ArticlePDF Available

Draughtsmen at Work

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

To obtain the representation of a contour, the visual system integrates fragments of a pattern. One of the 'binding rules' governing this process requires that a path of conjunction in which contrast polarity is preserved be followed. Here we show that this rule has a corollary: where two alternative paths compete to emerge in opposite directions, the one with greater contrast luminance is likely to prevail.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Abstract. To obtain the representation of a contour, the visual system integrates fragments of a
pattern. One of the `binding rules' governing this process requires that a path of conjunction
in which contrast polarity is preserved be followed. Here we show that this rule has a corollary:
where two alternative paths compete to emerge in opposite directions, the one with greater contrast
luminance is likely to prevail.
Figure 1a shows a checkerboard with four columns of draughtsmen, arranged away
from the rectangle centres to partly cover one horizontal edge and tangential to a
vertical dividing line. Though geometrically parallel, the columns appear to diverge/
converge. The illusory effect is strengthened if each draughtsman is flanked by a twin
isoluminant disk as in figure 1b.
A small displacement of the disks helps to locate the source of the illusory effect.
In figure 1c each pair of draughtsmen has been slid apart: they now appear as they
actually are, arranged in vertical parallel columns. This vanishing of the illusory tilt
suggests that it originates within a particular pattern formed by the two side-by-side
disks at the convergence of four surfaces (forming a so-called X-junction, visible after
the disks have been separated). Within this configuration, the contour completion can
take two different paths. Each rectangle side can continue straight along the collinear
edge or else deviate by 908, going round the rectangular perimeter; these two options
are shown in figure 2a as two dotted lines which demonstrate that, whichever the
solution, one rule is obeyed and the other is violated. When the rectangle side con-
tinues along the collinear side, the Gestalt law of `good continuity' is obeyed, but
the sign of the contrast (or contrast polarity, CP) inverts. When the side turns up the
perpendicular side (going round the rectangle perimeter), the abrupt change in direction
violates the `good continuity' law, but the CP is preserved. The checkerboard therefore
presents a condition in which the law of `good continuity' is challenged by a competing
binding rule
ö
already documented by Roncato and Casco (2003)
ö
which states that
of two directions of contour completion, the one preserving the CP will perceptually
prevail. It is this competition that can play a role in the occurrence of misperception
of linearity on a checkerboard when the X-junctions are erased. As can be seen in the
striking demonstrations of Pierce (1898) and Kitaoka et al (2004), these illusory distor-
tions document contour completions deviating from linearity in which the CP rule is
to some extent responsible. A simple stratagem can turn this conflict into an alliance.
Figure 2b illustrates how this may happen. A portion of the X-junction is hidden by
a dark circle that is tangential to two rectangle sides. As a result, a new direction of
completion opens up because the CP is preserved at a cost of a small smooth deviation
from straightness (a straight side extending into an arc
ö
see figure caption). Direct
manifestation of this binding effect is not found in figure 1b because we do not perceive
LAST BUT NOT LEAST
Draughtsmen at work
Perception, 2010, volume 39, pages 255 ^ 2 59
Oronzo Parlangeli
Dipartimento di Scienze della Comunicazione, Universita
©di Siena, Banchi di Sotto 55, 53100 Siena,
Italy; e-mail: parlangeli@unisi.it
Sergio Roncato
Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Universita
©di Padova, via Venezia 8, 3 5131 Padua, Italy;
e-mail: sergio.roncato@unipd.it
Received 24 June 2009, in revised form 7 January 2010
doi:10.1068/p6500
(a)
(b)
(c)
Figure 1. Checkerboards with draughtsmen: the disks are arranged in vertical columns but in (a)
and (b) appear to tilt.
256 Last but not least
a straight curved border distinct from the others. We can only infer its occurrence
on the basis of the assumption that this local deviation from straightness generates
a cue of local slant, and this, together with similar collinear effects, gives rise to an
overall effect for the whole column of disks. The configurations in figure 1 move us a
step forward in understanding the role of luminance contrast in contour binding,
providing a demonstration of the local effects of slanting as well as the role of contrast
polarity and magnitude. A check of this double role of contrast is essential, in view of
the recent challenge by van Lier and Csatho
¨(2006), who found an exception to the CP
rule. In particular, we aim to demonstrate that the effects of contour binding arising
between edges of the same CP are stronger when the contrast magnitude is higher.
