ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Evans, Wilson, Needham, and Brentnall (20036. Evans, J. J., Wilson, B. A., Needham, P., & Brentnall, S. (2003). Who makes good use of memory aids? Results of a survey of people with acquired brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(6), 925–935. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703960127View all references) investigated memory aid use by people with acquired brain injury (ABI) and found little use of technological memory aids. The present study aims to investigate use of technological and other memory aids and strategies 10 years on, and investigate what predicts use. People with ABI and self-reported memory impairments (n = 81) completed a survey containing a memory aid checklist, demographic questions and memory questionnaires. Chi-square analysis showed that 10 of 18 memory aids and strategies were used by significantly more people in the current sample than in Evans et al. (20036. Evans, J. J., Wilson, B. A., Needham, P., & Brentnall, S. (2003). Who makes good use of memory aids? Results of a survey of people with acquired brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(6), 925–935. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703960127View all references). The most commonly used strategies were leaving things in noticeable places (86%) and mental retracing of steps (77%). The most commonly used memory aids were asking someone to remind you (78%), diaries (77%), lists (78%), and calendars (79%) and the most common technologies used were mobile phone reminders (38%) and alarms/timers (38%). Younger people who used more technology prior to their injury and who use more non-technological memory aids currently were more likely to use technology. Younger people who used more memory aids and strategies prior to their injury and who rated their memory as poorer were more likely to use all types of memory aids and strategies.
Technological Memory Aid Use by People with
Acquired Brain Injury
Jamieson, Matthew1,2, Cullen, Breda1, McGee-Lennon, Marilyn3, Brewster, Stephen2 and
Evans, Jonathan1
1 University of Glasgow Institute of Health and Wellbeing, Gartnavel Hospital, Glasgow,
Scotland
2Human Computer Interaction, Department of Computing Science, University of Glasgow,
Scotland
2Computer and Information Science, University of Strathclyde, Scotland, UK.
Abstract
Evans, Wilson, Needham and Brentnall (2003) investigated memory aid use by people with
acquired brain injury (ABI) and found little use of technological memory aids. The present
study aims to investigate use of technological memory aids and other memory aids and
strategies ten years on, and investigate what predicts use. People with ABI and self-reported
memory impairments (n = 81) completed a survey containing a memory aid checklist,
demographic questions and memory questionnaires. Chi-square analysis showed that ten of
18 memory aids and memory strategies were used by significantly more people in the
current sample than in Evans et al. (2003). The most commonly used strategies were
leaving things in noticeable places (86%) and mental retracing of steps (77%). The most
commonly used memory aids were asking someone to remind you (78%), diaries (77%), lists
(78%) and calendars (79%) and the most common technologies used were mobile phone
reminders (38%) and alarms/ timers (38%). Younger people who used more technology prior
to their injury and who use more non-technological memory aids currently were more likely
to use technology. Younger people who used more memory aids and strategies prior to their
injury and who rated their memory as poorer were more likely to use all types of memory
aids and strategies.
Keywords: assistive technology, memory aids, memory rehabilitation, acquired brain injury.
Author correspondence: Matthew Jamieson; m.jamieson.1@research.gla.ac.uk
Introduction
Individuals who have suffered acquired brain injury (ABI) have a high prevalence of memory
impairments (Ownsworth, & McFarland 1999). These impairments make it difficult for people
with ABI to perform everyday tasks which require memory such as personal care, cooking, or
tasks related to health such as remembering appointments, treatment plans and medication.
From both an economic and psychological point of view it is beneficial to help those with
memory difficulties to live independently at home, rather than in care homes, where possible
(Pollack, 2005). ABI is estimated to cost the UK government around £7bn per year
(Department of Health, 2005). This is similar in other countries, for example the cost of
caring for those who have had a head injury is estimated to be $50 billion annually in the
USA (Finkelstein, Corso and Miller, 2006). Many people with ABI who live within the
community are cared for by family members or friends who help alleviate the strain on care
1
services. However, caring for people with memory difficulties can lead to psychological
stress for those providing the support and care (Brodaty & Donkin, 2009; Caprani, Greaney
& Porter, 2006). Interventions which improve independence can be beneficial socially and
economically, by allowing people to stay in their homes for longer and by reducing
carer/caregiver strain.
Memory aid use
Pencil and paper memory aids and memory strategies can be useful and are used by many
people with ABI. Evans and colleagues (2003) investigated which memory compensation
aids and strategies people with ABI (n=94) used and which factors predicted use (Evans et
al., 2003). They found that calendars (54% of participants), wallcharts (64%) and notebooks
(72%) were commonly used by participants. Strategies such as asking others to remind
(49%) and mental retracing (48%) were also commonly used. The use of these
compensatory aids and strategies requires effort and time from the client. Clinicians can play
a role in encouraging and developing memory aid use and it is important to understand
which strategies are most commonly used, what factors predict use and how use changes
over time.
Wilson et al. (1996) and Evans et al. (2003) investigated which factors predicted use of
memory aids. They found that people who were younger, had a greater amount of time since
injury, used more memory aids prior to injury, had a higher level of independence and better
attentional functioning used more memory aids (Wilson et al., 1996; Evans et al., 2003).
Assistive technology
One type of memory aid which Evans and colleagues (2003) found was rarely used was
assistive technology for memory compensation. They predicted that use would increase as
technology becomes more widely available and more advanced. Technology has an
advantage over pencil and paper reminders because it can actively prompt participants
about memory tasks. In previous years, portable or wearable Personal Digital Assistants
(PDAs) such as Palm PDA or NeuroPage pager have been used, and can be programmed
by either the patient or carer to give prompts throughout the day to remind a person with
memory problems to attend appointments or take their medication (e.g. Wilson, Emslie,
Quirk & Evans, 2001; Svoboda & Richards, 2009). The more recent development of
Smartphone and Tablet devices has allowed these functions to be carried out using easily
accessible mainstream hardware and software. Other recently developed bespoke systems
can guide people through a single task with several sub-steps such as food preparation
(Kinempt; Chang, Chen & Chuang, 2011) or hand-washing (COACH; Mihailidis Carmichael
& Boger, 2004) or remind people of events which they may have forgotten by playing back a
series of photos taken during the day (SenseCam; Hodges et al., 2005).
Efficacy of assistive technology
In a review of assistive technology for cognition, Gillespie, Best and O’Neill (2012) reviewed
the application of technology for different cognitive functioning domains using the World
Health Organisation International Classification of Functioning (ICF) framework. The
domains relevant to everyday activities which involve memory included ‘organisation and
planning’, ‘time management’, ‘memory’ and ‘attention’. Gillespie and colleagues found that
the majority of technologies that were designed to improve ICF ‘organisation and planning’
2
were micro-prompting systems which supported step-by-step completion of tasks with sub-
steps, such as cooking or washing hands. All of the technologies which were designed to
help with ICF ‘time management’ were prospective memory reminding systems which
prompted to interrupt one task in order to carry out another. A smaller number of storing and
displaying and alerting technologies were also investigated in the literature and these aimed
to help with the ICF domains of ‘memory’ and ‘attention’ respectively.
In a recent systematic review we investigated studies which tested the efficacy of
prospective memory reminding systems and micro-prompting systems for improving memory
performance on everyday tasks (Jamieson, Cullen, McGee-Lennon, Brewster & Evans,
2013). We found good evidence for the efficacy of both of these types of devices and a
meta-analysis of seven group studies testing prospective prompting devices gave a large
overall effect size (d = 1.27) (n=147). There is therefore good evidence that technology
exists that can improve performance on two ICF domains when compared to practice as
usual or a non-technological equivalent.
