ArticlePDF Available

Training the brain: Fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral remediation

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Putatively safe and effective for improving cognitive performance in both health and disease, products purported to train the brain appeal to consumers and healthcare practitioners. In an increasingly health-centered society, these applications constitute a burgeoning commercial market. Sparse evidence coupled with lack of scientific rigor, however, leaves claims concerning the impact and duration of such brain training largely unsubstantiated. On the other hand, at least some scientific findings seem to support the effectiveness and sustainability of training for higher brain functions such as attention and working memory. In the present paper we provide a tectonic integration and synthesis of cognitive training approaches. Specifically, we sketch the relative merits and shortcomings of these programs, which often appeal to parents who must choose between side-effect-laden medication and other less conventional options. Here we examine how neuroplasticity allows the healthy as well the impaired to benefit from cognitive training programs. We evaluate the evidence and consider whether brain training can be a stand-alone treatment or an adjunct to pharmacotherapy, outline promising future prospects, and highlight what training outcomes are plausible in line with available data. Future research would determine whether the field of brain training realizes its potential to revolutionize education and rehabilitation or withers away engulfed in controversy.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/copyright
Author's personal copy
Special Invited Review
Training the brain: Fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral remediation
Sheida Rabipour
a
, Amir Raz
a,b,
a
Department of Neurology & Neurosurgery, McGill University, Canada
b
Departments of Psychiatry & Psychology, McGill University, Canada
article info
Article history:
Accepted 13 February 2012
Available online 30 March 2012
Keywords:
Attention
Brain training
Cognitive remediation
Computerized training programs
Neuroplasticity
Working memory
abstract
Putatively safe and effective for improving cognitive performance in both health and disease, products
purported to train the brain appeal to consumers and healthcare practitioners. In an increasingly
health-centered society, these applications constitute a burgeoning commercial market. Sparse evidence
coupled with lack of scientific rigor, however, leaves claims concerning the impact and duration of such
brain training largely unsubstantiated. On the other hand, at least some scientific findings seem to sup-
port the effectiveness and sustainability of training for higher brain functions such as attention and work-
ing memory. In the present paper we provide a tectonic integration and synthesis of cognitive training
approaches. Specifically, we sketch the relative merits and shortcomings of these programs, which often
appeal to parents who must choose between side-effect-laden medication and other less conventional
options. Here we examine how neuroplasticity allows the healthy as well the impaired to benefit from
cognitive training programs. We evaluate the evidence and consider whether brain training can be a
stand-alone treatment or an adjunct to pharmacotherapy, outline promising future prospects, and high-
light what training outcomes are plausible in line with available data. Future research would determine
whether the field of brain training realizes its potential to revolutionize education and rehabilitation or
withers away engulfed in controversy.
Ó2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Increasingly ubiquitous, training programs foster the putative
promise of enhancing or rehabilitating behavior and brain func-
tion. This trend comprises a burgeoning market of products alleged
to enhance cognition, emotion, thought and action. Catering to
individuals of all ages, but targeting young children and the elderly
in particular, such programs claim to benefit healthy populations
as well as those diagnosed with specific disorders. Many commer-
cial programs take advantage of computerized training over the
Internet, offering the comfort and privacy of home-based brain
exercise. Appealing to a wide clientele – from the ambitious and
healthy to the desperate and sick – brain training targets parents
and professionals looking for an edge in a competitive society,
symptom relief, or a potential cure.
Broadly defined, brain training refers to the engagement in a
specific program or activity that aims to enhance a cognitive skill
or general cognitive ability as a result of repetition over a circum-
scribed timeframe. Such training can produce changes measured at
the behavioral as well as the neuroanatomical and functional lev-
els. Many forms of brain training appear to improve cognitive func-
tion and emotional control, particularly programs that exercise
attention (Rueda, Posner, and Rothbart, 2005). By practicing games
or tasks that require choosing between two competing responses,
the training of attention aims to strengthen the neural networks
underlying control processes (Raz & Buhle, 2006). A strong modu-
lator of cognition and affect, attention refers to the selective focus
on specific aspects of our environment or to the concentration on
specific mental thoughts and operations (Raz & Buhle, 2006). Sim-
ilar to attention training (AT), many programs target working
memory (WM), a system that mediates temporary information
storage, modification, and protection from interference (Bledowski,
Kaiser, & Rahm, 2010). Apart from attention and WM, studies sug-
gest that practicing sustained attention through meditation (Tang
& Posner, 2009), schooling (Diamond, Barnett, Thomas, & Munro,
2007), interaction with nature (Kaplan, 1995a), exercise (Kubesch
et al., 2009), and musical training (Kraus & Chandrasekaran,
2010) can also improve cognitive ability and emotional control.
The effects of such varied methods on cognitive ability and emo-
tional stability attest to the advantages of specific exercises.
0278-2626/$ - see front matter Ó2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.006
Abbreviations: AT, attention training; WM, working memory; FFW-L, Fast
ForWord-Language; HRT, habit-reversal training; IBMT, integrative body-mind
training; BBC, British Broadcasting Corporation; AKTIVA, Active Cognitive Stimu-
lation-Prevention in the Elderly; TS, Tourette’s Syndrome; COI, conflicts of interest.
Corresponding author. Address: Institute of Community and Family Psychiatry,
4333 Cote Ste. Catherine, Montreal, QC, H3W 1E4 Canada.
E-mail address: Amir.Raz@McGill.ca (A. Raz).
Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect
Brain and Cognition
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c
Author's personal copy
Brain training is especially relevant for developmental psycho-
pathology. This approach has potential to ameliorate undesired
symptoms of disorders such as attention deficit hyperactivity dis-
order (ADHD), a condition characterized by deficits in behavioral
inhibition associated with cognitive processes that mediate goal-
directed behaviors (Barkley, 1997). ADHD comprises a useful lens
through which researchers examine the effects of training. A spec-
trum disorder, ADHD contains various degrees of severity that in-
flict mild to severe impairments, many of which relate to
executive attention and may improve as a result of training (Illes
& Sahakian, 2011). Currently, primary treatments for developmen-
tal psychopathologies such as ADHD often involve psychotropic
medications, which sometimes show marginal effects. Even these
effects, however, attenuate over time and can generate a number
of unwanted side-effects. As a result, parents and clinicians are of-
ten reluctant to embrace drug-based therapy despite the scarcity of
safe and effective treatment alternatives. Recent allegations add
controversy to this dilemma by claiming that certain psychiatrists
may have surreptitious ties with drug companies, biasing the re-
search surrounding the production and distribution of medication
for youth (‘‘Credibility Crisis in Pediatric Psychiatry,’’ 2008). In light
of such limitations in pharmacological-based remedies, brain
training may represent an attractive adjunct to common pharma-
cological treatment.
The generalizability of brain training represents one of the ma-
jor claims-to-fame of publicly distributed programs. With scarce
data to support advertised claims, however, patrons of brain train-
ing often invest considerable resources pursuing programs that
promote unsupported, arguably unrealistic, outcomes. While stud-
ies of computerized AT and working memory training (WMT)
show, perhaps unsurprisingly, that trainees can improve signifi-
cantly on cognitive skills related to the intervention (Westerberg
et al., 2007), at least some findings suggest that training may gen-
eralize beyond task-specific skills and apply to untrained overarch-
ing abilities (Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, & Perrig, 2008; Jaeggi
et al., 2010). Reported improvements sometimes extend to in-
creased fluid intelligence, which refers to the ability to solve prob-
lems in novel situations (Buschkuehl & Jaeggi, 2010; Mackey, Hill,
Stone, & Bunge, 2010). Such transfer effects may result from over-
lapping neural networks in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), which
underlie both WM and fluid intelligence (Gray, Chabris, & Braver,
2003; Klingberg, 2010). Claims regarding the transfer of practiced
skills to other untrained cognitive domains are contentious, how-
ever, because the appearance of transfer may, in fact, result from
training-to-task (Snyder, 2011). Specifically, training programs
may obliquely tax the very abilities that researchers subsequently
test (Diamond, 2011). In addition, being the wild west of neuropsy-
chology, many studies report experimental results using inade-
quate controls, if any (Papp, Walsh, & Snyder, 2009). Rather than
generalizability, therefore, the improvements observed throughout
various programs may arise due to reasons such as training-to-
task.
Apparent from adequately controlled studies, brain training is a
groundbreaking approach with potential to transform the pano-
rama of non-pharmacological therapy. Despite the mounting prev-
alence of such products, the immense potential of such
interventions for various populations, and the increasing evidence
both supporting and disclaiming the effectiveness of training, no
other article, to our knowledge, has thoroughly amalgamated the
evidence surrounding cognitive programs and the populations that
may benefit from training. In the present article, we critically eval-
uate the validity of claims regarding various brain exercises and
cognitive remediation approaches. In an attempt to elucidate the
effectiveness of brain training, we examine the impact of these
programs in both healthy developing individuals and pathological
populations. We further investigate the potential use of such
training as an adjunct – or possible substitute – to current drug-
based therapies for children with psychopathologies. We conclude
by discussing specific conflicts that may hinder the advancement
of research in this field and outline plausible outcomes of brain
training with regards to factors that may alleviate or aggravate
undesirable childhood behavior.
2. Neural and behavioral basis of brain training
Attention plays a central role in social behavior and academic
performance. Due to brain plasticity, training can alter the neural
correlates of attention and improve attentional control. In this sec-
tion, we focus on a current, widely recognized model that subdi-
vides attention into three separate systems. We discuss the
function of these systems as well as their related neural networks,
and delineate how these systems control behavior throughout
development. Ultimately, we underline why attention is a suitable
faculty to train in both children and adults.
2.1. Neuroplasticity and training
Brain training thrives on the lure of neuroplasticity, a change in
neural structure and function in response to experience or environ-
mental stimulation (Shaw, Lanius, & Vandendoel, 1994). Research
suggests that both genetic and environmental factors impact the
development and physical structure of the brain (Lenroot & Giedd,
2008). Investigations of executive attention in children have
uncovered notable disparities associated with socio-economic sta-
tus, even when performance levels are comparable (Hackman &
Farah, 2009; Mezzacappa, 2004). Other studies report that severe
stress and maltreatment experienced early in life can severely im-
pact neuroanatomy, showing reduced volumes and attenuated
development of several neural structures (Teicher et al., 2003). Ta-
ken together, these findings indicate that the developing brain is
susceptible to change in response to environmental stimuli.
Evidence suggests that neuroplastic processes are present in the
adult brain. Repeated practice of skills required for a profession, for
example, appears to induce lasting changes within neural struc-
ture; London taxi drivers display larger gray matter volumes in
neural areas associated with spatial memory (Maguire, Woollett,
& Spiers, 2006; Maguire et al., 1998), professional typists undergo
greater development of neural regions related to the programming
of motor tasks (Cannonieri, Bonilha, Fernandes, Cendes, & Li, 2007),
and musicians appear to acquire increased cortical representations
of their digits (Elbert, Pantev, Wienbruch, Rockstroh, & Taub, 1995)
as well as enlarged motor, auditory, and visual-spatial regions (Ga-
ser & Schlaug, 2003). Even among the elderly, neuroplasticity con-
tinues to facilitate changes leading to improvement in cognitive
function (Calero & Navarro, 2007). These studies indicate that the
greatest changes occur through repeated practice of a skill over
an extended period of time, even when learned in adulthood. In
addition, extensive training or practicing a specific new skill may
modify neural structures and functions over a relatively short per-
iod of time; magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) reveals increases in
gray matter volumes of regions associated with the processing of
complex visual motion in young adults, following 3 months of
training on a juggling task (Draganski et al., 2004). Diffusion tensor
imaging, furthermore, indicates changes in white matter configu-
ration after just 4 weeks of juggling (Scholz, Klein, Behrens, &
Johansen-Berg, 2009). Another study reported changes in the gray
matter density of medical students, following 3 months of exten-
sive studying for a medical school exam (Draganski et al., 2006).
Recent studies show, moreover, that increases in gray matter vol-
ume may occur following as little as 1 week of training in a partic-
ular task (Driemeyer, Boyke, Gaser, B üchel, & May, 2008).
160 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
Functional and morphological changes in the adult brain may
therefore arise as a result of expertise in a field as well as mastering
novel skills.
2.2. Attention networks
Attention encompasses distinct neural processes that mature
independently, at different stages of life. Fundamental to cognitive
function, attention begins to develop during childhood (Posner &
Rothbart, 2007b) and contributes to self-regulation, the ability to
regulate our thoughts and actions (Karoly, 1993; Raz & Buhle,
2006; Rueda, Posner, et al., 2005). William James first proposed
that attention may contain multiple ‘‘varieties’’ (James, 1890),
deviating from long-held theories of attention as a unitary system.
A century later, Michael Posner further elaborated on this idea by
putting forth a theory in which attention consists of three highly
connected yet independent networks (Posner & Petersen, 1990).
This attention trilogy comprises the alerting, orienting, and execu-
tive attention systems (Posner & Rothbart, 2007a).
The alerting and orienting networks constitute the more prim-
itive components of attention; the alerting system denotes sus-
tained attention, vigilance, or alertness, and refers to response
readiness in preparation for an impending stimulus (Raz & Buhle,
2006). Sometimes considered the foundational form of attention,
the alerting system may continue developing well into adulthood
(Rueda, Fan, et al., 2004; Rueda, Posner, Rothbart, Davis-Stober,
2004). The orienting network, on the other hand, involves selecting
specific information from multiple sensory stimuli (Raz & Buhle,
2006). Believed to develop fully by the age of four, this network
mediates shifts of the sensory organs to bring objects of interest
into focus (Posner & Rothbart, 2007b).
Of the three attention networks, executive attention is most
pertinent to brain training. Also termed supervisory or selective
attention, executive attention mediates voluntary control and acti-
vates in situations requiring the monitoring and resolution of con-
flict between computations in separate neural areas (Raz & Buhle,
2006). These conflicts may include planning or decision-making,
error detection, execution of new or ill-acquired responses,
involvement in stressful conditions, regulation of thoughts and
feelings and overcoming of habitual actions. Executive attention
involves processes of self-regulation that include effortful control,
the ability to suppress a dominant response in favor of a subdomi-
nant response (Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000), as well as
inhibitory control, the termination of an ongoing response (Scha-
char, Tannock, & Logan, 1993). Accordingly, measures of this sys-
tem often involve conflict-related tasks such as the Stroop task,
which requires participants to name the ink color of a color-word
by suppressing the tendency to read the word itself (Stroop, 1935).
In addition, executive attention plays a role in emotional regula-
tion, the control of emotional responses based on actions of the self
or others (Raz & Buhle, 2006). Neural correlates of executive atten-
tion lie within the lateral PFC, the ACC, and the basal ganglia, and
draw on the dopaminergic system (Posner & Rothbart, 2007b).
The ACC itself is believed to have several functionalities. While the-
ories initially implicated the dorsal portion in cognitive conflict
and the ventral–rostral portion in emotional conflict (Bush, Luu,
& Posner, 2000), recent evidence suggests that the dorsal ACC
may integrate negative affect, pain, and cognitive control to facili-
tate appropriate action based on punishment-related information
(Shackman et al., 2011). Furthermore, genes that modulate the
dopaminergic system strongly influence executive attention and
also appear to associate with impulse-control disorders such as
ADHD (Fan, Fossella, Sommer, Wu, & Posner, 2003; Fossella et al.,
2002). With rapid development commencing at 4 years of age,
the executive attention network may not change significantly after
the age of seven (Rueda, Fan, et al., 2004; Rueda, Posner, et al.,
2004). This early development in executive attention appears to
have strong potential for environmental modification, including
targeted training (Rueda, Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, Pos-
ner, 2005). By developing executive attention, children learn to
regulate cognition and behavior, and gradually conform to societal
norms.
Executive attention strongly links to WM in situations requiring
attentional control and focus. Similar to attention, WM is a modu-
lar system and is recruited during processes such as reading and
language comprehension, learning, and reasoning (Baddeley,
1992; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Tsianos, Germanakos, Lekkas,
Mourlas, & Samaras, 2010). Originally hypothesized to mediate
executive attention, WM is now believed to draw upon this net-
work in order to maintain and prioritize temporary stores of infor-
mation (D’Esposito, 2007; Jarrold & Towse, 2006). Neuroimaging
studies in humans and nonhuman primates consistently correlate
the use of WM with activation patterns involved in executive func-
tion, a faculty that encompasses executive attention, including cir-
cuits within the lateral PFC (D’Esposito, 2007; Klingberg, 2010).
These findings suggest that WM and executive attention rely upon
one another’s functionality to control and monitor specific neural
processes.
2.3. Attention and brain training: transfer of behavioral control
Studies suggest that the attention networks exert differential
control over behavior, with specific networks contributing more
strongly at certain stages of life (Gupta & Kar, 2009; Rueda, Fan,
et al., 2004; Rueda, Ponsner, et al., 2004). Whereas the executive
attention network plays a critical role in many adult pursuits, the
orienting network most strongly dictates behavior at earlier devel-
opmental periods. In infants as young as 3 months of age, visual
and auditory distraction can temporarily dampen distress (Posner,
Rothbart, Sheese, & Voelker, 2011). This early reliance on the ori-
enting network may underlie the relative novelty of environmental
stimuli for infants and children and often provides refuge for care-
givers seeking to soothe a distressed baby by using a distracter.
Similar effects have also been reported in adults (Harman, Roth-
bart, & Posner, 1997). Whereas infants rely most strongly on the
orienting network to carry out attentional shifts, adults incorporate
the executive attention system in addition to the potent orienting
signal (Kanske, Heissler, Schönfelder, Bongers, & Wessa, 2011). Sci-
entists are unsure, however, when the precise shift from orienting
to executive control occurs. Studies report the presence of rudi-
mentary forms of control through executive attention in the form
of cautious approach to novel stimuli (Sheese, Rothbart, Posner,
White, & Fraundorf, 2008) and behavioral inhibition during conflict
at 40 months of age (Jones, Rothbart, & Posner, 2003). By three to
4 years of age, children recognize errors and begin to display phys-
ical control strategies to inhibit inappropriate responses, although
verbal self-regulation of behavior tends to manifest later in life
(Jones et al., 2003). The gradual development of executive control
in children foreshadows its eventual control over behavior,
although the stage for this transfer of control between the orient-
ing and executive attention networks remains elusive.
The overwhelming tendency of infants to attend to novel stim-
uli in their external environment may serve an evolutionary pur-
pose and simultaneously facilitate the development of executive
attention (Posner & Rothbart, 2011). Whereas children – and cer-
tainly infants – are not expected to make executive decisions in
daily life, adults require a strong command of executive function
to thrive in society. Dependence on the rapidly-maturing orienting
network during early life allows infants to explore their surround-
ings and become acquainted with their new environment. Activa-
tion of the orienting network may also play a key role in the
maturation of the executive network through their complementary
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 161
Author's personal copy
activation in response to presentation of novel stimuli (Posner
et al., 2011). In this fashion, caregiver interactions may promote
self-regulatory processing through regular activation of the execu-
tive network.
The control of emotion and behavior may occur through altered
planes of consciousness such as hypnosis. Scientists recognize that
children are considerably more hypnotizable than adults (Kohen &
Olness, 2011). This observation may arise from a natural tendency
to delegate control to the orienting network of attention, where an
external source – the child’s caregiver, for example – becomes the
source upon which children depend to regulate their behaviors and
emotions. Similarly, the hypnotic state promotes an external
source of control – this time, by the hypnotist. In such a way, the
hypnotic state may promote an increased reliance on the orienting
system for behavioral control, rather than the more commonly
used executive system (Posner & Rothbart, 2011). In adults, who
presumably have fully-developed attention systems, hypnosis af-
fords the opportunity to achieve different states of awareness, to
experience emotional realizations, and to perform neural computa-
tions that may otherwise have been difficult to achieve. Highly
hypnotizable adults, for example, demonstrate elimination of con-
flict-related interference on the Stroop task after receiving post-
hypnotic suggestion (Raz, 2004). Altered mental states may
therefore induce differential control over attentional networks
and may prove important for modulating behavior and emotion.
