ArticlePDF Available

Survey results regarding use of the Boston Naming Test: Houston, we have a problem

Taylor & Francis
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology
Authors:

Abstract

Members of the National Academy of Neuropsychology were surveyed in 2005 to assess then current practices regarding Boston Naming Test (BNT) administration, interpretation, and reporting procedures. Nearly half of 445 respondents followed discontinuation rules that differed from instructions published with the test, and nearly 10% did not administer items in reverse order to achieve the required 8 consecutive item basal. Of further concern, between 40% and 55% of respondents indicated that they did not interpret BNT scores in light of linguistic and ethnic background, and over 25% reported that they did not consider educational level. Despite the fact that non-normal distribution of BNT test scores renders use of percentiles misleading, nearly 60% of respondents endorsed using percentiles when reporting BNT data. The implications of these results are discussed, and recommendations are provided.
This article was downloaded by: [kyle boone]
On: 05 September 2013, At: 08:20
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20
Survey results regarding use of the Boston Naming
Test: Houston, we have a problem
Kirsty E. Bortnik
a
, Kyle Brauer Boone
bc
, Johnny Wen
d
, Po Lu
e
, Maura Mitrushina
f
, Jill
Razani
f
& Teresa Maury
g
a
Fuller Graduate School of Psychology, Pasadena, CA, USA
b
California School of Forensic Studies, Alliant International University, Los Angeles,
CA, USA
c
UCLA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Los Angeles, CA, USA
d
Private Practice, Torrance, CA, USA
e
UCLA Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA
f
Department of Psychology, California State University–Northridge, Northridge, CA,
USA
g
Department of Psychology, California State University–Dominguez Hills, Carson,
CA, USA
Published online: 03 Sep 2013.
To cite this article: Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology (2013): Survey results regarding use of the
Boston Naming Test: Houston, we have a problem, Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2013.826182
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors
make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability
for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions
and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of
the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of
information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands,
costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or
indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution
in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 2013
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2013.826182
Survey results regarding use of the Boston Naming
Test: Houston, we have a problem
Kirsty E. Bortnik
1
, Kyle Brauer Boone
2,3
, Johnny Wen
4
,PoLu
5
, Maura Mitrushina
6
,
Jill Razani
6
, and Teresa Maury
7
1
Fuller Graduate School of Psychology, Pasadena, CA, USA
2
California School of Forensic Studies, Alliant International University, Los Angeles, CA, USA
3
UCLA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, Los Angeles, CA, USA
4
Private Practice, Torrance, CA, USA
5
UCLA Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA
6
Department of Psychology, California State University–Northridge, Northridge, CA, USA
7
Department of Psychology, California State University–Dominguez Hills, Carson, CA, USA
Members of the National Academy of Neuropsychology were surveyed in 2005 to assess then current practices
regarding Boston Naming Test (BNT) administration, interpretation, and reporting procedures. Nearly half of
445 respondents followed discontinuation rules that differed from instructions published with the test, and nearly
10% did not administer items in reverse order to achieve the required 8 consecutive item basal. Of further con-
cern, between 40% and 55% of respondents indicated that they did not interpret BNT scores in light of linguistic
and ethnic background, and over 25% reported that they did not consider educational level. Despite the fact
that non-normal distribution of BNT test scores renders use of percentiles misleading, nearly 60% of respon-
dents endorsed using percentiles when reporting BNT data. The implications of these results are discussed, and
recommendations are provided.
Keywords: Boston Naming Test; Naming; Word retrieval; Language; Aphasia.
The Boston Naming Test (BNT) is a visual con-
frontation naming measure used by neuropsychol-
ogists and speech therapists to assess integrity of
word-retrieval skills, and it serves as a diagnos-
tic tool for dementia. The original version was
published in 1978 and consisted of 85 simple line-
drawn pictures for which patients were instructed
to report the common names (Kaplan, Goodglass,
& Weintraub, 1978). In 1983 a shortened 60-
item version was published (Kaplan, Goodglass, &
Weintraub, 1983), and, subsequently, a four-choice
recognition format was added to the 60-item spon-
taneous labeling task for use when examinees fail
to provide correct names (Kaplan, Goodglass, &
Weintraub, 2001).
The BNT has been one of the most commonly
administered neuropsychological tests. Survey
results from 1991 showed that at that time it was
the third most frequently administered measure,
with 63% of survey respondents endorsing use of
the instrument (Butler, Retzlaff, & Vanderploeg,
1991). Approximately a decade later, Camara,
Nathan, and Puente (2000) documented that
the BNT was the eighth most frequently used
assessment measure by neuropsychologists who
conducted assessment services for 5 or more hours
in a typical week. Within the past decade, a study
surveying neuropsychological assessment practices
found that it was the 13th most frequently used
instrument, and the fourth most frequently used
Address correspondence to: Kyle Boone, Alliant International University, 1000 S. Fremont Avenue, Alhambra, CA 91803, USA
(E-mail: kboone@kyleboonephd.com).
© 2013 Taylor & Francis
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
2 BORTNIK ET AL.
measure in the area of memory assessment (Rabin,
Barr, & Burton, 2005). The BNT has also been
commonly utilized by neuropsychologists as part of
forensic evaluations (Lees-Haley, Smith, Williams,
& Dunn, 1996).
Despite the widespread popularity of the BNT,
concerns have been raised regarding inconsistencies
in administration procedures, problematic psycho-
metric properties, and incomplete normative data,
as well as its cultural and language biases.
Studies have pointed to differing administration
and scoring practices for the BNT, in part due
to confusion regarding instructions (e.g., Lopez,
Arias, Hunter, Charter, & Scott, 2003; Nicholas,
Brookshire, MacLennan, Schumacher, & Porrazzo,
1989)—for example, as in what constitutes a cor-
rect response. The test manual indicates that credit
is only to be given for correct labels provided
spontaneously or after stimulus cues, but some
practitioners give credit for correct labels obtained
after phonemic cues. These are not inconsequen-
tial issues given that Ferman, Ivnik, and Lucas
(1998) found that total scores differed in some nor-
mal controls (3%; up to 16-point difference) and
in Alzheimer’s patients (31%; up to 10-point differ-
ence) depending on whether “lenient” (credit given
for correct responses after phonemic cue) or more
“rigorous” (credit given only for correct responses
spontaneously provided or after stimulus cue) inter-
pretation of the discontinuation rule was applied.
A subsequent study by Lopez et al. (2003) cor-
roborated that different approaches to scoring may
result in large differences in total scores. Further
complicating the picture, some test instructions
have changed across versions without explanation;
the 2001 edition indicated that test administration
was to cease after 8 consecutive incorrect responses,
whereas the 1983 version instructed discontinuation
after 6 consecutive incorrect responses.
