ArticlePDF Available

Identifying Cognitive and Demographic Variables That Contribute to Carer Burden in Dementia

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background/aims: Carer burden has been associated with other carer-reported factors (e.g. depression), but less is known about the influence of more independent variables. We aimed to determine the impact of cognitive deficits, demographic variables and dementia subtype on carer burden. Methods: Patients with Alzheimer's dementia (n = 35) or frontotemporal lobar degeneration (n = 61) underwent assessment of anterograde memory, word generation, impulse control and emotion recognition. Age, sex, relationship type, disease duration and diagnosis were also considered. Carers completed the Zarit Burden Interview. Results: In bivariate regression analyses, carer burden was related to age, diagnosis, memory, impulse control and emotion recognition. Stepwise multivariate regression revealed independent contributions by patient age, memory and emotion recognition, explaining 23% of the variance. Conclusion: The findings could help refine interventions and carer support.
Content may be subject to copyright.
© 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel
1420–8008/13/0000–0000$38.00/0
Original Research Article
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
Identifying Cognitive and
Demographic Variables That Contribute
to Carer Burden in Dementia
Laurie A. Miller
a, b Eneida Mioshi
c, d Sharon Savage
b–d Suncica Lah
b, e
John R. Hodges
b–d Olivier Piguet
b–d
a Neuropsychology Unit, Royal Prince Alfred Hospital and Sydney Medical School,
b ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and Its Disorders,
c Neuroscience Research Australia,
d School of Medical Sciences, University of New South Wales, and
e School of Psychology,
University of Sydney, Sydney, N.S.W. , Australia
Key Words
Carer burden · Alzheimer’s dementia · Frontotemporal lobar degeneration · Cognitive
deficits · Demographic variables
Abstract
Background/Aims: Carer burden has been associated with other carer-reported factors (e.g.
depression), but less is known about the influence of more independent variables. We aimed
to determine the impact of cognitive deficits, demographic variables and dementia subtype
on carer burden. Methods : Patients with Alzheimer’s dementia (n = 35) or frontotemporal lo-
bar degeneration (n = 61) underwent assessment of anterograde memory, word generation,
impulse control and emotion recognition. Age, sex, relationship type, disease duration and
diagnosis were also considered. Carers completed the Zarit Burden Interview. Results: In bi-
variate regression analyses, carer burden was related to age, diagnosis, memory, impulse con-
trol and emotion recognition. Stepwise multivariate regression revealed independent contri-
butions by patient age, memory and emotion recognition, explaining 23% of the variance.
Conclusion: The findings could help refine interventions and carer support.
Copyright © 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel
Introduction
Patients with dementia typically exhibit changes in cognition, memory, emotional
processing, behaviour and personality, which compromise their adaptive functioning and
independence. For carers, dementia often leads to a diminished bond with the patient,
Accepted: Januar y 3, 2013
Published online:
Dr. Laurie Miller
Neuropsychology Unit, Royal Prince Alfred Hospital
Camper down, NSW 2050 (Australia)
E-Mail laurie.miller
@ sydney.edu.au
www.karger.com/dem
DOI: 10.1159/000347146
DEM347146.indd 1DEM347146 indd 1 15.02.2013 14:47:0415 02 2013 14 47 04
2
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
DOI: 10.1159/000347146
Miller et al.: Identifying Cognitive and Demographic Variables That Contribute to Carer
Burden in Dementia
www.karger.com/dem
© 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel
depression and social isolation [1, 2] . The importance of establishing the variables that
contribute most to carer stress and burden cannot be understated, as this will enable clini-
cians and policy makers to identify those carers who are most at risk and design targeted
interventions to alleviate burden when possible.
Previous research indicates that among carers of dementia patients, those who report
higher levels of depression
[3] , neuroticism [4] or less caring relationships with the patient
[1] experience increased carer burden. Specific mood/behavioural changes such as apathy
and depression in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD)
[5] and disinhibition in patients
with frontotemporal dementia (FTD)
[6, 7] have also been linked with high carer burden.
Some carer-related demographic variables influence the level of burden reported: women
tend to report higher levels than men
[2, 8, 9] and those with fewer social contacts have higher
perceived burden/stress
[3, 10] . In contrast, the type of relationship between patient and
carer (e.g. spouse vs. offspring)
[11, 12] and dwelling status (i.e. at home or in an institution)
[3, 13] , appears not to be related to carer burden.
To date, however, few studies have investigated which patient-centred variables (i.e.
those independent from the carer’s report) influence burden of care. When this has been
done, dementia subtype appears to be an important variable modulating burden: carers of
patients with behavioural variant FTD (bvFTD) experience more stress and carer burden
than carers of patients with other forms of dementia
[1, 3, 6, 14, 15] . This increased stress
may be related to the marked deficits in impulse control found more commonly in bvFTD than
in AD patients
[16–19] . In contrast, patient scores on global cognitive screening tools [10] or
measures of activities of daily living
[3] seem to be largely unrelated to carer burden. To our
knowledge, no previous study has directly examined the potential influence of the patient’s
specific cognitive impairments on burden. Thus, in addition to a global cognitive screening
score, we chose to investigate anterograde memory, word generation skills, impulse control
and emotion recognition, because these domains are known to be affected by AD and FTD
[16,
20–22] .
The aim of this study was to establish which of the following variables contribute to carer
burden in the early stages of dementia: (i) dementia diagnosis, (ii) patient demographics (sex,
age, disease duration), (iii) carer demographics (age, sex, relationship to patient) and (iv)
patient’s cognitive deficits.
Methods
Participant s
Participants were recruited from FRONTIER, the FTD clinical research group in Sydney, Australia. The
study sample included 35 patients with suspected underlying Alzheimer pathology (AD = 23; logopenic
progressive aphasia = 12) and 61 patients with frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) (bvFTD = 26,
semantic dementia = 16, progressive nonfluent aphasia = 8, corticobasal syndrome = 11). All participants
were examined by the same senior neurologist (J.R.H.) and clinical diagnosis was established based on
clinical interview, comprehensive neuropsychological assessment and presence of brain atrophy on struc-
tural MR according to established clinical diagnostic criteria
[23, 24] . The bvFTD patients considered nonpro-
gressive, or ‘phenocopy’ cases
[16] were excluded. All participants were living at home and carer information
was obtained within 6 months of the clinical and cognitive investigations. The majority of carers were spouses
of the patients (82%). Disease duration was determined on the basis of the carer’s report of initial symptoms
as recorded in the clinical interview at baseline.
M e a s u r e s
Carer Burden
All carers completed the abridged Zarit Burden Interview
[25, 26] , a 12-item questionnaire investi-
gating different aspects of carer burden, such as whether the carers feel that their own health has suffered
DEM347146.indd 2DEM347146 indd 2 15.02.2013 14:47:2915 02 2013 14 47 29
3
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
DOI: 10.1159/000347146
Miller et al.: Identifying Cognitive and Demographic Variables That Contribute to Carer
Burden in Dementia
www.karger.com/dem
© 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel
or whether they feel stressed or strained. Scores range from 0 to 48, with higher scores reflecting more
perceived burden. A cut-off score of 17 is generally taken as an indication of a significantly high burden.