3. Origins and evolution of brain training
Whereas various cultural practices have influenced states of
attention for centuries, the implications of altered mental states
on cognitive function were sparsely documented before the late
20th century (Jevning, Wallace, & Beidebach, 1992). Studies in
the 1960s and 1970s on relaxation therapy and early forms of AT
were among the first research efforts leading to modern behavioral
modification paradigms (Douglas, Parry, Marton, & Garson, 1976;
Paul, 1969; Pressley, 1979). This era of research witnessed neuro-
psychological AT, which initially aimed to maximize the functional
independence and adjustment of individuals with brain damage
(Park & Ingles, 2001). The learning model of recovery (Stuss, Wino-
cur, & Robertson, 1999) triggered theories relating experience,
practice, and environment to the restoration of impaired capaci-
ties, substantiating the use of AT-like programs for cognitive reha-
bilitation. Following an early description concerning the
therapeutic value of self-monitoring and self-control (Kanfer,
1970), a vast literature has emerged regarding improving self-con-
trol in children, a population known to be ill-equipped at maintain-
ing composure in situations that may provoke inappropriate
behavior (Pressley, 1979). The potential to improve self-control
in children prompted scientists to explore these techniques as
non-pharmaceutical treatment alternatives for children with im-
pulse-control impairments. A program aiming to improve inhibi-
tory control in hyperactive children (Douglas et al., 1976)
included self-reinforcement strategies as well as verbalization
techniques. After teaching children to cope more effectively and
independently when faced with cognitive problems in social and
academic situations, the program facilitated significant improve-
ments in performance on standardized intelligence tests. Following
such reports, scientists began to appreciate the implications of
training behavioral control in children.
Research has increasingly focused on training the attention sys-
tems as a means of altering behavior. Specific emphasis on AT orig-
inated with Attention Process Training, a cognitive rehabilitation
method through which scientists trained individuals with neuro-
logical impairments to employ specific subtypes of attention
(Tamm et al., 2008). In one promising study, individuals suffering
from brain injury showed significant improvements in attentional
processing which lasted as long as 8 months following training
(Sohlberg & Mateer, 1987). In another study, patients with unilat-
eral neglect verbally regulated the sustained-attention network by
engaging in an analogous AT program (Robertson, Tegner, Tham,
Lo, & Nimmosmith, 1995) and displayed improvements in un-
trained tasks of sustained attention and neglect after the 5-h train-
ing period. These studies corroborate the use of targeted AT in
individuals with specific functional deficits in their attention net-
works. In the spirit of rehabilitation, Benedict et al. (1994) created
a 15-h program to remediate the attentional ability of schizo-
phrenic patients using tasks aimed at increasing information pro-
cessing capacity. Although the schizophrenic population
improved performance on trained tasks, observed enhancements
in attention remained below the attentional baseline of controls
(Benedict et al., 1994). A later study, however, reported that
schizophrenic individuals with comparable baseline measures as
controls improved in vigilance, distractibility, and psychiatric sta-
tus following 18 AT sessions (Medalia, Aluma, Tryon, & Merriam,
1998). Research on AT branched out to target a variety of atten-
tion-based disorders. For example, studies in autistic children re-
ported improved attentional capacity that generalized to
untrained attention tasks as a result of training on joint-attention
tasks (Whalen & Schreibman, 2003). A study examining the 8-h
Pay Attention! program (Kerns, Eso, & Thomson, 1999) found
improvements in the attentive abilities and academic efficiency
of children with ADHD. Research efforts have rapidly expanded
the implementation of AT and similar variants to restore specific
deficits in executive function and supplement these processes in
typically-developing individuals. One innovative attempt modified
a program used to train monkeys for space travel and created a
promising AT program for children. Following 5 days of training,
the study reported significant improvements in performance of
both 4- and 6-year-old participants on tests of attention and intel-
ligence (Rueda, Rothbart, et al., 2005). In addition, electrophysio-
logical recordings demonstrated maturation of neural activation
patterns associated with the executive attention network to more
adult-like signals, including increased amplitude of the electroen-
cephalographic (EEG) N2 component. The study additionally dem-
onstrated an association between stronger effortful control,
decreased extraversion, and the long allele of the DAT1 dopamine
transporter gene. Thus, training programs that target attention ap-
pear to produce measurable structural and functional changes in
the brain.
4. Culture, lifestyle, and brain training practices
Brain training has significantly impacted mainstream society.
From claims of improving the negative symptoms of psychopathol-
ogies and neurological impairments to assertions of significantly
boosting cognitive skills among the healthy, commercialized soft-
ware and interactive programs are increasingly capturing the
interest of parents, educators, students, and clinicians (see Tables
1 and 2). Tutoring services (e.g., (Kumon North America, 2011; Syl-
van Learning, 2011)) entice parents looking to enhance the aca-
demic success of their children. While some practices appear to
produce measurable improvements in their target population, oth-
ers lack scientific rigor behind their claims. Here, we discuss the
data surrounding select brain training programs and techniques
in randomized-controlled studies, unless otherwise indicated.
4.1. Computerized training
Computerized cognitive exercises are among the most popular
forms of brain training. In a controlled study assessing the effects
162 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
Table 1
A list of select commercial software aiming to enhance or rehabilitate specific cognitive skills in health and pathology.
Company (website) Product name Product type (as specified on website) Target population Scientifically published evaluations
BrainMaster Technologies, Inc.
(www.brainmaster.com/)
BrainMaster –
Neurofeedback
Systems
Neurofeedback training Individuals of all ages Martin and Johnson (2006)
Brain Train (braintrain.com/) Captain’s Log,
SoundSmart,
SmartDriver
Cognitive training Individuals of all ages with ADHD,
head injuries, Learning Disabilities,
schizophrenia, and other cognitive
impairments
Angelakis, Lubar, Stathopoulou, and Kounios, (2004) See
also: www.braintrain.com/main/
ivaplus_research_bibliography.htm www.braintrain.com/
main/cognitive_training_research.htm
TNT Reading
SmartMind 2
Cogmed (www.cogmed.com) Cogmed JM WM training for children and adults with attention
deficits, learning disorders, brain injury or stroke, and
adults experiencing ‘‘information overload’’ or the
natural effects of aging
Ages 4–6 See: www.cogmed.com/references
Cogmed RM Ages 7 and up
Cogmed QM Adults
Cognifit (www.cognifit.com) Individualized
Training
System
Cognitive training Individuals of all ages See: www.cognifit.com/science/scientific-validation
HAPPYneuron (www.happy-neuron.com/) Various games Exercising memory, attention, language, visual-spatial
and executive function skills
Individuals of all ages Croisile (2006)
Lumosity (www.lumosity.com/) Various games Exercising memory, attention, processing speed, and
problem-solving skills
Individuals of all ages Kesler, Lacayo, and Jo (2010)
MindHabits, Inc. (www.mindhabits.com/) MindHabits Exercises to decrease stress and improve confidence Individuals of all ages Dandeneau and Baldwin (2004)
Nintendo (www.brainage.com) BrainAge Training with math- and literature-related activities Individuals of all ages
Posit Science (www.positscience.com) Brain Fitness Auditory training Older adults See: www.positscience.com/science/proven-in-labs
InSight Visual training
Drive Sharp Cognitive training for driving skills
Scientific Learning Corporation
(www.scilearn.com)
Fast ForWord Language and reading training – adjuncts to classroom
material
Kindergarten – grade 12 students Merzenich et al. (1996) and Fey et al. (2010)
Reading
Assistant
Children and adults wishing to build
vocabulary, fluency and
comprehension.
Timocco (site.timocco.com) Growing with
Timocco
Training motor and cognitive skills Children with cerebral palsy, ADHD,
Developmental Coordination
Disorder, and Learning Disabilities
Unique Logic and Technology
(www.playattention.com)
Play Attention Attention training, memory training, cognitive skill
training, social skills training, motor skills training,
behavior shaping
Children with ADHD
Your Baby Can, LLC.
(www.yourbabycanread.com)
Your Baby Can
Read
Language training Infants
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 163
Author's personal copy
of computerized AT and computer-assisted instruction (Rabiner,
Murray, Skinner, & Malone, 2010), children with attentional diffi-
culties improved in measures of attention, with more severe
symptoms showing larger improvements. In another study using
games that activate all three attention networks, children with
ADHD improved in academic skills and decreased ADHD symp-
tomatology, compared to controls (Shalev, Tsal, & Mevorach,
2007).
While the effectiveness of many commercial brain training
products is questionable, evidence suggests that some programs
facilitate marked improvements in cognitive function. One product
repeatedly demonstrated to improve WM capacity in both children
and adults is Cogmed, which entails training on WM tasks for
5 days per week over a period of 5–6 weeks. Using this software,
children with ADHD as well as healthy adults appear to improve
in measures of WM as a result of training (Klingberg, 2007; Kling-
berg et al., 2005).
Another widely used program is the Fast ForWord-Language
(FFW-L) software (Scientific Learning Corporation, 2011), adver-
tised to increase brain fitness, accelerate learning, and improve
test scores for children (Semrud-Clikeman & Ellison, 2009). Exper-
iments without a proper control group included tasks of percep-
tual identification and phonetic element recognition (Merzenich
et al., 1996). After training for 19–28 sessions of 20 min over a per-
iod of 4 weeks, children with language-based learning impair-
ments significantly improved in auditory perception. Thousands
of public school districts in the United States incorporate the
FFW-L program into their curricula, and a large number of children
across North America use the product for both scholastic and ther-
apeutic purposes (Scientific Learning Corporation, 2011). Reports
published outside the peer-review scientific system suggest that
this program enables educational benefits following repeated
practice (Schultz Center for Teaching, 2009). A recent study, fur-
thermore, demonstrated that children taking part in either the
FFW-L program or a narrative-based language intervention im-
proved on time-related effects of narrative measure and non-word
repetition (Fey, Finestack, Gajewski, Popescu, & Lewine, 2010). De-
spite these results, the participants did not display any significant
inter-group variability in benefits, suggesting that the FFW-L pro-
gram scarcely holds superior benefits to conventional language
interventions (Fey et al., 2010). Hence, while FFW-L appears to
produce training-related gains in temporal processing of informa-
tion related to language development, the program hardly fosters
significant improvements in language and reading ability. Reports
show, however, that training with FFW-L alongside a coach in-
creased neural activation in regions associated with selective audi-
tory attention – including the ACC – in children with specific
language impairment during language listening tasks, compared
to typically developing children who received no training (Stevens,
Fanning, Coch, Sanders, & Neville, 2008). These findings also ex-
tended to typically developing children, although to a lesser ex-
tents. Studies therefore suggest that the FFW-L program
improves auditory perception of stimulus tones and may indirectly
exercise executive attention.
4.1.1. Neurofeedback training
Neurofeedback, also called EEG biofeedback, represents another
computerized technique that appears to hold advantages for spe-
cific populations. This method entails training individuals to ac-
tively control and change their neural activation patterns by
viewing the brainwaves they emit a few milliseconds after they oc-
cur (Angelakis et al., 2007; Hammond, 2006). Administered in re-
search and clinical endeavors since the 1960s, neurofeedback
converts EEG signals from specific cortical areas to visual or audi-
tory representations that participants receive and subsequently at-
tempt to regulate through training (Congedo, Lubar, & Joffe, 2004).
Table 2
A list of select programs aiming to enhance specific skills related to academic performance in children.
Program name (website) Description Scientifically published evaluations
Arrowsmith School
(www.arrowsmithschool.org)
Cognitive exercises to improve learning dysfunctions that impact learning and social skills
Brain Gym International
(www.braingym.org)
Specific set of movements aiming to improve concentration and focus, memory, reading, writing, math, test-taking, physical
coordination, relationships, self-responsibility, organization skills, attitude
Center for Applied Special
Technology (www.cast.org)
Universal Design for Learning program that includes principles for curriculum development to provide equal opportunities to learn
for all individuals
See: http://www.cast.org/research/index.html
Kumon Learning Centers
(www.kumon.com)
Math and reading enrichment programs for children
Strategic Learning Centre
(www.strategiclearning.ca)
One-on-one teaching center to help children with learning disability, ADHD, dyslexia become better and more confident learners
Sylvan Learning
(tutoring.sylvanlearning.com)
Tutoring in various academic subjects for children
Tools of the Mind
(www.toolsofthemind.org)
Preschool and kindergarten curricula to improve self-regulation and executive function See: http://www.mscd.edu/extendedcampus/
toolsofthemind/about/effectiveness.shtml
University City Children’s Center
(uccc.org)
Early care and education system for values & character development, psychodynamic development, early literacy development
164 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
More recently, neurofeedback systems leverage the increased spa-
tial precision of fMRI technology (deCharms, 2008).
Advertising to improve mental functioning and increase aware-
ness of brain states, neurofeedback companies (e.g., (BrainMaster
Technologies, 2009)) offer their products to individuals seeking
to sharpen their cognitive skills. Studies suggest that this tech-
nique may be beneficial for enhancing cognitive function in elderly
populations (Angelakis et al., 2007) and in improving symptoms
associated with epilepsy (Kotchoubey et al., 1999; Tan et al.,
2009), substance abuse (Sokhadze, Cannon, & Trudeau, 2008),
and a number of psychiatric conditions (Heinrich, Gevensleben, &
Strehl, 2007). Neurofeedback appears particularly promising for
individuals diagnosed with ADHD. After training with this technol-
ogy, children with ADHD appear to increase scores on tests of intel-
ligence and continuous performance, improve cooperation and
school work in the classroom and demonstrate better attentional
and behavioral control (Monastra, 2005). Such benefits reportedly
endure, in some cases, for several years following the intervention.
Similar reviews of randomized controlled trials in children with
ADHD (Fox, Tharp, & Fox, 2005; Monastra, 2005) report mixed
findings; while some children showed little or no training effects,
others demonstrated enhanced intelligence and significant
improvements in attention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. Studies
comparing neurofeedback to medication support such training as a
serious contender for non-pharmaceutical ADHD treatment. A 12-
week, controlled trial comparing neurofeedback with methylphe-
nidate treatment suggested that neurofeedback leads to similar
behavioral changes, with decreased parent and teacher reports of
ADHD-related symptoms in both groups (Fuchs, Birbaumer, Lut-
zenberger, Gruzelier, & Kaiser, 2003). Another study found no sig-
nificant difference in either treatment effects or clinical
improvement of individuals with ADHD after 20 sessions of neuro-
feedback training or use of stimulant medication (Rossiter & LaVa-
qe, 1995). The sustainability of neurofeedback effects, however,
remains questionable and may vary on a case-by-case basis. Simi-
lar to other forms of computerized training, long-term therapy
using neurofeedback technology may prove effective for improving
disease-related symptoms of developmental psychopathologies,
especially when administered at childhood. Studies therefore pro-
vide compelling evidence for the potential of such training as a
non-pharmacological treatment alternative for a variety of neuro-
logical disorders.
4.2. Applied attention as school-based interventions
Student attentiveness is essential to a positive and productive
classroom dynamic and plays a fundamental role in shaping scho-
lastic performance (Duncan et al., 2008). Assessments of learning
approaches reveal that child attentiveness is positively associated
with academic competence and achievement, as well as relations
with both teachers and peers (Li-Grining, Votruba-Drzal, Maldona-
do-Carreno, & Haas, 2010). Variability in attentiveness may ac-
count for differences in child learning speed or the amount of
information children can extract from an event (Ruff & Rothbart,
1996). Studies on children with ADHD, moreover, reveal comorbid-
ities between the disorder and a number of learning difficulties,
especially pertaining to reading ability (Carlson, Tamm, & Gaub,
1997; Willcutt, Pennington, Olson, Chhabildas, & Hulslander,
2005; Willcutt, Pennington, Olson, & DeFries, 2007). An uncon-
trolled European study in 3 cohorts of children with ADHD not only
supported the association between scholastic impairment and
ADHD symptomatology, but reported a 2- to 10-fold increase in
impairments of reading, writing, and mathematics in children with
symptoms related more strongly to inattention (Rodriguez et al.,
2007). Thus, abounding evidence presents attention as an integral
component in the academic success of children.
Theories surrounding school readiness stipulate that children
must attain social–emotional competencies by practicing effortful
control in order to grasp the lessons learned in both social and aca-
demic settings (Liew, 2011). With fundamental roles during social
interactions, effortful control and emotional regulation draw upon
executive function to exert attentional and inhibitory control and
help children develop inter-personal relationships (Kochanska
et al., 2000; Liew, 2011; Ruff & Rothbart, 1996). At the start of
grade school, children exhibiting strong effortful control are more
likely to express social competence and have fewer behavior prob-
lems, whereas those who struggle to control attention and behav-
ior tend to develop impaired relationships with teachers and peers,
and have greater risk of developing academic difficulties (Liew,
2011; McClelland et al., 2007). Studies further show that the man-
ifestation of effortful control and other self-regulatory skills in ele-
mentary-age children correlates with higher grades, and may
improve early mathematical and literacy prowess (Liew, 2011).
Likewise, individuals with attention-related disorders such as
ADHD are generally impaired in social, academic, familial, and
occupational areas of life (de Boo & Prins, 2007). As a result,
school-based programs often involve AT to bolster effortful control
and emotional regulation (Kring & Sloan, 2010), critical to success
among peers in academic and other environments involving inter-
personal relations.
School-based interventions may particularly aid children of
lower socio-economic status – a population with marked deficits
in control through executive function (Bierman, Nix, Greenberg,
Blair, Domitrovich, 2008; Hackman & Farah, 2009; Noble, McCand-
liss, & Farah, 2007; Stevens, Lauinger, & Neville, 2009). Programs
such as ‘‘Tools of the Mind’’ (Barnett et al., 2008; Diamond et al.,
2007) and the ‘‘Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies’’ curric-
ulum, tested in the absence of matched-controls (Kelly, Longbot-
tom, Potts, & Williamson, 2004), for example, appear to decrease
behavioral problems and improve emotional understanding, exec-
utive function, and academic performance in children with disad-
vantaged socio-economic backgrounds. Similarly, studies
assessing the school-based ‘‘Head Start Research-based Develop-
mentally Informed’’ curriculum (Bierman, Domitrovich, et al.,
2008) demonstrate enhanced academic skills as well as improved
behavioral and emotional control in children. With increased
awareness of the importance of attentional processes in academic
and social settings, researchers are beginning to pool more re-
sources into developing effective training programs in schools.
Such programs may particularly benefit disadvantaged children
and minimize their disparities in cognitive ability.
Acquiring proper social skills during childhood is essential for
shaping appropriate behavior and ensuring healthy development
(de Boo & Prins, 2007). Training children to exhibit effective social
skills appears to improve their social knowledge and assertiveness,
and may further generalize to the school setting (McBurnett &
Pfiffner, 2008). Social skill training may be particularly relevant
for children with impulse-control disorders such as ADHD, who of-
ten experience peer rejection and social isolation due to aggressive
behavior and lack of inhibitory control (DuPaul, McGoey, Eckert, &
VanBrakle, 2001; McBurnett & Pfiffner, 2008). Although individual
parameters such as extent of play and teaching strategies remain
widely debated in American school districts (Barnett et al., 2008),
social skills training appears to promote constructive peer interac-
tions among children.
4.3. Bilingualism
Contrary to the belief that bilingual education hinders cognitive
ability and dampens intelligence, proficiency in two languages ap-
pears to enhance the development of cognitive control systems
relating to attention. Bilinguals develop the capacity to indepen-
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 165
Author's personal copy
dently process two languages, which requires selection of the cor-
rect lexical representations of one language while suppressing the
representations of the other (Costa, Hernández, & Sebastián-Gallés,
2008). Although scientists scarcely understand the mechanisms
underlying this ability, bilingual individuals may employ the exec-
utive attention system to suppress an unwanted linguistic re-
sponse much like inhibition during the Stroop task (Bialystok,
2010; Green, 1998). This process enables control over competing
neural networks and communication in the intended language,
thereby promote development of the underlying neural systems.
Indeed, research reveals that bilingual individuals perform better
at non-verbal problem solving tasks requiring inhibition of irrele-
vant responses and the formation of new conceptual representa-
tions compared to monolinguals (Bialystok & Martin, 2004).