Prevalence of assistive technology use
While it seems that the need for memory rehabilitation is great and that technology can
improve everyday memory performance, it is less clear whether or not technological memory
aids are actually used by people with memory difficulties after ABI. Evans et al. 2003 found
that only 3.2% of people with ABI (n=94) were using a mobile phone to help their memory. At
present there is little provision for assistive technology within the National Health Service
(NHS) in the UK and use is likely to be driven by the person with memory difficulties, their
family members or suggested by a caregiver. It is likely that the situation is similar in
countries with a similar infrastructure to the UK. Use of assistive technology is likely to
require support from clinicians and caregivers who may themselves lack confidence with
technology. A study by Hart and colleagues (2003) found that clinicians of people with
traumatic brain injury believed that technology could help with cognitive difficulties memory,
planning, organization and task initiation. However participants reported low confidence in
their abilities to guide clients in using technology, especially if their experience with
technology was limited. In the last decade, personal technology has become highly
advanced and available, in particular with the popularisation of mobile phones and
smartphones. In 2015 almost 5 billion people use a mobile phone and 1.75 billion use
smartphones (Statista, 2015). In 2013 it was reported that 7 out of 10 people in Britain used
smartphones (Styles, 2013). These devices are now so widespread that they are likely to
already be used by many people with ABI and their caregivers. Mobiles, smartphones and
other widely available and accessible technology such as alarms, timers, tablets, personal
computers and cameras have the ability to provide reminders to help with prospective
memory, provide pictures and videos to help with retrospective memory and can provide
prompts to guide people through everyday tasks.
The aim of this study was to investigate the use of memory aids and strategies by people
with ABI. We also wished to investigate if the increase in the availability of mobile and
Smartphone devices with memory aid capabilities has been accompanied by an increase in
the use of digital memory aids by people with memory impairment, and to quantify and
describe that use. Any technologies which can help compensate for various types of memory
difficulties during everyday activities were included. If there is an increase in use of memory
3
aid technology then it will be interesting to investigate whether this use is predicted by the
same or different factors that predict non-technological memory aid use.
Study aims
1) To compare prevalence of memory aid use between 2003 (results from Evans et al.
(2003) and 2014.
2) To investigate the prevalence of technological and non-technological memory aid use, and
memory aid strategy use amongst people with ABI, and to find out which types of technology
are most commonly used and in what way.
3) To investigate which factors are associated with use of technological and non-
technological memory aids, and memory aid strategies.
Method
Participants
Participants were recruited between November 2013 and June 2014 and were identified
through NHS services in Scotland: Community Treatment Centre for Brain Injury (CTCBI)
within the United Kingdom National Health Service Greater Glasgow and Clyde (NHS
GG&C), and NHS Grampian. Recruitment was also undertaken through the UK brain injury
charity Headway, via meetings in Scottish localities (Glasgow, Falkirk, Lothian, Dumfries and
Aberdeen). Inclusion criteria were a diagnosis of ABI and memory difficulties as reported by
self or other. For participants recruited through Headway, memory impairment was self-
reported during initial discussion with the researcher. Participants recruited through the NHS
were only approached if improving memory had been established as a rehabilitation aim
after self-report of memory difficulties and / or a formal assessment from clinicians within the
service. Only people aged 18 and over who were able to give informed consent to participate
in the study were approached.
Materials
In the following order the survey consisted of:
1) Demographic questions (age, gender, work status and education level)
2) A memory aid use checklist adapted from Evans et al. (2003)
3) A self reported memory questionnaire (the Prospective and Retrospective Memory
Questionnaire - PRMQ (Crawford, Smith, Maylor, Della Sala, & Logie, 2003)).
Details about how the injury was acquired and time since injury were obtained from the
recruiting NHS service where available. Participants who were recruited through Headway
were asked to provide information about their injury on the first page of the survey below the
demographic information section.
The memory aid checklist was taken from Evans et al. (2003). Because this checklist
questionnaire was administered during face-to-face interviews in the original study, it was
adapted for the present study so that it could be easily understood in a postal survey format.
Types of memory aid were split into three categories – non-technological memory aids (such
4
as paper diaries or calendars), technological memory aids (such as mobile phone or alarm
based reminders) and strategies (such as leaving objects in noticeable or unusual places)
(see appendix for full list of items). In the technological reminders checklist the item ‘a mobile
phone to remind you’ and the item ‘asking someone to text you’ were both included to
separate those using a mobile phone calendar, reminding app or alarm from those simply
using a mobile phone to receive texts from a carer or family member to remind them about
tasks. For each item participants were asked whether they used it before their brain injury,
whether they use it now, how often they use it (daily, weekly or monthly) and how useful it is
(helps a lot, helps a little or does not help). After the technology reminders checklist there
was a space for people to write what they used tech memory aids for.
Procedure
This study took the form of a cross-sectional postal survey. Three hundred and eight people
with ABI were sent the survey with the expectation of a 1 in 3 response rate. The target
sample size of 100 was similar to the number of participants recruited by Evans et al. (2003)
(94 people with ABI). People with ABI were approached via the CTCBI in Glasgow and brain
injury services in NHS Grampian, with questionnaires being passed on to participants either
in person or through the post. Participants with ABI recruited through Headway were given
the forms by the researcher, Headway staff or volunteers at support group meetings. All
participants returned the survey to the researchers using a free-post envelope provided. The
study methods and the survey were approved by the University of Glasgow research ethics
committee on 14th October 2013.
Statistical analysis
Survey responses were only included in the analysis if both the memory aid checklist and
the PRMQ were fully completed. Five of the 86 returned surveys did not have both sections
completed, or had sections partially completed. These were removed from the analysis
leaving 81 fully completed surveys.
Independent t-tests were used to compare the current sample with the 2003 sample on
demographic variables. Chi squared tests were used to analyse the difference in proportion
of participants indicating they used each piece of technology between the two study
samples.
The outcome variables for the regression analyses were number of technological reminders
used after injury, and number of all types of memory aids used after injury. The ‘technological
reminders used’ variable was highly skewed a large number of participants used zero or
one technological memory aid only (59%). For this reason negative binomial regression was
used to investigate which factors predicted technological reminder use.
For negative binomial regression analysis, incidence rate ratio (IRR) was reported, with 95%
confidence interval (CI). IRR indicates the estimated relative change in the dependent
variable for each unit increase in the independent variable. For example, within a negative
binomial regression model predicting technological memory aid use, an IRR for age of 0.97
indicates that for every one-year increase in age, the number of technological memory aids
used would reduce by 3%.
5
A linear regression analysis was used to investigate the factors which predicted the number
of aids used (all types) as this variable was normally distributed. Predictors were added to
each model in a set order based on the findings reported by Evans et al. (2003). For the
models predicting technology use, age, pre-morbid technology use and current non-
technology use were added to the model first in a hierarchical manner followed by the other
factors. For models predicting all memory aid use, age and pre-morbid all memory aid use
were added to the model first in a hierarchical manner followed by the other factors. As each
factor was added to the model, an ANOVA analysis was performed to test whether the model
was significantly improved when the new factor was added.
Pearson’s correlations were used to investigate the relationship between memory ability and
memory aid use. The technological memory aid use variables (for both before and after
injury) were highly positively skewed and the ‘all memory aid use before injury’ variable was
also moderately positively skewed. These variables could not be assumed to be normally
distributed. For this reason non-parametric methods (Spearman’s rank for correlations) were
used when analysing these variables.
Participants’ comments about what they used technological memory aids for were grouped
according to the kinds of memory being supported. For example if a participant wrote ‘for
appointments’ then this would be coded as using technology to help with prospective
memory (future intentions). Three of the authors coded this written feedback independently
and then came to a consensus about any disagreement.
6
Results
Most participants (total n = 81) were recruited through CTCBI NHS GG&C (n=40, 49%) and
Headway (n=33, 41%) with a small number from NHS Grampian (n=8, 10%). Participants’
mean age was 51.2 years (range = 27 76, SD = 10.34) and 32 (40%) were female. The
most common aetiology of injury was traumatic brain injury (n=48, 59%) followed by
subarachnoid haemorrhage (n=9, 11%), stroke (n=5, 6%), aneurysm (n=4, 5%), encephalitis
(n=4, 5%), infection (n=4, 5%) and other (n=7, 9%). Median time since injury was 3.56 years
(range = 0.44 – 61, SD = 9.77, median reported due to a participant with a long time since
injury) and (n=20, 25%) were employed at the time of the survey. Mean number of years in
education was 12.74 (range 10 18, SD = 2.47).Table 1 shows participants’ PRMQ overall
and sub-scores, number of all memory aids used, technological aids, strategies and non-
technological aids.
