ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Interpersonal Neural Entrainment during Early Social Interaction

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Currently, we understand much about how children's brains attend to and learn from information presented while they are alone, viewing a screen - but less about how interpersonal social influences are substantiated in the brain. Here, we consider research that examines how social behaviors affect not one, but both partners in a dyad. We review studies that measured interpersonal neural entrainment during early social interaction, considering two ways of measuring entrainment: concurrent entrainment (e.g., 'when A is high, B is high' - also known as synchrony) and sequential entrainment ('changes in A forward-predict changes in B'). We discuss possible causes of interpersonal neural entrainment, and consider whether it is merely an epiphenomenon, or whether it plays an independent, mechanistic role in early attention and learning.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Review
Interpersonal Neural Entrainment during Early
Social Interaction
Sam V. Wass,
1,
*M. Whitehorn,
1
I. Marriott Haresign,
1
E. Phillips,
1
and V. Leong
2,3
Currently, we understand much about how childrens brains attend to and learn
from information presented while they are alone, viewing a screen but less
about how interpersonal social inuences are substantiated in the brain. Here,
we consider research that examines how social behaviors affect not one, but
both partners in a dyad. We review studies that measured interpersonal neural
entrainment during early social interaction, considering two ways of measuring
entrainment: concurrent entrainment (e.g., when A is high, B is high’–also
known as synchrony) and sequential entrainment (changes in A forward-
predict changes in B). We discuss possible causes of interpersonal neural
entrainment, and consider whether it is merely an epiphenomenon, or whether
it plays an independent, mechanistic role in early attention and learning.
Tracking Dynamic, Social Inuences on Early Attention and Learning
During the rst years of our lives, in particular, our waking hours are spent almost entirely in the
company of others. Yet currently, and paradoxically, most of our knowledge of how the developing
brain functions during social interaction comes from studies that examine individual humans in iso-
lation [14]. From early life, however, we know that social factors inuence how we allocate our at-
tention and learn. For example, 9-month-old infants learn new speech sounds better through live
interaction with an adult than through watching an equivalent video of someone speaking [5].
When a 16-month-old infant initiates an exchange by pointing to an object, their memory retention
for functions subsequently demonstrated on that object is increased [6]. And when a parent pays
attention to a particular object while interacting with their 12-month-old infant, this immediately in-
creases the infants own duration of attention to that object [7]. Yet, we presently understand little
about how these transient, interpersonal inuences are substantiated in the brain.
Recent research, building on advances in adult [3]andanimal[8]socialneuroscience,hasbegunto
explore these dynamic, social inuences by doing two things differently: rst, rather than recording
from one individual brain in isolation, they record from two interacting brains concurrently (sometimes
known as hyperscanning, see Glossary)[9]; second, rather than examining unidirectional
inuences using predesigned, screen-presented experimental stimuli, they examine naturally occur-
ring moments of reciprocal inuence during free-owing interpersonal naturalistic interactions. This
research is starting to uncover a range of important new discoveries about which brain regions
are active during social interaction, that illustrate the importance of studying social interaction in eco-
logically valid contexts. For example, mentalizing and reward networks show markedly different pat-
terns of activity during live interaction, compared to when passively viewing equivalent social stimuli
on a screen [3,4].
Here, we examine a different question: what this research has taught us about the neural tempo-
ral dynamics of early social interactions. In the rst part of this review, we consider two important
and well-researched aspects of social interaction: ostensive cues (signals that are sent during
social interaction to indicate communicative intent), and contingent responsiveness (behaviors
Highlights
Social factors exert transient inuences
on the brains of both partners during an
interaction.
Interpersonal neural entrainment during
early learning interactions has been doc-
umented at multiple temporal scales,
replicating ndings with adults and
animals.
Neural entrainment can be a conse-
quence of behavioral coordination, but it
can also arise in the absence of behav-
ioral coordination: shared understanding
involves temporally co-occurring pat-
terns of brain activity.
Neural entrainment may inuence learn-
ing in multiple ways; for example, by
allowing the sender to ensure information
delivered arrives at an optimal time for
encoding by the receiver.
1
Department of Psychology, University
of East London, London, UK
2
Department of Experimental Psychol-
ogy, University of Cambridge, Cam-
bridge, UK
3
School of Social Sciences, Nanyang
Technological University, Singapore
*Correspondence:
s.v.wass@uel.ac.uk (S.V. Wass).
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.006 1
© 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
TICS 2012 No. of Pages 14
that indicate communicative sensitivity within an interaction). We conclude that the unidirectional
neural correlates of both that is, how one partner transiently inuences the other, with both
partners considered independently are well understood; but that our understanding of how
ostensive cues and contingent responsiveness alter the interpersonal neural dynamics of the
interaction that is, how the partners inter-relate to one another is currently limited.
In the second part of the review, we consider research that has directly examined interpersonal
neural dynamics by measuring interpersonal neural entrainment during social interaction. We
describe key methodological challenges in measuring entrainment and outline the evidence for
the different types of entrainment that emerge during early social interaction. Building on the ev-
idence for unidirectional inuences described in the rst section, we also consider the mecha-
nisms through which bidirectional interpersonal neural entrainment could be achieved and
maintained. In the nal section, we discuss whether interpersonal neural entrainment is merely
an epiphenomenon; or whether it may play a mechanistic role during early attention and learning.
Ostensive Signals
Social interactions are complex, fast-moving, and multilayered: they require the brain to process
rapidly changing information from multiple visual and auditory sources in a time-sensitive manner.
During social interaction, we use signals known as ostensive cues to indicate communicative in-
tent; these tend to be concentrated on moments where the sender wants to convey particularly
important information to the receiver [10]. Historically, the majority of previous research has exam-
ined how adults use ostensive cues towards children, consistent with pedagogical approaches
that primarily emphasize a ow of information from an adult sender to a child receiver. However,
more recent research has recognized that even young infants also use ostensive cues [11,12];
and that, rather than acting as purely passive recipients of information sent by an adult, they
also play an active role as senders of information (such as interrogative cues), which inuence
learning exchanges [1317]. Thus, in addition to studying the direction of inuence from adult
sender to child receiver, recent approaches are acknowledging that children can also act as
senders of social information, and adults as receivers [13].
Ostensive cues lead to a range of changes in behavior during the time period immediately follow-
ing the cue [10]. Although ostensive signals are unidirectional by denition (insofar as they are sig-
nals sent from Partner A to Partner B), extensive research suggests that they also affect the
relationship between Partner A and Partner B. Specically, research has shown that ostensive
cues lead to increased behavioral entrainment (Box 1) in the time period following the ostensive
cues. For example, ostensive cues such as direct eye gaze lead to increases in behavioral mimicry
[18] and the mirroring of facial affect [19]. Similarly, in language, increased vocal mirroring is
observed following the use of child-directed speech contours [20]. Direct eye gaze [21], child-
directed speech [22], and pointing [23] all lead to increases in gaze following, which is another
form of sequential behavioral entrainment (Box 2).
Considerable research has investigated the transient unidirectional effects of ostensive cues
(i.e., how the receiver, considered independently, is affected by the sending of a social signal).
This research has suggested that both child and adult brains are highly sensitive to ostensive
cues [24]: for example, infants show larger neural evoked-response or event-related potential
(ERP) responses (specically, a larger amplitude N170 component) to images of faces showing
direct compared to averted gaze even shortly after birth [25]. During live adultinfant play, cortical
activity (measured from the level of oxyhemoglobin in the medial prefrontal cortex) increases in
7-month-old infants during direct gaze compared with averted gaze [26,27]. Similarly, child-
directed speech evokes greater neural responses (a larger amplitude N250 ERP component)
Glossary
Allostasis: process by which internal
equilibrium is maintained.
Contingent responsiveness:
behaviors that indicate communicative
sensitivity within an interaction.
Cross-correlation: measure of the
similarity between two time-series as a
function of the displacement of one
relative to the other. Cross-correlations
examine whether changes in one time-
series tend to anticipate, or follow on
from, changes in another.
Dyadic attention: two-way sharing of
attention either between a person and
an object, or a person and another
person.
Electroencephalography: method for
recording naturally occurring electrical
brain activity.
Entrainment: in this paperwe consider
two forms of entrainment. The rst is
concurrent entrainment (a zero-lag
relationship between two time series,
e.g., when A is high, B is highor when
A is high, B is low), which is commonly
known as synchrony. The second is
sequential entrainment (changes in A
forward-predict changes in B). See Box
1for further details of how these terms
are quantied.
Functional near-infrared
spectroscopy: method for recording
blood oxygenation levels near the scalp.
Granger causality: method for
quantifying sequential entrainment by
analyzing how one time-series
inuences another; similar to cross-
correlations, but incorporating
information about the self-similarity of
each time series.
Hyperscanning: neuroimaging studies
that record brain activity in two
individuals at the same time.
Ostensive cues: signals from a
communicator to generate an
interpretation of communicativeintention
in an addressee.
Partial directed coherence:
technique to examine cross-spectral
Granger-causal relationships in
multivariate time series.
Phase entrainment: concurrent
entrainment in the phase of ongoing
oscillatory activity.
Phase resetting: abrupt shift
(e.g., advancement or delay) in the
phase of ongoing oscillatory activity,
usually in response to perturbation by a
sensory cue.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
2Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx
in 612-month-old infants compared with adult-directed speech [28], and during live
interactions, uctuations in the child-directedness of speech correlate with uctuations in
prefrontal cortex activity in 915-month-old infants [29]. In addition to neural activity directly
in response to the gaze cue, neural responsiveness is also increased for objects presented
immediately following an ostensive cue. For example, when an adult gazes rst to a
9-month-old infants face before looking to a screen-presented object, the infantsevoked
neural responses to the subsequently presented object are greater (measured as a larger
amplitude Nc ERP component) [30,31].
Currently, however, little research has investigated the bidirectional neural effects of ostensive
cues (i.e., how ostensive cues alter the relationship between the two partnersneural activity in
the time period following the cue). In order to assess how ostensive cues alter the interpersonal
neural dynamics of the interaction, it is necessary to examine change from the perspective of
not one, but both members of the dyad.
Phase-locking value: technique for
estimating concurrent entrainment
between thephase series of two signals.
Synchrony: in this paper, we treat the
term synchrony as synonymous with
concurrent entrainment. See Box 1 for
further details.