Regarding BNT psychometric properties, in rela-
tively high functioning populations distribution of
test scores is negatively skewed with extreme kurto-
sis, resulting in limited score variability and small
standard deviations (Mitrushina, Boone, Razani, &
D’Elia, 2005). When z scores and percentiles are
obtained on such scores, performances minimally
below average are overpathologized. For example,
using Boston Naming Test metanorms (Mitrushina
et al., 2005), a score of 52 in a 40-year-old would
be judged to be borderline impaired (i.e., 7th per-
centile; z score = –1.46), although this score is only
8 points from the ceiling. Additionally, test items
do not appear to be ordered consistently in terms of
increasing difficulty, and some items contain redun-
dant information (i.e., have comparable levels of
difficulty) and do not discriminate differing levels
of naming ability (Pedraza, Sachs, Ferman, Rush,
& Lucas, 2011).
Additionally, limited normative data are included
with the test (limited to ages 5 1/2 and 10 1/2, and
ages 18–59 years for the 1983 version), and the nor-
mative data provided in the 1983 60-item edition
of the test were extrapolated from participants who
had been administered the earlier 85-item version
(Nicholas et al., 1989). Given that these norms were
adapted from data published in 1978, the “Flynn
effect” is of concern (i.e., inflation in test scores
over time; Flynn, 1984), leading Strauss, Sherman,
and Spreen (2006) to recommend that only more
recently published norms be employed.
Of particular note, research investigating per-
formance on the BNT has found that Caucasians
typically score higher than ethnic minority individ-
uals (e.g., Boone, Victor, Wen, Razani, & Pontón,
2007), with lowered scores in the latter group
found to be related to linguistic background and
level of acculturation. Roberts, Garcia, Desrochers,
and Hernandez (2002) observed that monolingual
English speakers performed better on the BNT than
either Spanish/
English or French/English
bilingual
speakers, and they documented that even profi-
cient bilingual English speakers score lower on
the BNT than do monolingual English speakers.
This difference in performance remains even for
those bilingual individuals who have resided in the
United States for a significant period of time. While
Boone et al. (2007) observed that BNT perfor-
mance was related to number of years of residence
in the United States, Touradji, Manly, Jacobs, &
Stern (2001) found that foreign-born non-Hispanic
Anglos who rated themselves as speaking English
“very well” scored significantly lower than US-born
subjects on an abbreviated version of the BNT,
despite having lived in the United States for more
than five decades on average. In fact, BNT per-
formance is significantly and negatively related to
age at which conversational English was learned
(Boone et al., 2007). Manly and colleagues (1998)
have shown that BNT scores are lowered even in test
takers born in the United States but who are rela-
tively unassimilated to the Anglo-American culture.
Of note, a recent investigation identified several
BNT test items that were associated with different
correct endorsement rates between healthy African
American and Caucasian older adults, indicating
that a test taker has a significant probability of
responding either correctly or incorrectly on these
items based on racial/ethnic group membership
(Pedraza et al., 2009).
Educational characteristics have also been found
to impact performance on the BNT, with lower
BNT scores associated with decreased educational
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
3 BOSTON NAMING TEST
levels (e.g., see listing of studies in Mitrushina et al.,
2005, p. 180; Zec, Burkett, Markwell, & Larsen,
2007a, 2007b), poor educational quality (Manly,
Byrd, Touradji, & Stern, 2004), and extent of edu-
cation obtained outside of the United States (Boone
et al., 2007).
Preliminary data also indicate that geographic
location is associated with performance on the
BNT. Bauer, O’Bryant, Hilsabeck, and McCaffrey
(2004) reported that the mean BNT scores of young
adults living in the South fell at the 10th percentile
of normative data from Canada, and that 67% of
the Southern sample performed at least 1 standard
deviation below the mean of normative data from
California. Additionally, Goldstein and colleagues
(Goldstein et al., 2000), found that individuals liv-
ing in the Midwest outperform those living in the
South on the BNT.
Due to the multiple concerns regarding adminis-
tration, scoring, and interpretation of the Boston
Naming Test, we surveyed the members of the
National Academy of Neuropsychology to assess
how clinicians are currently navigating these
difficulties.
METHOD
Subjects
Permission was granted from the Institutional
Review Board at Harbor-UCLA Medical Center
and from the Board of Directors of the National
Academy of Neuropsychology (NAN) in 2004 to
survey via e-mail the members of NAN. Although
anonymity could not be provided (due to
potentially identifying information on return
e-mail addresses), confidentiality was ensured by
only entering nonidentifying information in the
database and deleting the e-mail addresses. Of the
2969 surveys sent via e-mail between February
2005 and March 2005, 449 were undeliverable due
to incorrect e-mail address. A total of 599 survey
responses were returned. Sixty-three respondents
returned partially completed surveys, and these
individuals were contacted a second time and
were requested to complete only the sections of
the survey that they did not finish; 13 individuals
responded to this request. Seventy-six respondents
stated that they did not administer the BNT, and
their surveys were excluded. Because the focus
was on use of the BNT in experienced clinicians
currently using the test, 25 surveys submitted
by students-in-training and 3 surveys returned
by retired practitioners were also excluded. The
resulting 445 surveys reflected a response rate of
18% for actually delivered e-mail requests. This
response rate is slightly higher than the 15% rate
obtained by Rabin et al. (2005) in their survey of
test usage by NAN members.
Materials
A two-page survey was developed (see the
Appendix) to assess BNT administration pro-
cedures and interpretation strategies, as well as
to examine how respondents incorporated and
described BNT data in their neuropsychological
reports. At the conclusion of the survey, respon-
dents were asked to cite any concerns regarding the
test, to provide suggestions for improving the mea-
sure, and to rate the BNT in terms of importance
within their neuropsychological battery.
RESULTS
Sample characteristics
As shown in Table 1, 72% of respondents who
endorsed using the Boston Naming Test had
completed two years of postdoctoral training, and
TABLE 1
Sample characteristics of BNT survey respondents
Respondents
Characteristic N % Yes (%) No (%) M SD Range
Number of years in practice 437 12.57 8.82 0–51
Postdoctoral training 443 72.0 28.0
Diplomate 443 21.2 78.8
Population served 443
Pediatric 34.1
Adolescent 49.7
Adult 89.8
Geriatric 78.8
Notes. BNT = Boston Naming Test. The percentage totals for the “Population served” category do not sum to 100% because the
categories are not mutually exclusive, and thus many respondents endorsed serving more than one type of population.
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
4 BORTNIK ET AL.
21% were board certified by the American Board
of Clinical Neuropsychology (ABCN) and/or the
American Board of Neuropsychology (APN).
On average, respondents had over 12 years of clin-
ical experience. A large majority of members indi-
cated that they conducted assessments of adult
and geriatric patients, although half indicated that
they evaluated adolescents, and a third conducted
testing on children.
