ArticlePDF Available

Zoo Biology

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

High among-individual variation in mating success often causes problems in conservation breeding programs. This is also the case for critically endangered European mink and may jeopardize the long-term maintenance of the species' genetic diversity under the European mink EEP Program. In this study, breeding success of wild and captive born European minks at Tallinn Zoological Garden are compared, and the mating behavior of the males is analyzed. Results show that wild born males successfully mate significantly more often than captive born males (89% and 35%, respectively). On the basis of an extensive record of mating attempts, both male aggressiveness and passivity are identified as primary causes of the observed mating failures. All other potential determinants have only a minor role. Mating success as well as a male's aggressiveness and passivity are shown to depend more strongly on the male than the female partner. We did not find any evidence that the behavior of an individual is dependent on the identity of its partner. We suggest that aggressiveness and passivity are two expressions of abnormal behavior brought about by growing up in captivity: the same individuals are likely to display both aggressive and passive behavior. The results point to the need to study and modify maintenance conditions and management procedures of mink to reduce the negative impact of the captive environment on the long-term goals of the program. Zoo Biol. XX:XX–XX, 2013. © 2013 Wiley Periodicals Inc.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
The Causes of the Low Breeding Success of
European Mink (Mustela lutreola) in Captivity
Kairi Kiik,
1
* Tiit Maran,
2,3
Astrid Nagl,
4
Kadri Ashford,
2
and Toomas Tammaru
1
1
Department of Zoology, Institute of Ecology and Earth Sciences, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
2
Species Conservation Lab, Tallinn Zoological Gardens, Tallinn, Estonia
3
Institute of Mathematics and Natural Sciences, Tallinn University, Tallinn, Estonia
4
Department of Biomedical Sciences and Biochemistry, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, Austria
High amongindividual variation in mating success often causes problems in conservation breeding programs. This is also the
case for critically endangered European mink and may jeopardize the longterm maintenance of the speciesgenetic diversity
under the European mink EEP Program. In this study, breeding success of wild and captive born European minks at Tallinn
Zoological Garden are compared, and the mating behavior of the males is analyzed. Results show that wild born males
successfully mate signicantly more often than captive born males (89% and 35%, respectively). On the basis of an extensive
record of mating attempts, both male aggressiveness and passivity are identied as primary causes of the observed mating
failures. All other potential determinants have only a minor role. Mating success as well as a males aggressiveness and
passivity are shown to depend more strongly on the male than the female partner. We did not nd any evidence that the behavior
of an individual is dependent on the identity of its partner. We suggest that aggressiveness and passivity are two expressions of
abnormal behavior brought about by growing up in captivity: the same individuals are likely to display both aggressive and
passive behavior. The results point to the need to study and modify maintenance conditions and management procedures of
mink to reduce the negative impact of the captive environment on the longterm goals of the program. Zoo Biol. XX:XXXX,
2013. © 2013 Wiley Periodicals Inc.
Keywords: ex situ; conservation; behavior; aggression; mustelids
INTRODUCTION
A biodiversity crisis is underway. According to recent
reports, approximately 21% of extant mammal species are
classied as threatened [IUCN, 2012; www.iucnredlist.org].
Despite this gloomy picture, recent analyses show that
conservation efforts can and do make a difference. According
to Hoffmanns [2010] calculation, in situ conservation
efforts have played a role in the case of 7% of species (68
of 928; for birds 19882008, mammals 19962008, and
amphibians 19802004) whose threat level has been recently
reduced [Hoffmann et al., 2010]. Sometimes efforts in situ
alone are not sufcient to secure the survival of an
endangered species [Witzenberger and Hochkirch, 2011].
Here, ex situ conservation breeding can play an important
role and has proven to be effective [Seddon, 2010; Conde
et al., 2011]. A number of cases exemplify the role
conservation breeding can have in securing the survival of
species [Pereladova et al., 1999; Alcaide et al., 2010; Biggins
et al., 2011; Lindsey et al., 2011; ZafarUl et al., 2011].
Managed captive populations act also as survival insurance
for endangered species. Even after ultimate extinction in the
wild, properly managed ex situ populations provide an
opportunity to restore wild populations to existing empty
habitats [Conde et al., 2011].
A prerequisite of success in conservation breeding is
that all the animals of reproductive age can be used for
breeding when needed [Price, 1999]. However, this is not
always the case. Captive breeding programs are often plagued
by a mortality rate that is too high or irregular breeding
[Wolf et al., 2000; Dalerum et al., 2006; Hawkins and
Battaglia, 2009; Peng et al., 2009], supposedly resulting from
*Correspondence to: Kairi Kiik, Institute of Ecology and Earth Sciences, University
of Tartu, Vanemuise 46, 51014 Tartu, Estonia. Email: svetsjeb@ut.ee
Received 25 August 2012; Revised 15 November 2012; Accepted 04 January
2013
DOI 10.1002/zoo.21062
Published onlinexx xx xxxx in WileyOnline Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).
