ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Objective: This study explored the friendship characteristics of 8 to 12 year old children with and without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Friendship characteristics included number of nominated and corroborated friends, duration of friendships, amount of contact with friends, and the proportion of friends with learning and behavioral problems. Method: The sample comprised 92 children, 50 with a diagnosis of ADHD and 42 comparison children. Result: While children with ADHD did not differ from comparison children in the number of friends they nominated, parents and teachers of children with ADHD were less likely to corroborate that these friendships existed. The friendships of children with ADHD were also shorter in duration. While children with ADHD were indistinguishable from comparison children with regards to the amount of telephone contact with friends, they spent less time with friends outside of school than comparison children. Children with ADHD had a higher proportion of friends with learning and behavior problems. Conclusion: While children with ADHD differ from comparison children in the above friendship characteristics, it is promising that they still fall within the average range for the number of corroborated friendships and they demonstrate adequate stability in their friendships.
Journal of Attention Disorders
XX(X) 1 –10
© 2012 SAGE Publications
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1087054712458971
http://jad.sagepub.com
Articles
Children who have few close reciprocal friendships are more
likely than other children to be poorly adjusted at school, to
be involved with the youth justice system, and to experience
childhood and adult mental health problems (Rose & Asher,
2000). It is therefore concerning that approximately 50% of
children with ADHD have significant difficulties in their
social relationships (see McQuade & Hoza, 2008, for review).
Despite the well-documented social-behavioral and peer
relationship problems of children with ADHD, there are few
studies explicitly examining their friendships (Hoza, Gerdes,
et al., 2005). The overall objective of the present study was to
closely examine the friendship characteristics of children
with and without ADHD. In particular, we were interested in
the number of friends they have, the characteristics of their
friends, the length of their friendships, and the amount of
time they spend with their friends (i.e., companionship).
Friendship is a dyadic relationship based on mutual affec-
tion and reciprocity (Bagwell, Molina, Pelham, & Hoza,
2001). Due to its dyadic nature, friendship can be assessed by
asking children to list their friends and determining how many
of these nominated friends are either reciprocated by the nom-
inated children or corroborated by parent or teacher reports.
Of the large number of benefits that having friends and having
friendships of high quality are believed to serve, six have
received significant theoretical and empirical support (see
Rose & Asher, 2000, for review). That is, friends provide
companionship; feelings of self-validation and ego support;
emotional security; a context for self-disclosure, help, and
guidance; and they are reliable allies. Having a close friend-
ship has been shown to mitigate the consequences of peer
rejection (Parker & Asher, 1993) and buffer against adjust-
ment difficulties (Lauren, Bukowski, & Nurmi, 2007).
During the middle childhood years, children without best
friends report more feelings of loneliness than children with
friends, regardless of how accepted they are by peers (Parker
& Asher, 1993). Even children who were well liked by their
peer group reported feeling more lonely if they did not have
a friend than children who had close friendships (Parker &
Asher, 1993). This finding suggests that friendships provide
emotional supports that are distinct from those provided by
the larger peer group (Asher & Parker, 1989).
Friendship problems have been defined in a number of
ways, including being friendless, having poor quality
458971JADXXX10.1177/108705471245897
1Journal of Attention DisordersMarton et al.
© 2012 SAGE Publications
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
1Toronto District School Board, Ontario, Canada
2University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada
3The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
4Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada
Corresponding Author:
Imola Marton, Department of Psychology, Toronto District School Board,
Level 3, 140 Borough Drive, Toronto, Ontario M1P 4N6, Canada
Email: imola.marton@tdsb.on.ca
Friendship Characteristics
of Children With ADHD
Imola Marton1, Judith Wiener2, Maria Rogers3, and Chris Moore4
Abstract
Objective: This study explored the friendship characteristics of 8 to 12 year old children with and without Attention-
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Friendship characteristics included number of nominated and corroborated
friends, duration of friendships, amount of contact with friends, and the proportion of friends with learning and behavioral
problems. Method: The sample comprised 92 children, 50 with a diagnosis of ADHD and 42 comparison children. Result:
While children with ADHD did not differ from comparison children in the number of friends they nominated, parents
and teachers of children with ADHD were less likely to corroborate that these friendships existed. The friendships of
children with ADHD were also shorter in duration. While children with ADHD were indistinguishable from comparison
children with regards to the amount of telephone contact with friends, they spent less time with friends outside of school
than comparison children. Children with ADHD had a higher proportion of friends with learning and behavior problems.
Conclusion: While children with ADHD differ from comparison children in the above friendship characteristics, it is
promising that they still fall within the average range for the number of corroborated friendships and they demonstrate
adequate stability in their friendships.(J. of Att. Dis. 2012; XX(X) 1-XX)
Keywords
ADHD, friendship, children
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
2 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)
friendships, being friends with children who have behavioral
or deviant characteristics, and having short or unstable friend-
ships (Rose & Asher, 2000). Children with such friendship
problems are at an increased risk for being victimized by bul-
lies (Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, & Bukowski, 1999), engaging
in deviant behaviors (Vitaro, Tremblay, Kerr, Pagani, &
Bukowski, 1997), having difficulties at school (Ladd,
Kochenderfer, & Coleman, 1996), and experiencing feelings
of loneliness (Parker & Seal, 1996).
Because friends support and defend one another, it is not
surprising that studies have reported that friendships serve a
protective function against victimization, even for children
with externalizing problems (Cardoos & Hinshaw, 2011).
Friendships are also believed to be important for school adjust-
ment because friends can provide instrumental (e.g., help with
assignments) and emotional support (e.g., provide encourage-
ment to keep trying when things get difficult), creating more
opportunities for enjoyment at school. Children with friends
look forward to attending school because it provides them with
opportunities to play with friends, talk to them about personal
issues, and work with them on group assignments (Rose &
Asher, 2000). In fact, research shows that children with friends
in their class, especially children with stable friendships, were
more likely than children without friendships to develop posi-
tive perceptions toward school and children with good-quality
friendships were at particular advantage with regard to school
adjustment (Ladd et al., 1996).
The potential influence of friends, however, is not uni-
formly positive; friends who exhibit deviant behaviors can
have negative influences (Berndt, 1999). The similarity-attrac-
tion hypothesis suggests that children with similar attitudes
and attributes are more likely to be attracted to one another,
and thus become friends (Byrne & Nelson, 1965). Friends may
exert bidirectional influences on one another, resulting in
greater similarity over time (Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995).
Thus, aggressive children frequently become friends with
other aggressive children (Cairns, Cairns, Neckerman, Gest,
& Gariepy, 1988) and observational research shows that
these children’s conversations encourage deviant behavior in
one another (Dishion, Spracklen, Andrews, & Patterson, 1996).
Furthermore, longitudinal data suggest that children who had
aggressive friends at the first time of assessment were more
likely to become more aggressive and engage in delinquent
behavior themselves 4 years later than children without aggres-
sive friends (Kupersmidt, Burchinal, & Patterson, 1995).
In a review of the friendship literature pertaining to chil-
dren with ADHD, Normand, Schneider, and Robaey (2007)
indicated that the limited number of studies make it difficult
to draw conclusions about the friendship patterns and char-
acteristics of children with ADHD. Research to date has
shown that girls with ADHD have fewer mutual friends and
are more likely to have no friends in a summer camp context
than girls without ADHD (Blachman & Hinshaw, 2002;
Cardoos & Hinshaw, 2011), and children with ADHD have
fewer reciprocated friendships at school (Hoza, Mrug, et al.,
2005) and fewer friends according to parent report (Bagwell
et al., 2001; Heiman, 2005). Girls with both inattentive and
combined subtypes of ADHD had more difficulty maintain-
ing friendships in the context of a 5-week summer camp
than girls without ADHD (Blachman & Hinshaw, 2002).
