ChapterPDF Available

The Interplay of Objective and Subjective Factors in Empirical Aesthetics

Authors:

Abstract

The field of empirical aesthetics sets out to understand and predict our aesthetic preferences (Palmer et al., Annual Review of Psychology 64(1):77–107, 2013). Its history dates back to the birth of visual psychophysics and the work of Gustav Fechner (Vorschule der aesthetik (Vol. 1). Brietkopf & Härtel, 1876), while multiple models of aesthetic experience have been proposed in the intervening years (Chatterjee A, Vartanian O, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18(7):370–375, 2014; Leder H et al., British Journal of Psychology 95(4):489–508, 2004; Pelowski M et al., Physics of Life Reviews 21:80–125, 2017). This chapter briefly sets out the history of empirical aesthetics, and the state of the research field at present. I outline recent work on inter-observer agreement in aesthetic preference, before presenting empirical work that argues the importance of first objective (characteristics of stimuli) and then subjective (characteristics of context) factors in shaping aesthetic preference. Considering the role of properties of the stimulus, I will review literature on the relationship between aesthetic preference and symmetry, shape, compositional structure, colour and complexity as well as considering the potential role of statistical properties of images. I will then review putative subjective predictors of aesthetic preference including the role of context, framing and the influence of information about the artist and the artistic process. Both subjective and objective approaches will be evaluated from an individual differences perspective, focusing on the mediating role of familiarity, expertise, culture, cognitive ability and personality. Finally, I will attempt to draw these approaches together with reference to aesthetic sensitivity: an individual observer’s propensity to have an aesthetic response to a particular objective image characteristic, and will explore some putative factors that may modulate and explain individual differences in aesthetic sensitivity.
The interplay of objective and subjective factors
in empirical aesthetics
Rebecca Chamberlain
Goldsmiths, University of London, 8 Lewisham Way, London SE14 6NW.
r.chamberlain@gold.ac.uk
Abstract The field of empirical aesthetics sets out to understand and predict our
aesthetic preferences (Palmer et al., 2013). Its history dates back to the birth of vis-
ual psychophysics and the work of Gustav Fechner (1876), while multiple models
of aesthetic experience have been proposed in the intervening years (Chatterjee &
Vartanian, 2014; Leder et al., 2004; Pelowski et al., 2017). This chapter briefly sets
out the history of empirical aesthetics, and the state of the research field at present.
I outline recent work on inter-observer agreement in aesthetic preference, before
presenting empirical work that argues the importance of first objective (character-
istics of stimuli) and then subjective (characteristics of context) factors in shaping
aesthetic preference. Considering the role of properties of the stimulus, I will review
literature on the relationship between aesthetic preference and symmetry, shape,
compositional structure, colour and complexity as well as considering the potential
role of statistical properties of images. I will then review putative subjective predic-
tors of aesthetic preference including the role of context, framing and the influence
of information about the artist and the artistic process. Both subjective and objective
approaches will be evaluated from an individual differences perspective, focusing
on the mediating role of familiarity, expertise, culture, cognitive ability and person-
ality. Finally, I will attempt to draw these approaches together with reference to
aesthetic sensitivity: an individual observer’s propensity to have an aesthetic re-
sponse to a particular objective image characteristic, and will explore some putative
factors that may modulate and explain individual differences in aesthetic sensitivity.
Introduction
The field of empirical aesthetics sets out to understand and predict human aes-
thetic preferences (Palmer et al., 2013). The origins of modern-day empirical aes-
thetics reside in the early psychophysical experiments of Gustav Fechner (1876) in
his seminal work ‘Vorschule der Aesthetik’. Fechner’s aesthetics ‘from below’ po-
sitioned objective stimulus properties at the heart of the empirical aesthetic project,
2
providing the foundation for later efforts to establish lawful relationships between
stimulus properties and aesthetic preferences (Birkhoff, 1933; Eysenck, 1940) with
reference to psychobiological mechanisms of arousal (Berlyne, 1974). Such efforts
focused on the predictive value of low-level stimulus properties, such as colour,
symmetry, proportion, contrast, contour, and later on collative properties such as
order, complexity and ambiguity (Berlyne, 1974). Such an approach remains com-
mon in empirical aesthetics. However, more recent research in the field has placed
focus on sensory and cognitive processing dynamics, modelling how observers re-
spond to salient properties of the stimulus (Flavell et al., 2020; Reber et al., 2004),
but also incorporating the sensory and cognitive history of the observer (Cutting,
2003; Zajonc, 1968). The latter approach highlights the critical role subjective as-
pects such as context and exposure play in shaping our aesthetic experiences. Ob-
jective and subjective perspectives have been brought together in comprehensive
aesthetic models in recent years, bringing both psychological and neuroscientific
understanding to the numerous objective and subjective mechanisms identified by
researchers in the field (Chatterjee & Vartanian, 2014; Leder et al., 2004; Leder &
Nadal, 2014; Pelowski et al., 2017; Tinio, 2013). Finally, contemporary accounts
focus on the additional role of curiosity and expectation violation in responses to
artworks (Muth et al., 2015; Van de Cruys & Wagemans, 2011).
This chapter will seek to address two key questions in the field of empirical aes-
thetics. The first is to what extent aesthetic preferences are shared or unique. If pref-
erences are found to be completely idiosyncratic this would strongly suggest that
attempts to establish lawful relations between stimulus properties and aesthetic pref-
erences are bound to fail. However, if preferences are found to be shared to some
degree, this does not necessarily entail that the shared variance among observers is
determined by objective stimulus properties, rather than common subjective expe-
riences (Vessel, 2010; Vessel et al., 2018). Therefore, the second question is to what
extent objective (characteristics of stimuli) and subjective (characteristics of con-
text) properties are responsible for shaping aesthetic preferences. Having addressed
these two critical questions, I will attempt to integrate an individual difference ap-
proach with stimulus-based approaches by exploring recent research on aesthetic
sensitivity. It is worth noting here that the focus of this chapter is on behavioural
empirical studies of preferences for visual stimuli. Much insight can be drawn from
neuroscientific perspectives on visual aesthetics (Chatterjee & Vartanian, 2014) and
from empirical work in other stimulus domains such as music (Brattico & Pearce,
2013), but such perspectives lie beyond the scope of this chapter.
Are aesthetic preferences shared or unique?
Aesthetic preferences are idiosyncratic (Vessel, 2010; Vessel et al., 2018),
but the extent of this idiosyncrasy appears to be strongly dependent on the stimulus
category at the focus of research. Vessel and Rubin (2010; 2018) investigated the
3
proportions of ‘shared’ and ‘private’ taste adult observers displayed across different
stimulus categories. Participants were required to make pairwise preference judg-
ments on pictures of real-world scenes and abstract images, and across-observer
agreement was computed via pairwise correlations between preference judgments
of every pair of participants (Vessel, 2010). Participants showed a high degree of
cross-observer agreement for pictures of real-world scenes (46%), while cross-ob-
server agreement for abstract images was significantly lower (20%). In addition,
within-observer reliability (correlations in participants’ preference estimates be-
tween the first and second half of the testing session) was high for both sets of im-
ages suggesting that variability in cross-observer agreement could not be attributed
to measurement error. In a follow-up study Vessel and Rubin (2018) measured pref-
erences for a much larger stimulus set including: faces, natural landscapes, interior
and exterior architecture, and visual art. Cross-observer agreement was highest for
an ethnically diverse sample of faces (66%), and a sample of natural landscapes
(29%), lower for architecture (12%), and lower still for visual art (8%). The reasons
for variance in cross-observer agreement across these domains could be due to prop-
erties of the stimulus; for example averageness, facial symmetry and sexual dimor-
phism have been shown to be consistent predictors of facial attractiveness (Fink &
Penton-Voak, 2002). On the other hand, such variance could be due to shared or
unique environmental mechanisms such as mere exposure, which posits that ob-
servers develop a preference for stimuli that they have had greater amounts of ex-
posure to (Zajonc, 1968). The following section will explore putative objective pre-
dictors of aesthetic preference in more detail.
Objective predictors of aesthetic preference
Symmetry
Symmetry has been described as an ‘aesthetic primitive’ due to the special status
conferred to it by the visual system (Makin et al., 2018). Increased regularity in
patterns appears to elicit more fluent visual processing, evidenced by increased ac-
curacy and reduced reaction times in behavioural data (Makin et al., 2016) and by
a greater amplitude of the sustained posterior negativity (SPN) in occipital elec-
trodes in event-related potential (ERP) studies (Makin et al., 2016). Correspond-
ingly, increased regularity strongly predicts observers’ implicit (Makin et al., 2012)
and explicit preferences for random dot patterns (Höfel & Jacobsen, 2003; Jacobsen
& Höfel, 2002), an effect that has been replicated in cross-cultural samples (Makin
et al., 2018). Preference for symmetry can be conceptualised as a broader preference
for perceptual goodness, or Prägnanz in the Gestalt psychological tradition (Palmer
4
& Griscom, 2013). In the context of Makin et al. (2016; 2018) perceptual goodness
was mathematically quantified using the Holographic Weight of Evidence Model
(Van der Helm & Leeuwenberg, 1996), which is defined as the relationship between
the evidence for regularity and the total amount of information in a pattern. These
mathematical approaches to stimulus properties overlap with computational ap-
proaches to aesthetics which are further elaborated in the section on Global Image
Properties below. Beyond the simple dot patterns used in the aforementioned studies
(Höfel & Jacobsen, 2003; Jacobsen & Höfel, 2002; Makin et al., 2012, 2016, 2018),
symmetry is also a predictor of preference for more complex and ecologically-valid
stimuli such as faces, flowers and landscapes (Bertamini et al., 2019; Hůla & Flegr,
2016; Perrett et al., 1999). Two distinct mechanisms may underlie preference for
symmetry. The first is perceptual fluency (Reber et al., 2004); more symmetrical
stimuli are easier to process by the visual system as evidenced by neuroscientific
and behavioural data, and ease of processing gives rise to feelings of pleasure and
reward (Makin et al. 2018). On the other hand preference for symmetry may result
from sexual selective mechanisms via an association between symmetry and phys-
ical fitness, a view that is supported by the fact that symmetry preference is strong-
est for faces compared with other non-biologically relevant stimuli (Little, 2014).