C o g n i t i o n
On all of the following measures, higher scores indicated better performance.
General Cognition. Global cognitive performance was estimated using the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive
Examination – revised (ACE-R)
[27] , a dementia screening tool.
Memory. Integrity of anterograde memory was established using the Doors A subtest from the Doors
and People memory test
[28] . Memory for 12 photographs of doors is tested immediately after presentation
using a 4-choice recognition format. The number correct was converted to an age-scaled score.
Word Generation and Impulse Control. Measures of word generation and impulse control were taken
from the Hayling test
[29] . In this test, participants are first asked to finish 15 incomplete sentences by saying
a single word that fits (as quickly as possible). Mean time to complete these sentences is converted to an age-
adjusted score reflecting word generation ability (Hayling score A). In the second part of the test, participants
are again asked to complete 15 new, incomplete sentences, but this time with a word that is unrelated to the
sentence. They have to suppress the obvious response. The number of related (incorrect) words generated
under these conditions is converted to an age-adjusted score and reflects impulse control integrity (Hayling
score C). Given the nature of the tasks, some patients with severe language problems were unable to complete
the Hayling test.
Emotion Recognition. Facial emotion recognition was measured using the Facial Affect Selection test
(FAST)
[30] . In this test, participants view 42 arrays of faces showing seven alternative emotional expres-
sions and they are asked to choose the face that corresponds with a spoken emotion label (e.g. surprised,
sad).
D a t a A n a l y s e s
All analyses were performed using SPSS19 (IBM, Chicago, Ill, USA). The AD and FTLD groups were first
compared using χ
2 tests of independence for categorical variables, and independent t tests or Mann-Whitney
tests for continuous variables depending on score distribution. Next, linear regression analyses were carried
out to identify the best predictor variables of carer burden among the diagnostic, demographic and cognitive
variables. In the first instance, univariate regression analyses were conducted to identify the contribution of
each predictor variable to burden of care, independently from all the other predictors. Secondly, predictors
that reached statistical significance in the univariate analyses were entered into a multiple linear regression
model using a stepwise method in order to examine their concurrent effect in predicting carer burden. This
model was refined by discarding any variable found to become non-significant using an increment test, until
all variables were found to contribute significantly to the model.
Results
Demographic characteristics for the patients and carers in the two diagnostic groups are
shown in table 1 . The groups were well matched for age, sex distribution and disease duration.
On the cognitive measures, groups differed only on the Hayling C score, with the FTLD group
making more impulse control errors. Burden of care as measured by the Zarit Burden
Interview was also significantly higher in FTLD compared to AD carers.
Within the set of demographic and cognitive variables investigated, sex, relationship
(spouse vs other), disease duration, word generation ability and ACE-R score did not reach
significance as predictors of carer burden. However, six variables were found to make contri-
butions to the Zarit Burden Interview score ( table 2 ); three were demographic (patient age,
carer age, diagnosis) and three were cognitive (emotion recognition, memory, impulse
control). More specifically, younger age, a diagnosis of FTLD and lower scores on the cognitive
tasks were predictive of higher carer burden.
Multiple regression analyses combining these six variables yielded a significant model
(F(3, 92) = 8.94, p <.0001) containing three predictor variables, which explained 23% of the
DEM347146.indd 3DEM347146 indd 3 15.02.2013 14:47:3015 02 2013 14 47 30
4
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
DOI: 10.1159/000347146
Miller et al.: Identifying Cognitive and Demographic Variables That Contribute to Carer
Burden in Dementia
www.karger.com/dem
© 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel
score variance on the Zarit Burden Interview ( table 2 ). All these variables (patient age,
emotion recognition and memory) independently contributed significantly to the final model.
None of the remaining variables identified in the bivariate analyses were found to contribute
significantly to the model. Again the regression analyses showed that younger patient age and
lower scores on the emotion and memory tests were associated with higher burden of care
( table 2 ).
Table 1. Clinical characteristics for the AD and FTLD patients and their carers (mean ± SD)
AD
(n = 35)
FTLD
(n = 61)
p value
Patient
Male, % 63 71 NS
Age 66.1 ± 8.5 63.1 ± 8.8 NS
Disease duration, years 3.6 ± 2.9 4.1 ± 2.4 NS
ACE-R (max 100) 67.6 ± 17.6 68.2 ± 16.4 NS
Doors A (max 12) 7.3 ± 3.3 6.8 ± 2.6 NS
FAST (max 42) 30.8 ± 5.9 29.0 ± 7.2 NS
Hayling A (max 7)a3.5 ± 1.7 3.2 ± 1.9 NS
Hayling C (max 8)b4.6 ± 2.4 3.3 ± 2.4 0.001
Carer
Male, % 26 26 NS
Spouse, % 83 82 NS
Age 61.9 ± 10.7 59.4 ± 11.4 NS
ZBI (max 48) 14.7±9.4 19.6 ± 10.4 0.024
FAST = Facial Affect Selection test; ZBI = Zarit Burden Interview.
a Data available for 23 AD and 38 FTLD patients.
b Data available for 21 AD and 35 FTLD patients.
Table 2. Univariate and multivariate regression analyses on demographic and cognitive variables for
prediction of the Zarit Burden Interview score
Variable B SE B β p
Univariate analyses
Patient age –0.368 0.114 –0.315 0.002
Diagnosis 5.155 2.128 0.242 0.017
Carer age –0.234 0.097 –0.244 0.017
FAST –0.416 0.15 –0.275 0.007
Doors and People A –1.072 0.352 –0.300 0.003
Hayling C –1.179 0.527 –0.291 0.030
Final multivariate model
Patient age –0.368 0.109 –0.315 0.001
FAST –0.372 0.146 –0.246 0.012
Doors and People A –0.701 0.346 –0.196 0.046
FAST = Facial Affect Selection test.
DEM347146.indd 4DEM347146 indd 4 15.02.2013 14:47:3015 02 2013 14 47 30
5
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
DOI: 10.1159/000347146
Miller et al.: Identifying Cognitive and Demographic Variables That Contribute to Carer
Burden in Dementia
www.karger.com/dem
© 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel
Discussion
Several new variables that contribute to carer burden in the early stages of dementia
were identified. Our aim was to consider variables that were independent of carer report (e.g.
of the patient’s behaviours or of their own mood). We found that patient age, anterograde
memory performance and emotion recognition ability all made independent contributions to
the prediction of carer burden. Diagnosis (FTLD), carer age (younger) and impulse control
(lower score) were also predictive of carer burden, but only in the univariate analyses; that
is, when considered in isolation and independently from the other predictor variables of
interest. Once examined concurrently with other predictors, contributions of these variables
to the final model were no longer significant. These findings are consistent with recent work,
which has demonstrated considerable overlap in performance on anterograde memory
[21]
and emotion processing tasks
[30] between AD and FTLD patient groups.
