-0230
REFERENCES
Adamson, L., & Bakeman, R. (1985). Affect and attention: Infants observed with mothers and peers. Child Development,56(3),
582593. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129748
Als, H., Tronick, E., & Brazelton, T. B. (1979). Analysis of facetoface interaction in infantadult dyads. In M. E. Lamb, S. J.
Suomi, & G. R. Stephenson (Eds.), Social interaction analysis: Methodological issues (pp. 3376). Madison: University of
Wisconsin.
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in motherinfant and peerinfant
interactions. Child Development,55(4), 12781289. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129997
Bushnell, E. W., & Boudreau, J. P. (1993). Motor development and the mind: The potential role of motor abilities as a deter-
minant of aspects of perceptual development. Child Development,64(4), 10051021. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131323
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., &Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to
15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 63, 4, Series n.176.
Clearfield, M. W. (2011). Learning to walk changes infants'social interactions. Infant Behavior & Development,34(1), 1525.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.008
Cohn, J. E., & Tronick, E. Z. (1987). Motherinfant facetoface interaction: The sequence of dyadic states at 3, 6, and 9
months. Developmental Psychology,23(1), 6877. https://doi.org/10.1037/00121649.23.1.68
de Barbaro, K., Johnson, C. M., & Deák, G. O. (2013). Twelvemonth social revolutionemerges from motherinfant
sensorimotor coordination: A longitudinal investigation. Human Development,56(4), 223248. https://doi.org/10.1159/
000351313
D'Entremont, B., Hains, S. M. J., & Muir, D. W. (1997). A demonstration of gaze following in 3to 6monthsolds. Infant
Behavior & Development,20(4), 569572. https://doi.org/10.1016/S01636383(97)900485
12 of 14 AURELI ET AL.
Evans, C. A., & Porter, C. L. (2009). The emergence of motherinfant coregulation during the first year: Link to infants' devel-
opmental status and attachment. Infant Behaviour and Development,32(2), 147158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
infbeh.2008.12.005
Flom, R., & Pick, A. D. (2005). Experimenter affective expression and gaze following in 7montholds. Infancy,7, 20172018.
Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: origins of communication, self, and culture. Great Britain: Chicago UP.
Fogel, A. (1994). Coregulation coding system. University of Utah: Unpublished manual.
Fogel, A. (2000). Beyond individuals: a relationalhistorical approach to theory and research on communication. In M. L. Genta
(Ed.), Motherinfant communication). Carocci: Rome, IT.
Fogel, A., de Koeyer, I., Secrist, C., Sipherd, A., Hafen, T., & Fricke, M. (2003). The revised Relational Coding System.
Unpublished manual. Salt Lake City: University of Utah.
Fogel, A., Dedo, J. Y., & McEwen, I. (1992). Effect of postural position and reaching on gaze during mother infant facetoface
interaction. Infant Behavior & Development,15(2), 231244. https://doi.org/10.1016/01636383(92)80025P
Fogel, A., Garvey, A., Hsu, H., & WestStroming, D. (2006). Change processes in relationships: a relationalhistorical research
approach. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489686
Fogel, A., & Lyra, M. (1997). Dynamics of development in relationships. In F. Masterpasqua, & P. Perna (Eds.), The psychological
meaning of chaos). Washington, DC: APA. https://doi.org/10.1037/10240003
Fogel, A., Messinger, D., Dickson, K. L., & Hsu, H. (1999). Posture and gaze in early motherinfant communication:
Synchronization of developmental trajectories. Developmental Science,2(3), 325332. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467
7687.00078
Fogel, A., Nwokah, E., Hsu, H. C., Dedo, J. Y., & Walker, H. (1993). Posture and communication in motherinfant interaction. In
G. J. P. Savelsbergh (Ed.), The development of coordination in infancy (pp. 395422). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. https://
doi.org/10.1016/S01664115(08)609619
Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant
Behavior & Development,26(1), 6486. https://doi.org/10.1016/S01636383(02)001698
Grossmann, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Selective prefrontal cortex responses to joint attention in early infancy. Biological
Letters,6(4), 540543. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2009.1069
Hains, S., & Muir, D. (1996). Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction. Child Development,67(5), 19401951. https://doi.org/
10.2307/1131602
Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult's eye trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological
Science,9(2), 131134. https://doi.org/10.1111/14679280.00024
Hsu, H., & Fogel, A. (2003). Stability and transitions in motherinfant facetoface communication during the first 6 months: A
microhistorical approach. Developmental Psychology,39(6), 10611082. https://doi.org/10.1037/00121649.39.6.1061
Kaye, K. (1982). The mental and social life of babies: How parents create persons. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Kaye, K., & Fogel, A. (1980). The temporal structure of facetoface communication between mothers and infants.