Children with ADHD were more likely to meet with friends
at school or on the playground and less likely to meet with
them at home than comparison children (Heiman, 2005). A
recent multimethod study found that children with ADHD
were more likely than comparison children to have friends
with high levels of ADHD and oppositional symptomatol-
ogy, perceived their friendships to have more negative and
fewer positive features, and were generally less satisfied
with their friendships (Normand et al., 2011). Children with
ADHD also exhibited more insensitive and self-centered
proposals than comparison children when negotiating with
their friends and were often more dominant in their friend-
ships (Normand et al., 2011). However, no studies have
reported whether children with and without ADHD are sim-
ilar with respect to the amount of time they spend talking to
their friends on the telephone or interacting with their
friends outside of school.
Objectives and Hypotheses
The overall objective of the present study was to compare
children with and without ADHD with regard to their friend-
ship characteristics. Friendship characteristics considered
included the number of nominated and corroborated friends,
the proportion of corroborated friendships with children who
have learning or behavioral problems, the stability of cor-
roborated friendships (measured as the average length of
friendships), and companionship (measured as the amount of
time spent talking with friends on the telephone and spend-
ing time with friends). In accordance with previous research,
it was hypothesized that although children with ADHD
would likely report that they have a similar number of friends
as children without ADHD, their friendship nominations are
less likely to be corroborated by parents and teachers, sug-
gesting that they have fewer mutual friends. Furthermore,
due to their intrusive style of social interaction, they would
likely have friendships of shorter duration and interact with
their friends less often. The similarity-attraction hypothesis
suggests that children with similar attitudes and attributes are
more likely to be attracted to one another, and thus become
friends (Byrne & Nelson, 1965). Based on this hypothesis
and the high proportion of children with ADHD who have
comorbid learning and behavioral problems (Mayes &
Calhoun, 2000), it is likely that children with ADHD would
have a higher proportion of friends with learning and behav-
ioral problems than children without ADHD.
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Marton et al. 3
Method
Participants
The sample consisted of 92 children (66 boys, 26 girls) 8 to
12 years of age. In all, 50 children had a diagnosis of ADHD
(36 male, 14 female) and 42 were comparison children with-
out any behavior problems (30 male, 12 female). Children
who scored below a standard score of 80 on a standardized
measure of intelligence were excluded from analysis.
Children with ADHD were referred from two local hospital
clinics and from the community through posters at chil-
dren’s mental health centers. Children without ADHD were
recruited from local schools and through newspaper adver-
tisements and posters in community.
For inclusion in the ADHD sample, participants were
required to have an existing diagnosis of ADHD according
to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
(4th ed.; DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994)
criteria from a physician or a mental health professional.
Parents and teachers were also asked to complete the
Conners’ Parent/Teacher Rating Scale–Revised: Long Form
(CPRS/CTRS-R:L; Conners, 1997) to ensure that symp-
toms continued to be present. ADHD symptomatology was
confirmed if the participant was rated within the clinical
range (T-score 70) by one informant and within the bor-
derline (T-score 65) or clinical range by the second infor-
mant on the DSM-IV-oriented ADHD subscales of the
CPRS and CTRS. For children taking psychostimulant
medication, parents and teachers were asked to think of the
child off medication when completing the questionnaires.
For inclusion in the comparison sample, children were
required to have no diagnosis of ADHD or other behavioral
disorder. Parents and teachers were asked to complete the
CPRS/CTRS to ensure that children did not display a signifi-
cant number of ADHD symptoms. Children in the compari-
son group who received a T-score below 60 on the CPRS
and CTRS were included in the sample. The original sample
consisted of 110 children. Three participants with a previous
ADHD diagnosis were dropped due to low IQ scores, one
because an autism spectrum diagnosis was suspected, and
one was unable to complete most of the assessment due to
behavioral difficulties. In the comparison sample, 8 children
were dropped because they received T-scores above 60 on at
least one of the ADHD DSM-IV subscales of the CTRS-R:L.
In the ADHD sample, 5 participants were dropped because
they did not have T-scores above 70 on any of the relevant
scales on the CPRS or CTRS. With respect to missing data,
teachers for 3 participants did not return data.
Of the remaining sample, 24 participants with ADHD
(48%) were taking psychostimulant medication at the time
of data collection and 27 had at least one comorbid diagno-
sis (54%) from a mental health professional (11 learning
disabilities, 11 anxiety disorder, 10 oppositional defiant
disorder, 4 mood disorder). Chi-square tests were conducted
to determine whether there were any demographic differ-
ences between children with and without ADHD. These
analyses revealed no significant differences in gender, χ2(1,
N = 92) = 0.004, p = .95; languages spoken in the home
(English or other), χ2(1, N = 92) = 1.640, p = .20; or parent
marital status, χ2(1, N = 92) = 3.608, p = .06. A series of
independent samples t tests showed no ADHD status differ-
ences in age, ADHD: M = 10.08, SD = 1.39; comparison: M
= 10.20, SD = 1.46, t(90) = 0.40, p = .69, but children with
ADHD scored lower on standardized measures of intelli-
gence (M = 103.64, SD = 12.81) than comparison children
(M = 112.00, SD = 12.49), t(90) = 3.15, p < .01. Children
with ADHD also attained higher scores on the total DSM-IV
ADHD scales of the parent (M = 78.12, SD = 7.84) and
teacher (M = 70.78, SD = 11.58) forms of the Conners
Rating Scales than comparison children, M = 46.26, SD =
5.05; M = 48.90, SD = 5.80, t(90) = 22.66, p < .001 and
t(89) = 11.09, p < .001, respectively. Parents in the compari-
son sample generally attained higher levels of education (M
= 9.02, SD = 1.15; on average having completed an under-
graduate university degree) than parents of children with
ADHD (M = 7.76, SD = 1.79; completed a college program
or some university), t(85) = 4.07, p < .001.
Measures
The CPRS-R:L and CTRS-R:L are well-established measures
that assess problem behaviors. Respondents rate children’s
behavior on a 4-point scale ranging from “not true at all” to
“very much true.” The three DSM-IV ADHD subscales (i.e.,
DSM-IV: Inattentive, DSM-IV: Hyperactive-Impulsive, and
DSM-IV: Total) were used in this study. The Conners’ ques-
tionnaires are frequently used in research and have strong
psychometric properties (Conners, 1997).
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fourth Edition
(WISC-IV) and Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI).
Clinic-referred children with ADHD (44% of the sample)
were administered the WISC-IV (Wechsler, 2003) as part of
a complete psychological assessment. Community-referred
children with ADHD (56% of the sample) and all of the com-
parison children were administered the WASI (Wechsler,
1999), a screening measure of overall intelligence in children
(Sattler, 2001). The WISC-IV and the WASI are commonly
used measures of intelligence with good reliability, construct
validity and test–retest stability (Sattler, 2001; Wechsler,
2003). The full-scale scores for the WASI and the WISC-IV
were used in analyses.
The friendship interview and questionnaires were adapted
from Berndt (1984) to assess the number of children’s
friends, as well as the length and the amount of contact in
corroborated friendships. In an individual interview, chil-
dren were asked to nominate all of their “best friends” at
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
4 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)
school and outside of school by first name and last initial
and to indicate how long they have been friends, how often
they speak with them on the telephone, and how frequently
they spend time with each friend. Parents and teachers were
asked parallel questions, in addition to questions about
whether the friend has learning or behavioral problems. The
frequency of telephone contact between friends was assessed
on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 = almost every
day to 6 = never and the frequency of contact outside of
school was assessed on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging
from 1 = once a week to 5 never. Individual children listed
on the friendship interview are referred to as nominated
friends; any of these individuals who were also listed on
either the parent or teacher friendship questionnaire are
referred to as corroborated friends. Using similar method-
ology, Wiener and Schneider (2002) established that parent
and teacher corroboration of friendship serves as a valid
measure of mutual relationships when access to reciprocal
nominations from the nominated friends is restricted. Thus,
in the present study, the number of nominated and corrobo-
rated friends is reported, with larger numbers indicating
more friends. Parents and teachers were also asked to indi-
cate whether each friend they nominated had a learning or
behavioral problem. The number of corroborated friends
with learning or behavior problems was calculated by exam-
ining which of the corroborated friends were rated as having
learning and behavioral problems by either parent or teacher
reports. The number of corroborated friends with learning or
behavior problems was divided by each child’s number of
corroborated friends to derive the proportion of corroborated
friends with learning or behavior problems. The average
length of corroborated friendships was obtained by sum-
ming the length of friendships across all corroborated friends
listed and dividing by the number of friends listed. To take
age effects into account, this average length of corroborated
friendships score was then divided by the age of the child
(in years) to obtain a score reflecting the proportion of chil-
dren’s age on average that they have maintained corrobo-
rated friendships. The amount of telephone and outside of
school contact among corroborated friends was calculated
by reverse coding the friendship questionnaire (so that
higher scores indicate more contact) and adding the amount
of contact between all corroborated friends separately on
the parent- and child forms.
