ArticlePDF Available

Sleep patterns in college students

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Objective: Since gender effect is inconsistent and grade effect has not been addressed in previous studies, we investigated both effects on the daily sleep patterns in a group of young college students. Methods: The sample consisted of 237 students aged 18–24 years. Each subject completed a 7-day sleep log. Results: Gender differences were found in several sleep variables and those were mostly not dependent on weekday/weekend difference. The female students went to bed and rose earlier and had longer sleep latency, more awakenings, and poorer sleep quality than the male. Gender differences were also shown in the relationship between sleep quality and other sleep variables. The correlation between sleep quality and rise time, time in bed, and sleep efficiency was stronger in men than in women. In contrast, grade differences were mostly dependent on weekday/weekend difference. The freshmen rose earlier and had shorter sleep time than did the other students on weekdays only. Sleep latency was the longest in seniors on weekdays only. Conclusion: This study showed that gender differences in sleep patterns and sleep difficulties were remarkable in the group of young college students. Alarmed by the high prevalence of sleep difficulties among general college students, it is recommended that the students should be informed of their sleep problems and the consequences.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Sleep patterns in college students
Gender and grade differences
Ling-Ling Tsai*Sheng-Ping Li
Department of Psychology, National Chung-Cheng University, 160 San-Hsing, Ming-Hsiung, Chia-Yi 621, Taiwan, ROC
Received 16 September 2002; accepted 3 July 2003
Abstract
Objective: Since gender effect is inconsistent and grade effect
has not been addressed in previous studies, we investigated both
effects on the daily sleep patterns in a group of young college
students. Methods: The sample consisted of 237 students aged
1824 years. Each subject completed a 7-day sleep log. Results:
Gender differences were found in several sleep variables and
those were mostly not dependent on weekday/weekend differ-
ence. The female students went to bed and rose earlier and had
longer sleep latency, more awakenings, and poorer sleep quality
than the male. Gender differences were also shown in the
relationship between sleep quality and other sleep variables. The
correlation between sleep quality and rise time, time in bed,
and sleep efficiency was stronger in men than in women. In
contrast, grade differences were mostly dependent on weekday/
weekend difference. The freshmen rose earlier and had shorter
sleep time than did the other students on weekdays only. Sleep
latency was the longest in seniors on weekdays only. Conclusion:
This study showed that gender differences in sleep patterns and
sleep difficulties were remarkable in the group of young college
students. Alarmed by the high prevalence of sleep difficul-
ties among general college students, it is recommended that
the students should be informed of their sleep problems and
the consequences.
D2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Gender differences; Sleep disturbances; Undergraduates; University students
Introduction
Previous studies in general adult population have shown
that sleep patterns are both quantitatively and qualitatively
related to the age and gender [14]. The prevalence of
insomnia is found to be more common among old people
and in women [2,511]. A recent study showed that the
prevalence of global dissatisfaction with sleep increased
with age and was higher in women [12]. Significant age
differences are also found in both habitual sleep patterns and
sleep disturbances in adolescents. Among both girls and
boys, bedtime [3,13 15] is delayed, total sleep time is
decreased [3,13 17], and the prevalence of sleep problems
increases with age [16]. Compared to older adolescents,
young adults go to bed even later and sleep even less
[3,13,18]. Disrupted sleep, daytime sleepiness, and dissat-
isfaction with sleep are more common in young adults than
in adolescents [19].
In contrast to age difference, few gender differences in
sleep patterns and sleep disturbances have been reported in
adolescents [3,14 17]. Although girls go to bed later,
wake up earlier, and thus sleep less than boys, they do
not have sleep problems more frequently than boys [16].
Compared to adolescents, gender differences in young
adults seem to be more frequently reported. Women aged
between 18 and 24 years go earlier to bed and wake up
earlier than men [3] but have equivalent amounts of total
sleep time, number of awakenings, and daytime naps [3,9].
With a wider age range, a study found that women (age 17
to 30 years) were more likely to have nightmares, delayed
sleep onset, and frequent night awakenings [20]. Another
study using Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) showed
that women aged 2029 years have poorer sleep quality
than men [1]. Reyner and Horne [4], using sleep logs and
actimetry, found no significant gender differences in sleep
latency, wake up time, total sleep time, or sleep quality in
the 2034 years group, but they found that women had
0022-3999/04/$ see front matter D2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00507-5
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +886-5-2720411x32201; fax: +886-5-
2720857.
E-mail address: psyllt@ccu.edu.tw (L.-L. Tsai).
Journal of Psychosomatic Research 56 (2004) 231 – 237
more nocturnal awakenings than men. The major cause of
awakenings in these young women is children. A home-
based electrophysiological sleep recording study in the
group of 2034 years also failed to find significant gender
differences in total sleep time, sleep latency, wake after
sleep onset, and sleep efficiency. However, it did show that
young men had less slow wave sleep than young women
[21]. One epidemiological study found that the prevalence
of insomnia was higher but that of insufficient sleep was
lower in young women aged 20 39 years than in young
men [2]. Taken together, although gender differences in
sleep patterns and sleep disturbances are global and
substantial in general adult population, they are inconsis-
tent and, if any, relatively small effect size in young adults.
Furthermore, some sleep parameters, which are more
related to sleep disturbances, e.g., sleep latency, number
of nocturnal awakenings, and sleep quality, show gender
differences starting only in the age of 20s. Some biological
maturational processes may contribute to gender differ-
ences in the sleep patterns and cause sleep disturbances in
young adults. However, factors other than biological ones
such as role in family, response to stress, and life style
(eating, drinking, smoking, exercise, etc.) can also be
involved in gender differences. For example, it has been
reported that child caring and disturbances due to the bed
partner are the two major causes of nocturnal awakenings
in young women [4].
Many college students are single and at their late
adolescent and early adult ages. Their sleep patterns and
sleep disturbances could be different from those of non-
students at their ages [13]. Compared to nonstudent adults
in their 20s, college students have later bedtime and rise
time and show higher incidence of daytime sleepiness in
addition to physical and mental health complaints. Al-
though the student group has the lowest prevalence of
global sleep dissatisfaction of all occupations [12], 71% of
college students express dissatisfaction with their sleep
[22]. Previous studies in college students do not show
consistent gender differences in sleep patterns. One study
showed that gender differences were not associated with
time in bed, number of awakenings, or sleep quality [23].
