ArticlePDF Available

Cumulative consequences of stigma: Possessing multiple concealable stigmatized identities is associated with worse quality of life

Authors:

Abstract

Large health disparities exist between stigmatized and nonstigmatized groups. In addition to experiencing and anticipating greater discrimination, members of stigmatized groups also tend to demonstrate greater ruminative tendencies in response, which may lead to these poor health outcomes. Even among stigmatized groups, differences in the visibility of stigma lead to different mechanisms through which stigma takes its toll. Previous work has primarily focused on the impact of belonging to a single marginalized group; however, people often belong to multiple marginalized groups, and this likely affects both their health outcomes and their anticipation of stigma. In the current study, we focused on individuals with concealable stigmatized identities (CSIs)—socially stigmatized identities that are not immediately apparent to others—and created a measure of concealable marginalization that captures multiple group memberships. We predicted that those possessing a greater number of CSIs would anticipate more stigma from others, and, in turn, ruminate more about the stigma, which would negatively impact the health. Surveying N = 288 adults with CSIs, we found that possessing a greater number of marginalized concealable identities predicted worse self‐reported physical quality of life. These relationships were partially mediated by greater anticipated stigma and brooding rumination in regard to their CSI. This work illuminates a more complete picture of how living with CSIs can take its toll on health.
J Appl Soc Psychol. 2020;00:1–9. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jasp
|
  1© 2020 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
1 | INTRODUCTION
Stigma has been classified as a “fundamental cause” of health in-
equalities in that it limits access to health resources, affects multiple
health outcomes, and predicts worse health even when other inter-
vening mechanisms change (Hatzenbuehler, Phelan, & Link, 2013;
Link & Phelan, 1995). Hatzenbuehler and colleagues (2013) note that
researchers likely underestimate the negative effects of stigma on
health because they often focus on just one stigmatized identity or
one outcome or mechanism at a time. Similarly, others have called
for greater emphasis on the intersection of multiple identities in the
study of stigma (Williams & Fredrick, 2015), as well as in psychology
more broadly (Rosenthal, 2016). In the current work, we begin to
address this gap by considering how multiple marginalized identities
and multiple psychological mediators affect the physical quality of
life. To do so, we focus on a sample of people living with a conceal-
able stigmatized identity (CSI)—a socially stigmatized identity that is
not immediately apparent to others—and consider how possessing
multiple additional CSIs relates to greater anticipated stigma, brood-
ing rumination, and worse quality of life.
Received: 5 June 2019 
|
  Revised: 17 October 2019 
|
  Accepted: 10 January 2020
DOI: 10.1111/jasp.12656
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Cumulative consequences of stigma: Possessing multiple
concealable stigmatized identities is associated with worse
quality of life
Mora A. Reinka1| Bradley Pan-Weisz2| Elizabeth K. Lawner1| Diane M. Quinn1
1Department of Psychological Sciences,
University of Connecticut, Storrs, C T, USA
2Depar tment of Psychology, California State
University, Long Beach, Long Beach, CA,
USA
Correspondence
Mora A . Reinka, Depar tment of
Psychological Science s, University of
Connec ticut , Storrs , CT, 06521-1020, USA .
Email: mora.reinka@uconn.edu
Abstract
Large health disparities exist between stigmatized and nonstigmatized groups. In ad-
dition to experiencing and anticipating greater discrimination, members of stigma-
tized groups also tend to demonstrate greater ruminative tendencies in response,
which may lead to these poor health outcomes. Even among stigmatized groups,
differences in the visibility of stigma lead to different mechanisms through which
stigma takes its toll. Previous work has primarily focused on the impact of belonging
to a single marginalized group; however, people often belong to multiple marginalized
groups, and this likely affects both their health outcomes and their anticipation of
stigma. In the current study, we focused on individuals with concealable stigmatized
identities (CSIs)—socially stigmatized identities that are not immediately apparent to
others—and created a measure of concealable marginalization that captures multiple
group memberships. We predicted that those possessing a greater number of CSIs
would anticipate more stigma from others, and, in turn, ruminate more about the
stigma, which would negatively impact the health. Surveying N = 288 adults with
CSIs, we found that possessing a greater number of marginalized concealable identi-
ties predicted worse self-reported physical quality of life. These relationships were
partially mediated by greater anticipated stigma and brooding rumination in regard to
their CSI. This work illuminates a more complete picture of how living with CSIs can
take its toll on health.
2 
|
   REINKA Et Al.
1.1 | Intersectional approaches
The term “intersectionality” is often credited to Dr. Kimber
Crenshaw (1989) in her seminal essay proposing that the burdens
Black women face are greater than that of their race or sex alone.
Although it began as a legal argument, the concept of intersection-
ality—that one must consider the whole of a person's identities to
best understand their experiences—has expanded into numerous
academic and applied disciplines, including psychology (Else-Quest
& Hyde, 2016; Rosenthal, 2016; Williams & Fredrick, 2015), pub-
lic health (Bowleg, 2012; Goodin et al., 2018), and even the com-
mon vernacular, as it was added to Webster's Dictionar y in 2017
(Merriam-Webster, 2017). Accordingly, researchers have recently
used an intersectional approach to study discrimination (e.g., Lewis
& Van Dyke, 2018; Liu & Wong, 2018; Sugarman et al., 2018), sexual
and domestic violence (e.g., Armstrong, Gleckman-Krut, & Johnson,
2018; Conwill, 2010; Powell, Hlavka, & Mulla, 2017), as well as physi-
cal and mental health (e.g., Dlugonski, Martin, Mailey, & Pineda,
2017; Goodin et al., 2018; Lewis & Van Dyke, 2018; Velez, Moradi, &
DeBlaere, 2014), among others.
Perhaps due to its roots as a study of the “double jeopardy” that
Black women face (Beal, 2008; Crenshaw, 1989), much of the work
within an intersectional framework focuses on visible stigmatized
identities, such as race and gender. While a number of studies ex-
amine the interactions between these visible identities and a con-
cealable identity, like sexual orientation (e.g., Mereish & Bradford,
2014) or mental illness (e.g., Torres, Mata-Greve, Bird, & Herrera
Hernandez, 2018), few studies have applied this line of inquiry
specifically to the effects of possessing multiple CSIs. Concealable
and visible stigmatized identities share many characteristics and
outcomes due to the broader nature and processes of stigma (see
Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998; Goffman, 1963; Link & Phelan, 2001,
2014); however, the possibility of concealment uniquely af fects the
psychological experience of a CSI (e.g., Quinn & Chaudoir, 2009;
Quinn & Earnshaw, 2013). Therefore, it may be reasonable to expect
that the intersection of multiple CSIs, although perhaps resulting
in similar outcomes as visible stigmas (e.g., poor health; Grollman,
2014; Pachankis et al., 2018), would proceed through different
mechanisms.
1.2 | Anticipated stigma
One aspect of living with a CSI is anticipating when, where, and from
whom one will experience discrimination. Although anyone with a
devalued identity may evaluate the potential to face stigma, those
with a visible identity will know with some certainty another per-
son's reaction to them; if their interaction partner becomes with-
drawn or defensive, they may attribute such behavior to their skin
color, gender, or weight. Indeed, these individuals may even become
vigilant to such threatening cues in their environment (for a discus-
sion, see Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002). For those with a con-
cealable stigma, however, they must anticipate what might happen
should their interaction partner discover their identity. The stigma-
tizing experience they fear has not yet occurred, but the stress—the
anticipated stigma—lies in the possibility that the interaction part-
ner may figure out that the individual is concealing something. As
Goffman (1963) puts it: those with a CSI are tasked with managing
information regarding their identity, whereas those with a visible
stigma are tasked with managing the nuance of social situations.
Indeed, for people with CSIs, one predictor of poorer health
is greater anticipated stigma (Quinn & Chaudoir, 2009; Quinn et
al., 2014), or the concern that one will be treated poorly if others
find out about a stigmatized identity. Previous work with CSIs has
shown that to the extent that people anticipate more devaluation
from others were they to reveal their identity—or come “out”—the
greater psychological distress (anxiety and depression; Chaudoir &
Quinn, 2016; Quinn & Chaudoir, 2009) and physical health symp-
toms (O’Donnell, Corrigan, & Gallagher, 2015; Quinn & Chaudoir,
2009) they report. Most of this previous research has focused on the
outcomes of anticipating stigma, largely as it relates to health and
well-being. However, it remains unclear what influences a person's
level of anticipated stigma regarding their CSIs.