Mean self-reported memory problems score, measured on the PRMQ, was around 1.5 to 2
standard deviations higher than the mean score for the general population (38.88, range =
17 - 67). This score was calculated in a large sample (n = 551) of healthy people between
the ages of 17 and 94 (Crawford et al., 2003). Crawford et al. found that age and gender did
not influence PRMQ scores so comparison to an age and gender matched sample is not
necessary. One third of the participants (33%) were within 1 standard deviation of the mean
PRMQ score for the general population.
[Table 1 about here]
Aim 1 To compare prevalence of memory aid use between 2003 and 2014.
The participants in the current study were significantly older than the participants in the 2003
study, who had a mean age of 39.53 (SD = 13.38) (t = 6.38, df = 173, p = 0.00001). The
mean years since injury in the 2003 sample (5.89, SD = 4.79) was lower than the current
sample but this difference was not significant (t = 1.0006, df = 173, p = 0.318). The current
sample spent significantly longer in education compared to the 2003 sample (2003 mean =
11.95 years, SD = 2.13) (t = 2.272, df = 173, p = 0.0243).
Table 2 compares the proportion of participants in the 2003 and 2014 samples who indicated
that they used each memory aid. Only the items which could be directly compared between
2003 and 2014 were included in this analysis. Chi-square analysis was used to examine
which aids and strategies were used by significantly different proportions of participants in
each study. For the technological memory aids, mobile phones and alarms/ timers were
used by a significantly higher proportion of people in the current study. Among the non-
technological aids, a significantly higher proportion of participants stated that they asked
someone to remind them, used lists on paper and used diaries. Five strategies were used by
a significantly greater proportion of participants in the current study compared to the
participants in the 2003 study. These strategies were mental retracing, repetitive practice,
objects in noticeable places, rhymes or phrases and alphabetic searching.
[Table 2 about here]
Aim 2 To investigate the prevalence of technological and non-technological memory aid
use, and memory aid strategy use amongst people with ABI, and to find out which types of
technology are most commonly used and in what way.
7
The proportion of people using each technology-based reminder, with participants’ perceived
helpfulness ratings, are shown in Figure 1.
[Figure 1 about here]
The prevalence of use of each non-technological strategy or aid, with participants’ perceived
helpfulness ratings, are shown in Figure 2.
[Figure 2 about here]
How memory aids were used
When coding the answers to the comment box question, If you use any of these
technological memory aids, what do you use them to remind you about?’, t here was
reasonable level of agreement between the three raters with 80% of the comments coded in
the same category by each rater. Thirty five participants (43.2%) gave relevant answers to
this question in the space provided. Some of the participants’ comments contained
information about more than one different use of technology and so there were 46 separate
comments analysed. The majority (n=30, 65%) of answers referred to reminders about
future intentions. These included using phone calendars, text messaging and alarms to alert
about appointments, household tasks, social events and medications. The second most
common use of technology was to wake up in the morning or after a nap (n=11, 24% of
comments mentioned using technology in this way). Three comments (6.5%) mentioned
using technology to help orient to time and date. One comment talked about using a mobile
phone to store information (e.g. who they had called) to prevent them doing the same thing
twice. There was also a single comment about using technology to help with emotional
regulation. Mobile phone use or texting was mentioned in 34.3% (n=16) of the comments
and all of these comments mentioned it in reference to setting and receiving reminders for
future intentions.
Aim 3 To investigate which factors are associated with use of technological and non-
technological memory aids, and memory aid strategies.
Memory aid technology:
Greater use of technological reminders post-ABI was associated with younger age (IRR =
0.97, CI = 0.956 to 0.987, p < 0.001), higher premorbid technological memory aid use (IRR =
1.23, CI = 1.15 to 1.32, p < 0.001), and higher current use of non-technological memory
aids/strategies (IRR = 1.09, CI = 1.04 to 1.15, p < 0.001). These variables explained 75.8%
(Nagelkerke R2 = 0.758) of variance in technological memory aid use.
All memory aids:
Greater use of all reminders and strategies post-ABI was associated with younger age
(estimate = -0.11, CI = -0.19 to -0.04, p < 0.01), higher use of all memory aids before injury
(estimate = 0.53, CI = 0.34 to 0.71, p < 0.001) and higher PRMQ scores (estimate = 0.2, CI
= 0.097 to 0.304, p < 0.001). These variables explained 38.5% (R2 = 0.385) of the variance in
memory aid use.
8
Discussion
A postal survey was used to examine the types of memory aids currently used by people
with acquired brain injury living in the community. The proportions of different memory aids
used were compared to the proportions reported in a 2003 survey, and the factors which
influence memory aid use were examined.
Memory Aid Use
Ten of the 18 memory aids compared were used by a significantly greater proportion of
people in the current study compared to the participants in Evans et al. (2003). These
included many different types of aids including technological aids such as mobile phones
and alarms/ timers, and non-technological aids and strategies such as asking others to
remind, lists on paper diaries, mental retracing, repetitive practice, objects in noticeable places,
rhymes or phrases and alphabetic searching. It is possible this increase represents a general
increase in memory aid and strategy use for people with ABI. The increase could also be
explained by other differences between the two study samples. The studies were carried out
in Cambridgeshire (2003) and Scotland (current) and so participant overlap is unlikely. The
current study participants were, on average, older by around ten years. It seems unlikely that
this would account for the difference in memory aid use, as both studies found that younger
age predicted use of all types of memory aids. The participants in the current study reported
significantly more years in education than the 2003 participants. Education level was not a
significant predictor of memory aid use in the current study. However, higher education level
could indicate higher socio-economic status (SES) and factors related to higher SES such as
better social/family support may contribute to greater use of memory aids. While Evans and
colleagues (2003) did not test the impact of level of education on memory aid use, they did
investigate pre-morbid intelligence using the National Adult Reading Test – revised (NART;
Nelson and Willison, 1991). They found that the NART was not significantly associated with
memory aid use.
Greater time since injury was found to be related to increased memory aid use in Evans et
al. (2003). The current sample had, on average, just over one year more since their injury,
although this difference was not significant. Differences in recruitment method mean that
severity of injury could be different for the two groups. Eighty-one of the 94 participants in
Evans et al. (2003) had a history consistent with a period of coma and posttraumatic
amnesia (PTA). Mean coma time was 7 days and mean PTA time was longer than 4 weeks.
Therefore many of the participants in the study fell into the PTA category of ‘very severe’.
Methodological limitations prevented such detailed information about participants’ injuries
being collected in the current study, but it is possible that the Evans et al. (2003) study
included participants who had more severe difficulties compared with the current study
sample and this may have impacted on their ability to use memory aids effectively.
People with ABI who were younger, used more memory aids prior to injury and who had
poorer self-rated memory were found to use more of all types of memory aid in the present
study. Age and pre-morbid memory aid use were also found to be influential in Evans et al.
(2003). They did not find objective memory ability (Rivermead Behavioural memory test
RBMT (Wilson, Cockburn & Baddeley, 1999)) to be a significant predictor of memory aid use
in a regression analysis (self-reported memory ability data were not gathered). However,
Wilson and colleagues (1996) did find that RBMT score influenced memory aid use and,
9
using a bi-variate analysis, Evans et al. (2003) found that a RBMT screening score above 3
was related to use of six or more memory aids. Therefore it does seem that previous studies
have found that better objective memory ability is associated with higher use of aids. These
findings contrast the current findings that poorer self-reported memory leads to greater use
of strategies in this group. An explanation for this could be that better objective memory is
related to higher cognitive functioning which may lead to greater insight into memory
difficulties. This could lead to low memory self-evaluation and to increased use of memory
aid strategies. Alternatively somebody with very poor memory might lack insight into their
difficulties and be unaware of their need for memory aids. In the absence of objective
memory data in the present study sample, it is difficult to clarify the relationship between
objective memory ability, self-reported memory ability and memory aid use.
Technological aid use
Comparing the results of this study to those of Evans et al. 2003, use of some technological
memory aids does appear to have increased. Use of mobile phones as memory aids has
increased from around 3% to 38% amongst people with ABI in the last 10 years. Alarm/timer
use has also seen a large increase from 9% to 38%. This could reflect the general trend of
greater memory aid use in the current sample compared to the 2003 sample. It could also be
due to the advancement in and greater availability of mobile phone technology for personal
use. Two of the most commonly used technological memory aids were mobile phones, and
asking someone to text them. Use of other technologies studied in both papers has not
increased and this is likely because pagers, dictaphones and electronic organisers have
become obsolete in the last 10 years and their functions are now performed on
smartphones.