Bilingual individuals, moreover, display quicker response times in
attention-related tasks – including trials involving conflict resolu-
tion – as well as more efficient use of their alerting and executive
attention networks (Costa et al., 2008). As early as childhood, bil-
inguals appear to perform better on non-linguistic tasks requiring
attentional control (Bialystok & Majumder, 1998) and develop the
ability to exert selective attentional control earlier than monoling-
uals (Bialystok, 1999). Such advantages of bilingualism grow
increasingly powerful at old age, with research indicating that el-
derly bilinguals have significantly greater inhibitory control com-
pared to monolinguals of the same age group (Bialystok, Craik, &
Ryan, 2006). Continuously switching between two languages may
further promote the capacity to maintain two sets of instructions
in mind and select the correct response in a particular situation
another ability that is stronger in elderly bilingual populations
(Bialystok et al., 2006). Thus, having command of more than one
language appears to enhance executive function similarly to partic-
ipation in training programs. Bilingual education may therefore
represent a favorable tool for the development of executive control
in children and the further strengthening of this system through-
out adulthood.
4.4. Music training
Stemming in part from the widespread ‘‘Mozart effect’’ myth
(Steele, Bass, & Crook, 1999) – which suggests that spatiotemporal
abilities increase after listening to music composed by Mozart –
musical training increasingly captivates the public. Despite the fal-
lacy of the Mozart effect, evidence suggests that musical training
does produce significant improvements in faculties such as verbal
memory (Chan, Ho, & Cheung, 1998; Ho, Cheung, & Chan, 2003)
and general intelligence, as demonstrated in children randomly as-
signed to either music training, drama, or no-training controls
(Schellenberg, 2004). Requiring a high degree of repetition, con-
centration, and devotion over many years of continuous practice,
this form of training shares many of the qualities possessed by
more structured cognitive training programs. Furthermore, music
training generates positive emotions, which have been linked with
improved plasticity (Altenmüller, 2009). Following 6 months of
piano keyboard training, children have demonstrated enhanced
spatiotemporal reasoning compared to children receiving private
computer lessons or no training (Rauscher et al., 1997). Studies
also show enhanced development of visuospatial WM ability and
non-verbal reasoning, in addition to increases in child IQ scores
as a result of musical training (Bergman Nutley, 2011). Musicians
have also displayed superior control in certain auditory tasks com-
pared to non-musicians (Bialystok & DePape, 2009). Recent re-
search further links musical proficiency to enhanced executive
control during conflict-related tasks unrelated to music (Bialystok
& DePape, 2009). Considering the attentional and WM load as well
as the knowledge of specific acoustic and syntactic rules that music
requires (Kraus & Chandrasekaran, 2010), this association is not
surprising. The widespread interest in musical training as a tool
for general cognitive enhancement therefore appears to have sci-
entific merit.
Due to the cognitive demands of musical practice, music train-
ing may facilitate changes that enhance the functionality of regions
related to auditory perception as well as executive attention. The
behavioral benefits of music training are accompanied by struc-
tural modifications within specific brain regions, as well as changes
in gray matter volume (Gaser & Schlaug, 2003; Munte, Altenmuller,
& Jancke, 2002). In addition to the structural changes, music prac-
tice appears to alter neural activation patterns that underlie audi-
tory discrimination and executive attention. Musicians show more
pronounced neural signals in response to irrelevant sound signals
and can better detect meaningful information such as speech amid
a noisy background (Kraus & Chandrasekaran, 2010). Neuroimag-
ing studies further reveal an association between neural areas re-
lated to WM function and parietal brain regions during sight
reading of musical notes (Bergman Nutley, 2011). Hence, the com-
plex process of learning to play a musical instrument has influ-
ences on neural function and anatomy that cannot be attributed
to pre-existing qualities, and may constitute another promising
form of brain training.
4.5. Physical exercise
Aside from advantages to physical health, exercise appears to
have a beneficial impact on cognitive function, particularly in chil-
dren (Hillman, Erickson, & Kramer, 2008) and elderly individuals
(Colcombe & Kramer, 2003). The relatively recent infatuation with
sedentary lifestyle seen in developed countries creates the prece-
dent of minimal physical activity throughout a typical school or
work day. With childhood obesity on the rise and an increasing
prevalence of weight-related health conditions such as diabetes,
educational and public health campaigns attempt to heighten pub-
lic awareness about the importance of exercise to physical health.
Studies report that as little as 30 min of aerobic exercise per day
significantly enhance children’s capacity for creativity and the
capacity to deduce several correct answers in response to a given
question (Tuckman & Hinkle, 1986) – a measure of cognitive flex-
ibility (Diamond & Lee, 2011). Studies further indicate that aerobic
exercise may significantly enhance executive function, improve
performance in mathematics, and increase activity in the PFC (Da-
vis et al., 2011). In addition, a meta-analysis of fitness training pro-
grams revealed that fitness programs encompassing aerobic
exercise may enhance executive control and visuospatial ability
in healthy but sedentary elderly adults (Colcombe & Kramer,
2003). The evaluated studies further reported improvements in
all of types of cognitive tasks and following all methods of training,
with combinations of strength and aerobic training producing
greater benefits than aerobic exercise alone. Critics have nonethe-
less pointed out that many fitness training studies do not ade-
quately control for experimenter involvement and, in some cases,
lack control groups altogether (Green & Bavelier, 2008). Therefore,
while aerobic exercise may promote the development of various
cognitive abilities in children and the elderly, proper controls are
necessary to discern the scientific validity of such claims.
4.6. Interaction with nature
Perhaps surprisingly to fervent believers in circumscribed train-
ing programs, regular interaction with nature appears to facilitate
improvements in cognitive function and behavioral control. Propo-
nents of the beneficial effects of nature strive to increase outdoor
exposure lost to the industrial ideals of modern society, offering
nature retreats, environmental awareness workshops (Charles,
Louv, Bodner, Guns, & Stahl, 2009; Civic Results, 2008), and even
166 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
university courses that teach students to harness nature as a con-
text for therapeutic interventions (Naropa University, 2011). Some
academic programs, moreover, educate children outdoors rather
than in traditional classrooms (Denison Pequotstepos Nature Cen-
ter, 2009; Forestry Commission Scotland, 2009) and, according to
teacher reports, appear to improve interpersonal work habits,
classroom behaviors, and engagement with the learning process.
Many of these programs stem from the attention restoration the-
ory, which posits that nature has the capacity to relieve the atten-
tion system of its functional load in order to restore effectiveness in
cognition (Kaplan, 1995b). According to this theory, nature pro-
vides a medium that requires less inhibition of competing stimuli,
giving executive function a chance to rest. Controlled and uncon-
trolled studies suggest, for example, that leisurely outdoor activity
may relieve ADHD symptomatology in children (Taylor & Kuo,
2009; Taylor, Kuo, & Sullivan, 2001). In addition, children who have
greater exposure to nature from their home environments appear
to attain superior attentional control (Wells, 2000) and to react
better in response to stressful life events, experiencing lower psy-
chological distress and higher perceptions of self-worth (Wells &
Evans, 2003). Studies suggest that walking in natural environments
as opposed to urban areas or viewing pictures of nature may im-
prove executive attention (Berman, Jonides, & Kaplan, 2008), and
that having near-home views of nature may improve concentra-
tion, inhibition, and self-discipline (Taylor, Kuo, & Sullivan, 2002).
Interacting with natural environments may therefore enable more
efficient executive function and benefit attention as well as self-
control.
4.7. Meditation training
Often regarded as methods of relaxation and mental clarity
alone, meditative practices aim to train attention and awareness
as a means of increasing control over mental processes (Walsh &
Shapiro, 2006). Meditation training programs often include one
or a combination of focused attention and open monitoring, two
common Buddhist techniques that target specific cognitive pro-
cesses (Slagter, Davidson, & Lutz, 2011). Focused attention medita-
tion involves voluntarily sustaining focus on a given object,
whereas open monitoring consists of non-reactively monitoring
the content of an ongoing experience, to become aware of the nat-
ure of associated cognitive or emotional patterns (Lutz, Slagter,
Dunne, & Davidson, 2008; Raffone & Srinivasan, 2010; Slagter
et al., 2011). Mindfulness is another common contemplative tech-
nique related to open monitoring (Raffone & Srinivasan, 2010) and
constitutes one of the most widely studied meditative practices
(Walsh & Shapiro, 2006). One definition describes mindfulness as
the practice of purposefully and objectively attending to thoughts,
emotions, and daily actions (Allen, Blashki, Gullone, & Melbourne-
Acad-Mindfulness-Interes, 2006; Tang & Posner, 2009). Meditation
therefore represents another technique that fosters the develop-
ment of cognitive and attentional capacity.
Studies have shown that training with open monitoring or fo-
cused attention can trigger neural processes underlying the execu-
tive attention system. Reports of activity in the ACC and in both the
medial and lateral areas of the PFC (Lutz et al., 2008; Raffone &
Srinivasan, 2010) suggest that meditation training (e.g., practicing
focused attention) improves conflict processing as well as emo-
tional- and self-regulation, with experienced meditators showing
increased activation in these areas compared to non-meditators
(Hölzel et al., 2007). In addition, focused attention and open mon-
itoring both associate with neural adaptations such as increased
regional blood flow and glucose metabolism in the PFC and ACC
(Baijal & Gupta, 2008). Accordingly, studies show that meditators
who practice methods of either focused attention or mindfulness
can better sustain their attention (Valentine & Sweet, 1999) and
demonstrate improved performance on a number of conflict-re-
lated tasks. These include superior perception during binocular riv-
alry tasks (Lutz et al., 2008), reduced semantic processing required
for lexical decision tasks (Pagnoni, Cekic, & Guo, 2008), decreased
response variability for dichotic listening tasks (Lutz et al., 2009),
and increased mismatch negativity for auditory tones (Raffone &
Srinivasan, 2010). Neuroimaging studies additionally reveal that
regular meditators who practice open monitoring or focused atten-
tion may activate attention-related neural regions more efficiently
in response to conflict (Kozasa et al., in press). The beneficial effects
of these meditative practices may be sustained throughout aging;
experienced meditators show fewer age-related declines in gray
matter volume of certain neural regions and display higher vol-
umes of several brain regions, including the PFC (Ott, Hölzel, &
Vaitl, 2011). Thus, specific forms of meditation can invoke activa-
tion of the executive attention system and may thereby improve
its functionality over time.
Recent evidence suggests that mindfulness meditation may en-
hance the neural processes underlying attention and WM. In light
of such findings, scientists are now considering the clinical impli-
cations of meditation training, particularly for conditions that in-
volve broad aspects of psychological well-being (Chiesa, Calati, &
Serretti, in press). Studies indicate that mindfulness training may
improve cognitive functioning and reduce stress, anxiety, negative
affect, and the symptoms associated with various diseases (Chiesa
et al., in press; Creswell, Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007).
Adults and adolescents with ADHD, furthermore, have demon-
strated improvements of behavioral and neurocognitive impair-
ments following a mindfulness group-training program
(Zylowska et al., 2008). This program may have an additional
favorable impact on the development of inhibitory control and
self-regulation. While experience may increase the benefits of
meditation, short-term training also appears to promote observa-
ble effects. A school-based mindfulness program in elementary-
aged children, for example, demonstrated improvements in behav-
ioral regulation and executive function after a mere 8 h of training,
administered over a period of 8 weeks (Flook et al., 2010). By pro-
moting a heightened state of concentration that triggers activity of
the attentional networks, meditative practices such as mindfulness
training may improve behavior (Jha, Krompinger, & Baime, 2007)
and prove useful – at least as ancillary treatment – for individuals
with attention-specific deficits.
Despite the promise of meditative practices in improving cogni-
tive function, research surrounding this form of training has yet to
unearth optimal protocols and administration strategies that are
most beneficial in various populations (Burke, 2010). A number
of factors contribute to this ambiguity, such as the use of different
scales to measure experimental findings and the scarce inclusion of
active control groups in studies (Davidson, 2010). An interesting
study addressing this latter issue incorporated sham meditation
as part of the experimental design and reported improvements in
psychological variables such as mood, depression ratings, and fati-
gue, following 3 days of mindfulness training (Zeidan, Johnson,
Gordon, & Goolkasian, 2010). Observed improvements in behavior,
however, may arise as a result of diverse meditative techniques. As
a result, in addition to incorporating active controls, studies exam-
ine combinations of potentially beneficial training techniques with
hopes of creating variants that would induce maximal benefits on
attention. One such variant, a Chinese technique known as integra-
tive body-mind training (IBMT), integrates aspects of several med-
itative practices (Tang & Posner, 2009). In one study, individuals
randomly assigned to practice IBMT for 5 days demonstrated sig-
nificantly better attentional capacity and control over stress com-
pared to individuals who practiced other meditative techniques
during the same period of time (Tang et al., 2007). Furthermore,
a mere 3 h of this training program was enough to induce an
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 167
Author's personal copy
increase in activation of the ACC and improve the self-regulation of
adult participants (Tang et al., 2010). Recently, Tang et al. (2010)
also showed that adults who trained in IBMT for 11 h had a higher
density of cortical white matter, including a region known to con-
nect the ACC with functionally important neural areas. Further re-
search is nonetheless required to properly attribute the different
components of meditative practices to the observed effects on cog-
nitive ability. Future studies may glean more precise information
by adequately controlling for all variables involved in specific pro-
grams to better parse the training components that promote such
positive outcomes.
4.8. Parenting
One of the strongest influences on childhood behavior is the
family setting, largely a function of parent or caregiver interactions
with a child. Animal studies attest to the importance of parenting,
with early monkey studies (Harlow & Mears, 1979) identifying the
primary purpose of nursing as a way to ensure intimate body con-
tact between mother and infant – essential for establishing sensory
stimulation that facilitates neural development (Illes & Sahakian,
2011). Rodent studies further show that maternal behaviors can re-
duce neuroendocrine response to stress (Liu et al., 1997) and even
alter the genetic expression underlying neuroendocrine and behav-
ioral stress responses (Weaver et al., 2004). Maternal behavior also
appears to promote synaptic development in the hippocampus, a
structure associated with memory, and may enhance spatial learn-
ing and memory in rats (Liu, Diorio, Day, Francis, & Meaney, 2000).
Such studies attest to the importance of parenting for the proper
development of cognitive ability and emotional stability.
The challenges of caring for children in a typical working-class
family increase for parents who have little support or assistance.
Studies suggest that disturbed family environments may contrib-
ute to the development of childhood psychopathologies. A review
of ADHD neurobiology, for example, identified six family-related
factors that significantly correlated with the development of child-
hood mental disturbances, including severe marital discord, low
social class, large family size, paternal criminality, maternal mental
disorder, and foster placement (Faraone & Biederman, 1998). In
light of such reports, a number of therapeutic programs target par-
ents and families as a whole, and sometimes include supplemental
interventions for the children. One such program, the Community
Parent Education method, implements a 10-week parent education
program with concurrent social skill building for children and has
gathered evidence showing improved parenting skills and fewer
child behavior problems (Tamm et al., 2008). Parent training also
appears to improve inattention, over-activity, conduct problems,
compliance, and aggression in children (Wells, 2008). Following a
12-session mindfulness training program for mothers with no fo-
cus on behavior management, mother–child interactions improved
and child compliance increased (Singh et al., 2010). Other studies
suggest, moreover, that developing effortful control in children
may offset the effects of negative or neglectful parenting (Liew,
2011). These findings highlight the importance of parent–child
relationships and of using proper parenting methods to guide
behavioral development in children.
The added difficulty of raising children with developmental
psychopathologies often leads parents to resort to unjust or inef-
fective punitive measures. For this reason, a central feature of sev-
eral parent training approaches includes proper allocation of
attention and appropriate management of disorderly behavior.
One study of a program for ADHD-related behavior management
(Barkley, 1998) revealed that positive attention may in itself in-
duce greater compliance in younger children and further illus-
trated the importance of attending to child behavior in the
school setting in addition to other public environments. Expan-
sions to this program show benefits of parent relaxation training
as well as stress-management (Wells, 2008). More recent parent
training paradigms have tested the effects of incorporating modern
technology or interactive components into the standard program.
Adding a sport or recreational component to a father-training pro-
gram, for example, may enhance the typical benefits of these pro-
grams by not only improving behavior-related symptomatology
and peer-interactions, but by further increasing attendance and
satisfaction with the program, as well as homework compliance
(Fabiano et al., 2009). Similarly, an evaluation of a program imple-
menting internet-based training for mothers with infants at risk for
poor social–emotional development found increases in mother–
child interactions in addition to marginal improvements in impair-
ments associated with interactive behavior and depressive symp-
tomatology (Baggett et al., 2010). These studies indicate that,
through training, parents can learn to interact positively with their
children, thereby improving parent–child relationships and help-
ing parents teach children proper standards of behavior.
5. The effectiveness of training practices: examining the
evidence
In this section, we provide a critical examination of the impact
of cognitive training in both healthy and pathological individuals.
We investigate the generalizability of training and whether specific
exercise can transfer to other domains of cognitive function.
5.1. Generalizability and transferability: improving overall function or
just specific skills?
As brain training rises in popularity, mounting skepticism chal-
lenges the effectiveness of such programs on cognitive ability.
While brain training programs may improve performance on a spe-
cific subset of skills or tasks, the benefits may not generalize to
other domains. One such example is the Bates Method, a behav-
ioral approach to improving visual acuity by altering attentional
states through practices such as hypnotherapy (Marg, 1952; Raz,
Marinoff, Zephrani, Schweizer, & Posner, 2004). Once considered
a groundbreaking technique for visual correction, the Bates Meth-
od was disproved by studies suggesting that attention can only
influence the priority or processing preference of the fovea, which
can impact parameters such as visual detection or reaction time
without improving visual acuity per se (Raz et al., 2004). Similarly,
a number of reviews have evaluated the quality and robustness of
training effects in an effort to determine the transferability of dif-
ferent brain training methods to other cognitive or behavioral
functions. Recent evidence has led many to believe that perhaps
they should not expect much from these methods. A group of Brit-
ish scientists, in collaboration with the British Broadcasting Corpo-
ration (BBC) television program ‘‘Bang Goes the Theory’’, stunned
participants and viewers of the program with the allegation that
computerized brain training does not benefit general cognitive
ability (Owen et al., 2010). Employing the largest sample size ever
used in cognitive-training research, the study reported no signifi-
cant increase in general cognitive performance following 6-weeks
of online training in WM tasks, apart from improvement in the
practiced tasks. In response to this negative outcome, countless
media articles suggested that brain training likely represents
wasted effort. Notwithstanding this gross generalization, largely
circulated by public media reports, a close examination of the
parameters used in the study reveals several limitations. Weak
environmental controls, insufficient training duration, and a ques-
tionable study population limit possible conclusions. Furthermore,
the study involved a sample of healthy individuals, although re-
search suggests that individuals with lower baseline scores may
168 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
benefit more from such training. This study nonetheless accentu-
ates the need for more properly controlled studies, thorough anal-
ysis of the available data, and careful interpretation of results, to
determine the capacity for transfer of computerized brain training
programs.
The question of transferability is perhaps most relevant for
commercial products. The Cogmed program – advertised as effec-
tive in both healthy and pathological populations, both young
and old – is among the most thoroughly studied of these products.
In an inaugural study, children with ADHD and healthy adults who
trained with components of the Cogmed program showed general-
ized improvements in cognitive control and general fluid intelli-
gence, with additional reduction in symptoms related to ADHD
in the pathological population (Klingberg, Forssberg, & Westerberg,
2002). The findings were later replicated in healthy adults (Olesen,
Westerberg, & Klingberg, 2004; Westerberg & Klingberg, 2007),
although both studies were unclear about the possibility of
improvements due to test–retest effects (Shipstead, Redick, &
Engle, 2010). A subsequent study in children with ADHD demon-
strated significant improvements on measures of attention and
intelligence compared to controls, which persisted 3 months after
completing 25 sessions of visuo-spatial, backward-digit, and let-
ter-span tasks from the Cogmed program (Klingberg et al., 2005).