Triadic attention: three-way sharing of
attention generally between two
people and an object.
Wavelet transform coherence:
technique that can be used to measure
both concurrent and sequential
synchrony of two signals in the time-
frequency plane.
Tre nd s
Tre nd s
in
in
Cognitive
Cognitive
Sciences
Sciences
Figure 1. Behavioral Paradigms Used to Measure Interpersonal Neural Entrainment. (A) examples of the experimental setups described in the main text, used
by: left [43,126]; middle [60]; right [52]. (B) Examples of raw data collected using the paradigms from [43,126]; even from this raw data sample, some signicant patterns
noted that overall data can be seen such as, parents pay more attention to infants during joint play than vice versa [114], and infants are more inattentive during solo play
than joint play [126]. (C) Example of a 30-s segment of dat a illustrating the further range of different types of events that can be identied in naturalistic interactions.
Abbreviation: EEG, electroencephalography.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx 3
Contingent Responsiveness
Another strand of research has investigated the role of contingent responding during a social
interaction: how the receiver indicates communicative sensitivity within an interaction by consis-
tently responding to behaviors from the sender. Considerable evidence suggests that both
children and adults are highly sensitive to whether or not their partner is responding contingently
to their social signals [14,24,32,33]. For example, one study observed 6.5- and 9.5-month-old
infantsreactions to adults who either responded to the infantsgaze cues by following their
gaze towards an object (congruent looking) or looked in the opposite direction (incongruent).
Older, but not younger, infants showed a visual preference for the congruent actor and showed
greater neural reactivity to the stimulus cued by the congruent (compared to the incongruent)
adult [34].
Behavioral research also suggests that interactions featuring greater behavioral contingency in
both members of the dyad are also more effective as teaching exchanges. For example, in a
task in which adults presented word labels either contingently in response to infant vocalizations
or noncontingently, only infants who received labels contingently in response to their own
attention learned the association [6,13,14,35,36]. However, not all forms of contingency are equally
effective: during videoed interaction only particular types of contingent responding (mirroring, and
marking with a smile) were predictive of the growth of these behaviors over time [18].
Considerable research has investigated the unidirectional correlates of contingent responding
(i.e., how the receiver of a social signal is inuenced by the sender). This research has suggested
that observing someone else perform an action involves neural activity in the observer becoming
more like neural activity in the person performing the action [37,38]. This pattern is similar to the
actorobserver correspondences documented while watching and performing actions [39], and
while watching someone else experience pain, anger, and reward [40,41]. There is also some ev-
idence that, in more contingently responsive social partners, these actorobserver correspon-
dences are stronger [38,42].
Recent research also suggests that similar principles might also apply in different contexts, such
as when considering how adultsbrain activity tracks infantsattention patterns during naturalistic
play. The study recorded dual-electroencephalography (EEG) from parents and 12-month-old
infants during free-owing play (Figure 1A,B). By tracking the continuous uctuations of brain
Tre nd s
Tre nd s
in
in
Cognitive
Cognitive
Sciences
Sciences
Figure 2. Neural Activity in the Parent Entrains to Attentional Fluctuations in the Child. (A) Schematic illustrating the analysis; (B) cross-correlation between child
EEG and child gaze, showing an association between child neural activity and child attention; (C) cross-correlation between adult EEG and adult gaze, also showing an
association between adult neural activity and adult attention; (D) cross-correlation between adult EEG and child gaze, showing an association between the adults
neural activity and the childs attention. Subsequent analyses showed that the association between the adults neural activity and the childs attention was independent
of the adults own attention. Whereas the child EEGchild gaze and adult EEGadult gaze relationships were predictive (i.e., strongest associations were found
between neural activity at a given moment and attention c. 750 ms after that moment), the adult EEGchild gaze associations were reactive (i.e., strongest associations
were found between the childsattentionatagivenmomentandtheadultsneuralactivityc.750msafterthatmoment). Reproduced, with permission, from [43].
Abbreviation: EEG, electroencephalography.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
4Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx
activity in the adult and attention patterns in the infant, their results suggested that adultsneural
activity entrained to uctuations in the childs attention, independent of the adults own attention
[43] (see Figures 1 and 2). When the adult shows greater neural entrainment to the childs atten-
tion, the child is more attentive [43].
Currently, however, and although contingent responsiveness is inherently a bidirectional
behavioral phenomenon (because it describes the relationship between the two partners
behaviors), little research has investigated how contingent responding alters the bidirec-
tional neural dynamics of the interaction. Previous research has mainly considered actor
observer similarities by recording separately from actors and observers; in order to inves-
tigate interpersonal neural dynamics, it is necessary to record from both partners
concurrently.
Box 1. Types of Interpersonal Entrainment and Measurement Caveats
The concept of ent rainment, and the related concept of synchrony, are imme nsely rich topics that have been studied
across all domains of knowledge [8285]. Within cognitive neuroscience, extensive previous research has examined
how different units within a brain, (from individual neurons to brain regions), entrain to one another (intrapersonal
entrainment) [8688]. Previous research has also examined entrainment between individual brains and temporal structures
in the environment (stimulusbrain entrainment) [89].
In this article, we focus on two forms of interpersonal entrainment [19,90]:
Concurrent entrainment a zero-lag relationship, for example, when X is high, Y is high’–measured using techniques
including Phase-locking value [91], wavelet coherence [92], and others. Concurrent synchronyis undirected (ABis
indistinguishable from BA). Concurrent entrainment is treated as synonymous with the term synchrony.
Sequential entrainment –‘changes in X forward-predict changes in Y’–measured using techniqu es derived from
Granger causality [93], including generalized partial directed coherence [94]. Sequential entrainment is directional
(ABBA).
Entrainment can also be estimated based on different aspects of the signal:
Amplitude. Some fNIRS studies [29] and most fMRI studies [95] measure couctuations in the amplitude of the signal
which, depending on the method, mea sures blood oxygenation/deoxygenation (fNIRS), the BO LD signal (fMRI), or
voltage (for EEG).
Phase. Many EEG studies measure the alignment of oscillatory phase between two signals. [59,61].
Combinations. Many fNIRS studies measure wavelet coherence, which includ es both amplitude and phase [51,52].
Other studies measure, for example, phaseamplitude coupling [96].
In the case of childadult interactions, one complication is that ad ults and children have different dominant frequency
bands of naturally occurring brain activity [97]. Although the majority of studies hitherto have not addressed this, techniques
for measuring cross-frequency coupling are available [55].
Researchers measuring interpersonal neural entrainment face a number of methodological caveats [90]:
The rst is that common intrinsic properties of the neural signal itself can create a false impression of entrainment [90]. For
example, two adults, each with a dominant alpha rhythm of 10 Hz, might show consistent phase relationships between
their alpha rhythms even inthe absence of any communication [90]. Similar considerationsapply when considering variability
in amplitude and power of the signal, where co-occurring oscillatory activity may be attributable to other sources (such as
autonomic activity [98]).
A second problem is that neural activity is also inuenced by common environmental perturbations. For example, neural ac-
tivity synchronizes to temporal structures in speech [89], and differentiating neural interpersonal entrainment from synchrony
attributable to shared external perturbation can be highly challenging [99]. This is particularly true for naturalistic paradigms
where factors such as the acoustic environment cannot be completely controlled for.
Common techniques for addressing these problems often use bootstrapping approaches in which corresponding epochs
from each dyad are either temporally translocated (shufed) or phase scrambled and the entrainment analysis is performed
repeatedly. In this way, it is possible to estimate how the observed entrainment would differ from the entrainment observed
by chance [29]. Of note, however, this approach does not control for environmental inuences in naturalistic studies where
the environment differs between dyads. Becauseof this inherent problem, most hyperscanning studies do not measure ab-
solute levels of synchrony, but rather examine relative changes in coupling between different experimental conditions (such
as the presence or absence of mutual gaze) while keeping other factors constant (such as the acoustic environment).
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx 5
Measuring Interpersonal Neural Entrainment in Development
So far, we have considered two well-researched topics within early social interaction: ostensive
cues and contingent responsiveness. We have concluded that the unidirectional neuralcorrelates
of both of these that is, inuences of the sender on the receiver, with the two partners consid-
ered independently are well understood. However, we have also concluded that our under-
standing of how ostensive cues and contingent responsiveness alter the interpersonal neural
dynamics of the interaction that is, how the partnersneural activity inter-relates is currently
limited.
In this section, we consider another strand of research that has directly investigated interpersonal
neural dynamics, by measuring interpersonal neural entrainment. In Box 1 we present a formal
denition of entrainment, distinguishing two ways of measuring entrainment: concurrent entrain-
ment (e.g., when A is high, B is high’–also known as synchrony), and sequential entrainment
(changes in A are followed by changes in B). We discuss several crucial methodological caveats
in measuring entrainment. In Boxes 2 and 3 we summarize recent research into interpersonal en-
trainment at the other levels behavior (Box 2) and physiology (Box 3). As we emphasize in these
boxes, previous research suggests that behavioral and physiological entrainment is not all or
none. Rather, during social interaction, the parentchild dyad oscillates between states of high
Box 2. Behavioral Entrainment
Research into concurrent and sequential behavioral entrainment in parentchild dyads has a long history [100,101], and
includes investigations using both qualitative [102] and quantitative [103] methods. Entrainment has been investigated
at multiple levels of behavior, including:
Vocalizations
Patterns of sequential entrainment during vocal exchanges between adults and infants have been identied at multiple
scales [104,105]. Weaker adultinfant coordination has been associated, for example, with increased attachment problems
and poorer cognitive outcomes [105,106].
Facial Affect
Concurrent and sequential entrainment in facial affect has been identied during tabletop play [19] that changes over time
and differs between fathers and mothers [107]. Stronger childparent and parentchild inuences associate with, for
example, later child self-control [108] and symbolic competence [109]. Not all ndings associate increased entrainment
with positive outcomes [110](Box 3).
Eye Gaze
Two principal types of eye gaze entrainment are of interest. (i) Concurrent partner gaze, referred to as mutual gaze. Mutual
gaze during infancy positively correlates with later attention control [111] and is reduced in some atypical dyads [112,113].