It is difficult to put these results into context through comparison with the general population
as few statistics on the general use of memory aid technologies are available. A comparison
can be made by using smartphone use as a proxy for being familiar and comfortable with
technology. Although statistics vary, it has been reported that around 50% of people between
the ages of 45 and 55 (the average age of the participants in the study) use a smartphone in
countries where smartphone penetration is high such as the UK and USA (Nerea, 2013).
This is higher than 41% of people who, in our survey, used 3 or more pieces of technology
and higher than 38% of people who commonly used mobile phone reminders. These
statistics allow the tentative suggestion that while technology use has increased markedly
over the last decade for people with ABI, this group is behind the general population in terms
of the uptake and use of smart technologies and mobile phone reminding technologies.
The most commonly used memory aids or strategies reported in the survey were leaving
items in noticeable or regular places, developing habits after repetitive practice, making lists
on paper, using wall calendars and asking other people to remind them about things. Diaries
and notebooks were also quite popular. These findings are useful when thinking about how
technology could be designed around people’s existing habits. Many reminding technologies
have been developed from non-technological strategies which people commonly use. For
example calendar and notes applications come as standard on modern smartphones.
Turning these memory aids into memory aid technology is useful because it allows active
prompting from the device at relevant times. However technological versions of some of the
most popular strategies have not become so widespread. For example, a technological
version of the strategy ‘placing items in regular places’ could be a system displaying
10
reminders which is placed in a highly visible regular place in the home. A tablet based
system which performed this function was developed by McGee-Lennon and colleagues
after several co-design sessions with older users (McGee-Lennon, Smeaton & Brewster,
2012). These results offer more evidence that this type of technology may be useful for
people with memory impairment after ABI.
In this study, people with ABI who were younger, used more technological memory aids prior
to their injury and who used more non-technological aids and strategies after their injury
tended to use a higher number of technological memory aids. When investigating which
factors predicted all memory aid use, Evans et al. (2003) found that a ge, time since injury,
previous use of memory aids, level of independence and attentional functioning were the
most important predictors. Therefore there is a similarity between the factors which predicted
all memory aid use in 2003 and the factors which predict technological memory aid use in
2014. It is interesting to note that the most commonly reported use for memory aid
technology was to remind about future intentions, with a small number of references to
waking up and orienting to time and date. There is growing interest in technologies which
can support autobiographical memory (Hodges et al., 2005) and working memory during
performance of tasks with several sub-steps (Mihailidis Carmichael & Boger, 2004). However
the current results suggest that prompting technologies which help organisation and
prospective memory and, to a lesser extent, alerting technologies which support orientation
are the types of assistive technologies currently being used by people with ABI to support
memory.
Implications
This study highlights factors which are associated with memory aid use and which explain
quite a large proportion of the variance in all memory aid use for people with ABI. These
factors are fairly easy to establish within a few minutes in a clinical setting and have potential
to be a good indication of the likelihood that somebody will make use of memory aids or not.
This information is useful when developing individual rehabilitation plans for patients and
when considering the use of technological and non-technological memory aids.
Methodological Considerations
The comparison between this study and Evans et al., 2003 is limited by their differing
methodologies. Variables such as independence, everyday attention and severity of head
injury cannot be compared as they were not possible to ascertain in a postal survey. The
methodology also meant it was not possible to distinguish how much help each participant
received from caregivers to complete the survey.
Although there was a wide range of self-reported memory ability, the PRMQ results show
that most participants reported some level of memory impairment and all participants in this
study self-reported impaired memory and/or had memory functioning as a rehabilitation goal.
However, objective assessment of memory performance was not carried out. The PRMQ
does correlate with global measures of memory in the general population (Rönnlund,
Mäntylä and Nilsson, 2008) and it has been found that prospective memory performance is
predicted by prospective memory complaints in older adults (Zeintl, Kliegel, Rast and
Zimprich (2006). However, people often have difficulty with insight and self-awareness after
ABI (Fleming and Strong, 1995. A number of participants were within one standard deviation
of the mean PRMQ score for the general population and it is difficult to tell whether this
11
reflects a weakness in the recruitment method or a lack of awareness from participants
about their memory difficulties.Acquired brain injury can often lead to memory impairment,
apathy and cognitive, sensory and motor difficulties. It could be claimed that a self-reported
survey administered without researcher supervision might fail to elicit many responses (due
to the difficulty of the task). Additionally, any responses which are obtained may not be
accurate (due to the difficulty of remembering or processing answers, or perseveration in
responses). Various steps were carried out when designing the survey in order to overcome
these potential hurdles. It was made clear on the instructions on the front of the survey that
while the survey was addressed to the person with ABI, it was recommended that a family
member or caregiver help with the completion of the survey. For the memory aid items it was
made clear, both in the description of the task and the individual items, that the participants
should only select the technologies, aids or strategies which they used for reminding. The
aim of this was to prevent participants from selecting items which they use for other
purposes (e.g. a mobile phone to stay in contact with people or a computer to play games).
Other steps such as making the questionnaire as short as possible so that it only took 30
minutes to complete and splitting the questionnaire into two parts with the suggestion that
people take a break between the sections were designed to improve the likelihood of
accurate completion. A draft questionnaire was also altered after consultation with an
acquired brain injury expert at the charity Headway and several changes were made
including the layout of the checklist (making the font larger and easier to read and grouping
each checklist item in its own box to hold people’s attention) and the wording of the
introduction to the different sections (making it as clear as possible and giving examples to
illustrate the points).
The postal survey method of this study may have lead to a selection bias. It is possible that
the 81 people who returned the survey were different from the 227 people who did not
respond. For example, completion might be more common from those who are motivated in
their rehabilitation. This may be particularly true of people who were approached through
Headway because these participants were voluntarily attending rehabilitation in the
community. Successfully responding to a postal survey may also reflect a high level of
functioning, organisation and insight into memory problems. The invitation in the survey for
caregivers to help participants to respond may have tempered selection bias by allowing
carers to scaffold the cognition required for survey completion for participants who may
otherwise have failed to complete and return the survey. Furthermore, although the PRMQ
data are difficult to interpret because of the issues with insight described above, it does
provide some evidence that this sample is representative of people with increased memory
difficulty after mild to moderate ABI.
Future research
Future studies might benefit from asking about extra technologies which were not included in
this survey, for example day/date clocks for orientation or smartwatches as an orientation or
memory support. It might also be interesting to survey caregivers separately to investigate
whether there is a difference between carer and self-report of memory aid use. Mobile
phones were one of the most commonly used memory aid technologies and they have many
potential uses for cognition. While the survey responses indicated that phones (and all
technology) were mostly used to aid prospective memory, future work could investigate in
greater detail how people are using mobile phones to support memory.
12
Rehabilitation
One potentially important predictor of memory aid use which was not investigated in this
study was level of neuropsychological rehabilitation each participant received. Evans et al.,
2003 looked at the influence that acute inpatient and post acute specialist rehabilitation had
on memory aid use. No association was found between memory aid use and rehabilitation
received. It was concluded that rehabilitation was either ineffective in teaching people to use
aids or it was not encouraging the use of aids. While the recruitment method of the present
study guaranteed that all participants had received some rehabilitation or input either
through the NHS or Headway, further details about rehabilitation were not investigated in this
study because of the limitations of the survey design. It was decided that questions about
rehabilitation services would be difficult for people with ABI to accurately report. There were
also concerns that the survey should not be too long as this would lower the response rate.
Future studies could investigate the impact that rehabilitation currently has on use of
technological and non-technological memory aids.
Design
This study found a large increase in use of technological memory aids amongst people with
ABI compared to previous research. However, in the sample as a whole, 23.5% did not
report using any technological memory aid and 59% used two or fewer pieces of technology.
Therefore there is great potential to increase the use of technology amongst people with ABI.