Notably, however, children randomly assigned to the control group
displayed increased scores at the 3-month evaluation period,
which may indicate insufficient level of difficulty in the testing
measures used (Shipstead et al., 2010). Furthermore, parent – but
not teacher – reports indicated reductions in ADHD symptoms
(Klingberg et al., 2005). Following 20 sessions of training with Cog-
med, children with low WM capacity showed enhanced WM per-
formance that persisted for 6 months, relative to controls
(Holmes, Gathercole, & Dunning, 2009). Although this study ap-
peared to demonstrate generalizability to cognitive domains unre-
lated to the training, the participants did not improve on measures
of intelligence, reading, or mathematical reasoning, and follow-up
measures did not include comparisons to the control group (Ship-
stead et al., 2010). A replication of Klingberg et al. (2005) showed
that, compared to no-treatment controls, adolescents with ADHD
displayed improvements in inattentiveness and executive function,
as well as symptoms related to the disorder, as indexed by parent
reports (Beck, Hanson, Puffenberger, Benninger, & Benninger,
2010). These effects persisted at the 4-month follow-up assess-
ment, and were mirrored by near-significant findings based on tea-
cher reports. Studies therefore suggest that certain commercial
brain training products may improve specific skills, although evi-
dence remains scarce regarding the transferability of training to
unrelated domains of cognitive function.
While computerized programs show promise along the short
term, studies often carry a number of caveats that restrict possible
interpretations of the experimental findings, and scarcely demon-
strate sustainability of more than several months. In children with
ADHD, for example, a program exercising verbal and visuo-spatial
short-term memory in addition to WM facilitated improvements
on measures of the trained abilities that lasted 6 months (Holmes
et al., 2010). Limitations of this study, however, include a lack of
control groups, direct ADHD measures, and transfer to IQ scores
(Shipstead et al., 2010). The transfer effects of brain training are
also inconsistent in healthy populations. In one study, participants
significantly improved performance on the Stroop task in addition
to reading comprehension as a result of a 4-week WM training pro-
gram, but did not display increases in general fluid intelligence or
in spatial reasoning (Chein & Morrison, 2010b). A different study
reported that, after participating in a WM-training program, pre-
school children exhibited better performance on attentional tasks
requiring monitoring, but did not show any improvements on
Stroop-like, inhibitory, or problem-solving tasks (Thorell, Lindq-
vist, Bergman Nutley, Bohlin, & Klingberg, 2009). After participat-
ing in 20 sessions of an adapted complex WM span task over 4-
weeks, healthy individuals improved on measures of temporary
memory and verbal reasoning and further increased their cognitive
control, as indexed by their performance on the Stroop task (Chein
& Morrison, 2010a). The participants also improved their reading
comprehension, which correlated with increases in spatial WM.
These findings suggest that this novel training paradigm may im-
prove certain aspects of general attentional mechanisms, such as
the management of information maintenance in concordance with
other neural processes (Chein & Morrison, 2010a). Such transfer of
WM training to untrained attention and memory tasks also ap-
pears in older adults (Berry et al., 2010). Although studies often re-
port promising findings, researchers have yet to reach a consensus
regarding suitable control groups, accurate measures of parame-
ters such as sustainability, and outcomes that simply result from
participation in a training program. As a result of such discrepan-
cies in experimental parameters and measurement tools, compar-
isons between studies that reportedly measure the same cognitive
or behavioral construct are difficult to establish.
5.2. Effectiveness without efficacy: ulterior benefits of brain training
The advantages of brain training may reside in the nuances of
effectiveness rather than efficacy. In clinical terms, efficacy refers
to the ability of a substance, usually a pharmaceutical agent, to
produce a desired effect through a particular mechanism of action.
Effectiveness, on the other hand, refers to the practical use of a sub-
stance. Research has often revealed cases of efficacy without effec-
tiveness (Glasgow, Lichtenstein, & Marcus, 2003); pharmaceutical
agents, for example, sometimes produce side-effects that are more
noxious than the condition they are meant to treat, thereby render-
ing them ineffective. In contrast, evidence also suggests that, in
some cases, a substance may prove effective without being effica-
cious. Such phenomena may occur via the placebo effect, or posi-
tive effects associated with sham or irrelevant treatment
(Benedetti, Mayberg, Wager, Stohler, & Zubieta, 2005). Similarly,
although the improvements observed as a result of brain training
may be induced due to reasons other than the training itself, such
interventions may still be effective through an analogous placebo-
like effect. The variety of programs that improve cognition and
behavior, coupled with reports of little transferability of brain
training, leads to speculation that brain training may not contain
a specific mechanism of action. The act of providing treatment
for a particular condition, however, may in itself decrease the asso-
ciated symptoms (Kermen, Hickner, Brody, & Hasham, 2010; Til-
burt, Emanuel, Kaptchuk, Curlin, & Miller, 2008). Improvements
in cognitive performance may also occur as a result of motivational
factors, including active interest in individual performance (Green
& Bavelier, 2008). Brain training may therefore represent an effec-
tive intervention or potential treatment for clinical populations
even if research deems the specific mechanism tenuous.
6. Who can benefit from brain training?
Brain training programs may impact an assortment of neurolog-
ical states. From the healthy to the neurologically impaired, pro-
grams aim to enhance or rehabilitate cognitive function in both
young and old. In this section, we examine evidence for the bene-
fits of brain training programs in various populations.
6.1. Brain training in typical development
Evidence indicates that brain training can enhance healthy
neural development. Unlike pathological populations, typically
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 169
Author's personal copy
developing children and adults train the brain to fortify skills al-
ready acquired in an effort to distinguish their abilities among
peers and thrive in an increasingly competitive society. While
the effects are often subtle, such training may sometimes yield dra-
matic results. One particularly effective technique comprises strat-
egy training, which involves learning effective approaches to
encode, maintain, and retrieve information from WM (Chein &
Morrison, 2010a). One study reported the memorization of 80 dig-
its after learning the strategy of grouping numbers into running
times (Ericsson & Chase, 1982). A more recent example of strategy
training is the story of Joshua Foer, the 2006 USA Memory Cham-
pion (Foer, 2011). A freelance journalist with no previous experi-
ence in competitive memorizing, Foer not only defeated his
competitors – experienced mnemonists – in the championship,
but also broke the world record in the speed card category. His
trick consisted of an age-old memorization technique by which
he trained himself to associate tedious facts, difficult for the hu-
man brain to remember, with eccentric images that an individual
is unlikely to forget. Neuroimaging studies suggest that competi-
tive mnemonists and high-rankers of World Memory Champion-
ships use similar stratagems during performance of WM and
long-term verbal memory tasks (Maguire, Valentine, Wilding, &
Kapur, 2003). Despite having typical brain morphology compared
to controls, this population displays increased regional activation
of areas associated with spatial memory and navigation, which
may underlie the learning of route strategies to recall long lists
of items (Maguire et al., 2003). Therefore, typically developing
individuals may also display enormous improvements in perfor-
mance following specific types of training.
6.2. Training the aging brain
With mounting evidence for cognitive decline in the elderly,
brain training programs for geriatric populations seem increasingly
relevant and enticing. In 2005, a global prevalence study estimated
that 24 million individuals were living with dementia, of whom 3–
4 million were residing in North America, and that this number
would double every 20 years (Ferri et al., 2005). While the leading
cause of age-related dementia is Alzheimer’s disease (Ferri et al.,
2005), studies have identified a number of risk factors associated
with impaired cognitive ability including decreased physical activ-
ity (Laurin, Verreault, Lindsay, MacPherson, & Rockwood, 2001;
Yaffe, Barnes, Nevitt, Lui, & Covinsky, 2001), lack of education
(Callahan et al., 1996), health conditions such as diabetes and
hypertension (Kuo et al., 2005), and the presence of certain patho-
logical (McKeith et al., 1996) and genetic traits (Duff et al., 1996).
Reports of delayed cognitive decline as a result of brain training
have propelled a market of products aimed at preventing or even
reversing the effects of age on cognition. These programs, often
administered through computerized media or video game con-
soles, carry varying degrees of scientific validity. The popular Bra-
inAge program (Nintendo, 2007) is one prominent example among
the myriad of commercialized products that holds little scientific
evidence of efficacy. A recent study using this game console, more-
over, suggests that elderly individuals work more efficiently using
a paper-and-pencil interface, although decreased technological
sophistication appears to evoke lower levels of arousal (Nacke,
Nacke, & Lindley, 2009). On the other hand, certain training tech-
niques have shown promise for the delay or prevention of neuro-
degenerative diseases. Animal studies indicate that enriched
environments may increase cognitive function (Arendash et al.,
2004) and reduce pathological traits for Alzheimer’s, including
neural deposition of amyloid protein (Lazarov et al., 2005). In hu-
mans, training appears to improve memory in individuals with
mild cognitive impairment (Belleville et al., 2011) or mild-to-mod-
erate Alzheimer’s (Zanetti et al., 1997). A recent evaluation by the
National Institutes of Health found little evidence for pharmaceu-
tical or dietary preventative measures for cognitive decline (Davig-
lus et al., 2010), underlining the importance of increasing research
pertaining to cognitive training in such populations. With positive
preliminary findings in elderly individuals with cognitive impair-
ments, this type of training may constitute a crucial source of
remediation for cognitive decline.
Training need not be circumscribed or even deviate from typical
daily activities. An interesting take on cognitive training in elderly
populations, the Active Cognitive Stimulation-Prevention in the El-
derly (AKTIVA) program entails engagement in leisurely activities
that nonetheless provide some form of cognitive stimulation (Tes-
ky, Thiel, Banzer, & Pantel, 2011). By combining cognitively
demanding activities previously shown to help prevent or delay
onset of dementia, AKTIVA offered a potentially fun and engaging
way to promote sustainability of cognitive function throughout
old age. A randomized controlled study assessing the effectiveness
of this program reported significant improvements in speed of pro-
cessing in participants over 75 years of age, as well as subjective
ratings of age-related memory declines in participants younger
than 75 years of age. Overall, however, this study revealed no sig-
nificant benefits of the AKTIVA program compared to controls (Tes-
ky et al., 2011). While such paradigms may offer an enjoyable and
convenient method for training, these findings highlight the
importance of discerning what components underlie the success
of effective programs.
In healthy elderly populations, brain training appears to delay
the natural progression of cognitive decline by enhancing learning
capacity and specific forms of memory (Buiza et al., 2009; Park,
Kwon, Seo, Lim, & Song, 2009). The Advanced Cognitive Training
for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) study (Jobe et al.,
2001) – the largest randomized controlled trial to date studying
cognitive decline in healthy elderly individuals – provides evidence
for the effectiveness of 10 1-h sessions of reasoning training, mem-
ory training, and speed of process training in improving perfor-
mance on the specific abilities trained (Ball et al., 2002).
Participants who received four additional reasoning and speed
training sessions at 11-months following program completion,
moreover, appeared to experience these benefits to a significantly
greater extent; these booster effects were not observed for the
memory training group. These effects were sustainable throughout
the 24-month follow-up (Ball et al., 2002), with later studies fur-
ther reporting delays in the decline of health-related quality of life
for speed-trained participants (Wolinsky et al., 2006) during the
same period of time. Nonetheless, effect sizes for these benefits
were small and decreased over time. Furthermore, while reasoning
and speed training benefited a relatively large percentage of partic-
ipants on the corresponding cognitive abilities tested, few partici-
pants improved on the memory-related cognitive abilities trained.
These improvements also generalized to cognitively demanding
daily abilities such as everyday processing speed and driving hab-
its, although only for the speed training group. Despite these find-
ings, all participants remained functionally independent
throughout the course of the observation period (Ball et al.,
2002). A review of studies on cognitive training in healthy elderly
individuals nevertheless concluded that while such training may
effectively improve performance on tasks related to the training,
little evidence demonstrates generalizability to general cognitive
domains (Papp et al., 2009). Thus, although brain training may im-
prove specific cognitive abilities in the healthy elderly, this type of
intervention does not appear to improve overall cognitive function.
6.3. Training for recovery after stroke
Cognitive training also appears to benefit the process of rehabil-
itation following stroke. Neuroimaging studies reveal that, once
170 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
stroke recovery occurs, patients undergo structural changes within
the brain that enable functional compensation for damaged neural
areas (Stuss et al., 1999). While the most common impairments
experienced after stroke involve motor-related disabilities, cogni-
tive deficits also manifest in a large percentage of patients and
may persist for years following the event (Langhorne, Bernhardt,
& Kwakkel, 2011; Skidmore et al., 2011). Evidence surrounding
the effectiveness of such interventions for stroke rehabilitation,
however, remains conflicting. On one hand, certain studies report
post-training improvements in functions such as alertness and sus-
tained attention (Lincoln, Majid, & Weyman, 2000), WM (Wester-
berg et al., 2007), as well as perception and spatial neglect
(Langhorne et al., 2011). One study revealed enhanced skill-acqui-
sition in stroke patients who participated in a meta-cognitive pro-
gram aimed at increasing their sense of autonomy (McEwen,
Polatajko, Davis, Huijbregts, & Ryan, 2010). Despite these encour-
aging results, the extent to which such training improves cognitive
function and whether it benefits quality of life for patients is un-
clear. Furthermore, other cognitive functions often damaged by
stroke, such as linguistic ability, do not appear to improve signifi-
cantly as a result of the cognitive training techniques used (De
Jong-Hagelstein et al., 2011). Cognitive remediation techniques
may therefore assist the natural recovery pattern in patients fol-
lowing stroke, although further study remains to elucidate the
most effective methods as well as the extent to which such training
measurably improves daily life.
6.4. The potential of brain training for psychopathology
With the dramatic surge in attention-related disorders, parents
and professionals are growing increasingly eager to optimize the
system of attention both in school and at work. One example is
ADHD, a disorder laced with symptoms relating to inattention,
hyperactivity-impulsivity, or a combination of the two (Barkley,
1997; Steinhausen, 2009; Wells, 2008). With an approximate prev-
alence of 5.3% worldwide, in boys more than in girls, ADHD is the
most common childhood-onset psychopathology and persists into
adulthood in 30–50% of clinically-diagnosed cases (Barkley, 1997;
Wallis, Russell, & Muenke, 2008). Another example is Tourette’s
Syndrome (TS), a neurodevelopmental impulse-control disorder
(Robertson, 2003). With a brief review of some complexities sur-
rounding the etiology and treatment of these disorders, we discuss
the potential of specific forms of brain training as alternatives to
medication.
6.4.1. The appeal of brain training to ADHD
ADHD affects neural structures associated with attentional pro-
cesses. Neuroimaging studies indicate that children with ADHD
have smaller global brain volumes compared to typically develop-
ing children, in addition to localized decreases in PFC, caudate, cer-
ebellum, and corpus callosum size (Kieling, Goncalves, Tannock, &
Castellanos, 2008; Steinhausen, 2009). Studies also report dimin-
ished activity in the circuits underlying executive attention,
including regions of the PFC and ACC (Spencer, Biederman, Wilens,
& Faraone, 2002), which may bring about the observed differences
in cognitive control. Developmental manifestations of ADHD can
be extremely pronounced and involve WM, speech internalization,
modulation of goal-directed behaviors, as well as self-regulation of
drive and affect (Faraone & Biederman, 1998). While these deficits
may be more prominent in children, studies report analogous
impairments of attention, self-control, and time estimation in ado-
lescents with ADHD (Barkley, Edwards, Laneri, Fletcher, & Metevia,
2001).
ADHD is perhaps best recognized through the marked difficul-
ties that children exhibit with interpersonal relationships. A study
in the United States found that, among a population of children
diagnosed with ADHD, over one third of parents reported signifi-
cant emotional and behavioral difficulties in their children, with
nearly 40% reporting deficits in aspects of daily living (Strine
et al., 2006). These difficulties may arise, in part, as a result of an
inferior capability to distinguish emotionally-charged facial
expressions (Pelc, Kornreich, Foisy, & Dan, 2006). In addition, stud-
ies show peer-rejection rates of children with ADHD span 52–82%,
which may be due to increased aggression, poorer social skills, and
inflated self-perception (Murray-Close et al., 2010). Such reports
indicate that children with ADHD stand to benefit from programs
that may teach them to control their emotions and behavior.
Treatment options for ADHD are sparse and most commonly
comprise psychostimulant medication, which shows marginal
effectiveness. Improvements in the conduct and academic perfor-
mance of medicated children (Elia, Ambrosini, & Rapoport, 1999)
confirm the short term efficacy of psychostimulants, of which
methylphenidate and amphetamine are most commonly pre-
scribed. This type of medication appears to reduce characteristic
behaviors associated with ADHD, including inattentiveness, hyper-
activity, and impulsivity (The MTA Cooperative Group, 1999), with
additional improvements in compliance, aggression, and academic
achievement (Halperin & Healey, 2011). Reports further indicate
varying levels of improvement in performance and reaction time
on various WM and executive function tasks in individuals with
ADHD taking stimulant medication (Barnett et al., 2001; Swanson,
Baler, & Volkow, 2011). These studies suggest that psychostimu-
lant medication effectively decreases ADHD symptoms.
Current ADHD medication, however, has several limitations
that incite parents and professionals to demand superior treatment
options. First, while short term effects of these drugs are well doc-
umented, there is ongoing debate regarding the effects of long-
term psychostimulant consumption. While some studies report
continued stimulant efficacy for a number of years following initial
treatment in children and adults (Bejerot, Ryden, & Arlinde, 2010;
The MTA Cooperative Group, 1999), high drop-out rates and find-
ings of a drop-off in effect after a few years render the collective
evidence inconclusive at best (Swanson et al., 2011). Even the po-
sitive short-term benefits of psychostimulant drugs come at the
price of unwanted side-effects and potential long-term risks. Both
methylphenidate and amphetamine trigger similar, dose-depen-
dent, adverse effects, of which insomnia and diminished appetite
– seen in approximately 80% of children with ADHD – are the most
common (Elia et al., 1999). Reports also identify cardiovascular
problems such as elevated resting heart-rate and blood pressure,
in addition to stunted growth and the development of tics (Bejerot
et al., 2010; Daughton, Liu, West, Swanson, & Kratochvil, 2010; Elia
et al., 1999). Another concern pertains to substance abuse, highly
comorbid with ADHD and sometimes triggered by continued stim-
ulant-use (Daughton et al., 2010; Szobot et al., 2011). Finally, not
all individuals respond to psychostimulants. Reports indicate that
70% of ADHD patients respond to the first stimulant drug adminis-
tered, and an additional 10–20% respond if a second class of stim-
ulant is tried in succession (Daughton et al., 2010). These response
rates, however, do not appear to vary with ADHD subtype (Solanto
et al., 2009). These findings clearly warrant demand for safer and
more effective treatment options.
Scientists have attempted treating ADHD with alternate types
of medication, including non-stimulant drugs, antidepressants,
antipsychotic medication, and alpha-adrenergic agonists. While
non-stimulant medications have reportedly fewer side-effects
and long-term risks, their impact on ADHD symptoms is not as
strong as psychostimulants (Daughton et al., 2010). Likewise, anti-
depressant, antipsychotic, and alpha-agonist medications show
only mild efficacy, although the findings come from small popula-
tions and include reports of adverse side-effects (Daughton et al.,
2010; Elia et al., 1999; Ipser & Stein, 2007). Reports of death in
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 171
Author's personal copy
children taking a combinations of psychostimulants and alpha-
agonists, moreover, represent a clear illustration of the need to
determine the ramifications of taking several classes of drugs
simultaneously (Elia et al., 1999). Such reports suggest that these
types of drugs may not constitute viable alternatives to psycho-
stimulant treatment in ADHD.
Given the potentially noxious effects of psychostimulant medi-
cation and the relative ineffectiveness of other drug types, wide-
spread efforts aim to develop non-pharmacological therapies that
would regulate unwanted ADHD symptomatology without the
threat of long-term adverse effects. Currently, many studies indi-
cate that children with ADHD strongly benefit from family therapy
– with particular emphasis on parent training – as well as social
skill training (Elia et al., 1999; McBurnett & Pfiffner, 2008; Wells,
2008). Certainly, providing a more structured and supportive envi-
ronment may offset conditions that play a role in triggering the on-
set of ADHD symptoms in children, and has potential to markedly
improve the pathophysiological course of the disorder.