Shape and composition
Rudolf Arnheim (1965) argues compellingly for the significance of perceptual
goodness in his seminal work ‘Art and Visual Perception’, demonstrating its rele-
vance for higher-order shape and compositional properties of visual stimuli. There
has been much speculation concerning whether the golden ratio (or golden section,
denoted by the symbol ϕ) is a signifier of perceptual goodness in works of art and
design, and the presence of the golden ratio was one of the first objective stimulus
properties to be investigated in empirical aesthetics (Fechner, 1876). However,
there is little evidence to support the existence of a preference for the golden ratio.
Rather, in-depth studies on this topic have revealed preferences converging on pro-
totypical geometric shapes (McManus, 1980; McManus & Weatherby, 1997) and
on compact triangular shapes (Friedenberg, 2012). In terms of shape contour, a ro-
bust preference for curvature relative to angularity has been found for abstract geo-
metric shapes, real-life objects and environments (Bar & Neta, 2006; Palumbo et
al., 2015, 2020; Vartanian et al., 2013), a preference which has found to be reliable
in cross-cultural research (Gómez-Puerto et al., 2016). The origin of a preference
for curvature remains a debate in the literature. Some authors suggest it derives from
optimal stimulation of the visual system via Gestalt principles such as good contin-
uation (Bertamini et al., 2016), while other researchers argue that a preference for
curvature derives from an evolutionary adaptive avoidance of sharp stimuli (Bar &
Neta, 2006),
5
Extending out from preference for proportion and contour of singular forms,
Arnheim (1965) referred to the tension inherent in the configuration of forms, even
in a stimulus as simple as a circle within a frame (Figure 1). Arnheim posited that
observers prefer specific compositional arrangements that ensure balance and pre-
serve meaning. This was explored empirically in a series of studies in which partic-
ipants rated the goodness of dots placed in different locations in relation to a sur-
rounding frame (Wickens et al., 2008). The authors discovered a preference for dots
located in the centre and along the medial axes of a rectangular frame, lending sup-
port for Arnheim’s conjecture. This ‘centre-bias’ has since been replicated in studies
on photographic composition (Abeln et al., 2016) and drives eye movements during
free viewing of visual images (Judd et al., 2011; Tseng et al., 2009).
Fig. 1. Arnheim’s (1965) example of the tensions inherent in a form within a frame; the disk may
be perceived as being ‘drawn toward the contour to the right’ (p.12) and if the distance between
the disk and frame is altered, the effect may be weakened or there may be a contrary repulsion
effect.
However, the positioning of objects within a frame also interacts with an object’s
identity, such that objects facing or moving left-to-right are more preferred in the
left-hand side of the frame, and vice-versa, a phenomenon termed the inward bias
(Wickens et al., 2008). In a similar manner, vertical positioning of objects in a frame
interacts with the affordance spaces of those objects, such that a bowl is most pre-
ferred in a lower position in a frame, and a light fitting is preferred in a higher po-
sition in a frame (Sammartino & Palmer, 2012a). Such interactions make it increas-
ingly difficult to make straightforward predictions concerning which arrangement
of forms within a frame will be judged to be the most aesthetically pleasing.
Colour
Palmer & Schloss (2010) demonstrated that there are robust relationships be-
tween colour attributes hue, lightness and saturation and preferences for those at-
tributes. Western observers show relative preferences for hues at the blue end of the
spectrum and for relatively more saturated colours. Ou et al. (2004, 2018) theorised
that colour preferences are based on semantic associations with particular hues,
whilst Hurlbert & Ling (2007) demonstrated that cone-opponent colour processing
predicted colour preference curves. However, colour preferences show intriguing
6
hue-lightness interactions, such that observers show a marked dislike for dark yel-
lows and oranges, which are not explained successfully in the aforementioned the-
ories. This pattern of colour preference is accounted for by Ecological Valence The-
ory (EVT; Palmer & Schloss, 2010) which posits that colour preferences are
determined by the emotional valence of objects associated with those colours. Thus,
dark yellows and oranges are arguably disliked due to their associations with bio-
logical waste, and blues are preferred due to their association with clear skies and
water. This theory was empirically supported by amalgamating data from partici-
pants on their: object-colour associations, object valence, and object-colour match
ratings, creating a weighted affective valence estimate (WAVE). WAVEs predicted
participants colour preference data remarkably well (Palmer & Schloss, 2010) and
colour preferences could be altered by experimental exposure to objects with nega-
tive or positive valence (Strauss et al., 2013). Subsequent studies revealed that col-
our preferences could also be linked to associations with abstract concepts, such as
an observer’s university and political affiliation (Schloss et al., 2011; Schloss &
Palmer, 2014).
Order, complexity and Global Image Properties
In his influential book ‘Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps towards
an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation’ Daniel Berlyne (1974) posited
that stimuli of intermediate complexity generate an optimal level of arousal, and
should therefore be most preferred by observers. However, this conjecture has found
limited support, with empirical findings obscured by different conceptualisations
and manipulations of complexity (Nadal et al., 2010). Recent research has high-
lighted the complementary role of order or unity in Berlyne’s Psychobiological The-
ory, demonstrating that an optimal balance or combination of order and complexity
predicts ratings of soothingness and fascination for images of organised objects
(Van Geert & Wagemans, 2019). The interplay of order and complexity was first
highlighted by Birkhoff (1933) who developed a mathematical formula for aesthetic
preference via a balance of order and complexity (Van Geert & Wagemans, 2020),
foreshadowing computational approaches to aesthetics (Brachmann & Redies,
2017).
Image statistical approaches in aesthetics aim to determine Global Image Prop-
erties (GIP) of a stimulus that can be automatically computed and related to image
preference (Letsch & Hayn-Leichsenring, 2020). Image statistical analysis can pro-
duce a number of different measures including: fractality, self-similarity, complex-
ity, and anisotropy (variation in gradient orientations in an image). Statistical anal-
ysis of artworks has revealed that they are similar to natural scenes (Graham et al.,
2009; Graham & Redies, 2010; Redies et al., 2012) and that different styles and
periods of art can be attributed to their underlying image statistics (Hayn-Leichsen-
ring et al., 2017; Mather, 2018). Furthermore, image statistics correlate with verbal
7
descriptions of artworks, suggesting that they capture meaningful aspects of visual
stimuli (Letsch & Hayn-Leichsenring, 2020; Lyssenko et al., 2016). Image statisti-
cal measures have also been used to study aesthetic responses to artworks, with
observers preferring less self-similar (statistical features of the whole image are
comparable with smaller parts of the image) paintings of representational still-lifes
and landscapes, and less complex portraits (Hayn-Leichsenring et al., 2017). How-
ever, research has revealed that image statistics are not robustly predictive of pref-
erence for abstract artworks (Letsch & Hayn-Leichsenring, 2020; Mallon et al.,
2014). Finally, a reliable preference for fractal images in a specific fractal domain
(1.3-1.5) has been found in both artworks and non-artistic images (Graham et al.,
2010; Graham & Redies, 2010; Spehar et al., 2003, 2015). Computational ap-
proaches constitute a highly objective approach to the study of stimulus-driven aes-
thetic preference, but as a result can present difficulties in interpretation of experi-
mental findings. This is especially true for images with higher ecological validity
which vary not only on these lower-level visual features, but also on mid-level fea-
tures associated with element grouping and higher-order properties such as semantic
associations with both abstract and representational content, and which are not cur-
rently captured by these computational methods.
Do aesthetic primitives exist?
It is easy to mistake the presence of robust relationships between stimulus prop-
erties and aesthetic preference as evidence for universal, evolutionarily hard-wired
preferences. However, even the most reliable preferences for particular stimulus
properties can be the result of shared enculturation or exposure. For example, Huang
et al. (2018) found that both adults and 4 year-old children spontaneously attend to
symmetrical patterns, but that preference for symmetrical patterns was evident in
adults but not in children, calling into question the argument that processing fluency
underpins preference for symmetry. Rather, Huang et al. (2018) posit that mere ex-
posure (Zajonc, 1968) may account for a preference for symmetry in adulthood.
Furthermore, while the story of empirical aesthetics centres around group-level re-
sponses to manipulation of objective stimulus properties, authors consistently high-
light a high level of reliable variance in observers’ aesthetic responses to even very
simple stimuli.