(a) (b) (c)
(d)
(e)
Figure 2. CPs (indicates the brighter side of an edge) and direction of edge-merging (dotted
curves) for various luminance conditions on a checkerboard. (a) Two directions of contour bind-
ing within an X-junction (dotted lines). The straight direction obeys the `good continuity' rule
but generates a contour of inverted CP. The angled path (white dotted line) preserves the CP.
In (b) ^ (e) a dotted line indi cates the path formed by two edges that bind through having the
same CP. (b) Illustration of the luminance conditions in figure 1a. A conjunction path forms that
is tilted clockwise from the vertical. (c) Illustration of the luminance conditions in figure 1b
(dark disks). Both the upper and lower straight edges bind with the adjacent circular edge of
same CP. Two local slant cues arise. (d) and (e) Illustration of the luminance conditions in figure 1b
(dark and light disks). The path of invariant CP binds a vertical edge with an arched edge on the
upper right and lower left. The local slant effects are more pronounced than in (b).
Last but not least 257
In figure 3 the draughtsmen are of the same colour. There is no effect of orientation
alteration, but the line separating the pairs of disks seems to wave. Concentrating on a
single vertical side gives the impression that it bends at its extremities, following round
the disk perimeter. This is simply the slant effect predicted by the hypothesis that the
rectangle side binds with the curved edge of same CP (see caption to figure 3).
Figure 4 challenges the CP rule. The grey shades chosen for the checkerboard and
draughtsmen are combined in such a way that all the binding paths between the rectangle
sides and arcs have invariant CP (see enlargement).
Figure 3. Disks of the same colour. In the enlargement, the dotted lines indicate straight edge
binding preserving the CP. Locally the bending effect generates the impression of a rounding
of the rectangle side. An overall effect arises of the column axis waving.
Figure 4. The circles have different colours, but grey shades intermediate between the rectangle
luminances. A curved path of same CP can continue along the same edge at both its extremities and
in opposite directions. Dotted arcs indicate paths ofedge completion with low contrast (0.04 and 0.27
of Michelson contrast on CRT screen); continuous lines indicate higher contrast paths (0.60 and
0.85).
258 Last but not least
In this condition local slants are not expected to occur because the rectangle sides
do not have a privileged direction to extend into. This prediction is disconfirmed by
the illusory effects visible in figure 4: they have the same direction as in figure 1b,
albeit less vivid.
What is the origin of this illusion? One important clue comes from close inspection
of all the paths of edge conjunction in the enlargement in figure 4. Of the two bind-
ing directions at each extremity, one has a high luminance contrast, the other a low
luminance contrast. Since the direction of the former coincides with the overall tilt
direction, our hypothesis is that the binding effects
ö
both local and overall
ö
are the
same as in figure 1b, with the difference that in this case it is the contrast magni-
tude that determines what local slant will emerge. In other words, where there are two
alternative directions of binding preserving CP, the one with the higher luminance
contrast will prevail. According to this hypothesis, the illusion will vanish as the differ-
ence in contrast magnitude between the two arched edges falls.
To test this hypothesis we set up an experiment with patterns similar to those in
figure 4. A basic configuration with disks very different in luminance (see figure 4 cap-
tion) served as an initial stimulus from which to generate other stimuli by gradually
lowering the contrast magnitudes. This was accomplished by gradually darkening the
light disks and lightening the dark ones until a final condition was reached of all
disks isoluminant. Five subjects were presented with the series of stimuli following
the method of limits. They were asked to judge whether they perceived parallel or
converging/diverging columns, making their judgments twice, starting with stimulus
disks (i) highly contrasted, (ii) isoluminant. The average luminances were calculated
that signal the transition in percept (impression) from converging/diverging to parallel;
these correspond to 9.3 cd m
ÿ2
(SD 0:54 cd m
ÿ2
) for dark disks and 16.6 cd m
ÿ2
(SD 1:1cdm
ÿ2
) for light disks. The Michelson contrasts calculated at the border of
these disks were found to be 0.50 and 0.63, respectively. For the luminance conditions
of figure 4, these can be considered the minimal contrast magnitudes necessary for
the binding path to prevail and the tilt illusion to arise.
