ArticlePDF Available

Pathological gambling and the five-factor model of personality

Authors:

Abstract

The goal of this investigation was to examine the personality differences between non-treatment seeking pathological gamblers (PGs) and non-pathological gamblers (NPGs) using the domain and facet traits of the five-factor model of personality (FFM), as measured by the NEO PI-R. Compared to NPGs, PGs scored significantly higher on the neuroticism domain and significantly lower on the conscientiousness domain. Significant differences between PGs and NPGs also emerged for three of four FFM facet traits associated with impulsivity, with PGs scoring higher on impulsiveness and lower on self-discipline and deliberation facets. Both PGs and NPGs had equally high scores (relative to the norm) on excitement-seeking, the fourth facet associated with impulsivity, suggesting that excitement-seeking characterizes gambling behavior rather than pathological gambling. The present findings suggest that the overall personality profile of the PG is one that combines high impulsivity with emotional vulnerability. Importantly, the results also suggest that excitement-seeking, a personality construct akin to sensation-seeking, may not be a specific marker of PG but rather a characteristic common to all those who gamble.
Pathological gambling and the five-factor
model of personality I
R. Michael Bagby
a,*
, David D. Vachon
b
, Eric L. Bulmash
c
,
Tony Toneatto
a
, Lena C. Quilty
d
, Paul T. Costa
e
a
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Canada
b
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Department of Criminology, University of Toronto, Canada
c
Department of Psychology, Queen’s University, Canada
d
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Canada
e
National Institute of Aging, Canada
Received 12 September 2006; received in revised form 18 January 2007; accepted 2 February 2007
Available online 18 April 2007
Abstract
The goal of this investigation was to examine the personality differences between non-treatment seeking
pathological gamblers (PGs) and non-pathological gamblers (NPGs) using the domain and facet traits of
the five-factor model of personality (FFM), as measured by the NEO PI-R. Compared to NPGs, PGs
scored significantly higher on the neuroticism domain and significantly lower on the conscientiousness
domain. Significant differences between PGs and NPGs also emerged for three of four FFM facet traits
associated with impulsivity, with PGs scoring higher on impulsiveness and lower on self-discipline and
deliberation facets. Both PGs and NPGs had equally high scores (relative to the norm) on excitement-seek-
ing, the fourth facet associated with impulsivity, suggesting that excitement-seeking characterizes gambling
behavior rather than pathological gambling. The present findings suggest that the overall personality profile
of the PG is one that combines high impulsivity with emotional vulnerability. Importantly, the results also
suggest that excitement-seeking, a personality construct akin to sensation-seeking, may not be a specific
marker of PG but rather a characteristic common to all those who gamble.
2007 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
0191-8869/$ - see front matter 2007 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.011
q
This study was supported by a grant obtained from the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre.
*
Corresponding author. Address: Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, College Street Site, 250 College Street,
Suite 647A, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5T 1R8. Tel.: +1 416 535 8501x6939; fax: +1 416 260 4125.
E-mail address: michael_bagby@camh.net (R.M. Bagby).
www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
Personality and Individual Differences 43 (2007) 873–880
Keywords: Pathological gambling; Five-factor model of personality; Impulsivity
1. Introduction
In the past several years, there has been an increase in gambling accessibility in North America,
and the proliferation of internet gaming sites, video lottery terminals, and legalized gambling ven-
ues has afforded more opportunity to gamble and increased the likelihood for some to develop
pathological gambling (PG) (Petry, 2005). PG is categorized as an impulse-control disorder in
the 4th edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; APA,
1994) and is defined as a pattern of ‘‘maladaptive gambling behavior that disrupts personal, fam-
ily, or vocational pursuits’’ (p. 615). Recent estimates indicate that 70–90% of North Americans
have engaged in some form of gambling activity (Raylu & Oei, 2002), yet epidemiological studies
indicate that only 1–2% of the general population meet criteria for PG (Petry, Stinson, & Grant,
2005), suggesting that the majority of individuals who gamble do not develop this disorder. Indi-
vidual differences in personality may play an important role in explaining risk for the development
of PG.