Developmental Psychology,16(5), 454464. https://doi.org/10.1037/00121649.16.5.454
Keppel, G. (1991). Design and analysis: A researcher's handbook (3rd ed.). Englewood, Cliffs, NJ: PrenticeHall.
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2004). Development of mutual responsiveness between parents and their young children. Child
Development,75(6), 16571676. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14678624.2004.00808.x
Libertus, K. & Needham, A. (2011). Reaching experience increases face preference in 3monthold infants. Developmental
Science 14(6), 13551364. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14677687.2011.01084.x
Libertus, K. & Needham, A. (2014). Face preference in infancy and its relation to motor activity. International Journal of
Behavioral Development,38(6), 529538. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414535122
LloydFox, S., Blasi, A., & Elwell, C. E. (2010). Illuminating the developing brain: The past, present and future of functional
near infrared spectroscopy. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews,34(3), 269284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neubiorev.2009.07.008
Lyra, M. D., & RossettiFerreira, M. C. (1994). Transformation and construction in social interaction: A new perspective on
analysis of the motherinfant dyad. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Child development within culturally structured environments.
Comparative cultural and coconstructionst perspectives(Vol. 3) (pp. 5177). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.
Palmer, C. (1989). The discriminating nature of infants' exploratory actions. Developmental Psychology,25(6), 885893.
https://doi.org/10.1037/00121649.25.6.885
Parise, E., Reid, V. M., Stets, M., & Striano, T. (2008). Direct eye contact influences the neural processing of objects in
5monthold infants. Social Neuroscience,3(2), 141150. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910701865458
AURELI ET AL.13 of 14
Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child temperament and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting (Vol. 1) Children and parenting (pp. 255277). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Reid, V. M., Striano, T., Kaufman, J., & Johnson, M. H. (2004). Eye gaze cueing facilitates neural processing of objects in
4monthold infants. Neuroreport,15(16), 25532555. https://doi.org/10.1097/0000175620041115000025
Rochat, P. (1989). Object manipulation and exploration in 2to5monthold infants. Developmental Psychology,25(6),
871884. https://doi.org/10.1037/00121649.25.6.871
Rochat, P., & Goubet, N. (1995). Development of sitting and reaching in 5to 6month old infants. Infant Behavior & Develop-
ment,18(1), 5378. https://doi.org/10.1016/01636383(95)900071
Savelsbergh, G. J. P., & van der Kamp, J. (1993). The coordination of infant's reaching, grasping, catching and posture: A
natural physical approach. In G. J. P. Savelsbergh (Ed.), The development of coordination in infancy (pp. 289317).
NorthHolland: Amsterdam, the Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1016/S01664115(08)609577
Silvén, M. (2001). Attention in very young infants predicts learning of first words. Infant Behavior & Development,24, 229237.
Sokal, R. R., & Rohlf, J. F. (1981). Biometry: The principles and practice of statistics in biological research(2nd ed.).
San Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Company.
Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3monthold Infants' perception of others'
actions. Cognition,96(1), B1B11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004
Soska, K. C., & Adolph, K. E. (2014). Postural position constrains multimodal object exploration in infants. Infancy,19(2),
138161. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12039
Striano, T., & Bertin, E. (2005). Socialcognitive skills between 5 and 10 months of age. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology,23(4), 559568. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X26282
Striano, T., & Stahl, D. (2005). Sensitivity to triadic attention in early infancy. Developmental Science,8(4), 333343. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.14677687.2005.00421.x
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore, & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: its origins and role in
development (pp. 103130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of
cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,28(5), 675691. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000129
Trevarthen, C. (1984). Emotions in infancy: Regulators of contact and relationships with persons. In K. R. Scherer, & P. Ekman
(Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 129162). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year.
In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183229). London, New York, San Francisco:
Academic Press.
Van Egeren, L. A., Barrat, M. S., & Roach, M. A. (2001). Motherinfant responsiveness: Timing, mutual regulation and interac-
tional context. Developmental Psychology,37(5), 684697. https://doi.org/10.1037/00121649.37.5.684
Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition,69(1), 134. https://doi.org/
10.1016/S00100277(98)000584
How to cite this article: Aureli T, Presaghi F, Garito MC. Motherinfant coregulation in dyadic and triadic
contexts at 4 and 6 months of age. Inf Child Dev. 2017;e2072. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2072
14 of 14 AURELI ET AL.
... Table 3. Ten studies used these questionnaires (Albers et al., 2007;Aureli et al., 2018;Henderson et al., 2001;Karass, 2002;Markova, 2008;Penela et al., 2012;Penela et al., 2015;Salley et al., 2013;Thompson & Lamb, 1982;Vaughan Van Hecke et al., 2007). Albers et al. (2007) examined the relationships between the IBQ-R and social play and interactions to investigate if negative emotionality in 3-month-olds was associated with the quality of social interaction with caregivers of childcare centres at 6 month of age. ...
... The lack of relationship between temperament and social communication did not appear to stem from the caregiver. Similarly, a longitudinal examination of the relationship between infant temperament at 4 months of age was also not associated with mother-child interactions at 6 months of age (Aureli et al., 2018). Aureli et al. (2018) examined infant temperament using the cuddliness, soothability, perceptual sensitivity, low intensity pleasure, and duration of orienting scales of the IBQ-R (Italian version) at 4 months and mother-child communicative interactions during face-to-face/dyadic and object/triadic play at 6 months of age. ...
... Similarly, a longitudinal examination of the relationship between infant temperament at 4 months of age was also not associated with mother-child interactions at 6 months of age (Aureli et al., 2018). Aureli et al. (2018) examined infant temperament using the cuddliness, soothability, perceptual sensitivity, low intensity pleasure, and duration of orienting scales of the IBQ-R (Italian version) at 4 months and mother-child communicative interactions during face-to-face/dyadic and object/triadic play at 6 months of age. ...
Article
Emotion regulation (ER) refers to the ability to regulate emotional reactions in response to stimuli. Social communication involves the knowledge and skills to engage in social interactions. Both processes develop in the first years of life and form the foundation for later functioning. Literature suggests a bidirectional relationship between ER and social communication; however, the majority of research examines these constructs independently. This review provides an in‐depth examination of research that has measured the relations between ER and social communication in children from 2 years of age and onwards. Findings revealed an age‐related pattern, where ER was related to later social communication and vice versa. However, there was no consensus regarding direct relationship(s) between ER and social communication due to heterogeneity among studies when defining and measuring these constructs. This review illustrates the importance of understanding the pathways between ER and social communication and may inform future studies, both in typical and atypical development.
... Studies based on the RCS have shown that three main patterns feature in mother-infant co-regulation over the first 2 years of the infant's life (Silven, 2001;Hsu & Fogel, 2003;Evans & Porter, 2009;Aureli, Presaghi, Garito, 2018): unilateral, with the mother paying attention to the infant, who instead pays attention to something else; asymmetrical, with the mother trying to engage a passively attentive infant; and symmetrical, with the two partners involved in mutual, active, and collaborative engagement. These patterns showed to change dramatically with the age: unilateral predominate in the first half of the infant's first year, then symmetrical becomes predominant at the end of the year, and this shift consolidates by the year after, when the co-regulation patterns are almost exclusively symmetrical. ...