Work on two similar concepts suggests that previous expe-
riences of discrimination based on different social identities may
prime people to anticipate more discrimination in the future. First,
stigma consciousness refers to an individual's expectation that
their actions will be interpreted through the lens of group-based
stereotypes (Pinel, 1999). Similar to anticipated stigma, stigma
consciousness is often conceptualized as an individual difference
trait; however, it is positively correlated with previous experiences
of discrimination (although the directionality of the relationship is
unclear; Pinel, 1999, 2004). Moreover, stigma consciousness can be
situationally heightened by reminders of prejudicial behavior (Pinel,
2004). Related work on the concept of status-based rejection sen-
sitivity, where people come to expect rejection from others based
on their stigmatized identity, shows that greater rejection sensitiv-
ity is also related to more experiences of discrimination in the past
(Mendoza-Denton, Downey, Purdie, Davis, & Pietrzak, 20 02). From
both ideas, we can extrapolate that for people living with stigma,
possessing more marginalized identities has likely exposed them to
more experiences of lifetime discrimination and perhaps provides
more consistent reminders of prejudicial attitudes. These cumulative
experiences likely lead people with CSIs to rationally anticipate more
stigma in the future. Although it may be rational to anticipate future
stigma in light of past stigma, this process can become maladaptive if
such anticipation becomes too frequent and repetitive.
1.3 | Brooding rumination
Brooding rumination—a maladaptive coping style of emotion regula-
tion marked by repetitive, self-focused thoughts—acts as one pathway
through which stigma negatively affects health (Hatzenbuehler, Nolen-
Hoeksema, & Dovidio, 2009). Past research has found that rumina-
tion is highly correlated with psychological distress (Lewis, Milletich,
  
|
 3
REINK A Et Al.
Mason, & Derlega, 2014), specifically depressive symptomology, both
cross-sectionally, and as a cross-lagged predictor (Moberly & Watkins,
2008; Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003). In addition, in-
creased brooding rumination has been strongly connected to worse
physical health (Ottaviani et al., 2016; Sansone & Sansone, 2012), likely
through the increased wear and tear of an extended somatic stress
response (Verkuil, Brosschot, Gebhardt, & Thayer, 2010).
While most people engage in this form of passive coping
at least some of the time (Moberly & Watkins, 20 08; Treynor
et al., 2003), previous research has shown that stigmatized indi-
viduals ruminate at a greater rate than their nonstigmatized peers
(Timmins, Rimes, & Rahman, 2017). Moreover, for these individu-
als, brooding rumination appears to be a common response to stig-
ma-related stress (Hatzenbuehler et al., 2009; Lewis et al., 2014).
By spending time ruminating about experiences of discrimination,
people—especially those expecting to face stigma again—may feel
that they are arming themselves for future misfortunes (Watkins
& Baracaia, 2001). However, past work has examined brooding
after experienced stigma; anticipated stigma has yet to be tested
as a stigma-related stressor that may prompt brooding rumination.
Moreover, it is possible that those who possess a greater number
of stigmatized identities ruminate more, as they may have more
previous experience with stigma as well as anticipate more stigma
in the future. Unfortunately, as mentioned before, this short-
term coping response can lead to long-term health consequences
(Ottaviani et al., 2016; Sansone & Sansone, 2012; Verkuil et al.,
2010).
1.4 | Hypotheses
We take as our starting point that (a) having a greater number of CSIs
will have an accumulating negative ef fect on self-reported physical
quality of life (QOL). This should conceptually replicate the findings of
previous survey studies that found possessing multiple marginalized
identities was related to poorer self-rated health and gre ater functional
limitations (Grollman, 2014; Pachankis et al., 2018). In addition, we pre-
dict that (b) possession of a greater number of CSIs will correspond
with greater anticipated stigma. Furthermore, anticipated stigma has
been found to be predictive of greater psychological distress (Chaudoir
& Quinn, 2016; Quinn & Chaudoir, 2009), and brooding rumination
can be triggered by stigma-related stress (Hatzenbuehler et al., 2009).
Therefore, we expect that (c) greater anticipated stigma will predict
more brooding rumination. Finally, in line with previous work (e.g.,
Sansone & Sansone, 2012), (d) more brooding rumination will predict
poorer physical QOL. In addition to testing direct effects, we hypoth-
esize a mediated model such that the link between multiple CSIs and
worse physical QOL will be mediated by increased anticipated stigma
and brooding rumination. See Figure 1 for the full mediational model.
2 | METHOD
2.1 | Participants
Participants were recruited as part of a larger study examining CSIs
(Quinn, Weisz, & Lawner, 2017; see the Supplemental Materials for
the full list of survey measures) through Amazon's Mechanical Turk
platform. After initial, online written consent, 288 “workers” (122
men, 165 women, 1 other; 236 White, 52 nonWhite identifying in-
dividuals) completed the 10-min survey in exchange for $1. Sample
size was determined by planned SEM analyses for the larger study
(see Kline, 2005), but post hoc power analyses using Monte Carlo
simulations (Schoemann, Boulton, & Shor t, 2017) reveal that, with
N = 288, we have virtually 100% power to detect the indirect paths
in which we are interested.
In order to screen into the study, participants had to report
possessing at least one of three CSIs—chronic illness (n = 113),
FIGURE 1 Unstandardized regression coefficients and standard errors (in parentheses) of the relationship between the possession of
M-CSI and physical quality of life, through anticipated stigma and brooding rumination, while controlling for CSI outness, race/ethnicity,
gender, and age. Total effect is printed in light gray . Solid lines represent significant pathways; dashed lines represent nonsignificant paths.
**p < .01
4 
|
   REINKA Et Al.
sexual orientation (n = 69), or history of mental illness (n = 106).
These three CSIs were chosen specifically because they are
known to be fairly common, but also vary in their level of social
stigmatization (see Pachankis et al., 2018). However, many par-
ticipants (58%) repor ted possessing more than one of these three
CSIs. In these cases, in order to reduce testing burden and ensure
equal time commitment for all individuals, participants chose the
CSI they found most important to their self-concept; they then
answered the stigma-related questions (anticipated stigma and
brooding rumination) in reference to that CSI. Recent research has
shown that the identity an individual rates as their most import-
ant has the largest impact on their wellbeing (Rodriguez-Seijas,
Burton, Adeyinka, & Pachankis, 2019). See Table 1 for full partic-
ipant characteristics. The University of Connecticut Institutional
Review Board approved all procedures prior to data collection in
accordance with the Belmont Report.
2.2 | Measures
2.2.1| Multiple concealable stigmatized identities
scale (M-CSI)
We constructed a concealable identity scale as our primary independ-
ent variab le to best capture t he potential effects of pos sessing multiple
CSIs, similar to other health models’ conceptualizations of personal
resource s (e.g., Johnson et al., 2010) or additive models of intersec tion-
ality (e.g., Reidpath & Chan, 2005; for a discussion, see Else-Quest &
Hyde, 2016). To do this, we constructed the M-CSI scale such that we
tallied1 each CSI that participant s repor ted possessing from a demo-
graphic list: mental illness, addiction,2 sexual orientation, sexual as-
sault, family abuse history, homelessness, and incarceration history.
Al th ough we conside r ch ro nic illne ss es and ca nc er to be CSIs, give n th e
physical nature of our dependent variable, we did not include these
tallies in our scale to avoid conflating predictor and outcome. Howeve r,
results were substantively the same with or without their inclusion.
1 There is m uch debate in the i ntersectio nality liter ature about whe ther the conce pt
works additively or multiplicatively—that is, examining the statistical interactions of
variou s identit ies (for a review, se e Else-Qu est & Hyde, 2016). As oth ers have note d
(Rodri guez-Seij as et al., 2019), a multi plicat ive approach qu ickly become s cumbersome
with inc reasing ident ities examine d. With our sam ple size, we d o not have enough po wer
to do a clus ter analysis (e. g., Pach ankis et al., 2018) t o help us id entif y what combinati on
of concea lable identit ies are the most “ harmful” in ter ms of their impac t on health.
Regard less, we tested a f our-way interact ion model in an explorat ory ana lysis betwee n
four CS Is in our conceal able sti gma scale (ment al illness, ad diction, sex ual identity, an d
put the r emainin g four into one “tr aumatic life hi story” cate gory) in order to p redict
physic al quality of lif e. The four-CSI mo del, as oppose d to our 7-item M-CSI , was chosen
for a semb lance of parsim ony in an already co mplicated int eraction mo del. While this
test was likely still underpowered, no interactions between any of the identities were
signif icantl y associa ted with phys ical QOL whe n control ling for CSI ou tness , age, race,
and gend er, all βs < |.35|, all ps > .12.