This result demonstrates that to be complete the CP-preserved path-conjunction
binding rule should have the corollary: ``if there are two contour completions that
preserve the CP, the one with the higher contrast will prevail''. This addendum may
contribute to a better understanding of the illusory effects generated by Kitaoka et al
(2001) and van Lier and Csatho
¨(2006).
References
Kitaoka A, Pinna B, Brelstaff G, 2001 ``New variations of the spiral illusion'' Percepti on 30
637 ^ 646
Kitaoka A, Pinna B, Brelstaff G, 2004 ``Contrast polarities determine the direction of Cafe
¨Wall
tilts'' Perce ptio n 33 11 ^ 20
Lier R van, Csatho
¨A
è, 2006 ``Dancing shapes: A comparison of luminance-induced distortions''
Perception 35 775 ^ 798
Roncato S, Casco C, 2003 ``The influence of contrast and spatial factors in the perceived shape
of boundaries'' Perception & Psychophysics 65 1252 ^ 1272
Pierce A H, 1898 ``The illusion of the kindergarten patterns'' Psychological Review 5233 ^ 253
ß 2010 a Pion publication
Last but not least 259
ISSN 0301-0 06 6 (print)
Conditions of use. This article may be downloaded from the Perception website for personal research
by members of subscribing organisations. Authors are entitled to distribute their own article (in printed
form or by e-mail) to up to 50 people. This PDF may not be placed on any website (or other online
distribution system) without permission of the publisher.
www.perceptionweb.com
ISSN 1468-423 3 (electronic)
N:/psfiles/banners/
final-per.3d
... [7,8,10]), the results confirmed that haptic sensations located on the palm of the hand, similar to "circle" and "square" stimuli used in our experiment, are easy to perceive but hard to discriminate one another. This kind of result can be ascribed to the receptive fields of the palm of the hand which are likely to be less developed than the ones responsible for many visual phenomena [18][19][20][21]. Some studies have shown that on the basis of successive stimuli presented to the palm of the hand, it is possible to integrate these sensations into a unique pattern, highlighting the spatial relationships between the single stimuli [22]. ...
Chapter
The way in which technology is designed requires reconsideration. Most importantly, a clearer consideration of ethics in the design process can ensure that, even in the planning phase, there is a move towards the creation of technologies that fit with, and support, human activity in a sustainable way. To this end, this paper presents some critical points which should be considered in the design phase to encourage reflection on the evolutionary aspects of user-centered design.KeywordsDesignGuidelinesEthicsHCIHuman-computer interaction evolution
Chapter
Universities and institutions of higher education have been forced to move study and work activities remotely to contrast the spread of the pandemic due to Covid-19. Given the differences in roles, opportunities and operational functions of students, faculty members and university employees, it was deemed important to investigate whether any difficulties encountered in this condition of isolation may have led the three different groups to behave in an ethically inappropriate manner. To this end a questionnaire was developed and administered to a sample of 706 respondents, i.e. faculty members, students and technical and administrative staff. The study participants filled in a questionnaire aimed at investigating the operating conditions of working and studying at a distance, relating them to the perception of possible misbehavior by their own group and the other two subgroups. Results put in evidence that the three groups considered faced different difficulties that had different effects on perceptions in relation to the misconduct they experienced or perpetrated. Faculty appear to be essentially suffering from problems experienced even before the pandemic, such as an excess of competition that leads them to see their own colleagues as unfair. Students have less satisfactory housing conditions and demonstrate an awe of faculty that leads them to express less dissent when they witness misconduct. Technical and administrative staff appear disadvantaged in terms of technical tools and less equipped to process the negative feedback they receive about their work. However, they more than the other two groups are convinced that misbehaviors have decreased during home quarantine. On the contrary, faculty and students reported that misconduct have increased during the period of isolation.
Chapter
This study explores the extent to which social media manages to meet the relational needs of individuals diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) during Italy’s first COVID-19 lockdown (March – May, 2020). To this end 22 caregivers of people with ASD completed a questionnaire investigating the living conditions of the affected people during this time, their use of social media/communication tools, their anxiety levels, their perception of the usefulness of social media, and the types of cognitive and/or relational dysfunction. The results show that social media did not prove useful, even in situations of obvious need such as those experienced during the lockdown. Caregivers, however, viewed social media in a positive light only for those individuals with ASD who had used them already prior to the lockdown. Further studies are therefore needed to better design social media for the needs of people with ASD.
