ArticlePDF Available

Very preterm children show impairments across multiple neurodevelopmental domains by age 4 years

Authors:
  • University of Otago, Christchurch School of Medicine

Abstract and Figures

Neurodevelopmental outcomes associated with preterm birth are of major health and educational concern. This study examined the neuromotor, cognitive, language and emotional/behavioural outcomes of a regional cohort of 4-year-old children born extremely preterm (EPT: 23-27 weeks' gestation), very preterm (VPT: 28-33 weeks) and full term (FT: 38-41 weeks). Of particular interest were children's risks of impairment across multiple neurodevelopmental domains. Data were gathered as part of a prospective longitudinal study of 105 very preterm (< or = 33 weeks gestation) and 107 FT children born during 1998-2000. At 4 years corrected age, children underwent a comprehensive multidisciplinary assessment that included a paediatric neurological examination, cognitive and language testing, and an assessment of child emotional and behavioural adjustment. At age 4 years, compared to FT children, EPT and VPT children had increased risks of cerebral palsy (EPT 18%, VPT 15%, FT 1%), cognitive delay (EPT 33%, VPT 36%, FT 13%), language delay (EPT 29%, VPT 29%, FT 10%) and emotional/behavioural adjustment problems (EPT 37%, VPT 13%, FT 11%). EPT and VPT children were three times more likely to have multiple domain impairments than FT children (EPT 30%, VPT 29%, FT 10%). A substantial proportion of preschool children born very preterm show clinically significant problems in at least one neurodevelopmental domain, with impairment in multiple domains being common. There is a need to monitor preschool development across a range of functional domains and to consider the likely cascading effects of multiple impairments on later development.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Very preterm children show impairments across
multiple neurodevelopmental domains by age
4 years
L J Woodward,
1,2
S Moor,
3
K M Hood,
1
P R Champion,
1
S Foster-Cohen,
1,4
T E Inder,
5
N C Austin
1,6
1
Canterbury Child Development
Research Group, Department of
Psychology, University of
Canterbury, Christchurch, New
Zealand;
2
Van der Veer Institute
for Parkinson’s and Brain
Research, Christchurch, New
Zealand;
3
Department of
Psychological Medicine,
University of Otago,
Christchurch, New Zealand;
4
The Champion Centre,
Burwood Hospital, Christchurch,
New Zealand;
5
Departments of
Paediatrics, Neurology and
Radiology, Washington
University School of Medicine,
St Louis, MO, USA;
6
Christchurch Women’s
Hospital, Christchurch, New
Zealand
Correspondence to:
Associate Professor
Lianne Woodward, Canterbury
Child Development Research
Group, Department of
Psychology, University of
Canterbury, Private Bag 4800,
Christchurch, New Zealand;
lianne.woodward@canterbury.
ac.nz
Accepted 6 February 2009
Published Online First
22 March 2009
ABSTRACT
Objectives: Neurodevelopmental outcomes associated
with preterm birth are of major health and educational
concern. This study examined the neuromotor, cognitive,
language and emotional/behavioural outcomes of a
regional cohort of 4-year-old children born extremely
preterm (EPT: 23–27 weeks’ gestation), very preterm
(VPT: 28–33 weeks) and full term (FT: 38–41 weeks). Of
particular interest were children’s risks of impairment
across multiple neurodevelopmental domains.
Methods: Data were gathered as part of a prospective
longitudinal study of 105 very preterm ((33 weeks
gestation) and 107 FT children born during 1998–2000. At
4 years corrected age, children underwent a compre-
hensive multidisciplinary assessment that included a
paediatric neurological examination, cognitive and lan-
guage testing, and an assessment of child emotional and
behavioural adjustment.
Results: At age 4 years, compared to FT children, EPT
and VPT children had increased risks of cerebral palsy
(EPT 18%, VPT 15%, FT 1%), cognitive delay (EPT 33%,
VPT 36%, FT 13%), language delay (EPT 29%, VPT 29%,
FT 10%) and emotional/behavioural adjustment problems
(EPT 37%, VPT 13%, FT 11%). EPT and VPT children were
three times more likely to have multiple domain
impairments than FT children (EPT 30%, VPT 29%, FT
10%).
Conclusions: A substantial proportion of preschool
children born very preterm show clinically significant
problems in at least one neurodevelopmental domain,
with impairment in multiple domains being common.
There is a need to monitor preschool development across
a range of functional domains and to consider the likely
cascading effects of multiple impairments on later
development.
The birth of a premature baby is a stressful time
for parents, with concerns about the future being a
major focus of discussion with clinicians. Accurate
information about potential neurodevelopmental
challenges ahead is essential to providing appro-
priate guidance to parents. It is also invaluable to
developmental surveillance teams responsible for
follow-up visits and the timely introduction of
intervention services.
12
This is important since
converging evidence shows that despite gains in
survival, rates of longer term morbidity remain
high amongst children born very preterm, with 5–
15% experiencing motor deficits such as cerebral
palsy,
34
and a further 25–50% developing clinically
significant difficulties that will impact their
educational progress and family life. Such
difficulties include intellectual delay,
5
speech/
articulation problems,
6
ADHD
57
and specific learn-
ing disorders affecting reading, writing and mathe-
matics.
89
These risks increase with decreasing
gestational age at birth.
10
Although these longer term consequences of
preterm birth have been well described, much less
is known about the early emergence of these
difficulties prior to school entry. Existing infant
and preschool follow-up studies have focussed
largely on the risks and sequelae of severe
neurodevelopmental disabilities such as cerebral
palsy and severe intellectual delay. In contrast,
little consideration is given to other important
domains of development (eg, language, behaviour)
that are also undergoing rapid and complex
development, and which together with motor
and cognitive functioning form the building blocks
of later social, behavioural and academic suc-
cess.
11 12
What is already known on this topic
cExisting preschool studies show that very
preterm children are at elevated risk of severe
neurodevelopmental disability including cerebral
palsy and severe intellectual delay.
cHowever, few studies have assessed functioning
across multiple neurodevelopmental domains or
the extent of less severe but clinically significant
functional impairments.
What this study adds
cNeurodevelopmental risks associated with
preterm birth extend beyond neuromotor and
cognitive functioning to language and
behavioural adjustment during the preschool
period.
cComorbid impairment across multiple
neurodevelopmental domains is common
amongst preschool children born very and
extremely preterm.
cFollow-up studies need to assess children
across a range of neurodevelopmental domains,
so as not to underestimate both current and
future risk.
Original article
Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2009;94:F339–F344. doi:10.1136/adc.2008.146282 F339
There is some evidence that even before school age, children
born very preterm show signs of language delay,
13
regulatory
and behavioural difficulties
14
and impaired motor function, as
well as general
3
and specific neurocognitive difficulties.
15
Although these studies have highlighted key areas of impair-
ment, few have examined the impacts of preterm birth across
multiple neurodevelopmental domains. Consequently, the
extent and nature of comorbid impairments within this
population remain unclear. This issue is of clinical importance
for two reasons. First, it is possible that the assessment of a
single or narrow range of domains may underestimate a child’s
developmental problems.
16
Second, it is highly likely that
children subject to multiple domain impairments will experi-
ence greater functional difficulties in everyday life, as well as a
poorer long term prognosis than children with no or single
domain impairments.