Procedure
This research was conducted at three sites in Toronto,
Canada: the University of Toronto, the Hospital for Sick
Children, and the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health.
During an initial screening, parents provided demographic
and medical information about their child and family and
completed the CPRS. Parents whose children met screen-
ing criteria were scheduled for an assessment. All children
were tested individually by one of two senior doctoral
level students trained in clinical child psychology.
Assessments at the hospitals were completed over two
approximately 5-hr testing sessions and assessments com-
pleted at the University of Toronto were conducted in one
5-hr session. Children with ADHD were medication free
on the day that they completed the research measures.
While children were being tested, parents completed
questionnaires in the waiting room. Once consent was
obtained, teachers were sent the CTRS and the friendship
questionnaire to complete. Families received a written
educational and social-emotional report outlining their
child’s functioning and children received a small toy of
their choice for participating.
Data Analyses
Preliminary analyses indicated that parent education level
was not correlated with any aspect of friendship examined.
Although a group difference was detected on IQ, it was not
covaried because it reflects an attribute commonly associ-
ated with ADHD (Kuntsi et al., 2004). Miller and Chapman
(2001) argued that controlling for preexisting group differ-
ences in a nonrandom research design violates the core
assumption of analysis of covariance that covariates are
statistically independent from the grouping variable.
Five independent samples t tests were conducted to
examine group differences in the number of nominated and
corroborated friends, and the proportion of friends with
leaning or behavior problems. Due to the use of multiple t
tests, the Bonferroni correction was used to control for Type
I error with the resulting alpha being p = .01. To control for
multicollinearity, two separate one-way multivariate analy-
ses were conducted to examine group differences in child-
and parent-reported duration of corroborated friendships,
and the amount of telephone and direct contact. Because
gender differences have been detected in many aspects of
friendship (e.g., Ladd, 2005), post hoc analyses were con-
ducted to examine gender main effects and gender by group
interaction effects. Finally, post hoc analyses were also con-
ducted to explore whether medication status among chil-
dren in the ADHD sample had an impact on friendship
outcomes.
Results
As shown in Table 1, there was no significant difference in
the number of friends children with and without ADHD
nominated. However, fewer friends were corroborated by
teacher and parent reports for children with ADHD than
comparison children. Children with ADHD had a higher
proportion of friends with either a learning or behavior
problem as reported by either their parents or teachers
(43%) than comparison children (16%).
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Marton et al. 5
The first MANOVA investigated child-reported length
of corroborated friendships (proportional to age) and the
amount of telephone and direct contact with corroborated
friends as dependent variables, and ADHD status as the
fixed factor. A significant multivariate effect was detected
for ADHD status, Wilks’s Λ = .88, F(3, 85) = 3.61, p =
.017, η2 = .113. The second MANOVA investigated par-
ent-reported length of corroborated friendships (propor-
tional to age) and the amount of telephone and direct
contact with corroborated friends as dependent variables,
and ADHD status as the fixed factor. A significant multi-
variate effect was detected for ADHD status, Wilks’s Λ =
.88, F(3, 81) = 3.00, p = .017, η2 = .118. Follow-up uni-
variate ANOVAs (Table 2) indicated that children with
ADHD had friendships of shorter duration according to
child and parent reports. Although children with and with-
out ADHD were indistinguishable in terms of amount of
telephone contact they had with friends, both parents and
the children themselves reported that children with ADHD
spent less time with their friends outside of school than
comparison children.
Post Hoc Analyses Involving Gender
and Medication Status Among
the ADHD Sample
Four separate univariate analyses of variance were con-
ducted with the number of nominated and corroborated
friends, and the proportion of friends with learning and
behavioral problems as dependent variables and group and
gender as fixed factors. In addition, two separate MANOVA’s
for child- and parent-reported length of friendship and
amount of telephone and direct contact with friends exam-
ined main effects for gender and group by gender interaction
effects. A gender effect, F(1, 85) = 4.27, p = .042, η2 = .048,
and an ADHD status by gender interaction effect, F(1, 85) =
4.11, p = .046, η2 = .046, were found for only one variable:
proportion of corroborated friends with behavior problems.
The difference between children with and without ADHD in
proportion of friends with behavior problems was greater
for boys (ADHD: M = 35.00, SD = 34.49; comparison: M =
10.23, SD = 15.93) than girls (ADHD: M = 10.07, SD =
15.34; comparison: M = 10.00, SD = 28.92).
Table 1. Nominated and Corroborated Friendships and Proportion of Friends With Learning and Behavioral Problems Among
Children With and Without ADHD
ADHD Comparison
M (SD)M (SD)t(df)p d
Number of child nominated friends 6.32 (3.60) 6.57 (3.51) 0.33 (90) .737 0.07
Number of nominated friends corroborated by parent or teacher 2.98 (1.64) 3.98 (1.86) 2.71 (90) .008 0.57
Proportion of corroborated friends with learning problems 27.79 (25.89) 9.48 (18.02) 3.90 (82)a.000 0.82
Proportion of corroborated friends with behavior problems 27.57 (32.05) 10.17 (20.10) 3.10 (78)a.003 0.65
Proportion of friends with either learning or behavior problems 42.53 (33.02) 15.74 (24.78) 4.35 (84)a.000 0.92
aThe Levene’s test for equality of variances was significant, therefore unequal variances assumed are reported.
Table 2. Child- and Parent-Reported Length of Corroborated Friendships and Amount of Telephone and Direct Contact With
Corroborated Friends in Children With and Without ADHD
ADHD Comparison
M (SD)M (SD)F p η2
Child friendship interview
Average length of corroborated friendshipsa0.28 (0.16) 0.36 (0.16) 5.66 .020 .061
Amount of telephone contactb11.68 (6.92) 13.90 (8.38) 1.87 .174 .021
Amount of direct contactb10.89 (5.91) 14.55 (7.71) 6.36 .013 .068
Parent friendship questionnaire
Average length of corroborated friendshipsa0.31 (0.19) 0.43 (0.17) 8.47 .005 .093
Amount of telephone contactb9.66 (5.74) 12.00 (5.39) 3.73 .057 .043
Amount of direct contactb10.11 (5.56) 12.78 (6.47) 4.16 .044 .048
aThe average length of corroborated friendships was divided by the child’s age to obtain a score reflecting the proportion of children’s age on average
that they have maintained friendships.
bThe amount of telephone and direct contact is a sum of all contact among children’s corroborated friends, with larger scores indicating more contact.
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
6 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)
Independent-samples t tests were conducted to examine
whether medication status among children with ADHD was
related to friendships outcomes. A difference was found for
only one variable; children with ADHD taking stimulant
medication (M = 36.68, SD = 27.20) were found to have a
higher proportion of friends with learning problems than
children with ADHD not on medication (M = 19.96, SD =
22.39), t(40) = 2.28, p = .028.
Discussion
The present study compared children with and without
ADHD with regard to the number of nominated and cor-
roborated friends, the proportion of friends with learning
and behavior problems, friendship stability, and compan-
ionship. Consistent with previous findings (e.g., Heiman,
2005), there was no difference between children with and
without ADHD in the number of friends they nominated.
However, fewer of the friends nominated by children with
ADHD were corroborated by either their parents or teach-
ers than those of comparison children. On average, children
in the ADHD sample had approximately three corroborated
friends, whereas comparison children had four corrobo-
rated friends. Previous research suggests that school-age
children have between three and five best friends on aver-
age (Hallinan, 1980), suggesting that children with ADHD
in the current sample are not atypical with respect to the
number of friends. In the present study, none of the com-
parison children were friendless, whereas three children
(6%) in the ADHD sample had no corroborated friends. This
finding is different from that reported by Hoza, Mrug, et al.