While we accept the possibility that more technological memory aid use may not equate to
better rehabilitation (and that using one or two memory aids effectively and often may be
better for some people), the evidence suggests that use of memory aid technology in
general can be an effective intervention for compensating for memory difficulties (Jamieson
et al., 2013; Gillespie et al., 2012). Designing technology which is appropriate for people with
cognitive impairment is one way in which to improve uptake and effectiveness of memory aid
technology, and future research could investigate how different designs influence people’s
perception and use of technology. The participants in the current study were using more
non-tech aids and strategies than technology. More appropriate design and improved
accessibility of technology may be necessary for it to become as prevalent as pencil and
paper methods. The psychological and practical barriers which impact upon uptake are also
important issues to investigate.
Conclusion
This study has highlighted a substantial increase in use of reminding technology by people
with ABI in the last ten years, showing that alarms, texting and mobile phone reminding are
the most commonly used technologies. It was also clear that people with ABI used more of
all types of memory aids than ten years ago. Technological memory aid use was best
predicted by age, pre-morbid technological memory aid use and amount of non-
technological strategies and aids used. While methodological limitations must be considered,
the results of this study give some important insights into which memory aids and strategies
people with ABI are using and who is making good use of them.
References
13
Brodaty, H., & Donkin, M. (2009). Family caregivers of people with dementia. Dialogues in
clinical neuroscience, 11(2), 217.
Caprani, N., Greaney, J., & Porter, N. (2006). A Review of Memory Aid Devices for an Ageing
Population, 4(3), 205–243.
Chang, Y.-J., Chen, S.-F., & Chuang, A.-F. (2011). A gesture recognition system to transition
autonomously through vocational tasks for individuals with cognitive impairments.
Research in developmental disabilities, 32(6), 2064–8.
Crawford, J., Smith, G., Maylor, E., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. (2003). The Prospective and
Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ): Normative data and latent structure in
a large non-clinical sample. Memory, 11(3), 261-275.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Department of Health. Reducing Brain Damage: faster access to better stroke care’. National
Audit Office, London, 2005.
Evans, J. J., Wilson, B. A, Needham, P., & Brentnall, S. (2003). Who makes good use of
memory aids? Results of a survey of people with acquired brain injury. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 9(6), 925–35.
Gillespie, A., Best, C., & O'Neill, B. (2012). Cognitive function and assistive technology for
cognition: A systematic review. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Society, 18(01), 1-19
Hart, T., O'Neil-Pirozzi, T., & Morita, C. (2003). Clinician expectations for portable electronic devices
as cognitive-behavioural orthoses in traumatic brain injury rehabilitation. Brain injury, 17(5),
401-411.
Hodges, S., Williams, L., Berry, E., Izadi, S., Srinivasan, J., Butler, A., ... & Wood, K. (2006).
SenseCam: A retrospective memory aid. In UbiComp 2006: Ubiquitous
Computing (pp. 177-193). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Evans, J. J., Wilson, B. a, Needham, P., & Brentnall, S. (2003). Who makes good use of
memory aids? Results of a survey of people with acquired brain injury. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 9(6), 925–35.
Finkelstein E, Corso PS, Miller TR. The incidence and economic burden of injuries in the
United States. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2006
Fleming, J., & Strong, J. (1995). Self-awareness of deficits following acquired brain injury:
Considerations for rehabilitation. The British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 58(2),
55-60
Gillespie, A., Best, C., & O'Neill, B. (2012). Cognitive function and assistive technology for
cognition: A systematic review. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Society, 18(01), 1-19
Jamieson, M., Cullen, B., McGee-Lennon, M., Brewster, S., & Evans, J. J. (2013). The
efficacy of cognitive prosthetic technology for people with memory impairments: A
systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychological rehabilitation, (ahead-of-
print), 1-26
McGee-Lennon, M., Smeaton, A., & Brewster, S. (2012,). Designing home care reminder
systems: lessons learned through co-design with older users. InPervasive
14
Computing Technologies for Healthcare (PervasiveHealth), 2012 6th International
Conference on (pp. 49-56). IEEE.
Mihailidis, A., Carmichael, B., & Boger, J. (2004). The use of computer vision in an
intelligent environment to support aging-in-place, safety, and independence in the
home. Information Technology in Biomedicine, IEEE Transactions on, 8(3), 238-247.
Mioshi, E., Dawson, K., Mitchell, J., Arnold, R., & Hodges, J. R. (2006). The Addenbrooke's
Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE‐R): a brief cognitive test battery for dementia
screening. International journal of geriatric psychiatry,21(11), 1078-1085.
Nelson, H. & Willison, J. (1991) The National Adult Reading Test (2nd ed.) Windsor, England;
NFER.
Nerea (2014, October, 25) Top countries with most smartphones users in the world.
Retrieved from http://zeendo.com/info/top-countries-with-most-smartphones-users-in-the-
world/
O’Neill, B., & Gillespie, A. (2014). Assistive Technology for Cognition. Assistive Technology
for Cognition: A Handbook for Clinicians and Developers, 1.
Ownsworth, T. L., & McFarland, K. (1999). Memory remediation in long-term acquired brain injury: two
approaches in diary training. Brain Injury, 13(8), 605-626.
Pollack, M. E. (2005). Intelligent technology for an aging population: The use of AI to assist
elders with cognitive impairment. AI magazine, 26(2), 9.
Rönnlund, M., Mäntylä, T., & NILSSON, L. G. (2008). The Prospective and Retrospective
Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ): Factorial structure, relations to global subjective
memory ratings, and Swedish norms. Scandinavian journal of psychology, 49(1), 11-
18.
Statista, (2015, January, 18) Number of mobile phone users worldwide from 2012 to 2018 (in
billions). Retrieved from http://www.statista.com/statistics/274774/forecast-of-mobile-
phone-users-worldwide/
Styles, K (2013, June 24) 7 in 10 People in the UK Now Own a Smartphone. Retrieved from
http://mobilemarketingmagazine.com/7-10-people-uk-now-own-smartphone/
Svoboda, E., & Richards, B. (2009). Compensating for anterograde amnesia : A new training
method that capitalizes on emerging smartphone technologies, 629–638.
Wilson, B. A. (2009). Neuropsychological rehabilitation: Theory, models, therapy and
outcome. Cambridge University Press.
Wilson, B. A., Emslie, H. C., Quirk, K., & Evans, J. J. (2001). Reducing everyday memory
and planning problems by means of a paging system: a randomised control
crossover study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 70(4), 477-482
Wilson, B. A., Cockburn, J., & Baddeley, A. D. (1999). The Rivermead Behavioural Memory
Test (RBMT): Extended VersionThames Valley Test Co. Bury St Edmunds.
Wilson, B.A. & Watson, P.C. (1996). A practical framework for understanding compensatory
behaviour in people with organic memory impairment. Memory, 4, 465– 486.
Zeintl, M., Kliegel, M., Rast, P., & Zimprich, D. (2006). Prospective memory complaints can
be predicted by prospective memory performance in older adults. Dementia and
geriatric cognitive disorders, 22(3), 209-215.
15
16
Appendix
Non-technological reminders - instructions
Below is a list of memory aids, devices and strategies that are sometimes used for
remembering things such as birthdays, doctor’s appointments, names or everyday tasks such
as shopping.
For each one, please indicate;
1. Tick a box to
indicate if you
used the
memory aid
before your
brain injury.
2. Tick a box to
indicate if you
use the
memory aid
now.
3. Tick one box
to indicate
how often you
use it
(monthly,
weekly or
daily).
4. Tick one box
to indicate
how useful
the aid or
strategy is for
you.
First we want to know about simple pencil and paper or verbal reminders which you use:
items
Asking others to remind you in person
A diary to help you remember things coming up in future (e.g. appointments or things to do)
A diary/journal to help you remember what you have done
Wall calendars
Whiteboard or wall chart
Making a list of things to do on a piece of paper (e.g. a things to do list or a shopping list)
Making notes of what you need to remember in a notebook.
Post-it notes
Technological reminders - instructions
Next, tell us about any technology (e.g. a mobile phone or computer) which you use to
remind yourself about things. For example, do you use technology to help you
remember to go to appointments, to remember social events such as birthdays, or to
help you perform everyday tasks such as shopping, cooking or cleaning?
Please only tick the boxes if you have used or currently use this technology to help
you remember things – many people will use a mobile phone as a phone but only tick
the box if you use it to help you remember things.