2 Accordi ng to the DSM-5, addi ctions are con sidere d a mental i llness (Amer ican
Psychi atric A ssociat ion, 2013). Howeve r, we decided t o separate addi ction into its o wn
catego ry bec ause of the extr a stigma atta ched to it compar ed to nonsubst ance use
menta l illnes ses (Bar ry, McGinty, Pesc osolido, & Gold man, 2014; Schome rus et al., 2011).
Result s were substan tively the sam e if we colla psed add iction into our t allies for men tal
illnes s and if we separat ed them.
TABLE 1 Summary of participant characteristics
Characteristic
Tot al N = 288
n (%)
Gender
Male 122 (42.4%)
Female 165 (57.3%)
Other 1 (.3%)
Race
White 236 (81.9%)
Asian 24 (8 .3%)
Black 16 (5.6%)
Latino 6 (2.1%)
Native American 2 (.7%)
Other/mixed 4 (1.4%)
CSIsa
Mental illness 206 (71.5%)
Addiction 121 (42. 0%)
Cancer history 34 (11 .8%)
Chronic illness 149 (51.7%)
Minority sexual orientation 102 (35.4%)
Physically or emotionally abusive family 114 (39.6%)
Previous jail or prison time 19 (6. 6%)
Sexual assault history 77 (26.7%)
Homeless (current or previously) 38 (13 .2%)
Education
Some high school 2 (.7%)
Graduated high school 36 (12 .5%)
Some college 92 (31.9%)
Associate's degree 21 (7.3%)
Bachelor's degree 105 (36.5%)
Master's degree 28 (9.7%)
Professional degree 2 (.7%)
Doctorate degree 2 (.7%)
Income
<$30 k 88 (30.6%)
$30,000–$39,999 42 (14. 6%)
$40,000–$49,999 35 (12 .2%)
$50,000–$69,999 54 (18.8%)
$70,000–$79,999 20 (6.9%)
$80,000–$89,999 13 (4.5%)
$90,000–$99,999 9 (3.1%)
>$100k 27 (9.4%)
Employed
Yes 276 (95.8%)
No 12 (4.2%)
Abbreviation: CSI, concealable stigmatized identity.
aPercentages sum to more than 100% because participants were allowed
to indicate as many CSIs as they possessed, but they responded to further
questions on the one CSI they indicated was most important to them.
  
|
 5
REINK A Et Al.
Our sample ranged from 0–7 points on this scale, with a mean of 2.35
(SD = 1.51). Table 2 provides a full list of included CSIs.
2.2.2 | Anticipated stigma
Participants completed a 15-item scale of anticipated stigma (Quinn
& Chaudoir, 2009) regarding expected outcomes if others knew about
their CSI (e.g., being “treated with less respect than other people”)
from 1 = not at all likely to 7 = extremely likely. Participants generally
felt outcomes were “slightly likely, M = 2.68, but with some consider-
able variability, SD = 1.42; standardized α =.958.
2.2.3 | Brooding rumination
Participants completed the 5-item brooding subscale of the Ruminative
Response Scale (Treynor et al., 2003) as a frequency measure of brooding
rumination. The instructions were modified to specifically reference the
individual's CSI: “People think and do many different things when they
are reminded of [their CSI]. Below is a list of possibilities, followed by
the standard items (e.g., “Think about a recent situation, wishing it had
gone better”). Participants responded on a scale from 1 = almost never
to 4 = almost always; M = 2.07, SD =.744; standardized α =.857.
2.2.4 | Physical QOL
Participants also completed the World Health Organization Brief Quality
of Life (WHO QOL) 7-item physical wellbeing subscale. Items assess
health from the perspective of QOL, using items such as, “Do you
have enough energy for everyday life?” By focusing on QOL rather
than medicalized symptoms, we can more confidently capture well-
being across a broad adult population. Answers are standardized to
a 1–5 scale, with higher scores indicating greater perceived QOL (The
WHOQOL Group, 1998; M = 3.67, SD =.795; standardized α =.874) .
2.2.5 |Outness
Finally, given that anticipated stigma presumes a concealed CSI, we
included as a control variable how “out” participants were about their
CSI to eight different individuals in their life, including parents, siblings,
friends, partners, and coworkers (Mohr & Fassinger, 2000), from which
an average outness score was calculated. Participants indicated the
level of the other person's knowledge from 1 = does not know at all
to my knowledge, to 5 = knows and we've talked about it, with an op-
tion to indicate if the specified person was not in their life (M = 3.80,
SD = 1.03; standardized α = .869). Unsurprisingly, outness was corre-
lated with anticipated stigma, r(286) = −.276, p < .001 (see Table 3 for
correlations between the measures). However, the following results
still stand without this covariate, and in fact are even a little stronger,
making this a relatively conservative inclusion.
3 | RESULTS
3.1 | Data-analytic plan
To address our hypotheses, we conducted our analyses in three
steps. First, we aimed to demonstrate a direct negative relationship
TABLE 2 Identities included in the CSIs scale
CSI N (%)
Mental illnessa206 (71.5%)
Anxiety disorder 131 (45.5%)
Major depression 112 (38.9%)
Bipolar disorder 37 (12.8%)
Cutting or self-mutilation 34 (11 .8%)
Eating disorder 31 (10. 8%)
Trauma disorder/PTSD 30 (10 .4%)
Obsessive-compulsive disorder 27 (9.4%)
Personality disorder 14 (4.9%)
Schizophrenia 3 (1.0%)
Other (participant provided) 5 (1.7%)
Addictiona121 (42. 0%)
Alcohol 66 (22.9%)
Drugs 55 ( 19.1 %)
Gambling 27 (9.4%)
Other (participant provided) 23 (8.0%)
Minority sexual orientationa102 (35.4%)
Bisexual 70 (24.3%)
Gay/ Le sb ia n 29 (10.1%)
Transgender 4 (1.4%)
Other (participant provided) 9 (3.1%)
Sexual assault historya77 (26.7%)
Victim of rape 47 (16. 3%)
Victim of childhood sexual abuse 49 (17.0%)
Physically or emotionally abusive family 114 (39.6%)
Previous jail or prison time 19 (6. 6%)
Homeless (current or previously) 38 (13.2%)
Abbreviation: CSI, concealable stigmatized identity.
aPercentages sum to more than 100% because participants were
allowed to indicate as many CSIs as they possessed.
TABLE 3 Correlations coefficients for variables of interest
Measure 123 4
1: M-CSI
2: Outness −.111
3: Anticipated stigma .290** −.276**
4: Brooding rumination .179** −.124* .456**
5: Physical QOL −.237** −.032 −.270** −. 43 6**
*p < .05; **p < .01.
6 
|
   REINKA Et Al.
between number of CSIs possessed (M-CSI) and physical QOL using
standard linear regression. Second, we tested whether possession
of multiple CSIs predicted greater anticipated stigma and greater
rumination. Next, we examined our prediction that anticipated
stigma mediated the relationship between the M-CSI and the coping
patterns of brooding rumination (Figure 2). Third and finally, given
the strong link between brooding and poor health (e.g., Sansone &
Sansone, 2012), we included both the anticipated stigma and brood-
ing rumination as serial mediators to help explain the relationship
between multiple CSIs and physical QOL (Figure 1). Of course, just
because an identity could be concealable does not necessarily mean
that it is; therefore, all analyses included outness as a covariate. For
analyses examining physical QOL as an outcome, we also included
age, gender, and race/ethnicity as covariates due to the general re-
lationship between these variables and health, as well as history of
cancer or chronic illness. The pattern of results held with and with-
out the covariates. Mediations were conducted in the PROCESS
macro for SPSS (version 2.16; Hayes, 2013) with 95% bias-corrected
confidence intervals for indirect effects derived from 5,000 boot-
strapped samples.
3.2 | Findings
As expected, those who reported possessing a greater number of
CSIs reported worse physical QOL , b = −.141, SE = .031, t(281) =
−4.15, p < .001. In addition, higher scores on the M-CSI were related
to greater anticipated stigma, b = .246, SE = .052, t(285) = 4.61, p <
.001, and more brooding rumination, b = .083, SE = .029, t(285) =
2.870, p = .004, regarding their CSI.
Crucially, anticipated stigma was not only positively associ-
ated with brooding rumination, b = .233, SE = .030, t(284) = 7.78,
p < .001, but also fully mediated the relationship between the
M-CSI and brooding rumination, as indicated by the bootstrapped
confidence intervals, b = .057, SE = .016 [.030, .093]—possess-
ing more CSIs predicted more anticipated stigma, which, in turn,
predicted more brooding rumination. Once anticipated stigma
was included as a mediator, the relationship between the M-CSI
and brooding rumination was no longer significant, direct effect
b = .025, SE = .027, t(284) = .932, p = .352. As shown in Figure 2,
while those possessing a greater number of CSIs were more likely
to ruminate about their CSI, this can be fully explained by the fact
that they were also likely to anticipate more stigma regarding their
most important CSI.