17
Drawing on prospective longitudinal data from a regional
cohort of children born very preterm in Christchurch, New
Zealand, the aims of this study were:
(1) To assess the extent and patterns of neuromotor, cognitive,
language and emotional/behavioural impairment at age
4 years amongst children born extremely preterm, very
preterm and full term.
(2) To examine relations between gestational status at birth
and children’s later risk of impairment across multiple
neurodevelopmental domains spanning neuromotor func-
tion, cognition, language and emotional/behavioural adjust-
ment.
METHODS
Participants
Study participants consisted of two groups of children. The first
group was a regional cohort of 105 children born very preterm
((33 weeks’ gestation) who were consecutively admitted to a
level III neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) at Christchurch
Women’s Hospital (New Zealand) over a 2-year period
(November 1998–December 2000). The second group of study
children, recruited at age 2 years, consisted of a sample of 107
full term children matched to the very preterm cohort for
gender, place and date of birth.
Preterm children had a mean gestational age of 28 weeks
(range: 23–33) and a mean birth weight of 1050 g (range: 440–
1790 g). In total, 92% of eligible infants were recruited at birth.
Excluding deaths (n = 3), 98% were followed up to age 4. An
additional child (blind) was not assessed on all measures. Since
prior research shows that those born at earlier gestational ages
may be especially vulnerable to later neurodevelopmental
challenges,
10
preterm children were divided into two groups:
those born extremely preterm (EPT: 23–27 weeks’ gestation,
n = 43) and those born very preterm (VPT: 28–33 weeks’
gestation, n = 62).
The full term born children (FT: 38–41 weeks’ gestation)
were identified from hospital birth records for the same period
by alternately selecting, in a forwards and backwards fashion,
the second child listed in the delivery schedule. Of those
identified, 62% were recruited at age 2. Reasons for non-
participation included untraced (47%), moved overseas (12.5%),
refused (12.5%) and agreed but couldn’t attend clinic appoint-
ment/s within the 4-week assessment window due to illness or
family circumstances (28%). Comparison of the socioeconomic
profile of families in the FT group with regional census data
18
showed that these families were highly representative of the
region from which they were recruited. Retention to age 4 was
96%. Data for one additional child were excluded due to
incompleteness. A descriptive profile of the infant medical and
family background characteristics of the three study groups is
provided in table 1.
Measures
Information relating to each child’s antenatal, perinatal and
postnatal course was collected from clinical notes, hospital
databases and parent interviews at term, 2 and 4 years. At
corrected age 4 years (¡2 weeks), all children underwent a
comprehensive multidisciplinary assessment of neuromotor
functioning, cognition, language and emotional/behavioural
adjustment. With the exception of cerebral palsy, clinical
impairment was determined on the basis of the score distribu-
tions of our regionally representative control group. This
approach avoided problems associated with the use of norma-
tive test data.
19
All procedures and measures were approved by
our regional ethics committee and written informed consent
was obtained from all parents/guardians.
Neuromotor functioning
The quality of children’s motor skills, coordination, gait and
behaviour was assessed by an experienced paediatrician.
Cerebral palsy was diagnosed using standard criteria including:
(a) anatomic location/body parts impaired (eg, hemiplegia,
diplegia); (b) degree of impairment to muscle tone and reflexes;
and (c) severity of impact on ambulation.
20
Cognitive ability
A short form of the revised Wechsler Preschool and Primary
Scales of Intelligence test (WPPSI-R)
21
provided a standardised
measure of general cognitive ability. This measure consisting of
two verbal (Comprehension and Arithmetic) and two perfor-
mance (Picture Completion and Block Design) subtests has been
shown to be reliable and to correlate well with the full WPPSI
(r = 0.89–0.92).
22
Cognitive delay was defined as an IQ score
.1 SD below the mean for the FT group. This criterion
included children with both mild (.1 SD and (2 SD) and
severe (.2 SD) delays.
Language development
The preschool version of the Clinical Evaluation of Language
Fundamentals test (CELF-P)
23
was used to measure children’s
expressive, receptive and overall language ability. This measure
is internally consistent, has good test–retest reliability and
correlates highly with other language scales.
24
Expressive,
receptive and overall (total) language delay was defined as a
standard score .1 SD below the mean for the FT group. This
definition spanned both mild (.1 SD and (2 SD) and severe
(.2 SD) delay.
Emotional and behavioural adjustment
The parent report Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
(SDQ)
25
measured the extent to which children were subject to
emotional problems, hyperactivity/inattention, conduct pro-
blems and peer relationship problems. Overall behavioural
adjustment was also assessed by summing children’s scores on
the emotional problems, hyperactivity/inattention and conduct
problems subscales. The SDQ correlates highly with other
established measures of child behaviour.
26
Given the young age
of study children, impairment was defined utilising a worst 10%
cut-point.
14
Based on the score distributions for the FT group, a
subscale score equal to or greater than the 90th percentile was
defined as a clinically significant problem (emotional problems
Original article
F340 Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2009;94:F339–F344. doi:10.1136/adc.2008.146282
.3; hyperactivity/inattention .5, conduct problems .4; peer
problems .3 for girls, .2 for boys).
RESULTS
Neurodevelopmental impairment at age 4
Table 2 shows the neuromotor, cognitive, language and
emotional/behavioural outcomes of children born extremely
preterm (EPT: ,28 weeks’ gestation), very preterm (VPT: 28–
33 weeks’ gestation) and full term (FT: 38–41 weeks) at 4 years
corrected age. Outcomes for the total sample of VPT children
are also reported. Across all domains, children born extremely
and very preterm were at increased risk of later problems
relative to FT children. The extent and severity of later
problems also increased with decreasing gestational age. These
findings were robust to statistical control for family socio-
economic status. For each neurodevelopmental domain, the
results in table 2 were as follows.
Table 1 Characteristics of the sample
Outcome
All preterm,
23–33 weeks’ GA
(n = 105)
Extremely preterm,
,28 weeks’ GA
(n = 43)
Very preterm,
28–33 weeks’ GA
(n = 62)
Full term,
38–41 weeks’ GA
(n = 107) F/x
2
p Value
Child clinical characteristics
Mean (SD) birth weight 1061.60 (310.94) 807.51 (233.67) 1237.82 (223.97) 3574.50 (409.84) 1534.51 ,0.001
% Male 50.5 46.5 53.2 55.6 1.01 0.60
% Singletons 65.7 72.1 61.3 96.3 34.59 ,0.001
% Small (,2 SD) for GA 10.5 16.3 6.5 0.9 13.75 0.001
% Oxygen at 36 weeks 34.3 65.1 12.9 0.0 96.14 ,0.001
% Intraventricular haemorrhage grade III/IV
and/or PVL
10.5 16.3 6.5 0.0 16.22 ,0.001
% Any postnatal corticosteroid use 10.5 23.3 1.6 0.0 34.49 ,0.001
% Patent ductus arteriosus 43.8 65.1 29.0 0.0 81.78 ,0.001
Family characteristics
Mean (SD) maternal age 30.83 (5.34) 31.02 (5.89) 30.69 (4.98) 31.02 (4.48) 0.10 0.91
% European ethnicity 86.7 83.7 88.7 88.0 0.65 0.72
% Single parent family 18.6 21.4 16.7 11.3 2.63 0.27
Family SES*
Professional/managerial 26.7 25.6 27.4 36.1
Technical/skilled 43.8 48.8 40.3 53.7
Semi/unskilled/unemployed 29.5 25.6 32.3 10.2 13.65 0.008
GA, gestational age; PVL, periventricular leukomalacia; SES, socioeconomic status.
