ArticlePDF Available

Legitimacy Revisited: Disentangling Propriety, Validity, and Consensus

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Recent research has conceptualized legitimacy as a multi-level phenomenon comprising propriety and validity. Propriety refers to an individual evaluator’s belief that a legitimacy object is appropri-ate for its social context, whereas validity denotes an institutionalized, collective-level perception of appropriateness. In this article, we refine this multi-level understanding of legitimacy by adding a third, meso-level construct of “consensus”, which we define as the agreement between evaluators’ propriety beliefs. Importantly, validity and consensus are distinct and can be incongruent, given that an institutionalized perception can hide underlying disagreement. Disentangling validity from consensus is a crucial extension of the multi-level theory of legitimacy, because it enables an im-proved understanding of the legitimacy processes that precede sudden and unanticipated institu-tional change. In particular, while previous works considered revised propriety beliefs as the start-ing point for institutional change, our account emphasizes that the disclosure of the actual (vs. merely assumed) belief distribution within a social context may instigate institutional change. To study the interplay of propriety, validity, and consensus empirically, we propose a set of experimental designs specifically geared towards improving knowledge of the role of legitimacy and its components in institutional change.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Legitimacy Revisited:
Disentangling Propriety, Validity, and Consensus
Patrick Haack (Corresponding author)
University of Lausanne
patrick.haack@unil.ch
Oliver Schilke
University of Arizona
oschilke@arizona.edu
Lynne Zucker
University of California, Los Angeles
zucker@ucla.edu
Lausanne, July 15, 2020
Paper accepted for publication in the Journal of Management Studies
Acknowledgements: We thank handling editor Dries Faems and three anonymous reviewers for
their guidance and developmental feedback during the review process. We received valuable
comments and suggestions from Anna Jasinenko and William McKinley. We are grateful for the
helpful feedback by participants at the 2016 Sociological Association Annual Meeting in Seattle
and the 2017 EGOS Colloquium in Copenhagen. Derek Harmon ideated the storytelling design
during a visit in Lausanne, and important aspects of the consensus constructs were developed in
ongoing empirical research with Jost Sieweke and Mike Pfarrer.
2
Legitimacy Revisited:
Disentangling Propriety, Validity, and Consensus
Abstract
Recent research has conceptualized legitimacy as a multi-level phenomenon comprising proprie-
ty and validity. Propriety refers to an individual evaluator’s belief that a legitimacy object is
appropriate for its social context, whereas validity denotes an institutionalized, collective-level
perception of appropriateness. In this article, we refine this multi-level understanding of legiti-
macy by adding a third, meso-level construct of “consensus”, which we define as the agreement
between evaluators’ propriety beliefs. Importantly, validity and consensus are distinct and can
be incongruent, given that an institutionalized perception can hide underlying disagreement.
Disentangling validity from consensus is a crucial extension of the multi-level theory of legiti-
macy, because it enables an improved understanding of the legitimacy processes that precede
sudden and unanticipated institutional change. In particular, while previous works considered
revised propriety beliefs as the starting point for institutional change, our account emphasizes
that the disclosure of the actual (vs. merely assumed) belief distribution within a social context
may instigate institutional change. To study the interplay of propriety, validity, and consensus
empirically, we propose a set of experimental designs specifically geared towards improving
knowledge of the role of legitimacy and its components in institutional change.
Keywords: consensus, experiments, institutional change, microfoundations, legitimacy, proprie-
ty, validity
3
INTRODUCTION
The burgeoning multi-level view of legitimacy seeks to reconcile the perspective that le-
gitimacy may reside at the collective level “independent from particular observers” (Suchman,
1995, p. 576) with the view that legitimacy involves the perceptions of individual evaluators
(Tost, 2011). Drawing on a distinction that was introduced more than 100 years ago by Weber
(1978[1918]), Bitektine and Haack (2015) advanced a multi-level theory of legitimacy that dif-
ferentiates between “propriety” and “validity” as the individual- and collective-level judgments
made in the legitimacy process, respectively. Following these theoretical advancements, empiri-
cal research has started to investigate changes in propriety beliefs (Finch et al., 2015; Jahn et al.,
2020) and to describe the promotion of propriety and validity through communication (Gauthier
and Kappen, 2017). In addition, scholars have examined cross-level interactions between propri-
ety and validity (Haack and Sieweke, 2018) and elaborated the effect of these interactions on
organizational change (Huy et al., 2014), practice implementation (Jacqueminet and Durand,
2019), and institutionalization (Arshed et al., 2019).
Recognizing that legitimacy results from a process of social judgment formation that en-
tails multiple levels has been a pivotal step in extending and revising insights from previous
conceptualizations of organizational legitimacy (Deephouse et al., 2017; Suddaby et al., 2017).
However, despite all the efforts that have been devoted to the study of legitimacy, the longstand-
ing practice of equating legitimacy with collective agreement has impaired scholars’ capacity to
theorize the precise legitimacy processes that precede institutional change, and has stalled intel-
lectual progress in this core area of organizational research. While extant research has often im-
plied that institutional change is initiated through intra-individual change in propriety beliefs
(e.g., Greenwood et al., 2002; Tost, 2011), we theorize that institutional change can also be in-
stigated when individual evaluators discern the distribution of others’ propriety beliefs in a ref-
erence group. Importantly, sudden disclosure of the extent to which an apparently valid legiti-
macy object is agreed upon in terms of propriety beliefs can have drastic institutional conse-
4
quences. In some instances, validity may involve low agreement or even significant disagree-
ment in propriety beliefs, making institutional stability inherently fragile (Bitektine and Haack,
2015; Suddaby et al., 2017). In other instances, an institutional arrangement may lack validity
even though a silent majority of evaluators deems it proper, suggesting that an alternative insti-
tutional order may enjoy greater approval and potential support than commonly assumed.
Hence, there is a clear need to theorize and study the scope and nature of the (potential) incon-
gruity between validity and the distribution of propriety beliefs.
We contribute to legitimacy research by addressing this major gap in two ways. First, we
advance the largely unexplored meso-level legitimacy component of “consensus”, which forms
the missing link connecting the micro- and the macro-levels of legitimacy. Whereas validity
denotes an institutionalized, collective-level perception of appropriateness and a status of supra-
individual exteriority illustrating “how things are,” consensus represents a meso-level construct
that denotes the degree to which evaluators in a given social collective agree that a legitimacy
object is proper. Although consensus and validity overlap in the sense that an institutionalized
perception may reflect agreement with respect to propriety beliefs, they are not the same, given
that an institutionalized perception can hide underlying disagreement. That is, while a legitimacy
object can be valid at the collective level, the actual consensus among the individuals within that
collective can be either high (i.e., virtually all evaluators agree that a legitimacy object is proper)
or low (i.e., a considerable share of evaluators disagrees that the legitimacy object is proper,
meaning that propriety beliefs are heterogeneously distributed). Adding the consensus construct
to legitimacy theory clarifies that seemingly widely endorsed institutional arrangements may
mask private dissent, which—when revealed—may lead individual evaluators to change their
perceptions and behaviors and to support institutional change efforts. Conversely, marginalized
institutional arrangements may appear invalid even though many evaluators consider them prop-
er in private, resulting in low validity and high consensus. We expect institutional change to
become likely whenever such an incongruity between validity and consensus is identified and
5
publicly revealed. Thus, introducing the concept of consensus has the potential to significantly
advance our understanding of the legitimacy processes that precede sudden and unanticipated
institutional change.
Second, theoretical and methodological choices necessarily co-evolve, such that theory
cannot progress in the absence of adequate methods for testing and validating the relationships
between its core concepts (Haack et al., 2020a). As such, theoretical progress requires advance-
ments in empirical research and methodology. Accordingly, in order to advance empirical re-
search on legitimacy as a multi-level process, we propose a set of experimental designs that are
geared specifically toward the study of the intricate interactions between propriety, validity, and
consensus, thus allowing for testing our proposed theory. Experimental designs can enable
scholars to explore relationships “that are often suggested but difficult to isolate in contextually
rich field studies” (David and Bitektine, 2009, p. 171), and they can be particularly useful for
differentiating between the distinct perceptual components of legitimacy. Recognizing the “val-
ue of method in advancing theory” (Greenwald, 2012, p. 106), our paper suggests that bringing
in experiments can help to significantly advance or even reorient legitimacy research in institu-
tional theory, in particular with respect to the underlying dynamics and legitimacy processes of
institutional change.
THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
Research on legitimacy, commonly defined as a “generalized perception or assumption
that the actions of an entity are desirable, proper, or appropriate” (Suchman, 1995, p. 574), has
become increasingly prominent in management and organization studies—and, in particular, in
institutional theory (Harmon et al., 2015; Suddaby et al., 2017)—to the extent that legitimacy
has been described as “perhaps the most central concept in institutional research” (Colyvas and
Powell, 2006, p. 308). The significant scholarly attention devoted to the topic is evidenced by
the increasing flow of papers devoted to the analysis of legitimacy (e.g., Castello et al., 2016;
Etter et al., 2018; Haack et al., 2014; Fisher et al., 2016; Hengst et al., 2020; Huy et al., 2014;
6
Jacqueminet and Durand, 2019; Lewis et al., 2020; Scherer et al., 2013). This line of research
stresses that through legitimacy, organizations acquire ideational and material support (e.g., ac-
cess to financial and human resources) and increase their prominence and influence, all of which
are fundamental to their growth and survival (Zimmerman and Zeitz, 2002).
The notion of legitimacy has been applied to a plethora of theoretical and empirical con-
texts, adding considerable surplus meaning to the construct (Suddaby et al., 2017). Scholars
have often relied on Suchman’s (1995) rather broad definition of legitimacy, without specifying
what particular aspect of legitimacy they are examining (Johnson et al., 2006). As a result, legit-
imacy research in institutional theory is currently entering a formative phase, characterized by
attempts to bring greater construct clarity and theoretical coherence to the subject area
(Deephouse et al., 2017). Recent works have specified different perspectives in legitimacy re-
search (Suddaby et al., 2017) and have distinguished legitimacy from related constructs such as
reputation and status (Bitektine, 2011; Bitektine et al., 2020; Washington and Zajac, 2005) as
well as stigma (Devers et al., 2009; Helms et al., 2019). Whereas institutional theory has focused
mainly on the legitimacy process at the collective level (Zelditch, 2004) and maintained that an
object’s legitimacy depends on the approval of a social group as a whole, the notion of legitima-
cy as a “generalized perception” (Suchman, 1995, p. 574) ultimately derives from the coales-
cence of the perceptions and judgments of individual evaluators (Tost, 2011; Zimmerman and
Zeitz, 2002). Hence, it is important to understand legitimacy judgment formation and change at
the individual level while simultaneously achieving greater clarity and theoretical coherence
about legitimacy’s inherent multi-level nature.
Drawing on the work of Weber (1978[1918]) and subsequent research in sociology (Dorn-
busch and Scott, 1975; Zelditch, 2001), Johnson and colleagues (2006) highlighted the dual na-
ture of legitimacy and re-conceptualized its formation as an intricate social process consisting of
validity, a collective-level perception of appropriateness, and propriety, an individual evaluator’s
belief that a legitimacy object is appropriate for its social context. In this view, legitimacy occurs
7
“simultaneously at the collective level (where perceptions of appropriateness are created, shared,
and validated) and at the level of the individual, who uses collective perceptions to derive his or
her judgment and engages in appropriate action” (Haack and Sieweke, 2018, p. 487). Bitektine
and Haack (2015) advanced the multi-level view of legitimacy and developed a theory of how
macro-level validity shapes or even determines micro-level judgments and behaviors and how
these judgments and behaviours coalesce to constitute validity. They highlighted that
institutional stability at the macro level can be based on “silenced” micro-level heterogeneity in
propriety beliefs, as individual evaluators may choose not to disclose their propriety beliefs for
fear of social sanctions, because they (sometimes erroneously) perceive their beliefs represent a
minority opinion. It is only after the removal of factors conducive to silencing that individual
evaluators may disclose their propriety beliefs and start to actively challenge the status quo.
Similarly, Tolbert and Darabi (2020) pointed out that different kinds of institutional pressures
may generate variations in motives for conformity; they illuminated how the explicit recognition
of these motives can explain observed heterogeneity in individual and organizational behavior.
These works suggest that to explain institutional change, it is crucial to understand the
antecedents and underlying dynamics of multi-level legitimacy formation, particularly with
respect to the interaction between propriety and validity.
Although the validity-propriety distinction is relatively established in the field of sociolo-
gy (e.g., Johnson et al., 2006), much legitimacy research in institutional theory, and more gener-
ally in organization and management studies, has ignored the propriety concept and focused
exclusively on validity as the collective-level conceptualization of legitimacy (Zelditch, 2004).
While most studies do not even make the focal level of analysis explicit, some of them confuse
propriety with validity. For instance, some authors imply a collective-level understanding of
legitimacy by referring to Suchman’s (1995) definition of legitimacy as a “generalized percep-
tion,” even though they are actually concerned with the analysis of individual judgments of le-
gitimacy (e.g., Brown and Toyoki, 2013; Foreman and Whetten, 2002; Higgins and Gulati,
8
2006). Suchman’s (1995) influential tripartite conception of legitimacy (cognitive, moral, and
pragmatic) itself implies a conflation of propriety and validity components, as pragmatic legiti-
macy is said to rest on the “self-interested calculations” of individual actors (p. 578), suggesting
that legitimacy is not “independent of particular observers” (p. 576). This conceptual ambiguity,
which characterizes much of the extant research on legitimacy, has made it rather difficult to
integrate empirical findings coherently and hindered progress in this important area of research.
Conflating propriety with validity is highly problematic, because these two constructs constitute
two fundamentally different components of legitimacy and are likely to have distinct anteced-
ents and behavioral consequences. Thus, failing to clearly distinguish between these two com-
ponents of legitimacy has introduced considerable concept ambiguity and impeded the ability of
legitimacy researchers to advance scholarly understandings on legitimacy as an explanandum in
its own right (Suddaby et al., 2017).
In this paper, we seek to bring clarity to existing legitimacy research in institutional theory
and further advance the understanding of legitimacy as a multi-level phenomenon. While the
model of Bitektine and Haack (2015) has drawn attention to the complex interactions between
micro and macro levels of legitimacy, their framework remains ambiguous, as it has equated the
institutionalized part of legitimacy with common agreement, defining validity as “consensus
opinion shared by others” (p. 51; emphasis added). Hence, although Bitektine and Haack (2015)
pointed to the possibility that validity may conceal heterogeneity in propriety beliefs, their re-
search has not conceptually disentangled validity from common agreement.
We address this important gap by introducing a missing link—that of consensus, a meso-
level construct that describes the degree to which evaluators within a social group agree that a
given legitimacy object is proper. We elaborate that consensus and validity are not the same,
given that a taken-for-granted perception of validity may conceal dissent among individual
evaluators, and we theorize that trustful communication and social interaction among evaluators
plays a key role in revealing the actual (vs. apparent) consensus within a reference group. In
9
addition, we explicate why the distinction between validity and consensus constitutes a crucial
extension of the multi-level theory of legitimacy and elaborate why theorizing and testing the
relationship between the legitimacy componentsi is essential for advancing scholarly understand-
ing of the legitimacy processes preceding sudden and unanticipated institutional change
(Clemente and Roulet, 2015; Oliver, 1992). In the following sections, we will first examine each
of the three legitimacy components and then develop theory on their complex interactions. We
will focus particularly on the role of consensus in these interactions and in related legitimacy
and institutional change processes.
Propriety: Legitimacy at the Individual Level
The individual level of legitimacy has so far received only scant attention in institutional
theory and organization and management studies. Zelditch (2004) even went so far as to suggest
that institutional theory has no concept of legitimacy at the individual level. Consequently, rela-
tively little is known about how the individuals within a specific evaluator group assess the pro-
priety of organizations, practices, structures, and other types of legitimacy objects and, by exten-
sion, how these individuals construe legitimacy. Indeed, before the conceptual contributions by
Tost (2011) and Bitektine and Haack (2015), virtually no attempt had been made by institution-
alists to theorize or examine the formation and change of propriety.