Like previous studies
[10, 11] , we found that patient performance on a general measure
of cognition (ACE-R) was not related to carer burden. However, deterioration in particular
cognitive domains (i.e. anterograde memory and emotion processing skills) was associated
with increased carer burden. These specific impacts might be overlooked when multiple
aspects of cognition are combined in more general cognitive screens.
Reduced impulse control, as measured by the Hayling test, failed to contribute to the final
multivariate model. Of note, in our study the Hayling test was not administered to patients
with significant language disturbance. It is therefore possible that the contribution of this task
to the final model might have been detected with a larger sample. Importantly, however,
while increased burden has been reported in carers of patients with behavioural disinhibition
[6, 7, 31] , other studies have shown that a carer’s reaction to problem behaviours and coping
style are more important predictors of carer burden than the actual frequency of impulsive
or disinhibited behaviours
[8, 13, 32] .
Level of carer burden was not influenced by type of relationship between the patient and
the carer, a finding consistent with previous studies
[11, 12] . Similar to most other reports, it
is worth noting that the vast majority of carers were spouses.
In this study, almost a quarter of the variance in carer burden score was explained by a
model involving cognitive and demographic variables. It is noteworthy that in a study by
Mioshi et al.
[3] , a similar approach incorporating the carer’s self-report of depression and
their number of social contacts predicted 54% of the variance in carer burden. Others have
found that burden is associated with carer’s report of behavioural symptoms and reduced
activities of daily living skills
[6, 7, 10, 12, 31] . Together, these studies indicate that carer
depression, paucity of social contacts, younger patient age and specific changes in the patient’s
behaviour and cognitive abilities (especially memory decline and loss of ability to recognise
emotional expressions) contribute to carer burden. It is likely that some of these factors are
related. For example, a lack of social contact may be particularly difficult for younger carers,
who may have reduced their employment and recreational activities to care for a person with
dementia. This requires further exploration so that such important information could help to
direct interventions.
The provision of structured group-based education programs to the carers of dementia
patients is modestly successful in reducing stress and burden
[33–35] . In contrast, behav-
ioural management strategies for the patients themselves have thus far proven less effective
(especially in FTLD)
[36] . Our results indicate that interventions to address problems in
anterograde memory and emotion recognition have the potential to be particularly helpful in
reducing carer burden. To help carers cope with patient deficits in anterograde memory, we
suggest emphasizing the use of external memory aids and routine schedules. To cope with
failures in emotion recognition, which potentially lead to confusion and inappropriate or
DEM347146.indd 5DEM347146 indd 5 15.02.2013 14:47:3015 02 2013 14 47 30
6
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
DOI: 10.1159/000347146
Miller et al.: Identifying Cognitive and Demographic Variables That Contribute to Carer
Burden in Dementia
www.karger.com/dem
© 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel
apparently apathetic responses in patients, it may be helpful for carers to view emotion
recognition impairments as a consequence of dementia and be taught strategies (e.g. verbal-
ization of feelings). Given the fact that carer burden was highest for younger patients, the
provision of appropriate day care facilities for this population is particularly important for
allowing spouses to continue to work, maintain social networks and reduce carer burden.
Acknowledgements
This project was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia
Project Grant (510106). E.M. is supported by an Early Career NHMRC Fellowship (1016399). J.R.H. is
supported by an ARC Federation Fellowship (FF0776229). O.P. is supported by an NHMRC Clinical Career
Development Fellowship (APP1022884). We thank Dr. Ilana Hepner for helping to organise the database and
perform analyses.
References
 1 Mioshi E, Foxe D, Leslie F, Savage S, Hsieh S, Miller L, Hodges J, Piguet O: The impact of dementia severity on
caregiver burden in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord, E-pub
ahead of print.
 2 Bookwala J, Schulz R: A comparison of primary stressors, secondary stressors, and depressive symptoms
between elderly caregiving husbands and wives: the Caregiver Health Effects Study. Psychol Aging 2000;
15:
607–616.
 3 Mioshi E, Bristow M, Cook R, Hodges JR: Factors underlying caregiver stress in frontotemporal dementia and
Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;
27: 76–81.
 4 Lautenschlager NT, Kurz AF, Loi S, Cramer B: Personality of mental health caregivers. Curr Opin Psychiatry
2013;
26: 97–101.
 5 Kaufer DI, Cummings JL, Christine D, Bray T, Castellon S, Masterman D, MacMillan A, Ketchel P, DeKosky ST:
Assessing the impact of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: the Neuropsychiatric Inventory
Caregiver Distress Scale. J Am Geriatr Soc 1998;
46: 210–215.
 6 de Vugt ME, Riedijk SR, Aalten P, Tibben A, van Swieten JC, Verhey FR: Impact of behavioural problems on
spousal caregivers: a comparison between Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr
Cogn Disord 2006;
22: 35–41.
 7 Mourik JC, Rosso SM, Niermeijer MF, Duivenvoorden JJ, Van Swieten JC, Tibben A: Frontotemporal dementia:
behavioral symptoms and caregiver distress. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2004;
18: 299–306.
 8 Robinson K, Adkisson P, Weinrich S: Problem behaviour, caregiver reactions, and impact among caregivers of
persons with Alzheimer’s disease. J Adv Nurs 2001;
36: 573–582.
 9 Torti FM, Jr., Gwyther LP, Reed SD, Friedman JY, Schulman KA: A multinational review of recent trends and
reports in dementia caregiver burden. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2004;
18: 99–109.
10 Coen RF, Swanwick GR, O’Boyle CA, Coakley D: Behaviour disturbance and other predictors of carer burden
in Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1997;
12: 331–336.
11 Nagatomo I, Akasaki Y, Uchida M, Tominaga M, Hashiguchi W, Takigawa M: Gender of demented patients and
specific family relationship of caregiver to patients influence mental fatigue and burdens on relatives as care-
givers. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;
14: 618–625.
12 Gonzalez-Salvador MT, Arango C, Lyketsos CG, Barba AC: The stress and psychological morbidity of the
Alzheimer patient caregiver. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;
14: 701–710.
13 Papastavrou E, Kalokerinou A, Papacostas SS, Tsangari H, Sourtzi P: Caring for a relative with dementia: family
caregiver burden. J Adv Nurs 2007;
58: 446–457.
14 Boutoleau-Bretonniere C, Vercelletoo M, Volteau C, Renou P, Lamy E: Zarit burden inventory and activities of
daily living in the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2008;
25: 272–
277.
15 Riedijk SR, De Vugt MD, Duivenvoorden HJ, Niermeijer MF, Van Swieten JC, Verhey FR, Tibben A: Caregiver
burden, health-related quality of life and coping in dementia care-givers: a comparison of frontotemporal
dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;
22: 405–412.