BNT administration procedures
When queried as to test version used, 82%
employed the 1983 60-item version, 20% used the
2001 60-item version with multiple-choice options,
10% used a 15-item version, and less than 3%
employed the original 1978 85-item format; some
respondents endorsed using more than one ver-
sion. (Because there are numerous 15-item versions
of the test, with test administration instructions
differing from those in the 60- and 85-item ver-
sions, data from respondents who endorsed using
only a 15-item version were confined to analyses
examining consideration of demographic informa-
tion in test interpretation and ratings of impor-
tance of the BNT in test batteries.) Only 7% of
respondents indicated that they employed cultur-
ally sensitive adaptations of the BNT, such as
the Pontón–Satz Spanish BNT version (Pontón
et al., 1996).
Although test instructions indicate that latency
of response should be tracked for each item, and
that 20 seconds with no response should elapse
before a cue is provided (unless the subject indi-
cates not knowing the word prior to the 20 seconds
elapsing), almost 60% of respondents indicated that
they did not record response times (see Table 2),
and just over a third reported that they did not
wait for 20 seconds prior to providing a stimulus or
phonemic cue.
Nearly 10% of respondents reported that they
did not administer items in reverse order to obtain
the required basal of 8 consecutive correctly named
items. Of particular concern, the sample diverged
in terms of identification of incorrect responses;
almost 55% of respondents operationalized failure
as an inability to name spontaneously or after a
stimulus cue, while approximately 46% of respon-
dents reserved failure only for those responses
that remained incorrect even after phonemic cuing.
The majority of respondents reported that they
provided immediate feedback regarding response
correctness, while almost 20% indicated that they
waited 20 seconds before providing such feedback.
Of the respondents who provided immediate feed-
back, 97% gave credit for the item if the patient
self-corrected without needing a phonemic cue.
In test/retest situations, 95% readministered the
full test, while just under 5% resorted to an odd–
even strategy across testing situations, and 6% used
unspecified “other” formats.
TABLE 2
BNT administration practices and interpretation strategies of survey respondents
Respondents
Clinical practices N Yes (%)
Administration
Record latencies 431 42.9
Wait 20 seconds for a response 432 63.4
Reverse order administration of test items 436 91.1
Definition of “failure” as: 429
Inability to spontaneously name item 54.3
Inability to name item following phonemic cue 45.7
Feedback 410
Immediate 80.5
After 20 seconds with provision of phonemic cue 19.5
Retesting 369
Readminister full test 95.4
Use even/odd administration 4.6
Interpretation—use of norms
BNT booklet 177 40.7
Spreen and Strauss (1999) 191 43.9
Mitrushina, Boone, and D’Elia (1999) 103 23.7
Heaton, Grant, and Matthews (1991) 78 18.0
Note. BNT = Boston Naming Test.
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
5 BOSTON NAMING TEST
BNT interpretation strategies
As shown in Table 2, virtually 100% of respon-
dents reported using normative data for interpret-
ing scores from the BNT. Of these, 44% used norms
provided in Spreen and Strauss (1998), and a nearly
equal number indicated using the norms repro-
duced at the back of the 1983 and 2001 BNT
booklets. Approximately 24% used norms cata-
loged in Mitrushina et al. (1999), and 18% utilized
the norms provided by Heaton et al. (1991).
Over half of the respondents indicated that they
did not consider patient ethnicity in interpretation
of BNT scores (Table 3), 41% reported that they
did not modify interpretation in those patients who
spoke English as a second language, and almost
30% indicated that they did not factor educational
level in interpretation of BNT data.
Citing of BNT data in neuropsychological
reports
As shown in Table 3, slightly more than two
thirds of respondents indicated that they provide a
total BNT score (i.e., number of items correct) in
their neuropsychological reports; just under a third
reported that they report the number of correct
items following phonemic cues, and less than 18%
endorsed reporting the number of correct items fol-
lowing stimulus cues. As also shown in Table 3,
over half of respondents indicated that they include
percentiles for BNT scores in their reports. Over
40% provide infor mation on phonemic paraphasias
in their reports, while less than one half include data
on semantic paraphasias.
Perceived importance of BNT and
suggestions/concerns
In addition to surveying the ways in which
respondents administered and interpreted the
BNT and incorporated the test results into
their neuropsychological reports, respondents were
asked to rate the importance of the BNT in their
assessment batteries and were also requested to
share their concerns and suggestions regarding the
test. On average, respondents rated the importance
of the BNT as 4.90 (SD = 1.47) on a 7-point scale
(with 1 reflecting “irrelevant” and 7 “extremely rel-
evant”), with some respondents indicating that test
relevance depended on the referral question and
type of patient tested. A variety of concerns were
expressed, including poor quality of the drawings
and out-of-date items (such as stilts and yoke), and
cultural bias; in this regard, several respondents
suggested that the picture of the noose be elimi-
nated due to its association with slavery and racial
segregation. Suggestions also included improved
normative data as well as credit given for accept-
able label synonyms and/or acceptance of mispro-
nounced words.
DISCUSSION
These survey results characterize clinical usage of
the BNT in members of the National Academy
TABLE 3
BNT reporting practices
Reporting practices N Yes (%)
In your neuropsychological report, do you report:
Total score (correct without phonemic cue) 435 66.9
Number correct following phonemic cues 435 29.7
Number correct following stimulus cues 435 17.5
Percentiles 435 57.5
Phonemic paraphasias 435 43.2
Semantic paraphasias 435 42.5
Do you make adjustments for:
Ethnicity 441 46.9
English as a second language 440 58.6
Education level 440 71.8
Do you use cultural adaptations of the BNT 437 7.6
Rate the importance of the BNT in your neuropsychological battery, 439
with “1” being the most irrelevant and “7” being extremely relevant
Mean 4.90
Median 5.00
Mode 5.00
Note. BNT = Boston Naming Test.
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
6 BORTNIK ET AL.
of Neuropsychology as of 2005. The respondents
reported an average of 12 years of experience and a
board certification rate of 21%, and over 70% cited
completion of 2 years of postdoctoral training; thus
this sample is likely representative of clinical usage
of the BNT in the mid-2000s. Whether these data
reflect current test usage is unknown, but given that
no updated test guidelines have been published in
the interim, it is reasonable to assume that clini-
cal administration and interpretative practices have
changed little.