© 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Zoo Biology 999 : 17 (2013)
insufcient knowledge of speciesspecic behavioral and
physiological requirements, or insufcient attention to such
requirements [Zeoli et al., 2008]. Several studies have
reported that abnormal mating behavior such as aggressive/
passive behavior toward potential mating partner is to be
blamed for poor breeding success [Wolf et al., 2000; Zhang
et al., 2004; MacKinnon et al., 2008]. Such abnormal
behavior has been reported to be caused by too small and/or
too homogeneous furnishing of the enclosure, presence of
natural enemies or humans, or social stress caused by the
physical presence of too many conspecics [Price, 1999;
Peng et al., 2007].
The European mink (Mustela lutreola) is a critically
endangered species according to the IUCN Redlist 2012
[IUCN, 2012; www.iucnredlist.org] assessment and one of
the most endangered mammals in Europe [Temple and
Terry, 2007]. Indeed, the species has disappeared from most
parts of its former range. The main factors contributing to this
extinction are habitat loss, overhunting, and the invasion of a
dominant competitor, the American mink [Maran and
Henttonen, 1995; Maran, 2003, 2007; Maran et al., 2004].
In 1992, the European Endangered Species Program
(EEP) for the European mink was launched by the European
Association of Zoos and Aquaria (EAZA). The aim of this
program is to maintainin European zoos and other breeding
facilities85% of the original heterozygosity of the founding
individuals for 50 years. Currently, there are 218 mink in
captivity under the EEP program. Nearly half of the captive
populationof European Mink is heldat TallinnZoo,Estonia,the
population is based on 22 founders. Currently (as of
December 31, 2011) the size of the population is n¼110
individuals, genetic diversity (GD) retained is 93%, generation
time1.62yearsandeffectivepopulationsize65.6individuals.In
additionto the preservationof the geneticdiversityofthespecies
in captivity,the EEP Programalso serves as a source for a mink
reintroductionprojectsin Estonia(on the islandsof Hiiumaaand
Saaremaa) as well as in Germany [Maran et al., 2009].
In the framework of the European minkEEP program, all
known breeding events in the captive population have been
recorded. Furthermore, since 2004, thebehavior of mink during
mating attempts has been recorded at Tallinn Zoological
Garden. These two sets of data provide us a unique opportunity
to look into the effect of mating behavior on the breeding
success in captivity. In this manner, we are forming an initial
insight into the role of abnormal mating behavior in breeding
failures of the European mink. In particular, our study aims to
answer the following question: Which traits of the male and the
female (such as origin, sex and partner identity, as well as age,
number of mating attempts in 1 year) are the main determinants
of mating success?
MATERIALS/METHODS
Captive Population and Its Management
At Tallinn Zoo, all animals are housed individually in
20animal modules,aligned in two rows with 10 enclosures
(2 m 2m4 m) in each. The rows are separated by a
service corridor. The enclosures are separated from each
other by double welded, wire mesh walls. Lower parts of the
wall are covered by plywood to reduce visual contact between
the animals. The enclosures are cleaned and the animals are
fed daily.
The Effect of Origin on Breeding Success
The effect of mink origin (wild or captive) on breeding
success was analyzed on the basis of the pedigree data being
accumulated in the SPARKS database (www.isis.org). From
the 1980s until 2003, the main goal of the program had been
to increase the size of the captive population and therefore
every animal was involved in the breeding program.
Therefore we assumed that any animal which left no
offspring was an unsuccessful breeder. Breeding records
and the data about the origin of bred mink until 2003 (both
wild and captive born) were used in the analyses.
Mating Trials
A detailed analysis of the determinants of mating
success could be conducted on the basis of breeding logs
which had been kept at the Tallinn Zoological Garden since
2004. By that time, all animals were captive born. From 2004
to 2010, all mating attempts were kept track of and classied
as successful or not. Mating attempt was classied successful
if copulation was observed. Also, cases of aggressive or
passive behavior were recorded. Aggressive behavior was
dened following to Poole [1966], that is, a study on the
related European polecat. A male was considered passive if
he did not show any interest towards the female (started to
forage, explored the area, receded into nestbox and stayed
there). Additionally, the logs contained basic information
(such as age) concerning every individual involved.
A mating trial was initiated when a female reached
oestrus, and a designated male partner was placed in her cage.
The selection of male partners was based on the analyses
conducted with the PM2000 software [Pollak et al., 2002]. An
observer monitored the couple until copulation occurred.
Subsequently, the animals were left to stay together for a few
days. The trials with no copulation lasted one hour after
which the animals were separated. In the cases of aggression,
the trials were terminated and the animals were immediately
separated to avoid injuries to the female. In such trials, the
male was scored as having behaved aggressively in the
particular mating attempt. If the attack was not very serious
and thus the female was not particularly disturbed, the trial
was continued for the predetermined time (1 hr), but the male
was still scored as having behaved aggressively. The animals
were also separated after an hour of observation if the male
was acting passively, that is, if the male did not pay any
attention to the female. In these cases, the male was scored as
passive for that breeding attempt.