16 Piguet O, Hornberger M, Mioshi E, Hodges JR: Behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia: diagnosis,
clinical staging, and management. Lancet Neurol 2011;
10: 162–172.
17 Miller BL, Ikonte C, Ponton M, Levy M, Boone K, Darby A, et al: A study of the Lund-Manchester research
criteria for frontotemporal dementia: clinical and single-photon emission CT correlations. Neurology 1997;
48: 937–942.
DEM347146.indd 6DEM347146 indd 6 15.02.2013 14:47:3115 02 2013 14 47 31
7
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
DOI: 10.1159/000347146
Miller et al.: Identifying Cognitive and Demographic Variables That Contribute to Carer
Burden in Dementia
www.karger.com/dem
© 2013 S. Karger AG, Basel
18 Gregory CA, Hodges JR: Clinical features of frontal lobe dementia in comparison to Alzheimer’s disease.
J Neural Transm Suppl 1996;
47: 103–123.
19 Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, Passant U, Stuss D, Black S, Freedman M, Kertesz A, Robert PH, Albert M, et
al: Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 1998;
51: 1546–
1554.
20 Kumfor F, Miller L, Lah S, Hsieh S, Foxe D, Leslie F, et al: Emotion Recognition in Subtypes of Frontotemporal
Dementia: Effects of Salience. Fremantle, College of Clinical Neuropsychologists, 2010.
21 Hornberger M, Piguet O, Graham AJ, Nestor PJ, Hodges JR: How preserved is episodic memory in behavioral
variant frontotemporal dementia? Neurology 2010;
74: 472–479.
22 Ricci M, Graef S, Blundo C, Miller LA: Using the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning test (RAVLT) to differentiate
Alzheimer’s dementia and behavioural variant fronto-temporal dementia. Clin Neuropsychol 2012;
26: 926–
941.
23 Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D, Mendez MF, Kramer JH, Neuhaus J, van Swieten JC, Seelaar H, Dopper EG,
Onyike CU, et al: Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal
dementia. Brain 2011;
134: 2456–2477.
24 Gorno-Tempini ML, Brambati SM, Ginex V, Ogar J, Dronkers NF, Marcone A, Perani D, Garibotto V, Cappa SF,
Miller BL: The logopenic/phonological variant of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology 2008;
71: 1227–
1234.
25 Zarit SH, Anthony CR, Bourselis M: Interventions with care givers of dementia patients: comparison of two
approaches. Psychol Aging 1987;
7: 225–232.
26 O’Rourke N, Tuokko HA: Psychometric properties of an abridged version of the Zarit Burden Interview within
a representative Canadian caregiver sample. Gerontologist 2003;
43: 121–127.
27 Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell J, Arnold R, Hodges JR: The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Revised
(ACE-R): a brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006;
21: 1078–1085.
28 Baddeley A, Emslie H, Nimmo-Smith I: Doors and People. Bury St Edmunds, Thames Valley Test Company,
1994.
29 Burgess PW, Shallice T: The Hayling and Brixton Tests. Bury St Edmunds, Thames Valley Test Company, 1997.
30 Miller LA, Hsieh S, Lah S, Savage S, Hodges JR, Piguet O: One size does not fit all: face emotion processing
impairments in semantic dementia, behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease
are mediated by distinct cognitive deficits. Behav Neurol 2012;
25: 53–60.
31 Allegri RF, Sarasola D, Serrano CM, Taragano FE, Arizaga RL, Butman J, Lon L: Neuropsychiatric symptoms as
a predictor of caregiver burden in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2006;
2: 105–110.
32 Matsuda O: The effect of coping on the caregiver of elderly patients with dementia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
1995;
49: 209–212.
33 Diehl J, Mayer T, Forstl H, Kurz AF: A support group for caregivers of patients with frontotemporal dementia.
Dementia 2003;
2: 151–161.
34 Hebert R, Levesque L, Vezina J, Lavoie J-P, Ducharme F, Gendron C, Preville M, Voyer L, Dubois M-F: Efficacy
of a psychoeducative group program for caregivers of demented persons living at home: a randomized
controlled trial. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2003;
58B:S58–S67.
35 Lough S, Garfoot V: Psychological interventions in frontotemporal dementia; in Hodges JR (ed): Frontotem-
poral Dementia Syndromes. New York, Cambridge University Press, 2007, pp 277–325.
36 LoGiudice D, Hassett A: Uncommon dementia and the carer’s perspective. Int Psychogeriatr 2005;
17(suppl
1):S223–S231.
DEM347146.indd 7DEM347146 indd 7 15.02.2013 14:47:3115 02 2013 14 47 31
... Poor FER ability has been shown to correlate with behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) (Gregory et al, 2002;Ibáñez and Manes, 2012;Kempnich et al, 2018;Polet et al, 2022;Santamaría-García et al, 2016;Shimokawa et al, 2001;Torralva et al, 2009), thereby impairing social relationships (Brown et al, 2018;Miller et al, 2013;Narme et al, 2017;Polet et al, 2021Polet et al, , 2022. More specifically, apathy, euphoria, disinhibition, and aberrant motor behavior have been negatively correlated with FER ability (Kempnich et al, 2018;Polet et al, 2022). ...
... More specifically, apathy, euphoria, disinhibition, and aberrant motor behavior have been negatively correlated with FER ability (Kempnich et al, 2018;Polet et al, 2022). For caregivers whose care recipients are unable to recognize emotions and generate appropriate emotional responses, there can be significant social disconnection, which may result in social isolation and caregiver burden (Brown et al, 2018;Cheng, 2017;Miller et al, 2013;Narme et al, 2017;Polet et al, 2021Polet et al, , 2022. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Facial emotion recognition (FER) is commonly impaired in individuals with neurodegenerative disease (NDD). This impairment has been linked to an increase in behavioral disorders and caregiver burden. Objective: To identify interventions targeting the improvement of FER ability in individuals with NDD and investigate the magnitude of the efficacy of the interventions. We also wanted to explore the duration of the effects of the intervention and their possible impacts on behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and caregiver burden. Method: We included 15 studies with 604 individuals who had been diagnosed with NDD. The identified interventions were categorized into three types of approach (cognitive, neurostimulation, and pharmacological) as well as a combined approach (neurostimulation with pharmacological). Results: The three types of approaches pooled together had a significant large effect size for FER ability improvement (standard mean difference: 1.21, 95% CI = 0.11, 2.31, z = 2.15, P = 0.03). The improvement lasted post intervention, in tandem with a decrease in behavioral disorders and caregiver burden. Conclusion: A combination of different approaches for FER ability improvement may be beneficial for individuals with NDD and their caregivers.