(2005), which indicated that 56% of children with ADHD
and 32% of comparison children had no dyadic friends.
Unlike Hoza, Mrug, et al., who restricted children’s friend-
ship nominations to their top two choices of children at
school, in accordance with the recommendation of Schneider,
Wiener, and Murphy (1994), we allowed unlimited friend-
ship nominations and collected information regarding
friends in and outside of school. The strength of this
approach is that it did not give children any preconceived
notions about the number of friends they should have
(Furman, 1996), and it captured friends in two important
domains of children’s lives. Using this methodology, we
were able to demonstrate that although children with ADHD
have fewer corroborated friends, their average number of
friends is typical, and only a small proportion of them are
actually friendless. This finding is similar to results reported
by Wiener and Schneider (2002), who found that children
with learning disabilities (a disorder frequently comorbid
with ADHD), had fewer mutual friends, and more of their
friends did not attend their school than friends of compari-
son children.
To our know le dge, thi s is the fi rs t s tu dy t o r ep ort the ave r-
age length of naturally occurring corroborated friendships
among children with ADHD. Our results indicate that the
corroborated friendships of children with ADHD are 9.6 to
14.4 months shorter (according to child and parent reports,
respectively) in duration than those of comparison children.
Blachman and Hinshaw (2002) also reported that girls with
both inattentive and combined subtypes of ADHD exhibited
poorer friendship stability during a 5-week summer camp
than comparison girls, and aggressive children have also
been reported to have difficulty maintaining friendships
(Ellis & Zarbatany, 2007). Although this shorter duration is
concerning because an additional year of friendships can
contribute to more warmth, closeness, companionship, and
better school adjustment among friends (Furman, 1996),
from a clinical perspective, it is promising that children
with ADHD maintain friendships for approximately 3 years.
A meta-analysis that assessed social contact (including
out-of-school contact and amount of talking between friends)
concluded that reciprocal friends spend more time with one
another and engage in more conversation than acquaintances
and nonfriends (Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995). Children with
ADHD in the present study reported spending as much time
as comparison children talking on the telephone with their
friends, but less time with friends outside of school.
Although, to our knowledge, no studies to date have exam-
ined the amount of time children with ADHD spend with
their friends, our findings are similar to those of Heiman
(2005), who reported that only 6% of children with ADHD
met with their friends at home, compared with 30% among
comparison children. There are a few possible reasons that
might explain group differences in the amount of direct con-
tact. First, it is possible that due to their often challenging
behaviors, children with ADHD require more supervision
and are thus less likely to be invited to friends’ houses. In
addition, parents of children with ADHD may be more con-
cerned about their children’s behaviors when they cannot
monitor them, so may be less likely to arrange social activi-
ties with friends. It is also possible that peers are less likely
to seek contact with these children because of their behav-
ioral excesses and less advanced social perspective taking
skills (Marton, Wiener, Rogers, Moore, & Tannock, 2009).
Lower levels of direct contact between friends among chil-
dren with ADHD is concerning because proximity between
friends is an important component of companionship for
children and because some researchers argue that the single
most important function of friends across development is
the fulfillment of needs for enjoyable companionship
(Savin-Williams & Berndt, 1990).
Although friendship problems such as not having a
friend, unstable friendships and friendships of short dura-
tion, and minimal contact between friends contribute to
poorer social-emotional functioning (e.g., Rose & Asher,
2000), the effects of friendships may not be uniformly posi-
tive. Berndt (1999) suggested that children become more
similar to the friends with whom they associate, and the
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Marton et al. 7
nature of this influence may either be positive or negative.
For example, longitudinal studies demonstrated that adoles-
cents became similar to their friends in classroom disrup-
tion and academic achievement, even after controlling for
children’s initial levels of disruptiveness and academic
achievement (Berndt & Hanna, 1995). The finding that chil-
dren with ADHD in the present study had a much higher
proportion of friends with either learning or behavioral
problems than comparison children may be explained by a
number of factors. First, 24% of children with ADHD in the
current sample had a comorbid diagnosis of a learning dis-
ability and 20% had oppositional defiant disorder. It is pos-
sible, as indicated by the similarity-attraction hypothesis
(Byrne & Nelson, 1965), that children with ADHD are
attracted to other children who exhibit learning, attention,
and activity levels similar to them. As such, our findings
add to existing studies reporting that children and adoles-
cents generally select friends who resemble themselves
(e.g., Aboud & Mendelson, 1996). On the other hand, given
that propinquity is important in friendship formation
(Epstein, 1989), and that children with ADHD are more
likely than non-ADHD children to be placed in special edu-
cation classes, it is also likely that proximity with other chil-
dren with behavioral and learning difficulties aids the
formation of such friendships. Because children with learn-
ing disabilities have fewer mutual, less stable, and lower
quality friendships (e.g., Wiener & Schneider, 2002), hav-
ing friends with learning difficulties may contribute, at least
in part, to the peer status and friendship choices available to
children with ADHD. In addition, it is concerning that chil-
dren with ADHD have a larger proportion of friends with
behavior problems given research regarding the iatrogenic
effects of associating with friends with behavior problems
(e.g., Berndt & Hanna, 1995; Poulin, Dishon, & Burraston,
2001). The present study also found that boys, but not girls,
with ADHD were more likely to have friends regarded as
having behavioral problems by either parents or teachers.
Given that externalizing problems are more commonly
reported among boys than girls (Abikoff et al., 2002) and
that children tend to develop friendships with children simi-
lar to themselves (e.g., Aboud & Mendelson, 1996), it is
possible that boys with ADHD have more opportunities to
develop friendships with other boys with behavioral prob-
lems. Our exploratory analysis regarding medication status
revealed that it was only related to one of the friendship
outcomes examined in the present study. Children with
ADHD taking medication were more likely to have friends
with learning problems than children with ADHD not tak-
ing medications. Although data were not collected regard-
ing this, it is possible that children with ADHD taking
medications (possibly because they have more severe
ADHD symptoms) may be more likely to be in special edu-
cation classes and develop friendships with other students
who also have learning difficulties.
Limitations and Implications
for Future Research
This study has some limitations. First, we relied on parent
and teacher reports of learning and behavioral problems of
corroborated friends. This limitation is difficult to over-
come, because gathering diagnostic assessments of the
learning and behavioral profiles of corroborated friends
would be time-consuming and complicated by issues of
consent. Second, one of the strengths of the present study,
that we obtained information about corroborated friends in
and outside of school, also came with a limitation. Although
children’s nominated friends were corroborated by either
parents or teachers, it is unknown how many of the friends
the children nominated would have reciprocated their
nominations. Although technically difficult, future studies
should examine the reciprocated friendships of children
with ADHD in and outside of school.
Implications for Clinical Practice
Given the reported friendship difficulties among children
with ADHD and the potential adverse outcomes of friend-
ship problems, clinicians working with this population
should routinely assess these children’s friendship charac-
teristics and implement interventions that focus on devel-
oping close dyadic friendships. Although psychostimulant
medication might help create better opportunities for chil-
dren with ADHD to develop friendships by reducing their
impulsive and challenging behaviors, the establishment of
mutual friendships requires more complex skills than sim-
ply the absence of aversive behaviors (Normand et al.,
2007). Recent research indicates that friendship interven-
tions that focus on the development of positive relation-
ships with one peer result in positive outcomes for children
with ADHD (Hoza, Mrug, Pelham, Greiner, & Gnagy,
2003). These peer-focused interventions are more likely to
be successful and are simpler to implement than interven-
tions aimed at improving peer status among a group.