A variety of dimensional personality models have been utilized to study personality in PG. Steel
and Blaszczynski (1996) employed Eysenck and Eysenck’s three-factor model of personality (Ey-
senck & Eysenck, 1975), which incorporates the personality domains of psychoticism, extraversion,
and neuroticism (PEN). Combining the PEN model with other psychological inventories, they
identified four constructs – psychological distress, sensation-seeking, criminal liveliness, and impul-
siveness – that explained 62% of the variance in gambling behavior. Nower, Derevensky, and Gup-
ta (2004) similarly reported PEN impulsivity and intensity-seeking as highly predictive of PG
behaviour in youth. An ‘‘alternative’’ five-factor model, developed by Zuckerman, Kuhlman,
Thornquist, and Kiers (1991) and composed of five personality factors – neuroticism-anxiety,
activity, sociability, impulsive sensation-seeking, aggression-hostility – has been popular in PG
research because of its focus on impulsivity and sensation-seeking. Investigations using this model
have reported mixed results: Although Breen and Zuckerman (1999) reported that impulsive sen-
sation-seeking predicted gamblers who ‘chased’ their losses, Langewisch and Frisch (1998) found
no evidence that this model could predict severity of PG symptomatology. Cloninger, Svrakic, and
Przybeck’s (1993) biopsychosocial seven-factor model, which differentiates between character (i.e.,
cooperativeness, self-directedness, and self-transcendence) and temperament (i.e., harm avoidance,
novelty-seeking, reward dependence, and persistence) dimensions, has also been utilized. Gerdner
and Svensson (2003), for example, employed this model and found no significant relationship be-
tween gambling behaviors and impulsivity, a lower-order facet trait located in the novelty-seeking
temperament. More recently, Slutske, Caspi, Moffitt, and Poulton (2005) used a modified version
of the multidimensional personality questionnaire (MPQ; Patrick, Curtin, & Tellegen, 2002)ina
prospective investigation with a birth cohort of young adults to examine the relationship between
personality and PG, and found that PG at 21 years of age was associated with higher levels of neg-
ative emotionality and lower levels of constraint measured at 18 years of age. Although each of
these models offers a valuable and unique contribution to the framing of personality in relation
to pathological gambling, inconsistencies among outcomes that use differently defined traits makes
clear the need for a single, overarching model.
874 R.M. Bagby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 43 (2007) 873–880
In the current investigation we examine differences in five-factor model (FFM) personality
traits between non-treatment seeking PGs and a sample of individuals who report engaging
in gambling activities but do not meet criteria for PG, that is, non-pathological gamblers
(NPGs). The gambling literature uses various terms (e.g., pathological, problem, disordered,
at-risk, and compulsive gambling) and definitional criteria to operationalize problematic gam-
bling behavior. In this investigation, we used the DSM-IV criteria as five or more of the 10 cri-
teria. As PG is conceptualized as an impulse-control disorder, the use of the FFM, as measured
by the revised NEO personality inventory (NEO PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992), seems partic-
ularly relevant for the examination of personality traits associated with PG. Four of the 30 facet
traits measured by the NEO PI-R – deliberation, excitement-seeking, impulsiveness, self-disci-
pline – account for two-thirds of the variance in the nine most commonly used measures of
impulsivity (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001), making the NEO PI-R an ideal platform to assess
the full range of impulsivity traits in PG. Moreover, each of the FFM domain traits demon-
strates a unique and substantial heritability coefficient (Jang, McCrae, Angleitner, Riemann,
& Livesley, 1998) and the FFM personality structure has been extracted in clinical samples
(Bagby et al., 1999) and across a variety of cultures, languages, and countries (McCrae & Costa,
1995).
We hypothesized that the PGs would have higher scores on the impulsivity and excitement-
seeking facet scales and lower scores on the self-discipline and deliberation facet scales compared
to the NPGs. As neuroticism is associated with and considered a vulnerability factor for psycho-
pathology in general (Malouff, Thorsteinsson, & Shutte, 2005), we also hypothesized that PGs
would have higher scores on the neuroticism domain scale compared to NPGs.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Participants (N= 292) from two separate investigations conducted at the Centre for Addiction
and Mental Health were solicited via advertisement in local newspapers for a study on gambling
behavior with the general requirement that they had gambled some time in their lives, including
the past year.
1
PG was diagnosed according to the DSM-IV criteria and interviews were con-
ducted by research assistants trained in structured diagnostic assessments.
3. Measures
Personality traits were assessed using the five domain and 30 facet personality traits of the five-
factor model of personality (FFM), as measured by the revised NEO personality inventory (NEO
PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992).
1
Other data from some of the participants (n= 204) in the current report have been reported in an earlier
investigation (Bagby, Vachon, Bulmash, & Quilty, in press). A 2 (investigation) ·2 (PG/NPG) comparison revealed no
significant differences for any of the domain or facet traits.
R.M. Bagby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 43 (2007) 873–880 875
3.1. Procedure and participant group assignment
Participants completed the NEO PI-R and other tests as part of other, larger research proto-
cols. Those included in the PG group (n= 106; 56 men; 50 women) had to have met the DSM-
IV diagnostic criteria for pathological gambling of five or more symptoms in the last 12 months
(current) or in the period prior to the past 12 months (lifetime).
2
The NPG group (n= 177; 96
women; 81 men) was composed of participants who did not meet DSM-IV criteria for either cur-
rent or lifetime PG.
4. Results
Means and standard deviations for the NEO PI-R domain and facet trait scores for the PGs
and NPGs groups, as well as the results from the statistical tests for between-group differences,
are displayed in Table 1. There was no significant difference in the distribution of men and women
across the PG and NPG groups; PGs were significantly older than the NPGs, mean age = 43.11
(SD = 12.79) vs. 38.75 (SD = 12.88), respectively, t(291) = 2.83, p< 0.01. Between-group differ-
ences were assessed using MANCOVA, with age serving as the covariate. Significant differences
were found for three of four impulsivity traits, with PGs scoring higher on impulsiveness and low-
er on deliberation and self-discipline compared to the NPGs; no significant differences emerged
for the excitement-seeking facet. Compared to NPGs, PGs also scored significantly higher on
the neuroticism domain and significantly lower on the conscientiousness domain, with moderate
effect sizes (i.e., Cohen’s d= 0.50) associated with both of these differences. For the facets within
neuroticism excluding impulsivity, PGs scored higher on the facet traits of depression, self-con-
sciousness, and vulnerability. For the facets within the conscientiousness domain excluding self-
discipline and deliberation, PGs scored higher on the competence and dutifulness facet traits.