Propriety is the individual-level component of legitimacy and refers to an individual eval-
uator’s endorsement of a legitimacy object or the evaluator’s belief that the essence, qualities, or
actions of that object are appropriate for its social context (Dornbusch and Scott, 1975; Tost,
2011). The term “legitimacy object” denotes the focal entity that is being assessed, such as an
organization, practice, or procedure (Johnson, 2004). Evaluators typically form judgments about
an object’s propriety in a “passive” mode, where they operate as “intuiters” who quasi-
automatically draw on judgment heuristics (Haack et al., 2014). Intuiters use heuristics such as
the judgments and actions of others (Rao et al., 2001; Rossman and Schilke, 2014) and draw on
associations, analogies, or similarities between one object and another already legitimate object
10
(Baum and Oliver, 1991; Stuart et al., 1999). Using such cues allows evaluators to reach a
judgment quickly and effortlessly (Kahneman and Frederick, 2002). However, under conditions
of competing or ambiguous institutional prescriptions (Greenwood et al., 2011) or in the after-
math of an exogenous shock (Greenwood et al., 2002; Tost, 2011), evaluators may also render a
propriety belief in a more “active” mode, in which they deliberately and carefully assess the
legitimacy object on the basis of specific standards and principles (Huy et al., 2014; Lamin and
Zaheer, 2012).
Propriety beliefs have important behavioral consequences. If the prescriptions of specific
judgment standards and principles are met, evaluators deem the object proper, view it as a desir-
able model of action, and ultimately support the legitimacy object (Walker, 2014). In contrast, if
the object is deemed not consistent with standards and principles, evaluators do not grant propri-
ety and may withhold support (potentially contingent on their perceptions of validity; see be-
low). In cases of severe transgressions, such as organizational wrongdoing or scandal (Greve et
al., 2010), evaluators will disapprove of the object and deem it improper (Hudson, 2008; Devers
et al., 2009), with behavioral reactions ranging from avoiding (Hudson and Okhuysen, 2009) or
resisting the legitimacy object (Huy et al., 2014) to openly condemning it (Maguire and Hardy,
2009).
Validity: Legitimacy at the Collective Level
Validity is the institutionalized, collective-level component of legitimacy and describes the
acceptability of an object at the collective level or the “generalized perception” that the nature or
activities of a legitimacy object are appropriate for its social context (Suchman, 1995). Once
formed, validity “has a life of its own” (Zelditch, 2006, p. 346), meaning it exists objectively as
a social fact and is largely independent of the subjective propriety beliefs of single evaluators.
The “facticity” of valid objects signals to evaluators how things are, creating the perception of a
natural, inevitable, and collectively approved order (Berger and Luckmann, 1967). Although an
inherently collective-level construct, validity as the objectified part of legitimacy also affects the
11
appraisal process of individual evaluators and enter evaluators’ cognition in the form of a “valid-
ity cue,” which they can use to form a subjective validity belief (Tost, 2011). Such a validity
belief represents an individual’s judgment that a legitimacy object is perceived as appropriate by
a collectivity of evaluators. Evaluators’ perceptions of validity (i.e., validity beliefs) are bol-
stered (1) by authorization, which means that an authority (e.g., an expert or high-status actor)
has acknowledged a given object as legitimate and (2) by endorsement, which means that an
evaluator’s peers have accepted the object’s legitimacy (Dornbusch and Scott, 1975). Indeed,
approval by “judgment validation institutions” such as the media, government, and the judicial
system (Bitektine and Haack, 2015, p. 51) and “voices of the common man” (Vaara, 2014, p.
506) constitute major sources of validity. It follows that the views and assessments of others
play an important role in the construction of validity (Johnson, 2004). In addition, the absence or
lacking visibility of contrary activities constitutes a subtle but equally powerful source of validi-
ty (Haack and Sieweke, 2018; Tost, 2011).
Validity, in turn, has important implications for evaluators’ propriety beliefs and their be-
havior and thus ultimately for the stability of social systems and institutions. Given that alterna-
tives are “literally unthinkable” in taken-for-granted contexts (Zucker, 1983, p. 5), the prescrip-
tions that derive from a valid object are binding. When an object is valid, individual evaluators
do not question it; they simply comply (Johnson, 2004). While evaluators consider proper ob-
jects desirable and thus follow prescriptions voluntarily and sometimes enthusiastically, compli-
ance with valid, institutionalized objects is based on social obligation and control (Johnson,
2004; Johnson et al., 2006). The coercive power of validity has been demonstrated by research
finding that validity attenuates negative emotions in response to injustice, and as a result it helps
sustain stratified order (Johnson et al., 2016). As mentioned above, in cases where validity con-
flicts with propriety, evaluators may choose to conceal their propriety beliefs because they fear
social sanctions and a loss of social approval (Bitektine and Haack, 2015), to the extent that they
may even actively enforce the support of a valid legitimacy object they privately consider im-
12
proper (Centola et al., 2005; Willer et al., 2009). By contrast, invalidity reduces people’s com-
pliance and support, even if evaluators, in private, consider a legitimacy object to be proper
(Massey et al., 1997).
Validity may also affect compliance indirectly through its effect on propriety. Works
based on the status theory of legitimacy suggest that referential structures (which represent legit-
imacy at the collective level) enable evaluators to develop their propriety beliefs (Berger et al.,
1998; Ridgeway and Berger, 1986). Other research has shown that evaluators are more likely to
judge a legitimacy object as proper that is (perceived as) valid (Walker et al., 1988; Yoon and
Thye, 2011), even if they initially deemed it improper (Hegtvedt and Johnson, 2000; Johnson et
al., 2000). For instance, evaluators have been found to gradually adapt their propriety beliefs
regarding inequality to the perceived validity of economic inequality (Haack and Sieweke,
2018). Validity thus has a bolstering effect on propriety and a “canceling effect” on impropriety
(Zelditch, 2001, p. 44). Conversely, when a legitimacy object lacks validity, evaluators may
begin to question the object and are more likely to disapprove of it personally.
Consensus: The Missing Link in Legitimacy Theoryii
Extending the legitimacy dichotomy of propriety and validity, we suggest that a meso-
level component is needed to bridge the micro and the macro of legitimacy—that of consensus.
While consensus has not yet been systematically elaborated in institutional theory, several
scholars have hinted at it in passing, noting that it constitutes an important element or outcome
of the legitimacy determination process. For instance, when describing the “legitimacy-as-
process” perspective, Suddaby, Bitektine, and Haack (2017, p. 452) suggest that legitimacy “is
understood to occur as the product of consensus between multiple actors in a social field”. Other
works have described legitimacy maintenance as “dynamics of agreement and disagreement”
(Patriotta et al., 2011, p. 1808) and suggested that legitimacy depends on evaluators’ consensus
about what features and actions a legitimacy object should have in order to be accepted in a spe-
13
cific context (Cattani et al., 2008). Similarly, Drori and Honig (2013, p. 371) proposed that legit-
imacy evolves and becomes validated by a “general consensus in society”.
Notwithstanding the potential importance of consensus for legitimacy theory, much ambi-
guity exists with respect to the construct of consensus, or “what people are actually agreeing on”
(Markóczy, 2001, p. 1014). Previous works (including our own) have regularly conflated con-
sensus with validity, often implying that consensual agreement and validity are literally the same
and treating these concepts quasi-synonymously. However, we propose that this practice is mis-
guided and that scholars are better served in treating these two components of legitimacy as ana-
lytically distinct. As discussed above, validity may lead individuals to hide their propriety be-
liefs; thus, a valid legitimacy object does not necessarily reflect agreement but can in fact be
subject to significant (yet concealed) disagreement. Although we acknowledge that validity may
inform propriety beliefs (as discussed above), a notable portion of evaluators may resist validity
pressures. Hence, we challenge the assumption, often made implicitly, that consensus and validi-
ty are virtually the same; we develop a refined understanding of consensus and disentangle it
from validity. In particular, the specification of consensus and validity as related but distinct
elements in the multi-level legitimacy concept offers an important correction to the model by
Bitektine and Haack (2015) and has the potential to proffer significant advancements in scholar-
ly understandings of the legitimacy processes preceding institutional change. Specifically, and as
elaborated below, disentangling validity from consensus supports the critical but largely over-
looked insight that institutional arrangements that are considered valid may in fact be inherently
fragile, facing contestation and sudden decline once the existence of substantial disagreement is
revealed (Patriotta et al., 2011).
We define consensus as the degree to which individual members of a reference group
(e.g., team, organization, industry, field, or society at large) agree that the essence, features, or
activities of a legitimacy object are proper for a given social context. High consensus indicates
that a propriety belief is shared by most evaluators within a reference group, whereas low con-
14
sensus indicates that propriety is contested (e.g., Pfarrer et al., 2008; Bundy and Pfarrer, 2015).iii
Hence, despite the existence of a “generalized perception” that the nature or activities of a legit-
imacy object are appropriate for its social context and are thus valid, in the sense of being exte-
rior to the subjective experience of individual evaluators (Berger and Luckmann, 1967; Such-
man, 1995), substantial heterogeneity in propriety beliefs may nonetheless exist, as a nontrivial
number of evaluators may privately oppose the object. It follows that a legitimacy object’s valid-
ity can be associated with very different, yet often concealed, distributions of propriety beliefs.
As Figure 1 illustrates, a valid legitimacy object may display a normal distribution of propriety
beliefs (panel 1.1), a distribution of greater or lower variation in propriety beliefs (panels 1.2 and
1.3), a left or right skewed distribution (panels 1.4 and 1.5), or even a bimodal distribution re-
flecting a polarization in evaluators’ propriety beliefs (panel 1.6). Many other distributions are
conceivable.
––FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE––
If silencing is present or communication through which evaluators can reveal their proprie-
ty beliefs is lacking, the true nature of the belief distribution will be masked by perceptions of
validity. In such contexts, evaluators have no, or only partial, information about the distribution
of propriety beliefs in their reference group. Evaluators may then openly support the legitimacy
object on the basis of their validity beliefs, and the absence of public challenge contributes fur-
ther to the object’s stability (Correll et al., 2017). While the exact belief distribution frequently
is unknown, the gradual or sudden disclosure of the degree to which a legitimacy object is con-
sensually approved may have drastic institutional consequences, depending on the size of the
congruity between validity and consensus. By “congruity,” we mean the degree to which con-
sensus maps onto validity. For instance, a valid legitimacy object can be matched by substantial
agreement in propriety beliefs (high validity-consensus congruity) or it could be associated with
heterogeneity in propriety beliefs (low validity-consensus congruity, or high incongruity). Alt-
hough an incongruity between validity and consensus may have been concealed, it looms in the
15
background and may erupt when being problematized, exposing the fissures of a seemingly sta-
ble institutional order. As such, it is important to analyze the distribution of propriety beliefs
about a given legitimacy object (consensus) and contrast this analysis with the (perceived) valid-
ity of that object.
The distinction between validity and consensus clarifies that wide consent is not a neces-
sary condition for validity. Even if evaluators do not endorse an object individually, this object
can still be considered valid if these evaluators falsely assume that others in their reference
group believe the object is proper, thus granting the object validity (Correll et al., 2017).
Suddaby and colleagues (2017) posited that, in extreme cases of collective misperceptions, legit-
imacy objects may remain valid even though not a single evaluator privately endorses the object.
In contrast, when a majority of evaluators privately hold favorable propriety beliefs about an
object (high consensus), this object can still lack validity if evaluators falsely assume that most
of the evaluators in their reference group hold unfavorable propriety beliefs and accordingly fail
to disclose their own beliefs (Zelditch and Walker, 2003; Suddaby et al., 2017). The validity of a
legitimacy object can thus exist and guide behavior even in the presence of incongruent consen-
sus—especially, as we shall elaborate next, when network ties among the members of a group
are weak, which makes it difficult for evaluators to communicate trustfully and detect the actual
or “true” propriety beliefs of the other members of a given reference group (Canales, 2016;
Westphal and Bednar, 2005; Zhu and Westphal, 2011). The following sections first explicate the
conditions that enable evaluators to discern that validity may not be congruent with actual con-
sensus and then theorize the legitimacy processes that unfold once evaluators discover this in-
congruity.
Figure 2 represents a schematic illustration of the key arguments we develop below. While
we mostly illustrate relevant dynamics in the context of delegitimation, our argument could be
laterally reversed to describe a legitimation process. That is, our model may also be applied to
explain the dynamics that contribute to strengthening the legitimacy of emerging and not yet
16
fully established institutional arrangements—that is, alternative “sociocultural constructions that
prescribe appropriate organizational behaviors and that shape and enforce patterns of interests
and privilege” (Micelotta et al., 2017, p. 1886). Indeed, delegitimation of the status quo is fre-
quently associated with the legitimation of innovation and novelty (Oliver, 1992); while the va-
lidity of an established legitimacy object may gradually erode, the validity of another not yet
taken-for-granted legitimacy object may grow.iv
––FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE––
WHY CONSENSUS MATTERS
Our proposed account emphasizes that consensus does not necessarily depend on, and is
conceptually distinct from, validity. The implications of the distinction between validity and
consensus are profound. Acknowledging that validity and consensus are not the same implies
that the persistence and stability of valid legitimacy objects—including status orders in organi-
zations (e.g., Berger et al., 1998; Correll et al., 2017), widely diffused yet unpopular norms (e.g.,
Prentice and Miller, 1993; Willer et al., 2009), and systems of social or political dominance
(e.g., Kuran, 1995)—can be solely based on an “illusion of support” and are thus inherently
fragile (Centola et al., 2005, p. 1010; Rao et al., 2001). As (the appearance of) consensus breaks
down, validity loses its influence (Asch, 1951). If consensus for a valid legitimacy object is
merely presumed but not real, then the disclosure that a valid object is in fact contested may
initiate a process of problematization, delegitimation, and deinstitutionalization, opening up op-
portunities for the legitimation of a new institutional order and thus institutional change (Green-
wood et al., 2002). Conversely, an “illusion of resistance” may prevail for those legitimacy ob-
jects that are seemingly contested or opposed but actually enjoy substantial private endorsement,
which—when publicly revealed—may trigger a process of legitimation.
Revealing the Actual Degree of Consensus
What conditions enable evaluators to identify the degree to which a valid legitimacy object
is actually consensually approved? Although several circumstances might play a role, we high-
17
light the critical importance of (1) communication ties and (2) collective action in helping evalu-
ators discern the actual degree of consensus in their reference group, i.e., whether most individ-
uals endorse or have concerns about a valid legitimacy object or whether there is significant dis-
agreement about the acceptability of the object. As illustrated in Figure 2 (T0), in the absence of
communication ties and interaction, evaluators may silence propriety beliefs which they consid-
er to be inconsistent with validity. For instance, research has found that investment analysts tend
not to disclose their reservations regarding the adoption of stock repurchase plans (a valid legit-
imacy object in this context) because they underestimate the extent to which other analysts share
their concerns—a phenomenon that is less pronounced when analysts communicate frequently
(Zhu and Westphal, 2011). Similarly, Westphal and Bednar (2005) suggested that managers are
less likely to silence their unfavorable propriety beliefs regarding a company’s strategy when
they have opportunities to casually interact and communicate with other managers. Research by
Canales (2016) indicated that informal dyadic communication and settings where evaluators can
speak “off the record” are particularly effective in promoting the disclosure of silenced propriety
beliefs, whereas communication in larger group settings, such as public forums, is found to con-
solidate the (perceived) validity of a legitimacy object. In addition, research on trust has shown
that as individuals interact with each other, they develop interpersonal trust and become increas-
ingly willing to engage in risk taking (Cook et al., 2009; Cook et al., 2005). Such risk taking can
take the form of revealing personal propriety beliefs to one’s counterparts, such that evaluators
may become aware of the level of consensus about a particular legitimacy object. All of these
insights suggest that evaluators who previously silenced their propriety beliefs may start to voice
their concerns and participate in public debates and deliberations. As illustrated in Figure 2 (T1
and T2), in such contexts evaluators increasingly discern that validity is contested; as a result, the
support for an established legitimacy object or institutional arrangement is gradually eroded
(Johnson et al., 2006; Oliver, 1992).