To nd out the effect of aggressive or passive behavior
of the male on mating success, the latter (as a binary variable:
2Kiik et al.
Zoo Biology
copulation observed or not) was analyzed as dependent on
aggression indexor passivity indexof the male (see
Table 1 for denitions of variables). In the analyses, each
mating attempt was treated as one observation. Several
additional independent variables were included in the model
(Table 1). For example, it is reasonable to assume that the
previous mating experience of the female (having encoun-
tered an aggressive/passive male earlier or not) can affect the
outcome of an ongoing mating attempt, assuming that a
females behavior has some impact on mating success at all.
In order to check whether aggressive males tend to act
normally or rather passively in those mating attempts in
which aggressive behavior does not occur, the analysis was
repeated on a reduced data matrix. In particular, the trials in
which the male was acting aggressively were excluded. The
relationship between mating success or passive behavior
(both binary variables) and the malesaggression index
was analyzed in a model with the same set of additional
independent variables (Table 1). An analogous analysis was
conducted to study the behavior of passive males during their
nonpassive breeding attempts. That is, the relationship
between mating success or aggressive behavior and the
malespassivity indexwas studied using the data matrix
with all the passive attempts omitted.
In all analyses with a binary dependent variable
(successful/unsuccessful; aggressive/nonaggressive, etc.)
mixed generalized linear models were applied. Statistical
analyses were performed with SAS/STAT 9.2 (SAS Institute,
Inc., Cary, NC) software using the procedure glimmix. Male
and female identities were used as random factors. Degrees of
freedom were estimated using the KenwardRoger method
[Littell et al., 2002] to avoid overestimationdue to repeated
measurements on particular individuals.
RESULTS
The Effect of Origin on Breeding Success
There was a signicant (x
2
¼38.3; df ¼1; P<0.001)
association between the origin (wild vs. captive born) of male
European minks: 23 out of 201 captive born males had
successfully bred at least once in their lifetime (11%), while
wild born males respective numbers were 13 out of 20 (65%;
Fig. 1). For females, no such association was found
(x2¼0.02; df ¼1; P¼0.89): 100 out of 229 captive born
females (44%) bred at least once successfully. The
corresponding gures for wild born females were 7 and 15
(53%), respectively (Fig. 2).
Aggressive and Passive Behavior in Mating
Attempt
Our nal matrix for 20042010 contained data on 579
mating events, 147 (25%) of which were classied as
successful. There were data on mating attempts of 96 males
TABLE 1. Independent variables used in the analyses to explain the variation in mating success
Effect Level of variation Mean SD Min Max
Male
Aggression index
a
i 0.26 0.35 0 1
Passivity index
a
i 0.35 0.38 0 1
Age
b
ma 2.02 1.3 1 9
Attempt
c
ma 2.72 2.04 1 13
Female
Experienced passivity
d
i 0.27 0.27 0 1
Experienced aggressiveness
d
i 0.13 0.19 0 1
Age
b
ma 2.9 1.8 1 9
Attempt
c
ma 2.68 1.86 1 9
Earlier experienced aggressiveness
e
ma 0.4 0 1
Earlier experienced passivity
e
ma 0.6 0 1
Some variables varied at the level of particular mating attempts (ma) while some had values attached to particular individuals(i).
a
Aggression
index (mean aggressiveness) of each particular male was presented as the share of the males aggressive attempts from the number of all his
mating attempts in the data base (0, never aggressive; 1, always aggressive). Passivity index (mean male passiveness) was calculated
analogously.
b
The age of the animal at the moment of the mating attempt (years).
c
Order number of the mating attempt. For each male the
numeration starts from one every breeding season and increases by one with every mating attempt. An analogous index for the female was
calculated similarly.
d
Mean female experienced aggressiveness was presented as the share of mating attempts in the data set in which the male
partner had been aggressive. Mean experienced passivity was calculated analogously.
e
Earlier experienced aggressiveness is a binary variable
telling if the female partner has, or has not, previous experience with an aggressive mating attempt. Earlier experienced passivity has an
analogous meaning.
Fig. 1. Wild and captiveborn male European minkswith successful
breedings at least once in their lifetime.
Abnormal Mating Behaviour in European Mink 3
Zoo Biology
and 84 females. Males were recorded to be aggressive on 103
occasions and passive during 203 mating events. Of all failed
mating attempts (432), 24% were judged to have been
unsuccessful because of aggressive behavior of the male
partner, whereas in 47% the cause was passive behavior of the
male, that is, a failure to be interested in the female. Only
four aggressivetrials resulted in successful mating. In total67%
of studied males failed in every mating attempt in their life (64
of 96).
