ArticlePDF Available

Mind-mindedness and parenting stress in mothers of preterm and full-term infants: The moderating role of perceived social support

Authors:

Abstract

The goal of this study was to examine the effects of preterm birth and maternal childbirth-related posttraumatic stress and parenting stress on maternal mind-mindedness (MM). The study also investigated the effects of perceived social support on parenting stress and MM. Sixty-five preterm (N = 32) and full-term (N = 33) mother-infant dyads were observed at 6 months. Measures of maternal MM were obtained from observations of mother-infant interaction. Mothers also provided ratings of their posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms, parenting stress, and perceived social support via an online survey. Experiencing a preterm birth did not affect mothers' use of mental state descriptors during mother-infant interaction. Neither childbirth-related posttraumatic stress nor parenting stress directly affected maternal ability to comment on the child's mental states appropriately. However, at medium and high levels of perceived social support, a negative association between parenting stress and MM was observed. Maternal perception of being emotionally supported by significant others promoted MM in mothers showing low or mild levels of parenting stress, but not in mothers experiencing high stress in parenting their infants. Results suggest that a proclivity to MM might be affected by the interaction between parenting stress and social support, rather than by childbirth-related variables, such as prematu-rity.
DOI: ./imhj.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Mind-mindedness and parenting stress in mothers of
preterm and full-term infants: The moderating role of
perceived social support
Chiara Suttora1Nicoletta Salerni2Elena Selvagno2Matteo Porro3,4
Silvana Gangi3,5Chiara Squarza3Laura Gardon3Odoardo Picciolini3,4
Department of Psychology, University of
Bologna, Bologna, Italy
Department of Psychology, University of
Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
Servizio di Follow-up del neonato a
rischio evolutivo, Fondazione IRCCS Ca’
Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico,
Milano, Italy
UO Medicina Fisica e Riabilitativa
Pediatrica, Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda
Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milano,
Italy
UO di Neonatologia e Terapia Intensiva
neonatale, Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda
Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milano,
Italy
Correspondence
Chiara Suttora, Department of Psychology,
University of Bologna, Viale Berti Pichat ,
 Bologna, Italy.
Email: chiara.suttora@unibo.it
Abstract
The goal of this study was to examine the effects of preterm birth and maternal
childbirth-related posttraumatic stress and parenting stress on maternal mind-
mindedness (MM). The study also investigated the effects of perceived social
support on parenting stress and MM. Sixty-five preterm (N=) and full-term
(N=) mother–infant dyads were observed at  months. Measures of mater-
nal MM were obtained from observations of mother–infant interaction. Mothers
also provided ratings of their posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms,
parenting stress, and perceived social support via an online survey. Experiencing
a preterm birth did not affect mothers’ use of mental state descriptors during
mother–infant interaction. Neither childbirth-related posttraumatic stress nor
parenting stress directly affected maternal ability to comment on the child’s men-
tal states appropriately. However, at medium and high levels of perceived social
support, a negative association between parenting stress and MM was observed.
Maternal perception of being emotionally supported by significant others pro-
moted MM in mothers showing low or mild levels of parenting stress, but not in
mothers experiencing high stress in parenting their infants. Results suggest that
a proclivity to MM might be affected by the interaction between parenting stress
and social support, rather than by childbirth-related variables, such as prematu-
rity.
KEYWORDS
childbirth-related posttraumatic stress, mind-mindedness, parenting stress, preterm birth,
social support
1 INTRODUCTION
Mind-mindedness (MM), a construct developed by Meins
(), represents the caregiver’s ability to read the child’s
internal mental states and treat her or him as an individ-
ual with a mind. Similarly to well-known constructs, such
as reflective functioning (Slade, ) and insightfulness
(Oppenheim & Koren-Karie, ), it refers to parental
engagement with the child at a mental level rather than in
response to her or his contingent physical and emotional
needs, that is, caregiver’s sensitivity.
In the first year of life, MM is measured as the caregiver’s
tendency to comment on the infant’s putative mental
states during social interaction (referred as “observational”
Infant Ment Health J. ;–. ©  Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health 1wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/imhj
2SUTTORA  .
MM), earning the description of a type of RF [Reflec-
tive Functioning] in action(Rosenblum, McDonough,
Sameroff, & Muzik, ). Later on, from toddlerhood
onward, MM assessment relies on the caregivers’ verbal
references to the child’s mental states during an interview
(defined “representational” MM), which simply asks care-
givers to describe their child (Meins, Fernyhough, Russell,
&Clark-Carter,). A relevant difference between the
two approaches is that the observational measure of MM
allows to differentiate the mind-related comments depend-
ing on their appropriateness or, vice versa, lack of attune-
ment to the infants’ ongoing mental state and activity in
the interaction (Meins et al., ).
A growing body of research on maternal MM, assessed
using the observational measure, which represents the
focus of the current study, indicated that MM promotes
parent–child relationships and the child’s socioemotional
and cognitive development. As for the parent–child
relationship, there is convincing evidence demonstrating
significant positive associations between the use of appro-
priate mind-related comments (AMRC) and parental
sensitivity (Laranjo, Bernier, & Meins, ; Meins et al.,
). Both Meins () and Laranjo et al. ()noted
that the propensity to treat infants as individuals with
minds could be a prerequisite for sensitivity; in order to
respond promptly and appropriately to infants’ cues, care-
givers should first attribute an intention to infants’ signals
and states. The relation between MM and attachment has
also been extensively investigated, with the main aim of
examining the capacity of MM to reduce the attachment
transmission gap by accounting for the relation between
caregiver’s attachment representation and infant attach-
ment security (Bernier & Dozier, ). In this context,
significant associations between caregiver’s proclivity to
MM and infant’s attachment security were found, even if
evidence of a robust predictive role of MM is still modest,
as recently reviewed by McMahon and Bernier ().
Regarding the effects on children developmental out-
comes, parental verbal references to infants’ mental state
during interaction have been related to the development
of children’s ability to attribute mental states to themselves
and others, that is, theory of mind (ToM), with the parental
use of AMRC being positively associated to children’s ToM
at preschool age (Kirk et al., ; Laranjo, Bernier, Meins,
& Carlson, ; Meins, Fernyhough, Arnott, Leekam, &
De Rosnay, ). Parental MM also seems to promote
children’s language development (Laranjo & Bernier, ;
Meins, Centifanti, Fernyhough, & Fishburn, )and,
although evidence is still limited, the development of
executive functioning (Bernier, Carlson, & Whipple, ;
Bernier, Carlson, Deschênes, & Matte-Gagné, ).
Considering its relevance for child development and the
quality of parent–child relationship, investigating which
variables can explain individual differences in parental
MM represents a crucial research question. As we describe
later on, research aiming at identifying the factors and
the conditions that may favor or somewhat interfere with
maternal MM is continuously growing. The present study
contributes to this line of research by investigating how
maternal MM can vary according to different childbirth
experiences (i.e., preterm birth) and different levels of
parenting stress. We hypothesize that experiencing stress
and/or trauma in the perinatal and postnatal period could
prevent mothers to attune to their children’s mental state
and activity, leading to a decrease in the use of AMRC. At
the same time, the role of perceived social support in mod-
erating the impact of maternal stress on MM is under inves-
tigation.
1.1 Understanding individual
differences in MM: The role of child and
parent characteristics
Since its origin, the nature of MM construct has been inten-
sively under debate. Relevant questions refer to whether
MM represents an individual parental trait that operates
similarly with different relational partners, or instead, it
can be considered as a quality of parent–child relationships
(Barreto, Fearon, Osório, Meins, & Martins, ; Meins,
Fernyhough, & Harris-Waller, ). Other questions con-
cern its stability across time (Kirk et al., ; McMa-
hon, Camberis, Berry, & Gibson, ) and across different
assessment approaches (i.e., observational vs. representa-
tional; Illingworth, MacLean, & Wiggs, ). A parallel
line of research that can contribute to clarifying the nature
of MM construct and better understand individual differ-
ences has investigated child, parent, and contextual vari-
ables that can interfere with or instead support parental
proclivity to interpret and attribute mental states to the
child.