Another study found no gender difference in sleep latency
[24]. In contrast, contradictory findings in gender differ-
ences have been reported. Both shorter [25] and longer
[26] sleep duration and both poorer [27] and better [26]
sleep quality have been reported in men than in women.
For sleep disturbances, both significantly lower [24] and
higher [27] incidence have been reported in men than in
women. These discrepancies cannot be simply attributed to
cultural differences of student samples since some studies
in the same country also showed inconsistent results, e.g.,
Refs. [2325]. Age, however, could be one of the con-
founding factors since gender difference effect sizes in
most sleep parameters are small and even smaller in
younger ages. Most studies included a wide age range of
college students, e.g., Refs. [2326].
In this study, we intended to determine gender effect on
daily sleep patterns in college students aged 18 24 years.
If gender differences occur in such a narrow age range, it
would be concluded that gender did play a significant role
in sleep patterns in early adulthood. Another purpose of
this study is to investigate whether grade (educational
level), related to biological as well as academic age, is
involved in sleep patterns in college students.
Methods
Subjects
The sample consisted of 237 college students enrolled
in the course of ‘‘Sleep Management’’ offered in the
spring semester in 1998 and open to all the students in
the authors’ university. The students were distributed
evenly in three classes at three different time slots
(8:1010:00 a.m., 2:104:00 p.m., 4:106:00 p.m.) on
Wednesday. The students who took this course were
required to record 7-day sleep logs in the first month
of the class as the course assignment. However, they
were solicited to give their permission on the incorpora-
tion of their sleep log data in this study as courtesy and
were informed that no extra credit would be given for
their participation. Of the 362 students enrolled in the
class, 314 completed the consent forms. Among them,
237 students were aged 18 24. Students from all aca-
demic fields of the university, i.e., arts, social sciences,
science, engineering, law, and business, were included in
the study.
Materials and measures
The daily sleep patterns were recorded in sleep logs. It
has been shown that subjective estimates of sleep time
and sleep latency in the mornings after the overnight
sleep recordings are positively correlated to recorded
sleep time and sleep latency, respectively [28,29]. The
sleep log form was designed for a 7-day recording period
and tended to show the daily sleep pattern of the users.
The form included a line space at the top for filling in
subject name, major, grade, and recording period. This
was followed by the instruction translated as ‘‘Please
record the daily sleep information in 5 minutes after
getting up. Record each nap in 5 minutes after waking
up as well.’’ A grid was placed below the instructions.
The grid included dates (listed as rows in the first
column) and headings for eight self-reported question
variables and four self-calculated variables (listed as
columns in the first row). The question variables included
bedtime, time falling asleep, number of awakenings
during the sleep period, time waking up, rise time, sleep
quality evaluation (from 1 = extremely awful to 10 = ex-
tremely great), naptime, and significant events on the
L.-L. Tsai, S.-P. Li / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 56 (2004) 231–237232
previous day. The self-calculated variables included sleep
latency, time asleep, time in bed, and sleep efficiency.
Data analysis
A total of 12 variables were derived from sleep logs.
Bedtime, rise time, number of awakenings, sleep quality,
and naptime were recorded by subjects. Sleep latency (the
time difference between bedtime and time falling asleep),
time asleep (the time difference between time falling asleep
and time waking up), time in bed (the time difference between
bedtime and rise time), sleep efficiency (time asleep 100/
time in bed), total sleep (the sum of sleep time at night and
naptime during the day), bedtime regularity (standard devia-
tion of bedtime over the 7-day recording period), and rise time
regularity (standard deviation of rise time over the 7-day
recording period) were then calculated. For bedtime, rise time,
time asleep, time in bed, and total sleep, we used 2
(genders) 4 (grades: freshman, sophomore, junior, and
senior) 2 (days: weekday, weekend) analysis of variances
(ANOVAs) with repeated measures on day. The data dis-
tributions of sleep latency (skewness g1 = 1.123), number of
awakenings (g1 = 2.886), sleep efficiency (g1 = 1.433),
sleep quality (g1 = 0.893), naptime (g1 = 1.045), bedtime
standard deviation (g1 = 1.365), and rise time standard
deviation (0.797) were significantly skewed. Since signifi-
cantly skewed sample violates the assumption of normality
associated with parametric statistical methods, nonpara-
metric statistical methods were used and medians/semi-
interquartile ranges were presented for those skewed data.
Gender effect was evaluated by Mann Whitney test with
chi-square approximation, grade effect by Kruskal Wallis
test, and day effect by Wilcoxon signed ranks test with
normal (z) approximation.
Based on the cutoff values with significantly increased
odds ratios of global sleep dissatisfaction in [12], we define
sleep difficulties as time in bed less than 7 h or mean sleep
latency longer than 30 min. We also arbitrarily defined
sleep difficulties as the number of awakenings more than
once, sleep efficiency less than 85%, rating on sleep quality
less than 6, or naptime longer than an hour. Pearson chi-
square tests were performed for evaluating the dependency
of the distribution of each sleep difficulty on gender and on
grade, separately.
Pearson correlation was calculated between sleep qual-
ity and other sleep variables for each subject. The Fisher’s
rto ztransformation was then performed for each corre-
lation coefficient. One-group ttest was used to test whether
the means of the zscores were zero and independent ttest
was used for the gender differences. We performed post
hoc comparisons of means by using Tukey’s test for
ANOVAs and MannWhitney test for KruskalWallis test.
All statistical analyses were performed using SYSTAT 7.0
for Windows.