... Furthermore, the situation described by Hsu and Fogel (2003) was only dyadic, whereas it was only triadic with Evans and Porter (2009), and it was a mix between these two in Silve'n (2001). As co-regulation shows different qualities in the dyadic context compared to the triadic context (Aureli, Presaghi, & Garito, 2018), such discrepancies are expected to occur. ...
Article
Dyadic interaction is the ecological niche in which early human development occurs and parent-infant co-regulation – the reciprocal adjustment between partners when interacting - is key for the dyadic functioning. Research showed that co-regulation changes with the advancing age; however how this development unfolds and by which variables would be affected is largely ignored. The present study investigated co-regulation longitudinal progression across seven-time points (4, 6, 9, 12, 15, 21 and 24 months of infants’ age) while exploring the moderating effects of three infant variables (object manipulation, alternated gaze and shared meanings). The sample included healthy mother-infant dyads (N = 79) interacting for 5′ in a context including objects. The interaction was coded by using the Fogel’s Relational Coding System that distinguishes three main co-regulation patterns, i.e., unilateral, asymmetrical and symmetrical. Object manipulation, alternated gaze and shared meanings were also observed in the first, middle and final parts of the observational period, respectively. We used multilevel modeling technique to identify the developmental trajectories of each co-regulation pattern; we also analyzed whether the trends were moderated by specific factors at specific time periods. Results were statistically significant in both cases. We found an acceleration effect of the age on all the three co-regulation patterns. To specify, asymmetrical pattern decreased very soon, unilateral increased sharply from 6 to 9 months and then declined, symmetrical increased in the 12–15 months period and jumping up at the end. We also found a moderating effect of all the three infant’s variables: infants who were higher in object manipulation increased unilateral pattern earlier and more than lower infants; infants who were higher in alternating the gaze between mother and object and in sharing meanings showed a higher and steeper trend of symmetrical pattern. This study is the first shaping co-regulation changes in their form and rate, so reliably accounting for the developmental nature of this process. It also showed that co-regulation changes related to different age dependent skills, thus contributing to define this process as a complex phenomenon.
... In order to enhance the generalizability of the findings, free play interactions took place in the naturalistic setting of the dyads' homes. The free play procedure allowed dyads the flexibility to engage in both dyadic and triadic (including toys) interactions, which elicit different patterns of co-regulation (Aureli et al., 2018). In accordance with the Dynamic Systems perspective, patterns in the dyads' communication were observed and analyzed using Hsu and Fogel (2003) patterns of co-regulation, rather than separate discrete behaviors of the mother and infant . ...
... Observing mother-infant dyads interact in a free play setting within their homes provided a naturalistic setting and increased generalizability of the study. However, research from Aureli et al. (2018) indicates that co-regulation is heavily influenced by context. Using only the free play task may have limited the variability of patterns of co-regulation that the dyads engaged in. ...
Article
From birth, mothers and infants co-regulate their interactions that are shaped by their socio-emotional development, relationship history, current circumstances, and goals. However, few studies have longitudinally explored co-regulation in the context of medical and psycho-social risk. The present 4-wave longitudinal study sought to shed light on factors associated with co-regulation over time in infants from 6- to 48-months. The objectives were to 1) identify differences in co-regulation among low- and at-risk infant-mother dyads, 2) explore changes in co-regulation over time, and 3) explore the associations between infant-mother co-regulation and parenting stress in these low- and at-risk groups over time. Participants included three groups of infant-mother dyads (full-term [FT], n = 48; very low birthweight/preterm [VLBW/preterm] born 26–32 weeks, weighing 800–1500 g, n = 61; psycho-socially at-risk where parents had histories of socioeconomic disadvantage, n = 54) followed longitudinally at 6-, 12-, 18-, and 48-months of age. Dyads engaged in a free play in their homes that was coded for co-regulation using Fogel, de Koeyer, Secrist, Sipherd, Hafen, and Fricke’s (2003) Revised Relational Coding System (RRCS), and mothers reported on their level of parenting stress. Results from MANOVAs at each time point indicated significant differences between the groups at 18-months, with psycho-socially at-risk dyads engaging in more one-sided interactions than FT and VLBW/preterm dyads, and more dysregulation and miscommunication than VLBW/preterm dyads. Multi-level models of co-regulation revealed that dyads became progressively less synchronous from 6- to 12-months, followed by greater synchrony and mutual reciprocity from 12-months onwards. Parenting stress was associated with less synchrony and less mutual reciprocity amongst the at-risk groups. Maternal education was associated with greater engagement and girls tended to engage in more synchronous interactions than boys. Our results underscore the value and implications of considering background risk and concurrent parent perceptions in the development and reciprocity of parent-infant co-regulation and their subsequent relationships from infancy onwards.