(ii) Sequential entrainment in object gaze, referred to as gaze following that is, that one partners look towards an object
forward-predicts the other partnerslook.Althoughgazefollowinghasbeenextensivelyinvestigatedusingsimplied
screen-based paradigms, research suggests that in real-world naturalistic settings (see Figure 1 in main text) infants
actually follow parentsgaze only rarely [114].
Both types of gaze entrainment, concurren t mutual gaze and sequential gaze following, are often combined as joint, or
triadic attention the three-way sharing of attention between a partner and an object, which involves both mutual gaze
and gaze following [115].
Touch
Current research may overemphasizethe role of gaze during shared parentchild attention and learning: gaze is a predominant
feature of western parentchild interaction, but less so in other cultures [116]. Similar tothe di stinctionbetw eenmutual ga zeand
joint attention, research has examined both touching one another during parentchild interactions [117], and combined touch
to an object [114].
Inducing Behavioral Entrainment
Some research has experimentally induced behavioral entrainment suggesting, for example, that this can be effective at
promoting shared understanding in adultchild dyads [118,119].
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
6Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx
and low synchrony [44]. Different types of entrainment are observed at different spatiotemporal
scales, and interactions can show excessive as well as insufcient entrainment.
Research with adults [3,45] and animals [8,46] has also already shown that interpersonal entrain-
ment develops during social interaction, independent of features such as shared entrainment to
common environmental inuences (Box 1). Previous research with adults has also suggested that
interpersonal neural entrainment inuences learning [47,48].
Interpersonal Entrainment at the Second-to-Minute Scale
Of the range of methods available to researchers for studying in vivo neural activity in infants
and children, the two most commonly used techniques are functional near-infrared
spectroscopy (fNIRS) and EEG. fNIRS, which measures changes in blood oxygenation in
the cortex [49], has a high spatial resolution but a low temporal resolution: the hemodynamic
response lags neural activity by ~2 s and takes ~5 s to reach its peak value [50], meaning
that this technique is best equipped to examine the couctuation of brain activity over
timescales of seconds or minutes.
Building on a rapidly emerging body of research in adults [45], a numberof studies have used fNIRS
to examine how brain activity couctuates between children and adults during social interaction
(see Figure 1). For example, one study [29] used fNIRS to examine entrainment between 9 and
15-month-old infants and an unfamiliar adult, and to examine how this differed between social in-
teraction and when conducting separate activities in the same room. Concurrent and sequential
entrainment was measured by calculating the cross-correlation in deoxyhemoglobin levels. Rel-
ative to bootstrapping analyses (Box 2), signicant concurrent entrainment was observed only dur-
ing interaction in 11 of 57 channel pairs (mainly in frontal areas). Of note, however, the
bootstrapping analyses would not have controlled for shared entrainment to the audiovisual envi-
ronment (Box 1), which was more similar during the interaction condition. Peak associations were
observed with infant brain activity forward-predicting adult brain activity by ~3 s.
Box 3. Physiological Entrainment
Whereas some research into parentchild interactions has examined how, for example, ind ividual heart beats become
coordinated over time [120], most research has studied how autonomic arousal covaries across time windows (both
concurrent, and sequentially see Box 1). Some research has administered how patterns of change covary within dyads
by administering experimental stressors in the laboratory [121,122]; other research has examined how autonomic arousal
levels couctuate in naturalistic, home settings [123,124].
Parents Use Diverse Tactics to Maintain Allostasis
One central aim of social interaction is thought to be to help individuals (particularly young individuals) to maintain a stable
state a process known as allostasis [75]. How parents respond to changes in their child in order to maintain allostasis is
thought to vary contingent on context. Short-term increa ses in concurrent parentchild physiological entra inment were
observed, for example, nega tive affect vocalizations from the child [123]. When the initial arousal level of the parents is
low, parents increase their own arousal in response to increases in child arousal matching their own arousal state to their
childs; but when the initial arousal level of the parent is high, parents respond to increases in the childs arousal in the op-
posite way by decreasing their own arousal [123]. This suggests that adu lts use diverse tactics to maintain allostasis
within the dyad [72,75]dynamically connecting, or disconnecting, their own level of arousal from their childs[76].
There When You Need Meversus Always On
Arousal levels in typical dyads do not routinely couctuate in naturalistic settings [123]. Instead, typical parents selectively
respond to peak changes in their childs arousal, but not otherwise. Parents with depression under-respond to peak mo-
ments of child arousal [110](see[125] for comparable neur oimaging results). By contrast, parents with higher anxiety
showed no difference in responsivity to peak child arousal moments but were more responsive to small-scale uctuations
in their child, and showed higher parentchild entrainment overall [124]. These observations echo similar behavioral ndings
[110] and question whether optimal outcomes always associate with greater parentchild entrainment.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx 7
Another study [51] used wavelet transform coherence to examine concurrent entrainment in
the 0.020.1-Hz range between 5-year-old children and their parents while solving a Tangram
puzzle either together or individually, separated by a screen. They recorded from right and left
frontal and temporoparietal areas and observed entrainment thatwas strongest in the right frontal
and temporoparietal areas. Stronger neural entrainment correlated with increased behavioral
synchrony, and with better problem-solving success while working together. Of note, however,
visual sensory input would also have been more similar during the cooperation condition (see
Box 1 and further discussions below). A third study controlled for this by positioning 59-year-old
children and adults facing forwards, in silence, conducting a computer task that involved either co-
operative or competitive behavior (Figure 1A). Using wavelet transform coherence, they observed
greater concurrent entrainment in 0.080.5-Hz power uctuations across left prefrontal and
frontopolar optode pairings between children and their parents only during the cooperative condi-
tion, and not with an unfamiliar adult even though the conditions were otherwise tightly matched:
auditory and visual information would have been identical between conditions, mutual gaze was not
permitted, and movements were not more synchronous during the cooperation condition [52].
These replicate other ndings using the same paradigm [53,54].
Interpersonal Synchrony at the Subsecond Scale
EEG measures electrical brain activity at the subsecond scale but has a low spatial resolution, making
strong conclusions about the neural generators of entrainment hard to draw [55]. Studies using EEG
generally decompose neural activity into frequency bands, the most commonly studied of which in
developmental research are theta (36/47Hzinyoungchildren/adults)andalpha(69/812 Hz in
children/adults). Activity in these bands has been associated with attention and learning [56,57].
Building on a large body of dual EEG research with adults [58], one recent study examined the
relationship between social learning and concurrent neural entrainment in adult12-month-old in-
fant dyads [59]. Parents modeled positive or negative emotions towards objects, and infants
subsequent choices were examined. Some infants tended to choose the positively modeled ob-
ject, and others the negatively modeled; but for both groups, parentinfant neural entrainment
(phase synchrony in 69 Hz, corresponding to the infant alpha band) during teaching predicted
the likelihood of social learning on a given trial. Interpersonal entrainment was most predictive
of learning across central and parietal electrodes [59]. Finally, trial-to-trial increases in interper-
sonal neural entrainment were associated with greater maternal use of ostensive signals such
as eye contact and speech pitch modulation.
Another recent study recorded concurrent and sequential entrainment in neural activity in
8-month-old infants and adults while an adult recited nursery rhymes while alternating between di-
rect gaze and indirect gaze with the infant [60](Figure 1). A control condition, direct-oblique, was
also presented in which adultsface angle was the same as for indirect gaze, but their eyes were
looking at the infant. Recording at the vertex only, separate bidirectional Granger-causal inu-
ences (childadult and adultchild) were identied during live interaction that were stronger dur-
ing direct and direct-oblique compared to indirect gaze in both theta and alpha bands. The auditory
environment did not differ between conditions, and speechbrain entrainment also did not differ
between conditions, suggesting that entrainment was independent of the shared environment.
Infants who vocalized for longer also had a stronger neural inuence on the adult [60].
A third study measured how both concurrent and sequential entrainment differed between parental
positive and negative affect [61]. Graph theory analyses suggested that parentsand 12-month-old
infantsinterpersonal neural networks were more closely connected during maternal positive affect,
and that mother to infant directional inuences were stronger during positive affect.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
8Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx
These studies have shown that ne-grained (subsecond) neural entrainment develops during so-
cial interaction. Next, we consider how this entrainment develops.
How is Entrainment Achieved and Maintained?
Building on work already conducted with adults andanimals,thestudiesreviewedabovehavesug-
gested that aspects of interpersonal neural entrainment develop during early adultchild interaction.
As yet, however, our understanding of how entrainment is achieved and maintained is limited.
Here, building on the discussion of unidirectional inuences in the rst section of the review, we spec-
ulate about two complementary, but distinct, possible causes of neural entrainment. First, we discuss
how the evoked responses that both children and adults show in response to ostensive cues such as
gaze onsets could involve concurrently phase resetting,leadingtophase entrainment.Inthesec-
ond section, we discuss how actorobserver correspondences could lead to concurrent patterns of
neural activity, potentially causing neural entrainment even in the absence of behavioral entrainment.
Neural Entrainment as a Consequence of Behavioral Cues
Social interactions involve the development of behavioral entrainment, both concurrent and
sequential including movements, gaze patterns, vocalizations, and facial expressions (Box 2).
Evidence reviewed above also suggests that ostensive cues cause immediate, transient in-
creases in behavioral entrainment. Since brainbehavior correspondences are equivalent across
different individuals, this behavioral entrainment is also likely to cause neural entrainment. Indeed,
some of the studies we reviewed have noted signicant correlations between the degree of be-
havioral synchrony observed in dyads, and the neural synchrony observed [51].
The EEG studies reviewed above have, consistent with adult [58] and animal studies [8,46], also
documented phase entrainment during social interaction at much ner timescale (up to 9Hz)
than the second-to-second scale over which behavioral entrainment has been observed. This
more ne-grained entrainment may also, however, have behavioral causes. As described above,
behavioral ostensive cues are known to cause strong neural evoked responses, even in newborn
infants. One possibility is that ostensive cues might operate as edgesin a similar way to the acous-
tic edges (i.e., sharp increases in signal intensity) in the speech amplitude envelope that are known
to drive theta- and delta-rate phase entrainment to cause speechbrain synchrony [62]. Phase re-
setting could take place in both partners to ostensive cues such as gaze onsets and vocalizations,
and this could be one driver that allows phase entrainment to be achieved and maintained
(Figure 3). One prediction that would test this hypothesis would be to assess whether interpersonal
neural entrainment, on both a second-to-second and a subsecond scale, shows transient in-
creases in the time window immediately following ostensive cues (see Outstanding Questions).