Social skills training programs and training parents as
“friendship coaches” to their children may be important in
teaching children with ADHD who have friendship prob-
lems social skills such as sharing, reciprocal help, support,
and consideration of the perspectives of others (Mrug, Hoza,
Pelham, Gnagy, & Greiner, 2007). A recent study in which
parents of children with ADHD were trained to be “friend-
ship coaches” to their children showed improvements in
these children’s friendships (Mikami, Lerner, Griggs,
McGarth, & Calhoun, 2010). Because previous research
shows that the ability to take the perspective of another peer
is related to the establishment of friendships (Selman, 2003),
and children with ADHD have been shown to exhibit
weaker social perspective taking skills (Marton et al., 2009),
it is possible that these children might benefit from social
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
8 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)
skills interventions that also explicitly teach social perspec-
tive taking. Selman and Schultz (1990) developed an inno-
vative pair therapy that fosters making and keeping friends
by enhancing social perspective taking and has been shown
to engender more consideration of the perspectives of close
peers and result in higher stages of friendship understand-
ing (Selman, 1980). In sum, because research shows that
even a few close friendships buffer against feelings of isola-
tion and adjustment difficulties (Lauren et al., 2007), focus-
ing on interventions that help children with ADHD develop
close dyadic friendships appears to be most critical.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: We
received funding for the study from Social Sciences and Humanities
Research Council of Canada (SSHRC).
References
Abikoff, H. B., Jensen, P. S., Arnold, L. L. E., Hoza, B.,
Hechtman, L., & Pollack, S. (2002). Observing classroom behav-
ior in children with ADHD: Relationship to gender and comor-
bidity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 349-359.
Aboud, F. E., & Mendelson, M. J. (1996). Determinants of friend-
ship selection and quality: Developmental perspectives. In
W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The
company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence
(pp. 87-112). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statis-
tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
Author.
Asher, S. R., & Parker, J. G. (1989). The significance of peer
relationship problems in childhood. In B. H. Schneider, G.
Attili, J. Nadel, & R. P. Weissberg (Eds.), Social competence
in developmental perspective (pp. 5-23). Dordrecht, Nether-
lands: Kluwer Academic.
Bagwell, C. L., Molina, B. S., Pelham, W. E., & Hoza, B. (2001).
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and problems in peer
relations: Predictions from childhood to adolescence. Journal
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
40, 1285-1292.
Berndt, T. J. (1984). Sociometric, social-cognitive, and behav-
ioral measures for the study of friendship and popularity. In
T. Field, J. L. Roopnarine, & M. Segal (Eds.), Friendships in
normal and handicapped children (pp. 31-52). Norwood, NJ:
Ablex.
Berndt, T. J. (1999). Friends’ influence on student’s adjustment to
school. Educational Psychologist, 34, 15-28.
Berndt, T. J., & Hanna, N. A. (1995). Intimacy and self-disclosure
in friendships. In K. J. Rotenberg (Ed.), Disclosure processes
in children and adolescence (pp. 57-77). New York, NY: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Blachman, D. R., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2002). Patterns of friendships
among girls with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 625-640.
Byrne, D., & Nelson, D. (1965). Attraction as a linear function of
proportion of positive reinforcements. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1, 659-663.
Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., Neckerman, H. J., Gest, S. D., &
Gariepy, J. L. (1988). Social networks and aggressive behav-
ior: Peer support or peer rejection? Developmental Psychol-
ogy, 24, 815-823.
Cardoos, S. L., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2011). Friendship as protec-
tion from peer victimization for girls with and without ADHD.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 1035-1045.
doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9517-3
Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners’ Ratings Scales—Revised: Tech-
nical manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.
Dishion, T. J., Spracklen, K. M., Andrews, D. W., & Patterson, G. R.
(1996). Deviancy training in male adolescent friendships.
Behavior Therapy, 27, 373-390.
Ellis, W. E., & Zarbatany, L. (2007). Explaining friendship forma-
tion and friendship stability: The role of children’s and friends’
aggression and victimization. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53,
79-104.
Epstein, J. L. (1989). The selection of friends: Changes across the
grades and in different school environments. In T. J. Berndt
& G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Peer relationships in child development
(pp. 158-187). Oxford, UK: John Wiley.
Furman, W. (1996). The measurement of friendship perceptions:
Conceptual and methodological issues. In W. M. Bukowski,
A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they
keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 41-65).
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Hallinan, M. T. (1980). Patterns of cliquing among youth. In
H. C. Foot, A. J. Chapman, & J. R. Smith (Eds.), Friendship and
peer relations in children (pp. 321-342). New York, NY: Wiley.
Heiman, T. (2005). An examination of peer relationships of chil-
dren with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
School Psychology International, 26, 330-339.
Hodges, E. V., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1999).
The power of friendship: Protection against an escalating
cycle of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35,
737-748.
Hoza, B., Gerdes, A. C., Mrug, S., Hinshaw, S. P., Bukowski, W.
M., Gold, J. A., . . . Wigal, T. (2005). Peer-assessed outcomes
in the multimodal treatment study of children with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 34, 74-86.
Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Bukowski, W. M.,
Gold, J. A., . . . Arnold, L. E. (2005). What aspects of peer
relationships are impaired in children with attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 73, 411-423.
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Marton et al. 9
Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Pelham, W. E., Greiner, A. R., & Gnagy, E. M.
(2003). A friendship intervention for children with attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Preliminary findings. Journal
of Attention Disorders, 6, 87-98.
Kuntsi, J., Eley, T. C., Taylor, A., Hughes, C., Asherson, P., Caspi, A.,
& Moffitt, T. E. (2004). Co-occurrence of ADHD and low IQ
has genetic origins. American Journal of Medical Genetics,
Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 124, 41-47.
Kupersmidt, J. B., Burchinal, M., & Patterson, C. J. (1995). Devel-
opmental patterns of childhood peer relations as predictors of
externalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopa-
thology, 7, 825-843.
Ladd, G. W. (2005). Children’s peer relations and social compe-
tence: A century of progress. London, England: Yale Univer-
sity Press.
Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. (1996). Friend-
ship quality as a predictor of young children’s early school
adjustment. Child Development, 67, 1103-1118.
Lauren, B., Bukowski, W. M., & Nurmi, J. E. (2007). Friendship
moderates prospective associations between social isolation
and adjustment problems in young children. Child Develop-
ment, 78, 1395-1404.
Marton, I., Wiener, J., Rogers, M., Moore, C., & Tannock, R.
(2009). Empathy and social perspective taking in children with
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology, 37, 107-118.
Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2000). Prevalence and degree
of attention and learning problems in ADHD and LD. ADHD
Report, 8, 14-16.
McQuade, J. D., & Hoza, B. (2008). Peer problems in attention defi-
cit hyperactivity disorder: Current status and future directions.
Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 14, 320-324.
Mikami, A., Lerner, M., Griggs, M., McGarth, A., & Calhoun, C.
(2010). Parental influence on children with attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder: II. Results of a pilot intervention train-
ing parents as friendship coaches for children. Journal of
Abnormal Children Psychology, 38, 737-749.
Miller, G. A., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding of analy-
sis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 40-48.
Mrug, S., Hoza, B., Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., & Greiner, A. R.
(2007). Behavior and peer status in children with ADHD: Con-
tinuity and change. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10, 359-371.
Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Children’s friendship
relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 117,
306-347.
Normand, S., Schneider, B., Lee, M., Maisonneuve, M., Kuehn, S.,
& Robaey, P. (2011). How do children with ADHD (mis)man-
age their real-life dyadic friendships? A multi-method investi-
gation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 293-305.
Normand, S., Schneider, B., & Robaey, P. (2007). Attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder and the challenger of close friendships.
Journal of Canadian Academy of Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 67-73.
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship
quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance
and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Develop-
mental Psychology, 29, 611-621.
Parker, J. G., & Seal, J. (1996). Forming, losing, renewing, and
replacing friendships: Applying temporal parameters to the
assessment of children’s friendship experiences. Child Devel-
opment, 67, 2248-2268.
Poulin, F., Dishon, T. J., & Burraston, B. (2001). 3-year iatrogenic
effects associated with aggregating high-risk adolescents in
cognitive-behavioral preventive interventions. Applied Devel-
opmental Science, 5, 214-224.
Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (2000). Children’s friendships. In
C. Hendrick, & S. S. Hendrick (Eds.), Close relationships: A
sourcebook (pp. 47-57). Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.
Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applica-
tions (4th ed.). La Mesa, CA: Jerome M. Sattler Publisher.