Finally, we extended this mediation model a step further to
co nsi der th e heal t h effe c t s of su c h a proc ess . As show n in Figu re 1,
we constructed a serial mediation model with the M-CSI scale
specified as the exogenous predictor variable, anticipated stigma
and bro od in g ru mination sp ecif ie d as mediat or s, and physic al QOL
specified as the endogenous outcome variable. Those who pos-
sessed a greater number of CSIs reported anticipating more stigma
if others knew of their C SI, b = .202, SE = .053 t(281) = 3.85, p
< .001, and in turn, those who anticipated more stigma reported
more brooding rumination when thinking about their CSI, b = .232,
SE = .030, t(280) = 7.61, p < .001. Consistent with a wealth of
previous work, greater brooding rumination was associated with
worse physical QOL, b = −.393, SE = .061, t(279) = −6.78, p < .001.
Importantly, anticipated stigma and brooding rumination partially
mediated the relationship between the M-CSI and physical QOL,
total effect b = −.141, SE = .031, t(281) = −4.61, p < .001; direct
effect b = −.097, SE = .028, t(279) = −3.45, p < .001. Bootstrapped
confidence intervals confirmed the significant indirect effect, b =
−.018, SE = .007 [−.036, −.007]. The full model accounted for 30%
of the variance in physical QOL.
We have thus far found support for a cross-sectional mediation
model showing that number of CSIs is associated with greater antic-
ipated stigma, which, in turn, is associated with greater brooding ru-
mination tendencies and, finally, may lead to poor health outcomes.
This mediational sequence has support in previous literature, summa-
rized above. However, it is possible that membership in concealable
stigmatized groups may interact with anticipated stigma to predict
an increase in brooding rumination. While the outcome would essen-
tially be the same, the mathematical relationship would be different.
However, when we tested this possibility, again controlling for outness,
the M-CSI and anticipated stigma did not interact to predict rumina-
tion, b = .012, SE = .017, t(283) = .704, p = .482. Given this null result,
it is then unsurprising that the M-CSI also did not moderate the medi-
ation effect of brooding rumination between anticipated stigma and
FIGURE 2 Unstandardized regression coefficients and standard errors (in parentheses) demonstrating the relationship between the
possession of M-CSI and brooding rumination tendencies, controlling for CSI outness, as fully mediated by anticipated stigma. Total effect is
printed in light gray text. **p < .01
  
|
 7
REINK A Et Al.
physical QOL, controlling for outness, race, gender, and age: index of
moderated mediation b = −.005, SE = .008 [−.021, .009].
4 | DISCUSSION
In order to gain a greater understanding of how stigma af fects health,
we examined whether possessing multiple CSIs was related to worse
reported physical quality of life, and, furthermore, whether this re-
lationship was mediated through the specific psychological mecha-
nisms of greater anticipated stigma and greater brooding rumination.
Our predictions were supported: Within a sample of adult s with CSIs,
people possessing multiple CSIs reported more anticipated stigma and
greater brooding rumination. People possessing multiple CSIs also re-
ported lower physical qualit y of life, and this relationship was partially
mediated by anticipated stigma and brooding rumination.
It is important to note that this is a cross-sectional sur vey, there-
fore the order of the variables and the direction of causality cannot be
established. In other words, while the order of our variables fits previ-
ous literature, it is possible that several of these outcomes may be
caught in a feedback loop. For example, perhaps brooding rumination
decreases quality of life, but worsening QOL will in turn increase
brooding rumination. It is also possible that our measure of multiple
concealable stigma does not reflect the true or whole experience of
every participant. For example, we weighed each identity equally3 in
our M-CSI scale, although we recognize that different identities may
be more or less central or salient to the self (Quinn et al., 2014). Even
the use of an additive scale, albeit a deliberative and theoretically
driven choice,1,3, may not capture the multidimensionality of an indi-
vidual's experience. Indeed, the methods for quantifying intersection-
ality are largely not agreed upon (see Else-Quest & Hyde, 2016). In
addition, in determining CSIs, participants were provided a research-
er-generated list of possible identities to check of f (see Table 2), and
therefore we may not have captured ever y CSI that every participant
possessed or was important to them (e.g., [non]religious identification;
Abbot & Mollen, 2018). Finally, our sample was predominantly White,
educated, and, as this research was conducted through online collec-
tion systems, internet savvy. It will be important to replicate these re-
sults with a more diverse sample in the future.
The current work does, however, build upon previous research
that has tested parts of the theorized causal chain, with longitudinal
work showing that anticipated stigma predicts changes in depression
over time (Chaudoir & Quinn, 2016) and that greater rumination pre-
dicts changes in health over time (Nolen-Hoeksema, Stice, Wade, &
Bohon, 2007). Previous research had not strung all the links together,
focusing on the impact of multiple marginalizations within a sample
of those concerned about concealable stigmatized identities, rather
than visible identities. We have now done that. There are, however,
many questions left to answer. We cannot say for certain exactly why
it is that people with multiple CSIs tend to anticipate more stigma or
ruminate more than their peers with fewer CSIs. It could be, in line
with work on stigma consciousness and status-based rejection sen-
sitivity, that people with more CSIs have had more experiences with
discrimination and are thus are wise to expect it (e.g., Grollman, 2014;
Mendoza-Denton et al., 2002; Pinel, 2004). In this way, anticipated
stigma may be used as an adaptive strategy to avoid future discrimi-
nation. Although outside the scope of this study, it would be import-
ant to test if anticipating stigma truly is protective, or only leads to
negative outcomes such as the brooding rumination examined here. It
could also be that to the extent that greater marginalization is related
to fewer material resources available to deal with discrimination (e.g.,
ability to change jobs or move housing locations), rumination is an eas-
ily available coping strategy. Although perhaps adaptive in the short
term, it could be that as one spends more time and energy anticipat-
ing and ruminating on potential or past experiences with stigma, the
stress may itself harm physical QOL (see Verkuil et al., 2010). Further
research is necessary to elucidate the mechanisms at play behind this
pattern of results.
Finally, it should be noted that a significant direct effect from
possession of multiple CSIs to physical QOL remains once the in-
direct effects are accounted for, replicating previous work showing
that possessing multiple marginalized identities is associated with
poorer self-reported health (Grollman, 2014). This is impor tant to
note because it reflects that, whereas individual psychological mech-
anisms such as anticipated stigma and rumination may account for
one pathway from stigma to health, there are many other pathways,
including social structures and institutional discrimination, that im-
pact people's health and that are not captured in the psychological
study of individuals. Only when multiple levels of discrimination are
taken into account can the full power of social stigma on health be
recognized (Hatzenbuehler et al., 2013).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank the editor and anonymous reviewers for their
helpful and insightful feedback, as well as Gabriel Camacho for his
remarks on early drafts of this project.
ORCID
Mora A. Reinka https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9903-2966
REFERENCES
Abbot, D. M., & Mollen, D. (2018). Atheism as a concealable stigmatized
identity: Outness, anticipated stigma, and well-being. Counseling
Psychologist, 46(6), 685–707. ht tps ://doi.org/10.1177/00110 00 018
792669
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Substance-related and addic-
tive disorders. In Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. https ://blog.apast yle.org/apast
yle/2013/08/how-to-cite-the-dsm5-in-apa-style.html
Armstrong, E. A., Gleckman-Krut, M., & Johnson, L. (2018). An intersec-
tional approach to sexual violence. Annual Review of Sociology, 44,
99–122 . ht tps ://doi.or g/10.114 6/an nur ev- soc-07 3117-0 41410
3 Altho ugh we chose to use an a dditive mode l in our con ceptual ization of
inters ectionalit y, we did not w ant to take a n additive theo retical appr oach whereby
identities are seen as independent, and, therefore, may encourage a ranking of
margin alizat ion, or an “oppre ssion Oly mpics ” (for a discussi on, see Else- Ques t & Hyde,
2016), such a s what woul d occur were we to tr y to weigh each ide ntity by what is th e
“most har mful” to QOL (se e also Footno te 1).
8 
|
   REINKA Et Al.
Barry, C. L ., McGinty, E. E., Pescosolido, B. A., & Goldman, H. A. (2014).