Analyses of survey responses revealed prominent
inconsistencies in administration procedures across
respondents. Specifically, while the test instructions
indicate that response latencies are to be recorded,
only slightly more than 40% of respondents indi-
cated that they followed this procedure. This finding
no doubt reflects the reality that many patients
respond so rapidly to test pictures it is not feasi-
ble to time responses, and that, on the other hand,
waiting for a full 20 seconds for a response seems
unduly long. Allowing 20 seconds for responses
undoubtedly results in more spontaneous correct
responses, and it can be argued that this makes
the test less sensitive to true word retrieval prob-
lems in that individuals who require 5 to 20 seconds
to recall picture names likely do have at least sub-
tle word-retrieval difficulties. However, the problem
with discarding or shortening time requirements for
responses is that normative data were presumably
collected using 20-second response time cutoffs.
Nearly 10% of respondents reported that they did
not administer items in reverse order to obtain the
required basal of 8 sequential spontaneously cor-
rect trials; failure to obtain the basal could result
in inflation of the test scores (i.e., with the clin-
ician incorrectly assuming that all items prior to
#30 would have been answered correctly by the
patient). This error is of particular concern given
that data show that test items are not correctly
sequenced in terms of an increasing gradient of dif-
ficulty (Pedraza et al., 2009); thus, it is relatively
likely for errors to occur prior to #30.
Also of note, the sample disagreed regard-
ing determination of what constitutes a correct
response. Test instructions published with the
1983 version state that the test is to be discon-
tinued after 6 sequential incorrect responses (and
after 8 incorrect responses in the 2001 version),
with failure defined as an inability to “respond cor-
rectly either spontaneously or following a stimulus
cue.” However, only slightly more than half of sur-
vey respondents reported following this procedure
(54%), while 46% indicated that they gave credit
for responses following a phonemic cue. This latter
alteration in standardized testing procedures would
have the effect of inflating total test scores, and in
fact published data show that differences of up to
16 points can occur across the two different scoring
procedures (Ferman et al., 1998).
Over half of respondents indicated that they did
not modify interpretation of BNT scores based
on ethnicity of patients, with 41% reporting that
they did not consider impact of English as a
second language on test scores. It is possible,
depending on the geographic location, that some
of the survey respondents may not have been test-
ing ethnic minority patients. However, given the
cultural diversity in most regions of the United
States, it would be expected that such respondents
would comprise a relatively small percentage of
total practitioners. Thus, the survey results sug-
gest that many neuropsychologists, when evaluating
ethnic minorities and English as a second lan-
guage individuals, are not appropriately weighing
the impact of these variables on test performance,
which arguably is out of compliance with American
Psychological Association (2010) Ethical Standards
(9.06: Interpreting Test Results). Of further concern,
28% of those surveyed indicated that they did not
take into consideration the educational level of the
patient in their interpretation of BNT scores.
As summarized in the introduction section of
this manuscript, available data suggest that eth-
nic minorities perform substantially poorer on the
BNT than Caucasian subjects. Subjects who speak
English as a second language score lower, but even
ethnic minorities who are fully bilingual in English,
or who are only English speaking, score lower
(Boone et al., 2007), indicating that more pervasive
cultural factors than solely linguistic are impact-
ing BNT performance. For example, when Hispanic
patients were divided into English as a sec-
ond language and native English-speaking groups,
the Hispanic native English-speaking patients still
scored on average 10 points lower on the BNT than
did Caucasian patients. Lowered educational level
has also been found to be predictive of lowered
BNT performance (Mitrushina et al., 2005)and
may have a major impact of test scores; for exam-
ple, in individuals aged 75–59 years with <12 years
of education, mean BNT score was 36.6, but in indi-
viduals in the same age group with 12 or more years
of education, mean BNT score was 53.4 (Welch,
Doineau, Johnson, & King, 1996). Further com-
plicating the picture, where education is completed
is critical; research suggests an inverse relationship
between the amount of education completed out-
side of the United States and BNT scores (Boone
et al., 2007
). Failure to adjust for the effects of
these
variables will result in overpathologizing of
cognitive function in ethnic minority, immigrant,
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
7
and low-education populations. Further, as dis-
cussed earlier, there may even be regional differ-
ences in BNT performance, with individuals in
the Southern portion of the United States scor-
ing more poorly than those from the Midwest and
West (California) and Canada (Bauer et al., 2004;
Goldstein et al., 2000). Thus, appropriate selection
of BNT normative data may involve matching for
geographic location. Of concern, norms included
with the 1983 and 2001 versions of the test contain
no information regarding ethnicity or geographic
location, requiring that clinicians who wish to
examine these issues employ normative data from
other published resources (Mitrushina et al., 2005;
Strauss et al., 2006). Finally, some populations are
under-represented in available norms (e.g., young to
middle-age adults and low education levels).
Almost 60% of respondents indicated that they
included percentiles when reporting performance
on the BNT. This is concerning because test scores
on the BNT are generally not normally distributed,
and questions have been raised concerning the
asymmetry and extreme kurtosis of the test (e.g.,
Hawkins & Bender, 2002; Mitrushina et al., 2005).
The small standard deviations associated with the
markedly skewed scores result in markedly low
percentile scores even when scores only deviate
slightly from normative means (e.g., a score of 50 is
associated with a percentile rank of 3 in 45–49-
year-olds even though the normative mean is only
56.75; meta-norms for BNT, Mitrushina et al.,
2005).
Recommendations
Survey respondents indicated that while they gener-
ally viewed the BNT as an important component of
their neuropsychological batteries (rated as nearly
a “5” on a 7-point scale), they expressed concerns
regarding the poor-quality drawings on the test,
out-of-date and culturally insensitive items, and the
lack of information regarding credit for acceptable
label synonyms. Surveys of test usage have shown
consistent decline in use of the BNT over the past
two decades, which is likely driven by the limitations
of the test, including unsophisticated test stimuli
and confusing test instructions.
Given evidence of widespread inconsistencies
in BNT test administration, it is strongly recom-
mended that clinical reports and research papers
specify the BNT version administered and the
discontinuation rule employed, as well as any
deviations in test administration from standard
instructions. If shortened test administration times
are necessary, rather than altering standard test
BOSTON NAMING TEST
administration procedures (e.g., failing to docu-
ment a basal of 8 correct), evaluators may want
to consider abbreviated BNT versions (see Graves,
Bezeau, Fogarty, & Blar, 2004). Also, due to the
limited norms provided with the test, clinicians
are encouraged to utilize norms published else-
where (such as those found in Heaton et al., 1991;
Mitrushina et al., 2005; etc.), which consider lin-
guistic, ethnic, educational, and geographic back-
ground. Finally, due to the asymmetry and kurtosis
of the test data, it is recommended that clinicians
refrain from including percentiles in their reports
and instead comment as to whether scores are
within normal limits after consideration of demo-
graphic factors.