Literature addressing the role of child-centered vari-
ables in understanding MM individual differences
observed significant relations between children’s emo-
tional, behavioral, and temperamental difficulties,
and low levels of maternal MM (Demers, Bernier,
Tarabulsy, & Provost, a; Walker, Wheatcroft, & Camic,
), but studies in this topic are still scant and often
restricted to studies approaching MM with the interview
measure.
Regarding parental characteristics, most studies
assessed maternal MM, with fathers’ MM being inves-
tigated only marginally. Mother-centered variables
correlated to MM were examined extensively, with result-
ing data reporting mixed findings. Meins, Fernyhough,
Arnott, Turner, and Leekam () reviewed the literature
SUTTORA  . 3
on this topic and documented the absence of significant
associations between MM and maternal variables such
as mothers’ socioeconomic status and educational level
(Meins et al., ;Walkeretal.,). It should be noted
that these studies assessed MM via interview. Considering
both observational and representational MM findings,
McMahon and Bernier () observed a mixed situation
with papers evenly divided in reporting significant and
nonsignificant associations with education and SES.
Other maternal variables contributing to MM were
investigated, for instance age and maternal psychological
functioning. Teen mothers were found to address fewer
appropriate positive mind-related comments to their
infants than adult mothers, and also fewer positive mind-
related comments, when the emotional valence of the
mental descriptors was coded (Demers, Bernier, Tarabulsy,
&Provost,b; Riva Crugnola, Ierardi, & Canevini,
). With respect to depression, data are conflicting
with studies reporting null associations (Crucianelli et al.,
; Meins et al., ) and others observing significant
relationships linking the presence of depression symp-
toms and the use of nonattuned mind-related comments
(NAMRC), although with small effect sizes (Meins et al.,
).
Rearing a child can also be a stressful experience, and
distress can in turn interfere with parental representations,
making it more difficult for caregivers to read and attribute
mental states to their child. Following this direction,
Demers et al. (a) investigated the associations between
parenting stress and maternal MM in a sample of mothers
of -month-old toddlers: findings showed that the use of
positive mind-related descriptors correlated with low lev-
els of parenting stress and with the perception of the child
as easy in temperament. Similarly, Dai, Lim, and Xu ()
observed that in mothers of  months old infants, the use
of AMRC significantly correlated with the stress perceived
in parenting, even controlling for maternal income and
education. As stress seems to undermine maternal MM,
Meins et al. () speculated that a lack of social support
would impact maternal tendency to use mind-related com-
ments during interaction; however, their data showed no
significant association with the perception of social sup-
port, as measured with a -item survey developed by Hen-
derson, Duncan-Jones, McAuley, and Ritchie ().
Other variables that captured the attention of
researchers in dealing with individual differences in
MM were related to mothers’ obstetric history. Meins et al.
() hypothesized that a mother’s perception of her preg-
nancy and labor experiences could play an essential role in
the development of adequate representations of the new-
born and the child’s future characteristics: women who
have more troubles in their obstetric histories would feel
less attuned to their infants’ emotional needs and mental
states from the beginning, resulting in lower MM attitudes.
Studies on this topic (Dai et al., ; Meins et al., )
confirmed that in Western and non-Western cultures, the
positive recollection of pregnancy and childbirth makes
mothers more likely to get attuned to their infants’ mental
state and to show AMRC during dyadic interactions.
Furthermore, mothers who recalled a highly positive
emotional reaction in the first contact with the newborn
later reported less parenting stress (Dai et al., ). Fur-
thermore, when addressing negative pregnancy and birth
experiences, Camisasca, Procaccia, Miragoli, Valtolina
and Di Blasio () indicated that mothers reporting
childbirth-related posttraumatic stress symptoms were
more likely to make fewer mind-related comments in the
interaction with their children aged  months.
1.2 Parental distress and MM: Effects of
perceived social support
A sudden interruption of pregnancy and its culmination
in a preterm birth constitute a very special condition that
could have a significant impact on parental psychological
health, disturbing the process of building representa-
tions of the child and of oneself as a parent (Spinelli
et al., ). Mothers and fathers coping with preterm
birth acknowledge the traumatic load of this experience,
reporting feelings of powerlessness and shock due to
the unpredictability of the event (O’Donovan & Nixon,
). Literature indicated that  months after childbirth,
mothers kept experiencing symptoms of posttraumatic
stress, with some evidence showing higher levels of
trauma being associated with infants’ low birth weight,
length of hospitalization, and illness severity (Feeley et al.,
; Holditch-Davis, Bartlett, Blickman, & Miles, ).
When not resolved, early maternal childbirth-related
posttraumatic stress could significantly affect parenting
experience and behavior. Accordingly, literature shows
that the presence of posttraumatic symptoms mediates the
association between preterm birth condition and levels of
parenting stress; mothers who experienced their infant’s
birth as a traumatic event reported higher levels of parent-
ing stress, but mothers who did not experience the event as
traumatic showed lower levels of parental stress (Suttora,
Spinelli, & Monzani, ). Other reports indicated that
preterm birth might lead parents to perceive their role
and the rearing of their child as particularly stressful and
demanding, with a higher level of parenting stress being
documented in both mothers and fathers (Chang & Fine,
; Gray, Edwards, O’Callaghan, Cuskelly, & Gibbons,
). However, findings in this field are controversial
because other studies obtained different results, indicating
no or small effects of preterm birth on parenting stress
4SUTTORA  .
(Howe, Sheu, Wang, & Hsu, ; Schappin, Wijnroks,
Venema, & Jongmans, ).
To date, there is only one study investigating the associa-
tion between preterm birth and maternal MM. In this study
(Costantini, Coppola, Fasolo, & Cassibba, ), mothers
of -month-old preterm children showed MM abilities
similar to those of mothers of full-term peers, but vari-
ables such as parenting stress were not considered in the
study design. The researchers focused on the role of MM
in children’s developmental outcomes, and findings indi-
cated that maternal MM contributed significantly to the
growth of linguistic abilities in the second year of life only
for preterm infants, suggesting a different impact of mater-
nal MM due to birth condition.
In this light, the present study aims at exploring
how birth condition (preterm vs. full-term), maternal
childbirth-related traumatic symptoms, and maternal lev-
els of parenting stress can relate to individual differences in
maternal MM measured at  months of age during mother–
child interaction. As the literature suggests (Holditch-
Davis et al., ), at  months mothers of preterm infants
are still recovering from the traumatic effects of child-
birth; thus, this age may constitute an interesting period for
such an investigation. Significant intercorrelations among
birth conditions, perinatal PTSD symptoms, and parenting
stress are expected. Our main hypothesis is that the sever-
ity of preterm birth and high levels of posttraumatic symp-
toms and parenting stress would interfere with mater-
nal development of adequate representations of the child,
leading to lower levels of MM.
Finally, maternal perceived social support will also be
considered. The perception of being supported by one’s
partner, family, or close friends could have different effects
on parenting stress and related outcomes. Cohen and Wills
() proposed two models describing the impact of social
support on individual wellbeing. The direct or main effect
model claims a direct protective effect of social support
on individuals’ wellbeing, independently from the level
of displayed stress, whereas the buffer model sustains the
idea of perceived social support as a moderator of the
relation between stress and adverse outcomes. Literature
often confirms both models, with data showing main and
moderating effects: a study based on an extensive sample
(Mcconnell, Breitkreuz, & Savage, ) found that social
support had strong direct effects on parent–child health
and wellbeing and a smaller, but significant, role in mod-
erating the impact of parenting stress on positive parent–
child interaction.
In this frame, we aim at understanding whether per-
ceived social support contribution to maternal attitudes
toward children’s mental states could be direct and/or
could buffer the negative effects of maternal distress on
MM.
2 METHOD
2.1 Participants
The sample includes  Italian mother–infant dyads (
male,  female;  firstborn), with  infants born preterm
(PT: F =%; firstborn =%) and the remaining  infants
born at gestational term (FT: F =%; firstborn =%).
Preterm and full-term infants did not differ in gender, Χ
(, ) =., p=., or birth order, Χ(, ) =., p=..