... It is increasingly recognised that people with behaviouralvariant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) are severely impaired on tasks of facial emotion recognition, particularly for negative emotions (see for meta analysis Bora, Velakoulis, & Walterfang, 2016;Boeve, Boxer, Kumfor, Pijnenburg, & Rohrer, 2022;Chiu et al., 2018;Goodkind et al., 2015;Kumfor et al., 2011;Piguet & Kumfor, 2020;Salamone et al., 2021). These emotional deficits contribute substantially to carer burden and distress (Miller et al., 2013). Clinically, bvFTD is characterised by profound changes to behaviour and personality, with early brain atrophy observed in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and insula (Rascovsky et al., 2011), extending to the temporal lobes and subcortical regions with disease progression (Landin-Romero et al., 2017). ...
Article
Disease-specific mechanisms underlying emotion recognition difficulties in behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD), Alzheimer's disease (AD), and Parkinson's disease (PD) are unknown. Interoceptive accuracy, accurately detecting internal cues (e.g., one's heart beating), and cognitive abilities are candidate mechanisms underlying emotion recognition. One hundred and sixty-eight participants (52 bvFTD; 41 AD; 24 PD; 51 controls) were recruited. Emotion recognition was measured via the Facial Affect Selection Task or the Mini-Social and Emotional Assessment Emotion Recognition Task. Interoception was assessed with a heartbeat detection task. Participants pressed a button each time they: 1) felt their heartbeat (Interoception); or 2) heard a recorded heartbeat (Exteroception-control). Cognition was measured via the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination-III or the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. Voxel-based morphometry analyses identified neural correlates associated with emotion recognition and interoceptive accuracy. All patient groups showed worse emotion recognition and cognition than controls (all P's ≤ .008). Only the bvFTD showed worse interoceptive accuracy than controls (P < .001). Regression analyses revealed that in bvFTD worse interoceptive accuracy predicted worse emotion recognition (P = .008). Whereas worse cognition predicted worse emotion recognition overall (P < .001). Neuroimaging analyses revealed that the insula, orbitofrontal cortex, and amygdala were involved in emotion recognition and interoceptive accuracy in bvFTD. Here, we provide evidence for disease-specific mechanisms for emotion recognition difficulties. In bvFTD, emotion recognition impairment is driven by inaccurate perception of the internal milieu. Whereas, in AD and PD, cognitive impairment likely underlies emotion recognition deficits. The current study furthers our theoretical understanding of emotion and highlights the need for targeted interventions.
... We similarly found that carer burden significantly differed between the dementia subtypes is controlling for total neurocognitive scores, a marker of dementia severity. However, these findings should be interpreted with caution given the small sample size and the mixed findings in the literature as to whether cognition influences carer burden (Kang et al., 2022;Kimura et al., 2021;Miller et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: Carer burden is common in younger-onset dementia (YOD), often due to the difficulty of navigating services often designed for older people with dementia. Compared to Alzheimer's disease (AD), the burden is reported to be higher in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD). However, there is little literature comparing carer burden specifically in YOD. This study hypothesized that carer burden in bvFTD would be higher than in AD. Design: Retrospective cross-sectional study. Setting: Tertiary neuropsychiatry service in Victoria, Australia. Participants: Patient-carer dyads with YOD. Measurements: We collected patient data, including behaviors using the Cambridge Behavioral Inventory-Revised (CBI-R). Carer burden was rated using the Zarit Burden Inventory-short version (ZBI-12). Descriptive statistics and Mann-Whitney U tests were used to analyze the data. Results: Carers reported high burden (ZBI-12 mean score = 17.2, SD = 10.5), with no significant difference in burden between younger-onset AD and bvFTD. CBI-R stereotypic and motor behaviors, CBI-R everyday skills, and total NUCOG scores differed between the two groups. There was no significant difference in the rest of the CBI-R subcategories, including the behavior-related domains. Conclusion: Carers of YOD face high burden and are managing significant challenging behaviors. We found no difference in carer burden between younger-onset AD and bvFTD. This could be due to similarities in the two subtypes in terms of abnormal behavior, motivation, and self-care as measured on CBI-R, contrary to previous literature. Clinicians should screen for carer burden and associated factors including behavioral symptoms in YOD syndromes, as they may contribute to carer burden regardless of the type.
... However, carer-reported worsening in the neuropsychiatric symptoms of PWD and faster disease progression over the first months of lockdown were significantly associated with higher burden and distress scores. Although we cannot exclude that carers' mental health status might have influenced subjective perception of burden and distress (46), it must be noted that very similar findings emerged from other investigations into the consequences of measures of social restrictions enforcement due to the COVID-19 pandemic (13,17,18). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background People with dementia (PWD) are vulnerable to abrupt changes to daily routines. The lockdown enforced on the 23rd of March 2020 in the UK to contain the expansion of the COVID-19 pandemic limited opportunities for PWD to access healthcare services and socialise. The SOLITUDE study explored the potential long-term effects of lockdown on PWD’s symptoms and carers’ burden. Methods Forty-five carers and 36 PWD completed a telephone-based assessment at recruitment (T0) and after 3 (T1) and 6 months (T2). PWD completed measures validated for telephonic evaluations of cognition and depression. Carers completed questionnaires on their burden and on PWD’s health and answered a customised interview on symptom changes observed in the initial months of lockdown. Longitudinal changes were investigated for all outcome variables with repeated-measures models. Additional post hoc multiple regression analyses were carried out to investigate whether several objective factors (i.e., demographics and time under social restrictions) and carer-reported symptom changes observed following lockdown before T0 were associated with all outcomes at T0. Results No significant changes were observed in any outcomes over the 6 months of observations. However, post hoc analyses showed that the length of social isolation before T0 was negatively correlated with episodic and semantic memory performance at T0. Carers reporting worsening of neuropsychiatric symptoms and faster disease progression in PWD also reported higher burden. Moreover, carer-reported worsening of cognitive symptoms was associated with poorer semantic memory at T0. Conclusion PWD’s symptoms and carers’ burden remained stable over 6 months of observation. However, the amount of time spent under social restrictions before T0 appears to have had a significant detrimental impact on cognitive performance of patients. In fact, carer-reported cognitive decline during social isolation was consistent with the finding of poorer semantic memory, a domain sensitive to progression in Alzheimer’s disease. Therefore, the initial stricter period of social isolation had greater detrimental impact on patients and their carers, followed then by a plateau. Future interventions may be designed to maintain an optimal level of social and cognitive engagement for PWD in challenging times, to prevent abrupt worsening of symptoms and associated detrimental consequences on patients’ carers.