As judged on the basis of estimates of among individual
variance, and respective standard errors, mating success, as
well as the occurrence of both aggressive and passive
behavior, depended remarkably more on the male than the
female participant of the mating event (Table 2). No evidence
was found that the behavior of an individual was dependent
upon the identity of its partner: the female by male interaction
term was estimated to be zero for mating success,
aggressiveness and passiveness in analyses analogous to
those in Table 2.
To study the determinants of mating success (as a
binary variable, one value for each mating attempt),
generalized linear models with 10 independent factors
(Table 1) were tted rst. The model was subsequently
simplied in a stepwise manner so that only statistically
signicant effects remained (Table 3). Indeed, both the
passiveness and aggressiveness indices of the male were the
primary determinants of mating success, whereas similar
indices calculated for the female partner had a weaker though
still a statistically signicant effect.
However, our analyses described above contained a
certain element of potential logical circularity. Indeed, as
most mating failed because of a male having been either
aggressive or passive, one may argue that a relationship
between mating success and abnormal male behavior might
partly result from the actual denition of successful mating.
To overcome this problem, we rescored male aggressiveness
and passiveness as binary variables (e.g., the value one of
aggressiveness was assigned to a male if he had been
observed to behave aggressively at least once). Thereafter, we
omitted these particular mating events which had served as
the basis of such judgment (i.e., mating events in which a
male had rst been observed to be aggressive or passive) from
the database, and repeated the analysis (Table 3). The results
were not qualitatively different. Male aggressiveness and
passiveness retained its statistically signicant effect on
mating success (Table 4).
Additionally, we tested whether a male which tends to
be aggressive is likely to behave passively on those occasions
it happens not to be aggressive, and vice versa. For this
purpose we created a subset of data with all aggressive
matings excluded, and found that both mating success
(F
1,137.3
¼12.97; P¼0.0004, in an analysis analogous that in
Table 3) and passiveness (F
1,156.4
¼9.03; P¼0.0031) were,
indeed, higher in the more aggressive males. Analogously, in
a data subset with passive mating occasions excluded, the
passiveness score of a male negatively affected mating
success (F
1,79.22
¼28.91; P<0.0001) and positively affected
the probability to behave aggressively (F
1,103.3
¼8.08;
P¼0.005).
DISCUSSION
Mating success appears to be critical in a European
mink captive breeding program. For example, in 2010, only
40% of attempted males copulated with a female and in 2009,
the percentage was 43. This study shows that breeding
success is positively autocorrelated among breeding seasons,
so a high proportion (67%) of males fail to pass their genes on
to succeeding generations (64 of 96 males studied from 2004
to 2010). An inevitable outcome is a reduced genetic diversity
in the population. The evidence accumulated from 1980 to
2003, when the European mink population at Tallinn
Zoological Garden consisted of both captive born and wild
born individuals, shows convincingly that mating failures
were primarily attributable to captive born males. This
suggests that the origin of the animals, rather than immediate
environmental conditions during breeding attempts, is
decisive in the minksbreeding performance. Indeed, in
the related blackfooted ferret (Mustela nigripes), it has been
found that the behavior of the animals changes considerably
between the rst and all the following generations in captivity
[Biggins, 2000]. Based on literature and our results, it
appears reasonable to assume that growing up in a captive
Fig. 2. Wild and captive born female European minks with
successful breedings at least once in their lifetime.
TABLE 2. Estimates of the effects of individual males (SE)
and females on the outcome of each mating attempt (a binary
variable: successful or not) as provided by generalized linear
model with male and female identity as random factors
Dependent variable Estimate of variance SE
Mating success Female 0.202 0.207
Male 2.01 0.51
Aggressiveness Female 0 Not estimable
Male 0.75 0.34
Passivity Female 0.056 0.11
Male 1.61 0.41
There were no xed factors in the model.
4Kiik et al.
Zoo Biology
environment inuences behavioral patterns in European
minks and these changes in behavior are the likely reason for
a low breeding success. However, as a number of captive
born mink bred successfully, it is obvious that the origin alone
is not the only determinant of (un)successful mating, but it is
also a function of unknown factor(s) that the mink experience
in captivity.
Detailed behavioral observations conducted from 2004
to 2010 allowed us to identify that, indeed, passivity and
aggression in captive born males were the main reasons for
failure in European mink breeding: (1) 24% of all failed
mating attempts were judged as unsuccessful because of
aggressive behavior in the male partner; (2) 47% of all mating
attempts failed because of the males passive behavior (i.e., a
failure to be interested in the female). Aggressive or passive
behavior in the male described far more variation in the
outcome of mating events than did any other explanatory
variables considered (e.g., age, previous experience of the
femalesee Table 1). In addition, our results suggest that
passivity and aggression are two alternative expressions of
the same phenomenon: the malesbehavioral inability to
mate. The males with a record of aggressive behavior were
also more likely to behave passively in nonaggressive trials
and vice versa. Similar to our ndings, mating failures have
also been identied as the cause of captive breeding problems
in blackfooted ferrets [Wolf et al., 2000]. Indeed, in the case
of blackfooted ferrets, the mating failure is, in addition to
physiological disorders, also attributable to abnormal
behavior. Males are either incapable of taking the proper
mating position or they are aggressive toward females.