The facet trait Trust, which is located in the Agreeableness domain, was also significant, although
the overall effect for this domain was not significant.
5. Discussion
Three of four impulsivity-related facet traits – impulsiveness, self-discipline, and deliberation –
distinguished PGs from non-pathological gamblers. The PGs and NPGs were indistinguishable on
the facet trait of excitement-seeking, however, and both groups demonstrated elevated scores rel-
ative to the normative sample on this facet trait (about 0.5 SD higher). High scorers on excite-
ment-seeking typically seek thrill and are drawn to colorful, noisy environments. Although this
description seems intuitively linked with gambling problems, the current findings suggest that
excitement-seeking is associated with gambling behavior in general, rather than with pathological
gambling specifically. This outcome and interpretation is generally consistent with a past report
2
There were no significant differences among those with lifetime but no current PG (n= 67), current but no lifetime
PG (n= 10), and current and lifetime PG (n= 97) for any of the personality domain and facet traits.
876 R.M. Bagby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 43 (2007) 873–880
indicating that sensation seeking, a variable akin to excitement-seeking, is associated with gam-
bling but not severity of gambling problems (Langewisch & Frisch, 1998).
Table 1
Means, standard deviations, test of significance, and effect size differences for the NEO PI-R scores of pathological and
non-pathological gamblers
Domains and facets PGs (n= 106) NPGs (n= 177) F(1,272) d
MSD MSD
Neuroticism 60.71 10.89 54.87 11.61 16.77
*
0.54
N1 Anxiety 55.57 10.37 52.80 10.23 4.95 0.27
N2 Angry hostility 56.97 10.72 54.28 11.16 4.07 0.25
N3 Depression 61.21 11.67 55.10 12.10 14.95
*
0.52
N4 Self-consciousness 55.92 10.73 51.81 10.16 10.33
*
0.38
N5 Impulsiveness 58.90 10.05 54.15 10.19 15.11
*
0.47
N6 Vulnerability 59.37 12.62 53.36 12.72 14.09
*
0.48
Extraversion 49.69 11.79 52.02 10.23 1.52 0.20
E1 Warmth 45.58 12.92 48.43 10.88 2.52 0.22
E2 Gregariousness 49.76 11.45 51.87 10.93 1.12 0.18
E3 Assertiveness 50.45 10.34 50.67 10.00 0.00 0.02
E4 Activity 47.46 9.80 49.33 10.29 1.33 0.19
E5 Excitement-seeking 56.37 9.37 56.47 10.59 0.26 0.01
E6 Positive emotions 47.63 12.12 50.44 10.32 3.34 0.23
Openness 50.02 11.11 51.08 11.49 0.40 0.10
O1 Fantasy 53.83 10.18 53.01 9.64 0.32 0.08
O2 Aesthetics 48.73 10.75 49.31 11.30 0.13 0.05
O3 Feelings 51.34 10.87 51.27 10.45 0.03 0.1
O4 Actions 45.23 11.39 46.48 12.28 0.29 0.11
O5 Ideas 50.22 10.07 51.92 11.11 1.38 0.17
O6 Values 49.87 10.34 51.54 10.52 1.46 0.16
Agreeableness 42.17 12.51 45.43 12.76 4.95 0.26
A1 Trust 41.13 12.62 45.19 11.33 9.05
*
0.32
A2 Straightforwardness 43.06 11.83 45.91 11.69 3.93 0.24
A3 Altruism 45.18 13.13 48.83 12.40 4.62 0.28
A4 Compliance 41.47 11.68 43.89 12.15 3.77 0.21
A5 Modesty 47.91 12.00 48.03 11.70 0.09 0.01
A6 Tender-mindedness 52.63 11.39 51.33 12.68 0.48 0.11
Conscientiousness 38.57 11.15 45.01 11.92 20.89
*
0.58
C1 Competence 40.85 11.94 46.54 11.69 15.74
*
0.48
C2 Order 45.17 11.28 48.28 10.90 5.72 0.28
C3 Dutifulness 40.04 11.54 45.69 11.54 19.47
*
0.49
C4 Achievement striving 41.80 11.56 45.79 12.16 5.94 0.35
C5 Self-discipline 39.04 11.06 44.14 12.42 12.79
*
0.46
C6 Deliberation 42.44 12.03 47.63 11.65 12.72
*
0.43
*
Note: After Bonferroni correction: p< 0.01 for domains (0.05/5 domains), p< 0.008 for all facets (0.05/6 facets per
domain). All means and standard deviations are expressed as T-scores. PGs = pathological gamblers, NPG = non-
pathological gamblers.