18
However, not all opportunities for communication are equally effective in triggering this
process, as distinct streams of research suggest that greater connectivity and repeated social in-
teraction do not automatically contribute to the disclosure of a potential incongruity between
validity and consensus. For instance, Canales (2016) offered anecdotal evidence that interaction
in public forums may not reveal but instead actually perpetuate silencing and thus harden incon-
gruity between validity and consensus. Further, small group research has theorized that con-
formity pressures can lead to “groupthink,” a psychological phenomenon that has been found to
promote poor group decisions (Janis, 1972). Groupthink is likely to occur under specific condi-
tions, such as high group cohesiveness, directive leadership, homogeneity of group members’
social background, lack of methodological procedures, high stress, and group members’ low
self-esteem (for a review, see Esser, 1998). These conditions hamper the expression of multiple
or contrary viewpoints and the evaluation of viable alternatives. While group decisions are often
unanimously accepted as valid and collectively approved, they may mask significant dissent
among group members’ preferences. From this research, it follows that creating favorable condi-
tions for the expression of dissent can help reduce incongruity between validity and consensus.
Hence, “genuine” communication reveals actual consensus while “conformist” communication
will only stabilize false impressions of unanimous support; and encouraging critique and institu-
tionalizing a culture of speaking up through the implementation of whistleblowing and om-
budsman policies will likely help reveal incongruity. Halbesleben and colleagues (2007) con-
cluded that the risk of silencing can be reduced when evaluators are systematically exposed to
the true beliefs of other evaluators; for example, through a culture of open communication where
conflict is encouraged, not suppressed, and where evaluators are emboldened to disagree with
the majority opinion and openly voice their concerns (Brodbeck et al., 2007; Priem et al., 1995).
The disclosure of incongruity between validity and consensus can also be facilitated
through the collective action of silenced dissenters who have a predisposition to speak up and
engage in institutional change efforts. Research has found that a previously silenced yet une-
19
quivocally committed minority of actors can tip a reference group into a new state, in which
other group members update their validity beliefs and adapt their propriety beliefs as a result
(Centola et al., 2018). Importantly, this dynamic can facilitate the abandonment of socially
harmful yet deeply institutionalized practices, such as female genital mutilation (Efferson et al.,
2020), foot binding (Mackie, 1996), and smoking (Christakis and Fowler, 2008). The knowledge
that a minority of silenced dissenters can instigate institutional change can also be used strategi-
cally, which is lucidly exemplified by Canales (2016): Only when a government agency identi-
fied silenced dissenters and coordinated informal meetings was the apparent (but false) support
for the status quo revealed and eventually disrupted. In this view, successful collective action in
the form of institutional entrepreneurship or institutional work (Lawrence and Suddaby, 2006)
does not necessarily require the creation and dissemination of novel viewpoints or a fundamental
shift in prevalent frames, narratives, or discourses. Rather, change can be prompted by reveal-
ing, organizing, and bringing to the fore hidden or suppressed consent, thereby objectifying the
desirability and feasibility of an alternative status quo (Berger and Luckmann, 1967).
Implications for Theories of Institutional Change
The distinction between validity and consensus highlights that institutional change does
not necessarily need to begin with evaluators changing their individual propriety beliefs, as pre-
viously theorized by Tost (2011) and implied by several influential works that have mobilized
the concept of legitimation/delegitimation to explain institutional change (e.g., Greenwood et al.,
2002; Maguire and Hardy, 2009; Suddaby and Greenwood, 2005). Rather, institutional change
may be instigated by evaluators interacting with other evaluators to update their perceptions of
consensus and eventually change their behavior on that basis. That is, because validity as an
institutionalized perception is “not uniformly taken for granted” (Dacin et al., 2002, p. 45; em-
phasis added), de/legitimation may occur when evaluators develop a more accurate assessment
regarding the distribution of propriety beliefs among other evaluators in their reference group
(see Figure 1). If evaluators discern that at least some evaluators share their concerns regarding a
20
legitimacy object that they hitherto had perceived as valid, they are more likely to voice their
silenced propriety beliefs and to challenge the previously valid but now increasingly contested
legitimacy object. In contrast, in a context where evaluators privately endorse a legitimacy ob-
ject but had silenced their beliefs because they perceived a lack of validity, evaluators are more
likely to voice and enact their favorable propriety beliefs when they learn that certain other eval-
uators hold similar views. For instance, the election of Donald Trump to the U.S. presidency has
led some U.S. citizens to voice more openly their dissent with the political establishment, pre-
sumably because Trump’s campaign and victory revealed that a significant share of the elec-
torate hold similar critical opinions regarding “political correctness” (Hahl et al., 2018).
As demonstrated by research on threshold models of collective behavior (Granovetter,
1978; Kuran, 1995) and social influence (Bonardi and Keim, 2005; Watts and Dodds, 2009), it
might suffice for a small group, or even a single evaluator, to reveal their deviating propriety
beliefs, either through communication or overt action, to meet the threshold level at which other
evaluators are willing to disclose their previously silenced beliefs. Gaining increased confidence
from the support of others, these other evaluators will be more likely to deviate from what they
perceive to be the norm (Schilke, 2018) and express their previously suppressed propriety be-
liefs publicly. This, in turn may contribute to further eroding the validity of the status quo while
at the same time supporting the legitimation of a novel or alternative institutional order. This
dynamic may generate a judgment cascade or “spiral of empowerment” (Lee and Chun, 2016),
in which evaluators increasingly follow the disclosures of other evaluators until a majority of
evaluators have disclosed their propriety beliefs. Eventually, these dynamics allow evaluators to
discern the actual (dis)approval of a presumably (in)valid legitimacy object. Such cascade dy-
namics are exemplified by the self-immolation of a provincial fruit vendor in Tunisia and the
ensuing discussion of this event on social media which revealed widespread private opposition
to a seemingly valid regime, igniting an information cascade and the “revolutionary bandwagon”
of the Arab Spring (Goodwin, 2011, p. 453).
21
In addition, as indicated in Figure 2 (T2), evaluators who had favorably assessed the le-
gitimacy object’s propriety prior to a judgment cascade will be exposed to the erosion of the
once-dominant validity assessment. They will recognize that the apparently broad-based consen-
sus was merely presumed rather than real. This realization may lead these previously supportive
evaluators to reassess and update their propriety beliefs, and act on that basis (Tost, 2011). In-
deed, exposure to minority opinions can be surprisingly powerful in leading people to change
their views (Wood et al., 1994), even if they had previously been advocates of the majority opin-
ion. For that reason, the greater public visibility of formerly silenced judgments can influence
the propriety beliefs and behaviors of evaluators who previously endorsed the now increasingly
invalidated legitimacy object. The insight that the disclosure of silenced propriety beliefs can
trigger cascade dynamics has also important implications for policy. In order to devise effective
interventions (e.g., aimed at deinstitutionalizing a harmful practice), policy makers need to take
into account not only the distribution of propriety beliefs but also how threshold levels at which
evaluators start disclosing their beliefs are distributed in a community of evaluators and to figure
both direct and indirect effects of legitimacy judgment cascades into their interventions (see Ef-
ferson et al., 2020).
To conclude, the distinction between propriety, validity, and consensus as well as our
discussion of the role of social influence and coercion in legitimacy judgment formation offers a
new window into understanding the legitimacy dynamics that precede institutional change.
Whereas previous models of institutional change have often used individuals changing their
propriety beliefs, either in response to exogenous shocks, institutional contradictions or individ-
uals’ reflexivity, as their starting point (Greenwood et al., 2002; Tost, 2011), our account theo-
rizes institutional change being triggered by resolved misconceptions regarding the pattern of
other evaluators’ beliefs. Acknowledging that validity and behavioral conformity may conceal
private dissent and that macro-level legitimacy may be inherently fragile thus offers a crucial
insight to legitimacy theory, which thus far has treated validity and consensus as essentially syn-
22
onymous. Given that theoretical and methodological developments are inherently intertwined
(Greenwald, 2012), our theoretical considerations have important implications for empirical
research, a point we elaborate next.
ADVANCING LEGITIMACY RESEARCH WITH EXPERIMENTS
Haack and colleagues (2020a) suggested that the trajectory of institutional research can be
interpreted as the result of theory-method co-evolution (Greenwald, 2012), with the advance-
ment of the field depending on a self-enforcing and continuous cycle between theory develop-
ment and empirical research aimed at testing and consolidating new theory. In this view, the
development of appropriate research designs and measurement approaches is crucial for the ad-
vancement of legitimacy theory. Having established the key conceptual distinction between
three components of legitimacy, a second important goal of our paper is hence to offer some
guidance for future empirical research that elucidates the intricate relationships between
propriety, validity, and consensus, particularly with respect to the antecedents and consequences
of the incongruity between validity and consensus. We believe that conventional methodologies
may not be well-equipped to disentangle distinct legitimacy components and their complex
interactions. Traditionally, legitimacy has often been approximated through crude indirect
measures such as population density or number of adoptions.v However, not only are such prox-
ies problematic because they leave open alternative explanations unrelated to legitimacy, but
more specifically, they are often unable to capture individual evaluators’ perceptions. Research
has also relied heavily on correlational designs and text-analytical approaches which tend to be
inadequate for developing a theory of the inter-subjective and socio-cognitive processes that
underlie legitimacy (Haack, 2012). As a result, extant research and measurement approaches
cannot effectively differentiate between the legitimacy components and the intricate relation-
ships between these components, and they are of limited utility in explaining or predicting the
occurrence of unanticipated institutional change.
23
As a solution to this problem, we encourage researchers to embrace experimental designs
more frequently while employing appropriate individual-level measures for legitimacy such as
those that clearly distinguish between propriety and validity beliefs as suggested by Haack and
Sieweke (2020). Given their unique ability to identify cognitive processes at the individual level
that would be difficult to study with other methods, experiments offer a highly attractive ap-
proach to rigorously and consistently studying the relationships between propriety, validity, and
consensus and their relevance in the context of institutional change (Schilke et al., 2019). Espe-
cially when investigating socially endogenous inferences (where evaluators consider the opinion
of other evaluators when forming their own judgments), experiments offer the important ad-
vantage of ruling out alternative explanations and isolating factors of theoretical interest, as
demonstrated by experimental research in sociology (e.g., Correll et al., 2017; Schilke and
Rossman, 2018), psychology (e.g., Paluck and Shepherd, 2012; Zou et al., 2009), and behavioral
economics (e.g., Allcott, 2011; Hallsworth et al., 2017). We thus believe the time is ripe to bring
experiments to the core of legitimacy research in organization theory.
In social science research, the term “experiment” refers to the random assignment of units
of analysis (typically human participants) to different groups (e.g., Shadish et al., 2002). Given
that groups vary only with respect to the manipulated variable (the “treatment variable”),
changes in the outcome variable can be attributed to the manipulation. Experiments have a
number of important advantages (Bitektine et al., 2018; Stone-Romero, 2009); namely, they
enable causal inference, they allow the researcher to control for the influence of external
variables, and they help the researcher explore the parameters of complex constructs. It may thus
not be surprising that we are currently witnessing a significant increase in experimental research,
particularly in institutional theory (Bitektine et al., 2018). Following up on Zucker’s (1977)
early experimental work studying the effects of institutionalization on cultural persistence, more
recent experimental investigations have demonstrated the rapid institutionalization of price
bubbles (Levine et al., 2014), experimentally manipulated institutional complexity (Raaijmakers
24
et al., 2015), institutionalized belief systems (Hafenbrädl and Waeger, 2017), political ideologies
(Jasinenko et al., 2020), and various types of institutional logics (Glaser et al., 2016). Recent
investigations have also tested the effects of organizational identity on resistance to institutional
pressures (Schilke, 2018). However, with the exception of the landmark study by Elsbach (1994)
and a recent investigation by Jahn and colleagues (2020), organization and management scholars
have yet to embrace experimental designs in legitimacy research.
We see an opportunity for “retooling” and an untapped potential to capitalize on
experiments to improve the understanding of legitimacy that we advance in this paper—as a
multi-level process comprising distinct legitimacy components. In this section, we offer three
separate yet interrelated experimental designs, all of which seek to examine the interactions
among propriety, validity, and consensus and to improve our knowledge of the inter-subjective
processes of perception, interpretation, and interactions that establish the core of a multi-level
understanding of legitimacy. All three designs are specifically geared towards developing a
better understanding of the role of consensus in the legitimacy processes preceding institutional
change. While we can only sketch selected aspects of these designs, we are hopeful that even a
rough outline can inspire legitimacy researchers to pursue these or similar approaches in the near
future, in an effort to help build strong and relevant theory.
To ensure that the internal validity of experimental research is not threatened we
recommend unexperienced experimentalists to study best-practice suggestions for experimental
methods (e.g. Aguinis and Bradley, 2014; Lonati et al., 2018). We also suggest that researchers
develop a thorough understanding of the role of deception in experimental research. Given that
our theory focuses on the antecedents and consequences of collective misperceptions and the
role of communication in the formation and dissolution of these misperceptions, deception may
be a useful or even necessary element in future experimental work on legitimacy. However,
there are stark differences between psychological and economic research traditions with respect
to the acceptability of deception. While psychologists and sociologists tend to see deception as a
25
legitimate tool in experimentation whenever it would be practically impossible to run a study
without (Cook and Yamagishi, 2008; Smith and Richardson, 1983), laboratories in behavioral
economics enforce bans on deceiving subjects and there is a strong professional norm among
economists that employing deception in experiments is unethical, making them de facto
unpublishable (Dickson, 2011). Experimental procedures involving deception in economics-
oriented schools and programs thus may require a convincing justification for approval by ethics
committees.
Survey Design
The first suggested approach consists of a set of survey experiments to examine the effect
of perceived validity and consensus on propriety beliefs. This design builds on and extends
previous works in sociology that have focused on the relationship between validity and propriety
(e.g., Johnson et al., 2016; Massey et al., 1997; Walker et al., 1988). Survey experiments embed
the experimental manipulation within a conventional survey, through which researchers may
also gather individual-level characteristics of participants for use as additional predictor
variables in the data analysis. Survey experiments combine some of the benefits of surveys with
those of experiments: they have a high degree of internal validity because the experimental
element allows researchers to randomize assignment to conditions and control variables of
interest. At the same time, survey experiments can also have a high degree of external validity
because the use of web platforms and vignettes (such as realistic news stories invented
specifically for the purposes of the experiment) increase the similarity between the experimental
situation and everyday experiences (Aguinis and Bradley, 2014; Finch, 1987).
Based on the theoretical notions developed above, we posit that the formation of propriety
beliefs regarding a legitimacy object is contingent on both the perceived validity of that object
and the degree to which it is consensually regarded as appropriate for its social context.
Specifically, it can be assumed that propriety beliefs are likely to be revised and also disclosed
in contexts characterized by high validity and low consensus, because a lack of consensus casts
26
doubt on whether legitimacy objects perceived as valid represent an objective and collectively
approved fact (for a related argument in a field setting, see Haack et al., 2020b).