Inclusion criteria for preterm infants were having a
weight of less than , g and a gestational age < weeks
at birth. Exclusion criteria for the preterm and full-term
group were the presence of genetic abnormalities, severe
neurofunctional impairment, defined as Neurofunctional
Assessment greater than  (Picciolini, Gianni, Vegni,
Fumagalli, & Mosca, ), and/or neurosensory disabil-
ities (blindness or deafness). On average, preterm children
were hospitalized for . (N=;SD =.) days and
had a gestational age of . (SD =.) weeks and a birth
weight of , (SD =) g, whereas full-term children
had a gestational age of . (SD =.) weeks and a birth
weight of , (SD =) g.
Because infant birth weight and gestational age were
highly correlated (r=., p<.), a Prematurity Index
was calculated for all infants by standardizing and sum-
ming these two variables (Poehlmann, Schwichtenberg,
Bolt, & Dilworth-Bart, ). The Prematurity Index scores
ranged from –. to –. (M=–., SD =.) for
preterm infants and from . to . (M=., SD =.)
for term infants, with higher scores representing less pre-
maturity.
Mothers had a mean age of . (SD =.) years in
the preterm group and of . (SD =.) in the full-
term group, with a significant difference between groups, t
() =–., p=.; maternal education was lower for the
preterm group, with . (SD =.) years for PT mothers
and . (SD =.) for FT mothers, t() =., p=..
Only two mothers out of  were originally from another
country, but they were very fluent in Italian, their second
language. All mothers were Caucasian.
Parents of preterm infants eligible for the study were
approached for enrollment in the follow-up service of the
Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Poli-
clinico; this service only includes patients from its own
neonatal intensive care unit (NICU). Parents of full-term
infants were recruited to participate in the study by writ-
ten invitations that were sent to families based on birth
records provided by neighboring cities. The ethics commit-
tees of authors’ institutions approved the study and written
informed consent was obtained from all parents.
Hospitalization data from one preterm participant were missing.
SUTTORA  . 5
2.2 Procedure and measures
When the infants were -month old (M=., SD =.
for term infants; M=., SD =. corrected age for
preterm infants), dyads were invited to participate in a
videotaped play session lasting from  to  min, depend-
ing on the infant’s state and fussiness. Face-to-face inter-
actions with the infant in a baby seat were chosen in order
to favor the relational exchanges in dyads, as most of the
infants at this age are still not able to seat properly with-
out support. Dyads were free to use some age-appropriate
toys available in the room. For each dyad, maternal MM
was assessed using the first  min of the videotaped inter-
action to avoid infant’s fatigue and to obtain comparable
measures. First, maternal utterances directed at the child
were fully transcribed and then coded according to the
MM coding manual, version . (Meins & Fernyhough,
). A coder classified each maternal comment as mind-
related if it included a reference to the infant’s desires,
wishes, preferences, intentions, epistemic states, emotions
and attempts to manipulate others’ beliefs, and mental pro-
cesses, such as memories or decisions. In addition, mater-
nal utterances that were meant to be said by the infant
were coded as mind-related. Once a comment was coded
as mind-related, the coder further classified it as appropri-
ate or nonattuned according to the criteria included in the
Coding Manual . (Meins & Fernyhough, ). A mind-
related comment was considered appropriate when the
coder agreed on the maternal interpretation of the child’s
current mental state based on child observable behavior.
Mind-related comments were coded as nonattuned when
mothers failed to identify children’s current internal state
correctly. A second coder, trained by the first author and
blind to the study’s hypotheses, coded a randomly selected
% of the mother–infant interactions ( out of  pairs).
Interrater reliability between the two coders, tested with
intraclass correlation, was . for appropriate comments
and . for nonattuned comments.
In the present study, the amounts of AMRC and NAMRC
were used as measures of maternal MM. As suggested by
Meins and Fernyhough (), to control for maternal ver-
bosity, proportional frequencies of AMRC and NAMRC
were calculated relative to the total amount of maternal
verbal utterances.
Following the play session, mothers completed an
online survey including several questionnaires. Among
these, mothers completed the Perinatal PTSD Ques-
tionnaire (PPQ)—Modified version (Callahan, Borja, &
Hynan, ) and the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form
(PSI-SF; Abidin, ; Italian validation by Guarino, Di
Blasio, D’Alessio, Camisasca, & Serantoni, ). The PPQ
is a -item questionnaire that assesses childbirth-related
posttraumatic symptoms, including intrusiveness or re-
experiencing, avoidant behaviors, hyperarousal, and gen-
eral posttraumatic symptoms. Mothers were asked to indi-
cate on a -point Likert scale ( =notatallto  =often, for
more than a month) how often they experienced the symp-
toms after childbirth. The total PPQ score ranges from 
to , with high values representing greater posttraumatic
symptom severity (α=.).ThePSI-SFisacommonlyused
measure of stress related to parenting, including  items
rated from  to  on a Likert scale ( =strongly disagree;
=strongly agree) assessing parental distress, parent–child
dysfunctional interaction, and the burden of dealing with
a difficult child. In the present work, only the total stress
scale score (PSI) was considered, with high values indicat-
ing greater parenting stress (α=.).
Mothers were also asked to complete the Multidimen-
sional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; Zimet,
Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, ), a -item self-report ques-
tionnaire that investigates the social support a person expe-
riences from family, friends, and significant others. The
Italian version of the scale was used in this study (Di Fabio
& Busoni, ). Participants expressed their agreement
with the statements on a -point Likert scale ( =very
strongly disagree;=very strongly agree). High values rep-
resented high levels of perceived social support (α=.).
2.3 Analysis plan
Preliminary analyses included exploring data distribution
and examining differences between mothers of preterm
and full-term infants regarding the main variables, that
is, childbirth-related posttraumatic stress and parenting
stress, perceived social support, and MM using indepen-
dent t-tests. In the main analysis section, consistent with
our aims, associations among study variables in the whole
sample were examined using Pearson’s correlation. Mod-
eration analysis was then used to examine the main and
buffering effects of perceived social support on the asso-
ciation between maternal stress and MM. Two modera-
tion analyses were performed with the PROCESS macro
(Hayes, ) using IBM SPSS Statistics version . The
PROCESS macro uses bootstrapping methods to assess
the interaction between design variables. The interactions
between independent and moderating variables on the
dependent variable were tested at each of the three levels of
the moderator (, , and +SD). Significant interactions
are those in which % confidence intervals do not contain
zero. All variables were standardized into z-scores before
being entered into the moderation models.
6SUTTORA  .
TABLE 1 Descriptive statistics of the study main measures by group
Tot al sam ple
(N=65)
Preterm group
(N=32)
Full-term group
(N=33)
Mean (SD)Mean(SD)Mean(SD)t(p)
PPQ . (.) . (.) . (.) –. (.)
PSI . (.) . (.) . (.) . (.)
MSPSS . (.) . (.) . (.) . (.)
AMRC . (.) . (.) . (.) –. (.)
NAMRC . (.) . (.) . (.) –. (.)
Abbreviations: PPQ, Perinatal PTSD Questionnaire;PSI, Parenting Stress Index; MSPSS, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support; AMRC, mind-related
appropriate comments; NAMRC, mind-related nonattuned comments.
TABLE 2 Pearson correlations between the study variables
12345
. PI
. PPQ –. (.)a
. PSI . (.) . (.)
. MSPSS . (.) –. (.) –. (.)
. AMRC –. (.) –. (.) –. (.) –. (.)
. NAMRC –. (.) . (.) . (.) . (.) . (.)
Abbreviations: PI, Prematurity Index; PPQ,Perinatal PTSD Questionnaire; PSI, Parenting Stress Index; MSPSS, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support;
AMRC, mind-related appropriate comments; NAMRC, mind-related nonattuned comments.
ar(p).
3RESULTS
3.1 Preliminary analyses
The descriptive statistics of posttraumatic stress, parenting
stress, perceived social support, and MM measures in FT
and PT mothers are shown in Table . Estimates of skew-
ness and kurtosis for PPQ, PSI, and MSPSS scores were
within reasonable limits, with skewness ranging from
–. to . and kurtosis from –. to .. However,
scales were not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk test,
p<.). Distribution for AMRC was normal, with data low
in skewness, ., and kurtosis, –.. The data for nonat-
tuned were non normal, highly skewed, ., and high
in kurtosis, .. Because data were skewed, we also ran
nonparametric tests (Mann–Whitney’s Uand Spearman’s
ρ). The results did not differ, so t-tests and Pearson’s cor-
relations are reported in Table and Table , respectively.