*Family SES was assessed using the revised Elley and Irving Socio-Economic Index.
31
Table 2 Neuromotor, cognitive, language and behavioural adjustment outcomes at age 4 years of children born extremely preterm, very preterm and
full term
Outcome
All preterm
(n = 105)
Extremely preterm
(n = 43)
Very preterm
(n = 62)
Full term
(n = 107) F/x
2
p Value
Neuromotor
% Cerebral palsy 16.2 18.6 14.5 0.9 16.58 ,0.001
Cognitive ability
Mean (SD) WPPSI-R IQ score 94.91 (15.45) 93.86 (17.57) 95.65 (13.88) 104.70 (13.45) 12.28 ,0.001
% Cognitive delay 34.3 32.6 35.5 13.1 13.34 0.001
Language development*
Mean (SD) receptive language score 90.70 (12.22) 90.26 (12.75) 91.00 (11.96) 97.53 (13.10) 7.36 0.001
% Receptive language delay 30.0 34.1 27.1 15.2 7.10 0.03
Mean (SD) expressive language score 93.60 (13.47) 93.62 (13.94) 93.60 (13.28) 99.09 (13.17) 4.28 0.02
% Expressive language delay 25.0 22.0 27.1 12.4 5.83 0.05
% Overall language delay 31.0 31.7 30.5 15.2 7.22 0.03
Emotional and behavioural adjustment{
Mean (SD) emotional problems score 2.03 (1.78) 2.35 (1.86) 1.80 (1.70) 1.42 (1.40) 5.34 0.006
% Emotional problems 16.5 23.3 11.7 6.5 8.47 0.01
Mean (SD) conduct problems score 2.66 (2.10) 2.81 (2.39) 2.55 (1.88) 2.21 (1.77) 1.61 0.20
% Conduct problems 16.5 23.3 11.7 14.0 2.86 0.24
Mean (SD) hyperactive/attentional problems score 3.86 (2.55) 4.42 (2.90) 3.47 (2.21) 2.93 (2.23) 6.03 0.003
% Hyperactive/attentional problems 24.3 37.2 15.0 11.2 14.68 0.001
Mean (SD) peer problems score 1.62 (1.75) 2.28 (2.06) 1.15 (1.31) 1.16 (1.44) 8.93 ,0.001
% Peer relationship problems 20.4 27.9 15.0 10.3 7.38 0.03
Mean (SD) total difficulties score 10.17 (5.86) 11.86 (6.47) 8.97 (5.10) 7.73 (4.49) 9.98 ,0.001
% Overall behavioural difficulties 23.3 37.2 13.3 11.2 15.45 ,0.001
WPPSI-R, revised Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence.
*Excludes two extremely preterm (not assessed), three very preterm and two FT children whose families chose not to complete the additional language assessment.
{Excludes two very preterm (twin) children for whom parent questionnaire data were not returned.
Original article
Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2009;94:F339–F344. doi:10.1136/adc.2008.146282 F341
Neuromotor
For cerebral palsy, 19% (n = 8/43: 4 mild, 2 moderate, 2 severe)
of EPT, 15% (n = 9/62: 5 mild, 3 moderate, 1 severe) of VPT and
one FT child (mild) were diagnosed with this condition by age 4.
Since our 2-year evaluation, an additional three preterm and one
FT child (antenatal antiepileptic exposure) had met criteria for
mild cerebral palsy. All new cases had an abnormal neurological
examination previously. Three children (2 EPT, 1 VPT) with
severe cerebral palsy at age 2 were diagnosed as moderate at
age 4.
Cognition and language
With respect to cognitive development, 33% (n = 14/43: 9 mild,
5 severe) of EPT and 36% (n = 22/62: 17 mild, 5 severe) of VPT
children were delayed compared to only 13% (n = 14/107: 12
mild, 2 severe) of FT children. Preterm children were also
characterised by high rates of overall language delay, with
nearly a third of EPT (32%, n = 13/41: 7 mild, 6 severe) and VPT
children (31%, n = 18/59: 16 mild, 2 severe) being delayed
compared to FT children (15%, n = 16/105: 14 mild, 2 severe).
Receptive delay was more common than expressive delay
amongst EPT children.
Emotional and behavioural adjustment
The most frequently reported problem amongst preterm
preschoolers was hyperactivity/inattention, with 37% (n = 16/
43) of EPT and 15% (n = 9/60) of VPT children obtaining scores
above the 90th percentile. The next most common area of
difficulty was peer relationship problems (EPT 28%, n = 12/43;
VPT 15%, n = 9/60) followed by emotional problems (EPT 23%,
n = 10/43; VPT 12%, n = 7/60), with risks being greatest for
EPT children. No significant differences were found for conduct
problems (p = 0.22). In terms of overall behavioural adjustment,
more than a third of EPT children (37%, n = 16/43) obtained a
total difficulties score in the clinical/abnormal range compared
to 13% (n = 8/60) and 11% (n = 12/107) of VPT and FT
children, respectively.
Extent and nature of comorbid patterns of impairment
To examine the extent to which children born very preterm and
extremely preterm were subject to impairment across multiple
neurodevelopmental domains, the number of domains impaired
was summed for each child using the following measures:
cerebral palsy, cognitive delay, language delay and a total
SDQ emotional/behavioural adjustment score >14 (10th
percentile cut-point). To minimise data loss, children with
missing data in one domain were assigned a score of 0 (no
impairment) unless clear impairment was evident at ages 2 and
6 years (n = 1 VPT child with severe and persistent behaviour
problems).
Table 3 shows that across all domains, only 40% of preterm
children were free of any impairment compared to 74% of full
term children at 4 years. When examined by gestational status,
EPT children were the least likely to be free of impairment
(33%), followed by VPT (45%) and then FT children. Comorbid
patterns of impairment were also more common amongst
children born extremely preterm and very preterm, with almost
a third of both groups exhibiting clinically significant impair-
ment in two or more neurodevelopmental domains.
The patterns of comorbidity observed within each group are
shown in fig 1. Overall, 67% of EPT, 55% of VPT and 26% of FT
children exhibited problems in at least one neurodevelopmental
domain (table 3, fig 1). For all groups, comorbidity was
particularly marked amongst children with cerebral palsy
(EPT: n = 7/8; VPT: n = 6/9; FT: n = 1/1). However, even
excluding these children, comorbidity was common across
neurodevelopmental domains.
DISCUSSION
This study examined the neurodevelopmental outcomes of a
regional cohort of 4-year-old children born very preterm. Of
special interest was the extent to which children born extremely
and very preterm were subject to impairments across multiple
neurodevelopmental domains and the nature of these comor-
bidities. Study strengths included the prospective longitudinal
research design, the unselected nature of the preterm sample,
our demographically representative comparison group, high
sample recruitment and retention, and the examination of a
diverse range of functional outcomes.