Neural Entrainment Arising from Higher-Order Cognitive Processes?
Some of the studies reviewed above [52,54] have, however, also documented neural entrainment
that cannot be explained solely by behavioral entrainment consistent with recent animal
research that observed neural activity in socially interacting mice under conditions in which
behavioral synchrony and shared entrainment to external sensory input were tightly controlled
for [8]. Using in vivo recordings from populations of neurons in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex,
the results of this animal research suggested that concurrent entrainment (synchrony) was driven
by behavior-encoding neurons that show overlapping activity when an action is performed by
themselves, and when the same action is performed by a social partner [8] (see section above,
on contingent responsiveness and actorobserver correspondences).
Adult studies have further built on this, by suggestinginadditionthatneural entrainment may reect
higher-order cognitive processes such as comprehension, engagement, and shared understanding
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx 9
[63,64]. In one study, for example, concurrent interpartic ipant entrainment in neural activity was
recorded while adult participants listened to a real-life auditory narrative compared to a tempo-
rally scrambled version; interparticipant entrainment was increased in default mode network
areas (including medial prefrontal cortex) when participants had a shared understanding of a
story [65,66]. These results suggest that entrainment is not just a consequence of concurrent
brainbehavior correspondences and sensory cue-based phase-resetting, as described
above; rather, that neural entrainment may also be a consequence of temporally concurrent
patterns of activity-driven shared understanding in addition to shared entrainment to sensory
cues [37,67,68]. As yet, however, no research has investigated this from the perspective of
early learning.
As we describe further in the Outstanding Questions, further work also remains to uncover
whether, and if so how, these separate causes contribute to neural entrainment during early
learning. For example, one area for future investigation is the degree to which interpersonal en-
trainment may potentially affect later stages of information processing more than earlier stages
[38,69]. Research with 5-month-old infants has shown that different ostensive cues (eye gaze
and vocalizations) show differing patterns of activation in low-level processing regions, but over-
lapping patterns of activation in frontal areas [70]. It is possible that similar patterns would be ob-
served for interpersonal entrainment but no research has yet investigated this.
Is Synchrony Just an Epiphenomenon?
In this nal section, we consider whether interpersonal neural dynamics, quantied by measuring
entrainment, are best seen simply as epiphenomena as secondary consequences of common
entrainment to behavioral cues and of actorobserver correspondences. Alternatively, we hy-
pothesize two possible mechanistic routes through which interpersonal neural entrainment
might play a causal role during early learning.
First, there are inherent differences in brain function between infants and adults: developing brains
are intensely stochastic [71,72], with altered intrabrain connectivity [73] and entrainment to exter-
nal stimuli [74]. One key function of social interaction is allostasis (helping to maintain a stable
state) [75], achieved via bidirectional, dynamical mutual adaptation within the dyad [19,76]. For
Tre nd s
Tre nd s
in
in
Cognitive
Cognitive
Sciences
Sciences
Figure 3. Schematic Illustrating a Mechanistic Role for Interpersonal Entrainment during Early Learning. In a mutual responsive interaction, there is a mutual
timely exchange of phase-resetting cues between partners. Social ostensive signals may act as synchronizing cuesthattriggertransientincreasesin interpersonal
entrainment through phase-resetting, leading subsequently produced maternal speech to arrive at a high receptivity phase for optimal encoding by the infant.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
10 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx
children, then, entrained states might involve more mature patterns of functional activity; transient
phases of childadult entrainment could thus serve as a transitional stage towards mature
function.
Second, human perception is known to rely on oscillatory processes which shape conscious
experience [77]. Research has suggested that the phase of neural activity at the time of stimu-
lus presentation may relate systematically to the excitability of neural populations and the mag-
nitude of event-related responses [78,79] (although see [80]); accordingly, perceptual stimuli
that are delivered during a high excitability oscillatory phase may be more likely to be detected
and encoded than stimuli that arrive at an inhibitory oscillatory phase [79,81]. During an inter-
action, we may use social cues to nudge our partner into a transient state of entrainment
such that, for example, parent-initiated mutual gaze might trigger a short-term increase in par-
entchild phase synchrony. The effect of this would be to ensure that, for the duration of the
existence of a high synchrony state, high excitability oscillatory phases co-occur, thus ensuring
that information (e.g., word labels) is presented at optimal phases for encoding by the receiver
(Figure 3). In the Outstanding Questions section, we outline some predictions to test these
hypotheses.
Concluding Remarks
In this review, we have evaluated the evidence for how social behaviors affect not one, but
both partners in a dyad. We have concentrated on two important and well-researched as-
pects of early social interaction: ostensive cues (signals that are sent during social interaction
to indicate communicative intent) and contingent responsiveness (behaviors that indicate
communicative sensitivity within an interaction). We have concluded that the unidirectional ef-
fects of each that is, how one partner transiently inuences the other, with both partners con-
sidered independently are well understood. However, we have concluded that our
understanding of how ostensive cues and contingent responsiveness alter the interpersonal
neural dynamics of the interaction that is, how the partners inter-relate to one another is
currently limited.
We have also reviewed a smaller corpus of more recent research that has taken a different
approach, by recording from two individuals concurrently during social interaction and mea-
suring interpersonal neural entrainment. We concluded that, consistent with animal and
adult research, this evidence suggests that interpersonal neural entrainment does develop
during social interaction. Building on the discussion of unidirectional inuences in the rst
section of the review, we have discussed how concurrent and sequential neural entrainment
may arise as a result of two causes: (i) as a consequence of shared entrainment to behav-
ioral cues such as ostensive cues, and (ii) as a consequence of actorobserver correspon-
dences and shared understanding. And we have hypothesized two possible mechanistic
routes through which interpersonal neural entrainment may play a causal role during early
learning.
Our understanding of how early social interaction affects the bidirectional neural dynamics of
the two partners (i.e., how the two patterns of brain activity relate to one another) is still at an
early stage. Many important and fundamental questions remain (see Outstanding Questions).
Perhaps the two most important aspects of the results hitherto are, rst, that social inuences
affect early learning exchanges at a variety of different temporal scales, including both
subsecond as well as second-to-minute temporal scales; and, second, that these interactions
involve bi-directional sensitivity, with both partners - child, as well as adult - inuencing one
another.
Outstanding Questions
Intrabrain entrainment has been
shown to play an important role in
attention and learning; how does
interbrain entrainment relate to
intrabrain entrainment?
Are later stages of stimulus processing
relatively more inuenced by
interpersonal inuences than earlier
stages?
Do concurrent and sequentialentrainment
reect the same or distinct phenomena,
in terms of underlying causes and
consequences?
Does interpers onal entrainme nt show
transient increases in the time window
immediately following an ostensive
cue? And are these increases driven
by temporally co-occurring phase re-
setting in response to the ostensive
cue?
Does greater adultchild neural en-
trainment at the time of a learning
event associate with more effective
learning? If so, are differences medi-
ated by an increased likelihood of
learning items (e.g., word labels)
being presented during high excitatory
oscillatory phases?
How does interpersonal entrainment
change over development? Other
aspects of development show a
transition from coregulation (within the
dyad) to self-regulation (within the
individual) over time. Is the same true
for early learning? If so, is interpersonal
neural entrainment more important
during early learning than later on?
Does atypical development manifest
unusual patterns of neural responsivity
and entrainment? Certain clinical
populations show excessive behavioral
and physiological entrainment (Boxes
2 and 3); is more interp ersonal neural
entrainment always better?
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx 11
Acknowledgments
This research was funded by a Project Grant from the Leverhulme Trust, number RPG-2018-281. We wish to thank Chiara
Bulgarelli, Trinh Nguyen, Carina de Klerk, Vanessa Reindl, and Louise Goupil for reading and commenting on earlier versions
of this manuscript.
References
1. Schilbach, L. et al. (2013) Toward a second-person neurosci-
ence 1. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 393414
2. Wheatley, T. et al. (2019) Beyond the isolated brain: the
promise and challenge of interacting minds. Neuron 103,
186188
3. Redcay, E. and Schilbach, L. (2019) Using second-person
neuroscience to eluc idate the mechanisms of soc ial interac-
tion. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 495505
4. Redcay, E. and Warnell, K.R. (2018) A social-interactiveneuro-
science approac h to understanding the dev eloping brain. In
Advances in Child Development and Behavior,pp.144,
Elsevier
5. Kuhl, P.K. et al. (2003) Foreign-language experience in in-
fancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction
on phonetic learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100,
90969101
6. Begus, K. et al. (2014) Infant s learn what they want t o learn:
Responding to infant pointing leads to superior learning.
PLoS One 9, e108817
7. Yu, C. and Smith, L.B. (2016) The social ori gins of sustained
attention in one-year-old human infants. Curr. Biol. 26,
12351240
8. Kingsbury, L. et al. (2019) Correlated neural activity and
encoding of behavior across brains of socially interacting ani-
mals. Cell 178, 429446
9. Dumas, G. et al. (2010) Inter-brain synchronization during so-
cial interaction. PLoS One 5, e12166
10. Csibra, G. and Gergely, G. (2009 ) Natural pedagogy. Trends
Cogn. Sci. 13, 148153
11. Albert, R.R. et al. (2018) The social func tions of babbling:
acoustic and contextual characteristics that facilitate maternal
responsiveness. Dev. Sci. 21, e12641
12. Lucca, K. and Wilbourn, M.P. (2018) Communicating to learn:
Infantspointing gestures result in optimal learning. Child Dev.
89, 941960
13. Begus, K. and Southgate, V. (2018) Curious learners: how
infantsmotivation to learn shapes and is shaped by infantsin-
teractions with the social world. In Active Learning from Infancy
to Childhood, pp. 1337, Springer
14. Murray, L. and Trevarthen, C. (1986) The infantsrolein
motherinfant communications. J. Child Lang. 13, 1529
15. Stahl, A.E. and Feigenson, L. (2015) Observing the unexpected
enhances infantslearning and exploration. Science 348,
9194
16. Goupil, L. et al. (2016) Infants ask for help when they know they
dont know. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, 34923496
17. Goupil, L. and Kouider, S. (2016) Behavioral and neural indices
of metacognitive sensitivity in preverbal infants. Curr. Biol. 26,
30383045
18. Murray, L. et al. (2016) The functional architecture of mother-
infant communication, and the development of infant social ex-
pressiveness in the rst two months. Sci. Rep. 6, 39019
19. Feldman, R. (2007) Parentinfant synchrony and the construc-
tion of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental
outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry
48, 329354
20. Gratier, M. and Devouche, E. (2011) Imitation and repetition of
prosodic contour in vocal interaction at 3 months. Dev.