R.M. Bagby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 43 (2007) 873–880 877
Impulsivity and impulse disorders may be partially explained by genetic abnormalities in risk-
reward pathways. Comings, Rosenthal, Lesieur, and Rugle (1996) demonstrated that PG is asso-
ciated with a reduction in dopamine D
2
receptor density, which has been implicated in other im-
pulse-related disorders (Blum et al., 1996). Twin studies have indicated that approximately 50% of
the variance for risk of developing PG can be accounted for by genetic factors (Slutske et al.,
2001). Taken together, it is possible that the inability to exercise control over gambling may be
linked to neurobiological correlates that lead to an increased focus on reward, a lesser response
to aversive consequences, and impairments in altering behavior based on risk-reward learning.
In the current investigation PGs also scored significantly higher on the neuroticism domain and
three of its facet traits excluding the impulsiveness facet. Neuroticism represents a predisposition
to develop psychopathology and to experience a wide range of negative affects (Costa & McCrae,
1992). Similarly, Roy, Custer, and Lorenz (1989) reported a strong association with neuroticism,
as measured by the Eysenck personality questionnaire (EPQ; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975), and Slut-
ske et al. (2005) also reported high levels of negative emotionality, as measured by a modified ver-
sion of the multidimensional personality questionnaire (MPQ; Patrick et al., 2002), in those with
disordered gambling behaviour compared to a sample of non-gambling controls.
The conscientiousness domain is the trait with the strongest conceptual links with impulse-con-
trol as it captures the capacity to resist impulses, manage desires, and apply guiding principles to
actively control behavior (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Thus, it was not surprising that PGs scored
lower than the NPGs on four of six facet traits of this domain. This result is also consistent with
previous research in which PGs score significantly higher than normal controls on the psychoti-
cism dimension of the EPQ (Roy et al., 1989), a personality factor inversely correlated with the
conscientiousness personality domain (Costa & Widiger, 2001). Slutske et al. (2005) also reported
problem gambling to be significantly and negatively associated with the constraint personality
dimension of the MPQ.
In sum, results from the current study suggest the overall personality profile of the patholog-
ical gambler is one that combines high impulsivity with emotional vulnerability complemented
by a high level of excitement-seeking common to PGs and NPGs. Although speculative, one
possible interpretation for the development of pathological gambling is that it results from mal-
adaptive efforts to regulate affect or dampen the effects of high neuroticism. After gambling
behavior has been behaviorally conditioned and losses begin to accrue, high impulsivity may
render the PG unable to modify, control, or stop their gambling behavior. Alternatively, the
personality traits of the pathological gambler may be related solely to dysfunctional risk-reward
and biochemical pathways that cause mood disturbance, elevated impulsivity, and an inability
to regulate affect and/or behavior. Finally, PGs might not represent one homogenous popula-
tion, but rather qualitatively distinct subtypes that are influenced by different emotional and
biological factors but exhibit similar phenomenological features (Blaszczynski & Nower,
2002). Future investigations are needed to begin to examine and test these competing
explanations.
We believe this study has several methodological strengths, including the use of non-patholog-
ical gamblers (instead of non-gambling controls) as a comparison group and non-treatment seek-
ing PGs (instead of treatment seeking PGs). NPGs resemble pathological gamblers in their
attraction to gambling and engagement in gambling behavior, which decreases the probability
of conflating gambling behavior with personality and addresses directly potential differences in
878 R.M. Bagby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 43 (2007) 873–880
those who gamble with no gambling pathology from those who gamble with maladaptive and
debilitative patterns of gambling behavior. Treatment seeking PGs comprise less than 2% of
the PGs found in community samples and are therefore not representative of the population of
pathological gamblers (Wallisch, 1996); the use of non-treatment seeking PGs in this study en-
hances the generalization of our results.
There were also limitations associated with the current investigation. Due to the cross-sectional
nature of this study, the temporal relationships between personality and PG could not be assessed.
Whereas the use of non-treatment seeking PGs increases the overall generalizability of the results,
the personality traits that characterize the PGs in this study may not be applicable to treatment
seeking PGs. Several potential confounding factors related to social desirability, demographic dif-
ferences (e.g., socio-economic status, education, ethnicity), memory errors, and context effects
were not controlled for. Finally, we did not calculate inter-rater reliability estimates across the
interviewers who conferred the diagnosis of PG, as different raters were used in the two studies,
the participants from which were combined to maximize statistical power. We believe it unlikely
that systematic interviewer error influenced diagnosis, however, as the proportion of PGs to
NPGs across the two study samples did not differ and the interviewers were well trained and
bi-weekly consultation sessions were held in which diagnostic issues were discussed and cases
reviewed.
References
American Psychiatric Association (APA) (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Bagby, R. M., Costa, P. T., McCrae, R. R., Livesley, W. J., Kennedy, S. H., Levitan, R. D., et al. (1999). Replicating
the five factor model of personality in a psychiatric sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(6), 1135–1139.
Bagby, R. M., Vachon, D. D., Bulmash, E., & Quilty, L. C. (in press). Pathological gambling and personality disorders:
a review and re-examination of prevalence rates. Journal of Personality Disorders.
Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction, 97,
487–499.
Blum, K., Dherid, P. J., Wood, R. C., Braverman, E. R., Chen, T. J., Cull, J. G., et al. (1996). The D2 dopamine
receptor gene as a determinant of reward deficiency syndrome. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 89, 396–400.