Our results show that the outcome of a mating attempt
depends largely on the male partner. The decisive role of male
behavior in determining mating success has also been
reported for southern lesser galagos (Galago moholi;
Lipschitz et al., 2001] and for Giant pandas [Zhang
et al., 2004]. However, some other studies have shown the
oppositethe more important role of female behavior in
dening mating success. For instance, Poole [1966] reports
that the male European polecat (Mustela putorius)is
aggressive towards a female only if the latter rejects her
partner [Poole, 1966]. This does not hold for European mink,
as males have repeatedly been observed to be aggressive
towards female partner which is clearly interested in mating.
Furthermore, our data do not allow us to conclude that the
behavior of a male is altered with different female partners.
One of our hypotheses is that social stress [Blanchard
et al., 2001] is a factor which causes the abnormal behavior of
European mink raised in captivity. Indeed, the European
mink is known to lead a solitary life style in the wild
[Youngman, 1990]. However, in captivity, mink are usually
accommodated in enclosures close to each other so that
olfactory, visual and audio signals of the close presence of
conspecic reach them all the time. This might cause
stress resulting in distorted mating behavior. Such negative
effects of social stress resulting in low mating success
has been reported in captive wolverines, Gulo gulo [Dalerum
et al., 2006], black rhinoceroses, Diceros bicornis [Carlstead
et al., 1999], and various small felids [Mellen, 1991].
Besides creating social stress, the captive conditions
may also inuence an animals behavior through the effects
of size and environmental diversity of an enclosure. Indeed, it
TABLE 3. Generalized linear model for mating success (as binary variable) in captive European minks
Effect
Full model Reduced model
ddf FPddf FP
Male
Aggression index 81 25.31 <0.0001 92.73 24.68 <0.0001
Passivity index 81.48 63.90 <0.0001 87.35 63.95 <0.0001
Age 207.8 0.06 0.81
Attempt 307.9 10.50 0.001 294.2 10.59 0.001
Female
Experienced aggressiveness 115.8 13.07 0.0004 86.35 15.83 0.0001
Experienced passivity 101.9 10.35 0.002 84.15 9.04 0.004
Age 138.9 3.04 0.08
Attempt 567 1.72 0.19 572 4.74 0.03
Earlier experienced aggressiveness 188.3 0.25 0.62
Earlier experienced passivity 270 1.35 0.25
The left column shows the full model, the right column is the reduced model containing statistically signicant effects only. Female and male
identities were included as random factors. See Table 1 for denitions of the variables, and descriptive statistics.
TABLE 4. Generalized linear model for mating success with
male aggressiveness and passivity scored as binary variables
Effect ddf FP
Aggressiveness (1/0) 82.97 9 0.0036
Passivity (1/0) 93.08 32.72 <0.0001
Experienced aggressiveness 453 15.54 <0.0001
Experienced passivity 453 18.47 <0.0001
Attempt (male) 453 8.06 0.0047
Attempt (female) 453 6.06 0.0142
Mating events having served as the basis of classifying males as
aggressive or passive were omitted to avoid logical circularity.
Abnormal Mating Behaviour in European Mink 5
Zoo Biology
has been found that increasing the size and environmental
enrichment of the cage positively affects the breeding success
of giant pandas by raising the proportion of breeding events
with normal mating behavior [Peng et al., 2007].
For European minks, no data are currently available to
study immediate environmental factors associated with
abnormal mating behavior, or those eliciting malesaggres-
sive and passive behavior during breeding attempts.
Nevertheless, a manipulative study is feasible here, as the
sizes of the enclosures and their environmental diversity can
be easily changed (e.g., we may change the enrichment and/or
size of an enclosure and test whether or not there are any
changes with relation to breeding behavior). Knowing that
mating success primarily depends on male behavior and that
the same males are behaving aggressively and passively
allows us to properly identify target variables in such studies.
CONCLUSIONS
1. The negative effect of captive conditions on mating success in
European Mink was much more expressed in captive born
animals than in wild born animals: 89% of all captive born
males failed in breeding in contrast to 35% of wild born
males.
2. Abnormal mating behavior was the strongest predictor of
breeding failure in captivity. Twentyfour percent of all
recorded failures in mating were associated with aggres-
siveness and 47% with passiveness.
3. Mating success, as well as aggressiveness and passivity, were
all shown to depend much more strongly on the male than on
the female partner.
4. No evidence was found that the behavior of an individual was
dependent on the identity of its partner.
5. Passivity and aggressiveness are suggested to be two
alternative expressions of the malesbehavioral inability to
mate. The aggressive males tended to behave passively in
nonaggressive situations, and vice versa.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Hendrik Proosa for technical support in data
organisation and Bob Shelton for linguistic corrections. The
study was supported by Foundation LUTREOLA, EAZA
Carnivore Campaign, Thoiry Zoological Park, GaiaPark,
Zoological Society for Conservation of Species and
Populations, Estonian Environmental Board and targeted
nancing project SF0180122s08 and by the European Union
through the European Regional Development Fund (Center
of Excellence FIBIR).