Psychol. 47, 67
21. Arco, C.M. et al. (1979) The effect of increased maternal visual
regard on neonatal visual behavior. J. Clin. Child Adolesc.
Psychol. 8, 117120
22. Senju, A. and Csibr a, G. (2008) Gaze f ollowing in huma n in-
fants depends on communicative signals. Curr. Biol. 18,
668671
23. Butterworth, G. and Itakura, S. (2000) How the eyes, head and
hand serve denite reference. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 18, 2550
24. Young, K.S. et al. (2017) The neural basis of responsive care-
giving behaviour: investigating temporal dynamics within the
parental brain. Behav. Brain Res. 325, 105116
25. Farroni, T. et al. (2002) Eye contact detection in humans from
birth. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 99, 96029605
26. Urakawa, S. et al. (2015) Selective medial prefrontal cortex re-
sponses during live mutual gaze interactions in human infants:
an fNIRS study. Brain Topogr. 28, 691701
27. Grossmann, T. et al. (2008) Early cortical specialization for
face-to-face comm unication in human infa nts. Proc. R. Soc.
Lond. B Biol. Sci. 275, 28032811
28. Zhang, Y. et al. (2011) Neural coding of formant-exaggerated
speech in the infant brain. Dev. Sci. 14, 566581
29. Piazza, E.A. et al. (2020) Infant and adult brains are coupled to
the dynamics of natural communication. Psychol. Sci. 31, 617
30. Striano, T. et al. (2006) Neural mechanisms of joint attention in
infancy. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 28192823
31. Hoehl, S. et al. (2014) Eye contact during live social interaction
modulates infantsoscillatory br ain activity. Soc. Neurosci. 9,
300308
32. Grossmann, T. et al. (2013) Brain responses reveal young
infantssensitivity to when a social partner follows their gaze.
Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 155161
33. Bourvis, N. et al. (2018) Pre-linguistic infants employ complex
communicative loops to engage mothers in social exchanges
and repair interaction ruptures. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5, 170274
34. Rayson, H. et al. (2019) Building blocks of joint attention: early
sensitivity to having onesowngazefollowed.Dev. Cogn.
Neurosci. 100631
35. Goldstein, M.H. et al. (2010) Learning while babbling:
prelinguistic object-directed vocalizations indicate a readiness
to learn. Infancy 15, 362391
36. Lavelli, M. and Foge l, A. (2013) Interdyad diff erences in early
motherinfant face-to-face communication: Real-time dynam-
ics and developmental pathways. Dev. Psychol. 49, 2257
37. Gallotti, M. et al. (2017) Alignment in social interactions.
Conscious. Cogn. 48, 253261
38. Hasson, U. and Frith, C.D. (2016) Mirroring and beyond:
coupled dynamics as a gener alized framework for mode lling
social interactions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371,
20150366
39. Meltzoff, A.N. and Marshall, P.J. (2018) Human infant imitation
as a social survival circuit. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 24, 130136
40. Bernhardt, B.C. and Singer, T. (2012) The neural basis of
empathy. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 123
41. Singer, T. et al. (2004) Empathy for pain involves the affective
but not sensory components of pain. Science303, 11571162
42. Leerkes, E.M. et al. (2016) Pathways by which mothersphys-
iological arousal and regulation while caregiving predict sensi-
tivity to infant distress. J. Fam. Psychol. 30, 769
43. Wass, S.V. et al. (2018) Parental neural responsivity to infants
visual attention: how mature brains inuence immature brains
during social interaction. PLoS Biol. 16, e2006328
44. Gianino, A. et al. (1988) The mutual regulation model: the
infants self and interactiv e regulation and coping and defen-
sive capacities. In Stress and Coping across Development
Field, pp. 4768, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
45. Gvirts, H.Z. and Perlmutter, R. (2019) What guides us
to neurally and behaviorally align with anyone specic?
ANeurobiological Model Based on fNIRS hyperscanning
studies. Neuroscientist. Published online July 11, 2019.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858419861912
46. Zhang, W. and Yartsev, M.M. (2019) Correlated neural activity
across the brains of socially interacting bats. Cell 178, 413428
47. Davidesco, I . et al. (2019) Brain-t o-brain synch rony predicts
long-term memory retention more accurately than individual
brain measures. bioRxiv 644047
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
12 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx
48. Pan, Y. et al. (2018) Int erpersonal sync hronization o f inferior
frontal cortices tracks social interactive learning of a song.
Neuroimage 183, 280290
49. Reindl, V. et al. (2019) Conducting hyperscanning experiments
with function al near-infrar ed spectroscopy . J. Vis. Exp. 143,
e58807
50. Hong, K.-S. et al. (2015) Classication of prefrontal and motor
cortex signals for three-class fNIRSBCI. Neurosci. Lett. 587,
8792
51. Nguyen, T., Schleihauf, H., Kayhan, E., Matthes, D., Vrtička, P.
& Hoehl, S., Th e effects of inte raction qual ity on neural syn-
chrony durin g mother-chil d problem solv ing. Cereb. Co rtex,
124, 235249
52. Reindl, V. et al. (2018) Brain-to-brain synchrony in parent-child
dyads and the relationship with emotion regulation revealed by
fNIRS-based hyperscanning. Neuroimage 178, 493502
53. Pan, Y. et al. (2017) Cooper ation in lovers: an f NIRS -based
hyperscanning study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 831841
54. Miller, J.G. et al. (2019) Inter-brain sy nchrony in mother-child
dyads during cooperation: an fNIRS hyperscanning study.
Neuropsychologia 124, 117124
55. Noreika, V. et al.14challengesandtheirsolutionsfor
conducting social neuroscience and longitudinal EEG research
with infants. Infant Behav. Dev., 58, 101393
56. Jones, E.J. et al. (2015) Developmental changes in infant brain
activity during naturalistic social experiences. Dev. Psychobiol.
57, 842853
57. Orekhova, E.V. et al. (20 06) EEG theta rhythm in infant s and
preschool children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 10471062
58. Liu, D. et al. (2018) Interactive brain activity: review and prog-
ress on EEG-based hyperscanning in social interactions.
Front. Psychol. 9, 1862
59. Leong, V. et al. (2019) Mother-infant interpersonal neural con-
nectivity predicts infantssoci al learning. PsyArXiv. Published
online July 25, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/gueaq
60. Leong, V. et al. (2017) Speaker gaze increases information
coupling between infant and adult brains. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci. 114, 1329013295
61. Covarrubias, L.S. et al. (2020) Emotion al valence modu lates
the topology of the parent-infant inter-brain network.
NeuroImage 207, 116341
62. Doelling, K.B. et al. (2014) Acoustic landmarks drive delta
theta oscillations to enable speech comprehension by facilitat-
ing perceptual parsing. Neuroimage 85, 761768
63. Fishburn, F.A. et al. (2018 ) Putting our heads together: in ter-
personal neural synchronization as a biological mechanism
for shared intentionality. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13,
841849
64. Stolk, A. et al. (2014) Cerebral coherence between communi-
cators marks the emergence of meaning. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci. 111, 1818318188
65. Simony, E. et al. (2016) Dynamic reconguration of the default
mode network during narrative comprehension. Nat. Commun.
7, 12141
66. Liu, Y. et al. (2017) Measuring speakerlistener neural coupling
with functional near infrared spectroscopy. Sci. Rep. 7, 43293
67. Nummenmaa, L. et al. (2012) Emotions promote social interac-
tion by sync hronizing b rain activi ty across in dividuals . Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. 109, 95999604
68. Hu, Y. et al. (2018) Inter-brain synchrony and cooperation con-
text in interactive decision making. Biol. Psychol. 133, 5462
69. Konvalinka, I. and Roepstorff, A. (2012) The two-brain ap-
proach: how can mutually interacting brains teach us
something about social interaction? Front. Hum. Neurosci.
6, 215
70. Parise, E. and Csibra, G. (2013) N eural responses to multi-
modal ostensive signals in 5-month-old infants. PLoS One 8,
e72360
71. Robertson, S.S. (2014) Empty-headed dynamical model of in-
fant visual foraging. Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 11291133
72. Beebe, B. et al. (2016) A systems view of motherinfant face-
to-face communication. Dev. Psychol. 52, 556
73. Grayson, D.S. and Fair, D.A. (2017) Development of large-
scale functional networks from birth t o adulthood: a guide to
the neuroimaging literature. Neuroimage 160, 1531
74. Vakorin, V.A. et al. (2011) Variability of brain signals processed
locally transforms into higher connectivity with brain develop-
ment. J. Neurosci. 31, 64056413
75. Atzil, S. et al. (2018) Growing a social brain. Nat. Hum. Behav.
2, 624636
76. Fogel, A. (2017) Two principles of communication: co-regulation
and framing. In New Perspectives in Early Commun icative
Development, pp. 922, Routledge
77. Buzsaki, G. (2006) Rhythms of the Brain,OxfordUniversity
Press
78. Lakatos, P. et al. (2008) Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as
a mechanism of attentional selection. Science 320, 110113
79. Busch, N.A. et al. (2009) Th e phase of ongoing EEG oscilla -
tions predicts visual perception. J. Neurosci. 29, 78697876
80. Ruzzoli, M. et al. (2019) The relevance of alpha phase in human
perception. Cortex 120, 249268
81. Mathewson, K.E. et al. (2009) To see or not to see: prestimulus α
phase predicts visual aware ness. J. Neurosci. 29, 27252732
82. Hagen, E.H. and Bryant, G.A. (2003) Music and dance as a co-
alition signaling system. Hum. Nat. 14, 2151
83. Johnson, S. (2009) Emergence: the Connected Lives of Ants,
Brains, Cities, and Software, Simon and Schuster
84. Di Paolo, E.A. (2001) Rhythmic and non-rhythmic attractors in
asynchronous random Boolean networks. BioSystems 59,
185195
85. Kennedy, J. (2006) Swarm intelligence. In Handbook of Nature-
Inspired and Innovative Computing, pp. 187219, Springer
86. Engel, A.K. and Sing er, W. (2001) Temporal bi nding and the
neural correlates of sensory awareness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5,
1625
87. Calderone, D.J. et al. (2014) Entrainment of neural oscillations
as a modiable substrate of attention. Trends Co gn. Sci. 18,
300309
88. Regev, M. et al. (2018) Prop agation of informat ion along the
cortical hierarchy as a function of attention w hile reading and
listening to stories. Cereb. Cortex 29, 4107-4034
89. Giraud, A.-L. and Poeppel, D. (2012) Cortical oscillations and
speech processing: emerging computational principles and
operations. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 511517
90. Burgess, A.P. (2013) On the interpretation of synchronization in
EEG hypersc anning studi es: a cautiona ry note. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 7, 881
91. Lachaux, J.P. et al. (1999) Measuring phase synchrony in brain
signals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 194208
92. Grinsted, A. et al. (2004) Application of the cross wavelet
transform and wavelet coherence to geophysical time series.