Stigma, discrimination, treatment effectiveness, and policy: Public
views about drug addiction and ment al illness. Psychiatric Services,
65(10), 1269–1272. https ://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.20140 0140
Beal, F. M. (2008). Deoble jeopardy: To be Black and female. Meridians:
Feminism, Race, Transnationalism, 8(2), 166–176.
Bowleg, L. (2012). The problem with the phrase women and minorities:
Intersectionality—An important theoretical framework for public
health. American Journal of Public Health, 102(7), 1267–1273. https ://
doi.org /10.2105/AJ PH. 2012.3 00750
Chaudoir, S. R., & Quinn, D. M. (2016). Evidence that anticipated stigma
predicts poorer depressive symptom trajectories among emerging
adults living with concealable stigmatized identities. Self and Identity,
15, 139–151. https ://doi.org/10.1080/15298 868.2015.1091378
Conwill, W. L. (2010). Domestic violence among the Black poor:
Intersectionality and social justice. International Journal for the
Advancement of Counseling, 32, 31–45. https ://doi.org /10.1007/
s10 4 47- 0 09-9 087-z
Crenshaw, K. (1989). Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: A
Black feminist critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory,
and antiracist policy. The University of Chicago Legal Forum, 1, 139–167.
Crocker, J., Major, B., & Steele, C . (1998). Social stigma. In D. T. Gilbert,
S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp.
504–553). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Dlugonski, D., Martin, T. R., Mailey, E. L., & Pineda, E. (2017). Motives
and barriers for physical activity among low-income black single
mothers. Sex Roles, 77(5-6), 379-392. https ://doi.org/10.1007/
s11199- 016-0718-7
Else-Quest, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2016). Intersectionality in quantita-
tive psychological research: I. Theoretical and epistemological is-
sues. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40(2), 155–170. https ://doi.
org/10.1177/03616 84316 629797
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of a spoiled identity.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Goodin, B. R., Owens, M. A., White, D. M., Strath, L. J., Gonzalez, C.,
Rainey, R. L ., … Merlin, J. S. (2018). Intersectional health-related
stigma in persons living with HIV and chronic pain: Implications
for depressive symptoms. AIDS Care, 30(S2), 66–73. https ://doi.
org/10.1080/09540 121.2018.1468012
Grollman, E. A. (2014). Multiple disadvantaged statuses and health: The
role of multiple forms of discrimination. Social Disadvantages and
Health Disparities, 55(1), 3–19. ht tps ://doi.o rg /10.1177/00221 46514
521215
Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Dovidio, J. (2009). How
does stigma ‘get under the skin’? The mediating role of emotion
regulation. Psychological Science, 20 (10), 1282–1289. https ://doi.
org /10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.024 41.x
Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Phelan, J. C., & Link, B. G . (2013). Stigma as a
fundamental cause of population health inequalities. American
Journal of Public Health, 103(5), 813–821. https ://doi.org/10.2105/
AJPH.2012.301069
Hayes, A . F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional
process analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Johnson, B. T., Redding, C. A., DiClemente, R. J., Mustanski, B. S., Dodge,
B., Sheeran, P., … Fishbein, M. (2010). A network-individual-resource
model for HIV prevention. AIDS and Behavior, 14, 20 4–221. https ://
doi.org /10.10 07/s10461-010 -9803-z
Kline, R . B. (2005). Principles and practice of s tructural equation modeling
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Lewis, R . J., Milletich, R. J., Mason, T. B., & Derlega, V. J. (2014). Pathways
connecting sexual minority stressors and psychological distress
among lesbian women. Gay & Lesbian Social Services, 26(2), 147–167.
https ://doi.org/10.108 0/1053 8 720.2014.891452
Lewis, T. T., & Van Dyke, M. E. (2018). Discrimination and the health of
African Americans: The potential importance of intersectionalities.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(3), 176–182. https ://
doi.org/10.1177/09637 21418 7704
Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. (1995). Social conditions as fundamental c auses
of disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 35, 80–94. https ://
doi.org/10.2307/2626958
Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. C. (2001). Conceptualizing stigma. Annual
Review of Sociology, 27, 363–385. https ://doi.org/10.1146/annur
ev.soc.27.1.363
Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. (2014). Stigma power. Social Science and Medicine,
103, 24–32. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.socsc imed.2013.07.035
Liu , T., & Wong, Y. J. (20 18). The inter se ct io n of r ace and gender: Asian
American men’s experience of discrimination. Psychology of Men
and Masculinity, 19(1), 89–101. https ://doi.org/10.1037/men00
00084
Mendoza-Denton, R., Downey, G., Purdie, V. J., Davis, A., & Pietrzak,
J. (2002). Sensitivity to status-based rejec tion: Implications
for African-American students’ college experiences. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 896–918. https ://doi.
org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.4.896
Mereish, E. H., & Bradford, J. B. (2014). Intersecting identities and sub-
stance use problems: Sexual orientation, gender, race, and lifetime
substance use problems. Journal of Studies on Alchohol and Drugs,
75(1), 179–188. htt ps ://doi.org/10.15288/ jsad.2 014.75.179
Merriam-Webster. (2017, April). Words we’re watching: Intersec tionality.
What happens when forms of discrimination combine, overlap, and
intersect. Retrieved from: https ://www.merri am-webst er.com/
words-at-play/inter secti onali ty-meaning
Moberl y, N. J., & Watkin s, E. J. (2008). Ru minative self-focus an d negative
affect: An experience sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
117(2), 314–323. https ://doi.org /10.1037/0021-843X .117.2.314
Mohr, J., & Fassinger, R. (200 0). Measuring dimensions of lesbian and
gay male experience. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling
and Development, 33(2), 66–90. https ://doi.org/10.1080/07481
756.2000.12068999
N o l en - Ho ek se ma , S. , St ic e , E. , Wa d e , E. , & Bo ho n, C. ( 20 07 ). R e ci p ro ca l re -
lations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depres-
sive symptoms in female adole scents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
116(1), 198. htt ps ://doi.org /10.1037/0021-8 43X .116.1.198
O’Donnell, A. T., Corrigan, F., & Gallagher, S. (2015). The impact of an-
ticipated stigma on psychological and physical health problems in
the unemployed group. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1263. https ://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01263
Ottaviani, C ., Thayer, J. F., Verkuil, B., Lonigro, A., Medea, B.,
Couyoumdjian, A., & Brosschot, J. F. (2016). Physiological concomi-
tants of perseverative cognition: A systematic review and meta-anal-
ysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142(3), 231–259. https ://doi.or g/10.1037/
bul00 00036
Pachankis, J. E., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Wang, K., Burton, C. L ., Crawford,
F. W., Phelan, J. C., & L ink, B. G. (2018). T he burden of st igma on health
and well-being: A taxonomy of concealment, course, disruptiveness,
aesthetics, origin, and peril across 93 stigmas. Per sonalit y and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 44(4), 451–474. https ://doi.org /10.117 7/01461
67217 741313
Pinel, E. C. (1999). Stigma consciousness: The psychological legacy of
social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 (1),
114–128. ht tp s ://doi .org/10.10 37/0022-3514.76 .1.114
Pinel, E. C. (2004). You’re just saying that because I’m a woman: Stigma
consciousness and attributions to discrimination. Self and Identity,
3(1), 39–51. https ://doi.org/10.1080/13576 50034 2000031
Powell, A . J., Hlavka, H. R. , & Mulla , S. (2017). Int er sect ional it y an d credi-
bility in child sexual assault trials. Gender and Society, 31(4), 457–480.
h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 1 7 7 / 0 8 9 1 2 4 3 2 1 7 7 1 6 1 1 6
Quinn, D. M., & Chaudoir, S. R. (20 09). Living with a concealable stig-
matized identity: The impact of anticipated stigma, centrality, sa-
lience, and cultural stigma on psychological distress and health.
  
|
 9
REINK A Et Al.
Journal of Personality and So cial Psychology, 97, 652–666. https ://doi.
org/10.1037/a0 015815
Quinn, D. M., & Earnshaw, V. A. (2013). Concealable stigmatized identi-
ties and psychological well-being. Social and Personality Psychology
Compass, 7(1), 4 0–51. htt ps ://doi.o rg /10.1111 /spc3.1 20 05
Quinn, D. M., Weisz, B. M., & Lawner, E. K. (2017). Impact of active con-
cealment of stigmatized identities on physical and psychological
quality of life. Social Science and Medicine, 192, 14–17. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.socsc imed.2017.09.024
Quinn, D. M., Williams, M. K., Quintana, F., Gaskins, J. L., Overstreet,
N. M., Pishori, A., … Chaudoir, S. R. (2014). Examining effects of
anticipated stigma, centrality, salience, internalization, and outness
on psychological distress for people with concealable stigmatized
identities. PLoS ONE, 9(5), e96977. https ://doi.org/10.1371/journ
al.p one.0096977
Reidpath, D. D., & Chan, K. Y. (20 05). A method for th e quantitative anal-
ysis of the layering of HIV-related stigma. AIDS Care, 17(4), 425–432.
h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / 0 9 5 4 0 1 2 0 4 1 2 3 3 1 3 1 9 7 6 9
Rodriguez-Seijas, C., Burton, C. L., Adeyinka, O., & Pachankis, J. E. (2019).