Results confirm the presence of high rates of neurodevelop-
mental impairment amongst preschool children born very
preterm spanning neuromotor functioning, cognition, language
and emotional/behavioural adjustment. By 4 years corrected
age, 16% of preterm children met clinical criteria for cerebral
palsy, a third showed mild/severe cognitive delay and between a
quarter and a third had delayed receptive or expressive language
development. Rates of emotional and behavioural problems
were also high, ranging from 16% to 24%, with hyperactivity/
inattention being most common, followed by peer relationship
difficulties and emotional problems. These elevated rates of
emotional problems may reflect emotional regulatory difficul-
ties
27
and/or the early emergence of internalising problems.
Finally, as previously demonstrated,
10
neurodevelopmental risks
were greatest for children born extremely preterm, especially
with respect to cerebral palsy and behavioural adjustment. EPT
children also tended to be subject to more severe impairment.
These findings are generally consistent with existing preschool
studies documenting high rates of severe neurodevelopmental
disability,
28 29
as well as those examining specific impairments in
cognition and behaviour.
14 30
Expanding on existing research, we also examined the extent
to which EPT and VPT children were subject to impairments
across multiple neurodevelopmental domains. Findings showed
that only 40% of all preterm children were free of any
impairment. Amongst those with impairment in one domain,
Table 3 Number of neurodevelopmental domains impaired at age 4 years
Number of domains
impaired
All preterm
(n = 105)
Extremely
preterm
(n = 43)
Very preterm
(n = 62)
Full term
(n = 107) x
2
p Value
0 40.0 32.6 45.2 73.8
1 30.5 37.2 25.8 15.9
2 19.0 16.3 21.0 6.5
3 6.7 7.0 6.5 3.7
4 3.8 7.0 1.6 0.0 33.43 ,0.001
Original article
F342 Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2009;94:F339–F344. doi:10.1136/adc.2008.146282
almost half were experiencing difficulties in another. Within the
FT group, most children who showed impairment did so in only
one domain. One other study of 5-year-old children born very
preterm (,30 weeks) also found that only 39% scored in the
normal range on measures of neurological, motor, cognitive and
behavioural functioning, with nearly half (44%) scoring below
age norms in two or more developmental domains.
17
Whilst
informative, no full term comparison data were available and
the nature of the comorbid impairments was not reported in
this study.
In contrast, our analyses showed that across all groups,
children with cerebral palsy were characterised by high rates of
comorbid cognitive, language and behaviour problems.
However, multiple impairments were not unique to these very
high risk children but were shared to a lesser extent by other
EPT and VPT children. Examination of the patterns of
impairment amongst preterm children also revealed high levels
of comorbidity between cognitive and language delay, likely
reflecting their functional inter-dependence as well as shared
processing demands. However, in the interpretation of these
findings it is important to note that this analysis did not
consider comorbidities within the psychosocial domain such as
between attentional and conduct problems. Nonetheless,
findings do suggest that the assessment of a limited range of
functional outcomes is likely to underestimate the extent of a
child’s problems. They also raise concerns about the extent to
which multiple domain impairments may increasingly limit
children’s learning opportunities and have potentially cascading
effects on development over time.
Finally, several measurement issues are worthy of note. First,
our analyses lend support to concerns about the use of outdated
test norms for defining neurodevelopmental delay.
19
Examination of our data using test norms revealed considerable
variability across measures, with some measures greatly under-
estimating risk (eg, WPPSI-R) and others producing similar
results to those based on our comparison group (eg, CELF-P). A
second issue concerns the choice of clinical cut-points across
domains, since these can vary across measures. To optimise
comparability with other studies, we defined impairment using
established clinical criteria for each domain rather than apply a
uniform criterion. However, as part of this approach, cut-points
were selected to ensure base rates were similar across the
cognitive, language and behavioural domains (11–15%).
Nonetheless, further follow-up of this cohort will be important
in establishing the longer term clinical significance of these
classifications as well as the prognostic significance of multiple
domain impairment.
In conclusion, study results clearly demonstrate that neuro-
developmental problems are common and detectable before
school entry. They also raise serious concerns about the
preparedness of many preterm children, especially those with
multiple impairments, for the cognitive, behavioural and
interpersonal challenges of the classroom. As such, these
findings have a number of implications for neonatal follow-up
Figure 1 Patterns of impairment found at age 4 years in each study group. (A) Patterns for children born extremely preterm, very preterm and full
term. (B) Overall summary for the total sample of very preterm children in comparison with the full term group.
Original article
Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2009;94:F339–F344. doi:10.1136/adc.2008.146282 F343
and educational services. First, careful assessment of a child’s
developmental status, including both strengths and weaknesses,
will be important in assisting parent–teacher discussions about
how learning can best be supported. Second, early childhood
and primary school teachers, alongside other professional
groups, need to be skilled in the early identification and
effective management of the learning and behavioural disorders
that affect preterm children.
Acknowledgements: Special thanks to Jacqueline Knight and Carole Spencer for
assistance with data collection and to study families for their time and support of this
project.
Funding: This research was funded from grants from the Neurological Foundation of
New Zealand, Research Council of New Zealand, Canterbury Medical Research
Foundation, and the Lottery Grants Board.
Competing interests: None.
Ethics approval: All procedures and measures were approved by the regional ethics
committee.
REFERENCES
1. Allen M. Preterm outcomes research: a critical component of neonatal intensive
care. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2002;8:221–33.
2. Dorling JS, Field DJ. Follow up of infants following discharge from the neonatal unit:
structure and process. Early Hum Dev 2006;82:151–6.
3. Wood NS, Costeloe K, Gibson AT, et al. The EPICure study: associations and
antecedents of neurological and developmental disability at 30 months of age
following extremely preterm birth. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2005;90:F134–
F140.
4. Woodward LJ, Anderson PJ, Austin NC, et al. Neonatal MRI to predict
neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm infants. New Engl J Med 2006;355:685–
94.
5. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, et al. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school
aged children who were born preterm. J Am Med Assoc 2002;288:728–37.
6. Wolke D, Meyer R. Cognitive status, language attainment, and prereading skills of 6-
year-old very preterm children and their peers: the Bavarian Longitudinal Study. Dev
Med Child Neurol 1999;41:94–109.
7. Horwood LJ, Mogridge N, Darlow BA. Cognitive, educational and behavioural
outcomes at 7 to 8 years in a national very low birth weight cohort. Arch Dis Child
Fetal Neonatal Ed 1998;79:F12–F20.
8. Taylor HG, Klein N, Minich NM, et al. Middle-school-age outcomes in children with
very low birthweight. Child Dev 2000;71:1495–1511.
9. Litt J, Taylor GH, Klein NL, et al. Learning disabilities in children with very low
birthweight: prevalence, neuropsychological correlates, and educational interventions.
J Learn Disabil 2005;38:130–41.
10. Saigal S, Doyle LW. An overview of mortality and sequelae of preterm birth from
infancy to adulthood. Lancet 2008;371:261–9.
11. Spira EG, Fischel JE. The impact of preschool inattention, hyperactivity, and
impulsivity on social and academic development: a review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
2005;46:755–73.
12. McClelland MM, Cameron CE, Connor CM, et al. Links between behavioral
regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Dev Psychol
2007;43:947–59.
13. Foster-Cohen S, Edgin JO, Champion PR, et al. Early delayed language development
in very preterm infants: evidence from the MacArthur Bates CDI. J Child Lang
2007;34:655–75.