Chapter
The study investigates cyberbullying within the Contrade (singular “Contrada”) of Siena. The objective is to explore whether and how intermediate communities such as the Contrade of Siena, social bodies which are intermediate between the individual and the State, with their own rules and values, can play a protective role or increase the risk of detrimental behavior on the Net. Adolescents (N = 304) aged between 11 and 16 years completed an online questionnaire on moral disengagement [1], their relations with the Contrada, the values attributed to the Contrada, the use of social media [2], cyberbullying and cyber-victimization [3]. The study highlights that intermediate communities can have a protective function, through the values they manage to convey and represent, both to those who are part of them and to those who perceive them from the outside.
Chapter
Perceptual processes, starting from the processing of physical stimuli, lead to the representation of environments, that is, of contexts organized logically and distributed in space, and therefore to the understanding of meanings. Based on the understanding of objects and environments, human beings then formulate intentions that guide their actions. In the course of interacting with an environment, personal perspectives are assumed determined by certain factors such as our skills, our previous knowledge, our purposes. In doing so, the individual selects, among all those available, only the information useful to the achievement of their goals, develops mental models suitable for the optimal performance of their functions. The way objects and environments are designed ultimately affects the formulation of the subjects’ intentions and, in a cyclical way, their experiences of using them.
Conference Paper
Many steps have been taken in recent years to bring production processes closer to customer needs. This trend of mass customisation is best characterised by the emergence of online configurators which help to eliminate barriers between production and market needs. This study involved 82 participants who interacted with an online configurator for interior design products. Participants were administered two tests to measure their prospective memory abilities. After interacting with the product configurator, the participants were asked to rate aspects of the configurator - level of satisfaction, extent to which they had been able to satisfactorily conclude the configuration process, as well as their willingness to buy the product they created. The results show that prospective memory is relevant in relation to the degree of satisfaction with the interaction experience. The results also seem to suggest that prospective memory is involved in the successful completion of the configuration process and in the level of willingness to purchase the configured product.
Article
We demonstrate a perceptual effect whereby contours not physically present in a visual scene can yield striking illusory motion. The not physically present contours are paths of invariant contrast polarity (CP). For example, when a square checkerboard composed of dark and light square checks with small black and white discs covering the vertices is put in lateral motion, there is the striking perception of vertical expansion/contraction. Such a checkerboard has (not physically present) diagonal paths of CP presentation with vertical components. However, when a square checkerboard made up of square black and very light checks with gray discs of luminance intermediate to the checks is put in lateral motion, no expansion/contraction is seen. For this checkerboard the vertical components of paths of CP preservation cancel each other out, predicting the lack of perception of vertical expansion/contraction. We also discuss how not physically present contours can explain previously described effects and suggest new effects to be explored.
Article
Roncato and Casco (2003, Perception & psychophysics 65 1252-1272) had shown that in situations where the Gestalt principle of good continuity is put into conflict with preservation of contrast polarity (CP) the perception that preserves CP prevails. Parlangeli and Roncato (2010, Perception 39 255-259) have studied this question of preservation of CP more closely and have added an addendum to the rule. They have used stimuli consisting of a checkerboard of perpendicularly arranged rectangular bricks (white, gray, or black) and draughtsmen white, gray, or black disks placed at the corners of the bricks. This study has caused them to add an addendum to the rule of CP-preserved path-conjunction binding: if there are two contour completions that preserve the CP, the one with the higher contrast will prevail. Parlangeli and Roncato find that, for certain shades of the disks and bricks, the perpendicular lines of the checkerboard appear strikingly to be slanted or undulating. Here we consider all possible arrangements of relative magnitudes of checkerboards consisting of bricks of two different shades and disks of two shades as well, as such arrangements with widely varying differences in the magnitude of brightness. We have found a number of cases where the perception is not explained by the rule and addendum of Roncato and Casco, and Parlangeli and Roncato, and a case where preservation of "distant" as well as local CP plays a role in perception. The previously known cases, and the new exceptional unexplained stimuli we have found, warrant further study.