Based on this reasoning, we suggest that scholars could conduct survey experiments to test
the causal relationships between validity, consensus, and propriety. A first type of study could
employ a 2 (validity: high vs. low) ×2 (consensus: high vs. low) between-subjects design, in
which the experimenter randomly assigns participants to a condition and presents one of four
versions of a vignette about the focal legitimacy object. Validity and consensus could both be
manipulated in the vignette. A major source of validity is authorization by high-status actors,
granting a legitimacy object the status of a social fact (Johnson et al., 2006). Consensus, on the
other hand, indicates that evaluators within a given reference group agree that a legitimacy
object is appropriate for its social context. The vignette could thus indicate (1) that a legitimacy
object is either supported or criticized by an important authority (validity manipulation) and (2)
that the relevant reference group is either characterized by significant agreement or
disagreement with regard to the appropriateness of the focal legitimacy object (consensus
manipulation). For instance, the “high validity/low consensus” condition would highlight that
although a legitimacy object is strongly supported by an authority (and is thus high on validity),
social approval is not unanimous and significant dissent exists among other evaluators regarding
the acceptability of the legitimacy object. The manipulation of consensus could be implemented
graphically through the display of differently shaped opinion distributions, not unlike those
shown in Figure 1 above. For instance, a single-peaked distribution with low variance would
represent a high-consensus condition, whereas a double-peaked distribution with the same mean
value but much higher variance would represent a low-consensus condition (e.g., Reimann et al.,
2016, p. 1461).
In line with prior research in sociology (e.g., Hegtvedt and Johnson, 2000; Schilke and
Rossman, 2018; Walker et al., 1988), we expect that validity strengthens propriety (bolstering
effect) and reduces or even neutralizes perceptions of impropriety (canceling effect). The two
27
effects need to be separated, as previous research has suggested that propriety (and legitimacy
more generally) can be conceptualized as a bipolar construct, meaning that it ranges from
positive propriety to negative propriety (impropriety) (Hudson, 2008; Suddaby et al., 2017).
Consistent with the work of Haack and colleagues (2020b), we expect these effects to be
stronger within high-consensus conditions, as participants are more likely to follow their validity
beliefs (i.e., their beliefs about what others believe) and to hide their propriety beliefs in these
contexts. That is, if an evaluator holds unfavorable propriety beliefs about a valid legitimacy
object (e.g., a hierarchical system or status order) and also knows that this object is unanimously
approved (i.e., consensus is high), that evaluator will be less likely to voice unfavorable
propriety beliefs and more likely to adapt his or her propriety beliefs over time (strong canceling
effect). Meanwhile, if an evaluator holds a propriety belief that is consistent with the level of
perceived validity, a high-consensus condition will further strengthen the evaluator’s propriety
belief, and he or she will be more likely to voice the judgment openly (bolstering effect).
Finally, under conditions of low consensus, we expect that participants will pay less attention to
validity and be more likely to reveal what they personally consider appropriate.
Deliberation Design
A second, and particularly promising, opportunity to advance knowledge of the role of
consensus in the legitimacy processes preceding institutional change is to draw on the
experimental study of deliberations to examine whether and how communication may alter the
dynamics underlying the interactions among propriety, validity, and consensus. Deliberation can
be defined as “mutual communication that involves weighing and reflecting on preferences,
values and interests regarding matters of common concern” (Mansbridge, 2015, p. 27).
Importantly, deliberation designs have the advantage of combining the “internal validity
provided by experimental design, the external validity provided by actual deliberation about
real-world issues, and the generalizability provided by surveys” (Carpini et al., 2004, p. 333).
Suddaby and colleagues (2017) further stressed that deliberation settings allow for the
28
identification of supra-individual aspects of cognition, and thus help addressing the issue of how
everyday practices and conversations create intersubjectively shared typifications for a given
reference group (DiMaggio, 1997). Deliberation designs therefore facilitate the empirical
investigation of the negotiations and struggles underlying both the production of validity and the
silencing of propriety beliefs in processes of (de-)legitimation (Suddaby et al., 2017).
Specifically, future research could elaborate the conditions under which deliberation helps
to reveal vs. perpetuate a non-disclosed incongruity between validity and consensus. That is,
what are the factors and circumstances through which deliberation objectifies validity beliefs
about a legitimacy object (constituting validity) while silencing inconsistent propriety beliefs
(lowering perceived consensus)? Conversely, scholars could aim to identify the conditions under
which deliberation deobjectifies validity beliefs (constituting invalidity) and contributes to the
disclosure of incongruities and silenced propriety beliefs (raising perceived dissensus).vi
The starting point of a research agenda premised on deliberation designs is the insight
developed in this paper that validity often involves the appearance of consensus rather than
actual consensus. While apparent consensus may mirror actual consensus with relative accuracy,
it is important to point out that a legitimacy object can be valid and objectified, and thus can
induce a sense of obligation that guides behavior, when its acceptance is “assumedly shared” by
others in a social group (Suchman, 1995, p. 574). As discussed above, validity can thus be based
on “false consensus,” or a misperception of the propriety beliefs prevalent within a reference
group. Even if the majority of evaluators hold unfavorable propriety beliefs about a legitimacy
object (meaning only a few evaluators privately endorse it), this object can still be perceived as
highly valid if these evaluators falsely assume that most evaluators in their group believe that
this object is proper. The belief in the validity of a legitimacy object can thus exist as a social
fact and can guide behavior even in the presence of weak consensus. As discussed in greater
detail above, this kind of misperception is more likely to occur when the ties among the
members of a group are weak and trustful communication is sparse, which makes it difficult for
29
individual evaluators to detect the actual or “true” beliefs of others. In turn, denser network ties
and the possibility of communicating openly with group members in public deliberation can
reveal that validity is based on an illusion of support, making it more likely that evaluators will
disclose their actual propriety beliefs, leading to the deobjectification of validity.
There are manifold opportunities to further examine dynamics of (de)objectification in the
controlled setting of a deliberation experiment. For instance, researchers could draw on a design
developed by Schnider and colleagues (2020) and measure the impact of communication on
changes in propriety beliefs by employing a difference-in-differences design, where treatments
vary with respect to the degree of validity (e.g., through expert endorsements). In such a design,
within-subjects differences in propriety measures would be surveyed before and after
discussions and compared between the treatment and control conditions. In the treatment
condition, participants would discuss the focal legitimacy object (e.g., a controversial
organizational practice) with experts (constituting the source of validity), while in the control
condition participants would discuss the focal legitimacy object amongst themselves without
experts. To ensure that the endorsement or critique of the focal legitimacy object follows a
predefined script in group discussions, the researcher could hire professional actors and employ
standardized scripts, which would control for variance in idiosyncratic characteristics, such as
attractiveness or persuasiveness (e.g., Jacquart and Antonakis, 2015). If executed well, the
involvement of scripted actors may not comprise mundane realism while contributing to the
experience of the deliberation as an authentic and natural interaction for all participants. In
addition, participants would receive a standardized welcome address, which introduces the topic
to be discussed, invites them to make an effort to justify propositions and considerations, and
emphasizes the importance of mutual listening (Baccaro et al., 2016). Another opportunity to
study the dynamics of (de)objectification is the manipulation of group consensus. In this type of
design, researchers could assemble the deliberation groups based on pre-tested propriety beliefs
to match a specific propriety belief distribution (see Figure 1). This design embraces the
30
complexity of the variety of real-world propriety distributions and could help understand which
propriety distributions bolster or harm the silencing effect of validity. All deliberations should
be mediated by trained moderators. These would lead the discussion as an essentially free
conversation among the participants, whereby they manage the time, open and close the
discussion, and make sure that no single participant either dominates the conversation or refrains
from talking entirely.vii Deliberations could be videotaped and coded for relevant behavioral
patterns and interaction dynamics to triangulate self-reported information.
Storytelling Design
Finally, another promising approach is to examine whether validity, propriety and
consensus influence how and why some stories develop and persist over time while others fall
out of favor. Storytelling adds an important temporal element to the legitimacy research
described above and shifts the focus to the stories actors construct to make sense of controversial
legitimacy objects or issues. Further, storytelling is a key way in which cultural and institutional
knowledge is transmitted over time (Lounsbury and Glynn, 2001). Stories are passed down from
parents to children, from managers to new employees, and from newspapers to the public.
Stories are therefore among the most powerful mechanisms for creating and maintaining
legitimacy. While stories often are preserved as they are passed from person to person, they can
also change. We suggest dynamics of storytelling are particularly important in contentious and
complex contexts, such as those surrounding a controversial legitimacy object, where the stories
people choose to tell may help determine the momentum and eventual outcome of critical
decisions.
Here, we propose a set of laboratory experiments that are based on serial reproduction
research in cultural psychology (Clark and Kashima, 2007; Lyons and Kashima, 2003). In this
design, which is sometimes called “the telephone game,” participants receive information and
are asked to then share that information in their own words with another person, in a chain of
four people. We recommend this design to examine the conditions under which participants
31
either seek to reproduce a dominant story about the focal legitimacy object (Story A) or seek to
introduce changes to this story given the existence of a competing story (Story B). Our baseline
argument is that participants will, in general, tend to reproduce Story A whenever validity and
propriety pressures exist. However, participants will be likely to introduce changes to this story
under a specific combination of conditions. Specifically, when participants perceive that Story A
will likely be rejected by their group (i.e., validity for story A is perceived to be weak) and they
themselves hold discrepant beliefs (i.e., unfavorable propriety beliefs) with regard to Story A,
they will be more likely to introduce new information from Story B into their overall
communications. What this suggests is that a story’s reproducibility is driven primarily by a
belief that the collective will buy into the veracity of this story and, even when this is not the
case, by the personal belief that this story is true. Only when these conditions do not hold will
stories change.
In this type of serial reproduction experiment, participants would receive some infor-
mation about the focal legitimacy object through Story A, followed by information about wheth-
er or not the participant with whom they will be communicating will endorse this story (validity
belief manipulation: peer endorsement vs. peer rejection). Participants would be surveyed to
gauge their personal level of endorsement (propriety belief measure). They then would be given
a newspaper article that offers equivalent information about Story A and Story B. The experi-
menter would then ask the participant to write a summary of this news article for the next person
to read. The core outcome measure would be the amount of Story-A-consistent information in
relation to the amount of Story-B-consistent information provided in the participants’ written
summaries. Our expectation is that the only time where the amount of Story B information
would increase relative to Story A information is when a participant believes that the participant
he or she is communicating with will not endorse Story A and when this participant himself or
herself also has a lower level of endorsement for Story A.
32
We suggest including four participants in each reproduction chain. To control for order ef-
fects, a second set of four participants would start with Story B (counterbalancing). Thus, this
study would employ a 2 (validity: low vs. high) × 2 (sequence: starting with A vs. B) between-
subjects design with three measurement points (the stories of participants 2, 3, and 4). To ensure
variance in propriety beliefs, the researcher could pre-select participants to ensure that the sub-
ject pool is diverse in terms of propriety beliefs about a certain story (i.e., recruiting an equal
number of individuals holding either favorable or unfavorable propriety beliefs). This would
make it possible to build ideal-typical chains (a “supportive” chain comprising individuals with
favorable propriety beliefs vs. a “critical” chain comprising individuals with unfavorable propri-
ety beliefs). Varying the composition of chains (“supportive” vs. “critical”) would allow manip-
ulating the construct of consensus (i.e., the actual agreement that exists within a reproduction
chain). As a “supportive” chain resembles the notion of echo chambers discussed in social media
research, study results may generate insights about political polarization and ways to overcome
it. Moreover, research drawing on this kind of storytelling design has important practical
implications regarding the way governments, media, and the public create spaces in which
people feel safe to express discrepant beliefs about controversial issues.
Measurement
Propriety beliefs could be measured using a pre-tested and validated item battery (e.g.,
Alexiou and Wiggins, 2019). However, given that evaluators may silence their propriety beliefs
when they perceive them to be inconsistent with validity, measurement instruments based on
self-reports may, due to social desirability effects, capture only validity beliefs (Haack and
Sieweke, 2020). While the attention to validity beliefs is not a methodological artifact but much
in line with what we would expect theoretically, the question arises how scholars can measure
silenced propriety beliefs. Indirect or “implicit” measures of propriety beliefs, including tech-
niques such as the implicit association test and evaluative priming, may complement “explicit”
self-report measures (see, e.g., Humphreys and Latour, 2013). In addition, conjoint analysis can
33
help reduce the social desirability and retrospective reporting biases sometimes associated with
explicit measures (Siraz et al., 2020). Besides explicit and implicit measures of propriety,
scholars may also consider integrating a behavioral correlate—for example, by asking
participants about their willingness to make a donation to the focal legitimacy object or a
specific cause related to the legitimacy object (Haack and Sieweke, 2020). Indeed, even though
legitimacy research suggests a direct link between legitimacy judgments and behavior, it has
rarely tested this link in the controlled setting of an experiment (Gruban and Légeret, 2020).
DISCUSSION
Contributions
This article paired theory development with methodological recommendations to make
two important contributions to legitimacy research. First, we delineated three distinct legitimacy
components—propriety, validity, and consensus—that operate at different yet highly interde-
pendent levels of analysis—micro, macro, and meso—and explained why future research on
legitimacy should be structured around these three components. While propriety and validity
have received growing attention in legitimacy research (e.g., Bitektine and Haack, 2015; Tost,
2011), the meaning and role of consensus has oftentimes been ignored or treated only tangential-
ly. In fact, legitimacy scholars have often conflated the concepts of validity and consensus and
failed to define and differentiate the latter. To address this problem, we clarified that validity
denotes an institutionalized, collective-level perception of appropriateness for a given social
context, whereas consensus is a meso-level construct that describes the degree of agreement in
evaluators’ propriety beliefs. Separating validity from consensus therefore offers a crucial cor-
rection to earlier work (e.g., Bitektine and Haack 2015) that erroneously implied that validity
and consensus reflect one and the same construct.
Our clarification advances the multi-level conception of legitimacy and has important im-
plications for theories of institutional change. Specifically, whereas much prior research has
portrayed validity as virtually impervious to change, our theory highlights that validity and the
34
taken-for-granted aspects of institutions can be inherently fragile whenever consensus is low.
Such low consensus may oftentimes not be apparent, because evaluators have little reason to
openly voice propriety beliefs that are not congruent with a legitimacy object’s validity. Howev-
er, trustful communication among evaluators and collective action may make low consensus
visible and reveal the prevalence of dissent, with the result that evaluators who hold minority
opinions will be more likely to publicly disclose their propriety beliefs. In turn, the disclosure of
the once concealed propriety beliefs will affect the judgments and behavioral dispositions of
other evaluators. As illustrated in Figure 2, these dynamics may lead to a judgment cascade that
prompts the problematization, delegitimation, and ultimately, the decline of a previously valid
legitimacy object. At the same time, delegitimation may give room to the legitimation of an al-
ternative institutional order, contributing to the process of institutional change. Importantly, our
research clarifies that institutional change does not necessarily start with intra-individual change
in propriety beliefs, as has often been implied in extant scholarship (e.g., Greenwood et al.,
2002; Tost, 2011). Institutional change can also be instigated when evaluators become able to
gauge how propriety beliefs are distributed in their reference group and to identify a critical mass
of like-minded evaluators who are willing to disclose their dissenting beliefs and engage in institu-
tional change efforts. Importantly, this account offers a viable approach to addressing the contest-
ed issue of embedded agency by explaining why individual actors are capable of stepping out of
the current institutional order (Cardinale, 2018; Harmon et al., 2019).
Second, our paper draws attention to the need to broaden the methodological toolkit to ad-
vance empirical research on the perceptual components of legitimacy. Experimental designs
allow researchers in institutional theory to approach legitimacy in a new way—as a process of
social judgment formation that is subject to the evolving cognitions of, and pressures on, the
human evaluator, rather than merely as a fixed characteristic or asset of organizations that con-
tributes to organizational growth and survival (Deephouse and Suchman, 2008). We maintain
that an experimental research program will help examine legitimacy as an explanandum and
35
phenomenon in its own right, specify and test its causal determinants, corroborate (or refute) the
assumptions on which much of the multi-level research on legitimacy is based, and ultimately
empower researchers to advance their understanding of micro-institutional processes (Zucker,
1991, p. 104; also see Zucker and Schilke, 2020).