14. Delobel-Ayoub M, Kaminski M, Marret S, et al. Behavioral outcome at 3 years of
age in very preterm infants: the EPIPAGE study. Pediatrics 2006;117:1996–2005.
15. Edgin JO, Inder TE, Anderson PJ, et al. Executive functioning in preschool children
born very preterm: relationship with early white matter pathology. J Int Neuropsychol
Soc 2008;14:90–101.
16. de Kleine MJ, Den Ouden AL, Kollee LAA, et al. Development and evaluation of a
follow up assessment of preterm infants at 5 years of age. Arch Dis Child
2003;88:870–5.
17. Van Baar AL, Van Wassenaer AG, Briet JM, et al. Very preterm birth is associated with
disabilities in multiple developmental domains. J Pediatr Psychol 2005;30:247–55.
18. Statistics New Zealand. 2001 Census: regional summary (2001). See http://
www2.stats.govt.nz (accessed 22 June 2009).
19. Wolke D, Ratschinski G, Ohrt B, et al. The cognitive outcome of very preterm infants
may be poorer than often reported: an empirical investigation of how methodological
issues make a big difference. Eur J Pediatr 1994;153:906–15.
20. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, et al. Development and reliability of a system to
classify gross motor function in children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol
1997;39:214–23.
21. Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised. San
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, 1989.
22. LoBello SG. A short form of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of
Intelligence-Revised. J School Psychol 1991;29:229–36.
23. Wiig E, Secord W, Semel E. Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals-preschool:
examiners manual. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1992.
24. Morrow CE, Vogel AL, Anthony JC, et al. Expressive and receptive language
functioning in preschool children with prenatal cocaine exposure. J Pediatr Psychol
2004;29:543–54.
25. Goodman R. The Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 1997;38:581–6.
26. Goodman R, Scott S. Comparing the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire and the
Child Behavior Checklist: is small beautiful? J Abnorm Child Psychol 1999;27:17–24.
27. Clark CAC, Woodward LJ, Horwood LJ, et al. Development of emotional and
behavioral regulation in children born extremely preterm and very preterm: biological
and social influences. Child Dev 2008;79:1445–63.
28. Salt A, D’Amore A, Ahluwalia J, et al. Outcome at 2 years for very low birthweight
infants in a geographical population: risk factors, cost, and impact of congenital
anomalies. Early Hum Dev 2006;82:125–33.
29. Wood NS, Marlow N, Costeloe K, et al. Neurological and developmental disability
after extremely preterm birth. New Engl J Med 2000;343:378–84.
30. Reijneveld SA, de Kleine MJK, van Baar AL, et al. Behavioural and emotional
problems in very preterm and very low birthweight infants at age 5 years. Arch Dis
Child 2006;91:F423–F428.
31. Elley WB, Irving J. Revised socio-economic index for New Zealand. New
Zealand J Educ Stud 2003;38:3–17.
Original article
F344 Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2009;94:F339–F344. doi:10.1136/adc.2008.146282
... A preterm birth impacts diverse facets of development: physical health, child development, cognition, motor skills, language abilities, and so on (e.g., Brydges et al. 2018;de Kieviet et al. 2009;Woodward et al. 2009). The intensity of the effect of a preterm birth also varies, depending on various factors such as access to timely and effective intervention, how many weeks early the baby was delivered, and access to quality neonatal and prenatal care. ...
... They found a clear-cut relationship between IQ difference and the number of weeks born early: <28 weeks = 11.7 points, 17 studies; 28-34 weeks = 11.0 points (34 studies); and 34-37 weeks = 3.6 points (5 studies). Woodward et al. (2009) studied the degree of prematurity in a group of 4-year-olds who were tested on a four-subtest Wechsler short form as part of a prospective longitudinal investigation in New England. They compared short-form IQs of extremely preterm (23-27 weeks, n = 43), very preterm (28-33 weeks, n = 62), and full-term (38-41 weeks, n = 107) children. ...
... Zimmerman 2018), highlighting prematurity's impact on language development. Additionally, research has also indicated that receptive language delays were increasingly common over expressive language delays for children born extremely prematurely (Woodward et al. 2009). The literature on prematurity and language deficits extends to a variety of diverse languages and cultures, such as in Brazil (Verreschi et al. 2020), Chile, Estonia, and Bavaria (Putnick et al. 2017;Tulviste et al. 2020;Varela-Moraga et al. 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
The literature on children born prematurely has consistently shown that full-term babies outperform preterm babies by about 12 IQ points, even when tested as adolescents, and this advantage for full-term infants extends to the language and motor domains as well. The results of comprehensive meta-analyses suggest that the degree of prematurity greatly influences later test performance, but these inferences are based on data from an array of separate studies with no control of potential confounding variables such as age. This study analyzed Bayley-4 data for 66 extremely premature infants and toddlers (<32 weeks), 70 moderately premature children (32–36 weeks), and 133 full-term children. All groups were carefully matched on key background variables by the test publisher during the standardization of the Bayley-4. This investigation analyzed data on the five subtests: cognitive, expressive communication, receptive communication, fine motor, and gross motor. A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) assessed for group mean differences across the three subsamples, while controlling for the children’s age. Extremely premature children scored significantly lower than moderately premature children on all subtests, and both preterm groups were significantly outscored by the full-term sample across all domains. In each set of comparisons, the cognitive and motor subtests yielded the largest differences, whereas language development, both expressive and receptive, appeared the least impacted by prematurity. A follow-up MANOVA was conducted to examine full-term versus preterm discrepancies on the five subtests for infants (2–17 months) vs. toddlers (18–42 months). For that analysis, the two preterm groups were combined into a single preterm sample, and a significant interaction between the age level and group (full-term vs. preterm) was found. Premature infants scored lower than premature toddlers on receptive communication, fine motor, and cognitive. Neither expressive communication nor gross motor produced significant discrepancies between age groups The findings of this study enrich the preterm literature on the degree of prematurity; the age-based interactions have implications for which abilities are most likely to improve as infants grow into toddlerhood.
... Individuals born at ELBW are at greater risk of developing mental health difficulties than their peers born at normal birth weight (NBW; >2500 g; see Johnson & Marlow, 2014;Mathewson et al., 2017;Pyhälä et al., 2017 for reviews). Problems associated with emotional disorders, (e.g., Johnson et al., 2010;Taylor et al., 2015) and social functioning (e.g., Natalucci et al., 2013;Poole et al., 2020;Woodward et al., 2009) are well-documented in children, adolescents, and adults born extremely preterm. Externalizing behaviors are less frequently elevated in preterm children (e.g., Aarnoudse-Moens et al., 2009;Woodward et al., 2009, but see Elgen et al., 2012) and adolescents (Boyle et al., 2011), except for attention deficits, which are common at both ages (e.g., Anderson et al., 2021;Lindström et al., 2011;Woodward et al., 2009). ...
... Problems associated with emotional disorders, (e.g., Johnson et al., 2010;Taylor et al., 2015) and social functioning (e.g., Natalucci et al., 2013;Poole et al., 2020;Woodward et al., 2009) are well-documented in children, adolescents, and adults born extremely preterm. Externalizing behaviors are less frequently elevated in preterm children (e.g., Aarnoudse-Moens et al., 2009;Woodward et al., 2009, but see Elgen et al., 2012) and adolescents (Boyle et al., 2011), except for attention deficits, which are common at both ages (e.g., Anderson et al., 2021;Lindström et al., 2011;Woodward et al., 2009). Low birth weight (e.g., Faa et al., 2016;Gale & Martyn, 2004;Nomura et al., 2007) and factors limiting fetal growth (e.g., Tore et al., 2018) may be implicated in the etiology of affective disorders that emerge later in development. ...