Results
The demographic characteristics of the participating
college students are depicted in Table 1. No gender differ-
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of participating college students
Gender Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior
Woman Age, year (range) 18.9 F0.8 (18 – 21) 20.3 F1.05 (19 – 23) 21.1F0.8 (20 – 24) 22.2 F0.6 (21 – 23)
Subject number 29 24 31 26
Man Age, year (range) 19.1 F0.8 (18 – 21) 20.0 F0.9 (19 – 22) 21.6 F0.9 (20 – 24) 22.7F0.6 (22 – 24)
Subject number 48 19 48 12
Table 2
Sleep variables for weekdays and weekends by gender
Woman (n=110) Man (n= 127)
Sleep variable Weekday Weekend Weekday Weekend Statistical significance
Bedtime (hh:mm Fmin) 1:27 F57 1:21 F69 1:40 F55 1:45 F82 G*
Rise time (hh:mm Fmin) 8:27 F61 9:12 F79 8:39 F57 9:39 F89 G*, D***
Time asleep (min) 381 F57 428 F65 384 F52 438 F75 D***
Time in bed (min) 420 F57 471 F63 418 F51 474 F69 D***
Total sleep (min) 419 F60 466 F78 411 F60 465 F87 D***
Sleep latency (min) 17.5/6.4 15.0/5.7 14.0/5.7 12.0/6.5 G*, D**
Number of awakenings 0.8/0.5 1.0/0.5 0.6/0.4 0.5/0.5 G**
Sleep efficiency (%) 92.1/3.4 93.3/3.8 93.0/2.7 94.4/2.5 D**
Sleep quality 7.2/0.6 7.5/0.7 7.6/0.9 8.0/0.9 G*, D***
Naptime (min) 36.0/22.7 26.4/30.0 22.2/20.7 24.0/23.7 G*
Data are presented as means FS.D. or medians/semi-interquartile ranges. See the text for a detailed description. Subject numbers (n) are given in parentheses.
Gender effect is abbreviated as G and day effect as D.
* Significant difference P< .05.
** Significant difference P< .01.
*** Significant difference P< .001.
L.-L. Tsai, S.-P. Li / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 56 (2004) 231–237 233
ence was found in the mean age. The mean age consistently
increased with grade ( F= 164.24, df = 3.233, P=.0001). It
was noted that the number of males in sophomore and
senior years was less than that of freshmen and juniors.
However, we thought this would have, if any, few effect
on gender and grade interaction since interaction effect of
gender and grade was not significant on any sleep variable.
Weekday/weekend differences
As shown in Table 2, the rise time ( F= 84.36, df = 1.229,
P< .001), but not bedtime, was significantly earlier on
weekdays than on weekends. The students spent signifi-
cantly less time in bed ( F= 123.45, df = 1.229, P< .001),
slept shorter ( F= 102.22, df = 1.229, P< .001), had less total
sleep ( F= 74.40, df = 1.229, P< .001), longer sleep latency
(z= 3.274, P= .001) and lower sleep efficiency (z= 3.237,
P= .001), and gave significantly lower ratings on sleep
quality (z= 4.201, P< .001) on weekdays as compared with
those on weekends.
Gender differences
Table 2 also shows that the women had earlier bedtime
(F=5.35, df =1.229, P= .022) and rise time ( F=4.14,
df =1.229, P= .043), longer sleep latency (v
2
=6.00,
df =1, N= 237, P= .014 on weekdays; v
2
= 4.84, df =1,
N= 237, P= .028 on weekends), more awakenings during
night sleep (v
2
= 7.12, df =1, N= 237, P= .008 on week-
days; v
2
= 12.54, df =1, N= 237, P< .001 on weekends),
lower ratings on sleep quality (v
2
= 3.52, df =1, N= 237,
P= .061 on weekdays; v
2
= 9.92, df =1, N= 237, P= .002 on
weekends), and longer naptime (v
2
= 7.95, df =1, N= 237,
P= .005 on weekdays; v
2
= 5.48, df =1, N= 237, P= .019 on
weekends) than the men. Gender differences were not
significant in the bedtime regularity (median/semi-interquar-
tile range: 50/16 min in women vs. 52/18 min in men) or the
rise time regularity (66/18 in women vs. 73/22 in men).
Bedtime was more regular than rise time (Wilcoxon signed
ranks test, zapproximation = 6.13, P< .001).
We define sleep difficulties as time in bed less than
7 h, mean sleep latency longer than 30 min [12], number
of awakenings more than once, sleep efficiency less than
85%, rating on sleep quality less than 6, or naptime
longer than an hour. The percentage of difficulties in
sleep latency was significantly higher in women (11.82%
on weekdays and 15.45% on weekends) than in men
(11.81% on weekdays and 6.30% on weekends) on
weekends. Sleep difficulties in the number of awakenings
were also higher in women (40.91% on weekdays and
37.27% on weekends) than in men (25.20% on weekdays
and 18.11% on weekends). Lastly, more women (21.82%
on weekdays and 23.64% on weekends) than men
(11.02% on weekdays and 21.82% on weekends) took
mean naptime longer than an hour on weekdays. In
contrast, gender differences were not found in the per-
centage of sleep difficulties in time in bed (48.94% of all
the students on weekdays and 19.83% on weekends),
sleep efficiency (12.66% on weekdays and 15.19% on
Table 3
Correlations between sleep quality and other sleep variables
Sleep variable Woman (n= 110) Man (n= 127)
Bedtime .061 F.562 .029 F.545
Rise time* .042 F.546 .215 F.537
yyy
Time asleep .241 F.615
yyy
.370 F.571
yyy
Time in bed* .064 F.560 .258 F.586
yyy
Sleep latency (min) .304 F.575
yyy
.397 F.644
yyy
Number of awakenings .405 F.682
yyy
.396 F.925
yy
Sleep efficiency (%)* .374 F.594
yyy
.532 F.624
yyy
Naptime (min) .042 F.498 .139 F.917
Data are presented as means FS.D. The correlation coefficients of each
subject were converted to Fisher’s zscores, and the means and standard
deviations of the zscores were then converted back to correlation
coefficients. One-group ttest was performed to test whether the means of
the zscores were zero and independent ttest for gender differences.
* Significance level for gender differences, P< .05.
y
Significance level for one-group ttest, P< .05.
yy
Significance level for one-group ttest, P< .01.
yyy
Significance level for one-group ttest, P< .001.
Fig. 1. Mean and standard deviation of rise time (top), time in bed (middle),
and time asleep (bottom) across grade on weekdays and weekends.
L.-L. Tsai, S.-P. Li / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 56 (2004) 231–237234
weekends), or sleep quality (14.77% on weekdays and
10.55% on weekends).
The relationship between sleep quality and other sleep
variables is presented in Table 3. Sleep quality was posi-
tively correlated with time asleep and sleep efficiency but
negatively with sleep latency and number of awakenings in
both men and women. In contrast, sleep quality was
correlated with rise time and time in bed in men but not
in women. Furthermore, the relationships between sleep
quality and rise time, time in bed, and sleep efficiency were
stronger in men than in women.
Grade differences
Interaction effect of grade and day was found on rise time
(F=3.83, df = 3.229, P= .011), time in bed ( F= 4.19,
df = 3.229, P= .007), and time asleep ( F= 3.50, df = 3.229,
P= .016). The freshmen had a shorter sleep time at night
than other students on weekdays, which was not due to the
bedtime difference but an earlier rise time on weekdays (Fig.