REFERENCES
Alcaide M, Negro JJ, Serrano D, et al. 2010. Captive breeding and
reintroduction of the lesser kestrel Falco naumanni: a genetic analysis using
microsatellites. Conserv Genet 11:331338.
Biggins, DE. 2000. Predation on blackfooted ferrets (Mustela nigripes) and
Siberian polecats (Mustela eversmannii): conservation and evolutionary
implications [Ph.D. dissertation]. Fort Collins, CO: Colorado State
University.
Biggins DE, Livieri TM, Breck SW. 2011. Interface between blackfooted
ferret research and operational conservation. J Mammal 92:699704.
Blanchard RJ, McKittrick CR, Blanchard DC. 2001. Animal models of social
stress: effects on behavior and brain neurochemical systems. Physiol Behav
73:261271.
Carlstead K, Fraser J, Bennett C, Kleiman DG. 1999. Black rhinoceros
(Diceros bicornis) in US zoos: II. Behavior, breeding success, and
mortality in relation to housing facilities. Zoo Biol 18:3552.
Conde DA, Flesness N, Colchero F, Jones OR, Scheuerlein A. 2011. An
emerging role of zoos to conserve biodiversity. Science 331:13901391.
Dalerum F, Creel S, Hall SB. 2006. Behavioral and endocrine correlates
of reproductive failure in social aggregations of captive wolverines
(Gulo gulo). J Zool 269:527536.
Hawkins M, Battaglia A. 2009. Breeding behaviour of the platypus
(Ornithorhynchus anatinus) in captivity. Aust J Zool 57:283293.
Hoffmann M, HiltonTaylor C, Angulo A, et al. 2010. The impact
of conservation on the status of the worlds vertebrates. Science 330:
15031509.
IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2012. Version 2012.1. www.
iucnredlist.org.
Lindsey P, Tambling CJ, Brummer R, et al. 2011. Minimum prey and area
requirements of the vulnerable cheetah acinonyx jubatus: implications for
reintroduction and management of the species in South Africa. Oryx
45:587599.
Lipschitz DL, Galpin JS, Meyer D. 2001. Reproductive behavioral changes
during the ovarian cycle of lesser bushbabies (Galago moholi) in captivity.
Am J Primatol 55:101115.
Littell RC, Stroup WW, Freund RJ. 2002. SAS for linear models. Cary, NC:
SAS Institute.
MacKinnon K, Newberry R, Wielebnowski N, et al. 2008. Behavior and
corticosteroids predict breeding success in the clouded leopard felid taxon
advisory group (TAG). In: Association of Zoos & Aquariums. 2008 Annual
Report. p 3233.
Maran T, Henttonen H. 1995. Why is the European mink (Mustela lutreola)
disappearing?A review of the process and hypotheses. Ann Zoologici
Fennici 32:4754.
Maran T. 2003. European mink: setting of goal for conservation and the
Estonian case study. Galemys 15:111.
Maran T, Macdonald D, Kruuk H, Sidorovich V, Rozhnov V. 2004. The
continuing decline of the European mink (Mustela lutreola): evidence for
the intraguild aggression hypothesis. Symp Zool Soc Lond 71:297324.
Maran T. 2007. Conservation biology of the European mink, Mustela
lutreola (Linnaeus 1761): decline and causes of extinction [Tallinn
University Dissertations on Natural Sciences 15]. Tallinn: Tallinn
University Press.
Maran T, Põdra M, Põlma M, Macdonald DW. 2009. The survival of captive
born animals in restoration programmescase study of the endangered
European mink Mustela lutreola. Biol Conserv 142:16851692.
Mellen JD. 1991. Factors inuencing reproductive success in small captive
exotic felids (Felis spp)a multipleregression analysis. Zoo Biol 10:
95110.
Peng J, Jiang Z, Qin G, et al. 2007. Impact of activity space on the
reproductive behaviour of giant panda (Ailuropoda melanoleuca)in
captivity. Appl Anim Behav Sci 104:151161.
Peng J, Jiang Z, Lu X, et al. 2009. Mate preference and sexual selection
in giant panda, ailuropoda melanoleuca in captivity. Folia Zoologica
58:409415.
Pereladova OB, Sempere AJ, Soldatova NV, et al. 1999. Przewalskis
horseadaptation to semiwild life in desert conditions. Oryx 33:4758.
Pollak JP, Lacy RC, Ballou JD. 2002. Population management 2000,
version 1.163. Brookeld, IL: Chicago Zoological Society.
Poole TB. 1966. Aggressive play in polecats. Symp Zool Soc Lond 18:
2344.
Price EO. 1999. Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestica-
tion. Appl Anim Behav Sci 65:245271.