Drawing on some of the stimulus properties discussed above, Jacobsen and Höfel
(2002) found evidence of substantial individual differences in preference for sym-
metry, while Bertamini et al. (2019) found that individual differences for symmetry
for one stimulus class did not predict preference for symmetry in another stimulus
class, suggesting that a unitary preference for symmetry across stimulus categories
does not exist. Preference for complexity in artworks is determined to some extent
by individual differences in visual working memory capacity (Sherman et al., 2015)
and the soothingness of order is predicted by sub-clinical traits associated with
8
organising tendencies in obsessive compulsive disorder (Van Geert & Wagemans,
2019). Cross-cultural research has revealed differences in preferences for spatial
composition, finding that preference for an object’s location in a frame is mediated
by the observer’s culture’s prevailing reading direction (Chokron & De Agostini,
2000; Ishii et al., 2011; Pérez González, 2012). Furthermore, Schloss and Palmer
(2017) found that Chinese participants’ WAVEs were predicted better by symbolic
associations (red=revolution) and US participants’ WAVEs by object associations
(red=apple), while Taylor et al. (2013) found that WAVEs did not predict colour
preference in the Himba tribe of Namibia. Finally, McManus et al. (2010) discov-
ered large and stable individual differences in preferences for proportioned rectan-
gles, with very simple patterns being ascribed individualised meanings (McManus
& Wu, 2013). This finding lends support to the notion of the ‘Gestalt nightmare’, in
which even the weakest stimuli elicit complex semantic associations in the viewer,
which presents huge challenges for identifying group-level preferences (Makin,
2016). Indeed, semantic associations of stimuli often far outweigh the influence of
any lower-level stimulus features on aesthetic preference, as demonstrated in Mar-
tindale’s (1990) critical explorations of Daniel Berlyne’s (1974) Psychobiological
Theory. The prominent role of individualised semantic associations casts doubt on
the possibility of establishing lawful relations between stimulus properties and aes-
thetic preferences.
Subjective determinants of aesthetic preference
Having considered stimulus-based properties that influence aesthetic preference,
we can now turn to subjective properties. Subjective factors tend be broadly at-
tributed to observer-level (personality, expertise, exposure) and context-level (fram-
ing, knowledge about the artist and process) variables. The following section will
focus on the latter, and take an individual differences approach to these variables at
the conclusion of the section to ascertain the extent to which such contextual factors
have predictable effects on aesthetic preference across observers.
Effect of context
Context has a large impact on aesthetic preference, particularly for visual works
of art. Sammartino and Palmer (2012b) showed that the seemingly robust centre and
inward biases for spatial composition could be manipulated by the addition of titles
that changed the metaphorical meaning of an image. Complementarily, labelling an
artwork with a metaphorically congruent title leads to increased meaning (Cupchik
et al., 1994; Leder et al., 2006) and aesthetic appreciation (Leder et al., 2006; Millis,
2001; Russell & Milne, 1997) and providing titles that accentuate particular aspects
9
of the image (e.g. presence of depicted movement) leads to increased sensory
awareness of those attributes (Mastandrea & Umiltà, 2016). The presence of an ar-
tistic frame around a stimulus can also have an impact on the quality and intensity
of aesthetic judgements. Displaying objects in unexpected contexts (e.g. a post-box
on a tennis court) induces an aesthetic stance; observers are more likely to respond
at the poles of an aesthetic Likert scale for objects in abnormal contexts, and more
neutral aesthetic responses toward objects in semantically congruent contexts (Kirk,
2008). Informing observers that a set of photographs of mouldy food come from an
art exhibition in contrast to a health and safety booklet has no impact on reports of
disgust but does modulate positive valence toward the photographs (Wagner et al.
2014). Furthermore, perceived beauty and positive affective responses are more
tightly linked in artistic contexts (Wagner et al., 2014).
Effect of artist and process
Knowledge about the creative process and the artist herself can also modulate
aesthetic responses to artworks. Informing participants that an artwork was made
by a professional artist rather than the experimenter leads to increased aesthetic rat-
ings for the same stimuli (Kirk et al., 2009), while labelling an artwork as created
by a famous artists boosts its aesthetic appraisal further (Mastandrea & Crano,
2019). Contrariwise, attribution of part of the creative process to a computer algo-
rithm leads observers to downgrade their liking of an artwork (Chamberlain et al.,
2018) and artworks with an association with criminal activity such as graffiti tags
also elicit diminished aesthetic appraisal relative to visually similar artforms such
as calligraphy (Chamberlain et al., in press). These effects likely operate through
observers’ assumptions about the creative process. The effort heuristic (Kruger et
al., 2004) posits that perceived effort is used as proxy for quality in the absence of
disambiguating information. In a series of studies, Kruger et al. (2004) showed that
participants valued artworks and designed objects more if they were informed that
they took longer to create. This effect was most pronounced in situations in which
the quality of the object was difficult to determine purely on the basis of sensory
information (Kruger et al. 2004). However, the effort heuristic itself is malleable. If
observers are required to read a piece of text highlighting the role of talent (in con-
trast to effort) prior to evaluating objects, experimental effects are reversed and par-
ticipants rate more quickly created artworks as more valuable (Cho & Schwarz,
2008). Finally, the authenticity of an artwork plays a large role in its aesthetic re-
ception. An artwork’s history is important because, being a non-functional item in
the practical sense, it is prone to biases around contagion, the notion that it is the
end point of a performance, and intuitions about its originality and scarcity (New-
man & Bloom, 2012). In support, Newman and Bloom (2012) found that informing
observers that an object was a duplication of an existing object led to devaluation
of the duplicate, but only in the context of artworks (paintings) not artifacts (cars).
10
Supporting the notion that contagion is also an important factor in the valuation of
art, the contact level between creator and object had a much larger impact on per-
ceived value of artworks than artifacts (Newman & Bloom, 2012).
Stability of contextual influences
Whilst categorised as subjective determinants of aesthetic preference, some of
the contextual effects described above, such as the effort heuristic or essentialist
beliefs associated with duplication, could conceivably account for some of the
shared variance in aesthetic attitudes if sufficiently stable within a given cultural
setting (Vessel, 2010; Vessel et al., 2018), whilst others are by their nature transient.
Effects of authorship on aesthetic preference do not seem to diminish if the re-
sponses of expert artists are compared with non-experts (Chamberlain et al. 2018),
suggesting that these biases concerning artistic process are not superseded by artis-
tic knowledge. However, individual differences in expertise do determine the mag-
nitude of framing effects where the framing relies on adequate recognition of pres-
tige (Verpooten, 2018; Verpooten & Dewitte, 2017).
Finally, many studies have shown that the personal context of the observer in
terms of their demographics and personality affects the kind of artistic stimuli they
seek in the first instance. Both expertise and the Big Five personality factor of
‘openness to experience’ (McCrae, 2007) have been shown to be predictive of pref-
erence for abstract and modern art (Batt et al., 2010; Chamorro-Premuzic et al.,
2009; Kruger et al., 2004; McManus & Furnham, 2006; van Paasschen et al., 2015).
Openness to experience represents a tendency towards intellectual curiosity, aes-
thetic sensitivity, liberal values, and emotional differentiation (McCrae, 2007) and
also predicts preference for the visual arts more generally (Feist & Brady, 2004)
and the prevalence of aesthetic ‘chills’ (Silvia & Nusbaum, 2011). Need for cogni-
tive closure, an aversion toward semantic and sensory ambiguity which can be mod-
ulated in a state or trait-like manner, also predicts dislike for abstract art (Ostrofsky
& Shobe, 2015) and for ambiguous movie endings (Wiersema et al., 2012). Exper-
tise has a marked influence over how observers inspect and categorise artworks
(Augustin & Leder, 2006; Vogt & Magnussen, 2007; Zangemeister et al., 1995) and
an observer’s willingness to engage with abstract and ambiguous art (Silvia, 2013;
van Paasschen et al., 2015). Thus, it can be seen that stable and fluctuating observer-
centred and context-centred variables modulate aesthetic preferences in a complex
and interacting manner. The next section will attempt to summarise the effects of
both objective and subjective predictors of aesthetic preference and introduce an
approach that takes into account the action of objective features at the group-level
and individual differences at the subject level.
11
Considering the interplay between objective and subjective
approaches
The previous sections have summarised evidence for both objective and subjec-
tive between-groups effects on aesthetic preference. Objective features such as sym-
metry, proportion, contour, colour and composition show reliable associations with
aesthetic preference, particularly for simple stimuli that accentuate the target stim-
ulus property. Similarly, contextual information and inferences about effort and au-
thenticity demonstrate reliable effects on perceived value of visual stimuli. How-
ever, a consistent caveat to these group-level effects is the existence of substantial
and reliable differences which do not merely represent variance due to error but can
instead be attributed to person-level variables. The aetiology of these individual dif-
ferences can be attributed to multiple sources. Behavioural genetic research implies
that variation in genes account for a limited proportion (approximately 30%) of the
variance in perceived facial attractiveness (Germine et al., 2015), and a similar pro-
portion of the variance in the intensity of aesthetic appraisal of abstract objects and
scenes (Bignardi et al., 2020). The remaining variation likely lies within unique
environmental factors, due to individual differences in exposure and enculturation.
Individual differences in expertise and personality likely play a strong role in mod-
ulating the role of objective predictors, an issue that has been addressed with the
revival of the concept of aesthetic sensitivity.