... Problems associated with emotional disorders, (e.g., Johnson et al., 2010;Taylor et al., 2015) and social functioning (e.g., Natalucci et al., 2013;Poole et al., 2020;Woodward et al., 2009) are well-documented in children, adolescents, and adults born extremely preterm. Externalizing behaviors are less frequently elevated in preterm children (e.g., Aarnoudse-Moens et al., 2009;Woodward et al., 2009, but see Elgen et al., 2012) and adolescents (Boyle et al., 2011), except for attention deficits, which are common at both ages (e.g., Anderson et al., 2021;Lindström et al., 2011;Woodward et al., 2009). Low birth weight (e.g., Faa et al., 2016;Gale & Martyn, 2004;Nomura et al., 2007) and factors limiting fetal growth (e.g., Tore et al., 2018) may be implicated in the etiology of affective disorders that emerge later in development. ...
Article
We compared individual differences in the Ponderal Index [(PI; weight(kg)/height(m3)] and head circumference (HC) in predicting internalizing and externalizing behaviors in childhood and adolescence in a cohort of ELBW survivors prospectively followed since birth. ELBW infants who were born thinner or with smaller HC showed greater PI or HC growth in the first 3 years. Latent difference score (LDS) models showed that compensatory HC growth in the first year, controlled for birth HC, predicted ADHD behaviors in adolescence in those born with smaller HC. LDS models also indicated that larger increases in the PI in the first year and smaller net decreases over 3 years predicted more internalizing behaviors in adolescence. Early growth patterns prioritizing weight over height may have negative effects on later mental health in ELBW survivors, consistent with developmental programming theories.
... Preterm birth remains a public health emergency associated with high morbidity and mortality, leading to substantial emotional and financial burden for individuals, families, and communities (1)(2)(3)(4). Infants born very preterm (VP, before 32 weeks gestation) often require several months of hospitalization in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU). This is a time during which the preterm brain volume quadruples in size and is highly sensitive to both positive and negative environmental experiences (5). ...
... Short-term consequences may include poorer neurological reflexes and quality of movement, hypertonia, or hypotonia (13), suboptimal orientation, state regulation, and social engagement (14), and impaired ability to manage stress (12,15). Long-term, adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes of VP birth can range from more severe disabilities, including cerebral palsy; intellectual, language, or learning disability; hearing and vision impairments to high prevalence but low severity conditions such as developmental coordination disorders, fine motor deficits, and mild cognitive impairment (1)(2)(3)(4)16). Numerous factors including prenatal and perinatal events, neonatal morbidity, and postnatal exposures and sensory experiences are thought to be related to the neurobehavioral challenges experienced by VP children. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Children born very preterm (VP) remain at risk for long-term neurodevelopmental impairment. Patterns of brain growth and injury, and how early neuropromotive therapies might mitigate developmental risk in VP infants remain insufficiently understood. Methods This is a prospective cohort study of VP infants born at/before 32 weeks gestation. The study will enroll n = 75 consecutively-born VP infants in a level-III NICU. Exposed infants will be categorized into two groups (group 1: low-risk, n = 25 or group 2: high-risk, n = 25) based on the degree of neurological injury on early brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) at enrollment. Infants in the low-risk group (i.e., without significant injury defined as intraventricular hemorrhage with dilation, moderate or severe white matter injury, or cerebellar hemorrhage) will receive neurodevelopmental support utilizing the Supporting and Enhancing NICU Sensory Experiences (SENSE) program, while infants in the high-risk group (with neurological injury) will receive more intensive neurorehabilitative support (SENSE-plus). Age-specific, tailored sensory experiences will be facilitated contingently, preferentially by the infant's family with coaching from NICU staff. VP infants in exposure groups will undergo a brain MRI approximately every 2 weeks from enrollment until term-equivalent to monitor brain growth and evolution of injury. Exposed infants will be compared with a reference group (group 3: n = 25), i.e. VP infants whose families decline initial enrollment in SENSE, and subsequently undergo a term-equivalent brain MRI for other purposes. The primary aim of this study is characterization of term-equivalent brain growth and development among VP infants receiving NICU-based neuropromotive interventions compared to VP infants receiving the standard of care. Secondary aims include defining the timing and factors associated with total and regional brain growth on serial brain MRI among VP infants, (Aim 2), and using early imaging to tailor developmental intervention in the NICU while exploring associations with outcomes in VP infants at discharge and at two years corrected age (Aim 3). Discussion This study will address gaps in understanding patterns of brain growth and injury drawing on serial MRI of hospitalized VP infants. These data will also explore the impact of intensive, tailored neuropromotive support delivered prior to term-equivalent on child and family outcomes.
... Preterm infants are at high risk for short and long-term neurodevelopmental impairment in the neuro-motor, cognitive, language, and behavioral domains [1]. The estimated prevalence of cerebral palsy (CP) in preterm infants is around 15% in infants born at 22-27 weeks of gestation, and it decreases with increasing gestational age (GA) [2,3]. Language and speech delays occur in around 30% of very preterm infants examined at preschool age [3]. ...
... The estimated prevalence of cerebral palsy (CP) in preterm infants is around 15% in infants born at 22-27 weeks of gestation, and it decreases with increasing gestational age (GA) [2,3]. Language and speech delays occur in around 30% of very preterm infants examined at preschool age [3]. Other neurodevelopmental sequelae in children born preterm include cognitive delay, milder motor impairments (e.g., developmental coordination disorder (DCD)), hearing impairment, autism, and attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [4,5]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Currently, no local database in Israel collects neurodevelopmental outcomes of very low birth weight (VLBW) preterm infants. We investigated neurodevelopmental outcomes in one district of the largest healthcare organization in Israel. Methods: A cross-sectional study including all VLBW (<1500 g) preterm infants born between 1 January 2006 and 31 December 2016 who were followed in any of seven child development centers in Israel's Northern District. Data were retrospectively collected from the computerized medical record database. Results: Out of 436 participants, 55.1% had normal developmental outcomes. A total of 8.9% had cerebral palsy (CP), 12.2% had a global developmental delay (GDD), and 33.4% had a language delay. Out of the extremely preterm infants (n = 109), 20.2% had CP, 22.0% had GDD, and 44.9% had language delay. We found a statistically significant higher rate of abnormal neurodevelopment outcomes in non-Jews compared to Jews (57% vs. 37.8%, respectively, p < 0.0001). Conclusions: We found a relatively high overall rate of CP in our local population and a significant difference in neurodevelopmental outcomes between Jews and non-Jews. This study emphasizes the need for an expanded and detailed national database collecting post-discharge outcomes, as well as an assessment of national healthcare resource allocation and inequalities in preterm infants' post-discharge care.