Items
Mobile phone to remind you
17
Laptop computer or tablet computer (e.g. iPad) to remind you
Desktop computer to remind you
Television (e.g. automatic prompting about or recording of favourite shows)
Using a camera to take pictures of a holiday or special occasion to help you remember it
afterwards.*
Using a digital camera to take pictures of everyday events to remind you of what you have
done.
A pager to remind you
Electronic personal organiser
Dictaphone/ voice recorder to remind you
Alarm clock to wake up*
Alarm clock/ timer to remind you to do something
An internet based calendar to remind you (such as Google calendar)
Asking someone to send you a text message you to remind you about something
A watch with a date/timer to remind you
If you use any of these technological memory aids, what do you use them to remind you
about?
*These items were not included in analysis as the function of reminding was not prompted. These
items were added to prevent people from reporting that they used camera or alarm to remind them,
when they really only used them to take pictures on holiday or wake up.
Strategies – instructions
Finally, tell us about other tricks, habits or strategies do you use to remind yourself of things
Items
Mental retracing of your steps - to find misplaced items (e.g. ‘where did I last see the keys?’…)
Repetitive practice- repeating tasks until they become a habit
Leaving objects in places you will notice them to remind you to use them or take them with
you.
Leaving objects in the same place so you know where to find them
Rhymes or phrases to remember important information (e.g. ‘remember remember the 5th of
November’)
Changing passwords or PIN numbers to combinations you use regularly
Writing on your hand (or elsewhere)
Alphabetic searching- Considering if a name or object begins with the letter A, B , C.....etc.
Please give details here of any other memory aids or strategies which you use that were not in
the checklist and tell us what you use them to help you remember.
18
19
Table 1: Descriptive statistics for survey responses
Variables Descriptive statistics
(people with ABI, n = 81)
Mean PRMQ score (range, SD)
Overall
Prospective
Retrospective
Short term
Long term
Self-cued
Environmentally cued
52.98 (17 – 78, 15.87)
27.53 (8 - 40, 8.38)
25.44 (8 - 39, 8)
26.49 (8 - 40, 8.2)
26.48 (9 - 40, 8)
28.17 (8 - 40, 8.2)
24.8 (9 - 38, 8.2)
Mean number of all types of memory aids used (range, SD)
BEFORE injury
AFTER injury
6.14 (0 - 18, 4.52)
11.47 (2 – 26, 4.46)
Technological memory aid use prevalence (after injury) n (%)
One or more used
3 or more used
6 or more used
61 (75)
37 (41)
8 (10)
Non-technological memory aid use prevalence (after injury) n
(%)
One or more used
3 or more used
6 or more used
78 (96)
68 (84)
37 (46)
Strategy use prevalence (after injury) n (%)
One or more used
3 or more used
6 or more used
79 (97)
71 (88)
17 (21)
PRMQ = Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire; ABI = acquired brain injury; SD =
standard deviation
20
Table 2 – Prevalence of memory aid use reported in 2003 and 2014. The types of aid or
strategy are grouped in the following order; technological memory aids, non-technological
memory aids and memory strategies.
Memory aid or strategy
Number (%) of whole
sample using the aid
or strategy (Evans et
al., 2003, n = 94)
Number (%) of whole
sample using the aid
or strategy (this
study, n = 81)
Significant on X2
test?
(p value)
Mobile phone
Pager
Electronic personal organiser
Dictaphone
Alarm / timer
Watch with date / timer
3 (3)
5(5)
7 (7)
2(2)
9(10)
17(18)
31(38)
2(2)
4(5)
2(2)
31(38)
12(15)
YES (p < 0.001)
NO
NO
NO
YES (p < 0.001)
NO
Asking someone to remind
you
Diary
Wall calendar
Lists on paper
Notebook
Post-it notes
46(49)
51(54)
68(72)
59(63)
60(64)
32(34)
63(78)
61(77)
55(69)
62(78)
49(62)
32(41)
YES (p < 0.001)
YES (p < 0.01)
NO
YES (p < 0.05)
NO
NO
Mental retracing
Repetitive practice
Objects in noticeable places
Rhymes or phrases
Writing on your hand
Alphabetic searching
45(48)
28(30)
33(35)
2(2)
23(25)
7(7.4)
61(77)
36(46)
69(86)
25(31)
25(31)
28(36)
YES (p < 0.001)
YES (p < 0.05)
YES (p < 0.001)
YES (p < 0.001)
NO
YES (p < 0.001)
Figure 1: Survey respondents’ use of assistive technology, with usefulness evaluation.
21
Figure 2: Survey respondents’ use of strategies and non-technological memory aids, with
usefulness evaluation.
22
... These involve manipulation of the environment or the use of devices to store information that can be accessed at a later time, reducing memory demands (Harris 1980;Intons-Peterson and Fournier 1986;Wilson 2000). Environmental adaptations include labeling doors or items in the house, using arrows for directions, and strategically placing objects in specific locations relevant to when or how they are to be used (Evans et al. 2003;Jamieson et al. 2017;Kapur et al. 2002;Tate 1997). Memory aids include notebooks, calendars, personal digital assistants, and mobile phones that can be used for shopping lists or reminders for appointments or to take medications (Evans et al. 2003;Harris 1992;Kapur et al. 2004;Schacter and Glisky 1986;Velikonja et al. 2014;Wilson 2002). ...
... Advances in technology have allowed for further examination of systems such as NeuroPage, prosthetics, and smart house options to be used as external aids (Boman et al. 2010;Hersh and Treadgold 1994;Jamieson et al. 2017;Vasquez et al. 2021;Wilson et al. 1997). NeuroPage is a portable paging system for reminders and cues that can be personalized to each individual and circumvent the issue of forgetting to use the external memory aids such as notebooks and has demonstrated success with memory-impaired individuals (Jamieson et al. 2017;Wilson et al. 1997). ...
... Advances in technology have allowed for further examination of systems such as NeuroPage, prosthetics, and smart house options to be used as external aids (Boman et al. 2010;Hersh and Treadgold 1994;Jamieson et al. 2017;Vasquez et al. 2021;Wilson et al. 1997). NeuroPage is a portable paging system for reminders and cues that can be personalized to each individual and circumvent the issue of forgetting to use the external memory aids such as notebooks and has demonstrated success with memory-impaired individuals (Jamieson et al. 2017;Wilson et al. 1997). However, the use of pagers in general as assistive technologies has decreased as much of their functions are now included within the capacity of mobile phones (Jamieson et al. 2017;Lannin et al. 2014;Wong et al. 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Memory impairment following an acquired brain injury can negatively impact daily living and quality of life—but can be reduced by memory rehabilitation. Here, we review the literature on four approaches for memory rehabilitation and their associated strategies: (1) the restorative approach, aimed at a return to pre-morbid functioning, (2) the knowledge acquisition approach, involving training on specific information relevant to daily life, (3) the compensatory approach, targeted at improving daily functioning, and (4) the holistic approach, in which social, emotional, and behavioral deficits are addressed alongside cognitive consequences of acquired brain injury. Each memory rehabilitation approach includes specific strategies such as drill and practice (restorative), spaced retrieval (knowledge acquisition), memory aids (compensatory), or a combination of psychotherapy and cognitive strategies (holistic). Past research has demonstrated mixed support for the use of restorative strategies to improve memory function, whereas knowledge acquisition strategies show promising results on trained tasks but little generalization to untrained tasks and activities of daily living. Compensatory strategies remain widely used but require intensive training to be effectively employed. Finally, the holistic approach is becoming more widespread due to improvements in psychosocial wellbeing, yet there are considerable resource and cost requirements. Several factors can influence rehabilitation outcomes including metacognition and emotional disturbances. Considerations for future research to improve the applicability of strategies for memory rehabilitation include assessing memory impairment severity, examining memory needs in daily life, and exploring the long-term effects of memory rehabilitation.
... Despite the arrival of new technologies, the spontaneous strategies have not markedly changed over time. A recent study found that up to 90% of patients would leave objects in noticeable places, mentally retrace steps, or practice activities repeatedly [46]. A similar percentage used external aids such as asking someone to remind them of things to do, using a paper diary for future events, making lists on paper, or using a notebook. ...
... Future methods of brain stimulation (rTMS, tDCS, etc.) may possibly help to improve automatic recording of information [41,43]. Some patients rapidly turn to external aids, with an increasing choice of technical gadgets [46]. For many patients with severe memory impairment, independence in daily activities is the most important goal. ...