1). Of all the students, the seniors had the longest sleep
latency (v
2
= 8.83, df =3, N= 237, P= .032 on weekdays;
v
2
= 5.57, df =3, N= 237, P= .134 on weekends) and nap-
time (v
2
= 18.76, df =3, N= 237, P< .001 on weekdays;
v
2
= 9.35, df =3, N= 237, P=.025 on weekends; Fig. 2).
The percentage of sleep difficulties in time in bed was the
highest in the freshmen on weekdays (v
2
= 16.84, df =3,
N= 237, P< .001). Difficulties in sleep latency were the
highest in the seniors on weekdays (v
2
=9.14, df =3,
N= 237, P= .027). Grade differences were not found in
the percentage of sleep difficulties in awakenings, sleep
efficiency, sleep quality, or naptime.
Daytime napping and nighttime sleep
To determine whether longer naptime was related to
worse sleep patterns at night [29], we compared the sleep
variables of the students whose naptime was longer than an
hour with those of the rest of the students. Indeed, students
with long naptime had more awakenings at night
(v
2
= 11.13, df =1, N=237, P= .001 on weekdays;
v
2
= 6.99, df =1,N= 237, P= .008 on weekends) and poorer
sleep quality (v
2
= 6.04, df =1, N= 237, P= .014 on week-
days; v
2
= 3.10, df =1, N= 237, P= .078 on weekends).
Discussion
This study aimed to determine gender and grade effects
on daily sleep patterns in the college students aged 18 24
years. Significant gender and grade differences in several
sleep variables were found in this student group. Although
the student subjects were recruited exclusively from those
enrolled in a single course, the gender and grade effect on
sleep might not be specific to this particular group of
students. First, the means of all, except for naptime, sleep
variables in this study were comparable to those of a group
of undergraduates who did not take the course (n= 49,
unpublished data). Second, though the students enrolled in
the sleep management course could be somewhat more
concerned about their sleep, they probably did not have
more sleep problems than other students. The PSQI scores
of the students who took the course were not higher but
even lower than those of the students who did not took the
course (mean global PSQI score, 5.46 F1.75, n= 279 vs.
Fig. 2. Medians and semi-interquartile range of sleep latency (top) and naptime (bottom) across grade on weekdays and weekends.
L.-L. Tsai, S.-P. Li / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 56 (2004) 231–237 235
6.52 F2.42, n= 42; unpublished data). Nonetheless, we
could not definitively exclude the possibility that female
students and/or seniors tended to select the course because
of sleep difficulties, which might thereby be related to the
gender and grade effect shown in this study.
As previous studies have shown, the college students
woke up later, had longer time in bed and sleep time, and
reported better sleep quality on weekends [17,23,24].In
contrast to the results in [24], this study showed that the
bedtime on weekends remained the same as on weekdays.
The discrepancy could be explained by much later bedtime
on weekdays shown in this study than previously shown.
The students went to bed on weekends at the time equiv-
alent to those in [24]. On the other hand, not reported
previously, the students had shorter sleep latency and better
sleep efficiency on weekends. Stress, especially the emo-
tional responses to it, is related to several sleep aspects,
including sleep latency [31]. It is possible that for some
students, less stress on weekends is one of the factors related
to shorter sleep latency on weekends. Furthermore, based on
their relationship to sleep quality (Table 2), shorter sleep
latency, longer sleep time, and higher sleep efficiency all
directed to better sleep quality on weekends.
In contrast to previous studies, which mostly failed to
find gender differences in daily sleep patterns, we did find
those in several sleep variables. Given that female college
students reported a higher level of stress than male students
[32] and, as mentioned above, that stress is related to sleep
[31], stress could be involved in the gender difference in
sleep. On the other hand, since gender differences were also
shown in the relationship between sleep quality and other
sleep variables, sleep perception and cognition difference
could be another factor. However, future studies are still
needed to clarify how biological and psychological matura-
tional processes, stress, sleep perception, and cognition, as
well as other factors, relate to gender differences in self-
reported sleep patterns in college students.
One special aspect concerning gender differences is that
the poorer sleep patterns and the higher prevalence of sleep
difficulties in the female college students persist even on
weekends. It was further noted that the number of women
with sleep latency difficulty even increased on weekends
compared to weekdays. In contrast, the number of men with
sleep latency difficulty decreased on weekends. It seemed
that weekend was of benefit to men but not to women in
terms of reduction in sleep latency difficulty. We think this
finding is very important and merits further studies. Week-
ends are thought to be more relaxed and thus more helpful
to a good night sleep than weekdays. Why women could not
take the advantage of weekends like men needs further
studies to answer the question. Another thing to be pointed
out is that unexpected gender differences were also found in
the number of awakenings at night in these young college
students. One previous study in general young adults found
that the major causes of more awakenings in young women
were children and bed partners [4]. Of the 237 students
participating in this study, 235 students answered question
10 (‘‘Do you have a bed partner or roommate?’’) in PSQI
administered in class as a course activity. Only 4 of the 235
student reported having a partner in same bed. On the other
hand, married rate was below 16% and fertility rate was
below 7% in women aged 20 24 years in Taiwan (MOI
Statistical Information Service http://www.moi.gov.tw/W3/
stat/, Department of Statistics, Minister of the Interior,
ROC). It is likely that most female students in this study
were single and had no children. Even if most of them had
no children and had no bed partners, the women still had
more night awakenings than the men. The relationship
between naptime and awakenings suggests longer naptime
could be attributed to more awakenings in the female
college students. Future studies are still needed to clarify
whether other factors also cause the young female college
students to wake up more frequently at night.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to
show an interaction effect between grade and weekday/
weekend on sleep among college students. Grade differ-
ences in sleep patterns were shown mostly on weekdays
only. The freshmen got up earlier and slept less compared to
other students on weekdays but not on weekends. The
seniors having the longest sleep latency were shown only
on weekdays. Over twice more seniors (26.31%) than other
students (9.03%) had sleep latency difficulty on weekdays
but not on weekends. It is likely that grade differences
shown only on weekdays were due to different activity
schedules and academic demands on weekdays in different
grades instead of biological age differences. Although those
grade differences subsided on weekends, the much higher
number of short sleep time in the freshmen (67.53%) and
that of sleep latency difficulty in the seniors on weekdays
still warrant a heightened attention to the grade-related sleep
problems among college students.