... The study by Park et al. showed deficits in the recognition of emotion of negative valence; however, this phenomenon was not replicated for positive emotions. In contrast, the remaining two studies found no difference between the groups compared (Goodkind et al., 2012;Miller et al., 2013). Furthermore, a longitudinal study conducted over 6 years reported that patients' performance in emotional processing changed over time. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Neurodegenerative diseases might affect social cognition in various ways depending on their components (theory of mind, emotional processing, attribution bias, and social perception) and the subtype of dementia they cause. This review aims to explore this difference in cognitive function among individuals with different aetiologies of dementia. Methods:The following databases were explored: MEDLINE via PubMed, Cochrane Library, Lilacs, Web of Science, and PsycINFO. We selected studies examining social cognition in individuals with neurodegenerative diseases in which dementia was the primary symptom that was studied. The neurodegenerative diseases included Alzheimer's disease, Lewy body disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration. The search yielded 2,803 articles. Results: One hundred twenty-two articles were included in the present review. The summarised results indicate that people with neurodegenerative diseases indeed have deficits in social cognitive performance. Both in populations with Alzheimer's disease and in populations with frontotemporal dementia, we found that emotional processing was strongly affected. However, although theory of mind impairment could also be observed in the initial stages of frontotemporal dementia, in Alzheimer's disease it was only appreciated when performing highly complex task or in advanced stages of the disease. Conclusions: Each type of dementia has a differential profile of social cognition deterioration. This review could provide a useful reference for clinicians to improve detection and diagnosis, which would undoubtedly guarantee better interventions. Systematic Review Registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42020152562, PROSPERO, identifier: CRD42020152562.
... Our study offers important insights into the role of localised vascular white matter lesion and cortical atrophy on empathy. Given that changes in empathy are associated with caregiver distress, burden, and depression [17,120,121], further study into prevention and treatment of modifiable vascular risk factors that can lead to SVD should be undertaken. data curation, project administration, writing (review and editing). ...
Article
Change in empathy is an increasingly recognised symptom of neurodegenerative diseases and contributes to caregiver burden and patient distress. Empathy impairment has been associated with brain atrophy but its relationship to white matter hyperintensities (WMH) is unknown. We aimed to investigate the relationships amongst WMH, brain atrophy, and empathy deficits in neurodegenerative and cerebrovascular diseases. Five hundred thirteen participants with Alzheimer's disease/mild cognitive impairment, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, frontotemporal dementia (FTD), Parkinson's disease, or cerebrovascular disease (CVD) were included. Empathy was assessed using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. WMH were measured using a semi-automatic segmentation and FreeSurfer was used to measure cortical thickness. A heterogeneous pattern of cortical thinning was found between groups, with FTD showing thinning in frontotemporal regions and CVD in left superior parietal, left insula, and left postcentral. Results from both univariate and multivariate analyses revealed that several variables were associated with empathy, particularly cortical thickness in the fronto-insulo-temporal and cingulate regions, sex (female), global cognition, and right parietal and occipital WMH. Our results suggest that cortical atrophy and WMH may be associated with empathy deficits in neurodegenerative and cerebrovascular diseases. Future work should consider investigating the longitudinal effects of WMH and atrophy on empathy deficits in neurodegenerative and cerebrovascular diseases.
... Given the high rates of behavioral features and other factors, caregiver burden tends to be higher in FTLD than in other neurodegenerative diseases. 3,6,12,14,16,25 Studies have directly associated this caregiver burden with behavioral features in bvFTD, 3,5 svPPA, 7 and CBS. 12,26 In patients with ALS and FTD-ALS, behavioral features are a stronger predictor of caregiver burden than motor impairment. ...
Article
Introduction: Caregivers of patients with frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) spectrum disorders experience tremendous burden, which has been associated with the neuropsychiatric and behavioral features of the disorders. Methods: In a sample of 558 participants with FTLD spectrum disorders, we performed multiple-variable regressions to identify the behavioral features that were most strongly associated with caregiver burden, as measured by the Zarit Burden Interview, at each stage of disease. Results: Apathy and disinhibition, as rated by both clinicians and caregivers, as well as clinician-rated psychosis, showed the strongest associations with caregiver burden, a pattern that was consistent when participants were separated cross-sectionally by disease stage. In addition, behavioral features appeared to contribute most to caregiver burden in patients with early dementia. Discussion: Caregivers should be provided with early education on the management of the behavioral features of FTLD spectrum disorders. Interventions targeting apathy, disinhibition, and psychosis may be most useful to reduce caregiver burden.
... Although it was not consistently demonstrated throughout the literature, 6,10,36 some authors also suggested that the observed impairment of emotion processing tasks in AD is not necessarily related to impaired emotional decoding abilities, but may, in fact, occur secondary to generalized global cognitive decline. 1,13,34,37,38 However, other authors did not obtain the same results and concluded that people with AD may present impaired emotional recognition per se. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotional processing involves the ability of the individual to infer emotional information. There is no consensus about how Alzheimer's disease (AD) affects emotional processing. Objective: Our aim is to systematically review the impact of AD on emotion processing. Methods: We conducted a search based on the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA). The literature search was performed using the electronic databases MEDLINE (PubMed) and Science Citation Index (Institute for Scientific Information [ISI]). The following descriptors were used in the review process: emotion or emotional processing, cognition or cognitive functions, and Alzheimer disease or Alzheimer's disease. This systematic review was recorded in the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) under the number CRD42018115891. Results: We identified 425 articles, 19 of which met our criteria. Visual emotional stimuli were the most used among studies. Most studies used tasks of emotional naming, discrimination, identification, and correspondence. The results were contradictory. Many studies reported that individuals with AD were impaired on emotional perception tasks, while other results reported preserved skills. The relationship between emotional processing and cognition is also unclear. Some studies suggested that general cognitive performance affects performance in emotional perception tasks among people with AD, but other studies have shown deficits in recognizing emotion, regardless of cognitive performance. Conclusions: Studies are scarce, present contradictory results, and report impairment in emotional processing in relation to cognition. Moreover, the analyses of the correlation between emotion processing and cognitive functioning failed to reveal clear relationships.
Article
Objectives: Caregiving burdens are a substantial concern in the clinical care of persons with neurodegenerative disorders. In the Ontario Neurodegenerative Disease Research Initiative, we used the Zarit's Burden Interview (ZBI) to examine: (1) the types of burdens captured by the ZBI in a cross-disorder sample of neurodegenerative conditions (2) whether there are categorical or disorder-specific effects on caregiving burdens, and (3) which demographic, clinical, and cognitive measures are related to burden(s) in neurodegenerative disorders? Methods/design: N = 504 participants and their study partners (e.g., family, friends) across: Alzheimer's disease/mild cognitive impairment (AD/MCI; n = 120), Parkinson's disease (PD; n = 136), amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS; n = 38), frontotemporal dementia (FTD; n = 53), and cerebrovascular disease (CVD; n = 157). Study partners provided information about themselves, and information about the clinical participants (e.g., activities of daily living (ADL)). We used Correspondence Analysis to identify types of caregiving concerns in the ZBI. We then identified relationships between those concerns and demographic and clinical measures, and a cognitive battery. Results: We found three components in the ZBI. The first was "overall burden" and was (1) strongly related to increased neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPI severity r = 0.586, NPI distress r = 0.587) and decreased independence in ADL (instrumental ADLs r = -0.566, basic ADLs r = -0.43), (2) moderately related to cognition (MoCA r = -0.268), and (3) showed little-to-no differences between disorders. The second and third components together showed four types of caregiving concerns: current care of the person with the neurodegenerative disease, future care of the person with the neurodegenerative disease, personal concerns of study partners, and social concerns of study partners. Conclusions: Our results suggest that the experience of caregiving in neurodegenerative and cerebrovascular diseases is individualized and is not defined by diagnostic categories. Our findings highlight the importance of targeting ADL and neuropsychiatric symptoms with caregiver-personalized solutions.