On the quantitative study of multiple marginalization: Paradox and
potential solution. Stigma and Health, 4(4), 495–502. https ://doi.
org/10.1037/sah00 00166
Rosenthal, L. (2016). Incorporating intersectionality into psycholog y:
An opportunity to promote social justice and equity. American
Psychology, 71(6), 474–485. https ://doi.org/10.1037/a0040323
Sansone, R. A ., & Sansone, L . A . (2012). Rumination: Relationships with
physical health. Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience, 9(2), 29–34.
Schoemann, A. M., Boulton, A. J., & Short, S. D. (2017). Determining
power and sample size for simple and complex mediation models.
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 379–386. https ://doi.
org /10.117 7/19485 50 617 715068
Schomerus, G., Lucht, M., Holzinger, A., Matschinger, H., Cart a, M. G ., &
Angermeyer, M. C. (2011). The stigma of alcohol dependence com-
pared with other mental disorders: A review of population studies.
Alcohol and Alcoholism, 46(2), 105–112. https ://doi.org/10.1093/alcal
c/agq089
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (20 02). Contending with group
image: The psychological of stereotype and social identity threat.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 379–440.
Sugarman, B. D., Nation, M., Yuan, N. P., Kuperminc, G. P., Ayoub, L. H., &
Hamby, S. (2018). Hate and violence: Addressing discrimination based
on race, ethnicity, religion, sexual orientation, and gender identity.
Psychology of Violence, 8(6), 649–656. https ://doi.org/10.1037/vio00
00222
The WHOQOL Group. (1998). Development of the World Health
Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment.
Psychological Medicine, 28(3), 551–558.
Timmins, L., Rimes, K. A., & Rahman, Q. (2017). Minority stressors, ru-
mination, and psychological distress in monozygotic twins discor-
dant for sexual minority status. Psychological Medicine, h t t p s ://d o i .
org /10.1017/S00 33 29171 700321 X
Torres, L., Mata-Greve, F., Bird, C., & Herrera Hernandez, E. (2018).
Intersectionality research within Latinx mental health: Conceptual
and methodological considerations. Journal of Latino/a Psychology,
6(4), 304–317. https ://doi.org/10.1037/lat00 00122
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination re-
considered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
27(3), 247–259. https ://doi.org /10.1023/A:10239 10315561
Velez, B. L., Moradi, B., & DeBlaere, C. (2014). Multiple oppressions
and the mental health of sexual minority Latina/o individuals. The
Counseling Psychologist, 43(1), 7–38. http s ://doi.org/10.1177/00110
00014 542836
Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., Gebhardt, W. A., & Thayer, J. F. (2010). When
worries make you sick: A review of perseverative cognition, the
default stress response and somatic health. Journal of Experimental
Psychopathology, 1(1), 87–118. https ://doi.org /10.5127/jep.009110
Watkins, E., & Baracaia, S. (2001). Why do people ruminate in dysphoric
moods? Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 723–734. https ://
doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00053-2
Williams, S. L., & Fredrick, E. G. (2015). One size may not fit all: The
need for a more inclusive and intersectional psychological sci-
ence on stigma. Sex Roles, 73, 384–390. https ://doi.org/10.1007/
s11199- 015-0 491-z
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional supporting information may be found online in the
Supporting Information section.
How to cite this article: Reinka MA, Pan-Weisz B, Lawner EK,
Quinn DM. Cumulative consequences of stigma: Possessing
multiple concealable stigmatized identities is associated with
worse quality of life. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2020;00:1–9.
https ://doi.or g/10.1111/ jasp.12656
... Persons with mental illness and persons belonging to other vulnerable populations in which the prevalence of mental illness is high often engage in excessive rumination, which predicts significantly higher levels of self-stigma, anticipated stigma, and/or perceived stigma (Hatzenbuehler et al., 2009;Liao et al., 2015;Reinka et al., 2020;Timmins et al., 2020;Vilus & Perich, 2021;Wang et al., 2017). Mental illness stigma alone negatively impacts identity stability, help-seeking behaviors, self-efficacy, interpersonal relationships, goal attainment, and treatment effectiveness and adherence (Corrigan et al., 2016;Deacon & Baird, 2009;Drapalski et al., 2013;Fox et al., 2018;Horsfield et al., 2020;Stolzenburg et al., 2017;Walsh & Foster, 2020). ...
... It is possible that those with less severe depressive symptoms have more hope of finding a mental health professional who will not endorse mental illness stigma. Additionally, anticipated stigma is associated with poor physical health for American adults who possess multiple concealable stigmatized identities, including mental illness (Reinka et al., 2020). Even low levels of anticipated stigma are predictive of psychological distress and poorer health outcomes, particularly among individuals who view their mental illness(es) as central to their identity and/or are conscious of their mental illness(es) on a regular basis (Quinn & Chaudoir, 2009). ...
... Rumination as a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy has been reported by stigma experts in a variety of domains (e.g., psychiatry, sociology) to be significantly correlated with concealable stigmatized identities such as mental illness, perceived and actual public stigma, anticipated stigma, internalized stigma, and disruptive social interactions resulting from stigma (Pachankis et al., 2017;Reinka et al., 2020). Eating disorders, for example, are associated with both brooding and reflective rumination about internalized weight-related stigma . ...
Research
Full-text available
Persons with mental illness are highly susceptible to experiencing public, anticipated, and internalized mental illness stigma, which can manifest in a myriad of ways to negatively affect quality of life, treatment outcomes, and attainment of personally meaningful goals. Research also shows that the impostor phenomenon and rumination are prominent cognitive processes experienced by persons with mental illness in unique ways. This is the first study to examine the relationship among all three psychological constructs. Survey data were collected from an online sample of 90 participants with one or more professionally diagnosed mental illnesses. Mental illness stigma was assessed with the Inventory of Stigmatizing Experiences and the Internalized Stigma of Mental Illness scale. The impostor phenomenon was assessed with the Leary Impostorism Scale, and rumination was assessed with the Scott-McIntosh Rumination Inventory. Correlational analyses revealed that public, anticipated, and internalized mental illness stigma scores were significantly correlated with both impostor phenomenon and rumination scores. Impostor phenomenon scores were significantly correlated with rumination scores as well. Moderated multiple regression models revealed that the impostor phenomenon moderated the relationship between public mental illness stigma and the distraction dimension of rumination. These results add to the growing body of evidence documenting the nuanced ways in which stigma interacts with other psychological factors to impact persons with mental illness.
... Exacerbating structural and social stigma surrounding criminal involvement, many people involved in the CLS have multiple stigmatized attributes (e.g., minoritized race/ethnicity, substance use disorder, mental health disorder, poverty) which accumulate to increase social disadvantage and hinder community reentry (Hartwell, 2004;Reinka et al., 2020;Tyler & Brockmann, 2017). Specifically, research not only suggests cumulative consequences of being a member of multiple marginalized groups (Reinka et al., 2020), but it also suggests that certain intersecting stigmatized attributes subject people to particularly negative judgments, unique social disadvantages, and amplified stress (i.e., intersectionality theory; Cole, 2009). ...
... Exacerbating structural and social stigma surrounding criminal involvement, many people involved in the CLS have multiple stigmatized attributes (e.g., minoritized race/ethnicity, substance use disorder, mental health disorder, poverty) which accumulate to increase social disadvantage and hinder community reentry (Hartwell, 2004;Reinka et al., 2020;Tyler & Brockmann, 2017). Specifically, research not only suggests cumulative consequences of being a member of multiple marginalized groups (Reinka et al., 2020), but it also suggests that certain intersecting stigmatized attributes subject people to particularly negative judgments, unique social disadvantages, and amplified stress (i.e., intersectionality theory; Cole, 2009). For instance, the structural and social stigma surrounding a criminal record is particularly severe for Black men and women, who face more barriers than white individuals to accessing adequate health care, treatment, employment, and other resources (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2013), and who are subjected to specific stereotypes about violence, substance use, and criminality (Najdowski, 2023;Welch, 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to systematically review the types of stigmatized attributes that have been assessed and the causes and consequences of stigma for individuals involved in the criminal legal system (CLS). PubMed, GoogleScholar, and APA PsycInfo databases were searched to identify studies for inclusion through March 2021. Eligible studies were peer reviewed, quantitative, and assessed stigma from the perspective of the person involved in the CLS. Fifty-nine studies were included (total n = 21,738), assessing stigma associated with criminal involvement, HIV, substance use, race/ethnicity, help-seeking, and others. Experiencing criminal involvement stigma was linked to poor well-being, but less so for racial/ethnic minorities. Experiencing racial/ethnic stigma was associated with recidivism risk, and substance use stigma was associated with substance use risk. Several stigmas intersected to impact treatment engagement and well-being. In conclusion, individuals involved in the CLS experience many stigmatized statuses that impact their well-being, treatment adherence, community integration, and criminal behavior. Stigma must be addressed among individuals involved in the CLS and the systems they interact with to reduce health inequity and recidivism risk.
