PreprintPDF Available

Paving the Way for Memory Enhancement: Development and Examination of a Neurofeedback System Targeting the Medial Temporal Lobe

Authors:

Abstract

Neurofeedback (NF) shows promise in enhancing memory, but its application to the medial temporal lobe (MTL) still needs to be studied. Therefore, we aimed to develop an NF system for the memory function of the MTL and examine neural activity changes, and memory task score changes through NF training. We created a memory NF system using intracranial electrodes to acquire and visualise the neural activity of the MTL during memory encoding. Twenty trials of a tug-of-war game per session were employed for NF and designed to control neural activity bidirectionally (Up/Down condition). NF training was conducted with three patients with intractable epilepsy, and we observed an increasing difference in NF signal between conditions (Up−Down) as NF training progressed. Similarities and negative correlation tendencies between the transition of neural activity and the transition of memory function were also observed. Our findings demonstrate NF's potential to modulate MTL activity and memory encoding. Future research needs further improvements to the NF system to validate its effects on memory functions. Nonetheless, this study represents a crucial step in understanding NF's application to memory and provides valuable insights for developing more efficient memory enhancement strategies.
Article Not peer-reviewed version
Paving the Way for Memory
Enhancement: Development and
Examination of a Neurofeedback
System Targeting the Medial
Temporal Lobe
Koji Koizumi * , Naoto Kunii , Kazutaka Ueda , Keisuke Nagata , Shigeta Fujitani , Seijiro Shimada ,
Masayuki Nakao
Posted Date: 28 June 2023
doi: 10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Keywords: neurofeedback; memory enhancement; medial temporal lobe; intracranial electrode;
bidirectional control; memory encoding; intracranial electroencephalogram; intractable epilepsy
Preprints.org is a free multidiscipline platform providing preprint service that
is dedicated to making early versions of research outputs permanently
available and citable. Preprints posted at Preprints.org appear in Web of
Science, Crossref, Google Scholar, Scilit, Europe PMC.
Copyright: This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons
Attribution License which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Paving the Way for Memory Enhancement: Development and Examination of
a Neurofeedback System Targeting the Medial Temporal Lobe
Koji Koizumi1*, Naoto Kunii2, Kazutaka Ueda1, Keisuke Nagata2, Shigeta Fujitani2, Seijiro
Shimada2, Masayuki Nakao1
1 Department of Mechanical Engineering, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan.
2 Department of Neurosurgery, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
* Correspondence: kojikoizumi1007@gmail.com
Abstract: Neurofeedback (NF) shows promise in enhancing memory, but its application to the
medial temporal lobe (MTL) still needs to be studied. Therefore, we aimed to develop an NF system
for the memory function of the MTL and examine neural activity changes, and memory task score
changes through NF training. We created a memory NF system using intracranial electrodes to
acquire and visualise the neural activity of the MTL during memory encoding. Twenty trials of a
tug-of-war game per session were employed for NF and designed to control neural activity
bidirectionally (Up/Down condition). NF training was conducted with three patients with
intractable epilepsy, and we observed an increasing difference in NF signal between conditions
(UpDown) as NF training progressed. Similarities and negative correlation tendencies between the
transition of neural activity and the transition of memory function were also observed. Our findings
demonstrate NF's potential to modulate MTL activity and memory encoding. Future research needs
further improvements to the NF system to validate its effects on memory functions. Nonetheless,
this study represents a crucial step in understanding NF's application to memory and provides
valuable insights for developing more efficient memory enhancement strategies.
Keywords: neurofeedback; memory enhancement; medial temporal lobe; intracranial electrode;
bidirectional control; memory encoding; intracranial electroencephalogram; intractable epilepsy
1. Introduction
Several studies targeting patients with lesions in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) have reported
impairments in short-term memory function [15]. Recent research evaluating working memory in
rats has reported that the coordinated neural activity patterns between the dentate gyrus and the
hippocampal CA3 region contribute to working memory [6], suggesting the potential involvement of
the MTL in short-term function. Additionally, it was reported that a heightened propensity for
impairments in spatial memory, integral to navigation and self-location, following damage to the
right MTL [7]. Conversely, when the left MTL is compromised, there is an increased likelihood of
deficits in verbal memory, essential for memorising the auditory and visual linguistic information.
In recent years, the technique of applying electrical stimulation to deep brain regions has been
established, leading to an increase in studies aiming to enhance memory function through electrical
stimulation of the MTL [8]. However, several previous studies investigating electrical stimulation
specifically targeting the hippocampus, the central region of the MTL, have reported a decline in
memory function [912], with inconsistent findings across studies. Hescham et al. have suggested
that electrical stimulations within the hippocampus may cause an acute depolarization block, leading
to memory impairment [9]. Akin to transcranial electrical stimulation, neurofeedback (NF) exists as
a method for modulating brain function. NF involves providing users with real-time feedback of their
brain activity, typically in the form of perceptible information such as bar length or circle size,
allowing individuals to self-regulate their brain activity. Through repeated NF training, it is expected
to facilitate the regulation of cognitive functions associated with brain activity. In recent years, NF
has gained momentum in clinical applications, with reports of symptom alleviation in various mental
disorders such as anxiety, depression, schizophrenia [13], and ADHD [14] by utilising brain activity,
a biomarker of symptoms, for NF training. Additionally, there has been an increase in opportunities
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions, and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and
contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting
from any ideas, methods, instructions, or products referred to in the content.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
© 2023 by the author(s). Distributed under a Creative Commons CC BY license.
for NF training in healthy individuals to enhance memory [15], meditation [16], and attentional focus
[17]. Since NF relies on the brain's autonomic learning capability to regulate neural activity, it offers
a potentially lower risk of interfering with memory function compared to electrical stimulation
methods. Moreover, anatomical evidence reveals that memory function involves multiple brain
regions beyond the hippocampus, such as the thalamus, mammillary bodies, and the cingulate gyrus
[18]. In order to regulate such complex neural networks, we believe that a closed-loop control system
that provides feedback on MTL neural activity which is closely related to the memory function to be
enhanced, and relies on the brain's autonomic learning capability, would be more rational and
effective compared to open-loop control systems that simply deliver external electrical stimulation.
Patients with intractable epilepsy, who undergo intracranial electrode placement through
craniotomy for the purpose of epileptic focus diagnosis, provide an opportunity for intracranial
electroencephalogram (iEEG) measurements. Recently, there has been increased interest in applying
this invasive brain activity measurement method to brain-computer interfaces [19]. Signals obtained
from intracranial electrodes offer superior temporal resolution compared to functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) and reduce the impact of artefacts compared to scalp EEG. These
advantages of intracranial electrodes can be utilised in NF studies, such as modulating
somatosensory-motor function by NF [2022] and modulating emotional function [23]. However,
these approaches are still in their early stages, and in fact, there are few NF studies focusing
specifically on memory function in the MTL [24].
1.1. Learning Mechanisms in Neurofeedback
The mechanism by which NF training allows for the adjustment of brain activity and brain
function is believed to be rooted in the learning mechanisms of operant conditioning [25]. Operant
conditioning, a fundamental concept in behavioural psychology, involves increasing or decreasing
the occurrence probability of a response under a specific condition by providing a consequence
(reinforcement or punishment) in response to voluntary behaviour (operant behaviour). In this
context, the "specific condition" serves as an antecedent that function as cue for the voluntary
response. Operant conditioning involves learning the association between these antecedent,
behaviour, and consequence. The association of these three elements is known as three-term
contingency.
In the context of NF, let us consider an illustrative scenario where the user can increase the length
of a bar displayed on the screen when a specific brain activity pattern occurs during a specific
cognitive condition. Through NF training, as the user engages in trial and error, they may accidentally
produce the desired brain activity pattern and achieve success in increasing the length of the bar.
Through repeated these experiences, the user's probability of generating the desired brain activity
pattern in a specific cognitive condition gradually increases. In other words, in NF, the user learns
the contingency between the specific cognitive condition (antecedent), the brain activity pattern
(behaviour), and the success in bar length adjustment (reinforcement).