Experiments: The Promise and the Challenge
Experiments constitute a powerful means of establishing causal inferences and are often
considered the “gold standard” of science (Coleman, 1990). However, in private conversations
with colleagues, we frequently sense a concern that experiments may reinforce empiricism and a
positivist epistemology that emphasizes the existence of an objective and measurable reality, in
contrast to an interpretive epistemology, which postulates that knowledge derives from the sub-
jective interpretation of reality (Morgan and Smircich, 1980). Our view, however, is that exper-
iments that are carefully designed and interpreted are capable of supporting a holistic and more
sophisticated view that acknowledges the inherently contextual foundation of human cognition
and behavior (DiMaggio, 1997). In this sense, experiments have significant potential to
strengthen the social-constructionist roots of institutional theory, reinvigorating and extending
earlier works of Zucker (1977), Garfinkel (1967), and others.
Although we are not the first to encourage the greater use of experimental designs in insti-
tutional research, implementing such a research agenda has remained challenging. A major dif-
ficulty may stem from a lack of familiarity with relevant experimental methods in the organiza-
tion theory community. As David and Bitektine (2009) argued, institutional researchers may
need training in experimental designs, which may require them to collaborate with colleagues
from disciplines such as marketing, psychology, and organizational behavior. Indeed, engaging
in more intense dialogue and interdisciplinary collaboration could prove highly fruitful in that it
would help scholars integrate sociological and psychological perspectives of institutional phe-
nomena (DiMaggio, 1997; Zerubavel, 1997; Zucker and Schilke, 2020). This also means that
legitimacy researchers need to overcome old habits, look beyond short-term incentive structures,
36
and make a deliberate effort to gain experience in experimental methods (Haack et al., 2020a).
In the words of Bitektine (2009), scholars have to evolve from “method specialists”—that is,
researchers who are constrained by a narrow set of methods—into “domain specialists”—that is,
researchers who apply “more diverse, but sometimes less ‘legitimate’ (and therefore more
‘risky’) research methods to address research questions that cannot be explored through ‘more
legitimate’ methods” (Bitektine, 2009, p. 219).
Another potential obstacle to establishing an experimental research agenda is that the ap-
plication of experiments requires variables of interest, and clearly specified causal relationships
among these variables, that are suitable for standardized operationalization and external control
(McKinley, 2007). This means that legitimacy scholars need to clarify and refine the definitions
of key constructs and identify the scope conditions under which the proposed effects are likely
to hold (Suddaby, 2010). By drawing on Suchman (1995), legitimacy scholars have regularly
defined legitimacy rather broadly, and, possibly as a result, the construct has been measured in a
variety of ways that have contributed to increasing the range of meanings associated with it.
Closely following Suchman’s (1995) encompassing definition may have been detrimental to the
advancement of legitimacy theory and perhaps institutional theory as a whole. To reach a better
understanding of legitimacy in institutional theory, researchers need to specify more precisely
the perceptual components of legitimacy and their relationships. In Hirsch and Levin’s (1999)
words, we believe the time is ripe in the evolution of legitimacy research for umbrella advocates
to give way (at least in part) to validity police in order to avoid a construct collapse. Enhancing
construct clarity (rigor) will be also instrumental to have a meaningful impact on practice (rele-
vance), as a multi-level theory of legitimacy with a clear differentiation of its perceptual compo-
nents brings us closer to the reality and complexity of legitimacy dynamics within and about
organizations (Haack et al., 2020a).
Future Research and Implications for Related Literatures
37
We see ample opportunities to advance our understanding of legitimacy by drawing on the
context of social media. The rise of social media as a heterogeneous and co-produced environ-
ment changes how social judgments about organizations are produced and disseminated (Etter et
al., 2019) and can provide important insights into cascade dynamics underlying sudden and un-
anticipated institutional change. For instance, tracking the emergence and spread of new Twitter
hashtags may help flag abrupt institutional change so clearly that they make excellent classroom
examples, and can also provide visible evidence of cascades that reinforce legitimacy, destroy it,
or create something new. Sometimes the Twitter sequences include, as part of the message
stream, the older content that is being replaced, and associate it with relevant Twitter actors.
Here, tracks of related messages can expose otherwise hidden pervasive behavior that is seen by
one subset of actors as illegitimate, and as costly to change by another, such as repeated predato-
ry sexual behavior in the movie and photography industries. The speed of change in response to
these Twitter revelations, when confirmed by multiple high-status actors, lies visible for all to
see. From recounting personal experiences to later court cases, the mechanisms for institutional
change are laid out and can be subjected to research. YouTube and news organizations often
produce similar streams of delegitimation processes at work.
Similarly, mechanisms that maintain or reinforce different forms of sexism, ethnic target-
ing, and racism can be exposed, studied, and alternatives producing positive, ameliorating
changes identified. For Zucker (2019), this visibility of action helped her to informally test her
idea that legitimacy, and the related institutional order, can undergo sudden redefinition (see also
Zucker and Darby, 2020). From one day to the next, what was accepted with only secret, veiled
comment becomes part of a pressure campaign that leads to firing, company bankruptcy, court
cases and prison sentences. The cascade of revealed and documented actions, unknown when
previously concealed, is successfully defined as illegitimate, and that redefinition quickly pro-
duces real effects. While it might be possible to design a quasi-experiment using data from Twit-
ter, YouTube or newspapers for research purposes, the control of true experiments as we outline
38
them in the section above is invaluable at this stage of theoretical development. After legitimacy
scholars identify the relevant causal chains via experiments, other exploration as above would
help establish the range of conditions under which these processes take place outside of the lab.
Diving back into the lab for confirmation at the end might become second nature to most re-
searchers.
While our article primarily contributes to the literature on legitimacy and institutional
change, its insights into the concept of consensus are likely to be relevant for several adjacent
literatures. In fact, the general notion of consensus is not unique to the study of legitimacy, but
may be of great importance whenever certain perceptions can be collectively held among mem-
bers of an entity (Bowman and Ambrosini, 1997). Hence, our theorizing has the potential to con-
tribute to research on related types of social evaluation, such as corporate reputation (Lange et
al., 2011), status (Piazza and Castellucci, 2014), or stigma (Devers et al., 2009). Scholars have
often treated these types of evaluations as collective-level constructs that reflect public recogni-
tion and social approval, and are just beginning to examine the role of individual-level judg-
ments in the process through which an organization’s collective-level approval forms and
changes (Haack and Sieweke, 2020).
Perhaps most notably, scholars interested in trust have started to theorize the role of con-
sensus in the context of studying trust dynamics (Brattström et al., 2019; Schilke and Cook,
2013). One key finding from that literature is that substantial dissensus may exist whenever mul-
tiple actors form trust perceptions of a particular target, with some placing considerably higher
trust in it than others (Klein et al., 2000). The mechanisms explaining such variations in consen-
sus among individual trustors, as well as the consequences of trust consensus, are at the forefront
of this inquiry (de Jong et al., 2017; 2020). While our article shares an interest in these topics, it
introduces important novel aspects of consensus that have yet to be considered by trust scholars
and that could aid in developing a more nuanced understanding of how consensus may shape
trust dynamics. For example, extant trust scholarship mostly assumes that actors either have
39
knowledge of others’ trust perceptions or they do not, but the possibility and implications of
false impressions of others’ trust perceptions have not yet been examined, even though trust
misperceptions are likely to exist widely (Campagna et al., forthcoming). Our theorizing about
validity-consensus congruity may offer a useful starting point. Further, while our discussion of
Figure 1 emphasizes the need to move beyond average consensus levels, how trust beliefs are
distributed and whether distinct distributions may have unique consequences has not yet been
addressed by trust scholars (see de Jong et al., 2020 for a recent exception).
CONCLUSION
In this article, we illuminated the complex relationships between the propriety, validity,
and consensus components of legitimacy and highlighted that disentangling validity from con-
sensus allows for improved theorizing of the legitimacy processes that precede sudden and unan-
ticipated institutional change. We believe that future research on legitimacy needs to be struc-
tured around these three components in order to move forward. Our proposal of conceiving
legitimacy as a multi-level process and the elaboration of relevant experimental designs to study
this process aims to contribute to such an ambitious research agenda. Our suggestions, which are
meant to offer both programmatic and pragmatic advice, thus represent only the beginning of a
long journey. We would be delighted if you considered joining this effort.
40
FIGURES
Figure 1: Different distributions of propriety beliefs
41
Figure 2: Disclosure of incongruity between validity and consensus
42
REFERENCES
Aguinis, H. and Bradley, K. J. (2014). ‘Best-practice recommendations for designing and im-
plementing experimental vignette methodology studies’. Organizational Research
Methods, 17, 351–371.
Aldrich, H. E. and Fiol, C. M. (1994). ‘Fools rush in? The institutional context of industry
creation’. Academy of Management Review, 19, 645–670.
Alexiou, K. and Wiggins, J. (2019). ‘Measuring individual legitimacy perceptions: scale
development and validation.’ Strategic Organization, 17, 470–496.
Allcott, H. (2011). ‘Social norms and energy conservation.’ Journal of Public Economics, 95,
1082-1095.
Arshed, N., Chalmers, D. and Matthews, R. (2019). ‘Institutionalizing women’s enterprise
policy: a legitimacy-based perspective.’ Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 43,
553-581.
Angrist, J. D., Imbens, G. W. and Rubin, D. B. (1996). Identification of causal effects using
instrumental variable. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 91, 444-
455.Asch, S. E. (1951). ‘Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distor-
tion of judgments’. In Guetzkow, H. (Ed), Groups, leadership and men: research in
human relations. Oxford, England: Carnegie Press, 177-190.
Bansal, P. and Clelland, I. (2004). ‘Talking trash: legitimacy, impression management and
unsystematic risk in the context of the natural environment, Academy of Management
Journal, 47, 93–103.
Baum, J. and Oliver, C. (1991). ‘Institutional linkages and organizational mortality’. Adminis-
trative Science Quarterly, 36, 187–218.
Baum, J. and Powell, W. W. (1995). ‘Cultivating an institutional ecology of organizations:
Comment on Hannan, Carroll, Dundon, and Torres’. American Sociological Review,
60, 529–538.
Berger, P. L. and Luckmann, T. (1967). The Social Construction of Reality: A Treatise in the
Sociology of Knowledge. Garden City, NY: Anchor.
Berger, J., Ridgeway, C. L., Fisek, M. H. and Norman, R. Z. (1998). ‘The legitimation and
delegitimation of power and prestige orders’. American Sociological Review, 63, 379–
405.
Bitektine, A. B. (2011). ‘Towards a theory of social judgment of organizations: The case of
legitimacy, reputation, and status’. Academy of Management Review, 36, 151–179.
Bitektine, A. B. (2009). ‘What makes us faddish? Resource space constraints and the “gar-
bage’ model of social science’. Scandinavian Journal of Management, 25, 217–220.
Bitektine, A. and Haack, P. (2015). ‘The “macro” and the “micro” of legitimacy: towards a
multi-level theory of the legitimacy process’. Academy of Management Review, 40,
49-75.
Bitektine, A., Hill, K., Song, F. and Vandenberghe, C. (2020). Organizational legitimacy,
reputation and status: Insights from micro-level measurement. Academy of Manage-
ment Discoveries, 6, 107-136.
Bitektine, A., Lucas J. and Schilke, O. (2018). ‘Institutions under a microscope: Experimental
methods in institutional theory’. In Bryman, A. and Buchanan D. A. (Eds), Unconven-
tional methodology in organization and management research. Oxford University
Press, Oxford, 147-167.
Bonardi, J. P. and Keim, G. D. 2005. ‘Corporate political strategies for widely salient issues’.
Academy of Management Review, 30, 555–576.
Bowman, C. and Ambrosini, V. (1997). ‘Perceptions of strategic priorities, consensus and
firm performance’. Journal of Management Studies, 34, 241-58.
43
Brattström, A., Faems, D. and Mähring, M. (2019). ‘From trust convergence to trust diver-
gence: trust development in conflictual interorganizational relationships’. Organizati-
on Studies, 40, 1685-1711.
Brodbeck, F. C., Kerschreiter, R., Mojzisch, A. and Schulz-Hardt, ST. (2007). ‘Group deci-
sion making under conditions of distributed knowledge: the information asymmetries
model’. Academy of Management Review, 32, 459-479.
Brown, A. and Toyoki, S. (2013). ‘Identity work and legitimacy’. Organization Studies, 34,
875-896.
Bundy, J. and Pfarrer, M. D. (2015). ‘A burden of responsibility: The role of social approval
at the onset of a crisis’. Academy of Management Review, 40, 345-369.
Campagna, R. L., Dirks, K. T., Knight, A. P., Crossley, C. and Robinson, S. L. (forthcoming).
‘On the relation between felt trust and actual trust: examining pathways to and
implications of leader trust meta-accuracy’. Journal of Applied Psychology.
Canales, R. (2016). From ideals to institutions: Institutional entrepreneurship and the growth
of Mexican small business finance, Organization Science, 27, 1548-1573.
Cardinale, I. (2018). ‘Beyond constraining and enabling: Toward new microfoundations for
institutional theory’. Academy of Management Review, 43, 132–155.
Carpini, M. X. D., Cook, F. L. and Jacobs, L. R. (2004). ‘Public deliberation, discursive par-
ticipation, and citizen engagement: a review of the empirical literature’. Annual Re-
view of Political Science, 7, 315–344.
Castello, I., Etter, M. and Nielsen, F. A. (2016). ‘Strategies of legitimacy through social me-
dia: the networked strategy’. Journal of Management Studies, 53, 402-432.
Cattani, G. Ferriani, S., Negro, G. and Perretti, F. (2008). ‘The structure of consensus: Net-
work ties, legitimation and exit rates of U.S. feature film producer organizations’. Ad-
ministrative Science Quarterly, 53, 145-182.
Centola, D., Willer, R. and Macy, M. (2005). ‘The emperor’s dilemma: a computational mod-
el of self-enforcing norms’. American Journal of Sociology, 110, 1009–40.
Centola, D., Becker, J., Brackbill, D. and Baronchelli, A. (2018). ‘Experimental evidence for
tipping points in social convention’. Science, 360, 1116-1119.
Christakis N. A. and Fowler, J. H. (2008). ‘The collective dynamics of smoking in a large
social network’. New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 2249-58.
Clark, A. E. and Kashima, Y. (2007). ‘Stereotypes help people connect with others in the
community: A situated functional analysis of the stereotype consistency bias in
communication’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1028-1039.
Clemente, M. and Roulet, T. J. (2015). ‘Public opinion as a source of deinstitutionalization: A
"spiral of silence" approach’. Academy of Management Review, 40, 96-114.
Coleman, J. (1990). Foundations of Social Theory. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Colyvas, J. A. and Powell W. W. (2006). ‘Roads to institutionalization: The remaking of
boundaries between public and private science’. Research in Organizational Behavior,
27, 315-363.
Cook, K. S. and Yamagishi, T. (2008). ‘A defense of deception on scientific grounds.’ Social
Psychology Quarterly, 71: 215-221.
Cook, K. S., Yamagishi, T., Cheshire, C., Cooper, R., Matsuda, M. and Mashima, R. (2005).
‘Trust building via risk taking: A cross-societal experiment.’ Social Psychology Quar-
terly, 68, 121-142.
Cook, K. S., Levi, M. and Hardin, R. (2009). ‘Whom can we trust? How groups, networks,
and institutions make trust possible’. Russell Sage Foundation.
Correll, S. J, Ridgeway, C. L., Zuckerman, E. W., Jank, S., Jordon-Bloch, S. and Nakagawa,
S. (2017). ‘It’s the conventional thought that counts: How third-order inference pro-
duces status advantage’. American Sociological Review, 82: 297-327.
44
Dacin, T., Goodstein, J. and Scott, W. R. (2002). ‘Institutional theory and institutional
change: Introduction to the special research forum’. Academy of Management Journal,
45, 45-56.
David, J. D. and Bitektine, A. B. (2009). ‘The deinstitutionalization of institutional theory?
Exploring divergent agendas in institutional research’. In Buchanan, D. and Bryman
A. (Eds), The SAGE handbook of organizational research methods. Thousand
Oaks/CA: SAGE, 160-175.