The results of independent t-tests comparing the two
groups of mothers did not reveal any significant differ-
ences due to birth condition. Childbirth-related posttrau-
matic symptoms, assessed with PPQ, were similar among
mothers. In our sample,  mothers, eight of FT (%) and
seven of PT dyads (%), were at risk of perinatal PTSD—
according to Callahan et al. (), PPQ scores equal to or
higher than  are at risk, with scores ranging from  to
. As for the PSI, the results revealed that  of FT mothers
(%) and seven of PT mothers (%) scored at or above the
PSI’s th percentile (PSI >), exhibiting medium to high
distress in parenting their children. Mothers of preterm
and full-term children reported similar levels of social sup-
port.
As to the key study outcome, namely, maternal MM,
t-test results indicated the absence of statistically signifi-
cant differences due to birth condition. In agreement with
literature results, AMRC and NAMRC represented the %
and %, respectively, of the total number of comments.
3.2 Main analyses
Pearson’s correlation analyses were performed to inves-
tigate the associations between childbirth-related PTSD
symptoms, parenting stress, perceived social support, and
the use of mind-related comments during mother–infant
interactions. To maximize the statistical power and given
the results of our preliminary analyses, data from the
two groups of mothers were aggregated and the Prema-
turity Index was considered as a measure of prematurity
severity.
As reported in Table , maternal scores in the PPQ sig-
nificantly correlated with PSI scores; specifically, mothers
with pronounced posttraumatic stress symptoms related
to childbirth were also at higher risk of experiencing
parenting stress at  months. Furthermore, high parenting
stress was associated with lower levels of perceived social
SUTTORA  . 7
TABLE 3 Moderation analyses of PPQ and MSPSS on appropriate mind-mindedness (AMRC)
Model BSE t pLLCI ULCI
Constant –. . –. . –. .
PPQ –. . –. . –. .
MSPSS –. . –. . –. .
PPQ ×MSPSS –. . –. . –. .
Model summary RR
p
. . .
PPQ ×MSPSS Rchange p
. .
Abbreviations: PPQ, Perinatal PTSD Questionnaire; MSPSS, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support.
TABLE 4 Moderation analyses of PSI and MSPSS on appropriate mind-mindedness
Model BSE t pLLCI ULCI
Constant –. . –. . –. .
PSI –. . –. . –. .
MSPSS . . . . –. .
PSI ×MSPSS –. . –. . –. –.
Model summary RR
p
. . .
PSI ×MSPSS Rchange p
. .
Abbreviations: PSI, Parenting Stress Index; MSPSS, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support.
support (MSPSS). The results indicated a lack of signifi-
cant associations between the use of AMRC and NAMRC
and maternal reported levels of childbirth-related posttrau-
matic stress and parenting stress. Associations between
MM measures and MSPSS scores were also not significant.
The severity of prematurity failed to account for individual
differences in MM, childbirth-related posttraumatic stress,
parenting stress, and perceived social support.
As highlighted in the introduction, social support may
buffer the adverse effects of stress on parenting behaviors
and attitudes; in this sense, the interaction among mater-
nal stress, posttraumatic and/or parenting stress, and the
level of perceived social support may explain differences
in maternal MM. Only AMRC were considered in the fol-
lowing analyses. The latter decision relies upon previous
results indicating that only AMRC relate to positive par-
enting and maternal sensitivity (Meins et al., ). Two
moderation models were hypothesized.
The first model considered how levels of perceived social
support (MSPSS) moderated the association between peri-
natal posttraumatic symptoms and maternal MM. The
interactions were tested at each of three levels of the mod-
erator variable (, , and +SD; i.e., at low, medium, and
high levels of perceived social support). This first model
yielded no significant results, as shown in Table , thus
implying the absence of significant interactions between
the level of posttraumatic stress and perceived social
support on maternal MM. The second model tested the
moderation by perceived social support of the association
between parenting stress and maternal MM. The results
reported in Table indicated that although there was no
direct effect of parenting stress on maternal MM, the inter-
action between PSI and MSPSS was significant (B=–.,
t=–., p=., f=.) at average values of the moder-
ator (conditional effect =–., t=–., p=., % CI [–
., –.]) and at +SD of the moderator (conditional
effect =–., t=–., p=., % CI [–., –.]). The
interaction was not significant at – SD of the moderator
(conditional effect =–., t=., p=., % CI [–.,
.]). The effect size of the interaction was in the medium
range according to Aguinis, Beaty, Boik, and Pierce ().
At medium and high levels of perceived social support,
parenting stress was negatively related to maternal MM
(Figure ). For low levels of perceived social support, the
interaction was not significant (t=., p=., %
CI [–., .]).
4DISCUSSION
Considering the role of parental MM in promoting child
development and parent–child relationship, we aimed at
8SUTTORA  .
FIGURE 1 Perceived social support ×parenting stress on
appropriate mind-related comments (AMRC)
examining the impact on MM of a set of variables related to
childbirth and parenting experience. The principal intent
of the present study was to explore the effects of maternal
stress on maternal MM in a sample of mothers of preterm
and full-term infants aged  months. The study also con-
sidered the support that mothers perceived from their fam-
ilies, friends, and significant others as a variable that might
play a protective role in their psychological well-being and
parenting behaviors.
As reviewed in the introduction, previous literature sug-
gests that maternal MM could be affected by negative and
traumatic experiences during pregnancy and childbirth
(Camisasca, Procaccia, Miragoli, Valtolina, & Blasio, ;
Dai et al., ). Experiencing pregnancy and labor as neg-
ative can distort early representations of the child, as the
mother does not feel attunement with the newborn from
the outset (Meins et al., ). Considering preterm birth
as a potentially traumatic condition, we aimed at investi-
gating MM and its association with the caregiver’s perina-
tal PTSD symptoms and parenting stress in this population
too. Our findings suggested that mothers of preterm and
full-term children made comparable amounts of appropri-
ate and nonattuned mind-related comments during inter-
action. The experience of preterm birth did not affect
maternal tendency to refer to the child in terms of men-
tal and inner states. This result is consistent with the study
of Costantini et al. () that failed to highlight any dif-
ferences in the MM of mothers of preterm children in the
second year of life. Accordingly, recent results from the
study of Ruiz, Witting, Ahnert, and Piskernik ()indi-
cated no differences due to birth status in the overall lev-
els of reflective functioning between mothers of term and
preterm children. However, specifically investigating the
topics extracted from the Parental Development Interview
(Slade, Aber, Bresgi, Berger, & Kaplan, )withalatent
Dirichlet allocation modeling technique, the authors high-
lighted that mothers of children born preterm tended to
reflect more intensively than mothers of full-term children
on their children’s thoughts and feelings. It could then be
that mothers of preterm reveal similar uses of MM during
interaction than mothers of full-term infants, but, when
asked to reflect on their infants’ thoughts and feelings,
they exhibit a higher understanding of their infant mental
states.
In our findings, preterm birth and its severity also had
no effects on mothers in terms of stress, with mothers
from the two conditions reporting comparable levels of
posttraumatic and parenting stress. These results could be
unexpected considering the suddenness of preterm birth,
its threatening effects on a child’s survival, well-being,
and development. One possible interpretation could
concern the fact that preterm infants with severe neuro-
functional impairments and/or neurosensory disabilities
were excluded from the present study, as their conditions
are rare and difficult to compare with those of healthier
preterm (Picciolini et al., ; Tommiska, Östberg, & Fell-
man, ). Empirical evidence highlights that parents of
children with developmental disabilities are at increased
risk of experiencing psychological stress and trauma
compared to other parents (Lee, ). Future studies
should investigate how preterm birth and the presence
of developmental or neurosensory disabilities interact in
affecting parents’ well-being and emotional reactions.