The presence of consequence (reinforcement or punishment) is crucial for the success of operant
conditioning learning. If the reinforcements or punishments following operant behaviour are
insufficient, the changes in the occurrence probability of the response will be weak, making it difficult
for learning to take place. The importance of consequence can also be applied to the context of NF.
Davelaar [26] proposed a neurophysiological theory of NF, suggesting the significant contribution of
the striatum, a part of the reward system, to NF learning. Indeed, several NF studies have reported
the observed involvement of the striatum [2729].
1.2. Neurofeedback Research and Challenges in Memory Function
When constructing an NF system aimed at regulating neural activity related to memory
function, several challenges need to be considered. In this section, we summarise the challenges of
previous NF studies on memory function.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
1.2.1. Challenges in Brain Activity Measurement Methods
NF studies targeting memory function have extensively utilised scalp EEG [15,3037]. Memory
function has long been associated with theta oscillations [3840], and an increase in theta power
during memory encoding has been reported in numerous EEG studies [38,41,42]. Based on the
accumulation of such foundational research, many memory NF studies using EEG have implemented
NF to increase theta power. These EEG-NF studies have contributed to the understanding that the
upregulation of theta activity in the frontal midline is associated with improved control processes
during memory retrieval [31] and accelerated and enhanced memory consolidation [33,36], thus
accumulating knowledge regarding the relationship between theta oscillations and memory function.
Studies utilising intracranial electrodes have also reported increased theta power in the MTL during
successful encoding and retrieval [4349], and increased phase synchronization between the
hippocampus and other cortical regions (rhinal cortex, prefrontal cortex) during memory encoding
[50,51]. However, studies also report a decrease in MTL theta power during encoding [5258], and
the causal relationship between MTL theta power changes (increases or decreases) and memory
function (encoding success or failure) is inconsistent across studies. In our previous study [24], we
conducted NF using iEEG of MTL and reported an increase in theta power in one participant;
however, no accompanying changes in memory function were observed.
1.2.2. Challenges in Modulating Memory as a Cognitive Function
In NF studies aiming to improve mental disorders such as anxiety, depression, schizophrenia
[13], and ADHD [14], as well as enhance brain function in healthy individuals [1517], it is common
to perform NF during a resting state without imposing additional tasks on participants. This is
because the resting state already corresponds to the "specific cognitive condition" in the learning
mechanism of NF. In other words, in mental disorders such as anxiety, depression, and ADHD,
where chronic symptoms persist even during the resting state, NF during the resting state enables
the learning of three-term contingency. The same applies to healthy individuals training their
attention, relaxation, or meditation using NF. On the other hand, when conducting NF training for
memory function, the "specific cognitive condition" corresponds to the condition requiring memory
function. Therefore, it is essential to create a situation that necessitates memory function, such as
imposing memory tasks on participants, and conduct NF within that context to efficiently learn the
three-term contingency and achieve successful memory NF. Traditional memory NF studies have
often required several weeks or more for participants to gain self-regulation of brain activity [15,59].
We believe this may be attributed to the fact that previous memory NF studies have been conducted
during resting states, without creating a situation that necessitates memory function. Additionally,
during the resting state, participants lack clues regarding how to control their brain activity to adjust
the feedback signal, resulting in higher difficulty in learning self-regulation of brain function and
requiring a longer time for the effects to appear. If NF can be conducted within a context that
necessitates the desired brain function, rather than during the resting state, participants will have
clues on how to modulate their brain activity to change the feedback signal, making it easier to
acquire self-regulation of brain function in a shorter period.
However, when participants perform tasks requiring the desired brain function while
simultaneously receiving feedback, dual-task interference occurs, diverting attention away from the
feedback, making self-regulation difficult. Alternatively, participants may become overly focused on
the feedback, neglecting the task. To address these issues, a task-based NF approach has been
proposed [60,61], where the periods for task performance and feedback are separated, allowing for
alternating short periods of task engagement and feedback. This approach enables participants to
engage in tasks and feedback without divided attention and allows for conducting NF within a
context that requires the desired brain function. In this study, we adopt this task-based NF approach
to investigate whether the neural activity in the MTL associated with memory function can be
modulated.
1.3. Aims
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
This study aims to develop a task-based NF system, incorporating memory tasks and NF, and
to observe the control of neural activity and resulting effects on memory function through NF training
conducted to modulate MTL neural activity in both upward and downward directions.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
In this study, we targeted three patients with intractable epilepsy (P01, P02, P03) who underwent
intracranial electrode implantation for clinical purposes at the University of Tokyo Hospital. The
characteristics of each participant are shown in Table 1. The Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FIQ)
was evaluated using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-fourth edition (WAIS-IV). Language
dominance was assessed using functional MRI before electrode transplantation or cortical electrical
stimulation via subdural electrodes during language task [62]. Through these evaluation methods, it
was confirmed that the language-dominant hemisphere for all participants was the left hemisphere.
Since language dominance and language memory dominance are often concordant [63], it was
assumed that language memory dominance is also located in the left hemisphere. Prior to the surgery,
all participants underwent the Japanese version of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R),
with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. The index score for P01 was 51, confirming that
verbal memory function was impaired, which is speculated to be due to sclerosis in the left
hippocampus. The verbal memory of P02 and P03 was found to be within the normal range, with an
index score of 102 for P02 and 99 for P03, respectively.
This study was conducted with the approval of the Institutional Review Board of the University
of Tokyo Hospital (Approval No. 1797) and in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All
participants received sufficient explanations regarding the purpose and content of the study and
provided written informed consent.
Table 1. Participants characteristics.
ID
Age
Age at
onset
MRI
findings
Epileptic
focus
NF
target
Language
laterality
WMS-R verbal memory
index
P01
10s
6
Lt HS
Lt MTL
Rt MTL
Lt
51
P02
50s
17
No lesion
Obscure
Lt MTL
Lt
102
P03
50s
27
Rt HS
Rt MTL
Lt MTL
Lt
91
NF, neurofeedback; FIQ, full-scale intelligence quotient; WMS-R, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised;
Lt, left; Rt, right; HS, hippocampal sclerosis; MTL, medial temporal lobe.
2.2. Acquisition of iEEG Data
The placement of intracranial electrodes was determined based on clinical purposes, which
involved identifying the epileptogenic zone for each participant. The respective electrodes used and
their placement locations for each participant are described in the results section. iEEG data were
recorded using gUSBamp (gTec, Schiedlberg, Austria) at a sampling frequency of 512 Hz. At the time
of acquiring iEEG data, a bandpass filter of 0.1-200 Hz and a notch filter at 50 Hz were applied to the
data using the gUSBamp software settings to reduce power line noise.
2.3. Data Acquisition procedure
Prior to participating in the study, the participants watched an explanatory video and read an
instruction manual to ensure a sufficient understanding of the tasks. The participants were seated in
an electrically shielded room and instructed to look at a monitor screen displaying visual stimuli
during the tasks. The monitor screen was divided into upper and lower sections by a central line. The
upper screen displayed instructional text and the memory task, while the lower screen displayed NF
(Figure 1a).
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
At the beginning of each session, the participants were instructed to be relaxed while fixating on
a fixation point displayed in the centre of the upper screen for 20 seconds. The subsequent memory
NF paradigm consisted of 20 trials per session, with each trial comprising a memory task (encoding
period followed by recognition period) and an NF period (Figure 1b). During the encoding period,
five words were sequentially presented in the centre of the upper screen for 1.6 seconds each, with a
2.6-second interval between them. The participants were required to memorise these words. In the
subsequent recognition period, a single word was presented on the upper screen, and the participants
had to determine whether the word was one of the five words presented during the encoding period
(recognition task 1) and, if so, what number they saw it (recognition task 2). The question screens for
the recognition tasks were presented sequentially for 8 seconds each, and the participants provided
their responses using key inputs. In the following NF period, the neural activity in the medial
temporal lobe during the encoding period was visually presented as feedback on the lower screen in
the form of a tug-of-war game. The duration of each session was set to approximately 15 minutes,
considering the physical and mental fatigue of the patients.