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Berndt, T. J. (1990). Friendship and peer
relations. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the thresh-
old: The developing adolescent (pp. 177-207). Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Schneider, B. H., Wiener, J., & Murphy, K. (1994). Children’s
friendships: A giant step beyond peer acceptance. Journal of
Social and Personal Relations, 11, 323-340.
Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding.
New York, NY: Academic Press.
Selman, R. L. (2003). Social awareness: The growth of interper-
sonal understanding. In R. L. Selman (Ed.), The promotion
of social awareness: Powerful lessons from the partnership
of developmental theory and classroom practice (pp. 13-25).
New York, NY: Russell Sage.
Selman, R. L., & Schultz, L. H. (1990). Making a friend in youth:
Developmental theory and pair therapy. Chicago, IL: The
University of Chicago Press.
Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., Kerr, M., Pagani, L., & Bukowski, W. M.
(1997). Disruptiveness, friends’ characteristics, and delin-
quency in early adolescence: A test of two competing models
of development. Child Development, 68, 676-689.
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence.
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (2003). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale
for Children–Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX:
Psychological Corporation.
Wiener, J., & Schneider, B. H. (2002). A multimodal exploration of
the friendship patterns of children with and without learning dis-
abilities. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 127-141.
Bios
Imola Marton is currently a psychologist at the Toronto District
School Board, in Ontario, Canada. Previously, she has worked as a
psychologist at various local hospitals. She is registered as a school
and clinical psychologist with the College of Psychologists of
Ontario. Her areas of research interest involve the social-cognitive
skills and friendships of children with ADHD. She is also inter-
ested in parent characteristics that result in better adjustment for
children with ADHD. .
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
10 Journal of Attention Disorders XX(X)
Judith Wiener is a professor in the School and Clinical Child
Psychology Program in the Department of Human Development
and Applied Psychology at OISE, University of Toronto. Her
areas of research involve examining the peer relationships and
social adjustment of children and adolescents with learning dis-
abilities and ADHD, parenting of children and adolescents with
ADHD, social and affective impacts of inclusion, and the beliefs
of children and adolescents with learning disabilities and ADHD
about their difficulties.
Maria Rogers is a postdoctoral research fellow in the
Neurosciences and Mental Health Program at the Hospital for
Sick Children in Toronto, Canada. Her research interests involve
the school and family functioning of children with attentional dif-
ficulties. She is also a registered clinical psychologist with the
College of Psychologists of Ontario and has worked in various
school and hospital settings with children with ADHD. She is cur-
rently a visiting scholar in the Department of Educational and
Counselling Psychology at McGill University in Montreal, Canada.
Chris Moore is a professor of Psychology at Dalhousie University.
His research interests include the development of social understand-
ing in infants and preschool children. He has published on joint
attention, theory of mind, mental term use, future-oriented thinking,
and prosocial behavior. He is the author of The Development of
Commonsense Psychology (2006).
at UNIV OF OTTAWA LIBRARY on November 26, 2013jad.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... Children's difficulty with inter-personal empathy [39] could explain poorer quality interactions with friends, as cooperative play, games with rules, and successfully supporting another's needs in play require perspective-taking. As children with ADHD have difficulty anticipating the impact of their behaviour on their friend's emotional state and adjusting their actions accordingly, they may have fewer and shorter reciprocal friendships and develop friendships with children who have similar difficulties [35,39,40]. Children and youth with ADHD have reported impaired social informational processing which impacts their ability to respond to social situations appropriately [38]. ...
... Twelve studies reported children with ADHD had fewer reciprocated friendships compared to TD peers [24, 27, 30-33, 40, 63, 67, 71-73]. Furthermore, every study with a sample of children with ADHD in which reciprocal nominations or parent/teacher corroboration were used in conjunction with the nominations of children with ADHD showed that they have fewer friends [32,40,67]. In the two studies without a comparison group, children and youth with ADHD were reported to have none or a small number of friends [55,75]. ...
... and agency (p < .01) Marton, Wiener [40] • No significant between-group difference in no. of nominated friends for children (3 friends ADHD; 4 friends TD). Significantly less friends corroborated by parents and/ or teacher of children with ADHD (p = .008) ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) experience substantial difficulty maintaining meaningful friendships, which has implications for social functioning and mental health. No systematic review has investigated their friendship difficulties. Objectives: To systematically review and methodologically appraise the quality of existing studies reporting on friendships of children with ADHD. To compare their friendships to typically-developing children, and examine associations between friendship and children's social-emotional wellbeing and mental health. Method: Six databases were searched. The methodological quality of studies was assessed using the QualSyst appraisal tool and the Appraisal tool for Cross-Sectional Studies. Aspects of friendships measured were charted, along with comparisons between children with ADHD and typically-developing children and the associations between friendships and social-emotional wellbeing and mental health. Results: Twenty-three cross-sectional studies and one longitudinal follow-up study were included. Studies included 1509 participants with ADHD, with 1197 typically-developing participants used as a companion in 19 of the 24 studies. Friendship quantity was the most investigated aspect of friendship. Children and youth with ADHD had significantly fewer friends, lower quality friendships and poorer friendship interactions. There were mixed findings from studies investigating the role or impact of friendship on social-emotional wellbeing and mental health. Twenty-two had strong methodological quality. Conclusion: Limited longitudinal studies, small sample sizes and variability in measurement restrict the interpretations of friendship over time and the causal impact of friendship on social and emotional outcomes. Further research should investigate the role and impact of friendships on the social-emotional wellbeing of children and youth with ADHD.
... One possible explanation is that, despite the higher number of reciprocated friendships, these friendships may be of low quality. Indeed, extensive literature demonstrates that children with ADHD tend to have friendships with poor quality (Hoza, 2007;Lee et al., 2021;Marton, Wiener, Rogers, & Moore, 2015;Normand et al., 2021). Specifically, youths with ADHD experience more deterioration of friendship quality over time, including more conflict, less satisfaction (Normand et al., 2013), less social support (Rokeach & Wiener, 2020), and are prone to forming friendships with children who display more behavioral problems (Marton et al., 2015). ...
... Indeed, extensive literature demonstrates that children with ADHD tend to have friendships with poor quality (Hoza, 2007;Lee et al., 2021;Marton, Wiener, Rogers, & Moore, 2015;Normand et al., 2021). Specifically, youths with ADHD experience more deterioration of friendship quality over time, including more conflict, less satisfaction (Normand et al., 2013), less social support (Rokeach & Wiener, 2020), and are prone to forming friendships with children who display more behavioral problems (Marton et al., 2015). Following a social skill training, a minority of children with ADHDinattentive type exhibited worse social behaviors, suggesting that they may have imitated the negative behaviors displayed by children with ADHDcombined type (Antshel & Remer, 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have been consistently found to experience impairments in peer functioning. Irritability is highly prevalent in children with ADHD and may worsen social impairments given the frequent temper outbursts and low frustration tolerance characterizing irritability. However, it is still unclear how ADHD and irritability symptoms interact with peer functioning difficulties over time. Assessing these temporal dynamics using a novel longitudinal approach (i.e., temporal network analysis) may reveal precise targets for intervention. Methods This study investigates the dynamic associations between ADHD symptoms, irritability, and peer functioning in a community sample of 739 children (ages 8–11 years, Mage = 10.06 [SD = 0.59], 47.77% females) assessed at three timepoints, 6 months apart, in a school‐based study. Parents reported their child's ADHD symptoms using the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham Rating Scale (SNAP‐IV), and irritability symptoms using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) irritability items. Children's peer functioning (i.e., peer acceptance, peer rejection, number of friendships, and victimization) was measured via peer nomination. To estimate the longitudinal associations between the variables, we built a graphical vector autoregression model for panel data. Results The longitudinal network highlighted that poor peer functioning contributed to increases in symptoms over time. Specifically, (1) physical victimization predicted increases in inattention, hyperactivity, and irritability; (2) peer rejection predicted increases in inattention, which in turn predicted increases in irritability; (3) peer acceptance predicted decreases in inattention and irritability; and (4) higher numbers of mutual friendships increased inattention. Conclusions These results suggest that a negative social environment involving physical bullying and rejection may aggravate ADHD and irritability symptoms. Conversely, positive social interactions, such as being liked by peers, may improve inattention and irritability symptoms. Fostering social–emotional skills and positive social interactions and environments in children with ADHD and irritability may be a promising target for future interventions to reduce symptoms.