Nonlinear Process. Geophys. 11, 561566
93. Granger, C.W. (1969) Investigating causal relations by econo-
metric models and cross-spectral methods. Econometri ca
424438
94. Baccalá, L.A. and Same shima, K. (2001) Partial dire cted co-
herence: a new concept in neural structure determination.
Biol. Cybern. 84, 463474
95. Babiloni, F. and Astol,L.(2014)Socialneuroscienceand
hyperscanning techniques: past, present and future. Neurosci.
Biobehav. Rev. 44, 7693
96. Cohen,M.X. (2014) Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory
and Practice, MIT Press
97. Dustman, R.E. et al. (1999) Life-span changes in EEG spectral
amplitude, amplitude variability and mean frequency. Clin.
Neurophysiol. 110, 13991409
98. Shmueli, K. et al. (2007) Low-frequency uctuations in the car-
diac rate as a source of varianc e in the resting-state fMRI
BOLD signal. Neuroimage 38, 306320
99. Fairhurst, M. and Dumas, G. (2019) Reciprocity and alignment:
quantifying coupling in dynamicinteractions.PsyArXiv. Published
online 25 March 2019. http://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/nmg4x
100. Kaye, K. and Fogel, A. (1980) The temporal structure of face-
to-face communication between mothers and infants. Dev.
Psychol. 16, 454
101. Tronick, E. (1982) Social interchange in Infancy: Affect,
Cognition, and Communication, University Park Press
102. Leclère, C. et al. (2014) Why synchrony matters during
mother-c hild inter actions: a sy stematic r eview. PLoS One 9,
e113571
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx 13
103. Cohn, J.F. and Tronick, E.Z. (1988) Mot her-infant face-to-
face interaction: inuence is bidirectional and unrelated to
periodic cycles in either partnersbehavior.Dev. Psychol.
24, 386392
104. Trevarthen, C. (1979) Communication and cooperation in early
infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity.Before
speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication 1
pp. 530571
105. Jaffe, J. et al. (2001) Rhythms of dialogu e in infancy:
coordinated timing in developme nt. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child
Dev. i-149
106. Beebe, B. et al. (2010) The origins of 12-month attachment: a
microanalysis of 4-month motherinfant interaction. Attach
Hum. Dev. 12, 3141
107. Feldman, R. (2003) Infant-mother and infant-father synchrony:
the coregulation of positive arousal. Infant Ment. Health J. 24,
123
108. Feldman, R. et al. (1999) Motherinfant affect synchrony as an
antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Dev. Psychol. 35,
223
109. Feldman, R. and Greenbaum, C.W. (1997) Affect regulation
and synchrony in motherinfant play as precursors to the de-
velopment of symbolic competence. Infant Ment. Health J.
18, 423
110. Granat, A. et al. (2017) Maternal depression and anxiety, social
synchrony, and infant regulation of negative and positive
emotions. Emotion 17, 11
111. Niedźwiecka, A. et al. (2017) Mutual gaze during early mother
infant interactions promotes attention control development.
Child Dev. 89, 22302244
112. Harel, H. et al. (2011) Gaze behaviors of preterm and full-term
infants in nonsocial and social contexts of increasing dynamics:
visual recognition, attention regulation, and gaze synchrony.
Infancy 16, 6990
113. Field, T. et al. (1990) Behavior-state matching and synchrony in
mother-infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed
dyads. Dev. Psychol. 26, 7
114. Yu, C. and Smith, L.B. (2013) Joint attention without gaze
following: human infants and their parents coordinate visual at-
tention to objects through eye-hand coordination.PLoS One 8,
e79659
115. Siposova, B. and Carpenter, M. (2019) A new look at joint at-
tention and common knowledge. Cognition 189, 260274
116. Akhtar, N. and Gernsbacher, M.A. (2008) On privileging the role
of gaze in infant social cognition. Child Dev. Perspect. 2, 5965
117. Gliga, T. et al. (2018) Social touch: a new vista for developmen-
tal cognitive neuroscience? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 35, 14
118. Cirelli, L.K. (2018) How interpersonal synchrony facilitates early
prosocial behavior. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 3539
119. Levy, J. et al. (2017) Perc eption of social syn chrony induces
motherchild gamma coupling in the social brain. Soc. Cogn.
Affect. Neurosci. 12, 10361046
120. Feldman, R. et al. (2011) Mo ther and infant coo rdinate heart
rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant
Behav. Dev. 34, 569577
121. Ham, J. and Tronick, E. (2009) Relational psychophysiology: les-
sons from motherinfant physiology research on dyadically ex-
panded statesof consciousness. Psychother. Res. 19, 619632
122. Waters, S.F. et al. (2 014) Stress contagion: physiolo gical co-
variation between mothers and infants. Psychol. Sci. 25,
934942
123. Wass, S.V. et al. (2019) Parent s mimic and inuence their in-
fantsautonomicstatethroughdynamicaffectivestate
matching. Curr. Biol. 29, 24152422
124. Smith, C.G. et al. (2019) Anxi ous parents show higher
physiological synchrony with their infants. PsyArXiv. Published
online 20 June 2019. http://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/
m2rsghttp://dx.doi.org/
125. Azhari, A. et al. (2019) Parenting Stress undermines mother-child
brain-to-brain synchrony: a hyperscanning study. Sci. Rep. 9, 19
126. Wass, S.V. et al. (2018) I nfantsvisual s ustained att ention is
higher during joint play than solo play: is this due to increased
endogenous attention control or exogenous stimulus capture?
Dev. Sci. 21, e12667
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
14 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Month 2020, Vol. xx, No. xx
... Recently, increasing studies have demonstrated that even in very early ages, young children acted not only as purely passive recipients of information, but also played an active role as senders (Murray & Trevarthen, 1986;Stahl & Feigenson, 2015;Wass et al., 2020). The roles that each partner takes on are usually not fixed, but alternate throughout the interactions (Schilbach et al., 2013;Wass et al., 2020). ...
... Recently, increasing studies have demonstrated that even in very early ages, young children acted not only as purely passive recipients of information, but also played an active role as senders (Murray & Trevarthen, 1986;Stahl & Feigenson, 2015;Wass et al., 2020). The roles that each partner takes on are usually not fixed, but alternate throughout the interactions (Schilbach et al., 2013;Wass et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Mother–child interaction is highly dynamic and reciprocal. Switching roles in these back‐and‐forth interactions serves as a crucial feature of reciprocal behaviors while the underlying neural entrainment is still not well‐studied. Here, we designed a role‐controlled cooperative task with dual EEG recording to explore how differently two brains interact when mothers and children hold different roles. When children were actors and mothers were observers, mother–child interbrain synchrony emerged primarily within the theta oscillations and the frontal lobe, which highly correlated with children's attachment to their mothers (self‐reported by mothers). When their roles were reversed, this synchrony was shifted to the alpha oscillations and the central area and associated with mothers' perception of their relationship with their children. The results suggested an observer‐actor neural alignment within the actor's oscillations, which was related to the actor‐toward‐observer emotional bonding. Our findings contribute to the understanding of how interbrain synchrony is established and dynamically changed during mother–child reciprocal interaction.
... The behavioral coding approach is helpful for quantifying the extent to which specific communicative behaviors contribute to IBS, thereby interpreting neural-behavioral relationships [57,[65][66][67]. However, the subjective nature of behavioral coding raises reliability concerns, even when a detailed coding scheme is provided and multiple raters participate. ...
... These cognitive impairments lead to poor rehabilitation outcomes [123][124][125][126]. The interpersonal neural entrainment induced by oscillatory synchronization through phase alignment can facilitate the efficient transfer of information from one brain to another, resulting in better social interaction during instruction-based learning [66,127]. Consistent with this, in-phase theta hyper-tACS applied to the inferior frontal cortex in both instructors and learners enhanced interactive social learning [105]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Our actions and decisions in everyday life are heavily influenced by social interactions, which are dynamic feedback loops involving actions, reactions, and internal cognitive processes between individual agents. Social interactions induce interpersonal synchrony, which occurs at different biobehavioral levels and comprises behavioral, physiological, and neurological activities. Hyperscanning—a neuroimaging technique that simultaneously measures the activity of multiple brain regions—has provided a powerful second-person neuroscience tool for investigating the phase alignment of neural processes during interactive social behavior. Neural synchronization, revealed by hyperscanning, is a phenomenon called inter-brain synchrony- a process that purportedly facilitates social interactions by prompting appropriate anticipation of and responses to each other's social behaviors during ongoing shared interactions. In this review, I explored the therapeutic dual-brain approach using noninvasive brain stimulation to target inter-brain synchrony based on second-person neuroscience to modulate social interaction. Artificially inducing synchrony between the brains is a potential adjunct technique to physiotherapy, psychotherapy, and pain treatment- which are strongly influenced by the social interaction between the therapist and patient. Dual-brain approaches to personalize stimulation parameters must consider temporal, spatial, and oscillatory factors. Multiple data fusion analysis, the assessment of inter-brain plasticity, a closed-loop system, and a brain-to-brain interface can support personalized stimulation.