Article
Aim: The aim of the study was to evaluate the usability of the COPE Index in the assessment of subjective caregiving burden of family caregivers providing care for older people in their home environment. Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted among 110 family caregivers. The criterion of including caregivers in the study was their provision of care at home for an older person who needed and received regular long-term nursing care in their home environment. The study was carried out using the COPE Index questionnaire in assessing the situation of family caregivers. The study was reported according to the STROBE checklist. Results: The mean result of assessment of negative impact of care in the studied group of caregivers was 16.5 ± SD 3.0, positive impact of care - 10.4 ± SD 2.8 and the quality of support - 9.2 ± SD 1.8. A higher level of caregiving burden was found in 58.2% caregivers. The vast majority of the caregivers were women (83.6%). The care was most often provided by the elderly people's children (51.0%), less often by spouses (17.0%), siblings (10.0%), grandchildren (10.0%) and paid caregivers (10.0%). Conclusion: The COPE Index proved to be a useful instrument for routine application in home environment to identify care deficits. Greater caregiving burden of family caregivers of disabled older persons was associated with a higher number of nights devoted to care, growing limitations on professional life, choice to provide care, the caregiver's emotional state, the lack of support in the caregiving role, support from family members and the state of cognitive functions of the care recipient. Relevance to clinical practice: This study may help identify and characterise the profile of family caregivers who are at risk of caregiving burden, and can be used to apply well-designed activities aimed at reducing their suffering and providing them support.
Article
Full-text available
Patients with frontotemporal dementia (both behavioural variant [bvFTD] and semantic dementia [SD]) as well as those with Alzheimer's disease (AD) show deficits on tests of face emotion processing, yet the mechanisms underlying these deficits have rarely been explored. We compared groups of patients with bvFTD (n = 17), SD (n = 12) or AD (n = 20) to an age- and education-matched group of healthy control subjects (n = 36) on three face emotion processing tasks (Ekman 60, Emotion Matching and Emotion Selection) and found that all three patient groups were similarly impaired. Analyses of covariance employed to partial out the influences of language and perceptual impairments, which frequently co-occur in these patients, provided evidence of different underlying cognitive mechanisms. These analyses revealed that language impairments explained the original poor scores obtained by the SD patients on the Ekman 60 and Emotion Selection tasks, which involve verbal labels. Perceptual deficits contributed to Emotion Matching performance in the bvFTD and AD patients. Importantly, all groups remained impaired on one task or more following these analyses, denoting a primary emotion processing disturbance in these dementia syndromes. These findings highlight the multifactorial nature of emotion processing deficits in patients with dementia.
Article
Full-text available
In patients with focal lesions, patterns of learning, retrieval, and recognition deficits vary according to site of damage. Because different brain regions are affected by the underlying pathology in Alzheimer's dementia (AD) and behavioral variant fronto-temporal dementia (bvFTD), one might predict that the two disorders would result in different sorts of memory deficits on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT). The aim of this investigation was to find a way to differentiate AD, bvFTD, and normal controls (NC) reliably based on RAVLT scores from retrospective samples of 82 Italian and 43 Australian participants. Results indicated that the groups differed on measures of learning, retroactive interference, delayed recall, and delayed recognition. Although delayed recall distinguished participants in the three groups across both samples, no one set of cut-offs could be obtained with adequate sensitivity and specificity. However, when we created a combined score (the "RAVLT Memory Efficiency Index": {[(delayed recall A/15)/(RAVLT Trials 1-5/75)] + [(delayed recognition hits/15) - (false positive/total number of distractors)]}), we were able to find cut-offs that differentiated the groups with good sensitivity and specificity across variations in RAVLT methodology, participant samples, and languages. This index will increase the usefulness of the RAVLT in differential diagnoses of early dementia.
Chapter
Originally published in 2007, this book reflects the enormous advances in our understanding of frontotemporal dementia and related syndromes. The impetus for these advances has come from a number of directions including genetic discoveries, fresh approaches to neuroimaging and improved neuropsychological understanding of the cognitive aspects of the condition. This book provides a much needed review of the status of our knowledge of these syndromes. The book starts with chapters reviewing the history of the condition and describes the presenting clinical, neuropsychiatric and neuropsychological features, before reviewing, in detail, the areas of greatest recent research progress. The book concludes with a chapter proposing a multidisciplinary approach to patient management. Frontotemporal Dementia Syndromes will be essential reading for neurologists, psychologists, psychiatrists and other clinicians interested in cognitive and behavioural disorders, as well as to basic scientists working in the area of neurodegeneration.
Article
Caregiver burden is greater in frontotemporal dementia (FTD) than in Alzheimer disease (AD). However, little is known of the impact of the 3 main clinical variants of FTD- behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD), semantic dementia (SemDem), and progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA)-or the role of disease severity in caregiver burden. The Zarit Burden Inventory was used to measure caregiver burden of bvFTD (n=17), SemDem (n=20), PNFA (n=20), and AD (n=19) patients. Symptom duration, caregiver age, and relationship type were matched across groups. Moreover, a number of caregiver (mood, social network) and patient variables (functional disability, behavioral changes, relationship with caregiver, and dementia stage) were addressed to investigate their impact on caregiver burden. Caregivers of bvFTD patients reported the highest burden, whereas SemDem and PNFA caregivers reported burden similar to AD. A regression analysis revealed that caregiver burden in FTD, regardless of subtype, was explained by a model combining disease staging, relationship changes, and caregiver depression. Burden increased with disease severity in FTD. This study is the first to show that caregivers of SemDem, PNFA, and AD patients show similar burden, while confirming that bvFTD caregivers show higher burden than AD caregivers. More importantly, this study demonstrates that burden worsens with disease progression in FTD. (C) 2012 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Inc.