... Par leur comportement, ces enfants sont davantage confrontés à des situations de rejet par les pairs (Gardner & Gerdes, 2015) et ont tendance à être perçus négativement dès les premiers moments d'interactions sociales (Verret et al., 2016). Les enfants avec TDAH ont des relations amicales plus courtes et possèdent en moyenne trois amis contre quatre par rapport à ceux sans TDAH (Marton et al., 2015). Généralement, ils privilégient comme amis des enfants ayant des problèmes d'apprentissage scolaire et de troubles du comportement, comparé aux enfants sans TDAH (Marton et al., 2015;Normand et al., 2011). ...
... Les enfants avec TDAH ont des relations amicales plus courtes et possèdent en moyenne trois amis contre quatre par rapport à ceux sans TDAH (Marton et al., 2015). Généralement, ils privilégient comme amis des enfants ayant des problèmes d'apprentissage scolaire et de troubles du comportement, comparé aux enfants sans TDAH (Marton et al., 2015;Normand et al., 2011). ...
Thesis
Le Trouble Déficit d’Attention/Hyperactivité (TDAH) est un trouble neurodéveloppemental s’accompagnant fréquemment d’une dysrégulation émotionnelle venant complexifier le tableau clinique de l’enfant.Ce travail de thèse a pour objectif d’évaluer l’efficacité d’une intervention multimodale de Thérapies Cognitivo-Comportementales (TCC) par rapport à une intervention médiation par le théâtre (MPT) sur la gestion émotionnelle chez l’enfant avec TDAH et dysrégulation émotionnelle.Un premier article consiste en une revue systématique d’études évaluant les effets d’interventions psychologiques chez l’enfant avec TDAH et dysrégulation émotionnelle. Les études montrent des résultats prometteurs avec une réduction des comportements agressifs et des symptômes émotionnels, néanmoins leur faible qualité méthodologique ne permet pas d’en tirer une conclusion générale. Deux articles sont dédiés à l’étude sur l’efficacité d’une intervention multimodale de TCC par rapport à une de MPT sur les comportements agressifs chez l’enfant avec TDAH et dysrégulation émotionnelle. Les résultats montrent que l’intervention de TCC tend à améliorer les comportements agressifs entre l’évaluation initiale et à six mois post-intervention par rapport au groupe MPT sans que cette différence soit significative. Les interventions multimodales de TCC ciblées sur la gestion émotionnelle représentent des traitements thérapeutiques prometteurs et supposent d’autres recherches cliniques. Le dernier article aborde l’intérêt clinique de mesurer les traits et les troubles de la personnalité chez l’enfant et l’adolescent avec TDAH et dysrégulation émotionnelle. Evaluer leur profil de personnalité s’avère pertinent afin de proposer précocement des interventions et prévenir des difficultés futures de fonctionnement.
... The frequency of ADHD did not differ between individuals with higher or lower abilities to form friendships, despite the existing evidence that the presence of both ADHD and ASD may contribute to social difficulties (Humphreys et al., 2016;Marton et al., 2015;Zachor & Ben-Itzchak, 2018). Similarly, in this study, the severity of anxiety symptoms did not differ across friendship ability levels. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Follow-up studies of children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in early childhood that focus on friendship formation during adolescence are scarce. The present study focused on exploring characteristics possibly related to the ability to establish friendships during adolescence among children diagnosed with ASD in toddlerhood. Methods The cohort included 43 participants who underwent comprehensive assessments during toddlerhood and adolescence. Participants were divided into two groups [Friendship(+)/Friendship(-)] based on (1) adolescent social insight as assessed by professionals and (2) parental and adolescent self-reports regarding having or not having friends. No differences in IQ, ASD symptoms, or adaptive behavior during early childhood were found between the two groups. Results Different and better changes in social communication, adaptive socialization, and daily living skills were observed for the Friendship(+) group. Adolescents with ASD in the Friendship(+) group exhibited greater social independence. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder incidence, anxiety symptom severity, and placement in mainstream or special education classes did not differ between the two groups. Conclusion This long-term study highlights that for children with ASD, longitudinal growth in social communication and adaptive functioning is possible, highly important for and related to the development of the complex ability to establish friendship.
... Despite poor social functioning constituting a cardinal feature of youth with ADHD, especially with co-occurring disruptive behavior problems, aggressive behavior predicted worsening adolescent peer companionship specifically among non-ADHD youth. Youth with ADHD are more likely to befriend high-risk peers by virtue of ADHD, behavior problems, or other socially marginalized youth (Marton et al., 2015;Mikami, 2010). Dyadic relationships embedded in multiple behavior problems may alter perceptions around normative social interactions and peer relationship factors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Internalizing and externalizing problems predict poor academic and social outcomes. However, ADHD co-occurs with internalizing and externalizing problems and is itself a risk factor, thus preventing precise inferences. This study evaluated childhood anxiety, depression, and aggression as predictors of change in adolescent academic and social outcomes, including moderation by childhood ADHD. Methods: 182 ethnically-diverse 5- to 11-year-old youth with (54.7%) and without (45.3%) ADHD completed a separate baseline and 6- to 7-year prospective follow-up assessment, consisting of parallel measures across youth psychopathology, academic functioning (i.e., academic achievement, school competence), and friendship quality domains. Results: Whereas childhood ADHD inversely predicted academic competence, depression and aggression uniquely predicted worsening friendship quality. Interestingly, anxiety was unrelated to change in academic and friendship outcomes; similarly, neither ADHD, depression, nor aggression predicted change in objectively-measured academic achievement from a standardized assessment. Conclusion: Implications for intervention and prevention are considered, including school-based approaches, within a developmental psychopathology framework.
... Both studies revealed that participants were more willing to engage and had more positive attitudes when they knew someone with ADHD. These findings are significant as they suggest that familiarity might help mediate negative attitudes and BI, leading to improved peer acceptance and friendships (Marton et al., 2012;McDougall et al., 2004). However, unlike Law et al. (2007), both studies examined the peer attitudes and BI of high school and college students, not primary school children. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined the attitudes and behavioral intentions of 336 children aged 7–11 toward a hypothetical peer with symptoms of attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) to determine whether they were influenced by labeling, social devaluation, or familiarity. Children read one of five vignettes describing the behavior of a gender‐neutral peer before completing self‐report measures of attitudes and behavioral intentions. Results showed children held predominantly negative attitudes toward the hypothetical peer, which were more pronounced for inattentive than hyperactive/impulsive symptoms. These findings suggest that children were more likely to devalue internalizing rather than externalizing behaviors. Children also reported being more likely to engage in active/recreational and social activities rather than academic activities. In addition, the diagnostic label “ADHD” led to more negative attitudes and behavioral intentions, while knowing someone with ADHD mediated the negative effect of hyperactive/impulsive symptoms on attitudes and behavioral intentions. Finally, significant positive relationships were found between attitudes and children's willingness to engage in social, academic, and physical activities. Policy and practice implications of these findings are discussed.
... Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a highly prevalent psychiatric condition characterized by inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity [1]. ADHD is consistently associated with a host of social problems, including difficulties to manage peer relations and close friendships [2][3][4]. This is especially problematic during adolescence, the developmental period between childhood and adulthood, when belonging to the peer group becomes highly salient [5]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is consistently associated with a host of social problems, such as victimization and difficulties in maintaining close friendships. These problems are not limited to offline relations but also manifest in the online social world, as previous research shows that ADHD is associated with problematic use of social media. Given the ubiquitous nature of social media, the goal of the current review is to understand why adolescents with ADHD demonstrate more problematic social media use than their typically developing peers. To this end, we provide a narrative review on the evidence for the link between ADHD and social media use, and consequently present an integrative framework, which encompasses neurobiological mechanisms (i.e., imbalance theory of brain development and dual pathway model of ADHD) and social mechanisms, including influences from peers and parents. We conclude that empirical work shows most consistent evidence for the link between problematic social media use and ADHD (symptoms), while intensity of social media use is also associated with several other behaviors and outcomes. Finally, we hypothesize how existing interventions for ADHD may work on the identified mechanisms and provide at-hand clinical recommendations for therapists working with adolescents with ADHD who exhibit problematic social media use.