In summary, this study found significant gender and
grade differences in several sleep variables among college
students. The gender difference in most sleep variables did
not depend on weekday/weekend difference, but the grade
difference depended on weekday/weekend difference. The
poorer sleep patterns of the female students on both
weekdays and weekends warrant special attention to the
progress of their sleep problems in the long run. The
freshmen need to be especially aware of the consequences
of insufficient sleep and the seniors need to deal with long
sleep latency and naptime. On the other hand, this study
found that the percentage of sleep difficulties in general
college students was amazingly high, e.g., over 48% of the
college students had short sleep time, which suggests a
probably equivalent amount of students had insufficient
sleep on weekdays. Although taking naps during daytime
may be beneficial to make up for some insufficient sleep at
night and to reduce sleepiness in class, it was limited
because naptime over an hour would be related to more
awakenings at night. Since near one fifth of the young
college students had short sleep duration on weekends,
L.-L. Tsai, S.-P. Li / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 56 (2004) 231–237236
sleep debt could be chronically accumulated in those
students. In addition to insufficient sleep, other sleep
difficulties are also common among the college students.
These findings warrant sleep education programs and
interventions for the students as previously suggested
[24,30]. It is conceivable that sleep education in college
students may help them be aware of their own sleep
problems and thereby willing to choose activity schedules
and sleep habits good for sleep.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank teaching assistants Yi-
Ling Wang, Chung-Kang Shen, Chung-Shan Kao, Yu-Zhe
Tsai, and Yu-Tien Chang for helping in the administration
and collection of sleep logs. This study was supported by
National Science Council, R.O.C. College Student Grant
NSC88-2815-C194-024H.
References
[1] Doi Y, Minowa M, Uchiyama M, Okawa M. Subjective sleep quality
and sleep problems in the general Japanese adult population. Psychia-
try Clin Neurosci 2001;55:213 – 5.
[2] Liu X, Uchiyama M, Kim K, Okawa M, Shibui K, Kudo Y, Doi Y,
Minowa M, Ogihara R. Sleep loss and daytime sleepiness in the
general adult population of Japan. Psychiatry Res 2000;93:1 – 11.
[3] Park YM, Matsumoto K, Shinkoda H, Nagashima H, Kang MJ, Seo
YJ. Age and gender difference in habitual sleep – wake rhythm. Psy-
chiatry Clin Neurosci 2001;55:201 – 2.
[4] Reyner A, Horne JA. Gender- and age-related differences in sleep
determined by home-recorded sleep logs and actimetry from 400
adults. Sleep 1995;18:127 – 34.
[5] Ford DE, Kamerow DB. Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances
and psychiatric disorders. JAMA 1989;262:1479 – 84.
[6] Husby R, Lingjaerde O. Prevalence of reported sleeplessness in north-
ern Norway in relation to sex, age and season. Acta Psychiatr Scand
1990;81:542 – 7.
[7] Janson C, Gislason T, Backer WD, Plaschke P, Bjornsson E, Hetta J,
Kristbjarnason H, Vermeire P, Boman G. Prevalence of sleep distur-
bances among young adults in three European countries. Sleep 1995;
18:589 – 97.
[8] Kim K, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, Liu X, Ogihara R. An epidemio-
logical study of insomnia among Japanese general population. Sleep
2000;23:41 – 7.
[9] Lindberg E, Janson C, Gislason T, Bjornsson E, Hetta J, Boman G.
Sleep disturbances in a young adult population: can gender differ-
ences be explained by differences in psychological status? Sleep
1997;20:381 – 7.
[10] Ohayon M. Epidemiological study on insomnia in the general pop-
ulation. Sleep 1996;19:S7 – S15.
[11] Pallesen S, Nordhus IH, Nielsen GH, Havik OE, Kvale G, Johnsen
S, Skjotskift S. Prevalence of insomnia in the adult Norwegian pop-
ulation. Sleep 2001;24:771 – 9.
[12] Ohayon MM, Zulley J. Correlates of global sleep dissatisfaction in the
German population. Sleep 2001;24:780 – 7.
[13] Fukuda K, Ishihara K. Age-related changes of sleeping pattern during
adolescence. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001;55:231 – 2.
[14] Gau S-F, Soong W-T. Sleep problems of junior high school students
in Taipei. Sleep 1995;18:667 – 73.
[15] Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime function-
ing in adolescents. Child Dev 1998;69:875 – 87.
[16] Liu X, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, Kurita H. Prevalence and correlates
of self-reported sleep problems among Chinese adolescents. Sleep
2000;23:27 – 34.
[17] Strauch I, Meier B. Sleep need in adolescents: a longitudinal ap-
proach. Sleep 1988;11:378 – 86.
[18] Ohayon MM, Roberts RE, Zulley J, Smirne S, Priest RG. Prevalence
and patterns of problematic sleep among older adolescents. J Am
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000;39:1549 – 56.
[19] Ohayon MM, Roberts RE. Comparability of sleep disorders diagnoses
using DSM-IV and ICSD classifications with adolescents. Sleep
2001;24:920 – 5.
[20] Coren S. The prevalence of self-reported sleep disturbances in young
adults. Int J Neurosci 1994;79:67 – 73.
[21] Hume KI, Van F, Watson A. A field study of age and gender differ-
ences in habitual adult sleep. J Sleep Res 1998;7:85 94.
[22] Hicks RA, Fernandez C, Pellegrini RJ. Striking changes in the sleep
satisfaction of university students over the last two decades. Percept
Mot Skills 2001;93:660.
[23] Hawkins J, Shaw P. Self-reported sleep quality in college students: a
repeated measures approach. Sleep 1992;15:545 – 9.
[24] Buboltz WC, Brown F, Soper B. Sleep habits and patterns of college
students: a preliminary study. J Am Coll Health 2001;50:131– 5.
[25] Jean-Louis G, von Gizycki H, Zizi F, Nunes J. Mood states and
sleepiness in college students: influences of age, sex, habitual sleep,
and substance use. Percept Mot Skills 1998;87:507 – 12.
[26] Ban DJ, Lee TJ. Sleep duration, subjective sleep disturbances and
associated factors among university students in Korea. J Korean
Med Sci 2001;16:475 – 80.
[27] Liu X, Tang M, Hu L, Wang A. Sleep quality and its correlates in
college students. Chin Ment Health J 1995;9:148 – 50.