Cognitive treatments for ADHD entail brain training programs
and show promising effects in both children and adults (Galbiati
et al., 2009). Studies of programs such as Cogmed (Klingberg,
2008; Olesen et al., 2004), report improvements in both cognitive
ability and behavioral symptoms of ADHD (Halperin & Healey,
2011). Other programs appear to facilitate increased cognitive per-
formance and attentional ability in children, although these bene-
fits do not always extend to improvements in behavior (Kerns
et al., 1999). The effects of certain forms of training, furthermore,
are comparable with the effects of medication (Klingberg et al.,
2005). Similar to pharmacotherapy, however, many of these train-
ing programs do not afford children the opportunity to develop
their own self-control (Singh et al., 2010). Cognitive training pro-
grams supplemented by interpersonal interactions may therefore
allow children to receive behavioral monitoring and continual
feedback, thereby teaching them to monitor their actions and re-
spond in a situation-appropriate manner. Programs for children
combining medication with cognitive training, including specific
forms of meditation, reveal significant improvements in symptoms
related to inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity, as well as en-
hanced self-esteem and child-parent relationships (Rubia, 2009).
The reported improvements in cognitive performance and ADHD
symptomatology as a result of cognitive training attest to the
promise of such interventions as a component of ADHD treatment.
6.4.2. Brain training in Tourette’s Syndrome
While pharmacological options exist for the treatment of TS
(e.g., haloperidol, pimozide, or clonidine), general consensus posits
that such therapies are suboptimal, and prominent researchers
have recently lamented that medication therapies for TS are woe-
fully inadequate (Singer & Walkup, 1991). Drug efficacy for TS is
inconsistent and unpredictable, and at best, offers only symptom-
atic relief (Peterson & Cohen, 1998). Benefits often come at the ex-
pense of intolerable side-effects, including sedation, parkinsonism,
tardive dyskinesia, cognitive dulling, dry mouth, fatigue, dizziness,
weight gain, and metabolic problems (Swain, Scahill, Lombroso,
King, & Leckman, 2007). Most current treatments for TS are poten-
tially toxic to the central nervous system. Moreover, those treat-
ments tend to be terribly ineffective for many individuals and at
best provide a modest reduction of the symptomatology (Phelps,
2008). Specialists hence recognize the need for alternatives and
therapeutic adjuncts.
Behavioral interventions such as habit-reversal training (HRT)
have been shown to be effective in ameliorating the symptoms of
TS (Feldman, Storch, & Murphy, 2011; Himle, Woods, Piacentini,
& Walkup, 2006; Piacentini et al., 2010; Woods et al., 2011). We
have outlined how attentional interventions, including AT, can
aid in overcoming the debilitating symptoms of impulse control
disorders via improvements to this network, with a special focus
on TS (Raz, Keller, Norman, & Senechal, 2007). Similar to HRT, AT
reduced the symptoms of TS in a pilot study involving 12 experi-
mental and 12 control participants. Our preliminary findings sug-
gest that, compared to a control condition – watching popular
children’s videos, relaxing, and playing general video games with
intermittent dialogue pauses matching for child–adult interactions
– AT decreased visible tics and impulsivity in young individuals
with TS and increased their ability to regulate emotions and persist
with goals in the face of distractions. Findings from our pilot data
further purpose that these changes translate into an increase in the
quality of life (Raz, in press). Thus, tic-awareness programs, recog-
nizing internal urges, switching to voluntary behaviors that are
physically incompatible with the tic, relaxation guidance, and
learning to identify antecedents of the tics, appear to be a promis-
ing behavioral approach for people with TS.
7. The business of brain training and conflicts of interest
Brain training constitutes a lucrative market. Widespread con-
cern regarding cognitive decline in the aging population and obses-
sion with maximizing efficiency in school and at work have created
a society of brain trainers that spare little expense on cognitive fit-
ness. With individual programs costing hundreds to thousands of
dollars, this industry feeds on growing consumer interest to yield
enormous profit. In 2010, the Scientific Learning Corporation –
developer of programs such as FFW – generated revenues over
$43 million (Ernst, 2010). In the US alone, revenues of brain fitness
software attained $265 million in 2008, increased from $100 mil-
lion in 2005 (Martin, 2009), and may accumulate revenues in the
billions by 2015. While healthcare systems have contributed a
large portion of this figure, individual consumers are playing an
increasingly prominent role, as are educational systems, athletic
organizations, and the US military (Fernandez, 2008). In addition,
programs targeting age-related cognitive decline represent a par-
ticularly profitable market, with hundreds of retirement homes
now offering brain fitness products to tenants. Hence, brain train-
ing is nothing short of big business.
The brain training industry sometimes engenders conflicts of
interest (COI) that could bias the scientific integrity of published
work. Similarly to pharmaceutical and medical device companies
(Brennan et al., 2006), distributors of cognitive exercise programs
often fund studies evaluating their product or assign product test-
ing to academic shareholders (Corporation, 2010; Pearson, 2011;
Scientific Learning Corporation, 2011). Such COI may impede the
objectivity of studies by provoking the omission of results unfavor-
able to the desired outcome or the reporting of findings that are
favorable to the funding companies (Easterbrook, Gopalan, Berlin,
& Matthews, 1991; Lexchin, Bero, Djulbegovic, & Clark, 2003;
Turner, Matthews, Linardatos, Tell, & Rosenthal, 2008). Alternately,
authors with ties to industry (Smith et al., 2009) may overextend
the interpretations of their results by emphasizing statistical sig-
nificance while ignoring relatively small effect sizes that would
indicate little or no clinical significance. Thus, COI potentially com-
promise the integrity of research.
COI are especially troubling in clinical contexts (e.g., psycho-
therapy, neurofeedback), where target populations may ultimately
rely on biased research and thereby overlook a more appropriate
remedy when searching for a treatment. Defenders of COI specu-
late that scientists may not obtain the funds necessary to conduct
research on potentially groundbreaking treatments or programs
without the benefit of commercial support (Stossel, 2007, 2008).
In a similar vein, some brain training investigators working with
industry claim that separation of research and business is imprac-
tical because product distribution is less efficient outside of
172 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
commerce (Merzenich, 2011); however, little, if any, evidence sup-
ports this claim. The history of clinical research, furthermore, at-
tests to the dangers of distributing substances in need of more
rigorous testing before becoming publicly available (Brody,
2008). Proponents of COI also assert that segregation between clin-
ical goals and promotional motivation is impossible; even
researchers strive to put their work in a positive light and profes-
sionals of the medical field advertise in order to attract clientele
(Stossel, 2007). However, unlike such conflicts, which are inherent
to human ambition, commercial COI result from voluntary choice
and are therefore avoidable (Kassirer, 2009b). Furthermore,
although researchers often claim that no incentive could compro-
mise their impartiality, evidence suggests that self-serving biases
occur subtly and unintentionally (Kassirer, 2009b). COI may there-
fore tarnish experimental findings and bias clinical practice.
The scientific community often considers disclosure to absolve
the existence of COI in research. Researchers who shelter them-
selves behind the veil of disclosure, however, merely attest to the
existence of a COI, but neither confirm nor deny the existence of
partiality in their work (Kassirer, 2009a). The onus of identifying
bias therefore passes to individuals who have no objective measure
of the partiality of a study. In addition, disclosure may hinder the
objectivity of studies by allowing scientists to retain their favorable
ties with corporations so long as they make their potential partial-
ity known to the public. Instead of merely disclosing COI in re-
search and brushing the issue aside, a more effective stratagem
may include the elimination of potentially questionable relations,
the validation of scientific rigor within study design, and the inde-
pendent replication of all influential findings.
8. Conclusion
Brain training draws on both evidence and hype. Examination of
the findings reveals that consumers – largely oversold on individ-
ualized modules and programs (e.g., for ADHD) – often rely on
claims that are scientifically unsubstantiated. For these programs
to be clinically useful, they will have to accomplish what few inter-
ventions, if any, have achieved: generalize circumscribed labora-
tory and computer skills to tangible gains in the classroom,
during play, and in other ecological settings. This lofty goal, how-
ever, has hardly been achieved.
Few scholars have distinguished their research efforts by pro-
viding scientific evidence to support the impact of their computer-
ized training in both children and adults. Specific researchers have
demonstrated sustainable behavioral improvements using inde-
pendent programs. Following brief interventions, children have
demonstrated improvements in measures of non-verbal intelli-
gence, language development, and control over affect and execu-
tive function. In addition, electrophysiological and neuroimaging
studies report a shift of signature brainwaves toward more adult-
like patterns and maturation of neural modules implicated in
attention, respectively. Some of these benefits further extend to
both healthy and pathological adults. These programs, however,
have rarely made a trailblazing breakthrough in improving symp-
toms and resolving impairments. In this regard, such approaches
to cognitive remediation show promise, but hardly represent
stand-alone treatments.
While computerized programs constitute the most accessible
form of brain training, researchers have shown benefits from spe-
cific contemplative techniques and lifestyle-related practices. Pro-
grams encompassing mind–body meditative techniques in healthy
adults, for example, demonstrate training-related alterations of
white matter connectivity in neural areas associated with the exec-
utive attention system, and appear to alleviate feelings of stress
and pain. Meditation training has also proven beneficial in adults
with ADHD, enhancing their performance on conflict and inhibition
tasks, and decreasing their reported symptoms related to the disor-
der. Other methods of training show similar benefits for improving
cognitive function and behavioral control through executive atten-
tion. These include bilingualism, musical training, physical exer-
cise, regular interaction with nature, and proper parenting. Such
findings support practice in alternate forms of training that need
not involve circumscribed programs.
Cognitive training in psychopathology may represent an ad-
junct, if not a possible alternative, to some pharmacological treat-
ment options. Psychiatric treatment of developmental
psychopathologies largely relies on drug interventions. Pharmaco-
logical approaches may engender undesirable side effects and
sometimes carry only marginal benefits, especially over time. With
the current credibility crisis surrounding pediatric psychiatry and
pharmacotherapeutics, crafting effective drug-free treatment alter-
natives seems highly relevant. Current findings, although prelimin-
ary, suggest that training options are safe, fun, and provide larger
gains for children who suffer from greater cognitive deficits. Com-
bining training with medication, furthermore, appears to produce
maximal benefits that exceed behavioral or pharmacological treat-
ments alone. Thus, given extant knowledge about available treat-
ment options, brain training paradigms may be worthy of
consideration, especially for populations with specific develop-
mental deficiencies.
Despite promising findings in both healthy and cognitively-im-
paired individuals, studies often contain a number of caveats that
weaken the interpretations drawn from experimental results. Such
limitations relate to inadequate controls and measures of cognitive
function, behavior, and training sustainability. The context of brain
training research, furthermore, sometimes contains COI that
prompt overly ambitious conclusions. Notably, studies indicate
that such training does not constitute a ‘‘quick fix’’ for a lifetime
of impairments. Rather, long-term exposure and application of
the training is likely to create lasting results. These caveats high-
light the importance of skepticism concerning experimental find-
ings as well as the possible necessity to re-evaluate current
research standards in this field.
Incorporating brain training into school curricula – alongside
mainstream courses such as history, language, physical education,
and math – may provide the long-term exposure needed to reap
the benefits. By mimicking Eastern traditions that integrate con-
templative practices into daily routine, cognitive training may have
some tangible benefits to offer. In this regard, brain training likely
impacts parameters related to improved quality of life, including
self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and stress. While further research
will have to determine the forms of training that would most ben-
efit specific populations, the effectiveness and sustainability of
such programs will likely depend on training frequency and meth-
od of delivery.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Rose Golinksi and members of the Raz
Lab for providing helpful comments on earlier versions of this
manuscript. In addition, Drs. Stephen Hinshaw, Deborah Leong,
and Kimberly Kerns provided important insights regarding content
and organization. Dr. Raz acknowledges the kind support of the
Canada Research Chair program, the Canadian Institutes of Health
Research, and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research
Council of Canada.
References
Allen, N. B., Blashki, G., Gullone, E., & Melbourne-Acad-Mindfulness-Interes (2006).
Mindfulness-based psychotherapies: A review of conceptual foundations,
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 173
Author's personal copy
empirical evidence and practical considerations. Australian and New Zealand
Journal of Psychiatry, 40(4), 285–294.
Altenmüller, E. (2009). Apollo’s gift and curse: Brain plasticity in musicians. Music &
Medicine, 70.
Angelakis, E., Lubar, J. F., Stathopoulou, S., & Kounios, J. (2004). Peak alpha
frequency: An electroencephalographic measure of cognitive preparedness.
Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(4), 887–897. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.clinph.2003.11.034.
Angelakis, E., Stathopoulou, S., Frymiare, J. L., Green, D. L., Lubar, J. F., & Kounios, J.
(2007). EEG neurofeedback: A brief overview and an example of peak alpha
frequency training for cognitive enhancement in the elderly. Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 21(1), 110–129.
Arendash, G. W., Garcia, M. F., Costa, D. A., Cracchiolo, J. R., Wefes, I. M., & Potter, H.
(2004). Environmental enrichment improves cognition in aged Alzheimer’s
transgenic mice despite stable [beta]-amyloid deposition. NeuroReport, 15(11),
1751–1754.
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559.
Baggett, K. M., Davis, B., Feil, E. G., Sheeber, L. B., Landry, S. H., Carta, J. J., et al. (2010).
Technologies for expanding the reach of evidence-based interventions:
Preliminary results for promoting social–emotional development in early
childhood. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 29(4), 226–238. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/0271121409354782.
Baijal, S., & Gupta, R. (2008). Meditation-based training: A possible intervention for
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry (Edgmont), 5(4), 48–55.
Ball, K., Berch, D. B., Helmers, K. F., Jobe, J. B., Leveck, M. D., Marsiske, M., et al.
(2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults. JAMA: The
Journal of the American Medical Association, 288(18), 2271–2281. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.288.18.2271.
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive
functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin,
121(1), 65–94.
Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis
and treatment (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (2001). Executive
functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant
disorder (ODD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(6), 541–556.
Barnett, W. S., Jung, K., Yarosz, D. J., Thomas, J., Hornbeck, A., Stechuk, R., et al.
(2008). Educational effects of the tools of the mind curriculum: A randomized
trial. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(3), 299–313. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.03.001.
Barnett, R., Maruff, P., Vance, A., Luk, E. S. L., Costin, J., Wood, C., et al. (2001).
Abnormal executive function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The
effect of stimulant medication and age on spatial working memory.
Psychological Medicine, 31(6), 1107–1115.
Beck, S. J., Hanson, C. A., Puffenberger, S. S., Benninger, K. L., & Benninger, W. B.
(2010). A controlled trial of working memory training for children and
adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
39(6), 825–836. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2010.517162.
Bejerot, S., Ryden, E. M., & Arlinde, C. M. (2010). Two-year outcome of treatment
with central stimulant medication in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder: A prospective study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 71(12),
1590–1597. http://dx.doi.org/10.4088/JCP.09m05168pur.
Belleville, S., Clément, F., Mellah, S., Gilbert, B., Fontaine, F., & Gauthier, S. (2011).
Training-related brain plasticity in subjects at risk of developing Alzheimer’s
disease. Brain, 134(6), 1623–1634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr037.
Benedetti, F., Mayberg, H. S., Wager, T. D., Stohler, Christian. S., & Zubieta, J.-K.
(2005). Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect. The Journal of
Neuroscience, 25(45), 10390–10402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3458-
05.2005.
Benedict, R. H. B., Harris, A. E., Markow, T., McCormick, J. A., Nuechterlein, K. H., &
Asarnow, R. F. (1994). Effects of attention training on information-processing in
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20(3), 537–546.
Bergman Nutley, S. (2011). Development and training of higher order cognitive
functions and their interrelations. Stockholm: Karolinska Institutet.
Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., & Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting
with nature. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1207–1212. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02225.x.
Berry, A. S., Zanto, T. P., Clapp, W. C., Hardy, J. L., Delahunt, Peter. B., Mahncke, H. W.,
et al. (2010). The influence of perceptual training on working memory in older
adults. PLoS ONE, 5(7), e11537.
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual
mind. Child Development, 70(3), 636–644. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-
8624.00046.
Bialystok, E. (2010). Bilingualism. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science,
1(4), 559–572. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.43.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified
antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6), 1341–1354. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1341.
Bialystok, E., & DePape, A. M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive
functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology – Human Perception and
Performance, 35(2), 565–574. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012735.
Bialystok, E., & Majumder, S. (1998). The relationship between bilingualism and the
development of cognitive processes in problem solving. Applied
Psycholinguistics, 19(01), 69–85. 10.1017/S0142716400010584.
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children:
Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science,
7(3), 325–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.x.
Bierman, K. L., Domitrovich, C. E., Nix, R. L., Gest, S. D., Welsh, J. A., Greenberg, M. T.,
et al. (2008). Promoting academic and social–emotional school readiness: The
head start REDI program. Child Development, 79(6), 1802–1817. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01227.x.
Bierman, K. L., Nix, R. L., Greenberg, M. T., Blair, C., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2008).
Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and
mediation in the head start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology,
20(3), 821–843. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000394.
Bledowski, C., Kaiser, J., & Rahm, B. (2010). Basic operations in working memory:
Contributions from functional imaging studies. Behavioural Brain Research,
214(2), 172–179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.041.
BrainMaster Technologies, Inc. (2009). Neurofeedback Systems. <http://
www.brainmaster.com/>.
Brennan, T. A., Rothman, D. J., Blank, L., Blumenthal, D., Chimonas, S. C., Cohen, J. J.,
et al. (2006). Health industry practices that create conflicts of interest. JAMA:
The Journal of the American Medical Association, 295(4), 429–433. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.295.4.429.
Brody, H. (2008). A reply to Thomas Stossel on the AMA-CEJA draft report: Risk-
benefit ratio’’. Medscape Journal of Medicine, 10(7), 154.
Buiza, C., Gonzalez, M. F., Facal, D., Martinez, V., Diaz, U., Etxaniz, A., et al. (2009).
Efficacy of cognitive training experiences in the elderly: Can technology help?
Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5614, 324–333.
Burke, C. (2010). Mindfulness-based approaches with children and adolescents: A
preliminary review of current research in an emergent field. Journal of Child and
Family Studies, 19(2), 133–144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10826-009-9282-x.
Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2010). Improving intelligence: A literature review.
Swiss Medical Weekly, 140(19–20), 266–272.
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in
anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222.
Calero, M. D., & Navarro, E. (2007). Cognitive plasticity as a modulating variable on
the effects of memory training in elderly persons. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 22(1), 63–72.
Callahan, C. M., Hall, K. S., Hui, S. L., Musick, B. S., Unverzagt, F. W., & Hendrie, H. C.
(1996). Relationship of age, education, and occupation with dementia among a
community-based sample of African Americans. Archives of Neurology, 53(2),
134–140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1996.00550020038013.
Cannonieri, G. C., Bonilha, L., Fernandes, P. T., Cendes, F., & Li, L. M. (2007). Practice
and perfect: Length of training and structural brain changes in experienced
typists. NeuroReport, 18(10), 1063–1066. 1010.1097/
WNR.1060b1013e3281a1030e1065.
Carlson, C. L., Tamm, L., & Gaub, M. (1997). Gender differences in children with
ADHD, ODD, and co-occurring ADHD/ODD identified in a school population.
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(12),
1706–1714. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199712000-00019.
Chan, A. S., Ho, Y.-C., & Cheung, M.-C. (1998). Music training improves verbal
memory. Nature, 396(6707), 128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/24075.
Charles, C., Louv, R., Bodner, L., Guns, B., & Stahl, D. (2009). Children and nature
2009: A Report on the movement to reconnect children to the natural world. In
Network (Ed.).
Chein, J. M., & Morrison, A. B. (2010a). Expanding the mind’s workspace: Training
and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 17(2), 193–199. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/pbr.17.2.193.
Chein, Jason, & Morrison, Alexandra (2010b). Expanding the mind’s workspace:
Training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(2), 193–199. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/
pbr.17.2.193.
Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (in press). Does mindfulness training improve
cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clinical
Psychology Review.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003.
Civic Results (2008). Community action guide: Building the children & nature
movement from the ground up. In Network (Ed.).
Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of
older adults. Psychological Science, 14(2), 125–130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
1467-9280.t01-1-01430.
Congedo, M., Lubar, J. F., & Joffe, D. (2004). Low-resolution electromagnetic
tomography neurofeedback. IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and
Rehabilitation Engineering, 12(4), 387–397. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/
tnsre.2004.840492.
Corporation, Posit Science (2010). Posit science brain training software. <http://
www.positscience.com/>.
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict
resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59–86. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013.
Credibility Crisis in Pediatric Psychiatry (2008). Nature Neuroscience, 11(9), 983.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn0908-983.
Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural
correlates of dispositional mindfulness during affect labeling. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 69(6), 560–565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/Psy.0b013e3180f6171f.
Croisile, B. (2006). Memory stimulation: Which scientific benefits? Which
exercises? Revue de Gériatrie, 31, 421–433.
Dandeneau, S. P., & Baldwin, M. W. (2004). The inhibition of socially rejecting
information among people with high versus low self-esteem: The role of
attentional bias and the effects of bias reduction training. Journal of Social and
174 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
Clinical Psychology, 23(4), 584–602. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.
23.4.584.40306.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual-differences in working memory
and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450–466.
Daughton, J., Liu, H., West, M., Swanson, D., & Kratochvil, C. J. (2010). Practical guide
to ADHD pharmacotherapy. Psychiatric Annals, 40(4), 210–217. http://
dx.doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20100330-03.
Davidson, R. J. (2010). Empirical explorations of mindfulness: Conceptual and
methodological conundrums. Emotion, 10(1), 8–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0018480.
Daviglus, M. L., Bell, C. C., Berrettini, W., Bowen, P. E., Connolly, E. S., Jr, Cox, N. J.,
et al. (2010). National institutes of health state-of-the-science conference
statement: Preventing Alzheimer disease and cognitive decline. Annals of
Internal Medicine, 153(3), 176–181.
Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., McDowell, J. E., Austin, B. P., Miller, P. H., Yanasak, N.
E., et al. (2011). Exercise improves executive function and achievement and
alters brain activation in overweight children: A randomized, controlled trial.
Health Psychology, 30(1), 91–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021766.
de Boo, G. M., & Prins, P. J. M. (2007). Social incompetence in children with ADHD:
Possible moderators and mediators in social-skills training. Clinical Psychology
Review, 27(1), 78–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.03.006.
De Jong-Hagelstein, M., Van De Sandt-Koenderman, W. M., Prins, N. D., Dippel, D. W.
J., Koudstaal, P. J., & Visch-Brink, E. G. (2011). Efficacy of early cognitive-
linguistic treatment and communicative treatment in aphasia after stroke: A
randomised controlled trial (RATS-2). Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
Psychiatry, 82(4), 399–404.
deCharms, R. C. (2008). Applications of real-time fMRI. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
9(9), 720–729. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2414.
Denison Pequotstepos Nature Center (2009). Nature based preschool parent
handbook. In Center (Ed.).
D’Esposito, M. (2007). From cognitive to neural models of working memory.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B – Biological Sciences, 362(1481),
761–772. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2086.
Diamond, A. (2011). Personal communication.
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program
improves cognitive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387–1388. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1126/science.1151148.
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function
development in children 4–12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959–964.
Douglas, V. I., Parry, P., Marton, P., & Garson, C. (1976). Assessment of a cognitive
training program for hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
4(4), 389–410.
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004).
Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training – Newly honed
juggling skills show up as a transient feature on a brain-imaging scan. Nature,
427(6972), 311–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/427311a.
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H. G., Winkler, J., Buchel, C., et al.
(2006). Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during
extensive learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(23), 6314–6317.
Driemeyer, J., Boyke, J., Gaser, C., B üchel, C., & May, A. (2008). Changes in gray
matter induced by learning – Revisited. PLoS ONE, 3(7).
Duff, K., Eckman, C., Zehr, C., Yu, X., Prada, C. M., Perez-Tur, J., et al. (1996). Increased
amyloid-b42(43) in brains of mice expressing mutant presenilin 1. Nature,
383(6602), 710–713.
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P.,
et al. (2008). School readiness and later achievement (vol. 43, p. 1428, 2007).
Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-
1649.44.1.217.
DuPaul, G. J., McGoey, K. E., Eckert, T. L., & VanBrakle, J. (2001). Preschool children
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Impairments in behavioral, social,
and school functioning. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 40(5), 508–515.
Easterbrook, P. J., Gopalan, R., Berlin, J. A., & Matthews, D. R. (1991). Publication bias
in clinical research. The Lancet, 337(8746), 867–872. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
0140-6736(91)90201-y.
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., & Taub, E. (1995). Increased
cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science,
270(5234), 305–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.270.5234.305.
Elia, J., Ambrosini, P. J., & Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Treatment of attention-deficit-
hyperactivity disorder. New England Journal of Medicine, 340(10), 780–788.
Ericsson, K. A., & Chase, W. G. (1982). Exceptional memory. American Scientist, 70(6),
607–615.
Ernst & Young LLP. (2010). Scientific Learning Corp. (NMS: SCIL). <http://
www.mergentonline.com/competitors.php?compnumber=97992>.
Fabiano, G. A., Chacko, A., Pelham, W. E., Robb, J., Walker, K. S., Wymbs, F., et al.
(2009). A comparison of behavioral parent training programs for fathers of
children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behavior Therapy, 40(2),
190–204.
Fan, J., Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Wu, Y., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Mapping the genetic
variation of executive attention onto brain activity. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(12), 7406–7411.
Faraone, S. V., & Biederman, J. (1998). Neurobiology of attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 44(10), 951–958.
Feldman, Marissa A., Storch, E. A., & Murphy, T. K. (2011). Application of habit
reversal training for the treatment of tics in early childhood. Clinical Case
Studies, 10(2), 173–183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1534650111400728.
Fernandez, A. (2008). The state of the brain fitness market 2008. In SharpBrains
(Ed.), Brain fitness series.<http://www.slideshare.net/AlvaroF/state-brain-
fitness-market-2008> [Retrieved].
Ferri, C. P., Prince, M., Brayne, C., Brodaty, H., Fratiglioni, L., Ganguli, M., et al. (2005).
Global prevalence of dementia: A Delphi consensus study. The Lancet,
366(9503), 2112–2117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(05)67889-0.
Fey, M. E., Finestack, L. H., Gajewski, B. J., Popescu, M., & Lewine, J. D. (2010). A
preliminary evaluation of fast forword-language as an adjuvant treatment in
language intervention. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 53(2),
430–449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0225.
Flook, L., Smalley, S. L., Kitil, M. J., Galla, B. M., Kaiser-Greenland, S., Locke, J., et al.
(2010). Effects of mindful awareness practices on executive functions in
elementary school children. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 26(1), 70–95.
Foer, J. (2011). Moonwalking with Einstein: The art and science of remembering
everything. New York: Penguin Press.
Forestry Commission Scotland (2009). Woods for learning: Increasing the use of
trees, woods, and forests for education. In Scotland (Ed.).
Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Fan, J., Wu, Y., Swanson, J., Pfaff, D., et al. (2002). Assessing
the molecular genetics of attention networks. BMC Neuroscience, 3(1), 14.
Fox, D. J., Tharp, D. F., & Fox, L. C. (2005). Neurofeedback: An alternative and
efficacious treatment for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Applied
Psychophysiology Biofeedback, 30(4), 365–373.
Fuchs, T., Birbaumer, N., Lutzenberger, W., Gruzelier, J. H., & Kaiser, J. (2003).
Neurofeedback treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in
children: A comparison with methylphenidate. Applied Psychophysiology
Biofeedback, 28(1), 1–12.
Galbiati, S., Recla, M., Pastore, V., Liscio, M., Bardoni, A., Castelli, E., et al. (2009).
Attention remediation following traumatic brain injury in childhood and
adolescence. Neuropsychology, 23(1), 40–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013409.
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-
musicians. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(27), 9240–9245.
Glasgow, R. E., Lichtenstein, E., & Marcus, A. C. (2003). Why don’t we see more
translation of health promotion research to practice? Rethinking the efficacy-
to-effectiveness transition. American Journal of Public Health, 93(8), 1261–1267.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/ajph.93.8.1261.
Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid
intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6(3), 316–322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/
nn1014.
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 67–104.
Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2008). Exercising your brain: A review of human brain
plasticity and training-induced learning. Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 692–701.
Gupta, R., & Kar, B. R. (2009). Development of attentional processes in ADHD and
normal children. In Narayanan (Ed.). Progress in brain research (Vol. 176,
pp. 259–276). Elsevier.
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing
brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 65–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.tics.2008.11.003.
Halperin, J. M., & Healey, D. M. (2011). The influences of environmental enrichment,
cognitive enhancement, and physical exercise on brain development: Can we
alter the developmental trajectory of ADHD? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Reviews, 35(3), 621–634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.006.
Hammond, D. C. (2006). What is neurofeedback? Journal of Neurotherapy, 10(4),
25–36.
Harlow, H. F., & Mears, C. (1979). The human model: Primate perspectives.
Washington, New York: V.H. Winston; distributed solely by Halsted Press.
Harman, C., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (1997). Distress and attention
interactions in early infancy. Motivation and Emotion, 21(1), 27–43.
Heinrich, H., Gevensleben, H., & Strehl, U. (2007). Annotation: Neurofeedback –
Train your brain to train behaviour. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
and Allied Disciplines, 48(1), 3–16.
Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart:
Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1),
58–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2298.
Himle, M. B., Woods, D. W., Piacentini, J. C., & Walkup, J. T. (2006). Brief review of
habit reversal training for Tourette syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 21(8),
719–725. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/08830738060210080101.
Ho, Y.-C., Cheung, M.-C., & Chan, A. S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not
visual memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children.
Neuropsychology, 17(3), 439–450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.439.
Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to
sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental
Science, 12(4), F9–F15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00848.x.
Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., Place, M., Dunning, D. L., Hilton, K. A., & Elliott, J. G.
(2010). Working memory deficits can be overcome: Impacts of training and
medication on working memory in children with ADHD. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 24(6), 827–836. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp. 1589.
Hölzel, B. K., Ott, U., Hempel, H., Hackl, A., Wolf, K., Stark, R., et al. (2007).
Differential engagement of anterior cingulate and adjacent medial frontal
cortex in adept meditators and non-meditators. Neuroscience Letters, 421(1),
16–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2007.04.074.
Illes, Judy, & Sahakian, B. J. (2011). The Oxford handbook of neuroethics. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Ipser, J., & Stein, D. J. (2007). Systematic review of pharmacotherapy of disruptive
behavior disorders in children and adolescents. Psychopharmacology (Berl),
191(1), 127–140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0537-6.
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 175
Author's personal copy
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid
intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(19), 6829–6833. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0801268105.
Jaeggi, S. M., Studer-Luethi, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y. F., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J.
(2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning –
Implications for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38(6), 625–635. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.09.001.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York, NY, US: Henry Holt
and Co..
Jarrold, C., & Towse, J. N. (2006). Individual differences in working memory.
Neuroscience, 139(1), 39–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.neuroscience.2005.07.002.
Jevning, R., Wallace, R. K., & Beidebach, M. (1992). The physiology of meditation – a
review – A wakeful hypometabolic integrated response. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 16(3), 415–424.
Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies
subsystems of attention. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2),
109–119.
Jobe, J. B., Smith, D. M., Ball, K., Tennstedt, S. L., Marsiske, M., Willis, S. L., et al.
(2001). Active: A cognitive intervention trial to promote independence in older
adults. Controlled Clinical Trials, 22(4), 453–479. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
s0197-2456(01)00139-8.
Jones, L. B., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Development of executive
attention in preschool children. Developmental Science, 6(5), 498–504.
Kanfer, F. H. (1970). Self-monitoring – methodological limitations and clinical
applications. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 35(2), 148.
Kanske, P., Heissler, J., Schönfelder, S., Bongers, A., & Wessa, M. (2011). How to
regulate emotion? Neural networks for reappraisal and distraction. Cerebral
Cortex, 21(6), 1379–1388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq216.
Kaplan, S. (1995a). The restorative benefits of nature – Toward an integrative
framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15(3), 169–182.
Kaplan, S. (1995b). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative
framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15(3), 169–182. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2.
Karoly, P. (1993). Mechanisms of self-regulation – A systems view. Annual Review of
Psychology, 44, 23–52.
Kassirer, J. P. (2009a). Commentary: Disclosure’s failings: What is the alternative?
Academic Medicine, 84(9), 1180–1181. http://dx.doi.org/1110.1097/
ACM.1180b1013e3181b1117e1193.
Kassirer, J. P. (2009b). Medicine’s obsession with disclosure of financial conflicts: Fixing
the wrong problem science and the media. London: Academic Press.
Kelly, B., Longbottom, J., Potts, F., & Williamson, J. (2004). Applying emotional
intelligence: Exploring the promoting alternative thinking strategies
curriculum. Educational Psychology in Practice: Theory, Research and Practice in
Educational Psychology, 20(3), 221–240.
Kermen, R., Hickner, J., Brody, H., & Hasham, I. (2010). Family physicians believe the
placebo effect is therapeutic but often use real drugs as placebos. Family
Medicine, 42(9), 636–642.
Kerns, K. A., Eso, K., & Thomson, J. (1999). Investigation of a direct intervention for
improving attention in young children with ADHD. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 16(2), 273–295.
Kesler, S. R., Lacayo, N. J., & Jo, B. (2010). A pilot study of an online cognitive
rehabilitation program for executive function skills in children with cancer-
related brain injury. Brain Injury, 25(1), 101–112. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/
02699052.2010.536194.
Kieling, C., Goncalves, R. R. F., Tannock, R., & Castellanos, F. X. (2008). Neurobiology
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics
of North America, 17(2), 285–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2007.11.012.
Klingberg, T. (2007). Computerized training of working memory in children with
ADHD. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 17, S192–S193.
Klingberg, T. (2008). Computerized training of working memory. A promising
therapeutic strategy for ADHD. L’allenamento computerizzato della memoria
operativa. Una promettente strategia terapeutica nell’ADHD, 30(1), 25–26.
Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 14(7), 317–324.
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K., et al.
(2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD – A
randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177–186.
Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in
children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(6),
781–791.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early
childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social
development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220–232.
Kohen, D. P., & Olness, K. (2011). Hypnosis and hypnotherapy with children. New
York: Routledge.
Kotchoubey, B., Strehl, U., Holzapfel, S., Blankenhorn, V., Fröscher, W., & Birbaumer,
N. (1999). Negative potential shifts and the prediction of the outcome of
neurofeedback therapy in epilepsy. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110(4), 683–686.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00005-x.
Kozasa, E. H., Sato, J. R., Lacerda, S. S., Barreiros, M. A. M., Radvany, J., Russell, T. A.,
et al. (in press). Meditation training increases brain efficiency in an attention
task. Neuroimage.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.088.
Kraus, N., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of
auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(8), 599–605. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1038/nrn2882.
Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. M. (2010). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A
transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kubesch, S., Walk, L., Spitzer, M., Kammer, T., Lainburg, A., Heim, R., et al. (2009). A
30-minute physical education program improves students’ executive attention.
Mind, Brain, and Education, 3(4), 235–242.
Kumon North America, Inc. (2011). Kumon: Math. Reading. Success. <http://
www.kumon.com/>.
Kuo, H. K., Jones, R. N., Milberg, W. P., Tennstedt, S., Talbot, L., Morris, J. N., et al.
(2005). Effect of blood pressure and diabetes mellitus on cognitive and physical
functions in older adults: A longitudinal analysis of the advanced cognitive
training for independent and vital elderly cohort. Journal of the American
Geriatrics Society, 53(7), 1154–1161.
Langhorne, P., Bernhardt, J., & Kwakkel, G. (2011). Stroke rehabilitation. The Lancet,
377(9778), 1693–1702.
Laurin, D., Verreault, R., Lindsay, J., MacPherson, K., & Rockwood, K. (2001). Physical
activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons.
Archives of Neurology, 58(3), 498–504.
Lazarov, O., Robinson, J., Tang, Y.-P., Hairston, I. S., Korade-Mirnics, Z., Lee, V. M. Y.,
et al. (2005). Environmental enrichment reduces A[beta] levels and amyloid
deposition in transgenic mice. Cell, 120(5), 701–713. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.cell.2005.01.015.
Lenroot, R. K., & Giedd, J. N. (2008). The changing impact of genes and environment
on brain development during childhood and adolescence: Initial findings from a
neuroimaging study of pediatric twins. Development and Psychopathology, 20(4),
1161–1175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0954579408000552.
Lexchin, J., Bero, L. A., Djulbegovic, B., & Clark, O. (2003). Pharmaceutical industry
sponsorship and research outcome and quality: Systematic review. British
Medical Journal, 326(7400), 1167–1170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/
bmj.326.7400.1167.
Liew, J. (2011). Effortful control, executive functions, and education: bringing self-
regulatory and social–emotional competencies to the table. Child Development
Perspectives.
Li-Grining, C. P., Votruba-Drzal, E., Maldonado-Carreno, C., & Haas, K. (2010).
Children’s early approaches to learning and academic trajectories through fifth
grade. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1062–1077. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0020066.
Lincoln, N. B., Majid, M. J., & Weyman, N. (2000). Cognitive rehabilitation for
attention deficits following stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (4).
Liu, D., Diorio, J., Day, J. C., Francis, D. D., & Meaney, M. J. (2000). Maternal care,
hippocampal synaptogenesis and cognitive development in rats. Nature
Neuroscience, 3(8), 799–806. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/77702.
Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freedman, A., et al. (1997).
Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-
pituitary–adrenal responses to stress. Science, 277(5332), 1659–1662. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.277.5332.1659.
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Cognitive–emotional
interactions – Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163–169.
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Rawlings, N. B., Francis, A. D., Greischar, L. L., & Davidson, R. J.
(2009). Mental training enhances attentional stability: Neural and behavioral
evidence. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(42), 13418–13427. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1523/jneurosci.1614-09.2009.
Mackey, A. P., Hill, S. S., Stone, S. I., & Bunge, S. A. (2010). Differential effects of
reasoning and speed training in children. Developmental Science.http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01005.x.
Maguire, E. A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Frith, C. D., & O’Keefe, J.
(1998). Knowing where and getting there: A human navigation network.
Science, 280(5365), 921–924. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.280.5365.921.
Maguire, E. A., Valentine, E. R., Wilding, J. M., & Kapur, N. (2003). Routes to
remembering: The brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience, 6(1),
90–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn988.
Maguire, E. A., Woollett, K., & Spiers, H. J. (2006). London taxi drivers and bus
drivers: A structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus, 16(12),
1091–1101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20233.
Marg, E. (1952). Flashes of clear vision and negative accommodation with reference
to the Bates method of visual training. American Journal of Optometry & Archives
of American Academy of Optometry, 29(4), 167–184.
Martin, A. (2009). Brain fitness industry set to boom. MarketWatch. <http://
articles.moneycentral.msn.com/Insurance/InsureYourHealth/brain-fitness-
industry-set-to-boom.aspx> [Retrieved].
Martin, G., & Johnson, C. L. (2006). The Boys totem town neurofeedback project: A
pilot study of EEG biofeedback with incarcerated juvenile felons. Journal of
Neurotherapy, 9(3), 71–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J184v09n03_05.
McBurnett, K., & Pfiffner, L. J. (2008). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Concepts,
controversies, new directions. New York: Informa Healthcare.
McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M. D., Farris, C. L., Jewkes, A. M., &
Morrison, F. J. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’
literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 947–959.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947.
McEwen, S. E., Polatajko, H. J., Davis, J. A., Huijbregts, M., & Ryan, J. D. (2010). ‘There’s
a real plan here, and I am responsible for that plan’: Participant experiences
with a novel cognitive-based treatment approach for adults living with chronic
176 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
stroke. Disability and Rehabilitation, 32(7), 541–550. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/
09638280903180189.