Figure 1. Memory neurofeedback paradigm. (a) Example of monitor screen during the encoding
period. (b) One-trial sequence of memory neurofeedback and the process of online analysis. NF,
neurofeedback; ASR, Automatic Subspace Reconstruction.2.4. Memory task
The words selected for the memory task were obtained from the Word List by Semantic
Principles (WLDP) version 1.0, provided by the Centre for Language Resource Development,
National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics [64]. This lexical database comprises 96,557
words and employs crowdsourcing and Bayesian linear mixed models to estimate word familiarity
[65]. Initially, Japanese hiragana or katakana words consisting of three letters or three syllables were
extracted to form multiple sets of six words. The word combinations were adjusted to ensure an equal
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
average familiarity score across the sets. Each session included twenty word sets, with one set
assigned per trial. twenty word sets were extracted so that they would not overlap across sessions.
During the encoding period, five out of the six words in each set were presented. In the
subsequent recognition period, one of the five words presented during encoding was displayed as
an "old" stimulus, while the remaining word not presented during encoding was shown as a "new"
stimulus. Out of the 20 trials, 10 trials featured the presentation of "old" stimuli and the other 10 trials
featured the presentation of "new" stimuli. The sequence of trials with either "old" or "new" stimuli
was randomised.
Regarding recognition task 1, the accuracy score (the probability of correctly identifying whether
the stimulus was "old" or "new" among all responses) and the recall score (the probability of correctly
identifying an "old" stimulus as an "old" stimulus) were calculated. For recognition task 2, the
accuracy score (i.e., the probability of correctly identifying the order in which the "old" stimulus was
presented in task 1) was computed. As an example, in the trial depicted in Figure 1b, "Tuna" was the
first word presented among the five, indicating that the correct answer for task 2 is "first."
2.5. Neurofeedback
Real-time analysis and feedback of the acquired iEEG data were performed using a custom
program developed with g.HIsys (gTec, Schiedlberg, Austria) and MATLAB R2020a (MathWorks,
Natick, MA, USA). During the memory encoding period of 13 seconds ((1.0+1.6) s × 5 words), iEEG
data were extracted, and an artifact subspace reconstruction [66,67] (ASR) was applied to reduce
epileptic spike-related artifacts. The ASR parameters were adjusted based on the extent of epileptic
spike contamination. Specifically, waveform correction was performed with k = 10 when the
proportion of brain wave potentials exceeding the mean ± 3 standard deviations for 13 seconds was
less than 1%, with k = 6 when it was between 1% and 1.5%, and with k = 4 when it exceeded 1.5%.
Subsequently, the 12 seconds of iEEG data, excluding the initial 1-second period in which the
encoding words were not presented, were used to calculate the ratio of low theta band power (2-6
Hz) to the total power (0.5-32 Hz). The average ratio of low theta band power (LTR) across four
electrodes was then computed. For each trial (i), the relative magnitude of LTRi to LTR1 (LTRi/1 = 100
× LTRi/ LTR1) was used as the neurofeedback (NF) signal.
We employed a bidirectional control NF system and introduced gamification and reward
elements. The reason for adding gamification and reward elements is that attention, motivation, and
mood affect efficient learning and success in NF [68]. Specifically, we employed a tug-of-war game
with 20 trials per session, where a larger LTRi/1 value caused the rope to be pulled towards the right
(red team side), while a smaller LTRi/1 value resulted in the rope being pulled towards the left (white
team side), with the marker moving right and left accordingly (Figure 2a). Participants were
instructed that their brain activity during word memorization would be utilised for a tug-of-war
game, and the outcome would be determined after each trial. The participants were also informed
about which team to root for before the start of each session (Up/Down conditions based on the
direction of adjusting the LTRi/1 value) and were encouraged to strive for win through trial and error.
The Up and Down conditions were alternated between sessions. For P01 and P03, odd-numbered
sessions were assigned to Up and even-numbered sessions to Down, while for P02, it was the reverse.
The research conductors did not provide explicit instructions regarding mental strategies to the
participants, and the correspondence between the Up/Down conditions and the winning/losing
teams was blinded. Additionally, the research conductors and data analysts were separate
individuals, and the analysts were blinded to the information regarding which condition was used
in each session. When the supported team won, 10 points were added to their score. Moreover, if the
LTRi changed by more than 20% from LTR1 and the supported team won, the score was doubled (20
points), accompanied by the playback of cheering audio. Conversely, if the supported team lost, 10
points were added to the opponent's score, and if the LTRi changed by more than 20% from LTR1 and
resulted in the loss of the supported team, the opponent's score was doubled (20 points). A score table
was displayed at the top of the tug-of-war screen, allowing the participants to monitor the cumulative
scores for their supported team as the trials progressed. Figure 2b illustrates the transition of the NF
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
screen from the encoding period to the NF period in trial i of sessions rooting for the red team (Up
condition). After completing all 20 trials, the outcome of each session was determined based on the
cumulative scores. When the supported team won, a celebration screen and a loud cheer were
displayed as audio. In the case of a draw, only a notification was displayed informing the result of a
draw. In case of loss, a screen indicating disappointment was shown.
Figure 2. Neurofeedback visualization in tug-of-war. (a) Design of Neurofeedback screen. (b)
Transition of the NF screen from the encoding period to the NF period in trial i of sessions rooting for
the red team (Up condition). LTR, the average ratio of low theta band power; NF, neurofeedback.2.6.
Offline Data Analysis
To examine how the difference in NF signal values (LTRi/1) between the Up and Down conditions
changed before and after NF training, the difference in median NF signal values between trials was
calculated for each condition (Up and Down) in both the first and final sessions. Subsequently, these
differences were compared between the first and final sessions.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Next, the transition of NF signal values (LTRi/1) between sessions and within sessions was
examined for each participant. The transition between sessions for LTRi was also investigated.
Statistical analyses were conducted to determine if there were significant increases or decreases in
the transition of these values between sessions. Nonparametric tests were employed for all statistical
analyses conducted across all participants due to the lack of normality in the distribution of some
data, as assessed using the Shapiro-Wilk test. The significance level was set at α = 0.05. For each
session, the median of LTRi/1, which represented the NF signal values from trial 2 to trial 20, was
compared to the baseline, the LTR1/1 value (100) of trial 1, using the two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank
test. Additionally, the Wilcoxon rank-sum test was employed to examine if there were differences
between the conditions at the same session number, and if there were changes across sessions within
the same condition. The effect size (r) was calculated as the Z-statistic divided by the square root of
the sample size (N). Spearman's rank correlation was utilised to investigate the associations between
the changes in the three scores obtained from the recognition tasks and the changes in physiological
data (NF signal values and LTR). For the correlation analysis, only the trials relevant to each score
were selected, and their median NF signal values and median LTR values were calculated for each
session. Multiple comparisons were adjusted using the Holm-Bonferroni method, with a significance
level set at α = 0.05. Furthermore, since EEG tend to exhibit larger power in lower frequencies, there
is a possibility that the delta band (0.5-2Hz), which is even lower in frequency than the targeted low
theta band for NF, may influence the transitions of LTR. Therefore, we investigated the transitions of
the total power (0.5-32Hz), low theta power, and delta power between sessions.
To examine the frequency and temporal characteristics of neural activity during memory
encoding, a time-frequency analysis was performed. The iEEG power spectrum (obtained through
fast Fourier transform) and power spectrum using complex Morlet wavelets ( 󰇛󰇜, t: time,
f: frequencies increasing logarithmically from 1 Hz to 64 Hz with 31 log-spaced steps, σ=n/2πf:
bandwidth of each frequency band, n: wavelet cycle numbers increasing logarithmically from 4 to 10)
were multiplied for each trial data of each electrode. Then, the inverse fast Fourier transform
(convolution in the frequency domain) was applied to obtain the time-frequency representation. The
squared magnitude of the convolution results was defined as the power estimate at each time point
and frequency band. After that, the power estimates from the four electrodes were averaged,
resulting in time-frequency features for each trial. To investigate the time-frequency characteristics
related to memory encoding, trials in each session were divided into correct trials (Correct) and
incorrect trials (Error) based on the accuracy of recognition task 2. Specifically, the median of each
Correct/Error trial was calculated for each session. Then, the average power estimates from -600ms
to -100ms before the presentation of the first word were considered as the baseline activity. Using
this baseline activity, the power estimates during memory encoding were Z-transformed and
normalised for each condition. By calculating the average of the Z-scores across sessions for Correct
and Error trials, the time-frequency characteristics of each condition were obtained. Furthermore, the
difference in time-frequency characteristics between Correct and Error trials was assessed by
subtracting the Z-scores of Error trials from Correct trials for each session and calculating the average
across sessions. It is worth noting that statistical comparisons were not conducted due to constraints
on the number of sessions.