Another explanation is that posttraumatic stress reac-
tions in preterm parents may depend upon the level of hos-
pital care and support provided. In a work by Montirosso,
Provenzi, Calciolari, and Borgatti (), the parental
experience of stressors related to the NICU environment
was assessed in  Italian NICUs with the Parental Stressor
Scale: Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (PSS: NICU; Miles,
Funk, & Carlson, ). Findings showed that maternal
perception of care and support during hospitalization, cal-
culated separately for each NICU, made a significant con-
tribution to reducing maternal stress. These findings can
help give sense to the observed results, as acknowledged in
the study’s limitations section. At the same time, it is worth
noting that risk factors for the development of posttrau-
matic stress symptoms, such as obstetric complications,
inadequate intrapartum care, and history of psychological
problems, are often reported in full-term pregnancies, a
piece of evidence that could help to understand the lack
of significant differences in our sample (Ayers, Harris,
Sawyer,Parfitt,&Ford,; Simpson & Catling, ).
Putting aside the preterm birth, one of the main pur-
poses of the study was to understand the effects of
childbirth-related posttraumatic stress, parenting stress,
and social support on maternal MM. The results showed
the absence of associations among maternal MM, perina-
tal PTSD symptoms, and parenting stress; thus, childbirth-
related posttraumatic stress and parenting stress showed
SUTTORA  . 9
no direct effects on maternal MM, in contrast with previ-
ous studies’ findings. Camisasca et al. () observed that
mothers who used fewer mental descriptors during the
interaction with their  months old children were likely
to report more childbirth-related PTSD symptoms. As for
parenting stress, both Dai et al. () and Demers et al.
(a) observed negative associations with the caregivers’
tendency to appropriately attribute mental states to their
children when children were  and  months old, respec-
tively.
However, other analyses helped in clarifying these pre-
liminary results. First, parenting stress resulted negatively
correlated with perceived social support; mothers who per-
ceived good support from their partners, families, and
friends expressed less stress in dealing with the care of
their children. In this sense, maternal perception of being
sustained by her significant others may reduce parenting
stress appraisal or even prevent it. At the same time—
because our analyses are correlational—low levels of par-
enting stress may lead mothers to perceive their social net-
works as more supportive. Second, the results of moder-
ation analyses showed a significant interaction between
perceived social support and parenting stress on MM, at
specific levels of social support. For mothers reporting low
support from their interpersonal relationships, the asso-
ciation between parenting stress and maternal MM was
not significant. Contrariwise, for adequate and high lev-
els of social support, high levels of MM were associated
with low levels of stress. In other words, a high social sup-
port promoted MM for mothers experiencing no difficul-
ties in child-rearing but still resulted in low MM for moth-
ers reporting stress in dealing with their parental tasks.
We can suppose that mothers who perceive support from
their network, but at the same time still perceive high lev-
els of stress from parenting demands, can feel this pat-
tern as incongruent and attribute their difficulties to the
child or to dyadic relational aspects, reducing their ability
to comprehend and read the child’s mental states. In our
data, social support did not moderate the adverse effects of
stress on parenting behaviors but instead promoted MM in
mothers showing no or mild parenting stress. In this light,
our findings did not confirm the stress-buffering hypoth-
esis on social support, but rather provided data on a pro-
motive effect of support on maternal MM. Summarizing,
the perception of social support—a qualitative rather than
a quantitative aspect of this construct—resulted associ-
ated with lowered parenting stress overall, which could be
interpreted as a direct effect on stress, but fails at buffering
the negative effect of parental stress on MM.
Östberg and Hagekull () addressed how social sup-
port, and other variables such as daily workload or child
fussiness, affect parenting stress in a sample of Swedish
mothers, testing Cohen and Wills’s () dual model
of social support. They found social support to have
direct and indirect—through maternal workload—effects
on parenting stress; however, moderating effects of social
support were not observed as the model was shown to
apply to all mothers, independently from the amount of
social support reported. Other studies succeeded in finding
buffering effects of support on parenting stress in moth-
ers of preterm and full-term children (Crnic, Greenberg,
Ragozin, Robinson, & Basham, ; Lutz et al., ). For
instance, Lutz et al. () found that maternal percep-
tion of information support by family members (i.e., the
provision of knowledge and facts) improved child’s behav-
ioral regulation—assessed with the Parent-Child Early
Relational Assessment (PCERA; Clark, )—for moth-
ers who perceived more stress related to parenting but not
for those reporting less stress. Mothers who feel as more
demanding the task of parenting may benefit more from
information support as reflected in more positive mother–
child interaction. In our study, the measure of social sup-
port (MSPSS) captures the perception of emotional social
support rather than other kinds of social support, such as
information support, instrumental support (i.e., provision
of concrete resources, such as money or childcare), pres-
ence of tangible aid, and positive social interaction (Arm-
strong, Birnie-Lefcovitch, & Ungar, ; Lutz et al., ).
In this sense, it is possible to hypothesize that although
more concrete types of social support can moderate the
adverse effects of stress on the quality of parent–child inter-
action, emotional support can have beneficial effects only
in mothers not experiencing significant parenting difficul-
ties. In a similar direction, Guralnick, Hammond, Neville,
and Connor () found that for parents of children
with developmental disabilities high parenting support—
in form of advices on parenting specific problems and
assistance with child care responsibilities—promoted less
child- and parent-related stress, whereas emotional sup-
port and validation from others exerted limited effects on
parental stress. As social support is a multidimensional
construct, differences in the effects of support on stress
and other outcomes can depend on the functional and/or
structural aspects of social support considered in the study
design (Armstrong et al., ; Cohen & Wills, ; Gural-
nick et al., ).
5 CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS AND
LIMITATIONS
A better understanding of the effects of different dimen-
sions of social support on parenting stress and related out-
comes could inform healthcare professionals on how to
intervene in such circumstances. Based on our findings,
interventions and programs directed at promoting parents’
10 SUTTORA  .
social support can help them in their childrearing prac-
tices by reducing stress, both in typical and atypical con-
ditions, such as preterm birth. However, our results also
highlighted that high emotional social support per se is
not enough to reduce the negative effects of high parenting
stress on maternal MM. The early identification of parents
at risk of acute stress is pivotal to provide them with social,
educational, and psychological interventions oriented at
supporting and promoting their ability to attune to their
infants’ mental states, such as the Minding the Baby (Slade
et al., ) and the Mothering Inside Out (Suchman, Ord-
way, de las Heras, & McMahon, )programs.
Before closing the discussion, some important limita-
tions of our study design deserve to be mentioned. The
first refers to the study sample; putting aside the sample
size, the preterm group includes only very low birth weight
infants from a single NICU unit. As studies show, care and
support may vary dramatically from one NICU to another,
leading to results that are not always easy to interpret.
In this study, given the lack of differences in stress and
MM among the mothers in the groups, we may speculate
about the availability of good-quality NICU care; however,
this conclusion should be better addressed with specific
studies. A second limitation refers to the cross-sectional
nature of the study’s design, which does not permit to
draw conclusions about the causal relationships among
the measured variables. Longitudinal designs investigat-
ing preterm birth, stress, and different dimensions of social
support as predictors of maternal MM are needed. At last,
another limitation and an important topic for future stud-
ies concerns the lack of data on children’s development
and well-being. Studies of parenting are fundamental but
can lack impact when they do not consider children’s
developmental outcomes.
The study involves human participants. All procedures
performed in studies involving human participants were in
accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional
and/or national research committee (Ethic committee
of the University of Milano Bicocca, n) and with
the  Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments
or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was
obtained from all individual participants involved in the
study.
ORCID
Chiara Suttora https://orcid.org/---
REFERENCES
Abidin, R. R. (). Parenting Stress Index: A measure of the parent–
child system. In C. P. Zalaquett & R. J. Wood (Eds.), Evaluating
stress: A book of resources (pp. –). Lanham, MD: Scarecrow
Education.
Aguinis, H., Beaty, J. C., Boik, R. J., & Pierce,C. A. (). Effect size
and power in assessing moderating effects of categorical variables
using multiple regression: A -year review. Journal of Applied
Psychology,90(), –. https://doi.org/./-...