Original manuscript received 25 March 2013
Revised manuscript accepted 14 July 2013
First published online 3 September 2013
REFERENCES
American Psychological Association. (2010). Ethical
principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
Retrieved from http://apa.org/ethics/code/index.aspx
Bauer, L., O’Bryant, S. E., Hilsabeck, R. C., &
McCaffrey, R. M. (2004). An examination of perfor-
mance on the Boston Naming Test in a non-clinical
Southern sample. Poster presented at the Annual
Meeting of the International Neuropsychological
Society, Baltimore, MD. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 10.
Boone, K. B., Victor, T. L., Wen, J., Razani, J.,
& Pontón, M. (2007). The association between
neuropsychological scores and ethnicity, language,
and acculturation variables in a large patient pop-
ulation. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22,
355–365.
Butler, M., Retzlaff, P., & Vanderploeg, R. (1991).
Neuropsychological test usage. Professional Psycho-
logy: Research and Practice, 22, 510–512.
Camara, W. J., Nathan, J. S., & Puente, A. E. (2000).
Psychological test usage: Implications in professional
psychology. Professional Psychology: Research and
Practice, 31, 141–154.
Ferman, T. J., Ivnik, R. J., & Lucas, J. A. (1998). Boston
Naming Test discontinuation rule: Rigorous versus
lenient interpretations. Assessment, 5, 13–18.
Flynn, J. R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans: Massive
gains 1932 to 1978. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 29–51.
Goldstein, D., Mercury, M., Azrin, R., Millsaps, C.,
Ventura, T., & Pliskin, N. (2000). Cultural consid-
erations on the Boston Naming Test: The effects
of race and geographic region. Poster presented at
the 28th Annual Conference of the International
Neuropsychological Society, Denver CO.
Graves, R. E., Bezeau, S. C., Fogarty, J., & Blair, R.
(2004). Boston Naming Test short forms: A compari-
son of previous forms with new item response theory
based forms. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology, 26, 891–902.
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
8 BORTNIK ET AL.
Hawkins, K. A., & Bender, S. (2002). Norms and the
relationship of Boston Naming Test performance to
vocabulary and education: A review. Aphasiology, 16,
1143–1153.
Heaton, R., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. (1991).
Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead–
Reitan Battery: Demographic corrections, research
findings, and clinical applications. Odessa, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources.
Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1978).
The Boston Naming Test: Experimental Edition.
Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.
Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983).
The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea &
Febiger.
Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (2001).
The Boston Naming Test (2nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA:
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Lees-Haley, P. R., Smith, H. H., Williams, C. W.,
& Dunn, J. T. (1996). Forensic neuropsychological
test usage: An empirical survey. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 11, 45–51.
Lopez, M. N., Arias, G. P., Hunter, M. A., Charter,
R. A., & Scott, R. R. (2003). Boston Naming
Test: Problems with administration and scoring.
Psychological Reports, 92, 468–472.
Manly, J. J., Byrd, D. A., Touradji, P., & Stern, Y. (2004).
Acculturation, reading level, and neuropsychological
test performance among African American elders.
Applied Neuropsychology, 11, 37–46.
Manly, J. J., Miller, S. W., Heaton, R. K., Byrd,
D., Reilly, J., Velasquez, R. J., ... Grant, I.
(1998). The effect of African-American acculturation
on neuropsychological testing performance in nor-
mal and HIV-positive individuals. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 291–302.
Mitrushina, M. N., Boone, K. B., & D’Elia, L. F. (1999).
Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological
assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University.
Mitrushina, M. N., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., &
D’Elia, L. F. (2005). Handbook of normative data for
neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). New York,
NY: Oxford University.
Nicholas, L. A., Brookshire, R. H., MacLennan, D. L.,
Schumacher, J. G., & Porrazzo, S. A. (1989). Revised
administration and scoring procedures for the Boston
Naming Test and norms for non-brain-damaged
adults. Aphasiology, 3, 569–580.
Pedraza, O., Graff-Radford, N. R., Smith, G. E., Ivnik,
R. J., Willis, F. B., Peterson, R. C., & Lucas,
J. A. (2009). Differential item functioning on the
Boston Naming Test in cognitively normal African
American and Caucasian older adults. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 758–768.
Pedraza, O., Sachs, B. C., Ferman, T. J., Rush, B.
K., & Lucas, J. A. (2011). Difficulty and discrim-
ination parameters of Boston Naming Test items
in a consecutive clinical series. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 26, 434–444.
Pontón, M. O., Satz, P., Herrera, L., Ortiz, F.,
Urrutia, C., Young, R., . . . Namerow, N. (1996).
Normative data stratified by age and education for the
Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics
(NeSBHIS): Initial report. Journal of International
Neuropsychological Society, 2, 96–104.
Rabin, L. A., Barr, W. B., & Burton, L. A. (2005).
Assessment practices of clinical neuropsychologists
in the United States and Canada: A survey of INS,
NAN, and APA Division 40 members. Archives of
Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 33–65.
Roberts, P. M., Garcia, L. J., Desrochers, A., &
Hernandez, D. (2002). English performance of pro-
ficient bilingual adults on the Boston Naming Test.
Aphasiology, 16
, 635–645.
Spr
een, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuro-
psychological tests (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford.
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O.
(2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests:
Administration, norms and commentary (3rd ed.). New
York, NY: Oxford.
Touradji, P., Manly, J. J., Jacobs, D. M., & Stern, Y.
(2001). Neuropsychological test performance: A study
of non-Hispanic white elderly. Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 643–649.
Welch, L. W., Doineau, D., Johnson, S., & King, D.
(1996). Educational and gender normative data for the
Boston Naming Test in a group of older adults. Brain
and Language, 53, 260–266.
Zec, R. F., Burkett, N. R., Markwell, S. J., & Larsen, D. L.
(2007a). A cross-sectional study of the effects of age,
education, and gender on the Boston Naming Test.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21, 587–616.
Zec, R. F., Burkett, N. R., Markwell, S. J., & Larsen, D. L.
(2007b). Normative data stratified for age, education,
and gender on the Boston Naming Test. The Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 21, 617–637.
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
____________________________________________________________________________
9 BOSTON NAMING TEST
APPENDIX
Boston Naming
Test (BNT) Survey
Please provide check mark(s) where appropriate.