Similarly, the time-frequency characteristics of the two conditions (Up/Down) were compared
during the two NF training stages (first/final). For each combination of condition and stage, the
median values of trials, excluding trial 1 and trials contaminated with noise, were calculated. Then
the average power estimates from -600ms to -100ms before the first word presentation were used as
the baseline activity and underwent Z-score transformation. By the standardised, the time-frequency
characteristics of each condition were obtained. The differences between the two groups
(condition/stage) were statistically compared using nonparametric randomization tests. In the
randomization tests, first, the time-frequency maps of each trial were shuffled between the two
groups, and the test statistic t was obtained for each time-frequency pixel. By performing 1000
shuffles, the mean and standard deviation of the test statistics t for each pixel were calculated. The
actual t-values were then converted to z-scores based on the mean and standard deviation from the
shuffling procedure, creating a time-frequency map representing the differences between the two
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
groups. Cluster-based multiple comparison corrections were applied to identify statistically
significant spatiotemporal clusters. In this process, the maximum cluster size formed by pixels
exceeding the threshold (α = 0.05) of the t-distribution in each shuffle out of 1000 shuffles was
extracted to obtain the null distribution of cluster sizes. Subsequently, the threshold was set at the
95th percentile of this null distribution, and clusters of pixels exceeding this threshold were
considered statistically significant as large clusters.
3. Results
System errors or significant noise contamination led to the exclusion of trials or sessions where
proper NF could not be achieved. Additionally, trials in which key input was missed, resulting in an
inability to confirm responses for the recognition task, and trials where effective NF was not possible
were excluded during the calculation of recognition performance.
The difference in NF signal values for the Up condition relative to the Down condition (Up
Down) is shown in Figure 3. In the first session, the NF signal difference varied among participants,
ranging from positive values for P01 (27.35%) to negative values for P03 (52.66%), with P02 having
a value of 1.90%. In the final session, all participants exhibited positive NF signal differences and
showed larger values compared to the first session , with P01 at 42.35%, P02 at 40.25%, and P03 at
6.82%.
Figure 3. Changes of neurofeedback signal difference (Up Down).
The following section provides the results for each participant.
3.1. Results for P01
P01 completed four sessions of memory NF training over a span of five days. After the series of
NF training, P01 underwent surgery to remove the left temporal lobe, resulting in the cessation of
seizures. Post-surgery, the verbal memory Index Score on the WMS-R was below 50 and showed a
decline compared to pre-surgery levels.
After the end of session 2, it was reported that in the course of trial and error in the NF, P01
adopted a strategy of randomly answering the recognition task. Although we asked P01 to avoid
random responses from sessions 3 onwards, careful interpretation of the scores of the recognition
task in Session 2 is necessary. There were no mentions of mental strategies from session 3 onwards.
Furthermore, in session 3, due to a system error, the baseline for the NF signal was referenced not
from the encoding period data of trial 1, but from a period preceding it with higher LTR values
(specifically, 153.81% when LTR1 was set as 100%). Consequently, in session 3, NF was conducted
under conditions that made it difficult for the red team to win. Since NF was consistent with LTR
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
values in the MTL, it was not excluded from the analysis. However, careful interpretation of the
results in session 3 is required.
3.1.1. Intracranial Electrodes Used for Memory NF (P01)
In P01, deep electrodes (Unique Medical, Tokyo, Japan) were implanted in the right MTL. The
iEEG data were measured from four platinum electrodes (1mm in length), positioned at 5mm
intervals (centre to centre) from the tip located in the right hippocampus, and used for NF (Figure 4).
A reference electrode was placed subcutaneously on the right side for P01.
Figure 4. Electrodes of P01 used for neurofeedback.
3.1.2. Performance Changes in the Recognition Task (P01)
The session-to-session changes of the three scores for the recognition task are shown in Figure
5. The accuracy of recognition task 1 significantly decreased in Session 2, where random responding
occurred. The recall score was equivalent to Sessions 3 and 4. Comparing Up and Down conditions,
both in the first sessions (Session 1 vs. Session 2) and final sessions (Session 3 vs. Session 4), the
accuracy of recognition task 2 was higher for Up than for Down, and the difference increased in the
final sessions.
Figure 5. Changes of recognition task performance of P01 across sessions.
3.1.3. NF Signal Changes across Sessions (P01)
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
The session-to-session changes in the NF signal values (LTRi/1) are presented in Figure 6. The NF
signal values were significantly higher for Up in Session 1 (Mdn = 124.81, n = 19, z = 4.118, p < 0.001, r
= 0.945) and Session 3 (Mdn = 137.81, n = 18, z = 4.475, p < 0.001, r = 1.055) compared to the baseline
(LTR1/1), with large effect sizes. However, there were no significant changes in the Down condition in
Session 2 (Mdn = 95.82, n = 18, z = 1.835, p = 0.067, r = 0.432) and Session 4 (Mdn = 95.45, n = 16, z =
1.197, p = 0.231, r = 0.299) compared to the reference. When comparing Up and Down conditions for
the same session number, both the first session (z = 4.467, p < 0.001, r = 0.734) and second session (z =
4.934, p < 0.001, r = 0.846) showed significantly higher values for Up than for Down, with large effect
sizes. In the Up condition, the NF signal values significantly increased with each session, showing a
large effect size (z = 3.312, p < 0.001, r = 0.545). However, there were no significant changes between
sessions in the Down condition (z = 0.035, p = 0.986, r = 0.06).
Figure 6. Changes of NF signal of P01 across sessions.
The similarity between the performance changes in recognition task 2 and the NF signal changes
was observed. However, the results of the statistical analysis showed no significant correlations
between the performance on the recognition task and the NF signal values for the recognition task 1
accuracy (r = 0.80, p = 0.33), recognition task 1 recall (r = 0.63, p = 0.37), and recognition task 2 accuracy
(r = 1.00, p = 0.08). Furthermore, there were no significant correlations between the performance on
the recognition task and the LTR values for the recognition task 1 accuracy (r = 0.00, p = 1.00),
recognition task 1 recall (r = 0.32, p = 0.68), and recognition task 2 accuracy (r = 0.80, p = 0.33).
3.1.4. NF Signal Transition within Sessions (P01)
Regarding the first and final sessions, we compared the within-session transitions of NF signal
values between Up and Down conditions (Figure 7). Both in the first and final sessions, the NF signal
values exhibited higher overall trends in the Up condition compared to the Down condition across
trials. Additionally, in the Up condition, there was a steeper rise in NF signal values from trial 1
during the final session compared to the first session.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 7. Changes of NF signal of P01 within session.(a) The first sessions. (b) The final sessions.
3.1.5. Low Theta Power Ratio (LTR) Changes across Sessions (P01)
The LTR1, which served as the baseline, showed approximately a 10% difference across
conditions: 33.7% in Session 1, 43.4% in Session 2, 32.8% in Session 3, and 41.6% in Session 4. A
comparison between conditions was performed for the LTRi values from Trial 2 onwards, similar to
the NF signal analysis (Figure 8). When comparing Up and Down conditions for the same session
number, there were no significant differences in the first session (z = 0.091, p = 0.940, r = 0.015).
However, in the second session, Up showed significantly higher values than Down, with a moderate
effect size (z = 2.691, p = 0.006, r = 0.462). In the Up condition, the LTRi values significantly increased
with each session, showing a moderate effect size (z = 2.553, p = 0.01, r = 0.419). However, there were
no significant changes between sessions in the Down condition (z = 0.897, p = 0.384, r = 0.154).
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 8. Changes of Low theta power ratio of P01 across sessions.