[28] Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, Guilleminault C, Zar-
cone VP, Spiegel R. Self-reports versus sleep laboratory findings in
122 drug-free subjects with complaints of chronic insomnia. Am J
Psychiatry 1976;133:1382 – 8.
[29] Rogers AE, Caruso CC, Aldrich MS. Reliability of sleep diaries for
assessment of sleep/wake patterns. Nurs Res 1993;42:368 72.
[30] Brown FC, Buboltz WC. Applying sleep research to university stu-
dents: recommendations for developing a student sleep education
program. J Coll Stud Dev 2002;43:411 – 6.
[31] Verlander LA, Benedict JO, Hanson DP. Stress and sleep patterns of
college students. Percept Mot Skills 1999;88:893 – 8.
[32] Bush HS, Thompson M, van Tubergen N. Personal assessment of
stress factors for college students. J Sch Health 1985;55:370 – 5.
L.-L. Tsai, S.-P. Li / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 56 (2004) 231–237 237
... Lund et al. [7] found that, for college students, emotional and academic stress had the largest negative impact on sleep while the use of prescription, over-the-counter, and recreational drugs, as well as alcohol consumption, also impacted sleep. Gender impacts sleep, as Fatima et al. [6] reported that adult females have poorer sleep quality than their male counterparts and Tsai and Li [8] found that female college students tend to have poorer sleep quality compared to their male peers. In addition, Fatima et al. [6] illuminated how these factors associated with poor sleep are often interrelated, experienced simultaneously, and can have a compounding impact on adult sleep quality. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: This study's purpose was to determine the impact of weighted blanket use on moderate to severe insomnia in adults with sensory sensitivity greater than the average population. Methods: For this study, a four-week, single-case, multiple-participant ABA study design was used. Through convenience sampling, four participants scoring 15 or greater on the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI), which categorizes them as having moderate to severe insomnia, and much more than most people in sensory sensitivity on the Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile were recruited. First, seven-day baseline sleep data was gathered, followed by two weeks of weighted blanket use, concluding with a seven-day withdrawal phase. Additional outcome measures included: Tuck and Snooze Survey, Consensus Sleep Diary Morning, and Additional Sleep Diary Questions. Data analysis included visual analysis, mean comparisons, Tau-U calculations, and pre- to post-ISI category comparisons. Results: All participants' ISI scores were categorized as one level less severe postintervention. All participants demonstrated increased sleep quality, and three participants showed an increase in sleep duration based on individual mean comparisons between baseline and intervention phases. Conclusion: Weighted blankets appear beneficial in reducing insomnia severity in adults with much more than the average population sensory sensitivity. In addition, those with self-reported anxiety may have increased benefit from this intervention.
... Twenge and Martin (2020) found significant gender differences in digital media use among American and British youth, with females spending more time on the Internet and social media, while males allocated more time to gaming and electronic devices. A study of 237 college students found that younger college students had shorter sleep time, while female college students had poorer sleep quality than males (Tsai and Li 2004). A study in Canada found that there were significant gender differences in weekend time use and that adolescents had less leisure time as their grade level increased (Hilbrecht et al. 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
Everyone’s time is limited, and there is competition between different aspects of time use; this requires comprehensive consideration of the effects of different aspects of time use on cognitive achievement in adolescents. This study uses a dataset of 11,717 students from a nationally representative large-scale survey project conducted in 2013 to 2014 to clarify the relationship between time use (including working on homework, playing sports, surfing the Internet, watching TV, and sleeping) and cognitive achievement among Chinese adolescents, and explores the mediating role of depression symptoms in the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement. The results of the correlation analysis show that the average daily time spent on homework, playing sports, and sleeping is significantly positively correlated with cognitive achievement (p < 0.01), while time spent surfing the Internet and watching TV are significantly negatively correlated with cognitive achievement (p < 0.01). The results of the mediating effect model show that depression symptoms play a mediating role in the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement among Chinese adolescents. Specifically, time spent playing sports (indirect effect = 0.008, p < 0.001) and sleeping (indirect effect = 0.015, p < 0.001) have a positive effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators; time spent on homework (indirect effect = −0.004, p < 0.001), surfing the Internet (indirect effect = −0.002, p = 0.046), and watching TV (indirect effect = −0.005, p < 0.001) have a negative effect on cognitive achievement when using depression symptoms as mediators. This study contributes to the understanding of the relationship between time use and cognitive achievement among Chinese adolescents.
... Our study also replicated sex differences in chronotype (Tonetti et al., 2008;Tsai & Li, 2004). These results support the notion that males likely gained ancestral adaptive advantages with an evening chronotype profile and that they possibly underwent the influence of sexual selection (see Piffer, 2010;Randler et al., 2012;Putilov, 2014;Ponzi et al., 2015a, b). ...
Article
Full-text available
Dark triad (DT) refers to a set of antisocial and self-interested personality traits (Machiavellianism, sub-clinical psychopathy and narcissism). The nocturnal cognitive niche hypothesis suggests that the DT was advantageous in a nocturnal niche because it enables exploitative strategies and would be linked with an evening-oriented chronotype as part of a fast life history strategy. In the present self-report study (N = 300), this hypothesis was tested in a Brazilian sample. It was found that Machiavellianism was a predictor of this chronotype profile in females and secondary psychopathy was a predictor of this profile in males. Also, sex differences were replicated: on average, males exhibited “darker” personalities and more evening-oriented chronotype. Finally, these associations were tested with structural equation modeling controlling for sex and was found that the exploitative “darkcore” (psychopathy and Machiavellianism) predicts an evening-oriented chronotype. These results provide the first support for the nocturnal cognitive niche hypothesis in a non-WEIRD population.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose While there is a rising focus on sleep issues among athletes, a notable gap exists in the comparative analysis of sleep patterns between male and female athletes. This study aims to evaluate the sleep patterns of collegiate swimmers during a specific period (pre-competition training phase) based on the National Sleep Foundation’s recommendations and compares sleep differences between males and females. Patients and Methods 15 swimmers (6 males and 9 females) completed the Athlete Sleep Screening Questionnaire (ASSQ) and wore actigraphy devices for 8 consecutive nights to record objective sleep patterns including bedtime, wake time, sleep onset latency, total sleep time, wake after sleep onset, and sleep efficiency. Results The total sleep time of collegiate male (5.0±0.4 h, 4.6 to 5.4h) and female (6.0±0.7 h, 5.5 to 6.5h) swimmers was less than 7 hours per night, and male swimmers’ sleep efficiency (76.7±8.9%, 67.4 to 86.0%) was lower than the 85% standard. Male swimmers had less objectively measured sleep duration (p=0.006, d=1.66, large effect), lower sleep efficiency (p=0.013, d=1.51, large effect), and longer wake after sleep onset (p=0.096, d=0.94, moderate effect). Female swimmers had higher sleep difficulty scores (p=0.06, d=1.08, moderate effect), and there was a significant difference in the distribution of sleep difficulty scores between male and female swimmers (p=0.033, V=0.045, small effect). Conclusion Collegiate swimmers exhibited poor sleep patterns during pre-competition preparation, and the sleep fragmentation of male swimmers was more pronounced. There were sex differences in both subjective and objective measured sleep patterns, with male swimmers having less sleep and low efficiency, while female swimmers experienced more significant sleep disturbances.