Number of Years in Practice_______ Neuropsychological Post Doc Training (2 years) __Yes __ No
Memberof__ABPNor__ABCN
Population you serve __ Pediatrics __ Adolescents __ Adults __Geriatrics
Administration
(1) Which language measure(s) do you include in your neuropsychological battery? __ BNT
__ WAIS–III (Vocabulary) __ PPVT versions __ BDAE sections __ Verbal Fluency __
other________________________________________________________________________
(2) Do you administer the BNT? __ Yes __ No If yes, which version? __ 60-item (BNT, 1983)
__ 60-items with multiple choices (BNT–II, 2001) __ any 15-item version __ 85-item __
other ________________________
(3) Do you record latencies? __Yes __ No
(4) Do you wait a full “20 seconds” before providing a stimulus or phonemic cue? _ Yes _ No
(5) If a patient does not achieve 8 in a row correct without phonemic cues starting at item #30, do you work
backwards to obtain 8 correct consecutive responses? __ Yes __ No
(6) Administration is to be discontinued after 6 consecutive failures (1983 version). Do you understand
“failure” to mean,
__ unable to name spontaneously (but may generate correct name with phonemic cues) OR
__ unable to name a picture even with phonemic cue
(7) If a patient provides an incorrect label (e.g., lock for latch), do you
__ A. provide immediate feedback (e.g., I’m looking for a different word”)
If A, if the patient self corrects (e.g., “oh, it’s a latch”) without phonemic cue, do you count as
__ correct or __ failed
__ B. wait 20 seconds without giving feedback and then provide phonemic cue
__ Other ___________________________________________________________________
(8) For test–retest situations, do you ___ re-administer the full test ___ use even/odd administration
other______________________________________________________________________
Interpretation
(1) Do you use norms for the BNT? _Yes _No If not, how do you interpret the raw score?
If yes, which norm(s) do you use?
__Norms from the back of BNT booklet
__Norms from Spreen and Strauss (1999); (specify:)__________________________________
__Norms from Mitrushina, Boone, and D’Elia (1999); (specify:) _______________________
__other _____________________________________________________________________
Reporting
(1) In the neuropsychological report, do you report
__ total score (i.e., # correct without phonemic cue)
__ number correct following phonemic cues
__ number correct following stimulus cues
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
__________________________________________________________________________________________
__________________________________________________________________________________________
__________________________________________________________________________________________
__________________________________________________________________________________________
__________________________________________________________________________________________
__________________________________________________________________________________________
10 BORTNIK ET AL.
__ percentiles
__ phonemic paraphasias
__ semantic paraphasias
(2) Do you make adjustments for _ ethnicity? _ English as a second language? _ educational level?
(3) Do you use cultural adaptations of the BNT? (e.g., Ponton–Satz Spanish Version)? __ Yes __ No
Suggestions/Concerns
(1) Do you have concerns regarding specific BNT items (e.g., acceptable synonyms including use of regional
terms, culturally biased items, quality of drawings)? If so, please list and describe
(2) What suggestions do you have for making this a better test?
(5) Rate the importance of the BNT in your neuropsychological battery
irrelevant1 2 3 4 5 6 7 extremely relevant
Downloaded by [kyle boone] at 08:20 05 September 2013
... Поскольку тесты на называние выступают как исследовательский инструмент и как реализация теоретических моделей, лежащих в их основе, надежность и валидность традиционных тестов подвергается критике (Bortnik et al. 2013, Harry & Crowe 2014. Однако подобные попытки несистемны и часто недостаточно теоретически обоснованы, ибо фокусируются на проведении эксперимента и противоречиях в полученных данных, но не на архитектуре исследовательской парадигмы. ...
... Средний уровень образования в двух старших возрастных группах был выше (13.38 / 17.58 / 16.65) (Schmitter-Edgecombe et al. 2000) Некоторые участники используют более одной версии. 7 % применяют версии теста, адаптированные для конкретных языков и культур Протокол проведения: Почти 60 % не регистрируют время ответа Около 33 % не выжидают 20 секунд до подсказки Протокол оценки: Критерий неуспешности Почти 55 %: невозможность назвать объект сразу или после семантической подсказки Около 46 %: невозможность назвать объект только после фонематической подсказки Протокол оценки: Дополнительные переменные Более 50 % не учитывают этническую принадлежность 41 % не учитывают статус английского как второго языка Почти 30 % не учитывают уровень образования Диагностическая значимость теста: Gо семибалльной шкале участники оценивают как 4.90 (SD = 1.47) 445 членов Национальной академии нейропсихологии США приняли участие в опросе о практике применения BNT.(Bortnik et al. 2013) Предположительно более трудные стимулы идентифицированы более успешно, чем легкие: № 44 muzzle: неуспешность 28.5 % № 45 unicorn: неуспешность 2.5Проблема / ВыводПроцедура и Источник 105 участников не назвали № 48, из них 17.1 % (18 участников) при начислении дополнительного балла были отнесены к более высокой категории успешности. Изменение особенно заметно в наименее успешных группах (12 участников). ...
Article
Full-text available
The paper considers benefits and drawbacks of the Picture Naming Test (PNT) as a diagnostic method suggesting the ways to improve its diagnostic and research validity. Although this test is popular in both national and international neuropsychological assessment and research practice, its use is largely due to tradition and practical convenience. However, theoretical underpinnings for its effectiveness are not sufficiently represented in the pertinent literature, which determines the relevance of the study. The major objective of the paper is to consider the PNT as a research technique from the point of view of psycholinguistics, cognitive science, and corpus linguistics and to propose integrated approaches for the further development of these tests. The material comprises picture naming tests developed for the Russian and English languages. The study sets the following aims: 1) to define the main features of PNT and the principles of its application viewed from a historical perspective; 2) to identify the theoretical foundations (linguistic and neuropsychological) for its use as a diagnostic tool; 3) to highlight the problematic aspects of the method; 4) to suggest possible ways to eliminate them. To accomplish these tasks, various methods of corpus linguistics are applied throughout the article. The historical outline of PNT development suggests that the current design might stem from earlier contexts of use. A review of existing models of lexical access provides a theoretical basis for the test in its current form and suggests possible avenues for its development grounded in experimental research, advances in linguistics and big data analysis. A separate section of the article presents critique of the most popular tests. Finally, the analysis of the existing English and Russian tests through the corpus-based methodology clearly demonstrates the need for more detailed norming and stimuli selection. By way of conclusion the authors outline the principles of designing Picture Naming Tests for specific purposes and put forward a step-by-step algorithm that enables careful selection of the necessary indicators and parameters.
... Although an exact response rate could not be calculated for this study due to overlap in electronic mailing lists and targeted emails used for recruitment, our estimated response rate (15%) is low, which limits the generalizability of our results. However, response rates are best understood in relation to the response rate other researchers have obtained in comparable survey studies (Guterbock & Marcopulos, 2020), and the response rate for this study is comparable to those reported by other researchers utilizing web-based surveys (5-18%; Bortnik et al., 2013;Hirst et al., 2019;Rabin et al., 2008). ...