... Specifically, preterm birth has been widely recognized as a condition that can negatively affect language development with cascading effects on later development in multiple domains, depending on neonatal immaturity, pre-, peri-and postnatal complications, neurological alterations, and the association with other biological and environmental risk factors (Sansavini, Guarini & Caselli, 2011). Several studies conducted on preterm infants have shown that the risk for language delay increases as the level of neonatal immaturity increases, so it is greater among extremely preterm infants (gestational age < 28 weeks) and very preterm infants (gestational age < 32 weeks) compared to full-term children (Barre et al., 2011;Guarini et al., 2016;Nguyen et al., 2018;Sansavini et al., 2010Sansavini et al., , 2014Sansavini et al., , 2015Woodward et al., 2009). By contrast, fewer and inconsistent findings are available for low-risk preterm children (i.e., those having a gestational age < 37 weeks and lack of major cerebral damage or severe perinatal complications; Pérez-Pereira, 2021; Sansavini, Zuccarini et al., 2021;Suttora et al., 2020Suttora et al., , 2022Zambrana et al., 2021;Zuccarini et al., 2023). ...
... It is known that very preterm children have an increased risk of neurodevelopmental outcomes, such cerebral palsy, cognitive delay and emotional/behavioural adjustment problems. 28 The CELF-P2 assessment tool is not able to screen for cognitive and motor impairments, as it is strictly a language assessment tool. Assessing other developmental domains at 4 years would provide further insight into these children and allow us to differentiate between a specific language impairment and a more serious issue with cognitive or executive functioning, or motor development. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Very preterm children are at increased risk of language delays. Concerns have been raised about the utility of standardised English language tools to diagnose language delay in linguistically diverse children. Our study investigated the incidence of language delay at 4 years in linguistically diverse very preterm children. Methods: Very preterm children born in South Western Sydney, Australia, between 2012 and 2016, were assessed with the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool-2 (CELF-P2) tool at 4 years of age. We sought to determine the incidence of language delay in this cohort using language scores from the CELF-P2 assessment tool, and explore potential predictors associated with language delay. Results: One hundred and sixty very preterm children attended the 4-year assessment out of the included 270 long-term survivors. At 4 years, 76 (52%) very preterm children had language delay diagnosed using the CELF-P2 assessment tool. Children who preferred a language other than English had lower average core language scores on the CELF-P2 assessment tool (75.1±14.4) compared with children that preferred English (86.5±17.9); p=0.002. Very preterm children growing up in households that preferenced a language other than English and those who were born from multiple births had higher odds of language delay at 4 years (AOR 10.30 (95% CI 2.82 to 38.28); p<0.001 and AOR 2.93 (95% CI 1.20 to 7.14); p=0.018, respectively). Assessing these children using an English language tool may have affected language scores at 4 years. Conclusions: In this metropolitan setting, very preterm children from linguistically diverse backgrounds were found to be vulnerable to language delays at 4 years. Further large-scale studies evaluating the language outcomes of linguistically diverse preterm children with more culturally appropriate tools are warranted. We question the utility of standardised English language tools to assess language outcomes of linguistically diverse populations.
Preprint
Full-text available
It has been widely assessed that very preterm children (< 32 weeks gestational age) present language and memory impairments compared to full-term children. However, differences in their underlying semantic memory structure have not been studied yet. Nevertheless, the way concepts are learned and organized across development relates to children’s capacities in retrieving and using information later. Therefore, the semantic memory organization could underlie several cognitive deficits existing in very preterm children. Computational mathematical models offer the possibility to characterize semantic networks through three coefficients; average shortest path length (i.e., distance between concepts), clustering (i.e., local interconnectivity), and modularity (i.e., vocabulary enrichment). Here we assessed these coefficients in 38 very preterm schoolchildren (aged 8–10 years) compared to 38 full-term schoolchildren (aged 7–10 years) based on a verbal fluency task. Using semantic network analysis, very preterm children showed a lower interconnectivity at a local level than full-term children. However, we found no differences between very preterm and full-term children regarding their average shortest path length between concepts and their modularity at a global level. These findings provide preliminary evidence that very preterm children demonstrate subtle impairments in the organization of their semantic network, encouraging the adaptation of the support and education they receive.
Article
Background The use of artificial intelligence (AI) for pain assessment has the potential to address historical challenges in infant pain assessment. There is a dearth of information on the perceived benefits and barriers to the implementation of AI for neonatal pain monitoring in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) from the perspective of health care professionals (HCPs) and parents. This qualitative analysis provides novel data obtained from 2 large tertiary care hospitals in Canada and the United Kingdom. Objective The aim of the study is to explore the perspectives of HCPs and parents regarding the use of AI for pain assessment in the NICU. Methods In total, 20 HCPs and 20 parents of preterm infants were recruited and consented to participate from February 2020 to October 2022 in interviews asking about AI use for pain assessment in the NICU, potential benefits of the technology, and potential barriers to use. Results The 40 participants included 20 HCPs (17 women and 3 men) with an average of 19.4 (SD 10.69) years of experience in the NICU and 20 parents (mean age 34.4, SD 5.42 years) of preterm infants who were on average 43 (SD 30.34) days old. Six themes from the perspective of HCPs were identified: regular use of technology in the NICU, concerns with regard to AI integration, the potential to improve patient care, requirements for implementation, AI as a tool for pain assessment, and ethical considerations. Seven parent themes included the potential for improved care, increased parental distress, support for parents regarding AI, the impact on parent engagement, the importance of human care, requirements for integration, and the desire for choice in its use. A consistent theme was the importance of AI as a tool to inform clinical decision-making and not replace it. Conclusions HCPs and parents expressed generally positive sentiments about the potential use of AI for pain assessment in the NICU, with HCPs highlighting important ethical considerations. This study identifies critical methodological and ethical perspectives from key stakeholders that should be noted by any team considering the creation and implementation of AI for pain monitoring in the NICU.
Article
Full-text available
Background Infants born very and extremely premature (V/EPT) are at a significantly elevated risk for neurodevelopmental disorders and delays even in the absence of structural brain injuries. These risks may be due to earlier-than-typical exposure to the extrauterine environment, and its bright lights, loud noises, and exposures to painful procedures. Given the relative underdeveloped pain modulatory responses in these infants, frequent pain exposures may confer risk for later deficits. Methods Resting-state fMRI scans were collected at term equivalent age from 148 (45% male) infants born V/EPT and 99 infants (56% male) born at term age. Functional connectivity analyses were performed between functional regions correlating connectivity to the number of painful skin break procedures in the NICU, including heel lances, venipunctures, and IV placements. Subsequently, preterm infants returned at 18 months, for neurodevelopmental follow-up and completed assessments for autism risk and general neurodevelopment. Results We observed that V/EPT infants exhibit pronounced hyperconnectivity within the cerebellum and between the cerebellum and both limbic and paralimbic regions correlating with the number of skin break procedures. Moreover, skin breaks were strongly associated with autism risk, motor, and language scores at 18 months. Subsample analyses revealed that the same cerebellar connections strongly correlating with breaks at term age were associated with language dysfunction at 18 months. Conclusions These results have significant implications for the clinical care of preterm infants undergoing painful exposures during routine NICU care, which typically occurs without anesthesia. Repeated pain exposures appear to have an increasingly detrimental effect on brain development during a critical period, and effects continue to be seen even 18 months later.