... Early parent-infant co-regulation, also called pre-dyadic regulation or dyadic co-regulation (Sansavini et al., 2015;Taipale, 2016), is generally an interaction process between infants and parents on emotional and behavioral levels (Aureli et al., 2017). Newborns cannot yet distinguish ...
Article
Full-text available
This study, conducted in Germany, examines the role of maternal soothing strategies to explain the association of maternal self‐efficacy with infant regulation (crying and sleeping behavior). Questionnaire data of 150 mothers, living in Germany, with mixed ethnic and educational backgrounds were collected when infants were 3 and 7 months old. Two types of maternal soothing strategies were distinguished: close soothing , involving close physical and emotional contact, and distant soothing , involving physical and emotional distancing from the infant. A cross‐sectional SEM at 3 months indicated that maternal self‐efficacy is associated with reported infant regulation through distant soothing strategies. Low maternal self‐efficacy was associated with frequent maternal use of distant soothing, which in turn was related to reported infant regulation problems, that is, non‐soothability and greater crying frequency. Frequent use of close soothing was associated with reported infant sleeping behavior, that is, frequent night‐time awakenings. A longitudinal SEM further indicated that the effects of close soothing persisted at least until the infants' age of 7 months. The study showed how low maternal self‐efficacy, increased use of distant soothing, and reported early infant regulation problems are intertwined and that, due to their persisting positive effect on infant soothability, close soothing better supports infant development.
... While adult mediation is key to creating spaces for early interaction, the infants' progressively more active participation favours the dynamic interactive patterns that both co-construct (Rollins and Greenwald, 2013;Schneider et al., 2022). Some studies suggest that, in situations of triadic interaction with objects, a symmetrical co-regulation prevails between both participants (Aureli et al., 2017). This is not observed in conditions of dyadic interaction (i.e., without objects), in which it is the adult who tends to attract, maintain, and redirect the infant's attention. ...
Article
Full-text available
Infants’ early interactions with adults and everyday objects are key to socio-communicative development, but their emergence and development are still under debate. Aiming at describing the diversity of theoretical and methodological approaches on triadicity during the first year of life, we conducted a systematic and qualitative review of recent literature. Following PRISMA 2020 guidelines, we explored the scientific production of recent decades on triadic interactions up to 12 months of age. We initially screened 1943 items from which we obtained a final sample of 51 publications. Studies are usually conducted in laboratory settings, while ecological research is becoming increasingly common, especially in home settings. According to a thematic analysis of the data, we discussed the different perspectives on the origin and conceptualization of triadic interactions, and how they contribute to structuring and facilitating other developmental phenomena, such as the children’s communicative gestures and uses of objects. Prior to the origin of intentional communication, adults facilitate early forms of triadicity based on fostering opportunities for infants’ communication and engagement with both adults and materiality. However, there is a need for further research that explore the potential of early triadic interactions for parenting and early childhood education practises.
... days, SD = 15.78 days). Prior to 6-months, research suggests that mother-infant co-regulated states tends to be dominated by more unilateral patterns of interactions with mothers typically taking the lead in these interactions (see Aureli, Presaghi, & Garito, 2018). To capture the transition to increasingly more symmetrical patterns of co-regulation and the active role of the infant in guiding co-regulated states, we began with 6-month-old infants and then tracked participants in three months intervals. ...