Armstrong, M. I., Birnie-Lefcovitch, S., & Ungar, M. T. (). Path-
ways between social support, family well being, quality of par-
enting, and child resilience: What we know. Journal of Child and
Family Studies,14(), –. https://doi.org/./s--
-
Ayers, S., Harris, R., Sawyer,A., Parfitt, Y., & Ford,E. (). Posttrau-
matic stress disorder after childbirth: Analysis of symptom presen-
tation and sampling. Journal of Affective Disorders,119(–), –
. https://doi.org/./j.jad...
Barreto, A. L., Fearon, R. M. P., Osório, A., Meins, E., & Martins,
C. (). Are adult mentalizing abilities associated with mind-
mindedness? International Journal of Behavioral Development,
40(), –. https://doi.org/./
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M., & Matte-Gagné, C. ().
Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: A
closer look at the caregiving environment. Developmental Science,
15(), –. https://doi.org/./j.-...x
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (). From external regu-
lation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young chil-
dren’s executive functioning. Child Development,81(), –.
https://doi.org/./j.-...x
Bernier, A., & Dozier, M. (). Bridging the attachment transmis-
sion gap: The role of maternal mind-mindedness. International
Journal of Behavioral Development,27(), –. https://doi.org/
./
Callahan, J. L., Borja, S. E., & Hynan, M. T. (). Modification of the
Perinatal PTSD Questionnaire to enhance clinical utility. Journal
of Perinatology,26(), –. https://doi.org/./sj.jp.
Camisasca, E., Procaccia, R., Miragoli, S., Valtolina, G. G., & Bla-
sio, Di (). Maternal mind-mindedness as a linking mecha-
nism between childbirth-related posttraumatic stress symptoms
and parenting stress. Health Care for Women International,38(),
–. https://doi.org/./..
Chang, Y., & Fine, M. A. (). Modeling parenting stress trajecto-
ries among low-income young mothers across the child’s second
and third years: Factors accounting for stability and change. Jour-
nal of Family Psychology,21(), –. https://doi.org/./
-...
Clark, R. (). The parent–child early relational assessment: Instru-
ment and manual. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Medical
School, Department of Psychiatry.
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (). Stress, social support, and the buffer-
ing hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin,98(), –. https://doi.
org/./-...
Costantini, A., Coppola, G., Fasolo, M., & Cassibba, R. (). Preterm
birth enhances the contribution of mothers’ mind-mindedness to
infants’ expressive language development: A longitudinal investi-
gation. Infant Behavior and Development,49,.https://doi.
org/./j.infbeh...
Crnic, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., Ragozin, A. S., Robinson, N. M., &
Basham, R. B. (). Effects of stress and social support on moth-
ers and premature and full-term infants. Child Development,54(),
–. https://doi.org/./j.- ..tb.x
SUTTORA  . 11
Crucianelli, L., Wheatley, L., Filippetti, M. L., Jenkinson, P. M.,
Kirk, E., & Fotopoulou, A. K. (). The mindedness of mater-
nal touch: An investigation of maternal mind-mindedness and
mother-infant touch interactions. Developmental Cognitive Neuro-
science,35,.https://doi.org/./j.dcn...
Dai, Q., Lim, A. K., & Xu, Q. J. (). The relations between maternal
mind-mindedness, parenting stress and obstetric history among
Chinese mothers. Early Child Development and Care,189(), –
. https://doi.org/./..
Demers, I., Bernier, A., Tarabulsy, G. M., & Provost, M. A. (a).
Maternal and child characteristics as antecedents of maternal
mind-mindedness. Infant Mental Health Journal,31(), –.
https://doi.org/./imhj.
Demers, I., Bernier, A., Tarabulsy, G. M., & Provost, M. A. (b).
Mind-mindedness in adult and adolescent mothers: Relations to
maternal sensitivity and infant attachment. International Journal
of Behavioral Development,34(), –. https://doi.org/./

Di Fabio, A., & Busoni, L. (). Misurare il supporto sociale per-
cepito: Proprietapsicometriche della Multidimensional Scale of
Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) [Measuring perceived social
support: Psychometric properties of the Mutidimensional Scale
of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) in a sample of university
students]. Risorsa Uomo,14(), –. https://doi.org/./

Feeley, N., Zelkowitz, P., Cormier, C., Charbonneau, L., Lacroix, A.,
& Papageorgiou, A. (). Posttraumatic stress among mothers of
very low birthweight infants at  months after discharge from the
neonatal intensive care unit. Applied Nursing Research,24(), –
. https://doi.org/./j.apnr...
Gray, P. H., Edwards, D. M., O’Callaghan, M. J., Cuskelly, M., & Gib-
bons, K. (). Parenting stress in mothers of very preterm infants
- Influence of development, temperament and maternal depres-
sion. Early Human Development,89(), –. https://doi.org/
./j.earlhumdev...
Guarino, A., Di Blasio, P., D’Alessio, M., Camisasca, E., & Serantoni,
G. (). Validazione italiana del Parenting Stress Index forma
breve per l’identificazione precoce di sistemi relazionali genitore-
bambino stressanti. Firenze, Italy: Giunti OS.
Guralnick, M. J., Hammond, M. A., Neville, B., & Connor, R. T.
(). The relationship between sources and functions of social
support and dimensions of child- and parent-related stress. Jour-
nal of Intellectual Disability Research,52(), –. https://doi.
org/./j.- ...x
Hayes, A. F. (). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for
observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process
modeling . Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/
Henderson, S., Duncan-Jones, P., McAuley, H., & Ritchie, K. ().
The patient’s primary group. The British Journal of Psychiatry,
132(), –. https://doi.org/./S
Holditch-Davis, D., Bartlett, T. R., Blickman, A. L., & Miles, M. S.
(). Posttraumatic stress symptoms in mothers of premature
infants. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing,
32(), –. https://doi.org/./
Howe, T. H., Sheu, C. F., Wang, T. N., & Hsu, Y. W. (). Parent-
ing stress in families with very low birth weight preterm infants in
early infancy. Research in Developmental Disabilities,35(), –
. https://doi.org/./j.ridd...
Illingworth, G., MacLean, M., & Wiggs, L. (). Maternal mind-
mindedness: Stability over time and consistency across relation-
ships. European Journal of Developmental Psychology,13(), –
. https://doi.org/./..
Kirk, E., Pine, K., Wheatley, L., Howlett, N., Schulz, J., & Fletcher, B.
C. (). A longitudinal investigation of the relationship between
maternal mind-mindedness and theory of mind. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology,33(), –. https://doi.org/./
bjdp.
Laranjo, J., & Bernier, A. (). Children’s expressive language in
early toddlerhood: Links to prior maternal mind-mindedness.
Early Child Development and Care,183(), –. https://doi.
org/./..
Laranjo, J., Bernier, A., & Meins, E. (). Associations between
maternal mind-mindedness and infant attachment security:
Investigating the mediating role of maternal sensitivity. Infant
Behavior and Development,31(), –. https://doi.org/./
j.infbeh...
Laranjo, J., Bernier, A., Meins, E., & Carlson, S. M. (). The roles
of maternal mind-mindedness and infant security of attachment
in predicting preschoolers’ understanding of visual perspective
taking and false belief. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
125(), –. https://doi.org/./j.jecp...
Lee, J. (). Maternal stress, well-being, and impaired sleep in
mothers of children with developmental disabilities: A literature
review. Research in Developmental Disabilities,34(), –.
https://doi.org/./j.ridd...
Lutz, K. F., Burnson, C., Hane, A., Samuelson, A., Maleck, S., &
Poehlmann, J. (). Parenting stress, social support, and mother-
child interactions in families of multiple and singleton preterm
toddlers. Family Relations,61(), –. https://doi.org/./
j.-...x
Mcconnell, D., Breitkreuz, R., & Savage, A. (). From financial
hardship to child difficulties: Main and moderating effects of per-
ceived social support. Child: Care, Health and Development,37(),
–. https://doi.org/./j.-...x
McMahon, C., & Bernier, A. (). Twenty years of research on
parental mind-mindedness: Empirical findings, theoretical and
methodological challenges, and new directions. Developmental
Review,46,.https://doi.org/./j.dr...