McKeith, I. G., Galasko, D., Kosaka, K., Perry, E. K., Dickson, D. W., Hansen, L. A., et al.
(1996). Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of
dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): Report of the consortium on DLB
international workshop. Neurology, 47(5), 1113–1124.
Medalia, A., Aluma, M., Tryon, W., & Merriam, A. E. (1998). Effectiveness of attention
training in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24(1), 147–152.
Merzenich, M. (2011). Personal Communication.
Merzenich, M. M., Jenkins, W. M., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S. L., & Tallal, P.
(1996). Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children
ameliorated by training. Science, 271(5245), 77–81.
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental
properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of
young, urban children. Child Development, 75(5), 1373–1386.
Monastra, V. J. (2005). Electroencephalographic biofeedback (neurotherapy) as a
treatment for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Rationale and empirical
foundation. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14(1),
55–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2004.07.004.
Munte, T. F., Altenmuller, E., & Jancke, L. (2002). The musician’s brain as a model of
neuroplasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(6), 473–478. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1038/nrn843.
Murray-Close, D., Hoza, B., Hinshaw, S. P., Arnold, L. E., Swanson, J., Jensen, P. S., et al.
(2010). Developmental processes in peer problems of children with attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the multimodal treatment study of children
with ADHD: Developmental cascades and vicious cycles. Development and
Psychopathology, 22(4), 785–802. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
s0954579410000465.
Nacke, L. E., Nacke, A., & Lindley, C. A. (2009). Brain training for silver gamers:
Effects of age and game form on effectiveness, efficiency, self-assessment, and
gameplay experience. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 12(5), 493–499.
Naropa University (2011). Master of arts in transpersonal counseling psychology:
Wilderness therapy. In University (Ed.).
Nintendo, D. S. (2007). Brain age 2. <http://brainage.com/launch/index.jsp>.
Noble, K. G., McCandliss, B. D., & Farah, M. J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients
predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science,
10(4), 464–480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x.
Olesen, P. J., Westerberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2004). Increased prefrontal and parietal
activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 7(1), 75–79.
Ott, U., Hölzel, B.K., Vaitl, D. (2011). Brain structure and meditation. How spiritual
practice shapes the brain. Paper presented at the differences, Freiburg/Breisgau
2008.
Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., et al.
(2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465(7299), 775–U776. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature09042.
Pagnoni, G., Cekic, M., & Guo, Y. (2008). ‘‘Thinking about not-thinking’’: neural
correlates of conceptual processing during zen meditation. PLoS ONE, 3(9),
e3083.
Papp, K. V., Walsh, S. J., & Snyder, P. J. (2009). Immediate and delayed effects of
cognitive interventions in healthy elderly: A review of current literature and
future directions. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 5(1), 50–60. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.jalz.2008.10.008.
Park, N. W., & Ingles, J. L. (2001). Effectiveness of attention rehabilitation after an
acquired brain injury: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 15(2), 199–210. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.15.2.199.
Park, M. H., Kwon, D. Y., Seo, W. K., Lim, K. S., & Song, M. S. (2009). The effects of
cognitive training on community-dwelling elderly Koreans. Journal of
Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 16(10), 904–909. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/j.1365-2850.2009.01467.x.
Paul, G. L. (1969). Physiological effects of relaxation training and hypnotic
suggestion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 74(4), 425.
Pearson (2011). Cogmed. <http://www.cogmed.com/>.
Pelc, K., Kornreich, C., Foisy, M. L., & Dan, B. (2006). Recognition of emotional facial
expressions in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatric Neurology,
35(2), 93–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.peidatrneurol.2006.01.014.
Peterson, B. S., & Cohen, D. J. (1998). The treatment of Tourette’s syndrome:
Multimodal, developmental intervention. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59,
62–74.
Phelps, L. A. (2008). Tourette’s disorder: Genetic update, neurological correlates,
and evidence-based interventions. School Psychology Quarterly, 23(2), 282–289.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1045-3830.23.2.282.
Piacentini, J., Woods, D. W., Scahill, L., Wilhelm, S., Peterson, A. L., Chang, S., et al.
(2010). Behavior therapy for children with Tourette disorder a randomized
controlled trial. [article]. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association,
303(19), 1929–1937.
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Voelker, P. (2011). Control networks
and neuromodulators of early development. Developmental Psychology.
Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain.
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007a). Research on attention networks as a model
for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007b). Educating the human brain (1st ed.).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Brain states and hypnosis research.
Consciousness and Cognition, 20(2), 325–327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.concog.2009.11.008.
Pressley, M. (1979). Increasing children’s self-control through cognitive
interventions. Review of Educational Research, 49(2), 319–370.
Rabiner, D. L., Murray, D. W., Skinner, A. T., & Malone, P. S. (2010). A randomized
trial of two promising computer-based interventions for students with
attention difficulties. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(1), 131–142.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9353-x.
Raffone, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). The exploration of meditation in the
neuroscience of attention and consciousness. Cognitive Processing, 11(1), 1–7.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-009-0354-z.
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., Levine, L. J., Wright, E. L., Dennis, W. R., & Newcomb, R. L.
(1997). Music training causes long-term enhancement of preschool children’s
spatial–temporal reasoning. Neurological Research, 19(1), 2–8.
Raz, A. (in press). Translational attention: From experiments in the lab to helping
the symptoms of individuals with Tourette’s syndrome. Consciousness and
Cognition.
Raz, A., & Buhle, J. (2006). Typologies of attentional networks. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 7(5), 367–379.
Raz, A., Keller, S., Norman, K., & Senechal, D. (2007). Elucidating Tourette’s
syndrome: Perspectives from hypnosis, attention and self-regulation. The
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 49(4), 289–309.
Raz, A., Marinoff, G. P., Zephrani, Z. R., Schweizer, H. R., & Posner, M. I. (2004). See
clearly: Suggestion, hypnosis, attention, and visual acuity. International Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 52(2), 159–187.
Raz, A. (2004). Atypical attention: Hypnosis and conflict reduction. In Posner (Ed.),
Cognitive neuroscience of attention. New York: Guilford Press.
Robertson, M. M. (2003). Diagnosing Tourette syndrome – Is it a common disorder?
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55(1), 3–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0022-
3999(02)00580-9.
Robertson, I. H., Tegner, R., Tham, K., Lo, A., & Nimmosmith, I. (1995). Sustained
attention training for unilateral neglect – Theoretical and rehabilitation
implications. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17(3),
416–430.
Rodriguez, A., Jaervelin, M. R., Obel, C., Taanila, A., Miettunen, J., Moilanen, I., et al.
(2007). Do inattention and hyperactivity symptoms equal scholastic
impairment? Evidence from three European cohorts. Bmc Public Health, 7.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-7-327.
Rossiter, T. R., & LaVaqe, T. J. (1995). A comparison of EEG biofeedback and
psychostimulants in treating attention deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Journal of
Neurotherapy, 1(1), 48–59.
Rubia, K. (2009). The neurobiology of meditation and its clinical effectiveness in
psychiatric disorders. Biological Psychology, 82(1), 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.04.003.
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., et al.
(2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia,
42(8), 1029–1040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive
attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 28(2), 573–594.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Davis-Stober, C. P. (2004). Development
of the time course for processing conflict: An event-related potentials study
with 4 year olds and adults. BMC Neuroscience, 5.http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/
1471-2202-5-39.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I.
(2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of
executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 102(41), 14931–14936. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/
pnas.0506897102.
Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early development: Themes and
variations. Oxford University Press.
Schachar, R. J., Tannock, R., & Logan, G. (1993). Inhibitory control, impulsiveness, and
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 13(8), 721–739.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 15(8),
511–514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00711.x.
Scholz, J., Klein, M. C., Behrens, T. E. J., & Johansen-Berg, H. (2009). Training induces
changes in white-matter architecture. Nature Neuroscience, 12(11), 1370–1371.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2412. <http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/
v12/n11/suppinfo/nn.2412_S1.html>.
Schultz Center for Teaching and Leadership (2009). Fast ForWord Longitudinal
Impact Study Jacksonville, FL. <http://www.schultzcenter.org/pdf/
fast_forword_brief.pdf> [Retrieved].
Scientific Learning Corporation (2011). Scientific learning: Fit brains learn better.
<http://www.scilearn.com/>.
Semrud-Clikeman, M., & Ellison, P. A. T. (2009). Language-related and learning
disorders child neuropsychology. US: Springer, pp. 275–327.
Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., & Davidson, R. J.
(2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the
cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(3), 154–167. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1038/nrn2994. <http://www.nature.com/nrn/journal/v12/n3/suppinfo/
nrn2994_S1.html>.
Shalev, L., Tsal, Y., & Mevorach, C. (2007). Computerized progressive attentional
training (CPAT) program: Effective direct intervention for children with ADHD.
Child Neuropsychology, 13(4), 382–388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
09297040600770787.
Shaw, C. A., Lanius, R. A., & Vandendoel, K. (1994). The origin of synaptic
neuroplasticity – Crucial molecules or a dynamical cascade. Brain Research
Reviews, 19(3), 241–263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0165-0173(94)90014-0.
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 177
Author's personal copy
Sheese, B. E., Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., White, L. K., & Fraundorf, S. H. (2008).
Executive attention and self-regulation in infancy. Infant Behavior and
Development, 31(3), 501–510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.02.001.
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2010). Does working memory training
generalize? Psychologica Belgica, 50(3–4), 245–276.
Singer, H. S., & Walkup, J. T. (1991). Tourette syndrome and other tic disorders –
Diagnosis, pathophysiology and treatment. Medicine, 70(1), 15–32.
Singh, N., Singh, A., Lancioni, G., Singh, J., Winton, A., & Adkins, A. (2010).
Mindfulness training for parents and their children with ADHD increases the
children’s compliance. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19(2), 157–166. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10826-009-9272-z.
Skidmore, E. R., Holm, M. B., Whyte, E. M., Dew, M. A., Dawson, D., & Becker, J. T.
(2011). The feasibility of meta-cognitive strategy training in acute inpatient
stroke rehabilitation: Case report. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 21(2),
208–223.
Slagter, H. A., Davidson, R. J., & Lutz, A. (2011). Mental training as a tool in the
neuroscientific study of brain and cognitive plasticity [hypothesis & theory].
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5.http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00017.
Smith, G. E., Housen, P., Yaffe, K., Ruff, R., Kennison, R. F., Mahncke, H. W., et al.
(2009). A cognitive training program based on principles of brain plasticity:
Results from the improvement in memory with plasticity-based adaptive
cognitive training (IMPACT) study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,
57(4), 594–603. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02167.x.
Snyder, P. (2011). Personal Communication.
Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (1987). Effectiveness of an attention-training
program. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9(2), 117–130.
Sokhadze, T. M., Cannon, R. L., & Trudeau, D. L. (2008). EEG biofeedback as a
treatment for substance use disorders: Review, rating of efficacy, and
recommendations for further research. Applied Psychophysiology Biofeedback,
33(1), 1–28.
Solanto, M., Newcorn, J., Vail, L., Gilbert, S., Ivanov, I., & Lara, R. (2009). Stimulant
drug response in the predominantly inattentive and combined subtypes of
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent
Psychopharmacology, 19(6), 663–671. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cap.2009.0033.
Spencer, T. J., Biederman, J., Wilens, T. E., & Faraone, S. V. (2002). Overview and
neurobiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry, 63, 3–9.
Steele, K. M., Bass, K. E., & Crook, M. D. (1999). The mystery of the Mozart effect:
Failure to replicate. Psychological Science, 10(4), 366–369. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/1467-9280.00169.
Steinhausen, H. C. (2009). The heterogeneity of causes and courses of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 120(5), 392–399.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01446.x.
Stevens, C., Fanning, J., Coch, D., Sanders, L., & Neville, H. (2008). Neural mechanisms
of selective auditory attention are enhanced by computerized training:
Electrophysiological evidence from language-impaired and typically
developing children. Brain Research, 1205, 55–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.brainres.2007.10.108.
Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., & Neville, H. (2009). Differences in the neural mechanisms
of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: An
event-related brain potential study. Developmental Science, 12(4), 634–646.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00807.x.
Stossel, T. P. (2007). Regulation of financial conflicts of interest in medical practice
and medical research – A damaging solution in search of a problem. Perspectives
in Biology and Medicine, 50(1), 54–71.
Stossel, T. P. (2008). A biopsy of financial conflicts of interest in medicine. Surgery,
143(2), 193–198. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.surg.2007.11.013.
Strine, T. W., Lesesne, C. A., Okoro, C. A., McGuire, L. C., Chapman, D. P., Balluz, L. S.,
et al. (2006). Emotional and behavioral difficulties and impairments in everyday
functioning among children with a history of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. Preventing Chronic Disease, 3(2), A52.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
Stuss, D. T., Winocur, G., & Robertson, I. H. (1999). Cognitive neurorehabilitation.
Cambridge University Press.
Swain, J. E., Scahill, L., Lombroso, P. J., King, R. A., & Leckman, J. F. (2007). Tourette
syndrome and tic disorders: A decade of progress. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(8), 947–968. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1097/chi.0b013e318068fbcc.
Swanson, J., Baler, R. D., & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Understanding the effects of
stimulant medications on cognition in individuals with attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder: A decade of progress. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(1),
207–226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.160.
Sylvan Learning (2011). Sylvan learning. <http://tutoring.sylvanlearning.com/
index.cfm>.
Szobot, C. M., Roman, T., Hutz, M. H., Genro, J. P., Shih, M. C., Hoexter, M. Q., et al.
(2011). Molecular imaging genetics of methylphenidate response in ADHD and
substance use comorbidity. Synapse (New York, NY), 65(2), 154–159. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1002/syn.20829.
Tamm, L., McCandliss, B., Liang, A., Wigal, T. L., Posner, M. I., & Swanson, J. M. (2008).
Can attention itself be trained? Attention training for children at-risk for ADHD.
In McBurnett & Pfiffer (Eds.), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Concepts,
controversies, new directions. New York, NY: Informa Healthcare.
Tan, G., Thornby, J., Hammond, D. C., Strehl, U., Canady, B., Arnemann, K., et al.
(2009). Meta-analysis of EEG biofeedback in treating epilepsy. Clinical EEG and
Neuroscience, 40(3), 173–179.
Tang, Y. Y., Lu, Q. L., Geng, X. J., Stein, E. A., Yang, Y. H., & Posner, M. I. (2010). Short-
term meditation induces white matter changes in the anterior cingulate.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
107(35), 15649–15652. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1011043107.
Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y. H., Wang, J., Fan, Y. X., Feng, S. G., Lu, Q. L., et al. (2007). Short-term
meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(43),
17152–17156.
Tang, Y. Y., & Posner, M. I. (2009). Attention training and attention state training.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(5), 222–227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.tics.2009.01.009.
Taylor, A. F., & Kuo, F. E. (2009). Children with attention deficits concentrate better
after walk in the park. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12(5), 402–409. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/1087054708323000.
Taylor, A. F., Kuo, F. E., & Sullivan, W. C. (2001). Coping with add. Environment and
Behavior, 33(1), 54–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00139160121972864.
Taylor, A. F., Kuo, F. E., & Sullivan, W. C. (2002). Views of nature and self-discipline:
Evidence from inner city children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22(1–2),
49–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jevp. 2001.0241.
Teicher, M. H., Andersen, S. L., Polcari, A., Anderson, C. M., Navalta, C. P., & Kim, D. M.
(2003). The neurobiological consequences of early stress and childhood
maltreatment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(1–2), 33–44. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00007-1.
Tesky, V. A., Thiel, C., Banzer, W., & Pantel, J. (2011). Effects of a group program to
increase cognitive performance through cognitively stimulating leisure
activities in healthy older subjects. GeroPsych: The Journal of
Gerontopsychology and Geriatric Psychiatry, 24(2), 83–92.
The MTA CooperativeGroup (1999). A 14-monthrandomized clinicaltrial of treatment
strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 56(12), 1073–1086. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073.
Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Bergman Nutley, S., Bohlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009).
Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children.
Developmental Science, 12(1), 106–113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
7687.2008.00745.x.
Tilburt, J. C., Emanuel, E. J., Kaptchuk, T. J., Curlin, F. A., & Miller, F. G. (2008).
Prescribing ‘‘placebo treatments’’: Results of national survey of US internists
and rheumatologists. British Medical Journal, 337.http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/
bmj.a1938.
Tsianos, N., Germanakos, P., Lekkas, Z., Mourlas, C., & Samaras, G. (2010). Working
memory span and e-learning: The effect of personalization techniques on
learners’ performance. User Modeling, Adaptation, and Personalization,
Proceedings, 6075, 64–74.
Tuckman, B. W., & Hinkle, J. S. (1986). An experimental study of the physical and
psychological effects of aerobic exercise on schoolchildren. Health Psychology,
5(3), 197–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.5.3.197.
Turner, E. H., Matthews, A. M., Linardatos, E., Tell, R. A., & Rosenthal, R. (2008).
Selective publication of antidepressant trials and its influence on apparent
efficacy. New England Journal of Medicine, 358(3), 252–260. 10.1056/
NEJMsa065779.
Valentine, E. R., & Sweet, P. L. G. (1999). Meditation and attention: A comparison of
the effects of concentrative and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention.
Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 2(1), 59–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
13674679908406332.
Wallis, D., Russell, H. F., & Muenke, M. (2008). Review: Genetics of attention deficit/
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 33(10), 1085–1099.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsn049.
Walsh, R., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). The meeting of meditative disciplines and Western
psychology – A mutually enriching dialogue. American Psychologist, 61(3),
227–239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.61.3.227.
Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D’Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R.,
et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature
Neuroscience, 7(8), 847–854. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1276. <http://
www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v7/n8/suppinfo/nn1276_S1.html>.
Wells, N. M. (2000). At home with nature: Effects of ‘‘greenness’’ on children’s
cognitive functioning. Environment and Behavior, 32(6), 775–795. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/00139160021972793.
Wells, K. C. (2008). Parent training in the treatment of ADHD. Attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder: Concepts, controversies, new directions: Informa
Healthcare.
Wells, N. M., & Evans, G. W. (2003). Nearby nature. Environment and Behavior, 35(3),
311–330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013916503035003001.
Westerberg, H., Jacobaeus, H., Hirvikoski, T., Clevberger, P., Östensson, M. L., Bartfai,
A., et al. (2007). Computerized working memory training after stroke – A pilot
study. Brain Injury, 21(1), 21–29.
Westerberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2007). Changes in cortical activity after training of
working memory – A single-subject analysis. Physiology & Behavior, 92(1–2),
186–192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.041.
Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint attention training for children with
autism using behavior modification procedures. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44(3), 456–468.
Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., & Hulslander, J. (2005).
Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: In search of the common deficit.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(1), 35–78.
Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., & DeFries, J. C. (2007). Understanding
comorbidity: A twin study of reading disability and attention-deficit/
178 S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179
Author's personal copy
hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B –
Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 144B(6), 709–714. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
ajmg.b.30310.
Wolinsky, F. D., Unverzagt, F. W., Smith, D. M., Jones, R., Wright, E., & Tennstedt, S. L.
(2006). The effects of the ACTIVE cognitive training trial on clinically relevant
declines in health-related quality of life. Journals of Gerontology – Series B
Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61(5), S281–S287.
Woods, D. W., Piacentini, J. C., Scahill, L., Peterson, A. L., Wilhelm, S., Chang, S., et al.
(2011). Behavior therapy for tics in children: Acute and long-term effects on
psychiatric and psychosocial functioning. Journal of Child Neurology, 26(7),
858–865. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0883073810397046.
Yaffe, K., Barnes, D., Nevitt, M., Lui, L. Y., & Covinsky, K. (2001). A prospective study
of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: Women who walk.
Archives of Internal Medicine, 161(14), 1703–1708.
Zanetti, O., Binetti, G., Magni, E., Rozzini, L., Bianchetti, A., & Trabucchi, M. (1997).
Procedural memory stimulation in Alzheimer’s disease: Impact of a training
programme. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 95(3), 152–157. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/j.1600-0404.1997.tb00087.x.