Article
Objective: Functioning in daily life is an important consideration when differentiating between individuals with normal cognition, mild neurocognitive disorder, and major neurocognitive disorder. Despite this, there is no gold standard measurement approach for assessing functional abilities and few guidelines on how to do so. The objective of this study was to examine neuropsychologists' practices regarding the assessment of functional abilities across the spectrum of memory ability. Method: A total of 278 psychologists who routinely conduct neuropsychological assessments completed an online survey (estimated 15% response rate) querying their practices and perspectives with respect to the assessment of functional abilities. Results: Respondents identified that changes to several components of daily functioning, including activities of daily living, were important when evaluating functional abilities. Respondents reported utilizing a variety of instruments to assess functioning, with an overwhelming majority indicating the use of semi-structured interviews. Although most respondents are satisfied with existing tools, a quarter of respondents felt strongly that there was a need for more instruments of everyday functioning. Respondents further indicated that their recommendations to patients, particularly regarding compensatory strategies and follow-up with other professionals, were informed by results of their functional assessment. Conclusions: Overall, our survey results indicate that neuropsychologists perceive multiple factors of daily life to be important considerations when evaluating functioning, use a variety of techniques to assess functioning, and perceive a need for more measures of functional abilities.
... Therefore, results from such assessment tools-when used to assess Black, Hispanic, and Asian people with aphasia, as well as people with aphasia from other racial and ethnic backgrounds or those who do not speak standardized English (Hudley & Mallinson, 2015) as their primary dialect-should be interpreted cautiously through this lens. And while there are instructions in some of the examiner's manuals (e.g., the BNT-2) to interpret scores with respect to cultural-linguistic background, a survey study of >400 people showed that less than half of their respondents reported doing so, and only 30-40% reported taking native language background or educational status into consideration during interpretation (Bortnik et al., 2013). A survey of SLPs in the U.S. found low SLP-respondent familiarity with and use of culturally-based diagnostic modifications of aphasia assessments when assessing African American clients (Bond & Gooch, 2016). ...
Article
Background Discourse analyses yield quantitative measures of functional communication in aphasia. However, they are historically underutilized in clinical settings. Confrontation naming assessments are used widely clinically and have been used to estimate discourse-level production. Such work shows that naming accuracy explains moderately high proportions of variance in measures of discourse, but proportions of variance remain unexplained. We propose that the inclusion of circumlocution productions into predictive models will account for a significant amount more of the variance. Circumlocution productions at the naming-level, while they may not contain the target word, are similar to the content that contributes to discourse informativeness and efficiency. Thus, additionally measuring circumlocution may improve our ability to estimate discourse performance and functional communication. Aim This study aimed to test whether, after controlling for naming accuracy, the addition of a measure of circumlocution into predictive models of discourse-level informativeness and efficiency would account for a significant amount more of the variance in these discourse-level outcomes. Methods & Procedures Naming and discourse data from 43 people with poststroke aphasia were analyzed. Naming data were collected using 120 pictured items and discourse data were collected using two picture description prompts. Data scoring and coding yielded measures of naming accuracy, incorrect response type, communicative informativeness, and efficiency. We used robust hierarchical regression to evaluate study predictions. Outcomes & Results After controlling for naming accuracy, the inclusion of circumlocution into predictive models accounted for a significant amount more of the variance in both informativeness and efficiency. The subsequent inclusion of other response types, such as real word and nonword errors, did not account for a significant amount more of the variance in either outcome. Conclusions In addition to naming accuracy, the production of circumlocution during naming assessments may correspond with measures of informativeness and efficiency at the discourse-level. Reducing the burden of estimating patients’ functional communication will increase our ability to estimate functional communication using tools that are easy to administer and interpret.
Article
Full-text available
A picture naming test (PNT) has long been regarded as an integral part of neuropsychological assessment. In current research and clinical practice, it serves a variety of purposes. PNTs are used to assess the severity of speech impairment in aphasia, monitor possible cognitive decline in aging patients with or without age-related neurodegenerative disorders, track language development in children and map eloquent brain areas to be spared during surgery. In research settings, picture naming tests provide an insight into the process of lexical retrieval in monolingual and bilingual speakers. However, while numerous advances have occurred in linguistics and neuroscience since the classic, most widespread PNTs were developed, few of them have found their way into test design. Consequently, despite the popularity of PNTs in clinical and research practice, their relevance and objectivity remain questionable. The present study provides an overview of literature where relevant criticisms and concerns have been expressed over the recent decades. It aims to determine whether there is a significant gap between conventional test design and the current understanding of the mechanisms underlying lexical retrieval by focusing on the parameters that have been experimentally proven to influence picture naming. We discuss here the implications of these findings for improving and facilitating test design within the picture naming paradigm. Subsequently, we highlight the importance of designing specialized tests with a particular target group in mind, so that test variables could be selected for cerebellar tumor survivors.
Article
Objective We aimed to evaluate the psychometric properties and diagnostic accuracy of the 32-item version of the Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) in participants from 2 ethnic groups (European Americans [EA; n = 106] and Hispanic Americans [HA; n = 175]) with 3 diagnostic groups (cognitively normal [CN], n = 94, mild cognitive impairment [MCI], n = 148, and dementia, n = 39). Method An Item Response Theory model was used to evaluate items across ethnicity and language groups (Spanish and English), resulting in a 24-item version. We analyzed the MINT discriminant and predictive validity across diagnostic groups. Results A total of 8 items were differentially difficult between languages in the 32-item version of the MINT. EA scored significantly higher than HA, but the difference was not significant when removing those 8 items (controlling for Education). The Receiver Operating Characteristics showed that the MINT had poor accuracy when identifying CN participants and was acceptable in identifying dementia participants but unacceptable in classifying MCI participants. Finally, we tested the association between MINT scores and magnetic resonance imaging volumetric measures of language-related areas in the temporal and frontal lobes. The 32-item MINT in English and Spanish and the 24-item MINT in Spanish were significantly correlated with the bilateral middle temporal gyrus. The left fusiform gyrus correlated with MINT scores regardless of language and MINT version. We also found differential correlations depending on the language of administration. Conclusions Our results highlight the importance of analyzing cross-cultural samples when implementing clinical neuropsychological tests such as the MINT.
Article
Confrontation naming measures are commonly used for both diagnostic and clinical research purposes in populations of known or suspected neurodegenerative disorders. The Boston Naming Test (BNT) is the most widely used measure of confrontation naming but has been criticized for outdated and culturally biased content. A new naming measure, the Multilingual Naming Test (MiNT), has been developed that may address these limitations, but research regarding its validity and diagnostic performance relative to existing instruments is limited. The current study examined how the BNT and MiNT performed in a sample of older adults evaluated in an interprofessional memory disorders clinic. Eighty-six individuals (50.0% women) met the inclusion criteria and were included in the study. The average age of participants was 74.2 years (SD = 7.7), and the average education was 16.7 years (SD = 2.5). Most participants were non-Hispanic White (94.2%), and the remaining participants were Hispanic or Black. All participants completed a comprehensive evaluation in English and were administered both the BNT and the MiNT. The strength of agreement as indexed by CCC (.67) was modest for the sample as a whole. Eighty-seven-point five percent classification agreement for impaired vs. normal naming performance was obtained. Eleven cases showed disagreement between BNT and MiNT classification of impairment, with seven of these being borderline score cases. Overall, the results suggest that the MiNT performs similarly at the identification of naming impairments as the BNT, though performance may diverge across different diagnostic groups and may be influenced by age.