Article
Full-text available
Memory disorders are common in clinical practice. This review focuses on the rehabilitation of anterograde amnesia, the inability to learn and retrieve new information, in non-degenerative brain disease. Diverse mnemonic strategies may be helpful in learning specific pieces of information. Their success also depends on the severity of associated cognitive failures, in particular, executive dysfunction. However, unless transfer to everyday activities is specifically trained, such strategies are of limited value in promoting independence in daily life. External memory aids are often necessary to allow for independent living. Learning to use them requires intact capacities such as procedural learning or conditioning. This review further discusses the rehabilitation of confabulation, that is, the emergence of memories of events that never happened. The rehabilitation of memory disorders needs to be tailored to patients’ individual capacities and needs.
... However, goal setting in rehabilitation presents particular challenges for clients with brain injuries. Clients are likely to be experiencing disorientation, confusion, and memory issues [13] Memory impairment is a significant obstruction in return to previous levels of functioning for a person with brain injury [14]. Everyday tasks such as selfcare, managing medications, and cooking become difficult with reduced facility to plan ahead and organise previously routine activities [15]. ...
... In their study of memory aids and strategies among people with acquired brain injury, Jamieson et al. found that all participants included memory function as a rehabilitation goal or stated that they had memory difficulties. The authors confirmed that an important advantage of technological ways of assisting memory was the active prompting ability [14]. Evald [21] also reported that visual and audible prompts or notifications from smartphones to patients with traumatic brain injury were particularly helpful as reminders. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: We examine the use of a custom iPad application, the Rehab Portal, to provide clients in an inpatient brain injury rehabilitation service with access to short videos where clinicians-or the clients themselves-discuss their current rehabilitation goals. Materials and methods: We developed an initial version of the Rehab Portal app based on our previous co-design with service users, their families, and clinicians. This was examined in a field trial with a series of six clients over the course of their stays in inpatient rehabilitation, collecting quantitative data on clinician and client engagement with the Rehab Portal, alongside a thematic analysis of qualitative interviews with clients and clinicians at the point of discharge. Results: Engagement with the platform was high for two clients while it was limited with four more. In our thematic analysis we discuss how introduction of the Rehab Portal disrupted practice, changing how things are done, causing deviation from usual routines, adding burden, and threatening professional integrity. At the same time, where it worked well it led to a repositioning of goal planning away from being clinician directed and towards an ongoing, dynamic collaboration between clinicians, clients and their families. Finally, in some cases we identified a reverting to the status quo, with client demotivation having an unexpected impact on clinician behaviour leading to the process being abandoned. Conclusions: The current findings do not provide wholesale support for this approach, yet we continue to feel that approaches that support clinician-client communication using asynchronous video may offer considerable future value and are worthy of further investigation.
... this change should present new opportunities to overcome the challenges faced by individuals with ABi. For instance, smartphones and tablets devices can already assist those with cognitive impairments in maintaining attention and memory [34][35][36][37]. Additionally, assistive cognitive technologies can restore independence to individuals with ABi, enabling them to carry out daily tasks such as meal preparation and cleaning, ultimately improving their participation in society [38]. ...
Article
People with acquired brain injuries (ABI) face financial challenges that affect their daily lives. Managing finances is a crucial activity that can help avoid social isolation. However, this task becomes difficult for people with ABI because of their cognitive impairments. Recent advances in digital technology can help people with ABI manage their finances more effectively. This study aims to identify and describe available digital tools that can help ABI in budget management, and identify their effectiveness, barriers and facilitators to implementation. To address this issue, we conducted a rapid review of academic databases followed by a modified Google/Google Scholar search to identify the digital tools to support budgeting tasks (DBT) used and tested by people with ABI. Our rapid review included only two articles on the use of DBT. The first study showed that common portable electronic devices were acceptable and desirable as memory and organisational aids for people with ABI. The second study documented the development of a DBT and the perception of users (research participants) who found it appealing and user-friendly. However, for both articles, the technologies used are outdated and lack information on barriers and facilitators to using DBT. In conclusion, this literature review revealed that digital technologies have the potential to support budget management in people with ABI, but technology needs to be made available on the market to benefit the users. Further research and development are needed to create new ways to help people with brain injuries manage their budgets.
... Mobile devices, including mobile phones, smartphones, and tablets, are highly accessible forms of AT and are more widely used than personal computers and older portable electronic devices [43][44][45][46][47]. Smartphones and tablets provide adults with intellectual impairments a chance to access social media platforms, enabling them to connect, communicate, and participate in activities with others, which can help alleviate the negative effects of social exclusion and stigma [48]. ...
Article
Full-text available
People with Intellectual Disability (ID) encounter several problems in their daily living regarding their needs, activities, interrelationships, and communication. In this paper, an interactive platform is proposed, aiming to provide personalized recommendations for information and entertainment, including creative and educational activities, tailored to the special user needs of this population. Furthermore, the proposed platform integrates capabilities for the automatic recognition of health-related emergencies, such as fever, oxygen saturation decline, and tachycardia, as well as location tracking and detection of wandering behavior based on smartwatch/smartphone sensors, while providing appropriate notifications to caregivers and automated assistance to people with ID through voice instructions and interaction with a virtual assistant. A short-scale pilot study has been carried out, where a group of end-users participated in the testing of the integrated platform, verifying its effectiveness concerning the recommended services. The experimental results indicate the potential value of the proposed system in providing routine health measurements, identifying and managing emergency cases, and supporting a creative and qualitative daily life for people with disabilities.
... Regarding the mobile devices, including mobile phones, smartphones, and tablets, are highly accessible forms of AT and are more widely used than personal computers and older portable electronic devices [44][45][46][47][48]. Smartphones and tablets provide adults with intellectual impairments a chance to access social media platforms, enabling them to connect, communicate and participate with others, which can help alleviate the negative effects of social exclusion and stigma [49]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
People with Intellectual Disability (ID) encounter several problems in their daily living regarding their needs, activities, interrelationships, and communication. On this concept, an interactive platform is proposed, aiming to provide personalized recommendations for information and entertainment, including creative and educational activities, tailored to the special users’ needs of this population. Furthermore, the proposed platform integrates capabilities of automatic recognition of health related emergencies, such as fever, oxygen saturation decline and tachycardia, as well as location tracking and detection of wandering behavior based on smartwatch/smartphone sensors, while providing automated assistance and appropriate alerts and notifications to the caregivers.
... 10,12 For individuals with more severe memory impairments, external memory aids and environmental supports (e.g., diaries/notebooks, mobiles/ smartphones, paging systems, whiteboards) are found to be more beneficial than internal memory strategies. 12 In a study investigating self-reported use of memory strategies and predictors of use after brain injury, Jamieson and colleagues 13 found that the most used strategies included: leaving objects in noticeable places (86%), calendars (79%), reminders from significant others (78%), lists (78%), mentally retracing one's steps (77%), and diaries (77%). AT devices such as mobile phones for reminders (38%) and alarms/timers (38%) were less common, although more frequent in younger adults who were familiar with technology prior to their injury. ...
Article
Rapid technology advances have led to diverse assistive technology (AT) options for use in memory rehabilitation after traumatic brain injury (TBI). This systematic review aimed to evaluate the efficacy of electronic AT for supporting phases of memory in daily life after TBI. A secondary aim was to examine user perspectives on the utility of AT and factors influencing uptake or use. A systematic search of PsycINFO, MEDLINE, CINAHL, Embase, Scopus and Cochrane Library was conducted from database inception to June 13, 2022, to identify eligible studies. Methodological quality was assessed by two independent reviewers. 19 eligible articles involving a total of 311 participants included four randomized controlled trials (Class I), five single-case experimental designs (Class II) and 10 pre-post group (n > 10) or single-case studies without experimental control (Class III). Three Class I studies, two Class II studies and eight Class III studies supported the efficacy of AT for supporting memory functioning. Treatment fidelity was not examined in any study. There was the most empirical support for the efficacy of AT for facilitating retrieval and execution phases of memory (i.e., supported by 6/9 studies) with external support to encode memory intentions or pre-programmed reminders. Further controlled studies are needed to determine whether outcomes vary according to individuals' level of independence with use (e.g., self-initiated vs pre-programmed reminders) and to examine user characteristics and design features influencing uptake and effectiveness. Greater involvement of end-users with brain injury in the design and evaluation of AT features is also recommended to enhance usability and uptake in daily life.