... Recent advancements in technology, enabling simultaneous neuroimaging of two or more interacting persons (referred to as hyperscanning), have provided researchers with novel avenues to explore the intricate dynamics between two interacting brains during social interactions (Hamilton, 2021;Hasson and Frith, 2016;Hoehl et al., 2021;Holroyd, 2022;Novembre and Iannetti, 2021;Pérez and Davis, 2023;Dumas and Fairhurst, 2021;Turk et al., 2022;Nguyen et al., 2021;Wass et al., 2020;Kingsbury and Hong, 2020). ...
Article
Modern technology allows for simultaneous neuroimaging from interacting caregiver-child dyads. Whereas most analyses that examine the coordination between brain regions within an individual brain do so by measuring changes relative to observed events, studies that examine coordination between two interacting brains generally do this by measuring average intra-brain coordination across entire blocks or experimental conditions. In other words, they do not examine changes in inter-brain coordination relative to individual behavioural events. Here, we discuss the limitations of this approach. First, we present data suggesting that fine-grained temporal interdependencies in behaviour can leave residual artifact in neuroimaging data. We show how artifact can manifest as both power and (through that) phase synchrony effects in EEG and affect wavelet transform coherence in fNIRS analyses. Second, we discuss different possible mechanistic explanations of how inter-brain coordination is established and maintained. We argue that non-event-locked approaches struggle to differentiate between them. Instead, we contend that approaches which examine how interpersonal dynamics change around behavioural events have better potential for addressing possible artifactual confounds and for teasing apart the overlapping mechanisms that drive changes in inter-brain coordination.
... Again, a similar signi cant quadratic trend was observed in BtBC along with the presence or absence of statistical regularity, which strengthens the fundamental role of BtBC underlying the self-other integration. Although there is a lack of direct replications of BtBC in current related self-other integration studies, increased BtBC has been repeatedly reported in contexts involving interpersonal coordination, such as interpersonal cooperation 43 , interpersonal entrainment 44 , and even interpersonal observational learning 19 . ...
Preprint
Full-text available
While statistical learning has often been investigated in an individual context, it remains unclear whether humans are able to integrate information from both the self and from another to build a collective representation of regularities. Here, we investigated the dynamic self–other integration process and its multi-brain mechanism by recording EEG activity simultaneously from dyads. Participants (N = 112) each responded repeatedly to one half of a fixed stimulus sequence either with an active partner (i.e., joint context) or with a passive observer (i.e., baseline context). At individual level, we found that a significant statistical learning effect in the joint context characterized by decreased trends in reaction time (RT) and intra-brain neural responses (e.g., ERPs and functional connectivities) as well as a subsequent modulation by an insertion of an interference sequence. At dyad level, Brain-to-Brain Coupling (BtBC) in the theta band first showed an increasing trend followed by a subsequent modulation, providing direct neural evidence for the occurrence of a dynamic self–other integration process. Critically, the strength of BtBC was negatively correlated with RT and positively correlated with intra-brain functional connectivities. These findings suggest that BtBC serves as a crucial neural correlate of self–other integration underpinning the joint statistical learning effect, and that statistical regularity can both implicitly and spontaneously modulate the occurrence of the self–other integration process.
... This account furthermore suggests that temporal regularity of communicative behaviors and their subjective socio-emotional weighting facilitates mutual predictions Markova et al., 2019;Wass et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
It is a central tenet of attachment theory that individual differences in attachment representations organize behavior during social interactions. Secure attachment representations also facilitate behavioral synchrony, a key component of adaptive parent–child interactions. Yet, the dynamic neural processes underlying these interactions and the potential role of attachment representations remain largely unknown. A growing body of research indicates that interpersonal neural synchrony (INS) could be a potential neurobiological correlate of high interaction and relationship quality. In this study, we examined whether interpersonal neural and behavioral synchrony during parent–child interaction is associated with parent and child attachment representations. In total, 140 parents (74 mothers and 66 fathers) and their children (age 5–6 years; 60 girls and 80 boys) engaged in cooperative versus individual problem-solving. INS in frontal and temporal regions was assessed with functional near-infrared spectroscopy hyperscanning. Attachment representations were ascertained by means of the Adult Attachment Interview in parents and a story-completion task in children, alongside video-coded behavioral synchrony. Findings revealed increased INS during cooperative versus individual problem solving across all dyads (𝛸2(2) = 9.37, p = 0.009). Remarkably, individual differences in attachment representations were associated with INS but not behavioral synchrony (p > 0.159) during cooperation. More specifically, insecure maternal attachment representations were related to higher mother–child INS in frontal regions (𝛸2(3) = 9.18, p = 0.027). Conversely, secure daughter attachment representations were related to higher daughter–parent INS within temporal regions (𝛸2(3) = 12.58, p = 0.006). Our data thus provide further indication for INS as a promising correlate to probe the neurobiological underpinnings of attachment representations in the context of early parent–child interactions.
... As depicted in this definition, the concept of interpersonal synchrony encompasses behavioral, physiological, and neural levels of analysis of the dynamics occurring between interacting individuals. In recent years, several authors have attempted to embed recent findings in a more comprehensive context ultimately providing a broader definition of the terms themselves [for review, (2)(3)(4)]. Focusing on the motor aspects of IS, hereafter referred to as Interpersonal Motor Synchrony (IMS), evidence shows that people interact through coordination of eye and body movements, posture, gestures, and facial expressions across the entire life cycle (5,6). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Interpersonal motor synchrony (IMS) is the spontaneous, voluntary, or instructed coordination of movements between interacting partners. Throughout the life cycle, it shapes social exchanges and interplays with intra- and inter-individual characteristics that may diverge in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Here we perform a systematic review and meta-analysis to summarize the extant literature and quantify the evidence about reduced IMS in dyads including at least one participant with a diagnosis of ASD. Methods Empirical evidence from sixteen experimental studies was systematically reviewed, encompassing spontaneous and instructed paradigms as well as a paucity of measures used to assess IMS. Of these, thirteen studies (n = 512 dyads) contributed measures of IMS with an in situ neurotypical partner (TD) for ASD and control groups, which could be used for meta-analyses. Results Reduced synchronization in ASD-TD dyads emerged from both the systematic review and meta-analyses, although both small and large effect sizes (i.e., Hedge’s g) in favor of the control group are consistent with the data (Hedge’s g = .85, p < 0.001, 95% CI[.35, 1.35], 95% PI[-.89, 2.60]). Discussion Uncertainty is discussed relative to the type of task, measures, and age range considered in each study. We further discuss that sharing similar experiences of the world might help to synchronize with one another. Future studies should not only assess whether reduced IMS is consistently observed in ASD-TD dyads and how this shapes social exchanges, but also explore whether and how ASD-ASD dyads synchronize during interpersonal exchanges.
Article
Cooperation and competition are the most common forms of social interaction in various social relationships. Intergroup relationships have been posited to influence individuals’ interpersonal interactions significantly. Using electroencephalography hyperscanning, this study aimed to establish whether intergroup relationships influence interpersonal cooperation and competition and the underlying neural mechanisms. According to the results, the in-group Coop-index is better than the out-group, whereas the out-group Comp-index is stronger than the in-group. The in-group functional connectivity between the frontal-central region and the right temporoparietal junction in the β band was stronger in competition than cooperation. The out-group functional connectivity between the frontal-central region and the left temporoparietal junction in the α band was stronger in cooperation than competition. In both cooperation and competition, the in-group exhibited higher interbrain synchronization between the prefrontal cortex and parietal region in the θ band, as well as between the frontal-central region and frontal-central region in the α band, compared to the out-group. The intrabrain phase-locking value in both the α and β bands can effectively predict performance in competition tasks. Interbrain phase-locking value in both the α and θ bands can be effectively predicted in a performance cooperation task. This study offers neuroscientific evidence for in-group favoritism and out-group bias at an interpersonal level.
Chapter
Attention and perception form the underlying pivots around which social interactions function. This chapter explores the crucial roles of attention and perception in various clinical conditions by delving into how impairments in these domains affect individuals with conditions like autism spectrum disorder (ASD), social anxiety disorder (SAD), traumatic brain injury (TBI), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), prosopagnosia, and schizophrenia. The author discusses various theoretical models such as the theory of mind (ToM), and attribution theory among others that inform one's understanding of the clinical applications of attention and perception within social cognition highlighting the importance of tailored rehabilitative programs. The author also gives examples of five-session, attention-focused, and perception-focused rehabilitative plans for various clinical conditions. Future research into the neural mechanisms and interactions between attention, perception, and higher-order functions offers potential for more effective treatments.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Interpersonal synchronization involves the alignment of behavioral, affective, physiological, and brain states during social interactions. It facilitates empathy, emotion regulation, and prosocial commitment. Mental disorders characterized by social interaction dysfunction, such as Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), Reactive Attachment Disorder (RAD), and Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD), often exhibit atypical synchronization with others across multiple levels. With the introduction of the “second-person” neuroscience perspective, our understanding of interpersonal neural synchronization (INS) has improved, however, so far, it has hardly impacted the development of novel therapeutic interventions. Methods To evaluate the potential of INS-based treatments for mental disorders, we performed two systematic literature searches identifying studies that directly target INS through neurofeedback (12 publications; 9 independent studies) or brain stimulation techniques (7 studies), following PRISMA guidelines. In addition, we narratively review indirect INS manipulations through behavioral, biofeedback, or hormonal interventions. We discuss the potential of such treatments for ASD, RAD, and SAD and using a systematic database search assess the acceptability of neurofeedback (4 studies) and neurostimulation (4 studies) in patients with social dysfunction. Results Although behavioral approaches, such as engaging in eye contact or cooperative actions, have been shown to be associated with increased INS, little is known about potential long-term consequences of such interventions. Few proof-of-concept studies have utilized brain stimulation techniques, like transcranial direct current stimulation or INS-based neurofeedback, showing feasibility and preliminary evidence that such interventions can boost behavioral synchrony and social connectedness. Yet, optimal brain stimulation protocols and neurofeedback parameters are still undefined. For ASD, RAD, or SAD, so far no randomized controlled trial has proven the efficacy of direct INS-based intervention techniques, although in general brain stimulation and neurofeedback methods seem to be well accepted in these patient groups. Discussion Significant work remains to translate INS-based manipulations into effective treatments for social interaction disorders. Future research should focus on mechanistic insights into INS, technological advancements, and rigorous design standards. Furthermore, it will be key to compare interventions directly targeting INS to those targeting other modalities of synchrony as well as to define optimal target dyads and target synchrony states in clinical interventions.