Article
We mapped the left hemisphere cortical regions and fiber bundles involved in picture naming in adults by integrating task-based fMRI with dMRI tractography. We showed that a ventral pathway that “maps image and sound to meaning” involves the middle occipital, inferior temporal, superior temporal, inferior frontal gyri, and the temporal pole where a signal exchange is made possible by the inferior fronto-occipital, inferior longitudinal, middle longitudinal, uncinate fasciculi, and the extreme capsule. A dorsal pathway that “maps sound to speech” implicates the inferior temporal, superior temporal, inferior frontal, precentral gyri, and the supplementary motor area where the arcuate fasciculus and the frontal aslant ensure intercommunication. This study provides a neurocognitive model of picture naming and supports the hypothesis that the ventral indirect route passes through the temporal pole. This further supports the idea that the inferior and superior temporal gyri may play pivotal roles within the dual-stream framework of language.
Article
Full-text available
Despite the ubiquity of the Boston naming test (BNT) in clinical practice and research, concerns have been expressed about its poor quality pictures, insufficient psychometric properties, and cultural bias in non-English language backgrounds. We modified the black-and-white BNT with a set of color pictures since color effects have been suggested to improve naming accuracy in the visual naming test. This study aimed to examine and compare the reliability and validity of the color-picture version of BNT (CP-BNT) and the black-and-white version of BNT (BW-BNT) to differentiate amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) or mild Alzheimer's disease (AD) from the cognitive normals. This study included two subgroups, and each subgroup had 101 normal controls, 51 aMCI, and 52 mild AD. One subgroup undertook BW-BNT and the other conducted CP-BNT. The reliability, convergent and discriminant validity, and the diagnostic accuracy of two versions of BNT were evaluated. The CP-BNT showed a greater area under the curve (AUC) than the BW-BNT for aMCI (80.3 vs.s 69.4%) and mild AD (93.5 vs. 77.6%). The CP-BNT also demonstrated better convergent validity with CDR global scores and better reliability (Cronbach's coefficient 0.66 for the CP-BNT vs. 0.55 for the BW-BNT). At the optimal cutoff value of spontaneous naming, the CP-BNT demonstrated improved sensitivity and specificity for differentiating mild AD from NC with a higher positive predictive value, negative predictive value, and lower false-positive rate. Compared with BW-BNT, CP-BNT is a more reliable and valid test to assess cognitive and naming impairment.
Article
Full-text available
Background: The Boston Naming Test is widely used in several versions and languages. However, there are few studies of its use with bilingual adults. A recent study by Kohnert, Hernandez, and Bates (1998) found that Spanish/English bilingual adults scored well below unilingual adults. Aims: This study tested two hypotheses. (1) Fluently bilingual adults will obtain significantly lower scores than unilingual, English-speaking adults on the BNT, in English. (2) The order of difficulty of the 60 items will differ for the bilingual and unilingual groups. Methods & Procedures: This study compared the English performance of unilingual speakers (n = 42) to that of two groups of bilingual adults: Spanish/English (n = 32) and French/English (n = 49). All bilingual participants learned English as a second language as children and claimed high levels of ability in English. All participants completed high school (range 11–27 years of schooling). The three groups did not differ significantly in age or education. An ANOVA compared the mean Total Correct obtained by the three groups. Outcomes & Results: Both hypotheses were confirmed. The mean scores (Total Corrrect) for the bilingual groups (42.6 and 39.5/60) were both significantly below the mean score of the unilingual group (50.9/60) but not different from each other. Item difficulty showed some similarities but also important differences across groups. Conclusions: The English language norms cannot be used, even with proficient bilingual speakers. Cultural factors appear less important than bilingualism. Some items on the Boston Naming Test have more than one correct name and suggestions for “lenient” scoring are given.
Article
Full-text available
Quantifies the frequency of use of standard clinical neuropsychological tests. A survey listing 116 tests abstracted from the literature was mailed to 500 randomly selected International Neuropsychological Society members. A return rate of 56% was achieved. Endorsement frequencies are provided and discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Book
The book is a collection of normative data for commonly administered neuropsychological instruments. Included are summary tables of normative data from numerous studies and meta-analytic tables for some tests, detailed critique of the normative studies for each instrument, brief information regarding the tests themselves (history, cognitive constructs, and different test versions), as well as statistical and methodological issues relevant to interpretation of neuropsychological test data. This book represents an updated and expanded edition of the original published in 1999.
Article
Background: Performance on the Boston Naming Test (BNT) reflects influences from demographic and ability variables that were not well understood at the time of the publication of the test. Means for normal samples with limited educational backgrounds, or limited vocabularies, that fall as much as two standard deviations below the original norms have been reported (Hawkins et al., 1993). Failure to recognise these influences will lead to substantial levels of misdiagnosis of dysnomia. These influences have, however, been obscured by the publication of studies reporting lesser, or no, relationship between vocabulary, or educational level, and BNT performance. Aims: The present paper aims to review studies yielding BNT norms, and to identify factors that obscure the relationship between vocabulary, education, and test performance. Main Contribution: It is established that few studies generating BNT norms have been adequately representative of the population, with the vast majority featuring a disproportionate representation of highly educated subjects. In combination with the psychometric properties of the BNT, these imbalances obscure the relationship between vocabulary, or education, and BNT demonstrated in studies utilising more representative samples. Some attempts to account for education have been distorted by a disproportionate representation of subjects of higher intellectual ability than would be typical for their stated years of schooling. Conclusions: BNT norms should be finely stratified by education. Whenever possible, the clinical interpretation of BNT scores should be further moderated by estimations of premorbid vocabulary.
Article
Standard administration, response coding and scoring procedures were developed for the 60-item Boston Naming Test (BNT). Standard prompts to be given following certain types of incorrect responses were also developed. The BNT was administered to 60 non-brain-damaged adults using these standard procedures. Our results show that examiners can reliably carry out the revised test procedures reported in this paper. Our results also show that two response categories (Related Name and Don't Know) accounted for nearly 80% of incorrect responses. Several BNT test pictures frequently elicited a consistent name other than the one specified in the test. Prompting for an additional response following Related Name and Multiple Attempts responses frequently elicited the correct BNT name. Prompts following other types of error responses usually did not elicit the correct name. Neither the age nor education of our nonbrain-damaged subjects was strongly related to their total BNT score.