Deephouse, D. L., Bundy, J., Tost, L. P. and Suchman, M. (2017). ‘Organizational legitimacy:
Six key questions’. In Greenwood, R., Oliver, C., Lawrence, T. B. and Meyer, R.
(Eds), The SAGE Handbook of Organizational Institutionalism, 2nd edition. Thousand
Oaks, CA: SAGE, 27–54.
Deephouse, D. L. and Suchman, M. C. (2008). ‘Legitimacy in organizational institutional-
ism’. In Greenwood, R., Oliver, C., Sahlin, K. and Suddaby R. (Eds), The SAGE
Handbook of Organizational Institutionalism. London: SAGE, 49–77.
de Jong, B., Gillespie, N., Williamson, I. and Gill, C. (2020). ‘Trust consensus within cultur-
ally-diverse teams: A multi-study investigation’. Journal of Management, forthcom-
ing.
de Jong, B., Kroon, D. P. and Schilke, O. (2017). ‘The future of organizational trust research:
a content-analytic synthesis of scholarly recommendations and review of recent devel-
opments. In P. A. M. Van Lange, B. Rockenbach and T. Yamagishi (Eds), Trust in so-
cial dilemmas. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 173-94.
Devers, C. E., Dewett, T., Mishina, Y. and Belsito, C. A. (2009). ‘A general theory of stig-
ma’. Organization Science, 20, 154–71.
Dickson, E. S. (2011), ‘Economics versus psychology experiments: Stylization, incentives,
and deception’, in J. N. Druckman, D. P. Green, J. H. Kuklinski and A. Lupia (eds),
Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science, Cambridge: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press: 58–71.
DiMaggio, P. (1997). ‘Culture and cognition’. Annual Review of Sociology, 23, 263–278.
Donaldson, L. (1995). American Anti-management Theories of Organization. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Dornbusch, S. M. and Scott, W. R. (1975). Evaluation and the exercise of authority. San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Drori, I. and Honig B. (2013). ‘A process model of internal and external legitimacy’. Organi-
zation Studies. 34, 345–376.
Efferson C., Vogt, S. and Fehr, E. 2020. ‘The promise and the peril of using social influence
to reverse harmful traditions’. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 55-68.
Elsbach, K. D. (1994). ‘Managing organizational legitimacy in the California cattle industry:
The construction and effectiveness of verbal accounts’. Administrative Science Quar-
terly, 39, 57–88.
Etter, M., Ravasi, D. and Colleoni, E. (2019). ‘Social media and the formation of organiza-
tional reputation.’ Academy of Management Review, 44, 28-52.
Etter, M., Colleoni, E., Illia, L., Meggiorin, K. and D’Eugenio, A. (2018). ‘Measuring organi-
zational legitimacy in social media: Assessing citizens’ judgments with sentiment
analysis’. Business and Society, 57, 60-97.
Esser J. K. (1998). ‘Alive and well after 25 years: a review of groupthink research’. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 73, 116-141.
Finch, J. (1987). ‘The vignette technique in survey research.’ Sociology, 21, 105-114.
Fisher, G., Kotha, S. and Lahiri, A. (2016). ‘Changing with the times: an integrated view of
identity, legitimacy and new venture life cycles’. Academy of Management Review,
41, 383-409.
45
Foreman, P. and Whetten, D. A. (2002). ‘Members’ identification with multiple identity or-
ganizations’. Organization Science, 13, 618–635.
Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in Ethnomethodology. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs/N.J.
Gauthier, J. and Kappen, J. A. (2017). ‘Corporate rhetorical strategies in the legitimation of
genetically modified foods’. Journal of Communication Management, 21, 218-235.
Glaser, V. L., Fast, N. J., Harmon, D. J. and Green. S. (2016). ‘Institutional frame switching:
How institutional logics shape individual action’. Research in the Sociology of
Organizations 48A 35-69.
Goodwin, J. (2011). ‘Why we were surprised (again) by the Arab Spring’. Swiss Political
Science Review, 17, 452–456.
Granovetter, M. (1978). ‘Threshold models of collective behavior’. American Journal of So-
ciology, 83, 1420-1443.
Greenwald, A. G. (2012). ‘There is nothing so theoretical as a good method’. Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 7, 99-108.
Greenwood, R., Raynard, M., Kodeih, F., Micelotta, E. R., and Lounsbury, M. (2011).
‘Institutional complexity and organizational responses’. Academy of Management
Annals, 5, 317-371.
Greenwood, R., Suddaby, R. and Hinings, C. R. (2002). ‘Theorizing change: The role of pro-
fessional associations in the transformation of institutionalized fields’. Academy of
Management Journal, 45, 58–80.
Greve, H. R., Palmer, P. and Pozner, J. E. (2010). ‘Organizations gone wild: The causes, pro-
cesses, and consequences of organizational misconduct’. Academy of Management
Annals, 4, 53-107.
Gruban, M. and Légeret, M. (2020). ‘The role of fairness perceptions in legitimacy judgments
and their behavioral responses: Evidence from the lab.’ Working Paper, University of
Lausanne.
Haack, P. (2012). Legitimacy in institutional theory: Three essays on social judgments in a
globalized world. Zurich, Switzerland: University of Zurich.
Haack, P., Pfarrer, M., and Sieweke, J. (2020). ‘Legitimacy as a multilevel process: evidence
from the 2008 financial crisis’. Working paper, University of Lausanne.
Haack, P., Pfarrer, M. and Scherer, A. G. (2014). ‘Legitimacy-as-feeling: How affect leads to
vertical legitimacy spillovers in transnational governance’. Journal of Management
Studies, 51, 634-666.
Haack, P. and Sieweke, J. (2020). ‘Advancing the measurement of organizational legitimacy,
reputation, and status: first-order judgments vs second-order judgments’. Academy of
Management Discoveries, 6, 153-158.
Haack, P. and Sieweke, J. (2018). ‘The legitimacy of inequality: Integrating the perspectives
of system justification and social judgment’. Journal of Management Studies, 55, 486-
516.
Haack, P., Sieweke, J. and Wessel, L. (2020). ‘Microfoundations and multi-level research on
institutions’. In Haack, P. Sieweke, J. and Wessel L. (Eds), Microfoundations of Insti-
tutions (RSO, Vol. 65A), Bingley, UK: Emerald, 11–40.
Hafenbrädl, S. and Waeger, D. (2017). ‘Ideology and the micro-foundations of CSR: Why
executives believe in the business case for CSR and how this affects their CSR
engagements’. Academy of Management Journal, 60, 1582-1606.
Hahl, O., Kim, M., Zuckerman Sivan, E. W. (2018). ‘The authentic appeal of the lying dema-
gogue: Proclaiming the deeper truth about political illegitimacy’. American Sociologi-
cal Review, 83, 1-33.
Halbesleben, J. R. B., Wheeler, A. R. and Buckley, M. R. (2007). ‘Understanding pluralistic
ignorance in organizations: Application and theory’. Journal of Managerial Psycholo-
gy, 22, 65-83.
46
Hallsworth M., List, J. A., Metcalfe, R. D. and Vlaev, I. (2017). ‘The behavioralist as tax col-
lector: using natural field experiments to enhance tax compliance’. Journal of Public
Economics, 148, 14-31.
Harmon, D. J., Haack, P. and Roulet, T. J. (2019). ‘Microfoundations of institutions: A matter
of structure versus agency or level of analysis?’ Academy of Management Review, 44,
464–67.
Harmon, D., Green, S. and Goodnight, G. (2015). ‘A model of rhetorical legitimation: the
structure of communication and cognition underlying institutional maintenance and
change’. Academy of Management Review, 40, 76–95.
Hegtvedt, K. A., and Johnson, C. (2000). ‘Justice beyond the individual: A future with legiti-
mation’. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 298-311.
Helms, W. S., Patterson, K. D. W. and Hudson, B. A. (2019). ‘Let’s not “taint” stigma re-
search with legitimacy, please’. Journal of Management Inquiry, 28, 5-10.
Hengst, I.-A., Jarzabkowski, P., Hoegl, M. and Muethel, M. (2020). ‘Toward a process theory
of making sustainability strategies legitimate in action’. Academy of Management Jour-
nal, 63, 246-271.
Higgins, M. C. and Gulati, R. (2006). ‘Stacking the deck: the effects of top management
backgrounds on investor decisions’. Strategic Management Journal, 27, 1–25.
Hirsch, P. M. and Levin, D. Z. (1999). ‘Umbrella advocates versus validity police: A life-
cycle model’. Organization Science, 10, 199-212.
Hudson, B. A. (2008). ‘Against all odds: A consideration of core-stigmatized organizations’.
Academy of Management Review, 33, 252–66.
Hudson, B. A. and Okhuysen, G. (2009). ‘Not with a ten foot pole: Core-stigma, stigma trans-
fer, and improbable persistence of men’s bathhouses’. Organization Science, 20, 134–
153.
Human, S. E. and Provan, K. G. (2000). ‘Legitimacy building in the evolution of small-firm
multilateral networks: A comparative study of success and demise’. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 45, 327–365.
Humphreys, A. and Latour, K. A. (2013). ‘Framing the game: Assessing the impact of cultur-
al representations on consumer perceptions of legitimacy’. Journal of Consumer Re-
search, 40, 773-795.
Huy, Q., Corley, K. and Kraatz, M. (2014). ‘From support to mutiny: Shifting legitimacy
judgments and emotional reactions impacting the implementation of radical change’.
Academy of Management Journal, 57, 1650–1680.
Jacquart, P. and Antonakis, J. (2015). ‘When does charisma matter for top-level leaders? Ef-
fect of attributional ambiguity’. Academy of Management Journal, 58, 1051-1074.
Jacqueminet, A. and Durand, R. (2019). ‘Ups and downs: the role of legitimacy judgment
cues in practice implementation’. Academy of Management Journal, forthcoming.
Jahn, J., Eichhorn, M. and Brühl, R. (2020). ‘How do individuals judge organizational
legitimacy? Effects of attributed motives and credibility on organizational legitimacy’.
Business and Society, 59, 545–576.
Janis, I. L. (1972). Victims of Groupthink. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Jasinenko, A., Christandl, F. and Meynhardt, T. (2020). ‘Justified by ideology: Why
conservatives care less about corporate social irresponsibility.’ Journal of Business
Research, 114, 290-303.
Johnson, C. (2004). ‘Introduction: Legitimacy processes in organizations’. Research in the
Sociology of Organizations, 22, 1-24.
Johnson C., Dowd T. J. and Ridgeway C. L. (2006). ‘Legitimacy as social process’. Annual
Review of Sociology, 32, 53–78.
Johnson, C., Kaufman, J. and Ford, R. (2000). ‘Emotional reactions to conflict: Do
dependence and legitimacy matter?’ Social Forces, 79, 107-137.
47
Johnson, C., Hegtvedt, K. A., Khanna, N. and Scheuerman, H. L. (2016). ‘Legitimacy
processes and emotional responses to injustice’. Social Psychology Quarterly, 79, 95-
114.
Kahneman, D. and Frederick, S. (2002). ‘Representativeness revisited: attribute substitution in
intuitive judgment’. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. and Kahneman, D. (Eds), Heuristics
and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press, 49–81.
Klein, K. J., Palmer, S. L. and Conn, A. B. (2000). ‘Interorganizational relationships: a multi-
level perspective’. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds), Multilevel theory, re-
search, and methods in organizations: foundations, extensions, and new directions.
San Francisco, CA, US: Jossey-Bass, 267-307.
Kuran, T. (1995). Private Truths, Public Lies: The Social Consequences of Preference Falsi-
fication. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lamin, A. and Zaheer, S. (2012). ‘Wall Street vs. Main Street: Firm strategies for defending
legitimacy and their impact on different stakeholders’. Organization Science, 23, 47–
66.
Lange, D., Lee, P. M. and Dai, Y. (2011). ‘Organizational reputation: a review’. Journal of
Management, 37, 153-184.
Lawrence, T. B., and Suddaby, R. (2006). ‘Institutions and institutional work’. In Clegg, S. R.
Hardy, C., Lawrence, T. B. and Nord W. R. (Eds), Handbook of organization studies:
London: SAGE Publications, 215-254.
Lee, M. J. and Chun, J. W. (2016). ‘Reading others’ comments and public opinion poll results
on social media: Social judgment and spiral of empowerment’. Computers in Human
Behavior, 65, 479-487.
Levine, S. S., Apfelbaum, E. P., Bernard, M., Bartelt, V. L., Zajac, E. J. and Stark, D. (2014).
‘Ethnic diversity deflates price bubbles’. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 111, 18524-18529.
Lewis, A. C., Cordero, A. M. and Xiong, R. (2020). ‘Too red for crowdfunding: The
legitimation and adoption of crowdfunding across political cultures.’ Entrepeneurship
Theory and Practice, forthcoming.
Lonati, S., Quiroga, B. F., Zehnder, C. and Antonakis, J. (2018). ‘On doing relevant and
rigorous experiments: Review and recommendations’. Journal of Operations
Management, 64, 19-40.
Lounsbury, M. and Glynn, M. A. (2001). ‘Cultural entrepreneurship: Stories, legitimacy, and
the acquisition of resources’. Strategic Management Journal, 22, 545–564.
Lyons, A. and Kashima, Y. (2003). ‘How are stereotypes maintained through
communication? The influence of stereotype sharedness’. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 85, 989-1005.
Mackie, G. (1996). ‘Ending footbinding and infibulation: a convention approach’. American
Sociological Review, 61, 999-1017.
Maguire, S. and Hardy, C. (2009). ‘Discourse and deinstitionalization: The decline of DDT’.
Academy of Management Journal, 52, 148-178.
Mansbridge, J. (2015). ‘A minimalist definition of deliberation’. In Heller, P., and Vijayendra
R. (Eds), Deliberation and development, Washington, D.C.: World Bank Group, 27-
50.
Markóczy, L. (2001). ‘Consensus formation during strategic change’. Strategic Management
Journal, 22: 1013-1031.
Martin, J.M., Reimann, M. and Norton, M.I. (2016). ‘Experience theory, or how desserts are
like losses’. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 1460-1472.
Massey, K., Freeman, S. and Zelditch, M. (1997). ‘Status, power, and accounts’. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 60, 238-251.
48
McKinley, W. (2007). ‘Managing knowledge in organization studies trough instrumentation’.
Organization, 14, 123–146.
Micelotta, E., Lounsbury, M. and Greenwood, R. (2017). ‘Pathways of institutional change:
an integrative review and research agenda’. Journal of Management, 43: 1885-1910.
Morgan, G., and Smircich, L. (1980). ‘The case for qualitative research’. Academy of Man-
agement Review, 5, 491–500.
Oliver, C. (1992). ‘The antecedents of deinstitutionalization’. Organization Studies, 13, 563-
588.
Paluck, E. L. and Shepherd H. (2012). ‘The salience of social referents: a field experiment on
collective norms and harassment behavior in a school social network’. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 103, 899-915.
Patriotta G., Gond, J-P. and Schultz, F. (2011). ‘Maintaining legitimacy: controversies, orders
of worth, and public justifications’. Journal of Management Studies, 48, 1804-1836
Pfarrer, M. D., DeCelles, K. A., Smith, K. G. and Taylor, M. S. (2008). ‘After the fall:
Reintegrating the corrupt organization’. Academy of Management Review, 33, 730-
749.
Piazza, A. and Castellucci, F. (2014). ‘Status in organization and management theory’.
Journal of Management, 40, 287-315.
Prentice, D. A. and Miller, D. T. (1993). ‘Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus:
some consequences of misperceiving the social norm’. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 64, 243-256.
Priem, R. L., Harrison, D. A. and Muir, N. K. (1995). ‘Structured conflict and consensus out-
comes in group decision making’. Journal of Management, 21, 691-710.
Raaijmakers, A., Vermeulen, P., Meeus, M. and Zietsma, C. (2015). ‘I need time! Exploring
pathways to compliance under institutional complexity’. Academy of Management
Journal, 58, 85-110.