Article
Based on the recent literature and collective experience, an international consortium developed revised guidelines for the diagnosis of behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. The validation process retrospectively reviewed clinical records and compared the sensitivity of proposed and earlier criteria in a multi-site sample of patients with pathologically verified frontotemporal lobar degeneration. According to the revised criteria, 'possible' behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia requires three of six clinically discriminating features (disinhibition, apathy/inertia, loss of sympathy/empathy, perseverative/ compulsive behaviours, hyperorality and dysexecutive neuropsychological profile). 'Probable' behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia adds functional disability and characteristic neuroimaging, while behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia 'with definite frontotemporal lobar degeneration' requires histopathological confirmation or a pathogenic mutation. Sixteen brain banks contributed cases meeting histopathological criteria for frontotemporal lobar degeneration and a clinical diagnosis of behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer's disease, dementia with Lewy bodies or vascular dementia at presentation. Cases with predominant primary progressive aphasia or extra-pyramidal syndromes were excluded. In these autopsy-confirmed cases, an experienced neurologist or psychiatrist ascertained clinical features necessary for making a diagnosis according to previous and proposed criteria at presentation. Of 137 cases where features were available for both proposed and previously established criteria, 118 (86%) met 'possible' criteria, and 104 (76%) met criteria for 'probable' behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. In contrast, 72 cases (53%) met previously established criteria for the syndrome (P 5 0.001 for comparison with 'possible' and 'probable' criteria). Patients who failed to meet revised criteria were significantly older and most had atypical presentations with marked memory impairment. In conclusion, the revised criteria for behavioural variant fronto-temporal dementia improve diagnostic accuracy compared with previously established criteria in a sample with known fronto-temporal lobar degeneration. Greater sensitivity of the proposed criteria may reflect the optimized diagnostic features, less restrictive exclusion features and a flexible structure that accommodates different initial clinical presentations. Future studies will be needed to establish the reliability and specificity of these revised diagnostic guidelines.
Article
Patients with behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) present with insidious changes in personality and interpersonal conduct that indicate progressive disintegration of the neural circuits involved in social cognition, emotion regulation, motivation, and decision making. The underlying pathological changes are heterogeneous and are characterised by various intraneuronal inclusions. Biomarkers to detect these histopathological changes in life are becoming increasingly important with the development of disease-modifying drugs. Gene mutations have been found that collectively account for around 10–20% of cases. Recently, criteria proposed for bvFTD defi ne three levels of diagnostic certainty: possible, probable, and defi nite. Detailed history taking from family members to elicit behavioural features underpins the diagnostic process, with support from neuropsychological testing designed to detect impairment in decision making, emotion processing, and social cognition. Brain imaging is important for increasing the level of diagnostic certainty. A recently developed staging instrument shows much promise for monitoring patients and evaluating therapies, which at present are aimed at symptom amelioration. Carer education and support remain of paramount importance.
Article
Studies have shown variable memory performance in patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD). Our study investigated whether this variability is due to the admixture of patients with true bvFTD and phenocopy patients. We also sought to compare performance of patients with bvFTD and patients with Alzheimer disease (AD). We analyzed neuropsychological memory performance in patients with a clinical diagnosis of bvFTD divided into those who progressed (n = 50) and those who remained stable (n = 39), patients with AD (n = 64), and healthy controls (n = 64). Patients with progressive bvFTD were impaired on most memory tests to a similar level to that of patients with early AD. Findings from a subset of patients with progressive bvFTD with confirmed FTLD pathology (n = 10) corroborated these findings. By contrast, patients with phenocopy bvFTD performed significantly better than progressors and patients with AD. Logistic regression revealed that patients with bvFTD can be distinguished to a high degree (85%) on the immediate recall score of a word list learning test (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test). Our results provide evidence for an underlying memory deficit in "real" or progressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) similar to Alzheimer disease, though the groups differ in orientation scores, with patients with bvFTD being intact. Exclusion solely based on impaired neuropsychological memory performance can potentially lead to an underdiagnosis of FTD.
Article
There is a clear need for brief, but sensitive and specific, cognitive screening instruments as evidenced by the popularity of the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination (ACE). We aimed to validate an improved revision (the ACE-R) which incorporates five sub-domain scores (orientation/attention, memory, verbal fluency, language and visuo-spatial). Standard tests for evaluating dementia screening tests were applied. A total of 241 subjects participated in this study (Alzheimer's disease=67, frontotemporal dementia=55, dementia of Lewy Bodies=20; mild cognitive impairment-MCI=36; controls=63). Reliability of the ACE-R was very good (alpha coefficient=0.8). Correlation with the Clinical Dementia Scale was significant (r=-0.321, p<0.001). Two cut-offs were defined (88: sensitivity=0.94, specificity=0.89; 82: sensitivity=0.84, specificity=1.0). Likelihood ratios of dementia were generated for scores between 88 and 82: at a cut-off of 82 the likelihood of dementia is 100:1. A comparison of individual age and education matched groups of MCI, AD and controls placed the MCI group performance between controls and AD and revealed MCI patients to be impaired in areas other than memory (attention/orientation, verbal fluency and language). The ACE-R accomplishes standards of a valid dementia screening test, sensitive to early cognitive dysfunction.
Article
Several recent articles have pointed out that caregivers of patients with frontotemporal dementia (FTD) need counselling and support. To date, however, no support groups have been provided other than those available to caregivers of patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD). At our outpatient unit for cognitive disorders we initiated a specific support group for caregivers of patients with FTD. This pilot project had four objectives: 1) to provide information, advice and support to caregivers, 2) to learn more about the specific problems and needs of family carers of patients with FTD and to explore the differences to caregiver burden in AD, 3) to encourage mutual support and development of coping strategies, 4) to evaluate the intervention using a questionnaire completed by the caregiver. Eight spouse caregivers of patients diagnosed with frontotemporal dementia (FTD) participated in special support groups. Seven weekly sessions of 90 minutes' duration were held. To evaluate the program participants were asked to complete a questionnaire about their satisfaction with the support group immediately after the final session. Six months after the intervention they received a questionnaire by mail gathering information on coping efficacy. It became obvious that many problems faced by caregivers of patients with FTD are different from those encountered in AD. During group meetings participants were encouraged to express their own needs and to deal with painful emotions, including aggression, anger, mourning and guilt. Caregivers felt relieved by sharing their problems with others. They were able to learn from each other and to share coping strategies. The group also helped to establish new social relations contacts and even friendships. The participants rated the program as useful and said that benefits were sustained even six months after termination. We conclude from these initial observations that caregiver support groups are a useful component in the management of patients with FTD. Such groups should be tailored to the specific problems and needs of these caregivers. To maintain benefits, self-help groups are recommended even in the absence of professional input.
Article
Purpose of review: Caring for a family member with a chronic mental illness can be a major challenge with putting caregivers at risk of burden and depression. This review investigated the recent evidence on the role of personality traits and features for caregiver burden and depression in caregivers of care recipients with mental illness. Recent findings: Most of the evidence was found for caregivers looking after care recipients with dementia. Neuroticism was the personality trait showing the strongest association with caregiver burden and depression. Summary: Certain personality traits and features can increase the risk of caregiver burden and depression in caregivers looking after family members with a mental illness. More research is needed especially focusing on caregivers looking after care recipients with mental illnesses other than dementia as well as on interventions aiming to support vulnerable caregivers.