Aesthetic sensitivity
A useful way of conceptualising individual differences in empirical aesthetics is
through the notion of aesthetic sensitivity. This concept originates in the work of
Hans Eysenck, who posited the existence of an individual difference in the ability
to detect objective beauty in a stimulus, similar to the notion of a general intelli-
gence factor, g (Eysenck, 1940). It will have become clear that establishing an ob-
jective notion of beauty as a property of the stimulus was bound to fail. However,
recently researchers have revived the label if not the underlying meaning of Ey-
senck’s aesthetic sensitivity (Corradi et al., 2019, 2020). Under its revised concep-
tion, aesthetic sensitivity is the extent to which a particular objective feature (sym-
metry, contour, complexity) influences an observer’s aesthetic valuation. Empirical
support for the existence of aesthetic sensitivity was derived from a study which re-
examined stimuli from a seminal study on curvature preference (Bar & Neta, 2006)
and found both group-level preference for curvature as well as large individual dif-
ferences, across two different stimulus categories (real objects and abstract designs;
Corradi et al. 2019). A follow-up study using a larger range of stimuli again found
evidence for high variability in preference for curvature, symmetry, complexity and
balance in visual stimuli (Corradi et al. 2020). Furthermore, sensitivity across
12
different stimulus properties was not correlated, although sensitivity was stable over
time, echoing existing individual difference research (Bertamini et al., 2019;
McManus et al., 2010).
Research exploring the underlying determinants of aesthetic sensitivity is still in
its infancy. Individual differences in aesthetic sensitivity for contour and symmetry
was found to be weakly predicted by expertise, but not by personality factors such
as openness to experience (Corradi et al. 2020). In a similar study, Cotter et al.
(2017) found individual differences in preference for curvature could be explained
by personality and expertise. It is possible that visual sensitivity, that is the extent
to which individuals can visually detect differences in symmetry, contour, balance,
may be predictive of aesthetic sensitivity. Research on fractal patterns suggests that
observers’ preferences for levels of fractality and their visual sensitivity to those
particular patterns are tightly linked (Spehar et al., 2015). It would be valuable to
investigate whether an observer’s ability to detect the curvature of contours, the
presence of symmetry and the objective complexity of an image, predicted their
aesthetic sensitivity for the same stimulus feature. Whilst the focus of research on
aesthetic sensitivity is predominantly focused upon stimulus-based features which
influence aesthetic preference, it is reasonable to believe that aesthetic sensitivity
could be extended to the realm of subjective factors as well. Some observers may
be more or less sensitive to the effect of context, or of factors associated with the
artist or artistic process. This is indicated by a study finding that prestige effects
(stating that an artwork was located at the Museum of Modern Art in New York
rather than a local art gallery) only impact the aesthetic preferences of expert artist
observers (Verpooten, 2018; Verpooten & Dewitte, 2017). It is also possible that
aesthetic sensitivity functions in a domain-specific manner. Objective and subjec-
tive features of natural and man-made objects may influence aesthetic preferences
of observers in different ways. It is possible that biologically- relevant stimuli in-
duce sensitivity at the level of stimulus properties, while artworks elicit sensitivity
at the level of subjective factors (Vessel & Rubin, 2010; 2018). This domain-spec-
ificity may further interact with other individual differences measures (such as ex-
pertise) whereby sensitivity to objective and subjective features is determined by
the level of artistic knowledge an individual has. The notion of aesthetic sensitivity
is a useful tool with which to move beyond group-level principles in empirical aes-
thetics, and to categorise and predict the individual differences that permeate the
data collected in this domain.
Conclusion
This chapter has provided an overview of empirical psychological perspectives
to aesthetic preferences. It can be seen that contemporary approaches to the inves-
tigation of aesthetic preferences are still heavily influenced by early work in the
field (Fechner 1876; Berlyne, 1971) which strove to identify lawful relationships
13
between objective stimulus properties and aesthetic responses. This approach has to
a large extent failed, partly due to the combinatorial influence of objective factors
(Makin, 2016) and the myriad subjective influences that often supersede the effects
of stimulus properties on aesthetic preference. However, we have seen that there are
robust and replicable group-level effects of stimulus features like symmetry and
curvature which appear to be culturally invariant (Gomez-Puerto et al. 2018; Makin
et al. 2018), suggesting that it is not necessary to abandon all efforts to identify
objective determinants of aesthetic preferences. Contextual factors have recently
received more attention as researchers pursue more complete models of the aesthetic
process. It is clear that information about the artist and the artistic process has a
large impact on the strength of aesthetic judgments to artistic stimuli (Chamberlain
et al. 2018; Mastandrea & Cruno, 2019; Kirk et al. 2009; Kruger et al. 2004; New-
man & Bloom, 2012). Merely framing a sensory experience as being one of viewing
an artwork, impacts on the kind of emotional and evaluative response the observer
has to the artwork (Wagner et al. 2014; Kirk et al. 2008).
Group-level objective and subjective effects aside, permeating much of this re-
search is the observation that people significantly and reliably differ in their aes-
thetic responses to stimulus features. The question of why people differ in their aes-
thetic judgments has been present since the inception of empirical aesthetics, but
has gained much more prominence in recent years (Vessel & Rubin, 2010; Vessel
& Rubin, 2018; Cotter et al. 2017; McManus et al. 2010). Putative mechanisms for
individual differences in aesthetic span both genetic and environmental influences.
These sources of variance encapsulate differences in exposure via expertise (both
practical and intellectual knowledge of the artistic domain) and culture, and trait-
level differences due to cognitive ability and personality. While there is a promising
line of research exploring the aetiology of individual differences for stimulus fea-
tures, there is very little research exploring the effect of individual differences in
response to contextual manipulations, which is likely to be a fruitful line of research
in the future. Furthermore, findings concerning individual differences can be better
understood in relation to the notion of aesthetic sensitivity, which posits that indi-
viduals’ aesthetic responses are driven to a greater or lesser extent by different fea-
tures of the stimulus and context. By combining what we know about the relatively
stable subjective and objective features of an aesthetic experience alongside the
sources of variance surrounding them, it seems possible to develop a more complete
understanding of the seemingly unpredictable nature of individual aesthetic prefer-
ences.
Reference list
Abeln, J., Fresz, L., Amirshahi, S. A., McManus, I. C., Koch, M., Kreysa, H., & Redies, C. (2016).
Preference for Well-Balanced Saliency in Details Cropped from Photographs. Frontiers in Hu-
man Neuroscience, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00704
Arnheim, R. (1965). Art and visual perception: A psychology of the creative eye. Faber & Faber.
14
Augustin, D., & Leder, H. (2006). Art expertise: A study of concepts and conceptual spaces. Psy-
chology Science, 48(2), 135.
Bar, M., & Neta, M. (2006). Humans Prefer Curved Visual Objects. Psychological Science, 17(8),
645648. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01759.x
Batt, R., Palmiero, M., Nakatani, C., & van Leeuwen, C. (2010). Style and spectral power: Pro-
cessing of abstract and representational art in artists and non-artists. Perception, 39(12), 1659
1671. https://doi.org/10.1068/p6747
Berlyne, D. E. (1974). Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps towards an objective psy-
chology of aesthetic appreciation. Hemisphere.
Bertamini, M., Palumbo, L., Gheorghes, T. N., & Galatsidas, M. (2016). Do observers like curva-
ture or do they dislike angularity? British Journal of Psychology, 107(1), 154178.
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12132
Bertamini, M., Rampone, G., Makin, A. D. J., & Jessop, A. (2019). Symmetry preference in
shapes, faces, flowers and landscapes. PeerJ, 7, e7078. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.7078
Bignardi, G., Ticini, L. F., Smit, D., & Polderman, T. J. (2020). Domain-specific and domain-
general genetic and environmental effects on the intensity of visual aesthetic appraisal.
PsyArXiv. https://psyarxiv.com/79nbq
Birkhoff, G. D. (1933). Aesthetic measure. Harvard University Press.
Brachmann, A., & Redies, C. (2017). Computational and Experimental Approaches to Visual Aes-
thetics. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 11, 102.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2017.00102
Brattico, E., & Pearce, M. (2013). The neuroaesthetics of music. Psychology of Aesthetics, Crea-
tivity, and the Arts, 7(1), 4861.
Chamberlain, R., Mullin, C., Berio, D., Leymarie, F. F., & Wagemans, J. (in press). Aesthetics of
graffiti: Comparison to text-based an pictorial artforms. Empirical Studies of the Arts.
Chamberlain, R., Mullin, C., Scheerlinck, B., & Wagemans, J. (2018). Putting the Art in Artificial:
Aesthetic responses to computer-generated art. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the
Arts, 12(2), 177.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Reimers, S., Hsu, A., & Ahmetoglu, G. (2009). Who art thou? Personality
predictors of artistic preferences in a large UK sample: The importance of openness. British
Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 501516. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712608X366867
Chatterjee, A., & Vartanian, O. (2014). Neuroaesthetics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 370
375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.003
Cho, H., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Of great art and untalented artists: Effort information and the
flexible construction of judgmental heuristics. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18(3), 205
211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2008.04.009
Chokron, S., & De Agostini, M. (2000). Reading habits influence aesthetic preference. Cognitive
Brain Research, 10(12), 4549. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00021-5
Corradi, G., Belman, M., Currò, T., Chuquichambi, E. G., Rey, C., & Nadal, M. (2019). Aesthetic
sensitivity to curvature in real objects and abstract designs. Acta Psychologica, 197, 124130.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.05.012
Corradi, G., Chuquichambi, E. G., Barrada, J. R., Clemente, A., & Nadal, M. (2020). A new con-
ception of visual aesthetic sensitivity. British Journal of Psychology, 111(4), 630658.