Article
Full-text available
Understanding others is fundamental to interpersonal coordination and successful cooperation. One mechanism posited to underlie both effective communication and behavioral coordination is interpersonal neural synchrony. Although presumably foundational for children’s social development, research on neural synchrony in naturalistic caregiver-child interactions is lacking. Using dual-functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), we examined the effects of interaction quality on neural synchrony during a problem-solving task in 42 dyads of mothers and their preschool children. In a cooperation condition, mothers and children were instructed to solve a tangram puzzle together. In an individual condition, mothers and children performed the same task alone with an opaque screen between them. Wavelet transform coherence (WTC) was used to assess the cross-correlation between the two fNIRS time series. Results revealed increased neural synchrony in bilateral prefrontal cortex and temporo-parietal areas during cooperative as compared to individual problem solving. Higher neural synchrony during cooperation correlated with higher behavioral reciprocity and neural synchrony predicted the dyad’s problem-solving success beyond reciprocal behavior between mothers and children. State-like factors, such as maternal stress and child agency during the task, played a bigger role for neural synchronization than trait-like factors, such as child temperament. Our results emphasize neural synchrony as a biomarker for mother-child interaction quality. These findings further highlight the role of state-like factors in interpersonal synchronization processes linked to successful coordination with others and in the long-term might improve the understanding of others.
Article
Full-text available
Infancy is the foundational period for learning from adults, and the dynamics of the social environment have long been considered central to children’s development. Here, we reveal a novel, naturalistic approach for studying live interactions between infants and adults. Using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), we simultaneously and continuously measured the brains of infants ( N = 18; 9–15 months of age) and an adult while they communicated and played with each other. We found that time-locked neural coupling within dyads was significantly greater when dyad members interacted with each other than with control individuals. In addition, we characterized the dynamic relationship between neural activation and the moment-to-moment fluctuations of mutual gaze, joint attention to objects, infant emotion, and adult speech prosody. This investigation advances what is currently known about how the brains and behaviors of infants both shape and reflect those of adults during real-life communication.
Article
Full-text available
Emotional communication between parents and children is crucial during early life, yet little is known about its neural underpinnings. Here, we adopt a dual connectivity approach to assess how positive and negative emotions modulate the interpersonal neural network between infants and their mothers during naturalistic interaction. Fifteen mothers were asked to model positive and negative emotions toward pairs of objects during social interaction with their infants (mean age 10.3 months) whilst the neural activity of both mothers and infants was concurrently measured using dual electroencephalogram (EEG). Intra-brain and inter-brain network connectivity in the 6-9 Hz range (i.e. infant Alpha band) during maternal expression of positive and negative emotions was computed using directed (partial directed coherence, PDC) and non-directed (phase-locking value, PLV) connectivity metrics. Graph theoretical measures were used to quantify differences in network topology as a function of emotional valence. We found that inter-brain network indices (Density, Strength and Divisibility) consistently revealed strong effects of emotional valence on the parent-child neural network. Parents and children showed stronger integration of their neural processes during maternal demonstrations of positive than negative emotions. Further, directed inter-brain metrics (PDC) indicated that mother to infant directional influences were stronger during the expression of positive than negative emotional states. These results suggest that the parent-infant inter-brain network is modulated by the emotional quality and tone of dyadic social interactions, and that inter-brain graph metrics may be successfully applied to examine these changes in parent-infant inter-brain network topology.
Article
Full-text available
Synchrony refers to the coordinated interplay of behavioural and physiological signals that reflect the bi-directional attunement of one partner to the other’s psychophysiological, cognitive, emotional, and behavioral state. In mother-child relationships, a synchronous pattern of interaction indicates parental sensitivity. Parenting stress has been shown to undermine mother-child behavioural synchrony. However, it has yet to be discerned whether parenting stress affects brain-to-brain synchrony during everyday joint activities. Here, we show that greater parenting stress is associated with less brain-to-brain synchrony in the medial left cluster of the prefrontal cortex when mother and child engage in a typical dyadic task of watching animation videos together. This brain region overlaps with the inferior frontal gyrus, the frontal eye field, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which are implicated in inference of mental states and social cognition. Our result demonstrates the adverse effect of parenting stress on mother-child attunement that is evident at a brain-to-brain level. Mother-child brain-to-brain asynchrony may underlie the robust association between parenting stress and poor dyadic co-regulation. We anticipate our study to form the foundation for future investigations into mechanisms by which parenting stress impairs the mother-child relationship.
Preprint
Social learning allows infants to learn vicariously by observing adult behaviour, but how the infant brain accomplishes this feat remains unknown. Here, electroencephalography (EEG) signals were simultaneously measured from forty-seven mothers and infants (10.7 months) during a live social learning task. First, infants observed mothers demonstrate positive or negative emotions toward novel toys. Next, infants’ own toy interaction (learning) was measured. Infants’ social learning likelihood was robustly predicted by mother-infant interpersonal neural connectivity in the Alpha (6-9 Hz) band. Stronger dyadic neural connectedness predicted increased learning, and was associated with extended ostensive eye contact and maternal utterances. Intra-infant neural connectivity predicted learning valence (positive/negative) but was unrelated to learning likelihood. Therefore, interpersonal connectivity is a neural mechanism by which infants learn from their social partners.
Article
The use of electroencephalography (EEG) to study infant brain development is a growing trend. In addition to classical longitudinal designs that study the development of neural, cognitive and behavioural functions, new areas of EEG application are emerging, such as novel social neuroscience paradigms using dual infant-adult EEG recordings. However, most of the experimental designs, analysis methods, as well as EEG hardware were originally developed for single-person adult research. When applied to study infant development, adult-based solutions often pose unique problems that may go unrecognised. Here, we identify 14 challenges that infant EEG researchers may encounter when designing new experiments, collecting data, and conducting data analysis. Challenges related to the experimental design are: (1) small sample size and data attrition, and (2) varying arousal in younger infants. Challenges related to data acquisition are: (3) determining the optimal location for reference and ground electrodes, (4) control of impedance when testing with the high-density sponge electrode nets, (5) poor fit of standard EEG caps to the varying infant head shapes, and (6) ensuring a high degree of temporal synchronisation between amplifiers and recording devices during dual-EEG acquisition. Challenges related to the analysis of longitudinal and social neuroscience datasets are: (7) developmental changes in head anatomy, (8) prevalence and diversity of infant myogenic artefacts, (9) a lack of stereotypical topography of eye movements needed for the ICA-based data cleaning, (10) and relatively high inter-individual variability of EEG responses in younger cohorts. Additional challenges for the analysis of dual EEG data are: (11) developmental shifts in canonical EEG rhythms and difficulties in differentiating true inter-personal synchrony from spurious synchrony due to (12) common intrinsic properties of the signal and (13) shared external perturbation. Finally, (14) there is a lack of test-retest reliability studies of infant EEG. We describe each of these challenges and suggest possible solutions. While we focus specifically on the social neuroscience and longitudinal research, many of the issues we raise are relevant for all fields of infant EEG research.
Article
Everyone agrees that joint attention is a key feature of human social cognition. Yet, despite over 40 years of work and hundreds of publications on this topic, there is still surprisingly little agreement on what exactly joint attention is, and how the jointness in it is achieved. Part of the problem, we propose, is that joint attention is not a single process, but rather it includes a cluster of different cognitive skills and processes, and different researchers focus on different aspects of it. A similar problem applies to common knowledge. Here we present a new approach: We outline a typology of social attention levels which are currently all referred to in the literature as joint attention (from monitoring to common, mutual, and shared attention), along with corresponding levels of common knowledge. We consider cognitive, behavioral, and phenomenological aspects of the different levels as well as their different functions, and a key distinction we make in all of this is second-personal vs. third-personal relations. While we focus mainly on joint attention and common knowledge, we also briefly discuss how these levels might apply to other 'joint' mental states such as joint goals.
Article
As scientists, we brainstorm and develop experimental designs with our colleagues and students. Paradoxically, this teamwork has produced a field focused nearly exclusively on mapping the brain as if it evolved in isolation. Here, we discuss promises and challenges in advancing our understanding of how human minds connect during social interaction. Full article at: https://repository.ubn.ru.nl/bitstream/handle/2066/205908/205908.pdf
Article
An emerging body of hyperscanning functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) research shows interbrain neural synchrony (IBS) during different forms of social interaction. Here we review the recent literature and propose several factors that facilitate IBS, leading us to ask the following question: In a world full of people and opportunities to synchronize with them, what directs our neural and behavioral alignment with anyone specific? We suggest that IBS between what we deem the “mutual social attention systems” of interacting partners—that is, the coupling between participants’ temporoparietal junctions and/or prefrontal cortices—facilitates and enhances the ability to tune in to the specific interaction, its participants and its goals. We propose that this process is linked to social alignment, reinforcing one another to facilitate successful and lucrative social interactions. We further suggest that neurochemical mechanisms of dopamine and oxytocin underlie the activation of this suggested loop. Finally, we suggest possible directions for future studies, emphasizing the need to develop a brain-to-brain neurofeedback system with IBS between the mutual social attention systems of the participants as the direct regulating target.
Article
When we see someone experiencing an emotion, and when we experience it ourselves, common neurophysiological activity occurs [1, 2]. But although inter-dyadic synchrony, concurrent and sequential [3], has been identified, its functional significance remains inadequately understood. Specifically, how do influences of partner A on partner B reciprocally influence partner A? For example, if I am experiencing an affective state and someone matches their physiological state to mine, what influence does this have on me-the person experiencing the emotion? Here, we investigated this using infant-parent dyads. We developed miniaturized microphones to record spontaneous vocalizations and wireless autonomic monitors to record heart rate, heart rate variability, and movement in infants and parents concurrently in naturalistic settings. Overall, we found that infant-parent autonomic activity did not covary across the day-but that "high points" of infant arousal led to autonomic changes in the parent and that instances where the adult showed greater autonomic responsivity were associated with faster infant quieting. Parental responsivity was higher following peaks in infant negative affect than in positive affect. Overall, parents responded to increases in their child's arousal by increasing their own. However, when the overall arousal level of the dyad was high, parents responded to elevated child arousal by decreasing their own arousal. Our findings suggest that autonomic state matching has a direct effect on the person experiencing the affective state and that parental co-regulation may involve both connecting and disconnecting their own arousal state from that of the child contingent on context.