Seddon PJ. 2010. From reintroduction to assisted colonization: moving along
the conservation translocation spectrum. Restor Ecol 18:796802.
Temple HJ, Terry A. 2007. The status and distribution of European
mammals. Luxembourg: Ofce for Ofcial Publications of the European
Communities.
6Kiik et al.
Zoo Biology
Witzenberger KA, Hochkirch A. 2011. Ex situ conservation genetics: a
review of molecular studies on the genetic consequences of captive
breeding programmes for endangered animal species. Biodivers Conserv
20:18431861.
Wolf KN, Wildt DE, Vargas A, et al. 2000. Reproductive inefciency
in male blackfooted ferrets (Mustela nigripes). Zoo Biol 19:517
528.
ZafarUl Islam M, Ismail K, Boug A. 2011. Restoration of the endangered
Arabian Oryx Oryx leucoryx, Pallas 1766 in Saudi Arabia: lessons learnt
from the twenty years of reintroduction in arid fenced and unfenced
protected areas. Zool Middle East 125140.
Zeoli LF, Sayler RD, Wielgus R. 2008. Population viability analysis for
captive breeding and reintroduction of the endangered Columbia basin
pygmy rabbit. Anim Conserv 11:504512.
Zhang G, Swaisgood RR, Zhang H. 2004. Evaluation of behavioral factors
inuencing reproductive success and failure in captive giant pandas. Zoo
Biol 23:1531.
Youngman PM. 1990. Mustela lutreola. Mamm Species 362:13.
Abnormal Mating Behaviour in European Mink 7
Zoo Biology
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
A b s t r a c t . Giant panda is difficult to breed in captivity due to low oestrus and mating rate, high cub mortality and diseases. Thus, how to improve the mating success of giant pandas in captivity is an important conservation issue. After observation on eleven female and three male giant pandas from Beijing Zoo, Lanzhou Zoo, Chengdu Zoo and Giant Panda Breeding Center during their mating season in 2000–2001, we found that mate preference and sexual selection plays an important role in the mating success in giant panda. Both male and female pandas actively chose their mates. Successful copulations only occurred in those males and females that both showed high frequencies of courting behavior to opposite sex. Of those cases that only male or female showed one-sided high or low frequency of courting behaviour in the keeper-arranged panda pairs in random order, no copulation was observed. Only three out of twenty-four paired pandas successfully copulated. All three copulated female pandas in this study bore cubs. The results show that both male and female giant pandas have to be interested in each other in order to mate, but panda managers and researchers in China historically believed that females play no role in mate choice and that males are the choosy sex. This indicated that mating choice was one of the important factors resulted in unsuccessful copulation and failure of reproduction. We recommend that attention be paid to the mate preference and sexual selection in giant panda when breeding pandas in pens are paired for reproduction in the future.
Article
Full-text available
Success of captive breeding programs centers on consistent reproduction among captive animals, however, many individuals fail to reproduce even when presumably fully reproductive and healthy. Mate choice has been shown to affect multiple parameters of reproductive success including, but not limited to, mating success, offspring production, offspring survivorship, and offspring fecundity. We investigated the role of familiarity and preference on female Columbia Basin pygmy rabbit reproductive success as measured by litter production, size, and survivorship. Our findings suggest that both familiarity of mates and breeding with a preferred mate increased reproductive success in female pygmy rabbits. Rabbits that were neighbors prior to mating had at least three reproductive fitness measurements that were higher than animals who were not neighbors: litter production, kit survivorship to emergence, and survivorship to breeding age. Further, our study found that pairing rabbits with a preferred partner resulted in increased reproductive fitness by increasing litter production rates. Our results suggest that an integrated approach to breeding assignments in the pygmy rabbit captive breeding program, combining both genetic information and behavioral measures of preference, has the potential to increase the reproductive output of the program by as much as 211%.
Article
Full-text available
In South Africa there are efforts to manage reintroduced subpopulations of the Vulnerable cheetah Acinonyx jubatus in small reserves (10–1,000 km2) as a managed metapopulation. We estimated areas required to support cheetahs given varying prey densities, prey profiles and presence/absence of competing predators. A recent population and habitat viability assessment indicated that 20 subpopulations of 10 cheetahs or 10 subpopulations of 15 cheetahs are required to retain 90% of the heterozygosity of free-ranging cheetahs and to overcome stochastic events in the absence or presence of lions Panthera leo, respectively. We estimate that 203 ± SE 42 km2 (range 48–466 km2) is required to support 10 cheetahs in the absence of lions, whereas 703 ± SE 311 km2 (166–2,806 km2) is required to support 15 cheetahs given equal numbers of lions, and 2,424 ± SE 890 km2 (727–3,739 km2) given equal numbers of leopards Panthera pardus, spotted hyaenas Crocuta crocuta, wild dogs Lycaon pictus and lions. Existing subpopulations of cheetahs generally occur at densities higher than our mean predicted densities but usually within the range of predicted densities. The large area requirements of cheetahs have implications for the development of the managed metapopulation. Sourcing reintroduction sites of the sizes required to support recommended subpopulation sizes will be difficult. Consequently, innovative measures to increase the carrying capacity of reserves for cheetahs and/or to enlarge reserves will be required. Managers may be forced to stock cheetahs close to or beyond the carrying capacity of their reserves. Consequently, careful management of reintroduced subpopulations will be required to prevent declines in prey populations.