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12427
Cotter, K. N., Silvia, P. J., Bertamini, M., Palumbo, L., & Vartanian, O. (2017). Curve Appeal:
Exploring Individual Differences in Preference for Curved Versus Angular Objects. I-Percep-
tion, 8(2), 204166951769302. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669517693023
Cupchik, G. C., Shereck, L., & Spiegel, S. (1994). The Effects of Textual Information on Artistic
Communication. 20(1), 6278.
Cutting, J. E. (2003). Gustave Caillebotte, French Impressionism, and mere exposure. Psycho-
nomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 319343. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196493
Eysenck, H. (1940). The general factor in aesthetic judgements. British Journal of Psychology, 31,
94102.
15
Fechner, G. (1876). Vorschule der aesthetik (Vol. 1). Brietkopf & Härtel.
Feist, G. J., & Brady, T. R. (2004). Openness to Experience, Non-Conformity, and the Preference
for Abstract Art. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 22(1), 7789. https://doi.org/10.2190/Y7CA-
TBY6-V7LR-76GK
Fink, B., & Penton-Voak, I. (2002). Evolutionary Psychology of Facial Attractiveness. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 154158. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-
8721.00190
Flavell, J. C., Over, H., & Tipper, S. P. (2020). Competing for affection: Perceptual fluency and
ambiguity solution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfor-
mance, 46(3), 231240. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000702
Friedenberg, J. D. (2012). Aesthetic Judgment of Triangular Shape: Compactness and Not the
Golden Ratio Determines Perceived Attractiveness. I-Perception.
Germine, L., Russell, R., Bronstad, P. M., Blokland, G. A. M., Smoller, J. W., Kwok, H., Anthony,
S. E., Nakayama, K., Rhodes, G., & Wilmer, J. B. (2015). Individual Aesthetic Preferences for
Faces Are Shaped Mostly by Environments, Not Genes. Current Biology, 25(20), 26842689.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.08.048
Gómez-Puerto, G., Munar, E., & Nadal, M. (2016). Preference for Curvature: A Historical and
Conceptual Framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00712
Graham, D. J., Friedenberg, J. D., & Rockmore, D. N. (2009). Efficient visual system processing
of spatial and luminance statistics in representational and non-representational art. IS&T/SPIE
Electronic Imaging, 72401N-72401N. http://proceedings.spiedigitallibrary.org/proceed-
ing.aspx?articleid=811744
Graham, D. J., Friedenberg, J. D., Rockmore, D. N., & Field, D. J. (2010). Mapping the similarity
space of paintings: Image statistics and visual perception. Visual Cognition, 18(4), 559573.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280902934454
Graham, D. J., & Redies, C. (2010). Statistical regularities in art: Relations with visual coding and
perception. Vision Research, 50(16), 15031509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.05.002
Hayn-Leichsenring, G. U., Lehmann, T., & Redies, C. (2017). Subjective Ratings of Beauty and
Aesthetics: Correlations With Statistical Image Properties in Western Oil Paintings. I-Percep-
tion, 8(3), 204166951771547. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669517715474
Höfel, L., & Jacobsen, T. (2003). Temporal Stability and Consistency of Aesthetic Judgments of
Beauty of Formal Graphic Patterns. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 96, 3032.
Huang, Y., Xue, X., Spelke, E., Huang, L., Zheng, W., & Peng, K. (2018). The aesthetic preference
for symmetry dissociates from early-emerging attention to symmetry. Scientific Reports, 8(1),
6263. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-24558-x
Hůla, M., & Flegr, J. (2016). What flowers do we like? The influence of shape and color on the
rating of flower beauty. PeerJ, 4, e2106. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.2106
Hurlbert, A. C., & Ling, Y. (2007). Biological components of sex differences in color preference.
Current Biology, 17(16), R623R625. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.022
Ishii, Y., Okubo, M., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Imai, H. (2011). Lateral biases and reading direction:
A dissociation between aesthetic preference and line bisection. Brain and Cognition, 75(3),
242247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.12.005
Jacobsen, T., & Höfel, L. (2002). Aesthetic judgments of novel graphic patterns: Analysis of indi-
vidual judgments. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95, 755766.
Judd, T., Durand, F., & Torralba, A. (2011). Fixations on low-resolution images. Journal of Vision,
11(4), 1414. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.4.14
Kirk, U. (2008). The Neural Basis of Object-Context Relationships on Aesthetic Judgment. PLoS
ONE, 3(11), e3754. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003754
Kirk, U., Skov, M., Hulme, O., Christensen, M. S., & Zeki, S. (2009). Modulation of aesthetic
value by semantic context: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 44(3), 11251132.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.009
16
Kruger, J., Wirtz, D., Van Boven, L., & Altermatt, T. W. (2004). The effort heuristic. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 40(1), 9198. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-
1031(03)00065-9
Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, D. (2004). A model of aesthetic appreciation and
aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 95(4), 489508.
Leder, H., Carbon, C.-C., & Ripsas, A.-L. (2006). Entitling art: Influence of title information on
understanding and appreciation of paintings. Acta Psychologica, 121(2), 176198.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.08.005
Leder, H., & Nadal, M. (2014). Ten years of a model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judg-
ments: The aesthetic episode - Developments and challenges in empirical aesthetics. British
Journal of Psychology, 105(4), 443464. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12084
Letsch, P., & Hayn-Leichsenring, G. U. (2020). The composition of abstract images Differences
between artists and laypersons. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 14(2), 186
196. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000209
Little, A. (2014). Domain Specificity in Human Symmetry Preferences: Symmetry is Most Pleas-
ant When Looking at Human Faces. Symmetry, 6(2), 222233.
https://doi.org/10.3390/sym6020222
Lyssenko, N., Redies, C., & Hayn-Leichsenring, G. U. (2016). Evaluating Abstract Art: Relation
between Term Usage, Subjective Ratings, Image Properties and Personality Traits. Frontiers
in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00973
Makin, A. D. J. (2016). The Gap Between Aesthetic Science and Aesthetic Experience. 30.
Makin, A. D. J., Helmy, M., & Bertamini, M. (2018). Visual cortex activation predicts visual pref-
erence: Evidence from Britain and Egypt. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
71(8), 17711780. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1350870
Makin, A. D. J., Pecchinenda, A., & Bertamini, M. (2012). Implicit affective evaluation of visual
symmetry. Emotion, 12(5), 10211030. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026924
Makin, A. D. J., Wright, D., Rampone, G., Palumbo, L., Guest, M., Sheehan, R., Cleaver, H., &
Bertamini, M. (2016). An Electrophysiological Index of Perceptual Goodness. Cerebral Cor-
tex, 26(12), 44164434. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw255
Mallon, B., Redies, C., & Hayn-Leichsenring, G. U. (2014). Beauty in abstract paintings: Percep-
tual contrast and statistical properties. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00161
Martindale, C., Moore, K., & Borkum, J. (1990). Aesthetic Preference: Anomalous Findings for
Berlyne’s Psychobiological Theory. The American Journal of Psychology, 103(1), 53.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1423259
Mastandrea, S., & Crano, W. D. (2019). Peripheral Factors Affecting the Evaluation of Artworks.
Empirical Studies of the Arts, 37(1), 8291. https://doi.org/10.1177/0276237418790916
Mastandrea, S., & Umiltà, M. A. (2016). Futurist Art: Motion and Aesthetics As a Function of
Title. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00201
Mather, G. (2018). Visual Image Statistics in the History of Western Art. Art and Perception, 6(2
3), 97115. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134913-20181092
McCrae, R. R. (2007). Aesthetic Chills as a Universal Marker of Openness to Experience. Moti-
vation and Emotion, 31(1), 511. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9053-1
McManus, I. C. (1980). The aesthetics of simple figures. British Journal of Psychology, 71(4),
505524. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1980.tb01763.x
McManus, I. C., Cook, R., & Hunt, A. (2010). Beyond the Golden Section and normative aesthet-
ics: Why do individuals differ so much in their aesthetic preferences for rectangles? Psychology
of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(2), 113126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017316
McManus, I. C., & Furnham, A. (2006). Aesthetic activities and aesthetic attitudes: Influences of
education, background and personality on interest and involvement in the arts. British Journal
of Psychology, 97(4), 555587. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712606X101088
17
McManus, I. C., & Weatherby, P. (1997). The Golden Section and the Aesthetics of Form and
Composition: A Cognitive Model. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 15(2), 209232.
https://doi.org/10.2190/WWCR-VWHV-2Y2W-91EE
McManus, I. C., & Wu, W. (2013). “The square is … bulky, heavy, contented, plain, good-natured,
stupid …”: A cross-cultural study of the aesthetics and meanings of rectangles. Psychology of
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(2), 130139. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030469
Millis, K. (2001). Making meaning brings pleasure: The influence of titles on aesthetic experi-
ences. Emotion, 1(3), 320.