Article
Full-text available
When an edge can be perceived to continue either with a collinear edge of the opposite contrast polarity or with a noncollinear edge of the same contrast polarity, observers perceive an alignment between the edges of the same contrast polarity, even though they are noncollinear. Using several stimulus configurations and both free and tachistoscopic viewing, we studied the luminance and spatial factors affecting the perceived distortion and binding. The results showed that the two noncollinear edges tended to align when they had the same contrast polarity (Experiment 1A) and to misalign when they had opposite contrast polarity (Experiment 2), providing that (1) they were separated by a distance larger than 1 arcmin and smaller than 3-4 arcmin (for all configurations) and (2) they laterally overlapped for about 7 arcmin (Experiment 1B). The results also showed that the direction of apparent distortion depended on the direction of overlapping. The results of Experiment 3 ruled out the local attraction/repulsion explanation but, instead, supported the suggestion that the interaction concerned the global edges, or part of them, and produced an inward tilt, which made the edges of the same contrast polarity perceptually to align, or an outward tilt, so that the edges of opposite contrast polarity were perceived to be more misaligned. From the overlap and distance limits found, it can be inferred that for two noncollinear contours to join perceptually, the tilt must not exceed 18 degrees, a limit compatible with the orientation bandwidth of contrast-sensitive early cortical mechanisms.
Article
New variations of the spiral illusion are demonstrated. They include spiral illusions of the Café Wall illusion and the Zöllner illusion, as well as other new orientation illusions. Thus the spiral illusion is not limited to the Fraser illusion. We discuss the role that detectors of spirals in a higher visual area might play in the spiral illusion.
Article
Reports an experimental examination of the Munsterberg illusion on 3 Ss. The elementary form of the illusion, called 'The Shifted Checker-Board-Figure', is the optical illusion of kindergarten patterns, where at the point of maximum effect, the straight line appears oblique in appearance. Quantitative examination consisted of viewing figures which excluded the possibility of irradiation while retaining the other operative factors. Quantitative examination consisted of viewing cards whose amount of overlapping could be easily adjusted, thus varying the influence of various factors determining the amount of the illusion. Results of the former show that whenever irradiation was excluded, the illusion vanished. Quantitative examination showed that whatever altered the amount and character of irradiation produced an alteration in the amount of the illusion. Thus, irradiation alone seems adequate in explaining the phenomenon of this illusion. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
We propose an explanatory approach to Café Wall type illusions that is simple yet fairly comprehensive. These illusions are constructed out of basic elementary units in a jigsaw-like manner. Each unit, in general, contains both a solid body and a thin tail: the contrast polarity between the two determines the direction of the contributory illusory tilt produced by that element-according to a heuristic rule illustrated in figure 1. Ensembles of these elements exhibit illusory tilts only when the tails of the elements align along a common line in an additive manner. When elements of opposing polarity alternate, the illusion is cancelled. This approach extends and supersedes those presented in Pinna's illusion of angularity and Kitaoka's 'acute' corner effect. Furthermore, it appears to be, in part, compatible with existing mechanisms proposed to account for the emergence of local tilt cues, and it suggests several novel variations on the Café Wall theme.
Article
We present an illusory display in which a grid of outlined squares is positioned in front of a moving luminance gradient. Observers perceive a strong, illusory, 'wavelike' motion of the superimposed squares. We compared luminance effects on dynamic and static aspects of this illusion. The dynamic aspect was investigated by means of a temporal gradient, which induced an illusory pulsing of the outlined squares. The static aspect was investigated in two different ways. In one experiment, the outlined squares were positioned on a spatial gradient, which caused the squares to look like trapezoid shapes. In another experiment, the squares were positioned on different luminance fields, which affected their apparent size. In all experiments, luminance settings were the same, and observers were asked to indicate the direction and strength of the induced distortions. The overall results show large agreements between the dynamic distortion and the first-mentioned static distortion, whereas different tendencies emerged for the second static distortion. In a second series of experiments, we examined these distortions for various ranges of the luminance gradient and for border gradients as well. On the basis of these data, we explored how the directions of the perceived distortions of the single-gradient displays examined in this paper could be related to each other.