Article
Using Porges’ (2011) Polyvagal Theory as a backdrop, this study examined whether changes in parasympathetic functioning, as indexed by baseline measures of cardiac vagal tone at 6, 9, and 12 months of age, were linked to changes in infants’ (N = 101) dyadic co-regulation over these same time points. Mothers and infants were observed at each time point during a 15-minute unstructured free-play and co-regulated patterns of interactions were coded using the Revised Relational Coding System (Fogel et al., 2003). Analyses were carried out using multi-process growth curve modeling to examine baseline measurements (intercepts) and changes (slopes) in vagal tone, co-regulation as well as mothers’ report of infant temperament. Findings demonstrate links between infants’ vagal tone and changes in mother-infant co-regulation. Specifically, increasing levels of cardiac vagal tone was related to increases in symmetrical but decreases in unilateral patterns of co-regulation over time. These findings suggest that changes in the autonomic nervous system likely undergird infants’ improving capacity to engage in more mutually sustained patterns of co-regulation.
... The second half of the first year of life is marked by significant changes in infants' active interaction behavior (Callaghan et al., 2011). While infants engage in dyadic face-to-face interactions from 2 months on (Aureli, Presaghi, & Garito, 2017;Striano, 2001), they begin to develop competencies for triadic social interactions in the second half of the first year (Carpenter, 2010;Carpenter, Nagell, Tomasello, Butterworth, & Moore, 1998;Striano & Reid, 2006). In addition to social cognitive developments (including an emerging understanding of others as intentional agents, Tomasello & Carpenter, 2007), infants' social attention is marked by significant changes in social motivation, including an increasing interest in coordinating attention with others. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examined 7-to-13.5-month-old middle-class Western infants' visual orienting to third-party interactions in parallel with their social attention behavior during own social interactions (Leipzig, Germany). In Experiment 1, 9.5-to-11-month-olds (n = 20) looked longer than 7-to-8.5-month-olds (n = 20) at videos showing two adults interacting with one another when simultaneously presented with a scene showing two adults acting individually. Moreover, older infants showed higher social engagement (including joint attention) during parent-infant free play. Experiment 2 replicated this age-related increase in both measures and showed that it follows continuous trajectories from 7 to 13.5 months (n = 50). This suggests that infants' attentional orienting to others' interactions coincides with parallel developments in their social attention behavior during own social interactions.
... The rhythmic structure of the early social interactions helps the infants to give structure to the interaction itself and, in a broader sense, to their experience in the world . As infants develop, adults progressively let them take the lead in these rhythmic interchanges (Moreno-Núñez et al., 2017;Aureli et al., 2018). Therefore, during the first months of life, infants are familiar with how these rhythmic interchanges work, as well as with the social impact of these actions on the adult. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study is to analyze the relationship between rhythmic movements and deictic gestures at the end of the first year of life, and to focus on their unimodal or multimodal character. We hypothesize that multimodal rhythmic movement performed with an object in the hand can facilitate the transition to the first deictic gestures. Twenty-three children were observed at 9 and 12 months of age in a naturalistic play situation with their mother or father. Results showed that rhythmic movements with objects in the hand are a frequent behavior in children's repertoires. Rhythmic behaviors tend to decrease from 9 to 12 months, specifically when they are unimodal. Multimodal rhythmic behavior production at 9 months is positively related with proximal deictic gestures 3 months later. Multimodal rhythmic movements are not directly related to distal deictic gestures, but are indirectly related via proximal deictic gestures. These results highlight the relevance of multimodal behaviors in the transition to the use of early gestures, and can be considered as a transitional phenomenon between the instrumental action and early communicative gestures.
... Procesy samoregulacji i regulacji przez drugą osobę są ze sobą ściśle związane tak, że osoby tworzą rodzaj złożonej jednostki regulującej. Dlatego też mówi się o ko-regulacji (co-regulation; Aureli, Presaghi, Garito, 2018;Beebe, Lachmann, 1998;Evans, Porter, 2009). Ko-regulacja odbywa się w czasie rzeczywistym podczas wymian społecznych, kiedy to partnerzy interakcji wzajemnie się do siebie dopasowują -proces ten jest określany mianem regulacji interakcyjnej (zob. ...