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Gordon, N. S., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Effects of brief and
sham mindfulness meditation on mood and cardiovascular variables. The
Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 16(8), 867–873. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1089/acm.2009.0321.
Zylowska, L., Ackerman, D. L., Yang, M. H., Futrell, J. L., Horton, N. L., Hale, T. S., et al.
(2008). Mindfulness meditation training in adults and adolescents with ADHD a
feasibility study. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11(6), 737–746. 10.1177/
1087054707308502.
S. Rabipour, A. Raz/ Brain and Cognition 79 (2012) 159–179 179
... Some studies show promising results with cognitive training [21], metacognitive interventions [22], music [23], and video games [24]. However, the overall results show small effect sizes and difficulties in maintaining long-term benefits and transferability to daily life (generalizability and transferability) [25]. More evidence is needed with greater control of risk biases. ...
... The use of a computer allows more accessibility. The game is set in a natural environment, potentially beneficial for people with ADHD with animals that accompany the player throughout the training session [25]. Various games or tasks are incorporated into the game design with the purpose of training the most affected cognitive functions in ADHD. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most common neurodevelopmental disorder in childhood and adolescence, with a prevalence of 5% and associated difficulties and worse prognosis if undetected. Multimodal treatment is the treatment of choice. However, sometimes treatment can be insufficient or have drawbacks. Objective This study protocol aims to demonstrate the effectiveness of cognitive training through the serious video game The Secret Trail of Moon (MOON) in improving emotional regulation in people with ADHD. Methods This is a prospective, unicenter, randomized, unblinded, pre- and postintervention study. The groups will be randomized (MOON vs control) via an electronic case report form. The MOON intervention will be performed 2 times per week for 10 weeks (30 minutes per session). The first 5 weeks (10 sessions) will be conducted face-to-face at the Puerta de Hierro University Hospital, and the remaining weeks will be conducted via the internet at the participants’ homes. The total sample consists of 152 patients aged between 7 and 18 years. All participants have a clinical diagnosis of ADHD under pharmacological treatment. Data collection will be used to obtain demographic and clinical data. The data will be recorded using REDCap. Measures will be made through clinical scales for parents and objective tests of cognitive functioning in patients. Additional information on academic performance will be collected. The study has a power greater than 80% to detect differences. Student t test, 2-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA), and Mann-Whitney analyses will be performed according to each variable’s characteristics. Results The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Puerta de Hierro University Hospital on December 14, 2022. As of September 26, 2023, we have enrolled 62 participants, and 31 participants have completed the study. This clinical trial was funded by the Comunidad de Madrid (IND2020/BMD-17544). The approximate completion date is March 2024. Conclusions Serious video games such as MOON can be motivational tools that complement multimodal treatment for ADHD. Trial Registration ClinicalTrials.gov; NCT06006871; https://clinicaltrials.gov/study/NCT06006871 International Registered Report Identifier (IRRID) DERR1-10.2196/53191
... Cognitive training paradigms consist of standardized exercise sequences that are applied repeatedly and standardized as much as possible to regain a certain cognitive function by focusing directly on the function in which the deficit is observed (Clare andWoods 2004, Rabipour andRaz 2012). It is assumed that cognitive exercises based on continuity have the potential to improve performance in the relevant functional area and that this effect can be generalized beyond the gains achieved (Bahar-Fuchs et al. 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Aging is a life stage in which progressive deterioration occurs in biological, psychological, and socio-cultural processes. Therefore, along with all the other changes observed in aging, cognitive change is inevitable. In older adulthood, the speed of processing information, the ability to remember contextual information such as where and when events occur, and executive function performance are impaired. Moreover, this change in cognitive processes causes the deterioration of functionality in daily life. Although it is well known that physical activity, nutrition, and social support play a key role in preventing the adverse effects of aging, the impact of cognitive training and rehabilitation have been relatively less studied. This review aims to examine cognitive training and rehabilitation practices applied to different cognitive processes (episodic memory, working memory, executive functions, attention and processing speed) to help compensate for or regain cognitive functions that are impaired in older adults. In this context, the effectiveness of the practices, the transfer of gains to different cognitive areas, and whether they are preserved for long periods were examined. The contribution of conscious and systematic practices, such as cognitive training and rehabilitation, in reducing the adverse effects of aging has been discussed.
... It is hypothesised that cognitive training could be beneficial as ADHD is associated with cognitive deficits [34]. Cognitive training refers to the use of a specific program or activity to improve cognitive functioning through repetition of exercise over several weeks [35]. The proposed exercises specifically target one or more cognitive functions and are usually tailored to the individual's performance [36]. ...
Article
Full-text available
tAttention Deficit Disorder with or without Hyperactivity (ADHD is a neurodevelopmental disorder which affects the day-to-day functioning of children and adults with this condition. Pharmacological treatment can reduce the symptoms associated with ADHD, but it has some limitations. The objective of this symposium is to determine the effects of non-pharmacological approaches on ADHD symptoms. Results indicate that the following intervention are promising approaches: cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT),mindfulness-based interventions (MBI), yoga, cognitive and metacognitive intervention, neurofeedback and parental training programs. Current research advocates multimodal approaches in conjunction with school or work accommodations integrating innovative technologies.
... Consequently, many patients and/or their parents opt not to take such medications, resulting in high rates of treatment discontinuation [66]. This has prompted healthcare professionals, patients, and parents to seek non-pharmacological treatment options [49,50,57]. ...
... Some research shows promising results with cognitive training 16 , metacognitive interventions 17 , music 18 or video games 19 . However, the overall results show small effect sizes and difficulties in maintaining long-term benefits and transferability to daily life (generalizability and transferability) 20 . More evidence is needed with a greater control of risk biases. ...
Preprint
BACKGROUND Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is the most common neurodevelopmental disorder in childhood and adolescence (5%) with associated difficulties and worse prognosis if undetected. Multimodal treatment is the treatment of choice; however, sometimes treatment can be insufficient or have some drawbacks. OBJECTIVE To demonstrate the effectiveness of cognitive training through the serious video game “The Secret Trail of Moon” (MOON) in improving emotional regulation of people with ADHD. METHODS Design: prospective, unicenter, randomized, unblinded, PRE-POST intervention study. Randomization of the groups (MOON vs. Control) will be performed by electronic CRD. The MOON intervention will be performed 2 times per week for 10 weeks (30 minutes per session). The first five weeks (10 sessions) will be conducted face-to-face in Puerta de Hierro University Hospital; the remaining weeks will be conducted online at the participants' home. The clinical trial was registered in Clinical Trials NCT06006871. Sample: 152 patients aged between 7 and 18 years. All participants had a clinical diagnosis of ADHD (CGI between 3 and 6) under pharmacological treatment. Evaluation: Data collection will be used to obtain demographic and clinical data. The data will be recorded with an electronic CRD (REDCap). Measures will be made through clinical scales for parents and objective tests of cognitive functioning in patients. Additional information on academic performance will be collected. Statistical power analysis: The study has a power greater than 80% to detect differences. Statistical analysis: Classical statistics: T student, 2-factor ANOVA and Mann Whitney analyses will be performed according to the characteristics of each variable. Ethics: The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Puerta de Hierro University Hospital on December 14th, 2022. The authorization of the Spanish Agency of Medicines and Health Products was February 14th, 2023. Informed consent will be requested from legal guardians and minors protecting their personal data to the provisions of the Organic Law 3/2018 of 5 December, on Personal Data Protection and guarantee of digital rights. RESULTS To September 26, 2023, we have enrolled 62 participants. Thirty-one participants have completed the study. This clinical trial has been funded by the Comunidad de Madrid (Spain) IND2020/BMD-17544. The approximate completion date is March 2024. CONCLUSIONS Serious video games such as MOON can be motivational tools that complement multimodal treatment in ADHD. CLINICALTRIAL NCT06006871
Article
Dementia is a progressive neurological disorder which is clinically characterized by memory loss, behavioral symptoms, and loss of ability to live a normal life. Dementia patients may have to cope with permanent and irreversible symptoms. Although a new drug for dementia with beta-amyloid-removal function has recently received FDA approval, the effect of delaying the progression of dementia is still insignificant, but the cost is expensive. On the other hand, studies on the effects of exercise in dementia have implied that the incidence of dementia or cognitive decline could be efficiently controlled by exercise. The cognitive neuroprotective effect of exercise was supported by cross-sectional studies in which physically fit seniors showed larger hippocampal or gray matter volumes than unfit seniors. In addition, multiple animal studies demonstrate that exercise promotes neuroplasticity through induction of neurotropic factors, with improved outcomes on cognitive functions. In this review, we discuss the effects of conventional mode of physical exercise, cognitive (neuromuscular) exercise, and combined exercise in the prevention of dementia, and highlight the prospects for new exercise programs using digital technology that are being recently developed to reduce cognitive decline. Exercise should be importantly considered as a non-pharmacological therapeutic strategy for cognitive decline and dementia.
Chapter
This examination challenges the idea that mental health is exclusively influenced by genetic predisposition and aging. Lifestyle decisions substantially influence cognitive health, with social interaction, environmental influences, nutrition, physical activity, sleep quality, and stress management all playing pivotal roles. Meaningful social connections, a supportive environment, regular exercise, a balanced diet, adequate rest, and stress reduction are all associated with mental resilience. Although cognitive training programs exhibit potential, continuous evaluation remains imperative. An all-encompassing approach that considers emergent technologies in addition to socioeconomic and environmental variables is consistent with shifting viewpoints regarding cognitive well-being. The advancement of cognitive capacities and mental well-being is intergenerational, impacted by habitual decisions, healthy conduct, and nurturing surroundings on both the individual and societal scales.
Article
Full-text available
Background & Objectives: Hypertension is one of the most common chronic diseases in the world. It is considered one of the most important public health issues in the world and causes millions of deaths due to stroke and cardiovascular and kidney diseases. Research shows that people with hypertension suffer psychological, emotional, and social challenges. Stress, depression, and anxiety are among the most common psychological problems in this group of patients. Cognitive rehabilitation therapy and mindfulness-based therapy are among the treatments that studies have shown can affect anxiety. Therefore, the present study was conducted to compare the effects of cognitive rehabilitation therapy and mindfulness-based therapy on the anxiety of patients with hypertension. Methods: This double-blind clinical trial and quasi-experimental study has a pretest-posttest and follow-up design with a control group. The statistical population consisted of all the people with hypertension living in Rasht City, Iran, who were referred to the specialized heart hospital in the city in 2021. From this community, 51 qualified volunteers entered the study by the convenience sampling method and were randomly assigned to groups of cognitive rehabilitation therapy, mindfulness-based therapy, and control group (17 people in each group). The inclusion criteria were as follows: informed consent and willingness to participate in the research, having high blood pressure (according to the medical record), anxiety more than average (anxiety score 16-40 according to the Beck Anxiety Inventory), ability to read and write, age range from 30 to 60 years, not abusing drugs and alcohol, not suffering from severe personality and clinical disorders, lacking a history of head trauma and recent brain injuries, not receiving electric shock or special treatment in the last six months, and not participating in psychotherapy courses at the same time or in the last two months. The exclusion criteria were as follows: absence of more than two sessions, unwillingness to continue psychotherapy, a possible complication of the subject and the occurrence of an accident and the absence or event affecting the research variables. Beck Anxiety Inventory (Beck et al., 1988) was employed in this research. Cognitive rehabilitation therapy was conducted face-to-face and individually for the first experimental group (using the computer in 10 sessions of 60 minutes once a week) at the Cardiovascular Diseases Research Center located in Heshmat Hospital in Rasht. Mindfulness-based therapy sessions were held for 2 months (8 sessions, one 90-minute session each week) for another experimental group online (through Skyroom software) and the control group did not undergo any type of psychotherapy at the same time. For data analysis in descriptive statistics, mean and standard deviation chart were used. For inferential statistics, the Chi-square test, 1-way analysis of variance, analysis of variance of repeated measurements, and Bonferroni post hoc test were used. Data analysis was done in SPSS software version 26 at a significance level of 0.05. Results: The results showed that the effects of time (p<0.001), group (p<0.001), and the interaction of group and time (p<0.001) on the anxiety variable were significant. The posttest (p<0.001) and follow-up scores (p<0.001) of the anxiety variable in the mindfulness group and cognitive rehabilitation group decreased significantly compared to the pretest scores. In contrast, there was no significant difference between the anxiety scores of the control group at different times (p>0.05). There was a significant difference between cognitive rehabilitation and mindfulness groups in terms of effectiveness in the posttest and follow-up on reducing anxiety compared to the control group (p<0.001). While there was no significant difference between the two experimental groups in the posttest (p=0.616) and follow-up (p=0.221) in terms of effectiveness in reducing anxiety. Conclusion: According to the research findings, cognitive rehabilitation therapy and mindfulness-based therapy were effective on the anxiety of patients with hypertension, and there was no significant difference between the two therapies. Therefore, both types of psychotherapies are equally beneficial to help patients with hypertension manage a chronic and life-threatening disease.
Article
Objective: The primary aim of this paper is to accelerate the number of randomized experimental studies of the reliability and validity in-home tele-neuropsychological testing (tele-np-t). Method: We conducted a critical review of the tele-neuropsychology literature. We discuss this research in the context of the United States' public and private healthcare payer systems, including the Centers for Medicare & Medicaid Services (CMS) and Current Procedural Terminology (CPT) coding system's telehealth lists, and existing disparities in healthcare access. Results: The number of tele-np publications has been stagnant since the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. There are less published experimental studies of tele-neuropsychology (tele-np), and particularly in-home tele-np-t, than other tele-np publications. There is strong foundational evidence of the acceptability, feasibility, and reliability of tele-np-t, but relatively few studies of the reliability and validity of in-home tele-np-t using randomization methodology. Conclusions: More studies of the reliability and validity of in-home tele-np-t using randomization methodology are necessary to support inclusion of tele-np-t codes on the CMS and CPT telehealth lists, and subsequently, the integration and delivery of in-home tele-np-t services across providers and institutions. These actions are needed to maintain equitable reimbursement of in-home tele-np-t services and address the widespread disparities in healthcare access.
Article
Full-text available
Interest in applications of mindfulness-based approaches with adults has grown rapidly in recent times, and there is an expanding research base that suggests these are efficacious approaches to promoting psychological health and well-being. Interest has spread to applications of mindfulness-based approaches with children and adolescents, yet the research is still in its infancy. I aim to provide a preliminary review of the current research base of mindfulness-based approaches with children and adolescents, focusing on MBSR/MBCT models, which place the regular practice of mindfulness meditation at the core of the intervention. Overall, the current research base provides support for the feasibility of mindfulness-based interventions with children and adolescents, however there is no generalized empirical evidence of the efficacy of these interventions. For the field to advance, I suggest that research needs to shift away from feasibility studies towards large, well-designed studies with robust methodologies, and adopt standardized formats for interventions, allowing for replication and comparison studies, to develop a firm research evidence base. KeywordsMindfulness meditation-Children-Adolescents-Families-Schools
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) affects 3% to 7% of school-aged children and has been associated with a variety of comorbid mental illnesses and functional impairments, largely in clinical samples. However, little is known about the spectrum of emotional and behavioral problems and areas of impairment among children with a history of ADHD in nonclinical, nationally representative samples. Methods Data were analyzed from the 2003 National Health Interview Survey, an ongoing, computer-assisted, randomsample, personal-interview survey of the noninstitutionalized U.S. population. We examined the associations between history of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis and levels of parent-reported emotional and behavioral difficulties and related impairments among a nationally representative sample of U.S. children aged 4 to 17 years (n = 8681). The extended version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire was used to measure and score levels of difficulty and impairment. Results Approximately 5.9% of children had a history of ADHD diagnosis. Children with a history of ADHD were 6 times as likely as those without ADHD to have a high level of overall difficulties including emotional, conduct, and peer problems and were 9 times as likely to manifest a high level of impairment including interference with home life, friendships, classroom learning, and leisure activities. Conclusion: This study documents the significant level of current emotional and behavioral difficulties and impairments in everyday functioning experienced by children with a history of ADHD diagnosis, suggesting that people involved with the care of children - parents, health care providers, and teachers - need to be informed about the signs, symptoms, and appropriate treatment of ADHD and other comorbid disorders.
Article
Full-text available
Does the structure of an adult human brain alter in response to environmental demands? Here we use whole-brain magnetic-resonance imaging to visualize learning-induced plasticity in the brains of volunteers who have learned to juggle. We find that these individuals show a transient and selective structural change in brain areas that are associated with the processing and storage of complex visual motion. This discovery of a stimulus-dependent alteration in the brain's macroscopic structure contradicts the traditionally held view that cortical plasticity is associated with functional rather than anatomical changes.
Article
Full-text available
The current study reviews the attention training literature after an acquired brain injury using mere-analytic procedures. Through computerized searches we identified 30 relevant studies involving a total of 359 patients. Overall performance, measured by d(+), improved significantly after training. Then the set of 30 studies were categorized according to whether the study evaluated training efficacy by comparing pre- and posttraining scores only or included a control as well. Results show that performance improved significantly after training when assessed by the pre-post only measure, but nut in the pre-post with control condition; Further analyses showed that performance in studies providing specific skills training improved significantly, whereas performance in studies providing general attention training did not. The implications of these results Tor rehabilitation are discussed. (C) 2000 Academic Press.
Article
Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is a part of the brain's limbic system. Classically, this region has been related to affect, on the basis of lesion studies in humans and in animals. In the late 1980s, neuroimaging research indicated that ACC was active in many studies of cognition. The findings from EEG studies of a focal area of negativity in scalp electrodes following an error response led to the idea that ACC might be the brain's error detection and correction device. In this article, these various findings are reviewed in relation to the idea that ACC is a part of a circuit involved in a form of attention that serves to regulate both cognitive and emotional processing. Neuroimaging studies showing that separate areas of ACC are involved in cognition and emotion are discussed and related to results showing that the error negativity is influenced by affect and motivation. In addition, the development of the emotional and cognitive roles of ACC are discussed, and how the success of this regulation in controlling responses might be correlated with cingulate size. Finally, some theories are considered about how the different subdivisions of ACC might interact with other cortical structures as a part of the circuits involved in the regulation of mental and emotional activity.
Article
Clinic-referred teens (ages 12–19) with ADHD and ODD (N = 101) were compared to community control (CC) teens, equated for age and sex, (N = 39) on a variety of psychological tasks assessing executive functioning (EF), temporal reward discounting, and time estimation and reproduction. A factor analysis reduced the EF measures to three dimensions, representing CPT Inattention, Working Memory, and CPT Inhibition. Results indicated that the ADHD group had significantly more CPT Inattention than the CC group. No differences were found for Working Memory or CPT Inhibition. The ADHD group displayed significantly greater temporal discounting of delayed hypothetical monetary rewards relative to immediate ones and manifested more impaired time reproduction, but not time estimation, than did the CC group. Main effects for level of IQ were found only on the Working Memory factor and largely did not interact with the group factor otherwise. The group differences in CPT Inattention, temporal discounting, and time reproduction were not a function of level of comorbid oppositional defiant disorder, delinquency, or anxiety–depression. Results are reasonably consistent with past research on EF and sense of time in children with ADHD and extend these findings to the adolescent age group. Problems with working memory and CPT inhibition found in prior studies of children with ADHD, however, were not evident here, perhaps owing to age-related improvements or insufficient task difficulty.
Chapter
This chapter discusses the disclosure of financial conflicts in medical sciences. The principal threat of financial conflict of interest does not stem from its surreptitious nature but from its corrupting influence on the integrity of our medical information. Focusing on disclosure rather than on eliminating the conflicts is simply taking the easy way out. Disclosure is not the problem; lack of disclosure is not the problem. Bias from financial ties is the problem, and disclosure does not solve it. The chapter also defines that once a financial conflict is disclosed, a receiver of information must assess whether the conflict has had an influence on the information. Conflicts of interest influence individuals to be biased, but having a conflict of interest is no guarantee that an individual will be biased or act on that bias. Many people forget that just because someone has a conflict of interest is not an a priori reason for considering that person's opinion biased. Bias is often thought of as a deliberate choice of conflicted individuals.