Rao, H., Greve, H. and Davis, G. (2001). ‘Fool’s gold: Social proof in the initiation and
abandonment of coverage by wall street analysts’. Administrative Science Quarterly,
46, 502–26.
Ridgeway, C. L. and Berger, J. (1986). ‘Expectations, legitimation, and dominance behavior
in task groups’. American Sociological Review, 51, 603-617.
Rossman, G. and Schilke, O. (2014). ‘Close, but No Cigar: The Bimodal Rewards to Prize-
Seeking’. American Sociological Review, 79, 86-108.
Ruef, M. and Scott R. W. (1998). ‘A multidimensional model of organizational legitimacy:
hospital survival in changing institutional environments’. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 43, 877–904.
Scherer, A. G., Palazzo, P. and Seidl, D. (2013). ‘Managing legitimacy in complex and heter-
ogeneous environments: sustainable development in a globalized world’. Journal of
Management Studies, 50, 259-284.
Schilke, O. (2018). ‘A micro-institutional inquiry into resistance to environmental pressures’.
Academy of Management Journal, 61, 1431–1466.
Schilke, O. and Cook, K. S. (2013). ‘A cross-level process theory of trust development in
interorganizational relationships’. Strategic Organization, 11, 281-303.
Schilke, O., Levine, S. S., Kacperczyk, O., and Zucker, L. G. 2019. Call for papers—special
issue on experiments in organizational theory. Organization Science, 30, 232-234.
Schilke, O. and Rossman, G. (2018). ‘It’s only wrong if it’s transactional: Moral perceptions
of obfuscated exchange’. American Sociological Review, 83, 1079–1107.
Schnider, R., Haack, P., and Scherer, A. G. (2020). ‘Legitimacy judgment formation as delib-
eration: evidence from experiments on corporate tax avoidance’. Working paper, Uni-
versity of Zurich.
49
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D. and Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-
experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Houghton Mifflin: Boston,
MA.
Siraz, S., Claes, B. P. and De Castro, J. O. (2019). ‘Let’s do the fracking talk: An experi-
mental approach to understanding legitimacy perceptions.’ Academy of Management
Proceedings, 2019: 18193.
Smith, S. S. and Richardson, D. (1983). ‘Amelioration of deception and harm in psychologi-
cal research: The important role of debriefing.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 44, 1075.
Stone-Romero, E. F. (2009). ‘Implications of research design options for the validity of infer-
ences derived from organizational research’. In Buchanan, D. and Bryman, A. (Eds),
SAGE Handbook of Organizational Research Methods: Thousand Oaks/CA: SAGE,
302–327.Stuart, T. E., Hoang, H. and Hybels, R. C. (1999). ‘Interorganizational en-
dorsements and the performance of entrepreneurial ventures’. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 44, 315–49.
Suchman, M. C. (1995). ‘Managing legitimacy: strategic and institutional approaches’. Acad-
emy of Management Review, 20, 571–610.
Suddaby, R., Bitektine, A. and Haack, P. (2017). ‘Legitimacy’. Academy of Management An-
nals, 11, 451–478.
Suddaby, R. and Greenwood, R. (2005). ‘Rhetorical strategies of legitimacy’. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 50, 35–67.
Tolbert, P. S., and Darabi, T. (2020). ‘Bases of conformity and institutional theory: under-
standing organizational decision-making’. In Haack, P. Sieweke, J. and Wessel L.
(Eds), Microfoundations of Institutions (RSO, Vol. 65A), Bingley, UK: Emerald, 269–
290.
Tolbert, P. S. and Zucker, L. G. (1996). ‘The institutionalization of institutional theory’. In
Clegg S. R., Hardy, C. and Nord W. R. (Eds), Handbook of organization studies.
London. SAGE, 175–190.
Tost, L. P. (2011). ‘An integrative model of legitimacy judgments’. Academy of Management
Review, 36, 686–710.
Vaara, E. (2014). ‘Struggles over legitimacy in the Eurozone crisis: Discursive legitimation
strategies and their ideological underpinnings’. Discourse and Society, 25, 500-518.
Vergne, J-P. (2011). ‘Towards a new measure of organizational legitimacy: method, valida-
tion, and illustration’. Organizational Research Methods, 14, 484–502.
Walker, H. A. (2014). ‘Legitimacy and inequality’. In McLeod J., Lawler, E. and Schwalbe
M. (Eds), Handbook of the social psychology of inequality. Dordrecht: Springer, 353-
377.
Walker, H. A., Rogers, L. and Zelditch, M. (1988). ‘Legitimacy and collective action: A re-
search note’. Social Forces, 67, 216-228.
Walker, K., Schlosser, F. and Deephouse D. L. (2014). ‘Organizational ingenuity and the par-
adox of embedded agency: The case of the embryonic Ontario solar energy industry’.
Organization Studies, 35, 613-634.
Washington, M. and Zajac, E. J. (2005). ‘Status evolution and competition: theory and evi-
dence’. Academy of Management Journal, 48, 282–296.
Watts, D. and Dodds, P. (2009). ‘Threshold models of social influence’. In Hedström, P. and
Bearman, P. (Eds), Oxford Handbook of Analytical Sociology. Oxford University
Press: Oxford, 475-497.
Weber, M. [1918] 1978. Economy and society: An outline of interpretive sociology (Roth, G.
and Wittich, C. Trans.). Berkeley: University of California Press.
50
Westphal, J. D. and Bednar, M. K. (2005). ‘Pluralistic ignorance in corporate boards and
firm’s strategic persistence to low firm performance’. Administrative Science Quarter-
ly, 50, 262-298.
Willer, R., Kuwabara, K. and Macy, M. W. (2009). ‘The false enforcement of unpopular
norms’. American Journal of Sociology, 115, 451-490.
Wood, W., Lundgren, S., Ouellette, J. A., Busceme, S. and Blackstone, T. (1994). ‘Minority
influence: A meta-analytic review of social-influence processes’. Psychological Bulle-
tin, 115, 323-345.
Wood, M. S. and McKinley, W. (2017). ‘After the venture: The reproduction and destruction
of entrepreneurial opportunity’. Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal, 11, 18-35.
Yoon, J. and Thye, S. (2011). ‘A theoretical model and new test of managerial legitimacy in
new work teams’. Social Forces, 90, 639-659.
Zelditch, M. (2006). ‘Legitimacy theory’. In Burke, P. J. (Ed), Contemporary social
psychological theories: 324-352. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Zelditch, M. (2004). ‘Institutional effects on the stability of organizational authority’.
Research in the Sociology of Organizations, 22, 25-48.
Zelditch, M. (2001). ‘Processes of legitimation: Recent developments and new directions’.
Social Psychology Quarterly, 64, 4–17.
Zelditch, M. and Walker, H. A. (2003). ‘The legitimacy of regimes’. In Thye S., and
Skvoretz, J. (Eds), Advances in group processes: Power and status. London: Emerald,
217-249.
Zerubavel, E. (1997). Social Mindscapes: An Invitation to Cognitive Sociology. Cambridge:
Harvard University Press.
Zhu, D. H. and Westphal, J. D. (2011). ‘Misperceiving the beliefs of others: How pluralistic
ignorance contributes to the persistence of positive security analyst reactions to the
adoption of stock repurchase plans’. Organization Science, 22, 869-886.
Zimmerman, M. and Zeitz, G. (2002). ‘Beyond survival: achieving new venture growth by
building legitimacy’. Academy of Management Review, 27, 414–31.
Zou, X., Tam, K-P., Morris, M. W., Lee, S-L. and Lau, I. Y-M. (2009). ‘Culture as common
sense: perceived consensus versus personal beliefs as mechanisms of cultural influ-
ence’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 579-597.
Zucker, L. G. (2019). OMT Distinguished Scholar Award Talk, Annual Meeting Academy of
Management, Boston.
Zucker, L. (1991). ‘Postscript: the role of institutionalization in cultural persistence’. In Pow-
ell, W.W. and DiMaggio, P. J. (Eds), The New Institutionalism in Organizational
Analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 103–107.
Zucker, L. G. (1989). ‘Combining institutional theory and population ecology: No legitimacy,
no history’. American Sociological Review, 54, 542–545.
Zucker, L. G. (1983). ‘Organizations as institutions’. In Bacharach, S. B. (Ed). Research in
the Sociology of Organizations. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 1-47.
Zucker, L. (1977). ‘The role of institutionalization in cultural persistence’. American Socio-
logical Review, 42, 726–743.
Zucker, L. G. and Darby M. R. (2020). ‘Lightspeed change in science and society.’ NBER
Working Paper.
Zucker, L. G. and Schilke, O. (2020). ‘Towards a theory of micro-institutional processes: for-
gotten roots, links to social-psychological research, and new ideas’. In Haack, P.
Sieweke, J. and Wessel, L. (Eds), Microfoundations of Institutions (RSO, Vol. 65A),
Bingley, UK: Emerald, 371–390.
51
NOTES
i It is important to note that the legitimacy components of propriety, validity, and consensus are distinct from,
and must not be confused with, the “dimensions” of legitimacy. Approaches to conceptualizing the dimensions
of legitimacy range from two-dimensional (e.g., Aldrich and Fiol, 1994; Ruef and Scott, 1998) to three-
dimensional (e.g., Huy et al., 2014; Suchman, 1995; Tost, 2011) and four-dimensional (e.g., Walker et al., 2014).
Other studies address issue-specific types of legitimacy, such as corporate environmental legitimacy (Bansal and
Clelland, 2004) or more idiosyncratic conceptions of legitimacy (e.g., Higgins and Gulati, 2006; Human and
Provan, 2000). Each dimension or type of legitimacy can be conceptualized as comprising propriety, validity,
and consensus components.
ii Please note that important aspects of the consensus construct were co-developed in Haack et al., 2020b.
iii Note that consensus can describe the agreement regarding favorable or unfavorable propriety beliefs. By de-
fault, we conceptualize consensus such that it describes the agreement regarding favorable propriety.
iv Systems that are highly institutionalized may breakdown and delegitimate before construction of new systems.
Revolutions, both in government and in science, often share an “act first, legitimate later” character.
v For density measures, see Baum and Powell, 1995; Zucker, 1989; for indirect measures of adoption motivation,
see Donaldson, 1995; Vergne, 2011; for text-based measures, see Etter et al., 2018; Haack, 2012.
vi By “objectification” we refer to the development of an objective and valid external reality that is largely
independent of the subjective internal states of individual evaluators (Berger and Luhmann, 1967; Tolbert and
Zucker, 1996). Conversely, “deobjectification” refers to the development of an objective external reality that is
increasingly subjective and dependent on the private propriety beliefs of evaluators (Wood and McKinley,
2017).
vii A deliberation experiment cannot fully match the internal validity and control of confounders that other
experimental approaches afford. In particular, researchers cannot enforce deliberation on participants: while
some participants may actively participate in deliberations, others may mainly listen without saying much
(“lurkers”). Non-compliance becomes particularly relevant when deliberation effects are not monotonic, i.e.
participants contributing actively to the discussion (“activists”) experience stronger shifts in propriety beliefs
than “lurkers”. Researchers could address this crucial problem in a twofold way: on the one hand, the
involvement of moderators could help to reduce the number of “lurkers” by directly asking them to participate;
on the other hand, the causal effect of validity and consensus in deliberation could be estimated by employing
the so-called Complier Average Causal Effect approach (Angrist et al., 1996).
... explains institutional maintenance and change (e.g. : Haack, Schilke, & Zucker, 2021;Tost, 2011). ...
... Legitimacy subjects are 'social entities, structures, actions, and ideas whose acceptability are being assessed' (Deephouse and Suchman, 2008: 54). A central assumption in the literature, namely that legitimacy reflects a relationship between a legitimacy subject and an evaluator (or group of evaluators) who is assessing the subject based on a 'socially constructed system of norms, values, beliefs, and definitions' (Suchman, 1995: 574), has been largely overlooked in empirical research on legitimacy, and prior works have rarely considered individual-level characteristics when studying the legitimacy process (Haack et al., 2021;Tost, 2011). ...
... In this paper, we approach legitimacy from the third angle, the legitimacy-as-perception perspective. We focus 1 Extant legitimacy literature refers to the entities or actors that are being evaluated indistinctly as legitimacy objects and legitimacy subjects (e.g.: Deephouse and Suchman, 2008;Haack et al., 2021;Siraz et al., 2023;Suddaby et al., 2017). on the individual level, which reflects the micro level of legitimacy as perception. ...
Preprint
In this study, we draw on the legitimacy-as-perception perspective in organization and management studies (e.g.: Siraz, Claes, De Castro, & Vaara, 2023; Suddaby, Bitektine, & Haack, 2017; van den Broek, Langley, Ehrenhard, & Groen, 2022) to investigate how evaluators form individual legitimacy judgments (propriety) when exposed to multiple or even conflicting cues in the context of fracking. Previous research on legitimacy establishes it as a multilevel construct in which propriety refers to an individual’s own judgment of the appropriateness of a given legitimacy object, and validity refers to legitimacy at the collective level. Although an inherently collective-level construct, validity finds its way into the appraisal process of individual evaluators and enters their cognition in the form of ‘validity cues. While there has been significant interest in the interaction between validity and propriety (Haack & Sieweke, 2018; Ivanova Ruffo, Mnisri, Morin-Esteves, & Gendron, 2020), as well as in its behavioral correlates, including practice implementation (Jacqueminet & Durand, 2020), protest behavior (Walker, Thomas, & Zelditch Jr, 1986), organizational change (Huy, Corley, & Kraatz, 2014), and institutionalization (Arshed, Chalmers, & Matthews, 2019), an unresolved puzzle in the legitimacy-as-perception perspective is the question of under what conditions a given validity cue gains predominance in the formation of propriety and overrides the influence of other validity cues. As previous work has predominantly examined the impact of a single source of validity (e.g.: Elsbach, 1994; Johnson, Dowd, Ridgeway, Cook, & Massey, 2006; Walker et al., 1986), we know little about how the concurrent presence of multiple and potentially conflicting validity cues from different sources shapes propriety judgment formation. This question is particularly relevant in contested arenas where polarized legitimacy is common (Gond et al., 2016; Scherer et al., 2013; Siraz et al., 2023). In such settings, individual evaluators are confronted with ‘multiple validities’ (Bitektine & Haack, 2015: 59), and a priori it is not clear whether and how engagement with different validity cues affects evaluators’ propriety judgments about a focal subject of legitimacy. Legitimacy subjects are ‘social entities, structures, actions, and ideas whose acceptability are being assessed’ (Deephouse and Suchman, 2008: 54). A central assumption in the literature, namely that legitimacy reflects a relationship between a legitimacy subject and an evaluator (or group of evaluators) who is assessing the subject based on a ‘socially constructed system of norms, values, beliefs, and definitions’ (Suchman, 1995: 574), has been largely overlooked in empirical research on legitimacy, and prior works have rarely considered individual-level characteristics when studying the legitimacy process (Haack et al., 2021; Tost, 2011). Considering validity cues concomitantly, however, merely provides a partial understanding of formation of propriety judgments. Evaluators’ personal values are likely to affect how validity cues impact their judgment formation and thus contribute to shaping their propriety judgments (Finch, Deephouse, & Varella, 2015; Schwartz et al., 2012). According to Schwartz’s theory of basic individual values (1994; 2012), values influence evaluations because they serve as ‘guiding principles in the life of a person.’ In addition, values have been shown in numerous studies to influence the way evaluators make up their mind (e.g. Judge & Bretz, 1992; Lamin & Zaheer, 2012). In the context of fracking, evaluators’ environmentalism is particularly relevant (Gond et al., 2016). While scholars have sought to conceptualize the mechanisms and underlying processes of legitimacy judgment formation (Bitektine & Haack, 2015; Tost, 2011), and recent empirical research has progressed toward this goal (e.g. Finch et al., 2015; Haack & Sieweke, 2018), the role of evaluators’ values in judgment formation is yet to be further investigated. Building on the legitimacy-as-perception perspective (e.g., Suddaby et al., 2017) and the literature on environmentalism (e.g.: Dietz, Fitzgerald, & Shwom, 2005), we advance scholarly understanding of propriety judgment formation in a context of multiple and conflicting cues. We carried out full-profile conjoint experiments to investigate how multiple validity cues and evaluators’ values affect judgment formation. Evaluators were systematically exposed to four simultaneous and different cues varying in valence. This resulted in 7,904 propriety judgments nested in 247 individuals. Given the complex nature of propriety judgment formation involving multiple cues and evaluators’ personal values, empirical research in this area is difficult to conduct. By conducting conjoint analysis, we were able to capture how different validity cues are simultaneously used in propriety judgment formation and further investigate the effects of evaluators’ environmentalism. Moreover, the method allowed us to present various combinations of favorable and/or unfavorable cues to avoid biasing evaluators with a particular cue order or cue valence (Lohrke, Holloway, & Woolley, 2010; Shepherd, Patzelt, & Baron, 2013). Evaluators could thus assess multiple and conflicting cues. We make three contributions to the literature. First, this study contributes to the legitimacy-as-perception perspective by investigating how, and to what extent, different validity cues shape propriety. Our findings underscore the need to recognize that propriety is formed by concomitant interpretation of a bundle of validity cues, and they highlight that considering cues separately may lead to inaccurate conclusions about the impact of specific cues. This insight is particularly important given the increasing complexity and dynamism of the environment in which legitimacy subjects and evaluators are embedded. Second, we demonstrate that both validity cues and evaluators’ personal values play a significant role in propriety judgment formation. The concomitant consideration of both validity cues and values allows us to develop a holistic understanding of the judgment formation process. Our model illustrates how evaluators’ values change the weight that they attribute to different validity cues, thereby affecting their propriety judgments. This is a critical observation, because it underscores the importance for legitimacy subjects to consider evaluators’ values in the context in which they operate. Moreover, prior literature suggests that evaluators attenuate propriety judgments that are inconsistent with the perceived validity because they fear social sanctions (Bitektine and Haack, 2015), yet our findings show that attenuation also occurs when the expression of propriety judgments remains anonymous and without the risk of social sanctions. Finally, using a series of full-profile conjoint experiments allows us to investigate real-time propriety judgment formation in a context of multiple consistent and conflicting validity cues. This approach opens additional research opportunities for the legitimacy-as-perception perspective and social evaluations research more generally.