... SGM individuals living with mental illness experience intersectional stigma related to their inseparable marginalized identities of sexual orientation, gender identity, or both, and mental illness. As individuals encompassing more than one identity, SGM individuals living with a mental illness face unique and challenging experiences of intersectional stigma associated with multiple identities, leading to worse quality of life outcomes (Berger, 2010;Cole, 2009;Reinka et al., 2020;Turan et al., 2019). ...
Article
Introduction: Sexual and gender minority (SGM) individuals living with mental illness often experience stigma associated with marginalized identities of sexual orientation, gender identity, and mental illness (MI). Sharing stories of lived experiences is an effective approach to reducing various forms of stigma; however, it is unclear whether stories shared by SGM living with mental illness (SGM MI) can reduce MI- and SGM-related stigma. Methods: Using a randomized controlled trial design, participants watched digital stories of self-identified SGM individuals living with a mental illness, non-SGM individuals living with mental illness, or a control condition (TedTalks on environmental issues and growing up in China) to examine the use of representative digital stories in addressing SGM- and MI-related stigma. Results: In a sample of 218 participants, digital stories of SGM MI effectively reduced MI-related stigma (personal stigma (from 33.19 to 31.90) and discrimination (from 8.33 to 7.57)), but were ineffective at reducing SGM-related personal stigma (negative attitudes toward lesbians and gay men, transphobia, or genderism; p > .05).Conclusion: Our study highlights the need to develop culturally adapted anti-stigma programs in collaboration with individuals with lived intersectional SGM and MI experiences.
... Experiencing stigma can also exert negative influence on individuals' quality of life and physical, psychological, and social well-being (Reinka et al., 2020). The dissemination of scientific content in which stepfamily stigmatization is encased can exacerbate these issues. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite their ubiquity, stepfamilies generally hold a stigmatized status. The scientific community at large has not been immune to the influence of stepfamily stigmatization. Misusing the term “stepchild” in science is unnecessary on several fronts. “Stepchild” is often intended to denote neglect, oversight, or mistreatment. Scholars should consider using more direct and precise language, especially considering that scientific writing benefits from clarity, parsimony, and precision. In any case, it’s time to stop using “stepchild” as a pejorative term.
... On its own, internalized stigma has been found to reduce quality of life, self-esteem, hope in recovery or in the self, self-efficacy, vocational functioning, social adaptation, and increase avoidant coping, depressive symptoms, social avoidance, and shame (Corrigan et al., 2009;Shin et al., 2016;Yanos et al., 2010). Overall, stigmatizing experiences can compound on each other resulting in a series of cumulative costs for individuals with lived experiences of psychological distress (Reinka et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Discrimination toward individuals with lived experiences of mental illness is widespread within the field of clinical psychology. Further, there is some presence of clinical psychologists who are both consumers and providers of mental health services, termed prosumers. However, no research has evaluated how witnessing discrimination as part of professional activities may influence prosumers’ experiences with internalized stigma, anticipated stigma, and stigma resistance. This exploratory study aimed to establish associations and interactions between having witnessed discrimination toward others with lived experiences of mental illness and internalized stigma, anticipated stigma, and stigma resistance from the perspective of prosumers within the clinical psychology field. A cross-sectional quantitative approach was employed to understand these dynamics by utilizing descriptive, correlational, and multivariate regressions analysis. A total 175 prosumers (39 graduated doctoral-level clinical psychologists and 136 in training) completed survey measures pertaining to witnessed discrimination, internalized and anticipated stigma, and stigma resistance. Prosumers reported witnessing frequent subtle and overt discrimination by their colleagues, supervisors, and faculty members. Overt discrimination was reported as witnessed more frequently compared with subtle discrimination experiences or microaggressions. Our findings have implications for the prevalence of witnessed discrimination and how these may create cumulative experiences of stigma and stigma resistance among prosumers in clinical psychology. Further research should explore additional understanding of how clinical psychologists, including prosumers, may hold stigmatizing attitudes and perpetuate discrimination toward individuals with lived experiences of mental illness.
... Additionally, we did not explore how some shared identities are more easily hidden (e.g., LGBT) than others (e.g., Hispanic), which may impact quality of life 67 . Our results showed nearly equal acute stress effect sizes between concealable (LGBT, interpersonal violence victim, political affiliation) and non-concealable (race/ethnicity, gender) identities. ...
Article
Full-text available
When an individual or group trauma becomes a shared public experience through widespread media coverage (e.g., mass violence, being publicly outed), sharing a social identity with a targeted individual or group of victims may amplify feelings of personal vulnerability. This heightened perceived threat may draw people to engage with trauma-related media because of increased vigilance for self-relevant threats, which can, in turn, amplify distress. We studied this possibility among two U.S. national samples following the 2016 Pulse nightclub massacre in Orlando, FL (N = 4675) and the 2018 Dr. Christine Blasey Ford and Judge Brett Kavanaugh Supreme Court Senate hearings (N = 4894). Participants who shared LGBT or Hispanic identities with Pulse massacre victims reported greater exposure to massacre-related media and acute stress. Participants who shared Dr. Blasey Ford’s identities as a victim of interpersonal violence and a Democrat reported more hearings-related media exposure and acute stress. Indirect effects of shared single identity on acute stress through self-reported event-related media exposure emerged in both studies. Results for sharing dual identities with victims were mixed. These findings have implications for media use and public health.
... Since spontaneous self-affirmation and social support report similar findings on health, we predict that these two concepts would be positively correlated with one another. These psychological and physical health benefits may be especially important to people with CSIs who report lower psychological and physical quality of life (Quinn & Earnshaw, 2013;Reinka et al., 2020), but this relationship has yet to be empirically tested in a sample of people with CSIs. ...
Article
Full-text available
People with concealable stigmatized identities (CSIs)—when one possesses an identity that is devalued but can be hidden—report having high rates of poor health outcomes. Limited research examines how severity of a CSI (e.g., how much distress the CSI causes the person), or spontaneous self-affirmation (e.g., when one reaffirms themselves after a threatening experience) may be related to health among people with CSIs. Students from a large Midwestern University (N = 294) answered questions about their CSI (i.e., centrality, severity, active concealment), coping mechanisms (i.e., social support, spontaneous self-affirmation), and health (i.e., psychological and physical quality of life (QOL), life satisfaction). Using structural equation modeling, we found that severity was indirectly associated with lower life satisfaction through higher active concealment, lower social support, and lower psychological quality of life. Severity did not directly impact psychological quality of life. Spontaneous self-affirmation was indirectly associated with greater life satisfaction through greater psychological quality of life. This model illustrates how amplifiers (i.e., severity, centrality, active concealment) and potential coping mechanisms (i.e., social support, spontaneous self-affirmation) impact health downstream in individuals with CSIs.
Article
Full-text available
While possessing multiple stigmas is a common experience, research using a systematic method on this topic to quantify the total number of stigmas and their dimensions is very limited. The purpose of the current research is to examine the number and dimensions of multiple stigmas that university students experience and, moreover, to investigate whether self-compassion mediates the negative effect of multiple stigmas on resilience. Three studies (study 1: n = 476, study 2: n = 443, study 3: n = 321) were conducted in northern and southern locations of Appalachian United States, in which participants reported on their experience with multiple stigmas, self-compassion, and resilience. Depression, obesity, and poverty were the most frequently reported stigmas. Aligned with the hypotheses, the total number of multiple stigmas predicted lower resilience that was mediated by reduced self-compassion. Furthermore, after quantifying the six dimensions of stigma (disruptiveness, origin, visibility, peril, aesthetics, and persistence; Jones et al., 1984) with the taxonomy developed by Pachankis et al. (Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44:451–474, 2018), our results clarified that the disruptiveness of stigma consistently predicted lower resilience, mediated by weakened self-compassion. Other dimensions had significant but less consistent relationships with resilience and self-compassion. These results contribute to the literature on multiple stigmas and thereby their associations with outcomes such as resilience. The important mediating role of self-compassion is also highlighted and underscores an important pathway between multiple stigmas and resilience, which informs our discussion on the implications for the design of prevention and intervention programs on university campuses.