3.1.6. Frequency Band Power Changes across Sessions (P01)
The transitions between sessions of total power, delta power, and low theta power are presented
in Figure 9. Both delta power and low theta power had similar median magnitudes. All frequency
band powers showed a trend of higher median values in the Up condition compared to the Down
condition. The patterns of total power and low theta power changes exhibited similarities to the NF
signal changes and the performance changes in recognition task 2.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 9. Changes of band power of P01 across sessions.(a) total power (0.5-32Hz). (b) delta
power (0.5-2Hz). (c) low theta power (2-6Hz).
3.1.7. Time-Frequency Maps for Correct and Error Trials (P01)
The time-frequency maps for Correct and Error trials are shown in Figure 10a,b. Both maps
exhibited intermittent power increases in the theta and alpha bands. When comparing Correct to
Error trials, it was observed that during the mid-phase of the encoding period, the low theta band
power increased before stimulus presentation and decreased during stimulus presentation (Figure
10c).
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 10. Time-frequency maps of P01 during memory encoding. (a) the correct trials, (b) the error
trials, and (c) the difference (Correct trials Error trials). Time-frequency spectral power was
normalised to the baseline activity (0.6 to 0.1 sec), with time represented on the x-axis (0.6 to 12
sec, with the first stimulus presentation at 0) and frequency on the y-axis. The solid black vertical line
labelled "ws" indicates the onset of each stimulus presentation, while the number between "w" and
"s" denotes the number of the stimulus. The dotted black vertical line labelled "e" represents the offset
of each stimulus presentation. The two red horizontal dotted lines represent the lower and upper
boundaries of the low theta band (2 Hz and 6 Hz).
3.1.8. Time-Frequency Maps for Up/Down and First/Final Sessions (P01)
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
The time-frequency maps for the Up and Down conditions, as well as the first and final sessions,
along with the difference maps between sessions, are presented in Figure 11. In comparison between
the Up and Down conditions, in both the first and final sessions, sustained power increases in the
gamma band were observed for the Up condition compared to the Down condition. Additionally,
intermittent power increases in the alpha and beta bands during the middle of the encoding period
were observed for the Down condition in the first sessions. However, in the final sessions, the Up
condition exhibited more consistent power increases from the low theta to beta bands. When
comparing the first and final sessions, power increases in the Up condition and power decreases in
the Down condition from the low theta to beta bands were observed during the middle of the
encoding period. Furthermore, in the Up condition, a significantly large cluster indicating a decrease
in delta band power at the end of the encoding period was observed for the final session compared
to the first session.
Figure 11. Time-frequency maps of P01 (Up/ Down, the first/ final sessions, and the difference). Figure
caption omitted as it duplicates Figure 10's caption. The area enclosed by the solid white line indicates
clusters of significantly larger size.
3.2. Results for P02
P02 completed six sessions of memory NF training over a span of five days. After the series of
NF training, P02 did not undergo surgery; therefore, the WMS-R assessment was not conducted post-
surgery.
During sessions 1 and 2, it was reported that P02 was occupied with memorising the words.
After sessions 5 and 6, reports indicated that the white team tended to win when P02 remembered
the words with relaxation without concentrating too much. In contrast, when P02 concentrated hard
to memorise, the red team seemed to win. Regarding session 4, it was observed that artificial noise
with high amplitude was mixed during trial 1, making it difficult to adjust noise reduction using
ASR. Since the NF signal values used trial 1's LTR as the baseline, session 4 was excluded from the
analysis, and any trials with confirmed artificial noise contamination were also excluded.
3.2.1. Intracranial Electrodes Used for Memory NF (P02)
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
In P02, subdural electrodes (Unique Medical, Tokyo, Japan) were implanted in the left MTL,
covering the hippocampus. Four platinum electrodes (1.5mm in diameter) were longitudinally
placed at 5mm intervals (centre to centre) along the left parahippocampal gyrus, and iEEG was
measured and used for NF (Figure 12). A reference electrode placed on the dural side of the right
temporal lobe was used for P02.
Figure 12. Electrodes of P02 used for neurofeedback.
3.2.2. Performance Changes in the Recognition Task (P02)
The session-to-session changes of the three scores for the recognition task are shown in Figure
13. The recall score for recognition task 1 slightly decreased from session 1 to 2, but all aspects of the
task showed improvement from session 1 to 3 and a decline from session 5 to 6.
Figure 13. Changes of recognition task performance of P02 across sessions.
3.2.3. NF Signal Changes across Sessions (P02)
The session-to-session changes in NF signal values (LTRi/1) are presented in Figure 14. The NF
signal values were significantly higher for Up in session 2 (Mdn = 153.49, n = 19, z = 4.621, p < 0.001, r
= 1.060) compared to the baseline (LTR1/1) with a large effect size. However, in session 6, the NF signal
values for Up were not significantly higher (Mdn = 117.91, n = 17, z = 2.216, p = 0.027, r = 0.538). For
Down, the NF signal values were significantly higher in session 1 (Mdn = 151.58, n = 18, z = 3.800, p <
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
0.001, r = 0.896) compared to the baseline, with a large effect size. In session 3, there were no
significant changes (Mdn = 101.37, n = 19, z = 0.00, p = 1.00, r = 0.00), while in session 5, the NF signal
values were significantly lower compared to the baseline, with a large effect size (Mdn = 77.65, n = 19,
z = 3.834, p < 0.001, r = 0.880). When comparing Up and Down conditions for the same session
number, there were no significant differences in the first session (z = 0.091, p = 0.940, r = 0.015).
However, in the final session, Up showed significantly higher values than Down, with a large effect
size (z = 4.009, p < 0.001, r = 0.668). It is noteworthy that in the Up condition, there was a significant
decrease in NF signal values as sessions progressed, with a large effect size (z = 3.565, p < 0.001, r =
0.594). In the Down condition, NF signal values decreased significantly as sessions progressed, with
a large effect size (session 1 > session 3, z = 3.829, p < 0.001, r = 0.629; session 1 > session 5, z = 4.619, p
< 0.001, r = 0.759; session 3 > session 5, z = 3.547, p < 0.001, r = 0.575).
Figure 14. Changes of NF signal of P02 across sessions.
Negative correlations were observed between the performance changes in the recognition task
and the NF signal changes. However, the statistical analysis revealed no significant correlations
between the performance on the recognition task and the NF signal values for recognition task 1
accuracy (r = 0.10, p = 0.95), recognition task 1 recall (r = 0.10, p = 0.87), and recognition task 2
accuracy (r = 0.87, p = 0.05). Furthermore, there were no significant correlations between the
performance on the recognition task and the LTR values for recognition task 1 accuracy (r = 0.3, p =
0.68), recognition task 1 recall (r = 0.05, p = 0.93), and recognition task 2 accuracy (r = 0.10, p = 0.87).
3.2.4. NF Signal Transition within Sessions (P02)
For the first and final sessions, the within-session transitions of NF signal values were compared
between Up and Down conditions (Figure 15). In the first session, both the Up and Down conditions
exhibited higher values throughout almost all trials compared to trial 1. The Up condition had a
greater number of trials surpassing trial 1 compared to the Down condition. On the other hand,
during the final session, the NF signal value mostly exceeded that of trial 1 in the Up condition, while
remaining lower than trial 1 in the Down condition across most trials.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 15. Changes of NF signal of P02 within session.(a) The first sessions. (b) The final
sessions.
3.2.5. Low Theta Power Ratio (LTR) Changes across Sessions (P02)
The baseline LTR1 varied by a few percent between sessions: 5.65% in session 1, 6.04% in session
2, 7.23% in session 3, 10.02% in session 5, and 6.06% in session 6. A comparison between conditions
was performed for LTRi values from trial 2 onwards, similar to the NF signal analysis (Figure 16).
When comparing Up and Down conditions for the same session number, no significant differences
were observed in either the first session (z = 1.216, p = 0.233, r = 0.200) or the final session (z = 0.650,
p = 0.531, r = 0.108). In the Up condition, as sessions progressed, there was a significant decrease in
LTRi values, with a large effect size (z = 3.565, p < 0.001, r = 0.594). However, there were no significant
changes between sessions in the Down condition (session 1 vs session 3, z = 1.793, p = 0.075, r = 0.295;
session 1 vs session 5, z = 0.881, p = 0.391, r = 0.145; session 3 vs session 5, z = 0.803, p = 0.435, r =
0.130).