Article
Human infants spend most of their time sleeping, but over the first few years of life their sleep becomes regulated to coincide more closely with adult sleep (Galland et al., 2012; Paavonen et al., 2020). Evidence shows that co-sleeping played a role in the evolution of infant sleep regulation, as it is part of an ancient behavioral complex representing the biopsychosocial microenvironment in which human infants co-evolved with their mothers through millions of years of human history (Ball, 2003; McKenna 1986, 1990). This paper is a conceptual, interdisciplinary, integration of the literature on mother-infant co-sleeping and other mother-infant co-regulatory processes from an evolutionary (biological) perspective, using complexity science. Viewing the mother-infant dyad as a “complex adaptive system” (CAS) shows how the CAS fits assumptions of regulatory processes and reveals the role of the CAS in the ontogeny of mother-infant co-regulation of physiological (thermoregulation, breathing, circadian rhythm coordination, nighttime synchrony, and heart rate variability) and socioemotional (attachment and cortisol activity) development.
Code
Full-text available
This coding system is based on a view of communication as a creative relational process, rather than an exchange of discrete packages of information between individuals. In this perspective, information (meaning) is viewed as emerging spontaneously between continuously active communication partners. Communication that is both continuously coordinated and creative is coregulated ("a continuous unfolding of individual action that is susceptible to being continuously modified by the continuously changing actions of the partner" ). Creativity is a fundamental aspect of interpersonal communication, occurring when an innovation enters into communication. The Relational Coding system aims at assessing patterns of communication and focuses on the communication system as a whole. It is applicable to any type of dyadic communication (e.g., parent-child, therapist-client, husband-wife). In addition, it can be adapted for use with triads or larger groups (e.g., teacher-classroom, performer-audience).
Chapter
Weekly observations of thirteen mother-infant dyads, from one to five months of age, were used to examine whether the type and timing of maternal changes in infant postural position were related to infant gaze, facial expression and reaching. Results show that mothers changed the infant's position more frequently when the infant was gazing away from her compared to when the infant was gazing at her, and when the infant was facially either neutral or positive. When the infants gazed away, mothers alternated their position between sitting upright while facing her and upright while facing in the direction of the infant's gaze. Some mother's choice of postural position matched the infant's developmental changes in gaze, while other mothers lagged behind. These mismatches of gaze and postural position continued until the developmental onset of reaching. Social-developmental processes are discussed with respect to the role of postural and sensorimotor factors using a dynamic systems perspective.
Chapter
The point of departure of this chapter is the inseparability of perception and action. This coupling is approached from a developmental perspective. In order to lay a basis for this coupling, the concepts of affordances, laws of control, and the role of constraints on the development of coordination are discussed. The chapter continues with a search for empirical evidence from a natural physical perspective with respect to the development of reaching and posture. Informational (e.g., object size, time-to-contact information) and physical constraints (e.g., direction of the gravity force) are discussed. Further it is emphasized that postural stability is conditional for the coordination of reaching, catching and grasping.
Article
Just as each person develops from infancy to adulthood, all interpersonal relationships have a life history that encompasses the changes in how people communicate with each other. This book is about how a relationship transforms itself from one pattern of communication to another. The authors present a unique research method called 'relational-historical research', based on advances in dynamic systems theory in developmental psychology, and qualitative methods in life history research. it rests on three premises: That the developing relationship (not the individual) is the unit of analysis; that change emerges from, but is not entirely constrained by, the patterns of the past; and that the developmental process is best revealed by making frequent observations within a particular case before, during, and after a key developmental transition. Looking specifically at the mother–infant relationship, this is a compelling piece of research that will appeal to an international audience of intellectuals and practitioners. © Alan Fogel, Andrea Garvey, Hui-Chin Hsu, Delisa West-Stroming 2006 and Cambridge University Press, 2009.