Muth, C., Hesslinger, V. M., & Carbon, C.-C. (2015). The appeal of challenge in the perception
of art: How ambiguity, solvability of ambiguity, and the opportunity for insight affect appreci-
ation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(3), 206216.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038814
Nadal, M., Munar, E., Marty, G., & Cela-Conde, C. J. (2010). Visual Complexity and Beauty
Appreciation: Explaining the Divergence of Results. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 28(2), 173
191. https://doi.org/10.2190/EM.28.2.d
Newman, G. E., & Bloom, P. (2012). Art and authenticity: The importance of originals in judg-
ments of value. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(3), 558569.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026035
Ostrofsky, J., & Shobe, E. (2015). The Relationship Between Need for Cognitive Closure and the
Appreciation, Understanding, and Viewing Times of Realistic and Nonrealistic Figurative
Paintings. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 33(1), 106113.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0276237415570016
Ou, L.-C., Luo, M. R., Woodcock, A., & Wright, A. (2004). A study of colour emotion and colour
preference. Part I: Colour emotions for single colours. Color Research & Application, 29(3),
232240. https://doi.org/10.1002/col.20010
Ou, L.-C., Yuan, Y., Sato, T., Lee, W.-Y., Szabó, F., Sueeprasan, S., & Huertas, R. (2018). Uni-
versal models of colour emotion and colour harmony. Color Research & Application, 43(5),
736748. https://doi.org/10.1002/col.22243
Palmer, S. E., & Griscom, W. S. (2013). Accounting for taste: Individual differences in preference
for harmony. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 453461.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0355-2
Palmer, S. E., & Schloss, K. B. (2010). An ecological valence theory of human color preference.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(19), 88778882.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0906172107
Palmer, S. E., Schloss, K. B., & Sammartino, J. (2013). Visual Aesthetics and Human Preference.
Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 77107. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-
100504
Palumbo, L., Rampone, G., Bertamini, M., Sinico, M., Clarke, E., & Vartanian, O. (2020). Visual
preference for abstract curvature and for interior spaces: Beyond undergraduate student sam-
ples. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000359
Palumbo, L., Ruta, N., & Bertamini, M. (2015). Comparing Angular and Curved Shapes in Terms
of Implicit Associations and Approach/Avoidance Responses. PLOS ONE, 10(10), e0140043.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140043
Pelowski, M., Markey, P. S., Forster, M., Gerger, G., & Leder, H. (2017). Move me, astonish me…
delight my eyes and brain: The Vienna Integrated Model of top-down and bottom-up processes
in Art Perception (VIMAP) and corresponding affective, evaluative, and neurophysiological
correlates. Physics of Life Reviews, 21, 80125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2017.02.003
Pérez González, C. (2012). Lateral organisation in nineteenth-century studio photographs is influ-
enced by the direction of writing: A comparison of Iranian and Spanish photographs. Lateral-
ity: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 17(5), 515532.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2011.586701
18
Perrett, D. I., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., Lee, K. J., Rowland, D. A., & Edwards, R. (1999).
Symmetry and Human Facial Attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(5), 295307.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00014-8
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is
beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review,
8(4), 364382.
Redies, C., Amirshahi, S. A., Koch, M., & Denzler, J. (2012). PHOG-derived aesthetic measures
applied to color photographs of artworks, natural scenes and objects. European Conference on
Computer Vision, 522531. http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-642-33863-2_54
Russell, P. A., & Milne, S. (1997). Meaningfulness and hedonic value of painting: Effects of titles.
Empirical Studies of the Arts, 15(1), 6173.
Sammartino, J., & Palmer, S. E. (2012a). Aesthetic issues in spatial composition: Effects of vertical
position and perspective on framing single objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Hu-
man Perception and Performance, 38(4), 865879. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027736
Sammartino, J., & Palmer, S. E. (2012b). Aesthetic Issues in Spatial Composition: Representa-
tional Fit and the Role of Semantic Context. Perception, 41(12), 14341457.
https://doi.org/10.1068/p7233
Schloss, K. B., & Palmer, S. E. (2014). The politics of color: Preferences for Republican red versus
Democratic blue. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(6), 14811488.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0635-0
Schloss, K. B., & Palmer, S. E. (2017). An ecological framework for temporal and individual
differences in color preferences. Vision Research, 141, 95108.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2017.01.010
Schloss, K. B., Poggesi, R. M., & Palmer, S. E. (2011). Effects of university affiliation and “school
spirit” on color preferences: Berkeley versus Stanford. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(3),
498504. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0073-1
Sherman, M. T., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., & Kanai, R. (2015). Prior expectations facilitate met-
acognition for perceptual decision. Consciousness and Cognition, 35, 5365.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.04.015
Silvia, P. J. (2013). Interested Experts, Confused Novices: Art Expertise and the Knowledge Emo-
tions. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 31(1), 107115. https://doi.org/10.2190/EM.31.1.f
Silvia, P. J., & Nusbaum, E. C. (2011). On personality and piloerection: Individual differences in
aesthetic chills and other unusual aesthetic experiences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity,
and the Arts, 5(3), 208214. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021914
Spehar, B., Clifford, C. W. G., Newell, B. R., & Taylor, R. P. (2003). Universal aesthetic of frac-
tals. Computers & Graphics, 27(5), 813820. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0097-8493(03)00154-7
Spehar, B., Wong, S., van de Klundert, S., Lui, J., Clifford, C. W. G., & Taylor, R. P. (2015).
Beauty and the beholder: The role of visual sensitivity in visual preference. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00514
Strauss, E. D., Schloss, K. B., & Palmer, S. E. (2013). Color preferences change after experience
with liked/disliked colored objects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(5), 935943.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0423-2
Taylor, C., Clifford, A., & Franklin, A. (2013). Color preferences are not universal. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 10151027. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030273
Tinio, P. P. L. (2013). From artistic creation to aesthetic reception: The mirror model of art. Psy-
chology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 265275.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030872
Tseng, P. H., Carmi, R., Cameron, I. G. M., Munoz, D. P., & Itti, L. (2009). Quantifying center
bias of observers in free viewing of dynamic natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 9(7), 44.
https://doi.org/10.1167/9.7.4
Van de Cruys, S., & Wagemans, J. (2011). Putting reward in art: A tentative prediction error ac-
count of visual art. I-Perception, 2(9), 10351062. https://doi.org/10.1068/i0466aap
19
Van der Helm, P., & Leeuwenberg, E. (1996). Goodness of visual regularities: A nontransforma-
tional approach. Psychological Review, 103(3), 429456.
Van Geert, E., & Wagemans, J. (2019). Order, complexity, and aesthetic preferences for neatly
organized compositions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts.
https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000276
Van Geert, E., & Wagemans, J. (2020). Order, complexity, and aesthetic appreciation. Psychology
of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 14(2), 135154. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000224
van Paasschen, J., Bacci, F., & Melcher, D. P. (2015). The Influence of Art Expertise and Training
on Emotion and Preference Ratings for Representational and Abstract Artworks. PLOS ONE,
10(8), e0134241. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134241
Vartanian, O., Navarrete, G., Chatterjee, A., Fich, L. B., Leder, H., Modrono, C., Nadal, M., Ros-
trup, N., & Skov, M. (2013). Impact of contour on aesthetic judgments and approach-avoidance
decisions in architecture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(Supple-
ment_2), 1044610453. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1301227110
Verpooten, J. (2018). Expertise Affects Aesthetic Evolution in the Domain of Art: Evidence from
Artistic Fieldwork and Psychological Experiments. In Z. Kapoula, E. Volle, J. Renoult, & M.
Andreatta (Eds.), Exploring Transdisciplinarity in Art and Sciences (pp. 303326). Springer
International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-76054-4_16
Verpooten, J., & Dewitte, S. (2017). The Conundrum of Modern Art: Prestige-Driven Coevolu-
tionary Aesthetics Trumps Evolutionary Aesthetics among Art Experts. Human Nature, 28(1),
1638. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9274-7
Vessel, E. A. (2010). Beauty and the beholder: Highly individual taste for abstract, but not real-
world images. Journal of Vision, 10(2), 114. https://doi.org/10.1167/10.2.18
Vessel, E. A., Maurer, N., Denker, A. H., & Starr, G. G. (2018). Stronger shared taste for natural
aesthetic domains than for artifacts of human culture. Cognition, 179, 121131.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.009
Vogt, S., & Magnussen, S. (2007). Expertise in pictorial perception: Eye-movement patterns and
visual memory in artists and laymen. Perception, 36(1), 91100. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5262
Wagner, V., Menninghaus, W., Hanich, J., & Jacobsen, T. (2014). Art schema effects on affective
experience: The case of disgusting images. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts,
8(2), 120129. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036126
Wickens, T., Palmer, S. E., & Gardner, J. (2008). Aesthetic issues in spatial composition: Effects
of position and direction on framing single objects. Spatial Vision, 21, 421449.
Wiersema, D. V., van der Schalk, J., & van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Who’s afraid of red, yellow, and
blue? Need for cognitive closure predicts aesthetic preferences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Cre-
ativity, and the Arts, 6(2), 168174. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025878
Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 9, 127.
Zangemeister, W. H., Sherman, K., & Stark, L. (1995). Evidence for a global scanpath strategy in
viewing abstract compared with realistic images. Neuropsychologia, 33(8), 10091025.
... An active simultaneous involvement of emotional, cognitive, and contextual factors is suggested to mediate such aesthetic encounters (Chatterjee and Vartanian, 2014;Coburn et al., 2017). Among the various proposed theoretical models addressing the alternating roles of affect and cognition, it has been commonly agreed that evaluations/ judgments are the result of bottom-up stimulus properties and top-down appraisals (Leder et al., 2004;Mastandrea and Bartoli, 2011;Chatterjee and Vartanian, 2016;Chamberlain, 2022). Experiencing a positive and pleasant aesthetic encounter will therefore increase positive affect (Leder et al., 2004), potentially benefiting health and well-being (Coburn et al., 2017). ...