Chapter
Children, adolescents, and adults with ADHD have significant challenges in developing and maintaining social relationships. Children with ADHD are more likely to be rejected by their peers than children without ADHD. Children and adolescents with ADHD also have fewer close friends and less stable and lower-quality friendships than their typically developing peers. They tend to have friends who are two or more years younger or older than them or who have behavioral problems. Adolescents and adults with ADHD have less stable romantic relationships than their peers without ADHD. In addition, their friendships and romantic relationships have fewer positive features, such as mutual support and intimacy, and more negative features, such as conflict. Bullying in school, online, and within intimate partner relationships is a significant problem for individuals with ADHD. In addition to their ADHD symptoms, factors such as externalizing and internalizing disorders, inadequately developed social skills, problems with social perspective taking, and emotion dysregulation contribute to or result from their social relationship challenges. Positive parent and teacher behaviors and relationships are protective factors. Most studies on interventions for children, adolescents, and adults with ADHD do not measure social relationships as an outcome. There is some evidence, however, that psychostimulant medication, social skills training, and mindfulness cognitive behavior therapy programs that involve parents, teachers, or romantic partners in treatment are promising interventions. Implications for future research on social relationships of individuals with ADHD are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Love as a construct may be defined in countless ways and our perception of it in the early years of development stays with us for the rest of our lives. With adolescents suffering from disorders such as ADHD and Cerebral Palsy, there are certain impairments in different parts of the cortex, which have several direct and indirect connections to the hypothalamus, the part of the brain majorly involved in the perception of love. The review paper aims to understand how these impairments contribute towards behavioral changes and the understanding of love in children up to the ages of 18 and the impact it has on their lives in friendship, as well as in romantic settings. While it is assumed that there may be a detrimental effect on account of these impairments, it is also plausible that there may be an increased sensitivity and responsiveness to such emotional stimuli among these children. The importance of understanding the same, provides the world to be well aware of how to assist such children, from a young age to have healthy interpersonal relationships and grow up into adults with a well-functioning social life.
Article
Background: Many interventions have been developed to address the social difficulties commonly experienced by adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), yet they are largely ineffective. Objective: This study examined social impairment among adolescents with and without ADHD, determining whether gender, social anxiety, age, and ADHD symptom type (inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity) and severity are associated with social impairment. Method: Parents and primary caregivers of adolescents (aged 13-17) with (n = 76) and without ADHD (n = 36) completed the Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD and Normal Behavior, Social Responsiveness Scale 2nd Edition, and Spence Children's Anxiety Scale. Results: Adolescents with ADHD scored significantly higher than TD adolescents across social impairment domains. ADHD symptoms were associated with severity of impairment in all domains excluding Social Motivation. Hyperactivity/impulsivity and social anxiety predicted social impairment, whereas gender did not. Conclusion: Adolescents with ADHD are more likely to experience social impairment than TD adolescents, and interventions targeting symptom reduction and social anxiety may improve these social impairments.
Chapter
To be known, and to know others, is critical to all social relationships. This topic of 'disclosure processes' not only pertains to people's disclosure of daily thoughts and emotions, but to their disclosure of many controversial problems in contemporary society, such as divorce, AIDS and sexual abuse. The bulk of research has focused on disclosure processes in adults and relatively little attention has been given to that phenomena in children and adolescents. The collection of chapters in this book redresses the balance by systematically examining disclosure processes in children and adolescents. This covers the knowledge of how, to whom, and the conditions under which children and adolescents reveal their personal thoughts and emotions.
Article
This book examines the role of peer relationships in child and adolescent development by tracking research findings from the early 1900s to the present. Dividing the research into three generations, the book describes what has been learned about children's peer relations and how children's participation in peer relationships contributes to their health, adjustment, and achievement. Gary W. Ladd reviews and interprets the investigative focus and findings of distinct research eras to highlight theoretical or empirical breakthroughs in the study of children's peer relations and social competence over the last century. He also discusses how this information is relevant to understanding and promoting children's health and development. In a final chapter, the author appraises the major discoveries that have emerged during the three research generations and analyzes recent scientific agendas and discoveries in the peer relations discipline.
Article
Education specialists have written volumes on the best ways to help children learn to read and write, but who is helping them navigate the potentially treacherous waters of social interactions? While in school to study, children are also preoccupied with understanding the rules governing social relationships. Issues of trust and loyalty, rivalry and conflict, belonging and exclusion affect all school-aged children, but very few lesson plans include social development skills. The Promotion of Social Awareness summarizes thirty years of research on the social development of children in elementary and middle school, and shows how this work has led to a series of programs that promote the social competence of children and adolescents. Rich with lessons drawn from real life, the book includes an in-depth account of the author's partnership with an innovative program designed to help educators promote a sound ethic of social relationships among children, a case study of a teacher particularly gifted at promoting such relationships, and the tale of how the author's theoretical framework fared cross-culturally when exported to Iceland. The Promotion of Social Awareness documents Robert Selman's efforts both as a practitioner trying to help young people develop their interpersonal skills and as a researcher attempting to understand the factors that promote or hinder social development. Selman believes that getting along with others involves concrete and measurable social skills and actions that can be taught. The book underlines how the science of social development has given rise to initiatives and programs that can be used in educational settings to help children get along with each other, and may in the long run help prevent violence, drug abuse, and prejudice. Unique in its marriage of theory and practice, The Promotion of Social Awareness will appeal to a wide readership, including developmental psychologists, educators, and parents.
Article
Education specialists have written volumes on the best ways to help children learn to read and write, but who is helping them navigate the potentially treacherous waters of social interactions? While in school to study, children are also preoccupied with understanding the rules governing social relationships. Issues of trust and loyalty, rivalry and conflict, belonging and exclusion affect all school-aged children, but very few lesson plans include social development skills. The Promotion of Social Awareness summarizes thirty years of research on the social development of children in elementary and middle school, and shows how this work has led to a series of programs that promote the social competence of children and adolescents. Rich with lessons drawn from real life, the book includes an in-depth account of the author's partnership with an innovative program designed to help educators promote a sound ethic of social relationships among children, a case study of a teacher particularly gifted at promoting such relationships, and the tale of how the author's theoretical framework fared cross-culturally when exported to Iceland. The Promotion of Social Awareness documents Robert Selman's efforts both as a practitioner trying to help young people develop their interpersonal skills and as a researcher attempting to understand the factors that promote or hinder social development. Selman believes that getting along with others involves concrete and measurable social skills and actions that can be taught. The book underlines how the science of social development has given rise to initiatives and programs that can be used in educational settings to help children get along with each other, and may in the long run help prevent violence, drug abuse, and prejudice. Unique in its marriage of theory and practice, The Promotion of Social Awareness will appeal to a wide readership, including developmental psychologists, educators, and parents.
Chapter
In his 1917 sixteen-volume series Practical Child Training, educator R. C. Beery offered advice to mothers whose children have few friends and are reticent to approach others. Beery urged mothers to facilitate get-togethers with peers, such as backyard picnics that include the child’s schoolmates, and to help the children “have a royal good time” (p. 1325). Beery also suggested what to do when a child is fearful about approaching other children: “If your child ever comes to you to bury his head in your skirts, you should not scold or make any scene, but simply appear to pay no attention to him” (p. 741).
Article
The distinction between friendship adjustment and acceptance by the peer group was examined. Third- through 5th-grade children (N = 88 1 ) completed sociometric measures of acceptance and friendship, a measure of loneliness, a questionnaire on the features of their very best friendships, and a measure of their friendship satisfaction. Results indicated that many low-accepted children had best friends and were satisfied with these friendships. However, these children's friendships were lower than those of other children on most dimensions of quality. Having a friend, friendship quality, and group acceptance made separate contributions to the prediction of loneliness. Results indicate the utility of the new friendship quality measure and the value of distinguishing children's friendship adjustment from their general peer acceptance.