Breen, R. B., & Zuckerman, M. (1999). Chasing in gambling behaviour: personality and cognitive determinants.
Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 1097–1111.
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990.
Comings, D. E., Rosenthal, R. J., Lesieur, H. R., & Rugle, L. (1996). A study of the dopamine D2 receptor gene in
pathological gambling. Pharmacogenetics, 6, 223–234.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO five-factor inventory
(NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Costa, P. T., & Widiger, T. A. (2001). Personality disorders and the five factor model of personality. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the EPQ (Eysenck personality questionnaire). San Diego, CA:
Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
Gerdner, R., & Svensson, K. (2003). Predictors of gambling problems among male adolescents. International Journal of
Social Welfare, 12, 182–192.
Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Livesley, J. W. (1998). Heritability of facet-level traits in a
cross-cultural twin sample: support for a hierarchical model of personality. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 1556–1565.
R.M. Bagby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 43 (2007) 873–880 879
Langewisch, M. W. J., & Frisch, G. R. (1998). Gambling behaviour and pathology in relation to impulsivity, sensation
seeking, and risky behaviour in male college students. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14, 245–262.
Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., & Shutte, N. S. (2005). The relationship between the five-factor model of
personality and symptoms of clinical disorders: a meta-analysis. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 27, 101–114.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr., (1995). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52,
509–516.
Nower, L., Derevensky, J. L., & Gupta, R. (2004). The relationship of impulsivity, sensation seeking, coping, and
substance use in youth gamblers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviours, 18, 49–55.
Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J., & Tellegen, A. (2002). Development and validation of a brief form of the multidimensional
personality questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 14, 150–163.
Petry, N. M. (2005). Pathological gambling: etiology, comorbidity, and treatment. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Petry, N. M., Stinson, F. S., & Grant, B. F. (2005). Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other
psychiatric disorders: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry, 66, 564–574.
Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. S. (2002). Pathological gambling: a comprehensive review. Clinical Psychological Review, 22,
1009–1061.
Roy, A., Custer, R., & Lorenz, V. (1989). Personality factors in pathological gambling. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
80, 37–39.
Slutske, W. S., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Poulton, R. (2005). Personality and problem gambling: a prospective study
of a birth cohort of young adults. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 769–775.
Slutske, W. S., True, W. R., Goldberg, J., Eisen, S., Xian, H., Lyons, M. J., et al. (2001). A twin study of the
association between pathological gambling and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
110, 297–308.
Steel, Z., & Blaszczynski, A. (1996). The factorial structure of pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12,
3–20.
Wallisch, L. S. (1996). Gambling in Texas: 1995 surveys of adult and adolescent gambling behavior. Austin, TX: Texas
Commission on Alcohol and Drug Abuse.
Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality
to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 669–689.
Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Thornquist, M., & Kiers, H. (1991). Five (or three) robust questionnaire scale
factors of personality without culture. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 929–941.
880 R.M. Bagby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 43 (2007) 873–880
... Contrarily, participants with high conscientiousness became more cautious in the cooperation condition. Individuals with high conscientiousness tend to have an ability to resist impulses, manage desires, and apply guiding principles to actively control behavior (Bagby et al., 2007). Furthermore, conscientious team members are good at identifying the team's main task and available resources, and developing courses of action to complete the team's objectives (Bell, 2007). ...
... Cooperation with others might not be influential for individuals with low conscientiousness. It has often been reported that a higher level of problem gambling severity is associated with lower scores on conscientiousness (e.g., Bagby et al., 2007;Brunborg et al., 2016), and there is a need to focus on individuals with low conscientiousness. Thus, future studies should aim to determine more effective approaches to reduce problem gambling behavior for individuals with low conscientiousness. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social relationships are highly salient for problem gambling. This study investigated the effect of others' presence or cooperation with others on risky gambling behavior among healthy undergraduate students. Based on the findings that individual differences moderate the social facilitation effect and team performance, we also focused on personality traits. A total of 36 participants completed the Game of Dice Task (GDT) in three conditions, within a crossover design: the sole condition (each participant conducted the GDT individually), the parallel condition (each participant conducted the GDT in parallel with another participant), and the cooperation condition (participants conducted the GDT at the same time and were told to consult with each other). Before the experiment, all participants completed the NEO Five Factor Inventory. The results showed no significant differences among the sole, parallel, and cooperation conditions in terms of risky gambling behavior. However, safer gambling behavior in the cooperation condition was significantly and positively correlated with conscientiousness. These findings suggest that a new harm‐reduction strategy focusing on cooperation with others while gambling could be developed.
... This model describes five basic personality dimensions: Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Openness to Experience [6] encompasses an individual's behavioral, emotional, and cognitive patterns. Investigators using this model in addiction research generally report higher neuroticism in substance dependent individuals compared to controls for both substance use [7,8] and behavioral addictions [9,10]. Such individuals may engage in 'self-medication' to alleviate unpleasant or negative emotional states, characteristic of highly neurotic individuals [11]. ...
... Lower levels of conscientiousness have been reported in substance abusers [8,13], although more so in drug addicts than in alcohol addicts [14]. Lower levels of Conscientiousness have also been found in behavioral addictions such as gambling disorder (GD) and internet addiction (IA) [9,10,13,15]. Results for agreeableness, extraversion, and openness to experience are less consistent. They also seem to vary across addictions and substance types. ...