Article
Background and purpose: Very preterm infants (VPIs, <32 weeks gestational age at birth) are prone to long-term neurological deficits. While the effects of birth weight and postnatal growth on VPIs' neurological outcome are well established, the neurobiological mechanism behind these associations remains elusive. In this study, we utilized diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) to characterize how birth weight and postnatal weight gain influence VPIs' white matter (WM) maturation. Methods: We included VPIs with complete birth and postnatal weight data in their health record, and DTI scan as part of their predischarge Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI). We conducted voxel-wise general linear model and tract-based regression analyses to explore the impact of birth weight and postnatal weight gain on WM maturation. Results: We included 91 VPIs in our analysis. After controlling for gestational age at birth and time between birth and scan, higher birth weight Z-scores were associated with DTI markers of more mature WM tracts, most prominently in the corpus callosum and sagittal striatum. The postnatal weight Z-score changes over the first 4 weeks of life were also associated with increased maturity in these WM tracts, when controlling for gestational age at birth, birth weight Z-score, and time between birth and scan. Conclusions: In VPIs, birth weight and post-natal weight gain are associated with markers of brain WM maturation, particularly in the corpus callosum, which can be captured on discharge MRI. These neuroimaging metrics can serve as potential biomarkers for the early effects of nutritional interventions on VPIs' brain development.
Article
Full-text available
The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is a brief behavioral screening questionnaire that can be completed in 5 minutes by the parents or teachers of children aged 4 to 16; there is a self-report version for 11- to 16-year-olds. In this study, mothers completed the SDQ and the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) on 132 children aged 4 through 7 and drawn from psychiatric and dental clinics. Scores from the SDQ and CBCL were highly correlated and equally able to discriminate psychiatric from dental cases. As judged against a semistructured interview, the SDQ was significantly better than the CBCL at detecting inattention and hyperactivity, and at least as good at detecting internalizing and externalizing problems. Mothers of low-risk children were twice as likely to prefer the SDQ.
Article
Full-text available
To address the need for a standardized system to classify the gross motor function of children with cerebral palsy, the authors developed a five-level classification system analogous to the staging and grading systems used in medicine. Nominal group process and Delphi survey consensus methods were used to examine content validity and revise the classification system until consensus among 48 experts (physical therapists, occupational therapists, and developmental pediatricians with expertise in cerebral palsy) was achieved. Interrater reliability (kappa) was 0.55 for children less than 2 years of age and 0.75 for children 2 to 12 years of age. The classification system has application for clinical practice, research, teaching, and administration.
Article
Most previous studies of children with birthweight <750 g have focused on early childhood sequelae. To evaluate later outcomes, a regional sample of 60 <750-g birthweight children was compared at middle school age (M= 11 years) to 55 children with birthweight 750 – 1,499 g and 49 term controls. The groups were matched on age, gender, and demographic variables at the time of an early-school-age assessment (mean age 7 years). The <750-g birthweight group fared less well at middle school age than the term group on measures of cognitive function, achievement, behavior, and academic performance. In many instances, outcomes were less favorable for the <750-g children than for the 750 to 1,499-g group. Children in the <750-g group who were free of neurosensory disorders and global cognitive impairment performed more poorly on several tests than their term counterparts. Group differences in this subsample on tests of motor skills, math, and the ability to copy and recall a complex drawing remained significant even after controlling for IQ. Disparities between the <750-g and term groups increased with age for some measures. Despite favorable outcomes for many children in the <750-g group, this population is at risk for long-term developmental problems.
Article
Data from the standardization sample of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised (WPPSI-R) were used to develop a table that gives Full Scale IQs for a four-subtest abbreviated form of the scale. The short-form is composed of the Comprehension and Arithmetic subsets from the Verbal scale and the Picture Completion and Block Design subtests from the Performance scale. Reliability and validity coefficients and standard error of estimates for the abbreviated WPPSI-R IQs are reported, and guidelines for appropriate use of this scale are also discussed.
Article
This study describes the development of emotional and behavioral regulation in a regional cohort of children born extremely preterm (<28 weeks gestational age, n = 39), very preterm (<34 weeks gestational age, n = 56), and full term (n = 103). At 2 and 4 years, children born at younger gestational ages demonstrated poorer self-regulation across multiple contexts spanning observed interactions, formal cognitive testing, and parental report of child behavior at home. Among children born preterm, the 2 strongest predictors of impairments in self-regulation were the presence of moderate-to-severe cerebral white matter abnormalities on neonatal magnetic resonance and a less sensitive parenting style when children were aged 2 years. Findings support the importance of early neurological development and parenting for developing regulation in children born very preterm.
Article
The effects of relying on outmoded IQ-test norms and the use of arbitrary classifications of developmental delay on estimates of cognitive impairment of very preterm infants (VPI) was evaluated in a prospective population study. Cognitive assessments included the Griffiths test at 5 and 20 months and the Columbia Mental Maturity Scales (CMM) and a vocabulary test (Aktiver Wortschatz Test, AWST) at 56 months of age. Rates of cognitive impairment of 321 very preterm infants (VPI; < 32 weeks gestation or < 1500 g birth weight) were determined according to the published test norms, to scores of a full-term control group (FC n = 321), and to scores from a representative sample of children (NC n = 431) of the same birth cohort. IQ-scores were higher in the FC and NC children than in the original standardisation sample (SS). Using the concurrent test norms (FC, NC) up to 2.4 times more VPI were identified as seriously impaired (<-2 SD) than if the published (outdated) norms were used. Serious developmental delay was underestimated when arbitrary (e.g. DQ < 70) rather than across age comparable definitions (DQ <-2 SD) were used. VPI study drop-outs had mothers with lower educational qualifications and poorer cognitive developmental scores at 5 or 20 months of age. In conclusion, a lack of appropriate control groups and use of arbitrary criteria for judging serious delay leads to large underestimations of cognitive impairment in VPI. Findings from previous uncontrolled studies of VPI need re-interpretation.
Article
A novel behavioural screening questionnaire, the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), was administered along with Rutter questionnaires to parents and teachers of 403 children drawn from dental and psychiatric clinics. Scores derived from the SDQ and Rutter questionnaires were highly correlated; parent-teacher correlations for the two sets of measures were comparable or favoured the SDQ. The two sets of measures did not differ in their ability to discriminate between psychiatric and dental clinic attenders. These preliminary findings suggest that the SDQ functions as well as the Rutter questionnaires while offering the following additional advantages: a focus on strengths as well as difficulties; better coverage of inattention, peer relationships, and prosocial behaviour; a shorter format; and a single form suitable for both parents and teachers, perhaps thereby increasing parent-teacher correlations.
Article
To examine cognitive, behavioural, and educational outcomes in middle childhood among a birth cohort of very low birthweight children. Two hundred and ninety eight survivors from a national birth cohort of 413 New Zealand very low birthweight (VLBW) children born in 1986 were assessed at 7 to 8 years of age on measures of behaviour, cognitive ability, school performance and the need for special education. These outcomes were compared with the same measures in a general population sample of over 1000 children studied at a similar age. The VLBW children had significantly higher rates of problems and poorer levels of functioning across all outcome measures than the general child sample. These differences persisted even after control for variability in social, family, and other characteristics of the two samples and for the degree of sensorineural disability. There was evidence of a gradient of risk with birthweight, with extremely low birthweight children having generally higher rates of problems and difficulties than other VLBW children after covariate control. The findings are consistent with a growing body of research evidence which suggests that premature and VLBW infants are at increased risk of longer term morbidity and functional impairment in middle childhood.