Article
Objectives: This study investigated if unhealthy sleeping habits of university students can be explained by the Theory of Planned Behavior. Method: An online questionnaire was administered to 1006 undergraduate students at a Belgian university to measure their frequency of irregular sleeping times, daytime napping, and pre-bedtime alcohol or internet use, as well as the attitudes, perceived norms, perceived control and intentions for these behaviors. Results: Principal Component Analysis and internal consistency analysis confirmed the validity and reliability of the scales measuring the TPB dimensions. Expected outcomes, perceived norms and perceived control significantly explained intentions to avoid irregular sleeping times, daytime naps, pre-bedtime activity and pre-bedtime alcohol use. Intentions and perceived behavioral control explained self-reported irregular sleeping times, daytime napping, pre-bedtime activity, and pre-bedtime alcohol use. Significant differences in predictions were found for gender, study program, type of residence and age. Conclusions: The TPB provides a useful theoretical framework to explain students' sleeping behaviors.
Article
Full-text available
Background and Aim Students sometimes participate in harmful activities that adversely influence their behaviors and well‐being throughout college, which is one of the sensitive phases in an individual's life. Aim: To assess the health‐related behaviors of university students. Methods A cross‐sectional study that includes systematic randomly selected 383 students from various colleges of Ras Al Khaimah Medical and Health Sciences University (RAKMHSU), Ras Al Khaimah Emirate, United Arab Emirates. A self‐reported questionnaire included students' demographic traits and behaviors, including safety, medication intake, cigarette smoking, nutrition, physical activity, and health‐related topics. Results Most participants were females (69.7%), 13.3% were obese while 28.2% were overweight. The data revealed a significant difference between male and female students regarding medication intake without prescription, nutrition, physical activity, and health‐related topics. The data also revealed that the majority of the students were attempting to lose weight, and the former male smokers had fewer trials to quit the use of all tobacco products than females. Conclusion More than a quarter of participants were overweight, and the majority of students did not adhere to the guidelines for safety and nutritious eating. This study recognized significant health promotion opportunities for university students that can be carried out to establish a healthier youth for society.
Article
Full-text available
A substantial body of research indicates that higher education students from lower social class backgrounds tend to have poorer health than those from higher social class backgrounds. To investigate sleep as a potential mediator of this relationship, online survey responses of students from five large Australian universities, one Irish university and one large Australian technical college were analysed in three studies (Study 1 N = 628; Study 2 N = 376; Study 3 N = 446). The results revealed that sleep quality, sleep duration, sleep disturbances, pre-sleep worries and sleep schedule variability mediated the relationship between social class and physical and mental health. Sleep remained a significant mediator when controlling for related variables and other mediators. Thus, the findings suggest that sleep partly explains social class differences in health. We discuss the importance of addressing sleep issues among students from lower social class backgrounds.
Article
Full-text available
Sleeping is essential for student physical as well as mental health. Sleep disturbance is distressing state that affect quality of work. Objective: To determine the sleep disturbance in DPT students. Methods: This observational study recruited 884 participants out of which 800 filled the questionnaire with mean age 21.54 ±1.76 from different medical universities and medical colleges of Punjab. Non probability convenient sampling technique was used. Both male and female students of all the years of DPT were included. Students with psychiatric illness, chronic medical illness, smoking and taking drugs excluded from the study. The effects of insomnia were checked by Athens Insomnia Scale (AIS) questionnaire. For data analysis SPSS version 26 was used. Results: Mean age of the physical therapy students was 21.54 ±1.76. Out of 800, 61.75% students were insomniac. Most of the students had unsatisfactory sleep quality (55.87%), awakening at the night time (62%), and day time sleepiness (76.25%). Conclusion: According to present study prevalence of sleep disturbance was very high among physical therapy students
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter aims to assess the problems faced by the students doing B.Ed. The result revealed the continuous problems that are faced by the students and teachers of B.Ed. on a daily basis which are left unseen but has a need to be brought into consideration. The main issue that most pupil teacher had was the lack of resources between teachers and students and the technology divide, both of which hindered the ability to learn and train as skilled teachers. The learning that was imparted to B.Ed. students was also seen to be brief and constrained, which enabled the students to expand their creativity in the field of teaching while restricting themselves to theoretical knowledge. Since today's teachers' trainees will be the world's future creators, their upbringing as educators should be done with good planning and curricula in order to build a productive world. Teacher trainees are the foundation of a brighter future, so their problems should be considered legitimate.