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 16. Changes in Low Theta Power Ratio of P02 across sessions.
3.2.6. Frequency Band Power Changes across Sessions (P02)
The transitions between sessions of total power, delta power, and low theta power are presented
in Figure 17. It is important to note that the vertical scale of the low theta power graph differs from
the graphs of other frequency band powers. Delta power was consistently higher than low theta
power throughout the sessions. When comparing the trends of NF signal values with each frequency
band power, a negative correlation trend can be observed with all frequency bands, particularly with
low theta power. Furthermore, the patterns of low theta power changes exhibited similarities to the
performance changes in recognition task 2.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 17. Changes in band power of P02 across sessions. (a) Total power (0.5-32Hz). (b) Delta power
(0.5-2Hz). (c) Low theta power (2-6Hz).
3.2.7. Time-Frequency Maps for Correct and Error Trials (P02)
The time-frequency maps for Correct and Error trials are shown in Figure 18a,b. Both maps
exhibited intermittent power increases in the theta, alpha, and high beta bands. It was observed that
Correct trials displayed stronger and sustained power increases (Figure 18c).
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 18. Time-frequency maps of P02 during memory encoding. (a) the correct trials, (b) the error
trials, and (c) the difference (Correct trials Error trials). Figure caption omitted as it duplicates Figure
10's caption.
3.2.8. Time-Frequency Maps for Up/Down and First/Final Sessions (P02)
The time-frequency maps for the Up and Down conditions, as well as the first and final sessions,
along with the difference maps between sessions, are presented in Figure 19. In comparison between
the Up and Down conditions, sustained power increases in the gamma band were observed for the
Down condition compared to the Up condition, both in the first and final sessions. In the first session,
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
a slight power increase from the low theta to beta frequency bands was observed for the Up condition
compared to the Down condition. However, a significantly large cluster indicating a power decrease
in the delta frequency band for the Up condition compared to the Down condition spread from the
early to the mid-encoding period. In the final session, the Down condition exhibited a power increase
compared to the Up condition across all frequency bands starting before the presentation of the first-
word stimulus. Additionally, a significantly large cluster indicating increased powers from the low
theta to alpha frequency bands was observed for the Down condition compared to the Up condition
during the early-encoding period. When comparing the first and final sessions, regardless of the Up
or Down conditions, a power increase from the low theta to low gamma frequency bands was
observed in the final session compared to the first session. In the Up condition, a significantly large
cluster indicating a power increase was observed for the final session compared to the first session
during the late-encoding period, spanning from the low theta to alpha frequency bands. Similarly, in
the Down condition, a significantly large cluster indicating a power increase was observed for the
final session compared to the first session, spreading from the early to late-encoding periods,
encompassing the low theta to alpha frequency bands, and extending to the gamma frequency band
during specific time periods.
Figure 19. Time-frequency maps of P02 (Up/ Down, the first/ final sessions, and the difference). Figure
caption omitted as it duplicates Figure 10's caption. The area enclosed by the solid white line indicates
clusters of significantly larger size.
3.3. Results for P03
P03 completed seven sessions of memory NF training over a span of four days. After the series
of NF training, P03 underwent right selective hippocampal-amygdala resection surgery, which
resulted in the disappearance of seizures. In the post-surgery WMS-R assessment, the verbal memory
index score was 92, showing a small change from the preoperative score of 91, indicating that verbal
memory function was not impaired.
Throughout all sessions, P03 focused on memorization during the encoding period, and reports
indicated that P03 did not find out the strategy to make the supporting team win until the end. There
was a report of slight distraction after session 4. For session 5, it was observed that artificial noise
with high amplitude was mixed during trial 1, making it difficult to adjust noise reduction using
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
ASR. Since the NF signal values used trial 1's LTR as the baseline, session 5 was excluded from the
analysis.
3.3.1. Intracranial Electrodes Used for Memory NF (P03)
In P03, subdural electrodes (Unique Medical, Tokyo, Japan) were implanted in the left MTL.
Four platinum electrodes (3.0mm in diameter) were laterally placed at 10mm intervals (centre to
centre) along the left parahippocampal gyrus, and intracranial EEG was measured and used for NF
(Figure 20). A reference electrode placed on the dural side of the left temporoparietal lobe was used
for P03.
Figure 20. Electrodes of P03 used for neurofeedback.
3.3.2. Performance Changes in the Recognition Task (P03)
The session-to-session changes of the three scores for the recognition task are shown in Figure
21. The performance of the recognition task 1, for both accuracy and recall, decreased from session 1
to 2 and then increased. In contrast, the accuracy of the recognition task 2 decreased from sessions 1
to 3 and then increased from sessions 4 to 6.
Figure 21. Changes of recognition task performance of P03 across sessions.
3.3.3. NF Signal Changes across Sessions (P03)
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
The session-to-session changes in NF signal values (LTRi/1) are presented in Figure 22. The
medians of the NF signal values were higher for Up compared to the baseline (LTR1/1) in session 1
(Mdn = 115.29, n = 19, z = 2.140, p = 0.032, r = 0.491), session 3 (Mdn = 103.34, n = 19, z = 0.850, p = 0.396,
r = 0.195), and session 7 (Mdn = 101.26, n = 19, z = 0.295, p = 0.768, r = 0.068), although not significantly.
However, in session 2 (Mdn = 167.96, n = 19, z = −4.621, p < 0.001, r = 1.060) and session 4 (Mdn = 140.12,
n = 19, z = −4.621, p < 0.001, r = 1.060) for the Down condition, the NF signal values were significantly
higher compared to the baseline, with a large effect size. In session 6 (Mdn = 94.43, n = 19, z = −0.850,
p = 0.396, r = 0.195), the median of the NF signal values for Down was lower than the baseline, but
not significantly. When comparing Up and Down conditions for the same session number, Down
showed significantly lower values than Up in the first session (z = −4.744, p < 0.001, r = 0.770), as well
as in the second session (z = −4.160, p < 0.001, r = 0.675), with a large effect size. However, in the final
session, there was no significant difference in NF signal values between Up and Down (z = 0.336, p =
0.751, r = 0.054). In the Up condition, the NF signal values were significantly lower at the final session
(session 7) than at the first session (session 1), with a moderate effect size (z = −2.409, p = 0.015, r =
0.391). There were no significant changes in NF signal values between other sessions of Up condition
(session 1 vs session 3, z = 1.036, p = 0.311, r = 0.168; session 3 vs session 7, z = −1.445, p = 0.154, r =
0.234). In the Down condition, NF signal values decreased significantly as sessions progressed, with
a large effect size (session 2 vs session 4, z = 3.606, p < 0.001, r = 0.585; session 2 vs session 6, z = −5.270,
p < 0.001, r = 0.855; session 4 vs session 6, z = −5.065, p < 0.001, r = 0.822).
Figure 22. Changes of NF signals of P03 across sessions.
There were no significant correlations between the performance on the recognition task and the NF
signal values for recognition task 1 accuracy (r = −0.70, p = 0.12), recognition task 1 recall (r = −0.72, p
= 0.11), and recognition task 2 accuracy (r = 0.03, p = 1.00). Furthermore, there were no significant
correlations between the performance on the recognition task and the LTR values for recognition task 1
accuracy (r = −0.15, p = 0.77), recognition task 1 recall (r = 0.48, p = 0.34), and recognition task 2 accuracy
(r = −0.26, p = 0.66).
3.3.4. NF Signal Transition within Sessions (P03)
For the first and final sessions, the within-session transitions of NF signal values were compared
between Up and Down conditions (Figure 23). In the first session, the Up condition initially showed
a lower trend but gradually increased above trial 1 as the session progressed. On the other hand, the
Down condition consistently exhibited higher values than trial 1 throughout all trials, showing higher
values than the Up condition. However, in the final session, neither the Up nor Down condition
showed significant increases nor decreases from trial 1.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 23. Changes of NF signal of P03 within session.a) The first sessions. (b) The final sessions.