... Smartphones have built-in calendars and reminder programs that allow the user to enter their own reminders. But surveys over the last 20 years demonstrate that whilst use of reminding technology has increased, uptake remains relatively low, being around 38% amongst people with acquired brain injury (Jamieson et al., 2017), and less than 10% amongst people with dementia (Jamieson, 2016). There are several barriers to the uptake of reminding technology including the interaction of cognitive impairments and the complexity of user interfaces on today's smartphones (Jamieson et al., 2015). ...
Article
Objectives: To describe the use of mobile devices after acquired brain injury (ABI), from the perspectives of injured individuals and significant others, and to examine factors associated with mobile device use for cognition. Methods: Cross-sectional study with 50 adults with moderate/severe traumatic brain injury or stroke (42% women; mean of 50.7 years old, 4.6 years post-ABI), and 24 significant others. Participants completed questionnaires on mobile technology, cognitive functioning and the impact of technology. Results: Of 45/50 adults with ABI who owned a smartphone/tablet, 31% reported difficulties in using their device post-injury, 44% had received support, and 46% were interested in further training. Significant others reported motor/visual impairments and the fear of becoming dependent on technology as barriers for mobile device use, and 65% mentioned that their injured relative needed additional support. Mobile device use for cognition was common (64%), predicted in a regression model by lower subjective memory and more positive perception of the psychosocial impacts of technology, and also associated in univariate analyses with younger age, lower executive functioning, and greater use of memory strategies. Conclusion: Using mobile devices for cognition is common post-ABI but remains challenging for a significant proportion. Developing training approaches may help supporting technology use.IMPLICATIONS FOR REHABILITATIONUsing mobile electronic devices (smartphones and tablets) is common after acquired brain injury (ABI) but is challenging for a significant proportion of individuals.After the ABI, close to 50% of individuals receive support in using their mobile device, mostly from family members and friends, but rarely from rehabilitation clinicians or technology specialists.In a sample of 50 adults with ABI, more frequent use of mobile devices to support cognition was associated with poorer subjective memory and executive functioning, greater use of memory strategies, more positive perception of the psychosocial impacts of technology, and younger age.
Article
Full-text available
Injuries are one of the most serious public health problems facing the United States today. Through premature death, disability, medical cost, and lost productivity, injuries impact the health and welfare of all Americans. Deaths only begin to tell the story. Although many injuries are minor, a large proportion results in fractures, amputations, burns, or significant injuries that have far-reaching consequences. Now, for the first time in over fifteen years, there are comprehensive estimates of the impact of these injuries in economic terms. This book updates a landmark Report to Congress from 1989. Since that report, no undertaking has addressed the incidence and economic burden of injuries with more timely data, despite major changes in the fields of prevention, reporting and surveillance. Since the mid-eighties, new safety technologies have been developed to prevent injuries or to decrease the severity of injuries, and new policies and laws have been enacted to promote injury prevention. Chapter topics include incidence by detailed categorizations, lifetime medical costs, and productivity losses as a result of injuries, and a discussion of recent trends.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Technology for care at home is an important factor in supporting our ageing population. These technologies need to be both accessible and acceptable to a wide variety of users if they are to be taken up and successfully used in people's homes. This paper describes the user-centered co-design and evaluation of a multimodal reminder system for the home deployed on mobile devices. Six co-design sessions (N=25 users) were carried out with groups of older users to investigate the best methods and techniques for configuring reminders and how they should be delivered within the home. Both sketches and implemented prototypes were used to gather qualitative feedback on a variety of interaction features and techniques to find what worked best for an older user group. We present the findings from the sessions in terms of the re-design of a personalisable multimodal reminder system. We also present the co-design process used and go on to discuss the value this method adds to the design and evaluation of home care technologies for older users.
Article
Full-text available
Technology can compensate for memory impairment. The efficacy of assistive technology for people with memory difficulties and the methodology of selected studies are assessed. A systematic search was performed and all studies that investigated the impact of technology on memory performance for adults with impaired memory resulting from acquired brain injury (ABI) or a degenerative disease were included. Two 10-point scales were used to compare each study to an ideally reported single case experimental design (SCED) study (SCED scale; Tate et al., 2008 ) or randomised control group study (PEDro-P scale; Maher, Sherrington, Herbert, Moseley, & Elkins, 2003 ). Thirty-two SCED (mean = 5.9 on the SCED scale) and 11 group studies (mean = 4.45 on the PEDro-P scale) were found. Baseline and intervention performance for each participant in the SCED studies was re-calculated using non-overlap of all pairs (Parker & Vannest, 2009 ) giving a mean score of 0.85 on a 0 to 1 scale (17 studies, n = 36). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of technology vs. control in seven group studies gave a large effect size (d = 1.27) (n = 147). It was concluded that prosthetic technology can improve performance on everyday tasks requiring memory. There is a specific need for investigations of technology for people with degenerative diseases.
Article
Today, approximately 10 percent of the world's population is over the age of 60; by 2050 this proportion will have more than doubled. Moreover, the greatest rate of increase is amongst the "oldest old," people aged 85 and over. While many older adults remain healthy and productive, overall this segment of the population is subject to physical and cognitive impairment at higher rates than younger people. This article surveys new technologies that incorporate artificial intelligence techniques to support older adults and help them cope with the changes of aging, in particular with cognitive decline.
Article
Objectives: To evaluate a paging system designed to improve independence in people with memory problems and executive deficits. Methods: After a successful pilot study, a randomised control trial was conducted involving a crossover design with 143 people aged between 8 and 83 years. All had one or more of the following: memory, planning, attention, or organisation problems. Most had sustained a traumatic head injury or a stroke although a few had developmental learning difficulties or other conditions. The crossover design ensured that some people received a pager after a 2 week baseline whereas others were required to wait for 7 weeks after the baseline before receiving the pager. Participants were assessed at three time periods-namely, at baseline, 7 weeks, and at 14 weeks postbaseline. Results: More than 80% of those who completed the 16 week trial were significantly more successful in carrying out everyday activities (such as self care, self medication, and keeping appointments) when using the pager in comparison with the baseline period. For most of these, significant improvement was maintained when they were monitored 7 weeks after returning the pager. Conclusions: This particular paging system significantly reduces everyday failures of memory and planning in people with brain injury.
Article
“Stroke services in England have been improving and there are pockets of excellent practice on which to draw, but many patients are still denied fast and effective treatment and rehabilitation services. At £7 billion a year, stroke imposes significant economic costs. By giving stroke the attention and status it deserves, the Department will be able to make financial savings to the NHS and the wider economy. The NHS can help prevent more strokes and improve treatment, care and outcomes by re-organising services and using existing capacity more wisely. Much can be done to achieve real improvements in patients’ prognosis, treatment and rehabilitation and to reduce the toll that stroke takes on individuals and their families.”
Article
Limitations in self-awareness of deficits after acquired brain injury can hamper client participation in rehabilitation programmes. The concept of self-awareness encompasses awareness of brain injury related deficits, an appreciation of the functional consequences of deficits, and the ability to translate that information into realistic goals. A literature review suggests that the development of self-awareness can be associated with emotional distress in the individual. Self-awareness and emotional adjustment factors may both affect behavioural change in the individual, which may influence outcome. The widely hold belief that self-awareness is necessary for successful outcomes from rehabilitation requires further investigation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Book
Injuries are one of the most serious public health problems facing the United States today. Through premature death, disability, medical cost and lost productivity, injuries impact the health and welfare of all Americans. Deaths only begin to tell the story. Although many injuries are minor, a large proportion result in fractures, amputations, burns, or other significant injuries that have far-reaching consequences. Now, for the first time in over 15 years, we have comprehensive estimates of the impact of these injuries in economic terms. This book updates a landmark Report to Congress from 1989. Since the report, no undertaking has addressed the incidence and economic burden of injuries with more timely data, despite major changes in the fields of prevention, reporting, and surveillance. Since the mid-eighties, new safety technologies have been developed to prevent injuries or to decrease the severity of injuries, and new policies and laws have been enacted to promote injury prevention.