Article
Full-text available
Nicotine is the major reinforcing component of tobacco and it is believed that the pharmacological effects of nicotine motivate the initiation and maintenance of a smoking habit. HINT1 appears to play a role in the modulation of the effects of drug abuse. Hence, the aim of this study was the analysis of the association between the rs3864283 polymorphism of the HINT1 gene and cigarette use; the analysis of personality traits assessed by the means of the NEO-FFI Inventory; the analysis of anxiety measured by the STAI questionnaire; and the analysis of the interactions between the rs3864283 and both personality traits and anxiety. The study group consisted of 522 volunteers. Of these, 371 were cigarette users and 151 were never-smokers. The genomic DNA was isolated from venous blood using standard procedures. The results of both inventories, i.e., NEO-FFI and STAI., were reported as the sten scores. Genotyping was conducted with the real-time PCR method. Statistically significant differences were found in the frequency of rs3864283 genotypes and alleles in the tested sample of Cigarette Users when compared to the control group. The Cigarette Users compared to the control group obtained higher scores in the assessment of NEO-FFI extraversion scale, and significantly lower results were obtained for the NEO-FFI openness scale, the agreeableness scale, and the conscientiousness scale. There was a statistically significant effect of rs3864283 genotype interaction and Cigarette Use or not using (control group) on the extraversion scale. There was also a statistically significant effect of Cigarette Users or the control group on the extraversion scale score. The results obtained in the presented study indicated a significant association between the HINT1 rs3864283 variant and smoking status. Moreover, this is the first study incorporating genetic association of above-mentioned polymorphic site with interaction analysis of personality traits and anxiety. Overall, the results of this study suggest that HINT1 is an important genetic component associated with nicotine usage mechanisms.
... Personality may influence the development, expression, severity, and maintenance of problematic gambling (Bagby et al., 2007, Mackinnon et al., 2016Takada & Yukawa, 2020). A growing body of research is supporting the contention that disordered gamblers have a common personality profile -one that could provide clues as to the optimal ways to prevent and to treat disordered gambling (Dudfield et al., 2023;Mann et al., 2017;Miller et al., 2013;Quilty et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Understanding the correlates of problematic gambling among emerging adult university students is crucial for developing effective approaches to minimise harm. Methods This cross-sectional survey study reports on 397 18–25 year old emerging adults studying at Irish universities who completed an online survey about problematic gambling and a range of biopsychosocial variables. Chi-square and binary logistic regression analyses explored the relationships between problematic gambling and the biopsychosocial variables measured. Results Chi-square analyses showed that being male, having an online gambling account, having a mobile gambling app, novelty seeking (impulsivity), harm avoidance (fear of uncertainty), and high alcohol volume consumption were significantly associated with problematic gambling. Regression analyses showed that individuals were more likely to report problematic gambling if they were male (OR = 9.57 times), had an online gambling account (OR = 17.05 times), had a mobile gambling app (OR = 20.37 times), scored high in impulsivity (OR = 7.79 times), and reported high alcohol volume consumption (OR = 4.66 times). Individuals were less likely to report problematic gambling if they scored high in fear of uncertainty (OR = 0.26 times). Conclusions A high rate of problematic gambling was observed among the current study sample. Participants were more likely to reported problematic gambling if they were male, had online gambling accounts, mobile gambling apps, scored high in impulsivity, scored low in fear of uncertainty, or consumed high volumes of alcohol in typical drinking sessions. These findings have implications for Irish legislation and policy-makers, Irish higher education institutions, and young adult Irish university students.
... Using the Five-Factor Model of Personality, Bagby et al. (2007) aimed to investigate the personality differences between non-treatment seeking pathological gamblers (PGs) and non-pathological gamblers (NGPs). Findings showed that PGs had significantly higher levels of neuroticism and impulsiveness and lower levels of conscientiousness, self-discipline, and deliberation when compared to NGPs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: This paper aims to investigate the relationship between personality traits, gambling behavior, and social media use. Methods: A questionnaire comprised of the Five-Factor Model (Costa & McCrae, 1992), the Gambling Behavior Scale by Jeff and Gregory (2010) and the Social Networking Time Use Scale (Olufadi, 2015) was administered to 718 students in Northern Cyprus. Pearson’s correlation and multiple linear regression analysis were carried out to test if the five personality traits have a significant positive relationship with gambling behavior. Results: Data analysis showed a significant positive relationship between social media use and gambling behavior. The study reported that while the relationship between three personality traits (extraversion, neuroticism, and openness to experience) and gambling behavior is partially mediated by social media use, the relationship between agreeableness as a personality trait and gambling behavior is not mediated by social media use. Finally, findings showed that the impact of conscientiousness on gambling behavior is influenced by the frequency and time spent on social media. Conclusions: In light of the reported impact of personality traits on gambling behavior, further research should seek to find out what it is about social media that encourages gambling behavior, and what combination of factors is most likely to force social media users to part with money to play or gamble. Finally, different motivations for social media use can be tested as moderators in the relationship between personality traits and gambling behavior.