McMahon, C., Camberis, A. L., Berry, S., & Gibson, F. (). Mater-
nal mind-mindedness: Relations with maternal-fetal attachment
and stability in the first two years of life: Findings from an Aus-
tralian prospective study. Infant Mental Health Journal,37(), –
. https://doi.org/./imhj.
Meins, E. (). Security of attachment and the social development of
cognition. London, UK: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/./

Meins, E. (). Sensitivity, security and internal working models:
Bridging the transmission gap. Attachment and Human Develop-
ment,1(), –. https://doi.org/./
Meins, E., Centifanti, L. C. M., Fernyhough, C., & Fishburn, S.
(). Maternal mind-mindedness and children’s behavioral dif-
ficulties: Mitigating the impact of low socioeconomic status. Jour-
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology,41(), –. https://doi.org/
./s---
Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (). Mind-mindedness coding man-
ual, Version 2.2 (Unpublished manuscript). University of York,
York, UK.
12 SUTTORA  .
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R., & De Rosnay,
M. (). Mind-mindedness and theory of mind: Mediating roles
of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Development,
84(), –. https://doi.org/./cdev.
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Turner, M., & Leekam, S.
R. (). Mother- versus infant-centered correlates of maternal
mind-mindedness in the first year of life. Infancy,16(), –.
https://doi.org/./j.-...x
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., de Rosnay, M., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R., &
Turner, M. (). Mind-mindedness as a multidimensional con-
struct: Appropriate and nonattuned mind-related comments inde-
pendently predict infant-mother attachment in a socially diverse
sample. Infancy,17(), –. https://doi.org/./j.-.
..x
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., & Harris-Waller, J. (). Is mind-
mindedness trait-like or a quality of close relationships? Evi-
dence from descriptions of significant others, famous people, and
works of art. Cognition,130(), –. https://doi.org/./j.
cognition...
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Russell,J., & Clark-Carter, D. (). Secu-
rity of attachment as a predictor of symbolic and mentalising abil-
ities: A longitudinal study. Social Development,7(), –. https:
//doi.org/./- .
Miles, M. S., Funk, S. G., & Carlson, J. (). Parental stressor scale:
Neonatal intensive care unit. Nursing Research,42(), –.
https://doi.org/./--
Montirosso, R., Provenzi, L., Calciolari, G., & Borgatti, R. ().
Measuring maternal stress and perceived support in  Italian
NICUs. Acta Paediatrica,101(), –. https://doi.org/./j.
-...x
O’Donovan, A., & Nixon, E. (). “Weathering the storm:” Mothers’
and fathers’ experiences of parenting a preterm infant. Infant Men-
tal Health Journal,40(), –. https://doi.org/./imhj.

Oppenheim, D., & Koren-Karie, N. (). Mothers’ insightfulness
regarding their children’s internal worlds: The capacity underly-
ing secure child-mother relationships. Infant Mental Health Jour-
nal,23(), –. https://doi.org/./imhj.
Östberg, M., & Hagekull, B. (). A structural modeling approach
to the understanding of parenting stress. Journal of Clinical Child
and Adolescent Psychology,29(), –. https://doi.org/./
SJCCP_
Picciolini, O., Gianni, M. L., Vegni, C., Fumagalli, M., & Mosca,
F. (). Usefulness of an early neurofunctional assessment in
predicting neurodevelopmental outcome in very low birthweight
infants. Archives of Disease in Childhood: Fetal and Neonatal Edi-
tion,91(), F–F. https://doi.org/./adc..
Poehlmann, J., Schwichtenberg, A. M., Bolt, D., & Dilworth-Bart, J.
(). Predictors of depressive symptom trajectories in mothers of
preterm or low birth weight infants. Journal of Family Psychology,
23(), –. https://doi.org/./a
Riva Crugnola, C., Ierardi, E., & Canevini, M. P. (). Reflec-
tive functioning, maternal attachment, mind-mindedness, and
emotional availability in adolescent and adult mothers at infant
 months. Attachment and Human Development,20(), –.
https://doi.org/./..
Rosenblum, K. L., McDonough, S. C., Sameroff, A. J., & Muzik,
M. (). Reflection in thought and action: Maternal parent-
ing reflectivity predicts mind-minded comments and interactive
behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal,29(), –. https://doi.
org/./imhj.
Ruiz, N., Witting, A., Ahnert, L., & Piskernik, B. (). Reflective
functioning in fathers with young children born preterm and at
term. Attachment and Human Development,22(), –. https://
doi.org/./..
Schappin, R., Wijnroks, L., Venema, M. M. U., & Jongmans, M. J.
(). Rethinking stress in parents of preterm infants: A meta-
analysis. PLoS ONE,8(), e. https://doi.org/./journal.
pone.
Simpson, M., & Catling, C. (). Understanding psychological trau-
matic birth experiences: A literature review. Women and Birth,
29(), –. https://doi.org/./j.wombi...
Slade, A. (). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction.
Attachment and Human Development,7(), –. https://doi.
org/./
Slade, A., Aber, J. L., Bresgi, I., Berger, B., & Kaplan, M. (). The
parent development interview—Revised. New York, NY: City Uni-
versity of New York.
Slade, A., Sadler, L., de Dios-Kenn, C., Webb, D., Currier-Ezepchick,
J., & Mayes, L. (). Minding the baby: A reflective parenting
program. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child,60(), –.
Spinelli, M., Frigerio, A., Montali, L., Fasolo, M., Spada, M. S., &
Mangili, G. (). ‘I still have difficulties feeling like a mother’:
The transition to motherhood of preterm infants mothers. Psychol-
ogy and Health,31(), –. https://doi.org/./.
.
Suchman, N. E., Ordway, M. R., de las Heras, L., & McMahon, T. J.
(). Mothering from the Inside Out: Results of a pilot study test-
ing a mentalization-based therapy for mothers enrolled in mental
health services. Attachment and Human Development,18(), –
. https://doi.org/./..
Suttora, C., Spinelli, M., & Monzani, D. (). From prematurity to
parenting stress: The mediating role of perinatal post-traumatic
stress disorder. European Journal of Developmental Psychology,
11(), –. https://doi.org/./..
Tommiska, V., Östberg, M., & Fellman, V. (). Parental stress in
families of  year old extremely low birthweight infants. Archives
of Disease in Childhood: Fetal and Neonatal Edition,86(), F–
F. https://doi.org/./fn...f
Walker, T. M., Wheatcroft, R., & Camic, P. M. (). Mind-
mindedness in parents of pre-schoolers: A comparison between
clinical and community samples. Clinical Child Psychology and
Psychiatry,17(), –. https://doi.org/./
Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. ().
The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. Jour-
nal of Personality Assessment,52(), –. https://doi.org/./
sjpa_
How to cite this article: Suttora C, Salerni N,
Selvagno E, et al. Mind-mindedness and parenting
stress in mothers of preterm and full-term infants:
The moderating role of perceived social support.
Infant Ment Health J. ;–.
https://doi.org/./imhj.
... Thus, impairedmentalization likely plays a role in parenting deficits in parents with PTSD, regardless of trauma type. Indeed, findings confirm this for mothers who experienced perinatal trauma [34,35], and for veteran mothers and fathers [36,37]. One study [38] found mentalization to mediate the association between PTSD and child and family functioning in veterans with relatively high symptoms, but not in those with relatively low PTSD symptoms. ...