... Despite the remaining open questions of which subjective (top-down) and objective (bottom-up) features exactly drive (interindividual) differences in empirical aesthetics, consistent response patterns were found and attributed to certain aesthetic primitives. Stimulus properties such as contour shape (Bar and Neta, 2007;Vartanian et al., 2013), color Strauss et al., 2013;Elliot and Maier, 2014), as well as symmetry (Tyler, 2003;Bertamini et al., 2018Bertamini et al., , 2019, order, complexity (Nadal et al., 2010;Van Geert and Wagemans, 2021), and global image properties (e.g., fractality) were proposed as objective predictors of aesthetic preference (Chamberlain, 2022). However, other approaches stress the idiosyncrasies of preferences, demonstrating a stronger shared taste for natural or naturally inspired aesthetic domains as opposed to artifacts of human culture (Vessel et al., 2018). ...
... Despite the statistically significant main effects of contours in explaining the variability of aesthetic preference ratings, the percentage of explained variance was considerably low (i.e., 1% for beauty and liking) suggesting that factors other than contours may play a stronger role in the aesthetic response. In fact, both objective (characteristics of stimuli) and subjective (characteristics of context) factors are proposed to be important in shaping aesthetic experiences (Chamberlain, 2022). In a recent metaanalysis, the first to inspect the consistency of the curvature preference hypothesis, factors other than perceptual contour properties were identified as moderators of the effect, namely, presentation time, stimulus type, expertise, and task (Chuquichambi et al., 2022, pre-print). ...
Article
Full-text available
The interest in the response to contours has recently re-emerged, with various studies suggesting a universal preference for curved over angular stimuli. Although no consensus has yet been reached on the reasons for this preference, similar effects have been proposed in interior environments. However, the scarcely available research primarily depends on schematic or unmatched stimuli and faces heterogeneity in the reported results. In a within-subject design, we investigated the claimed contour effect in photo-realistic indoor environments using stimulus material previously tested in virtual reality (VR). A total of 198 online participants rated 20 living room images, exclusively manipulated on the contours (angular vs. curved) and style (modern vs. classic) levels. The scales represented aesthetic (beauty and liking) and stress (rest and stress) responses. Beyond our main focus on contours, we additionally examined style and sex effects to account for potential interactions. Results revealed a significant main effect of contours on both aesthetic (η 2 g = 1-2%) and stress (η 2 g = 8-12%) ratings. As expected, images of curved (vs. angular) contours scored higher on beauty, liking, and rest scales, and lower on stress. Regarding interactions with style, curvature was aesthetically preferred over angularity only within images depicting modern interiors, however, its positive effect on stress responses remained significant irrespective of style. Furthermore, we observed sex differences in aesthetic but not in stress evaluations, with curvature preference only found in participants who indicated female as their sex. In sum, our study primarily confirms positive effects of curvature, however, with multiple layers. First, the impact on aesthetic preference seems to be influenced by individual and contextual factors. Second, in terms of stress responses, which might be especially relevant for designs intended to promote mental-health, the consistent effects suggest a more generalizable, potentially biophilic characteristic of curves. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate these effects in fully-matched, photo-realistic, and multi-perspective interior design stimuli. From the background of a previous VR trial from our research group, whereby the same rooms did not elicit any differences, our findings propose that static vs. immersive presentations might yield different results in the response to contours.
... Researchers in the empirical aesthetics community have therefore tried to measure and quantify beauty and complexity. Understanding the relationship between beauty, complexity and objective image features, and the relationship between the beauty and complexity assessments themselves has been a topic of great interest over the last several decades (Chamberlain, 2022;Jacobsen, 2010;Machotka, 1980;McWhinnie, 1971;Nadal & Vartanian, 2021;Van Geert & Wagemans, 2020). While progress has been made, there is a general lack of consensus regarding the relationships found. ...
Article
Full-text available
The complexity of images critically influences our assessment of their beauty. However, studies relating assessments of complexity and beauty to potential objective measures are hampered by the use of hand-crafted stimuli which are hard to reproduce and manipulate. To tackle this, we developed a systematic method for generating 2D black-and-white pixel patterns using cellular automata and collected ratings of complexity and beauty from 80 participants. We developed various computational measures of pattern quantification such as density, entropies, local spatial complexity, Kolmogorov complexity, and asymmetries. We also introduced an “intricacy” measure quantifying the number of components in a pattern using a graph-based approach. We related these objective measures with participant judgments of complexity and beauty to find that a weighted combination of local spatial complexity and intricacy was an effective predictor (R²test = .47) of complexity. This implies that people’s complexity ratings depended on the local arrangement of pixels along with the global number of components in the pattern. Furthermore, we found a positive linear influence of complexity ratings on beauty, with a negative linear influence of disorder (asymmetry and entropy), and a negative interaction between the two quantities (R²test = .65). This implies that there is beauty in complexity as long as there is sufficient order. Lastly, a moderated mediation analysis showed that subjective complexity mediates the influence of objective complexity (OC) on beauty, implying that subjective complexity provides useful information over and above OC.
... In parallel, perceptual features that mediate the sensual beauty of artworks became less central for aesthetic judgements. Instead, image content and cultural context emerged as guides of what beholders prefer [56]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research in computational textual aesthetics has shown that there are textual correlates of preference in prose texts. The present study investigates whether textual correlates of preference vary across different time periods (contemporary texts versus texts from the 19th and early 20th centuries). Preference is operationalized in different ways for the two periods, in terms of canonization for the earlier texts, and through sales figures for the contemporary texts. As potential textual correlates of preference, we measure degrees of (un)predictability in the distributions of two types of low-level observables, parts of speech and sentence length. Specifically, we calculate two entropy measures, Shannon Entropy as a global measure of unpredictability, and Approximate Entropy as a local measure of surprise (unpredictability in a specific context). Preferred texts from both periods (contemporary bestsellers and canonical earlier texts) are characterized by higher degrees of unpredictability. However, unlike canonicity in the earlier texts, sales figures in contemporary texts are reflected in global (text-level) distributions only (as measured with Shannon Entropy), while surprise in local distributions (as measured with Approximate Entropy) does not have an additional discriminating effect. Our findings thus suggest that there are both time-invariant correlates of preference, and period-specific correlates.
... Researchers in the empirical aesthetics community have therefore tried to measure and quantify beauty and complexity. Understanding the relationship between subjective beauty, complexity and objective image features, and the relationship between the beauty and complexity assessments themselves has been a topic of great interest over the last several decades (McWhinnie, 1971;Machotka, 1980;Jacobsen, 2010;Van Geert & Wagemans, 2020;Nadal & Vartanian, 2021;Chamberlain, 2022). While progress has been made, common practices such as using handcrafted stimuli and measures have posed major challenges to encapsulating findings across studies. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The complexity of images critically influences our assessment of their beauty. However, studies relating assessments of complexity and beauty to potential objective measures are hampered by the use of hand-crafted stimuli which are hard to reproduce and manipulate. To tackle this, we developed a systematic method for generating 2D black-and-white pixel patterns using cellular automata, and collected ratings of complexity and beauty from 80 participants. We developed various computational measures of pattern quantification such as density, entropies, local spatial complexity, Kolmogorov complexity, and asymmetries. We also introduced an “intricacy” measure quantifying the number of components in a pattern using a graph-based approach. We related these objective measures with participant judgements of complexity and beauty to find that a weighted combination of local spatial complexity and intricacy was an effective predictor (R2test = 0.47) of complexity. This implies that people’s complexity ratings depended on the local arrangement of pixels along with the global number of components in the pattern. Furthermore, we found a positive linear influence of complexity ratings on beauty, with a negative linear influence of disorder (asymmetry and entropy), and a negative interaction between the two quantities (R2test = 0.65). This implies that there is beauty in complexity as long as there is sufficient order. Lastly, a moderated mediation analysis showed that subjective complexity mediates the influence of objective complexity on beauty, implying that subjective complexity provides useful information over and above objective complexity. Our data and scripts are available on Github.
... We can only speculate about the origins of the remaining variance, which is not covered by the SIPs. Besides higherorder visual features, possible sources of variance include environmental and genetic factors (Bignardi et al., 2020; for a review, see Chamberlain, 2022), as also found for the evaluation of face attractiveness (Germine et al., 2015). Personality factors also predict aesthetic ratings (Chamorro-Premuzic, 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Artificial intelligence has emerged as a powerful computational tool to create artworks. One application is Neural Style Transfer, which allows to transfer the style of one image, such as a painting, onto the content of another image, such as a photograph. In the present study, we ask how Neural Style Transfer affects objective image properties and how beholders perceive the novel (style-transferred) stimuli. In order to focus on the subjective perception of artistic style, we minimized the confounding effect of cognitive processing by eliminating all representational content from the input images. To this aim, we transferred the styles of 25 diverse abstract paintings onto 150 colored random-phase patterns with six different Fourier spectral slopes. This procedure resulted in 150 style-transferred stimuli. We then computed eight statistical image properties (complexity, self-similarity, edge-orientation entropy, variances of neural network features, and color statistics) for each image. In a rating study, we asked participants to evaluate the images along three aesthetic dimensions (Pleasing, Harmonious, and Interesting). Results demonstrate that not only objective image properties, but also subjective aesthetic preferences transferred from the original artworks onto the style-transferred images. The image properties of the style-transferred images explain 50 – 69% of the variance in the ratings. In the multidimensional space of statistical image properties, participants considered style-transferred images to be more Pleasing and Interesting if they were closer to a “sweet spot” where traditional Western paintings (JenAesthetics dataset) are represented. We conclude that NST is a useful tool to create novel artistic stimuli that preserve the image properties of the input style images. In the novel stimuli, we found a strong relationship between statistical image properties and subjective ratings, suggesting a prominent role of perceptual processing in the aesthetic evaluation of abstract images.