... Regarding personality traits, our findings point to lower extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and higher neuroticism scores in the risk group. Studies addressing the "Big Five" personality traits have reported significant differences between pathological and nonpathological gamblers in openness to experience, conscientiousness, agreeableness, and neuroticism (MacLaren, Best, et al. 2011;Bagby et al. 2007). In a study of "pure" gamblers without psychiatric comorbidities and addictions, including smoking, it was found that gamblers had higher neuroticism, lower extraversion, lower openness to experience, lower agreeableness, and lower consciousness, in addition to higher impulsivity scores compared to the control group. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Cryptocurrencies are a popular investment tool today. However, some studies highlight the investing behavior of cryptocurrencies similar to pathological gambling. Investing behavior becomes risky when it is not based on proper and adequate analysis and carries the possibility of big losses as well as big gains. For this reason, we aimed to determine the potential risky investor profile by age, gender, personality traits and impulsivity levels. Subjects and methods: Six hundred and fifty-four cryptocurrencies investors (529 was male, 125 was female, their mean age was 35.6 ± 9.0) participated in this study between June 2022 - August 2022. Participants were administered the Sociodemographic Data Form, the South OAKS Gambling Screen Test - revised (SOGS-r), the Big Five Inventory (BFI-10), and Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11). Results: We found higher neuroticism and impulsivity in possible problematic crypto investors. In addition, extraversion, agreeableness and conscientiousness scores were lower. Additionally, in our regression analyzes we found that younger age and male gender predicted SOGS-r scores. After controlling for age and gender, extraversion negatively and motor impulsivity positively predicted SOGS-r scores. Discussion: Investing in cryptocurrencies can become a behavioral addiction, similar to pathological gambling. It is important to identify profiles in which investment behavior is risky. Conclusion: Personality traits and impulsivity may have a significant impact on identifying risky crypto investors and in the treatment process.
... In addition to evidence showing that gambling is associated with high rates of depression and lifetime mood disorders (Bagby et al., 2007;Shek et al., 2012;Takamatsu et al., 2016), there is also research showing that suicidal ideation and suicidal attempts are associated with gambling problems. (Newman & Thompson, 2007;Black et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
The personal and social benefits and harms of gambling, which has been more accessible, diversified, and widespread thanks to global internet use, sometimes find their place in country agendas. Due to its characteristics, the gambling sector requires effective management through public policies. In this direction, governments determine sectoral policies in two ways: by choosing to ban gambling activities completely or by regulating the sector by legalizing certain types. In this context, taxes stand out as one of the policy tools adopted by many countries. This research aimed to evaluate the success of taxes-as a public policy tool-in controlling the social and economic effects of the gambling industry. Through a systematic review, the role and success of taxation against the social and economic influences of gambling have been evaluated in light of literature research and application examples. The present study revealed that taxes, as a policy tool, are insufficient and may lead to undesirable results in combating gambling, which has limited-positive contributions to the country's economies but harms society by creating severe addictions and other irreversible social costs.
... • Good grades in school are predicted by high Conscientiousness and low Neuroticism (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2003); • Risky sexual behaviors are predicted by high Extraversion, high Neuroticism, low Conscientiousness, and low Agreeableness (Allen & Walter, 2018;Miller et al., 2004;Trobst et al., 2002;Zietsch et al., 2010); • High alcohol consumption is predicted by high Extraversion and low Conscientiousness (Paunonen, 2003); • Pathological gambling is predicted by high Neuroticism and low Conscientiousness (Bagby et al., 2007); • Volunteering is predicted by high Agreeableness and high Extraversion (Carlo et al., 2005); • Declining to become a union member at work is predicted by low Extraversion and low Neuroticism (Parkes & Razavi, 2004); • Forgiveness is predicted by high Agreeableness and low Neuroticism (Brose et al., 2005); • Leadership effectiveness is predicted by high Extraversion, high Agreeableness, high Conscientiousness, and low Neuroticism (Hassan et al., 2016;Silverthorne, 2001); • Compliance with social distancing guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic in 2020 was predicted by high Conscientiousness and high Neuroticism (Abdelrahman, 2020;Aschwanden et al., 2021). ...
Chapter
This chapter summarizes the academic research on personality traits, beginning with Gordon Allport’s lexical approach in 1936 and explaining how it led to the emergence of the Big Five Model (Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism), now widely endorsed by psychologists. Additionally, this chapter explores how psychologists can predict specific outcomes based on the scores obtained by participants in questionnaires designed to evaluate these five personality dimensions.KeywordsPersonalityTrait theoryBig FiveOpennessConscientiousnessExtraversionAgreeablenessNeuroticism
Article
Although most individuals consider gambling to be an innocent and fun activity, when it develops into problem gambling, it can have detrimental outcomes to one's life, such as over-indebtedness. This cross-sectional study explores the role of maladaptive personality traits and gender in both problem gambling and over-indebtedness, in an online sample of 1479 adult gamblers (65% males) in Sweden. Participants were administered the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI), the Personality Inventory for DSM-5-Brief Form (PID-5-BF), and questions addressing subjective over-indebtedness and other risk factors. Quasi-Poisson loglinear models and logistic regression analyses demonstrated that Disinhibition (OR = 1.38, 95% CI [1.24, 1.53]), and Antagonism (OR = 1.23, 95% CI [1.14, 1.34]) showed the strongest associations to problem gambling, and that only Disinhibition (OR = 1.72, 95% CI [1.22, 1.43]) and Antagonism (OR = 2.00, 95% CI [1.52, 2.66]) were significantly related to over-indebtedness. The prevalence of problem gambling and over-indebtedness was more common among women, and gender moderated the univariate relationships of Negative Affectivity, Disinhibition and Psychoticism to problem gambling. These findings call for future research addressing maladaptive personality traits, problem gambling and over-indebtedness, and highlight the need for tailored interventions and prevention strategies, particularly for women who may be at higher risk.