Article
Full-text available
Parenting preterm infants is a unique experience distinct from parenting full-term infants, characterized by a delayed transition to parenthood and limited caregiving opportunities. This study explored mothers' and fathers' lived experiences of parenting during infancy in the context of preterm birth. Semistructured qualitative interviews were conducted with 13 parents (6 fathers, 7 mothers) of preterm infants. Data were analyzed using interpretative phenomenological analysis. Four superordinate themes emerged: (a) An unnatural disaster: The traumatic nature of preterm birth, (b) The immediate aftermath: Disconnected and displaced, (c) Breaking the ice: Moving from frozen to melted, and (d) Aftershocks: Transitioning home. Both parents experienced preterm birth as traumatic. Similarities and differences in mothers' and fathers' experiences were identified. Preterm birth posed challenges for nurturant and social caregiving and resulted in anxiety, hypervigilance, and overprotective parenting behavior. The results highlight the need for trauma-informed care and further research developing and testing empirically based interventions. © 2019 Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
Article
Full-text available
The present study assessed 322 parents of 173 children aged between 12 and 20 months (74 children born preterm) with the Parent Development Interview (PDI) to capture parents’ Reflective Functioning (RF). RF scores were obtained, and topics were disclosed, for which modeling with Latent Dirichlet Allocation (LDA) was applied. The study addressed (a) whether RF scores differed between fathers of children born preterm and at term, and diverged from the mothers’ RF and, (b) whether topics on fathers’ minds differed regarding parenting preterm or at-term children, and diverged from topics on parenting raised by mothers. Results indicated that parents of at-term children revealed similar RF scores, though fathers of children born preterm scored lower than mothers of children born preterm. Whereas fathers’ RF scores were associated with topics about the paternal role, interests and activities, mothers’ RF was related to concerns about how to meet the child’s needs.
Article
Full-text available
Relations between maternal mind-mindedness, parenting stress and obstetric history were investigated in a Chinese sample of 96 mother-infant dyads (Minfants' age = 15.95 months, Mmaternal age = 30 years). Parenting stress and obstetric history were assessed through questionnaires while mind-mindedness was observed through video recordings of mother-infant interactions. Among mothers who perceived their pregnancy to have been difficult, those who had planned to conceive were more likely to comment appropriately on their infants' mental states. Mothers who displayed a highly emotional reaction of an exclusively positive nature when their babies were first given to them experienced lower total parenting stress scores. Mothers who were more mind-mindedness experienced lower parenting stress. Main carer status and daily time spent with child were related to parenting stress. Results are discussed in relation to socio-cultural factors that might shape antenatal perceptions and influence parenting stress which in turn affect mothers' relationships with their children.
Article
Full-text available
The researchers of this study have two aims: The first one is to verify whether PTS symptoms, evaluated at 87 hours and at 3 months post-partum, are associated with MM and parenting stress, measured at 17 months post-partum. The second purpose is to investigate, at 17 months, the predictive effects of PTS symptoms on the dimensions of parenting stress and to explore whether MM mediates these associations. 41 mother-infant dyads participated in the study. The results show that, at 17 months, hyper-arousal symptoms predicted both MM and parenting stress. MM was a linking mechanism between maternal PTS symptoms and parenting stress.
Article
The goal of this study was to examine the effects of preterm birth and maternal childbirth-related posttraumatic stress and parenting stress on maternal mind-mindedness (MM). The study also investigated the effects of perceived social support on parenting stress and MM. Sixty-five preterm (N = 32) and full-term (N = 33) mother-infant dyads were observed at 6 months. Measures of maternal MM were obtained from observations of mother-infant interaction. Mothers also provided ratings of their posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms, parenting stress, and perceived social support via an online survey. Experiencing a preterm birth did not affect mothers' use of mental state descriptors during mother-infant interaction. Neither childbirth-related posttraumatic stress nor parenting stress directly affected maternal ability to comment on the child's mental states appropriately. However, at medium and high levels of perceived social support, a negative association between parenting stress and MM was observed. Maternal perception of being emotionally supported by significant others promoted MM in mothers showing low or mild levels of parenting stress, but not in mothers experiencing high stress in parenting their infants. Results suggest that a proclivity to MM might be affected by the interaction between parenting stress and social support, rather than by childbirth-related variables, such as prematurity.
Article
Increasing evidence shows that maternal touch may promote emotion regulation in infants, however less is known about how parental higher-order social cognition abilities are translated into tactile, affect-regulatory behaviours towards their infants. During 10 min book-reading, mother-infant sessions when infants were 12 months old (N = 45), we investigated maternal mind-mindedness (MM), the social cognitive ability to understand an infant's mental state, by coding the contingency of maternal verbal statements towards the infants' needs and desires. We also rated spontaneous tactile interactions in terms of their emotional contingency. We found that frequent non-attuned mind-related comments were associated with touch behaviours that were not contingent with the infant's emotions; ultimately discouraging affective tactile responses from the infant. However, comments that were more appropriate to infant's mental states did not necessarily predict more emotionally-contingent tactile behaviours. These findings suggest that when parental high-order social cognitive abilities are compromised, they are also likely to translate into inappropriate, tactile attempts to regulate infant's emotions.
Article
Maternal mind-mindedness has been shown to be a powerful predictor of many developmental outcomes and to buffer the impact of psychosocial risk conditions, but no study has investigated whether this parental feature might support child development in the presence of biological risk, such as preterm birth. The present study addresses this gap, by investigating whether early maternal mind-mindedness contributes to the growth of a child's linguistic abilities in the following two years of life, and if the contribution of this maternal feature might be stronger in the presence of preterm birth. Forty mother-child dyads (twenty with a preterm infant) were followed longitudinally, with maternal mind-mindedness assessed at 14 months of age and child's expressive linguistic abilities at 24 and 36 months through observational measures. Multilevel models showed that linguistic abilities increased from 24 to 36 months of age, but that this increase was stronger in full-term infants. Maternal mind-mindedness also contributed to this growth, playing a stronger role in preterm infants than in full-term infants. Altogether, these findings contribute more deeply to the understanding of language development in preterm infants and of the joint contribution made by biological risk and environmental factors; from a practical standpoint, they suggest the importance of addressing mother's mind-mindedness in order to support child's language development.
Article
The study evaluated reflective functioning (RF), maternal attachment, mind-mindedness, and emotional availability among 44 adolescent mother–infant dyads and 41 adult mother–infant dyads. At infant age 3 months, mother–infant interaction was coded with the mind-mindedness coding system and Emotional Availability Scales; mother attachment and RF were evaluated with the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI). Adolescent mothers (vs. adult mothers) were more insecure and had lower RF; they were also less sensitive, more intrusive and hostile, and less structuring of their infant’s activity; they used fewer attuned mind-related comments and fewer mind-related comments appropriate to infant development. In adult mothers, the Mother Idealizing and Lack of Memory AAI scales were correlated to non-attuned mind-related comments and the Father Anger scale to negative mind-related comments. In adult mothers, RF was associated with sensitivity. This was not the case with adolescent mothers. In both groups of mothers, there were no associations between sensitivity and mind-mindedness.
Article
Mind-mindedness refers to a caregiver’s tendency to treat the young child as an individual with a mind of his or her own. It is assessed in the first year of life by the caregiver’s tendency to comment appropriately on, and not misread, the infant’s mental states (thoughts, feelings, preferences) during interaction and in older children by the caregivers’ spontaneous use of mental state words in response to an invitation to describe their child. This narrative review first describes the construct and its theoretical origins as well as the different approaches to measurement. We then critically review 20 years of empirical literature linking mind-mindedness to indices of the parent–child attachment relationship and child developmental outcomes, and exploring the properties of the construct. We conclude by identifying key theoretical and methodological questions that need to be addressed in order to advance the field as well as potential clinical applications.
Article
Mothers who are involved with mental health services (for themselves or their children) rarely receive adequate support for their role as parents. Mental illness in a parent or child often exacerbates the challenges of managing psychological distress that is germane to the parenting roll. Mentalization-based approaches to psychotherapy for parents have the potential to address challenges of emotional regulation in parents by supporting their capacity to recognize and modulate negative affect during stressful parenting situations. In this study, we piloted Mothering from the Inside Out (MIO) with 17 mothers receiving services at a community-based mental health clinic. MIO is a 12-week, mentalization-based parenting intervention that demonstrated efficacy in two previous randomized controlled trials with substance using mothers. In this study, we were interested in determining whether community-based clinicians could deliver MIO with sustained fidelity. We were also interested in examining the preliminary feasibility, acceptability and efficacy of MIO when delivered by clinicians in a community mental health center. Finally, we were interested in replicating prior tests of the proposed treatment mechanisms. Treatment outcomes included maternal reflective functioning, psychiatric and parenting stress, and mother-child interaction quality. Our findings indicated that MIO was feasible and acceptable when delivered in the community-based setting and that all maternal indices improved. However, no improvement in mother-child interaction quality was found, possibly because of insufficient time for these changes to consolidate.