3.3.5. Low Theta Power Ratio (LTR) Changes across Sessions (P03)
The baseline LTR1 varied between sessions: 25.01% in session 1, 20.69% in session 2, 28.18% in
session 3, 25.01% in session 4, 34.81% in session 6, and 34.41% in session 7, with late sessions being
larger than early and mid sessions. A comparison between conditions was performed for LTRi values
from trial 2 onwards, similar to the NF signal values (Figure 24). The comparison of conditions for
the same session number revealed that the Down condition had significantly higher LTR values than
the Up condition in the first session (z = −2.817, p = 0.004, r = 0.457) and second session (z = −2.701, p =
0.006, r = 0.438), with a moderate effect size. In the Up condition, there were no significant differences
between sessions (session 1 vs. session 3, z = −0.219, p = 0.840, r = 0.036; session 1 vs. session 7, z =
−1.825, p = 0.070, r = 0.296; session 3 vs. session 7, z = −1.562, p = 0.123, r = 0.253). Similarly, in the Down
condition, there were no significant differences between sessions (session 2 vs. session 4, z = −0.511, p
= 0.624, r = 0.083; session 2 vs. session 6, z = 0.423, p = 0.686, r = 0.069; session 4 vs. session 6, z = 1.036,
p = 0.311, r = 0.168).
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 24. Changes of Low theta power ratio of P03 across sessions.
3.3.6. Frequency Band Power Changes across Sessions (P03)
The changes in total power, delta power, and low theta power across sessions are illustrated in
Figure 25. In the early sessions, delta power showed a more prominent difference than low theta
power between Up and Down conditions (Up > Down). Negative correlations were observed
between all band powers compared to the changes in NF signal values.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 25. Changes of band power of P03 across sessions.(a) total power (0.5-32Hz). (b) delta
power (0.5-2Hz). (c) low theta power (2-6Hz).
3.3.7. Time-Frequency Maps of Correct/Error Trials during Encoding Period (P03)
Time-frequency maps for Correct and Error trials are shown in Figure 26a,b. Both maps display
intermittent power increases in the theta and alpha bands, with Error trials exhibiting stronger and
more sustained power (Figure 26c).
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 26. Time-frequency maps of P03 during memory encoding. (a) the correct trials, (b) the error
trials, and (c) the difference (Correct trials Error trials). Figure caption omitted as it duplicates Figure
10's caption.
3.3.8. Time-Frequency Maps for Up/Down and First/Final Sessions (P03)
The time-frequency maps for the Up and Down conditions, as well as the first and final sessions,
along with the difference maps between sessions, are presented in Figure 27. In comparison between
the Up and Down conditions, both in the first and final sessions, the Up condition showed more
continuous power increases across a wide frequency range from delta to gamma. In the first session,
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
a significantly large cluster indicating a power increase, primarily centred around the delta and theta
bands, extended throughout the entire encoding period, expanding to the gamma band during
specific time periods. In comparison between the first and final sessions, both Up and Down
conditions showed power increases from the low theta to beta bands in the final sessions compared
to the first session. In the Up condition, a significantly large cluster indicating a power increase was
observed during the early-encoding period, covering the low theta to beta band. On the other hand,
in the Down condition, a significantly large cluster indicating a power increase was observed
throughout the entire encoding period, covering the low theta to alpha bands, expanding to the beta
band during specific time periods.
Figure 27. Time-frequency maps of P03 (Up/ Down, the first/ final sessions, and the difference). Figure
caption omitted as it duplicates Figure 10's caption. The area enclosed by the solid white line indicates
clusters of significantly larger size.
4. Discussion
In this study, we administered bidirectional NF training aimed at increasing and decreasing
(Up/Down conditions) the low theta power ratio (LTR) of the MTL in three patients with intractable
epilepsy. We observed that the difference in NF signals between the conditions (Up−Down) increased
as sessions progressed. The NF signal changed significantly only in one direction (Up or Down) for
all participants, yet the opposite-direction NF served as an important intra-subject control condition
demonstrating the NF effect, enabling us to exclude potential confounders such as motivation, visual
modality, and eye movement as their influences were almost identical across conditions.
The reason why changes in the session progression of NF signal values were observed only in
one direction can be attributed to the effect of the anterograde interference observed in motor and
perceptual learning [69,70]. When alternating between task A and task B, anterograde interference
reflects how the memory of task A impacts the learning of task B. The impact of Task B on the memory
of task A is termed retrograde interference, which is known to have a less effects than anterograde
interference [70]. The studies using fMRI-based bidirectional NF reported that anterograde
interference could influence the NF effect in the latter half NF sessions [71]. This anterograde
interference may also explain the discrepancy between our current and previous study [24]. In the
previous study, we performed NF using intracranial electrodes longitudinally placed along the left
parahippocampal gyrus and reported an increase in theta power (4-8Hz) for one participant. In this
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
study, despite the same electrode location for P02, the NF signal values decreased over sessions for
both Down and Up conditions. It is possible that the memory of Down NF influenced the learning of
Up NF. However, precise cause identification is challenging due to differences between the current
and the previous study, including the number of sessions for Up condition, NF signal values, and NF
modality used.
No correlation was observed between the transitions of memory task scores and the transitions
of NF signal values. The first potential reason is that, given the within-subject design in which
bidirectional NF was conducted, the two types of NF may have interfered with each other, preventing
observable changes at the behavioural level [71,72]. A second factor may be that the mental strategy
for NF interfered with the execution of the memory task. In this study, we did not provide
participants with a clear mental strategy for NF. Consequently, one participant (P01) adopted an
unexpected strategy of randomly answering the recognition task, resulting in a significant decrease
in accuracy in recognition task 1 compared to other sessions. Offering a clear mental strategy to
participants before the start of memory NF may prevent such unforeseen strategies and potentially
influence the success and efficiency of NF. While the relationship between the provision of a mental
strategy and the outcomes of NF is a topic of current investigation [73], systematic studies have been
limited [74], and there are no established standards for providing strategies [75]. Research on
instructional design suggests that feedback is more effective when goals are clearly defined and
specific [76]. NF training might be improved by providing participants with objectives, purposes, and
mental strategies, thereby reducing trial and error [73]. On the other hand, even if a mental strategy
that directly affects task performance is prevented, the mental strategy itself might create a dual-task
situation with the memory task, divide attention, and indirectly affect the performance of the memory
task [77,78]. Another factor may have been insufficient trials per session to evaluate memory
function. However, increasing the number of trials was difficult due to considerations for minimising
the burden on the patients. In future research addressing the second and third factors, it is
conceivable to introduce memory tasks separate from memory NF, allowing for independent
evaluation of memory function.
In the time-frequency analysis of the encoding period, we confirmed intermittent increases in
theta and alpha power for both the trials in which the order of word presentation was successfully
recognised (Correct) and the trials where it was not (Error). The association between theta oscillations
and memory has been suggested based on a wealth of past research [44,79], and an increase in theta
oscillations during the encoding period has been reported in scalp EEG studies [41,47,8082], MEG
studies [42,83], and iEEG studies [43,45,46,84], supporting our study results. Theta and alpha power
have also been suggested to be associated with memory retention [81,85], with reports of theta and
alpha power in the MTL increasing in response to memory load [86], and an increase in alpha activity
during the retention of the order of word sequences [87]. In this study, because participants were
questioned the order of word presentation in the recognition task 2, the memory load was heavier
and the neuronal activity involved in word retention may also have been reflected in the encoding
period than in previous studies that only asked whether words were present. In the comparison of
correct and error recognition of the order of word presentation, two participants (P01, P03) showed
a relative decrease in theta power during successful encoding, consistent with many intracranial
studies that asked whether words were present and conducted a correct and error comparison
[45,54,58,8890]. Particularly for P01, in the middle of the encoding period, a relative increase in low
theta power before stimulus presentation and a relative decrease in low theta power during stimulus
presentation were observed in relation to successful encoding. The increase in low theta power before
stimulus presentation has been reported in multiple studies [43,84,91], and is suggested to be
associated with the preparatory process for stimulus processing of encoding. For P01, the preparatory
state in the middle of the encoding may have affected the success of the recognition task. On the other
hand, P02 observed intermittent increases in theta and alpha power as neural activity associated with
successful encoding, and an increase in activity in the beta band was also observed. Most
conventional studies that asked whether words were present reported a decrease in power in these
bands [52,54,57,89,90,92,93], and the results of P02 in this study may seem contradictory at first
glance. However, in this study, we based the correct and error encoding on the recognition of the
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
order of word presentation, and we analysed 13 seconds including before and after the presentation
of five words. Therefore, compared to conventional studies, this study may reflect neuronal activity
related to word retention and memory of word order. P02 had lower scores on the recognition task
compared to other participants, so the memory load, or difficulty, may have been relatively high.