Article
Full-text available
This study examines whether problem gambling and behavioral financial cognitive biases can help explain frequent trading in a sample of active investors, suggesting that frequent trading is partly driven by a behavioral addiction to gambling-like activities. of 400 people (M=280, F=120) who reported trading at least once a month. We examined whether gambling and problem gambling were reliable predictors of reported trading intensity. Results showed that rates of gambling and problem gambling were highest among those who reported that stock trading activities were significantly related to measures of stock trading intensity based on time spent per day, the number of transactions, and the amount of money spent. Future research should examine whether gambling history and participation affect how people manage their stock market investments, including their propensity to make riskier decisions and experience more negative outcomes. So far, the relationship between structural characteristics and behavior in an Iranian stock market environment has not been explored. The present study examined the association between structural characteristics and stock market behavior in an environmentally responsible context, using data from real traders. This study supports the hypothesis that behavioral addiction to gambling-like activities is associated with frequent trading of the stock market.
Article
Full-text available
Pathological gambling has been characterised by DSM-III-R and DSM-IV as a disorder of impulse control with a proportion of gamblers identified as meeting criteria for a co-morbid diagnosis of Antisocial Personality Disorder. To date, empirical evidence in support of the notion that pathological gamblers as a group manifest elevated traits of impulsivity remains equivocal. Principal components analysis was used to investigate relationships between the constructs of impulsivity, psychopathy, DSM-III-R criteria for Antisocial Personality Disorder, psychological distress, criminal offending behavior and a range of other common psychological measures employed with pathological gamblers. The sample comprised 115 pathological gamblers, 80 consecutive gamblers seeking treatment from a general hospital psychiatric inpatient behavior therapy unit, and 35 volunteer Gamblers Anonymous attenders. Four primary factors were determined: psychological distress, sensation seeking, crime and liveliness, and impulsive-antisocial. Results suggest that pathological gambling consists of a number of discrete and reproducible factorial structures. The impulsive antisocial factor was found to be associated with gambling behavior and indices of poor psychosocial functioning.
Article
Full-text available
In this study we examined whether the factor structure and traits of the five-factor model of personality (FFM), derived from non-clinical samples, could be replicated in a sample of psychiatric patients. The revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) was administered to a study group of psychiatric patients ( n = 176). The test scores from these patients were intercorrelated, factor analyzed and the obtained factor structure was then compared to the factor structure of the normative data from the NEO PI-R. The factor structure from the psychiatric study group and that from the normative sample were virtually identical, with all five factors showing significant congruence. These results argue favorably for the clinical applicability of the FFM with psychiatric patients. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
This 2nd edition reprints 15 chapters from the 1st edition with updated references and reframes their emphases in terms of the empirical and conceptual advances that have occurred in the last 5 or 6 years. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Many individuals with a history of pathological gambling (PG) also have a history of engaging in antisocial behaviors, and this has often been interpreted as a result of the former causing the latter. In a sample of 7,869 men in 4,497 twin pairs form the Vietnam Era Twin Registry, the authors examined (a) the association between PG and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD), (b) the extent to which PG might be differentially associated with childhood conduct disorder (CD) and adult antisocial behavior (AAB), and (c) the contribution of genetic and environmental factors to the association of PG with ASPD, CD, and AAB. PG was significantly associated with all 3 antisocial behavior disorders, and the association of PG with ASPD, CD, and AAB was predominantly explained by genetic factors. The results of this study suggest that the greater-than-chance co-occurrence of PG and antisocial behavior disorders is partially due to their sharing a common genetic vulnerability. The antisocial behavior observed among many individuals with PG probably cannot be interpreted as being simply a consequence of the PG. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This book examines the prevalence and consequences of problem gambling as well as approaches to treatment. In this comprehensive book, the author, Nancy M. Petry, clarifies the current understanding of gambling as a disorder, including its levels of intensity; possible origins in biological, neurological, developmental, and environmental spheres; and special issues surrounding populations that seem to be more susceptible to problem gambling, including youth, ethnic minorities, and those with comorbid affective disorders such as depression. The author reviews treatments commonly used for pathological gambling as well as nonprofessionally guided interventions such as Gamblers Anonymous. Petry then presents her own brief cognitive-behavioral approach, whose success is empirically proven in the largest known study of psychosocial treatments of problem gamblers. In this book, Petry reviews what we currently know about problem gambling and analyzes promising treatment approaches, making it an invaluable, comprehensive resource for both therapists and researchers in the field of pathological gambling. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)