... This is especially the case with institutional theory, where legitimacy has become a cornerstone of theoretical and empirical work that helps explain the emergence, maintenance, and disruption of institutional order (Deephouse et al., 2017;Scott, 2008;Suchman, 1995;Suddaby et al., 2016;Suddaby & Greenwood, 2005). Moreover, scholars in organizational psychology and behavior have developed theoretical models to increase our understanding of how legitimacy judgments are formed and shaped (Bitektine & Haack, 2015;Haack, Pfarrer, & Scherer, 2014;Haack, Schilke, & Zucker, 2021;Tost, 2011;Zelditch, 2001). More recently, we have also seen legitimacy being linked with related concepts, such as reputation, status, or stigma (Bitektine, 2011;Deephouse & Suchman, 2008;Devers, Dewett, Mishina, & Belsito, 2008;Pollock, Lashley, Rindova, & Han, 2019). ...
... Finally, although there are important exceptions, including the "legitimacy as feeling" perspective (e.g., Haack et al., 2014) and the studies of the role of media reviewed earlier, research on discursive legitimation has mainly focused on how legitimacy is established, for example, through legitimation strategies, but usually not on legitimacy judgments or perceptions per se. This is partly because legitimacy judgment formation has traditionally been dominated by social psychological models and methods (Bitektine & Haack, 2015;Haack et al., 2021;Tost, 2011;Zelditch, 2001). Another reason is that scholars have usually lacked the empirical material and means to examine the role of communication in the formation of legitimacy judgments. ...
... However, this has changed with the availability of data in social media and other platforms that offer ample evidence of how actors attempt to establish legitimacy and how legitimacy judgments are constructed. As pointed out by recent research, such perceptions are not only black-and-white evaluations but involve multiple levels and forms, such as different types of consensus (Haack et al., 2021). It would be important to explore the discursive construction of such complexities and nuances in future research. ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, we have seen a proliferation of research on discursive legitimation, which has shed light on how legitimacy is established through communication. However, this body of work remains fragmented, and there is a need to synthesize and develop a more comprehensive and in-depth theoretical understanding of this vibrant area of research. This article aims to address this need by providing an integrative theoretical framework and outlining an agenda for future research. The framework encompasses five key elements of discursive legitimation: strategies, positions, foundations, temporality, and arenas. Drawing on this framework, we present a research agenda that highlights key topics related to these elements along with theoretical and methodological considerations cutting across them. Our contribution lies in conceptualizing discursive legitimation as a multifaceted and dynamic phenomenon, offering a complementary framework to existing models and paving the way for future studies, and placing discursive strategies-which have been the focus of prior research-in context by highlighting the critical role of key discursive elements in enabling or constraining legitimation processes. Acknowledgments: We thank Zeki Simsek and the reviewers for their insightful comments and constructive feedback. Special thanks to Alex Rustler for his valuable research assistance.
... From a more current perspective, organisational legitimacy is conceptualised by Díez-Martín, Blanco-González and Díez-de-Castro (2021a) as a favourable judgement about the acceptability of an organisation's actions based on criteria of usefulness, fairness, and appropriateness. Organisations improve or protect their legitimacy based on the expectations of institutions and stakeholders (Haack et al., 2021;Scott, 1995). Suchman (1995) argues that legitimacy can be assessed in three fundamental constructs: pragmatic, moral and cognitive. ...
... Recent research has conceptualised legitimacy as a multi-level phenomenon comprising ownership and validity. Ownership refers to an individual evaluator's belief that an object of legitimacy is appropriate for its social context, while validity denotes an institutionalised and collective perception of appropriateness (Haack et al., 2021). Authors such as Derakhshan et al. (2019) argue that there are currently several models created to explain the characteristics and measure the legitimacy of organisations, making it possible to understand the company's suitability within a social context. ...
... Authors such as Derakhshan et al. (2019) argue that there are currently several models created to explain the characteristics and measure the legitimacy of organisations, making it possible to understand the company's suitability within a social context. However, in some cases, they fail to measure society's expectations of the organisation operating in its territory (Eabrasu et al., 2021;Haack et al., 2021;Tucker et al., 2023). ...
Article
Ethnic-cultural festivals are considered public celebrations aimed at preserving and celebrating the ethnic culture of communities established in a region after migration. This research aims to measure and explore the dimensionality of residents’ perceptions of the legitimacy of ethnic cultural festivals. The study used 16 semi-structured interviews and literature research to generate the initial measurement items. Subsequently, exploratory factor was carried out with 338 residents of the cities of Blumenau and Santa Cruz do Sul (both in Brazil) to explore the underlying structure of legitimacy. Confirmatory factor was subsequently applied with 365 residents of the same cities to carry out the final validation of the proposed scale. The findings provide evidence that legitimacy in festivals can be measured with two constructs: social legitimacy using a 15-item scale and cultural legitimacy using 9 items. A scale on legitimacy is a welcome addition to the literature and can serve as a basis for future research on the topic. Furthermore, the scale provides a valid and reliable instrument for destination hosts to understand how the community gauges a festival's legitimacy and thus has a guiding element in event planning.
... Organizations seek legitimacy to gain acceptance and support from various stakeholders, including customers, employees, investors, regulators, and the broader society (Woodward et al. 1996). Legitimacy relates to adherence to the prevailing norms, values, and expectations of the environment, which include legal requirements, cultural standards, industry norms, and ethical considerations (Haack et al. 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
While prevalent in enterprise practice, business development (BD) activities have garnered limited attention within academic circles, leading to a noticeable dearth of discussions regarding their comprehensive understanding and the contemporary research landscape. Previous research has underscored the pivotal role and importance of business development activities in generating new growth opportunities and enhancing dynamic capabilities for enterprises. This article seeks to bridge this academic gap by synthesizing six decades of research literature on enterprise business development activities and tracing the progression of its research trajectory to uncover its complete narrative. Its contributions are manifold: firstly, we intensified the foundational elements of business development research by conducting a meticulous examination of various perspectives and providing insights into former ambiguities surrounding crucial aspects; secondly, it elucidates how contextual factors influence the emergence and evolution of enterprises’ business development activities, highlighting its research significance across different eras; thirdly, it conducts a comprehensive exploration of various research streams within business development and their nuanced interactions across key dimensions. Through these endeavors, this study aims to enrich academic discourse in the domain of business development, establishing a robust academic foundation for subsequent research.
... Insomuch that the scholars call for research in this area to deeper understand language use in a specific context (Vaara and Langley, 2021, p.549). In this sense, this study examines context-specific discourses with the idea that legitimacy is grounded in the social context (Haack, Schilke, and Zucker, 2021). Furthermore, in light of the calls for research on emerging countries and the mentioned importance of understanding context-specific arguments (Barkema, Chen, George, Luo, and Tsui, 2015;Filatotchev, Wei, Sarala, Dick, and Prescott, 2020;Morris, Aguilera, Fisher, and Thatcher, 2023), we explore the actors' rhetorical strategies during the transfer of a business model in a peripheral country. ...
Article
Full-text available
Actors' efforts against surrounding institutional pressures to pursue legitimacy are critical. Although scholars have mainly focused on the importance of gaining legitimacy, little is known about the context-specific rhetorical strategies of the actors to legitimate the business model in a peripheral country. In this article, based on extensive material, efforts behind the legitimation of a new airline business model in a peripheral country were explored. This study employed a qualitative research method. The data set consisting of multiple secondary data was analyzed based on the content analysis. Drawing on new institutional theory, this study contributes to previous studies on legitimation and business models by showing actors' arguments content and providing a more nuanced understanding of how actors work on legitimation and how they use context-specific explanations during this legitimation process.
Article
To realise their potential to lead in sustainability development, cities require both symbolic resources such as social capital and legitimacy and material resources such as financial and technical support. Recent research in urban studies has shown that cities overcome institutional barriers to urban sustainability by drawing on support from their wider environment. However, we argue that resource needs vary depending on whether cities spotlight or sideline sustainability. Drawing on in-depth interviews with sustainability managers in cities with variable seriousness about sustainability, and representatives of city networks and support organisations, we show that cities express different symbolic and material resource needs as well as means to acquire them. When cities express pragmatic needs, they seek to demonstrate political feasibility and look to peer cities for legitimation; when cities express progressive needs, they aim to push the boundaries of technical possibility and broadcast their achievements to the world. Since cities require dissimilar external support, skewed attention towards ‘leading’ cities in extant research limits our understanding of how cities can overcome institutional barriers to climate action, especially when these barriers are high. Our findings offer contributions to the literature on city strategies for climate change on the institutional drivers of urban sustainability.
Thesis
Full-text available
New venture legitimation has become an increasingly active topic of scholarly inquiry over the last two decades. Considering the lack of an up-to-date, wide-spectrum review on the issue, an integrative literature review is conducted to take stock of the current empirical and conceptual knowledge. The present study continues the work started by Überbacher (2014), and the classification developed in that review is now used to review scholarly output until February 2024. The present study reviews 245 peer-reviewed journal articles published between 1985 and February 2024 on new venture legitimation. The articles are classified with the perspective typology developed by Überbacher (2014). Shifts in the structure and substance of the literature are analyzed by comparing periods 1985–2012 and 2013–2/2024, which is in alignment with the cutoff year of Überbacher’s review. The findings illustrate a formidable, continuing expansion of the scholarly new venture legitimation literature. Most of the growth has stemmed from the burgeoning of empirical work, particularly over the last two decades. The most common type of study has been qualitative research with institutional theory as the theoretical framework. On the other hand, novel contributions based on organizational ecology have diminished. There has been only moderate activity in the conceptual proceedings on new venture legitimation, particularly towards the end of the period reviewed. Based on the review, an updated research agenda is proposed. The avenues identified as potential for further inquiry are 1) developing a unified legitimation strategy framework, 2) reaching consensus on legitimacy thresholds, 3) conceptualizing liabilities on a general level, 4) theorizing performative new venture legitimation, 5) explicating the metaphysical foundations of organizational legitimacy, and 6) aiming at more interdisciplinary legitimacy research. These six research streams align with understudied aspects of new venture legitimation identified by the present study.
Article
Full-text available
Research has long emphasized that being trusted is a central concern for leaders (Dirks & Ferrin, 2002), but an interesting and important question left unexplored is whether leaders feel trusted by each employee, and whether their felt trust is accurate. Across 2 field studies, we examined the factors that shape the accuracy of leaders' felt trust-or, their trust meta-accuracy-and the implications of trust meta-accuracy for the degree of relationship conflict between leaders and their employees. By integrating research on trust and interpersonal perception, we developed and tested hypotheses based on 2 theoretical mechanisms-an external signaling mechanism and an internal presumed reciprocity mechanism-that theory suggests shape leaders' trust meta-accuracy. In contrast to the existing literature on felt trust, our results reveal that leader trust meta-accuracy is shaped by an internal mechanism and the presumed reciprocity of trust relationships. We further find that whether trust meta-accuracy is associated with positive relational outcomes for leaders depends upon the level of an employee's actual trust in the leader. Our research contributes to burgeoning interest in felt trust by elucidating the mechanisms underlying trust meta-accuracy and suggesting practical directions for leaders who seek to accurately understand how much their employees trust them. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
For a policy-maker promoting the end of a harmful tradition, conformist social influence is a compelling mechanism. If an intervention convinces enough people to abandon the tradition, this can spill over and induce others to follow. A key objective is thus to activate such spillovers and amplify an intervention’s effects. With female genital cutting as a motivating example, we develop empirically informed analytical and simulation models to examine this idea. Even if conformity pervades decision-making, spillovers can range from irrelevant to indispensable. Our analysis highlights three considerations. First, ordinary forms of individual heterogeneity can severely limit spillovers, and understanding the heterogeneity in a population is essential. Second, although interventions often target samples of the population biased towards ending the harmful tradition, targeting a representative sample is a more robust way to achieve spillovers. Finally, if the harmful tradition contributes to group identity, the success of spillovers can depend critically on disrupting the link between identity and tradition.
Article
Institutional change is typically studied at the organizational field level; we leverage political culture to examine how the context in which these fields are embedded influence processes of institutional change within such fields. Specifically, we look at the effect of conservative political culture on legitimation and adoption of crowdfunding in the United States. We find that crowdfunding is less popular and more slowly legitimated in conservative regions. However, we also find that crowdfunding’s legitimacy is more important in these regions and that once a legitimacy threshold is reached, the adoption of crowdfunding in conservative regions surpasses that in liberal regions.
Article
We examine the cognitive and motivational process underlying the effect of consumers’ conservatism on their corporate social responsibility (CSR) perceptions of irresponsible versus responsible companies. Building on political psychology and system justification theory, we identify and test market system justification (MSJ) as a motivated social cognition underlying ideological differences in CSR perceptions and reactions. Using four empirical studies, we find that the relatively high MSJ of conservatives (compared to that of liberals) results in less critical CSR perceptions of irresponsible companies and, thus, in less penalizing reactions. Moreover, we find that conservatism influences CSR perceptions of irresponsible companies more strongly than of responsible companies because MSJ affects only perceptions of market behaviors that threaten notions of fair markets (i.e., low CSR). The results improve predictions of consumers’ perceptions and reactions to responsible and irresponsible companies.
Article
The sustainability problems of the production, distribution, and consumption of goods and services increasingly challenges the legitimacy of corporations. Corporate legitimacy, however, is vital to the survival of corporations in competitive environments. The literature distinguishes three strategies that corporations commonly employ to address legitimacy problems: adapt to external expectations, try to manipulate the perception of their stakeholders or engage in a discourse with those who question their legitimacy. This paper develops a theoretical framework for the application of different legitimacy strategies and suggests that corporations facing sustainability problems have to be able to activate all three legitimacy strategies, despite their inherent incompatibilities.