Article
The early declines in Central Europe and later in Finland took place before the spread of the American mink Mustela vison. On the other hand, the present decline of M. lutreola in Estonia seems to coincide well with the spread of M. vison. The early declines in Central Europe could have been caused by destruction of the natural river ecosystems, especially river banks. The European mink seems to be a much more specialized species than the American mink. Before the arrival of M. vison, the change in the preferred habitat, small sandy brooks, or the crash of the food source, could have been the underlying cause. With the arrival of the American mink, the European one loses even without the environmental change. -from Authors
Article
In Saudi Arabia, a conservation and restoration programme for Oryx Oryx leucoryx, Pallas, 1766 was started in 1989 by the Saudi Wildlife Commission (formerly the National Commission for Wildlife Conmservation and Development). Concurrent conservation programmes were launched for the protection of large areas within the former range of the Arabian Oryx, and captive breeding at the National Wildlife Research Center (NWRC). Together, these have enabled the restoration of wild self-sustaining populations of Arabian Oryx in Saudi Arabia using animals from the ‘World Herd’ to improve their genetic variability. The success of the oryx conservation programme is described here together with the constraints faced in the arid environments and the consequent lessons learnt. As rainfall has a strong influence on the presence of annual plants, it is the single most important factor in the production of grazing. Poor rainfall had a major detrimental impact on forage in the re-introduction sites from 1999 to 2008 and mortality of oryx was higher during this period. As oryx historically moved over great distances in response to rain, the fence around one site, the Mahazat as-Sayd Protected Area, prevents natural movements of animals and artificially concentrates ungulate populations into seasonally unfavourable habitat. We propose some management strategies to minimize mortalities in the wild, and assesses post-release monitoring and adoption of various estimation techniques to assess oryx populations in both the fenced and free-ranging areas. As poaching is still a problem, strict law enforcement and a public-awareness programme to inform citizens of the biological and historical significance of the Arabian Oryx is recommended.
Article
Environmental, genetic, and social factors associated with captive maintenance of small felids (Felis spp.) were systematically examined at eight different zoos to determine which of these factors most closely correlated with successful reproduction. Almost half of all the pairings examined failed to produce offspring. However, at least some representatives of the 20 species examined successfully reproduced, suggesting that failure to breed could not be solely attributed to species specific requirements. Multiple regression techniques revealed an association between number of litters produced and number of medical treatments, latitude range of wild counterparts, group size (all negatively correlated), and a positive correlation with husbandry style. These findings have direct relevance to how small cats are managed in captivity. Specifically, when small felids are maintained in groups larger than a pair (1.1), they are not likely to reproduce. Furthermore, a husbandry style in which keepers spent considerable time talking to, and interacting with, the cats under their care was more likely to result in offspring than one in which these interactions did not occur. These and other results generated suggestions for management techniques necessary to assure the continued survival of small cats in captivity.
Article
Breeding platypuses (Ornithorhynchus anatinus) in captivity had proved difficult until the last 10 years but with improvements in husbandry and a greater understanding of platypus biology there have been seven successes at two institutions since 1998. This study, using closed circuit video monitoring, took place between 2001 and 2007 at Taronga Zoo and covered three breeding successes with one female in 2002/03, 2004/05 and 2005/06. Results have revealed a consistency of behavioural patterns, which accurately predicted breeding events, mating, burrow construction, subsequent denning period and emergence of young. The female platypus had a receptive period of 46 days in the spring of each year, during which her behaviour changed from avoidance to initiating contact with the male. After mating she immediately commenced burrow preparation, ending with a 35-day intense period of collecting wet nesting material before retiring to the burrow to lay. The gestation period was estimated at 1521 days and incubation at less than 12 days. A suckling period of 114127 days followed, with the frequency of maternal burrow visits and length of stay decreasing as the juveniles grew. Juveniles emerged 1334 days after retirement. This study provides new and important information about breeding behaviour in this species and will contribute to future breeding success of platypus in captivity.
Article
We studied the impact of activity space on the reproductive behaviours of eleven healthy adult female and three adult male giant pandas with natural mating ability from the Beijing Zoo, the Lanzhou Zoo, the Chengdu Zoo and the Chengdu Giant Panda Breeding Center during their non-mating and mating season from November 1999 to May 2003. We contrasted the frequencies of reproductive behaviour and activities of the giant pandas that kept in different size of activity spaces during their estrous period and found that the frequencies of reproductive behaviours and other activities of the female and male giant pandas in large activity space were significantly higher than that of those when they were kept in small pens (P