Article
Substance dependence is a prevalent and urgent public health problem. In 2021, 60 million Americans reported abusing alcohol within the month prior to being surveyed, and nearly 20 million Americans reported using illegal drugs (e.g., heroin) or prescription drugs (e.g., opioids) for nonmedical reasons in the year before. Drug-involved overdose rates have been steadily increasing over the past 20 years. This increase has been primarily driven by opioid and stimulant use. Despite its prevalence, drug dependence is one of the most stigmatized health conditions. Stigma has myriad negative consequences for its targets, including limiting their access to employment and housing, disrupting interpersonal relationships, harming physical and mental health, and reducing help-seeking. However, because research on stigma toward people with substance use disorders (SUDs) is relatively sparse compared with research on stigma toward other mental illnesses, the field lacks a comprehensive understanding of the causes and consequences of SUD stigma. Moreover, it remains unclear how, if at all, these factors differ from other types of mental illness stigma. The goal of this review is to take stock of the literature on SUD stigma, providing a clear set of foundational principles and a blueprint for future research and translational activity.
Article
Full-text available
Many people possess multiple stigmatized statuses. To date, however, the quantitative study of the health of people who possess multiple stigma statuses has typically only examined two or three candidate stigmata. The current study examined a comprehensive array of 93 stigmatized statuses, their natural co-occurrence within persons, and their simultaneous associations with mental health outcomes. In doing so, we first illustrate a key paradox of applying quantitative methods to the study of multiple stigmatization. For instance, in the present sample of United States adults (N = 1,123), the average participant endorsed possessing nearly six stigmatized statuses (M = 6.08, SD = 4.58). The impossibility of meaningfully attending to all possible stigma combinations is illustrated by the existence of 2,354 unique pairs across the 93 stigmatized statuses examined in this study. As a potential solution, we examined the association between a rank-ordered list of the subjective importance that each participant assigned to each of their stigmatized statuses, and poor mental health outcomes. Using an objective index of stigma directed toward each of the 93 statuses, we find that only the stigma directed toward each participant’s self-rated most important stigmatized status (rather than their second-, third-, fourth-, and so on, most important statuses) was significantly associated with poor mental health. Findings are discussed in terms of implications for future quantitative approaches to multiple stigmatization and health.
Article
Full-text available
Social scientists have a potentially important role to play in combatting discrimination and hate-motivated aggression, as has been noted for over 50 years. Nonetheless, there is still relatively little research in this area, despite increasing recognition of discrimination on the basis of race, ethnicity, religion, gender identity, sexual orientation, and other characteristics. One of the most important scientific trends in research on both discrimination and violence has been recognition of the intersectionality among many of these phenomena, including the intersection of characteristics that confer privilege or disadvantage, of different types of hate-motivated aggression, or among hate-motivated aggression and other forms of victimization. Much work still needs to be done to unpack the diverse sources and complex nature of prejudicial attitudes. In light of current events around the world, we are in urgent need of evidence-based approaches for prevention and intervention that focus both on individuals who commit hate-motivated aggression and those who are the targets of such behaviors. In this introduction to the special issue on hate and violence, we highlight key themes of a series of articles that advance our knowledge in this area. Given that they cover some, but not all, of the topics related to discrimination, we also present several recommendations for future research.
Article
Full-text available
Intersectionality considers the meaning and consequences associated with multiple identities along interlocking systems of disadvantage and inequality (Cole, 2009; Crenshaw, 1991). In recent years, there has been increasing attention on examining the mental health outcomes associated with membership in multiple marginalized groups. Unfortunately, intersectionality research examining the unique experiences of Latinx groups remains scarce. The current article reviews theoretical and methodological considerations regarding intersectionality research within Latinx mental health. From a theoretical perspective, intersectionality brings a series of questions regarding the epistemological approaches to studying psychological phenomena. This, in turn, influences the methodological strategies used to examine these processes. The discussion advances the ongoing discourse regarding the benefits and limitations of integrating intersectionality within research that provides further insight into our Latinx communities.
Article
Full-text available
“Intersectional health-related stigma” (IHRS) refers to stigma that arises at the convergence of multiple health conditions. People living with HIV (PLWH) and chronic pain have two highly stigmatized health conditions, and thus may be at especially high risk for internalizing these stigmas and consequently experiencing depression. This study examined the intersectionality of internalized HIV and chronic pain stigma in relation to depressive symptoms in a sample of PLWH and chronic pain. Sixty participants were recruited from an HIV clinic in the Southeastern United States. Chronic pain was defined as pain that has been present for at least three consecutive months, and that has been an ongoing problem for at least half the days in the past six months. All participants completed the HIV Stigma Mechanisms Scale, Internalized Stigma in Chronic Pain Scale, the Short-Form Brief Pain Inventory, and the Center for Epidemiological Studies – Depression Scale. Clinical data was collected from medical records. An intersectional HIV and chronic pain composite variable was created and participants were categorized as either high (28%), moderate (32%), or low (40%). Results revealed that intersectional HIV and chronic pain stigma was significantly associated with severity of depressive symptoms (p = .023). Pairwise contrasts revealed that participants with high (p = .009) and moderate (p = .033) intersectional stigma reported significantly greater mean depressive symptom severity than those with low intersectional stigma. Participants who reported the highest levels of internalized HIV and chronic pain stigma also reported the greatest severity of depressive symptoms. This suggests that the experience of both HIV and chronic pain stigma (i.e., IHRS) among PLWH and chronic pain may synergistically perpetuate negative mood in a more profound manner than experiencing either one stigma alone.
Article
This article describes the development of new scales for assessing identity and outness in lesbians and gay men. Relevant measurement issues are reviewed.
Article
Internalized stigma undermines health among people diagnosed with HIV and other sexually transmitted infections (STI), yet limited research has examined how internalized stigma develops. Black gay and bisexual men (n = 151) reported their race and sexual orientation internalized stigma once before HIV/STI diagnosis and their HIV/STI internalized stigma monthly for 1 year after HIV/STI diagnosis. Multilevel analyses demonstrated that race and sexual orientation internalized stigma before diagnosis were associated with greater HIV/STI internalized stigma after diagnosis. More research is needed to understand how internalized stigma develops, including within the context of other identities and broader environmental characteristics to inform intervention efforts.
Article
In a preliminary exploration of atheists using a concealable stigmatized identity framework, we investigated outness, identity magnitude, anticipated stigma, and psychological and physical well-being. Atheists (n = 1,024) in the United States, completed measures of outness, atheist identity magnitude, anticipated stigma, and psychological and physical well-being online. Consistent with predictions, we found small but significant associations between (a) anticipated stigma and well-being, (b) social components of atheist identity magnitude and outness as well as well-being, and (c) outness and well-being. A significant and moderate association was found between anticipated stigma and outness. There were significant, small indirect effects of ingroup ties, a social component of atheist identity magnitude, on psychological and physical well-being via outness; and of ingroup affect, another social component of magnitude, on psychological well-being via disclosure of atheist identity. Implications for research, practice, and training are offered.
Article
Sexual violence reproduces inequalities of gender, race/ethnicity, class, age, sexuality, ability status, citizenship status, and nationality. Yet its study has been relegated to the margins of our discipline, with consequences for knowledge about the reproduction of social inequality. We begin with an overview of key insights about sexual violence elaborated by feminists, critical race scholars, and activists. This research leads us to conceptualize sexual violence as a mechanism of inequality that is made more effective by the silencing of its usage. We trace legal and cultural contestations over the definition of sexual violence in the United States. We consider the challenges of narrating sexual violence and review how the narrow focus on gender by some anti-sexual violence activism fails women of color and other marginalized groups. We conclude by interrogating the sociological silence on sexual violence.
Article
Research examining associations between self-reported experiences of discrimination overall (e.g., potentially due to race, gender, socioeconomic status, age) and health—particularly among African Americans—has grown rapidly over the past two decades. Yet recent findings suggest that self-reported experiences of racism alone may be less impactful for the health of African Americans than previously hypothesized. Thus, an approach that captures a broader range of complexities in the study of discrimination and health among African Americans may be warranted. This article presents an argument for the importance of examining intersectionalities in studies of discrimination and physical health in African Americans and provides an overview of research in this area.