Article
Full-text available
The research aims to reveal the content side of disgust, which makes it possible to consider it an aesthetic reaction in a broad sense. The paper reviews content-oriented and axiological approaches to the study of disgust. The scientific novelty of the research lies in adding the criteria of underestimation, deformation and distance from potential objects of aesthetic pleasure to the minimal definition of the notion of disgust. As a result, it has been proved that the content side of disgust reflects the conflict of the subjective, objective factors of its analysis and instances of the disgusting in art and cinema, which provides an expanded interpretation of the reactivity of disgust.
Article
Full-text available
In this exploratory study, we asked whether objective statistical image properties can predict subjective aesthetic ratings for a set of 48 abstract paintings created by the artist Robert Pepperell. Ruta and colleagues (2021) used the artworks previously to study the effect of curved/angular contour on liking and wanting decisions. We related a predefined set of statistical image properties to the eight different dimensions of aesthetic judgments from their study. Our results show that the statistical image properties can predict a large portion of the variance in the different aesthetic judgments by Ruta and colleagues. For example, adjusted R2 values for liking, attractiveness, visual comfort, and approachability range between 0.52 and 0.60 in multiple linear regression models with four predictors each. For wanting judgments in an (imagined) gallery context, the explained variance is even higher (adjusted R2 of 0.78). To explain these findings, we hypothesize that differences in cognitive processing of Pepperell's abstract paintings are minimized because this set of stimuli has no apparent content and is of uniform artistic style and cultural context. Under this condition, the aesthetic ratings by Ruta and colleagues are largely based on perceptual processing that systematically varies along a relatively small set of objective image properties.
Article
Full-text available
Smoothly curved objects elicit feelings of pleasantness and tend to be preferred over angular objects. Furthermore, individual differences (i.e., art expertise, openness to experience, holistic thinking), and the complexity of the stimuli are known to moderate the effect. We extended the study of individual differences to 2 theoretically relevant groups. Study 1 compared liking for curvature in individuals with autism and a matched neurotypical control group (for age, gender, and IQ). Because preference for curvature depends on both sensory (visuospatial) and affective input, for which individuals with autism exhibit anomalies, we hypothesized a difference in preference for curved stimuli between the 2 groups. Study 2 examined preference for curvature in a group of quasi-expert students of design. Because working architects and designers tend to regard curved interior spaces as beautiful, we hypothesized to replicate this effect within quasi-experts as well, thereby extending the effect across levels of expertise. Using an identical methodology across both studies, we administered abstract stimuli consisting of irregular polygons (angular vs. curved) and patterns of colored lines (angular vs. curved), as well as concrete stimuli consisting of images of interior spaces. Preference for curvature was confirmed with abstract stimuli in all 3 groups. For interior design, the curvature effect diminished in magnitude, and this was especially evident in individuals with autism. Interestingly, quasi-experts preferred rectilinear over curvilinear interiors. We discuss the results in relation to the impact of individual differences and expertise on preference for curvature, and their implication for design studies in ecologically valid settings.
Article
Full-text available
Graffiti art is a controversial art form, and as such there has been little empirical work assessing its aesthetic value. A recent study examined image statistical properties of text-based artwork and revealed that images of text contain less global structure relative to fine detail compared to artworks. However, previous research did not include graffiti tags or murals, which reside in the space between text and visual art. The current study investigated the image statistical properties and attractiveness of graffiti relative to other text-based and pictorial art forms, focusing additionally on the role of expertise. A series of images (N = 140; graffiti, text and paintings) were presented to a group of observers with varying degrees of art interest and expertise ( N = 169). Findings revealed that image statistics predicted attractiveness ratings to images, and that biases against graffiti art are less salient in an expert sample.
Article
Full-text available
Why do people like images of neatly organized compositions, collected on blogs like Things Organized Neatly? (http://thingsorganizedneatly.tumblr.com/) We explored which factors might contribute to aesthetic preferences for these images of a set of objects, or parts of objects, organized in a neatly or tidy way, focusing on both stimulus and person properties related to order, complexity, and the balance between order and complexity. In a large-scale online study, 421 participants chose which of two simultaneously presented images they preferred (100 pairs) and completed some personality questionnaires. The images within each pair were selected to be very similar except for how ordered and/or complex they were on certain perceptual dimensions. In a second part of the study, 84 of these participants also rated how ordered, complex, soothing, and fascinating they found the 184 images. Images high in order and high in complexity were perceived as more fascinating, whereas images high in order but low in complexity were perceived as more soothing. Aesthetic preferences increased with increasing differences in soothingness and fascination between the two images. Subjective order and subjective complexity were both related to aesthetic appreciation, and independently so, suggesting that the balance between order and complexity involves no interaction. Participants differed in how often they preferred the more ordered, complex, soothing, and fascinating image in a pair, which could partly be attributed to age and personality. In general, stimulus and person interact in determining aesthetic appreciation, but deeper theoretical understanding of these interactions requires further investigation with more parametrically varied stimuli.
Article
Full-text available
Human perceptual processes are highly efficient and rapidly extract information to enable fast and accurate responses. The fluency of these processes is reinforcing, meaning that easy-to-perceive objects are liked more as a result of misattribution of the reinforcement affect to the object identity. However, some critical processes are disfluent, yet their completion can be reinforcing leading to object preference through a different route. One such example is identification of objects from camouflage. In a series of 5 experiments, we manipulated object contrast and camouflage to explore the relationship between object preference to perceptual fluency and ambiguity solution. We found that perceptual fluency dominated the process of preference assessment when objects are assessed for "liking". That is, easier-to-perceive objects (high-contrast and noncamouflaged) were preferred over harder-to-perceive objects (low-contrast and camouflaged). However, when objects are assessed for "interest", the disfluent yet reinforcing ambiguity solution process overrode the effect of perceptual fluency, resulting in preference for the harder-to-perceive camouflaged objects over the easier-to-perceive noncamouflaged objects. The results have implications for preference and choice in a wide range of contexts by demonstrating the competition between perceptual fluency and ambiguity solution on preference, and by highlighting the critical factor of the form of preference decision. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Most people like symmetry, and symmetry has been extensively used in visual art and architecture. In this study, we compared preference for images of abstract and familiar objects in the original format or when containing perfect bilateral symmetry. We created pairs of images for different categories: male faces, female faces, polygons, smoothed version of the polygons, flowers, and landscapes. This design allows us to compare symmetry preference in different domains. Each observer saw all categories randomly interleaved but saw only one of the two images in a pair. After recording preference, we recorded a rating of how salient the symmetry was for each image, and measured how quickly observers could decide which of the two images in a pair was symmetrical. Results reveal a general preference for symmetry in the case of shapes and faces. For landscapes, natural (no perfect symmetry) images were preferred. Correlations with judgments of saliency were present but generally low, and for landscapes the salience of symmetry was negatively related to preference. However, even within the category where symmetry was not liked (landscapes), the separate analysis of original and modified stimuli showed an interesting pattern: Salience of symmetry was correlated positively (artificial) or negatively (original) with preference, suggesting different effects of symmetry within the same class of stimuli based on context and categorization.
Article
Full-text available
Which stimulus and person characteristics determine aesthetic appreciation? For many centuries, philosophers and scientists have been trying to solve this complex puzzle. Through the ages, order, complexity, and the balance between order and complexity have frequently been considered as an answer to this question. The literature on the topic, however, both theoretically and empirically speaking, is rather diffuse and contradictory. In this review, we give an overview of the main theories and empirical findings relating order, complexity, and their interplay to aesthetic appreciation, focusing on research concerning the visual modality. Additionally, we propose our own view on the interplay between order and complexity, in line with the reviewed theories and findings. Besides general relations, also individual differences in order, complexity, aesthetic appreciation, and their interrelations are discussed. With this review, we hope to conceptually clarify the literature and point to new roads for investigation in the field of human aesthetics.
Preprint
Visual aesthetic experiences are universally shared and uniquely diversified components of every human culture. The contribution of genetic and environmental factors to variation in aesthetic appraisal has rarely been examined. Here, we analysed variation in the intensity of aesthetic appraisal in 558 monozygotic and 216 dizygotic same sex adult twin pairs when they were presented with three kinds of visual stimuli: abstract objects, sceneries, and faces. We estimated twin resemblance and heritability for the three stimuli types, as well as a shared genetic factor between the three stimuli types. Genetic factors played a moderate role in the variation of intensity of aesthetic appraisal (heritability 26 to 41%). Both shared and unique underlying genetic factors significantly accounted for domain-general and domain-specific differences. Our findings are the first to show the extent to which variation in the intensity of aesthetic experiences result from the contribution of genetic and environmental factors.