Article
Introduction Insomnia is a serious problem after traumatic brain injury (TBI) and partially improves via sleeping pills. We investigated the efficacy of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) with a focus on the role of age and gender. Materials and Methods In a randomized double-blind clinical trial, 60 eligible TBI-induced insomnia patients were assigned to real and sham tDCS groups and were treated for three weeks. Sham but not real tDCS took sleeping pills for the first three weeks of the study and then used the placebo until the end of the study. The placebo was used by the real-tDCS group throughout the study. Sleep quality and insomnia severity were respectively evaluated by Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) and Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) at three time points. Results Real tDCS group reported lower mean ISI and PSQI scores at 3 weeks post treatment onset and maintained this decline for six weeks post treatment onset (P < 0.001). In younger participants and those identified as men, the treatment-induced attenuation of the mean PSQI score was reported higher and more lasting in real than sham tDCS groups. Conclusion Gender and age-specific tDCS protocols may be warranted to optimize the therapeutic effect of tDCS.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this investigation was to study the geographic variation in sleep complaints and to identify risk factors for sleep disturbances in three European countries: Iceland (Reykjavik), Sweden (Uppsala and Goteborg) and Belgium (Antwerp). The study involved a random population of 2,202 subjects (age 20–45 years) who participated in the European Community Respiratory Health Survey. The subjects answered a questionnaire on sleep disturbances. Participants in Iceland and Sweden also estimated their sleep habits and sleep times during a period of 1 week in a sleep diary. Habitual (≥3/week) difficulties inducing sleep (DIS) were reported by 6–9% and early morning awakenings by 5–6% of the subjects. The estimated number of awakenings and the prevalence of nightmares was significantly lower in Reykjavik. Participants in Reykjavik went to bed at night and woke in the morning approximately 1 hour later than participants at the Swedish centers (p < 0.001). Symptoms of gastroesophageal reflux (GER) were associated with DIS (odds ratio [OR] = 2.7), nightmares (OR = 4.4), longer sleep latency and frequent nocturnal awakenings. Smoking correlated positively to DIS (OR =1.8) and estimated sleep latency. We conclude that the prevalence of DIS was fairly similar at these four European centers but that there was a variation in the prevalence of nightmares and nocturnal awakenings. The significant correlation between reported GER and subjective quality of sleep should be followed up in studies using objective measurements.
Article
Full-text available
The authors compared the sleep laboratory recordings of 122 drug-free subjects who complained of chronic insomnia with the subjects' estimates of their habitual sleep characteristics and their estimated sleep time on the morning after sleeping in the laboratory. Most subjects consistently underestimated the amount of time they slept and overestimated the amount of time it took them to get to sleep in comparison with laboratory data. All subjects consistently underestimated the number of arousals they experienced. The authors discuss the implications of these findings for the treatment and definition of insomnia and for further research.
Article
Full-text available
A longitudinal assessment of sleep quality is presented. Undergraduates provided two levels of repeated measures data. Sleep log information was gathered across the 7 days of the week, in three replications, over a semester. There were several new findings. Across the week, students spent significantly more time in bed on weekend days as compared with weekdays. Further, a new measure for quality of sleep covaried with the amount of time spent in bed during the week. The students enjoyed their sleep more on the weekends. Finally, both the amount of time in bed and the number of awakenings reported dropped significantly as the semester progressed.
Article
As part of the National Institute of Mental Health Epidemiologic Catchment Area study, 7954 respondents were questioned at baseline and 1 year later about sleep complaints and psychiatric symptoms using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule. Of this community sample, 10.2% and 3.2% noted insomnia and hypersomnia, respectively, at the first interview. Forty percent of those with insomnia and 46.5% of those with hypersomnia had a psychiatric disorder compared with 16.4% of those with no sleep complaints. The risk of developing new major depression was much higher in those who had insomnia at both interviews compared with those without insomnia (odds ratio, 39.8; 95% confidence interval, 19.8 to 80.0). The risk of developing new major depression was much less for those who had insomnia that had resolved by the second visit (odds ratio, 1.6; 95% confidence interval, 0.5 to 5.3). Further research is needed to determine if early recognition and treatment of sleep disturbances can prevent future psychiatric disorders. (JAMA. 1989;262:1479-1484)
Article
Thesis (B.S.)--James Madison University, 1997. Includes bibliographical references.
Article
As part of a comprehensive population health survey in the municipality of Tromsø, north of the Arctic Circle, men between 20 and 54 years and women between 20 and 49 years were presented a questionnaire containing questions about sleeplessness and its possible association with season. Of the 14,667 respondents, 41.7% of the women and 29.9% of the men said they were sometimes bothered by insomnia. Insomnia not associated with any special time of the year was reported by 16.9% of women and 16.2% of men; insomnia in the "dark period" (midwinter insomnia) was reported by 17.6% of women and 9.0% of men; insomnia in the midnight-sun period or in spring or autumn was much less common. Difficulty falling asleep was the most common type of insomnia, especially in winter and summer. Overall, the frequency of insomnia increased with increasing age, but with some notable differences with regard to type (initial insomnia showed little relation to age, whereas middle and late insomnia increased markedly with age) and seasonal type (insomnia in the midnight-sun period decreased with age, whereas the other seasonal types increased with age).
Article
As part of the National Institute of Mental Health Epidemiologic Catchment Area study, 7954 respondents were questioned at baseline and 1 year later about sleep complaints and psychiatric symptoms using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule. Of this community sample, 10.2% and 3.2% noted insomnia and hypersomnia, respectively, at the first interview. Forty percent of those with insomnia and 46.5% of those with hypersomnia had a psychiatric disorder compared with 16.4% of those with no sleep complaints. The risk of developing new major depression was much higher in those who had insomnia at both interviews compared with those without insomnia (odds ratio, 39.8; 95% confidence interval, 19.8 to 80.0). The risk of developing new major depression was much less for those who had insomnia that had resolved by the second visit (odds ratio, 1.6; 95% confidence interval, 0.5 to 5.3). Further research is needed to determine if early recognition and treatment of sleep disturbances can prevent future psychiatric disorders.
Article
A sample of 190 male and female "high school" students completed a sleep questionnaire for the first time when they were 10 to 14 years old. The survey was repeated five times at 2 year intervals. Ninety-three subjects answered the questionnaire each time. Subjective sleep need was assessed by the indicated wish for more sleep. The wish for more sleep was very pronounced, varying between 54.3% and 74.5% across the years. Individual consistency, however, was low since only 14.5% of the adolescents indicated the wish for more sleep in each survey, emphasizing the state dependency of this variable. Within each total sample, subjects with the wish for more sleep (MSL) and with sufficient sleep (SSL) were compared. Subjective sleep need was consistently validated by a syndrome of morning-tiredness. In the last two surveys, there was reduced time in bed (TIB) on weekdays in MSL subjects and longer TIB during vacation in surveys 2 through 5. Furthermore, MSL subjects more often showed irregular sleep habits. The previous sleep history of the MSL subjects in the last survey indicated that concomitants of the wish for more sleep were already experienced earlier in adolescence. The desired sleep duration of these subjects was 1.7 h longer than their current sleep on weekdays, an amount they had not obtained on weekdays since early adolescence. It is concluded that a substantial proportion of the adolescents seem to have had difficulties adapting to the general sleep time reduction occurring in adolescence.