Since memory retention increases theta and alpha power depending on the load [81,86,87], P02 may
have been more affected by memory retention on the correctness of the recognition task than other
participants, resulting in different results. The increase in beta power in the MTL during successful
encoding has been reported in several previous studies [45,84], and is suggested to be associated with
sensory processing. Furthermore, increases in alpha and beta band activity before the presentation of
the encoding word have been reported in the neocortex and MTL [43,84], suggesting associations
with predictive and attention processes [48,54], and inhibitory top-down control of sensory
processing [43,45,54]. In P02, these cognitive functions, which are deeply involved in memory, may
have affected the succcess of encoding more than in other participants. In fact, P02 commented that
he took mental strategies such as adjusting his concentration during encoding, and previous studies
have suggested that volitional control can regulate a brain network composed of the hippocampus,
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and cerebellum, benefiting memory function [94].
In the time-frequency analysis, none of the participants showed a change in gamma power
between Up and Down conditions first and final sessions, compared to other frequency bands.
Specifically, regardless of the first or final sessions, two participants (P01 and P03) observed
intermittent increases in gamma power in the Up condition compared to the Down condition, while
P02 observed the opposite. One possible reason for the small change before and after training could
be that the gamma band was not included in the calculation of the NF signal values used in this study,
so the effect of NF might have been smaller than in other bands. The increase in gamma band activity
during encoding has been reported in previous studies [46,48,54,55,90,95,96], and has been suggested
to reflect an increase in attentional resources [48,96] and the association of stimuli with
spatiotemporal context, which supports the formation of episodic memories [56,92]. As for alpha and
beta power, in two participants (P02 and P03), the power increased in the final sessions compared to
the first sessions, regardless of the Up/Down condition. Low theta power also increased similarly but
considering that the NF signal values decreased over the sessions in both conditions, the influence of
increase in activity in the alpha and beta bands can be inferred to have been relatively high.
Considering the association of alpha and beta oscillations with predictive and attentional processes
during encoding [48,54], as well as the inhibitory top-down control [43,45,54], it is conceivable that
being accustomed to the tasks over multiple sessions may have affected these cognitive functions
related to encoding. On the other hand, in P01, although the relationship of alpha and beta power
between conditions was firstly Up < Down, the power increased in the Up condition and decreased
in the Down condition over the sessions, resulting in Up > Down in the final session. This suggests
that the current NF training, which aimed to adjust theta power, may have affected prediction and
attention processes during encoding [48,54], as well as inhibitory top-down control [43,45,54], and as
a result, alpha and beta power may also have been indirectly adjusted. However, Further study is
needed to determine the causes of these differences between participants. Next, we will proceed with
the interpretation of the results specific to each individual participant.
4.1. Discussion on the Results of P01
In the case of P01, it is inferred that the dominant side for verbal memory function was the left
side, as verbal memory function declined after surgery. Unlike the other participants, P01 underwent
NF for the right MTL, which is more closely associated with visual memory function, rather than the
left MTL, which is deeply related to verbal memory function. In this study, despite the use of a
standard verbal memory task, the NF training resulted in differences in neural activity in the right
MTL and performance on the recognition task 2 between the Up and Down conditions. The possible
reasons for this could be: 1. the nature of the memory task, which involved visually presenting words
on a monitor, might have evaluated not only verbal encoding function but also visual encoding
function; 2. the right side may have also been responsible for verbal encoding function, not just the
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
left side. For 1, in the future, when assessing the effect of NF solely on verbal encoding function, it
could be helpful to use stimuli that are less likely to involve visual processing, such as auditory
stimuli. For 2, based on the elapsed years since the onset of epilepsy and the presence of hippocampal
sclerosis in the left MTL, it can be inferred that verbal memory function may have been partially
reorganized towards the right MTL, potentially serving as an alternative or complementary role of
the left MTL. Regardless, as evidenced by the observed similarity in the trends of NF signal values
and performance on memory tasks, it is speculated that in the case of P01, there is a possibility that
NF not only adjusted neural activity but also memory function.
Considering the fact that there was a difference in total power, which is the denominator of NF
signal values, between Up and Down conditions (Up > Down), it can be interpreted that the trend of
NF signal values was largely contributed by the trend of low theta power. Moreover, seeing the
similarity with the transition of accuracy in recognition task 2, it can be considered that low theta
power may have played an important role in encoding in P1.
In the Up condition, the NF signal values significantly increased from the first session (session
1) to the final session (session 3), and in the intra-session trends of NF signal values, a steeper rise
from trial 1 was observed during the final session compared to the first session. Additionally, in the
comparison of time-frequency maps, it was confirmed that theta power was intermittently increasing
more in the final session than in the first session in the Up condition. These results may suggest an
accumulated NF effect in the Up condition. On the other hand, this adjustment of theta power may
have been caused by the expectation of reward. The relationship between reward and theta activity
has been suggested in rodent studies [97], and in humans, it has been suggested that reward
motivation is associated with successful memory encoding [98,99]. One study reported an increase in
theta power just before word presentation associated with successful encoding under high reward
conditions [100]. Although not mentioned by the authors, Figure.3C of this paper shows a visible
increase in theta power immediately after word presentation associated with successful encoding
under low reward conditions. In P01's session 3, due to a system error, NF continued to make it
difficult for the red team to win, which might have created a low reward condition unexpectedly,
potentially inducing an increase in theta power.
4.2. Discussion on the Results of P02
The transition in NF signal values across sessions and the transition in frequency band powers
across sessions demonstrated a negative correlation trend. This suggests that the transition in NF
signal values was more heavily influenced by power in other frequency bands rather than by low
theta power. Specifically, delta power had a higher value than low theta power, potentially exerting
a greater influence on total power. Consequently, the transition in NF signal values may have
strongly reflected the change in delta power.
We observed a negative correlation trend between the scores of the recognition task and NF
signal values. However, considering the similarity observed between the scores of the recognition
task and low theta power, it is plausible that in P02, low theta power played a crucial role in the
encoding process. In this study, we aimed to modulate neural activity contributing to memory
encoding with the Up/Down conditions. However, contrary to our intentions, it is possible that the
adjustments performed in the Up/Down conditions had resulted in the opposite direction of
modulation.
In the Up condition, we observed a trend of declining NF signal values as the sessions
progressed. Even the comparison of LTRi values across sessions, excluding trial 1, showed a
downward trend in the Up condition. This could be attributed to the rise in total power, especially
the rise in delta power being stronger than the increase in low theta power. The same declining trend
of NF signal values was observed in the Down condition, along with a rising trend in power across
frequency bands (total power, delta power, low theta power), just as in the Up condition. However,
unlike Up, the comparison of LTRi values across sessions, excluding trial 1, did not show any
significant differences within the Down condition. This could be because, in the Down condition, low
theta power rose as much as total power and delta power, resulting in no significant difference in
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
LTRi across sessions. The strong upward trend in low theta power in Down compared to Up may be
due to the unintentional adjustment of low theta power in the opposite direction in the Up/Down
condition. The direct cause for the decline in NF signal values in Down is inferred to be the magnitude
of LTR1 value. The values of LTR1 in Down were increasing, from 5.65% in session 1, to 7.23% in
session 3, and 10.02% in session 5. One interpretation for this rise in LTR1 values in Down is the
carryover effect due to NF training in the previous Up session aimed at increasing LTRi. Furthermore,
as there is no significant difference in LTRi values between conditions in the final session, the
significantly lower NF signal value in session 5 in the Down condition compared to session 6 in the
Up condition (LTR 1 value: 6.06%) is likely to be due to the magnitude of LTR1 value.
P02 reported in the later sessions that concentrating to remember led to the Red team winning,
while remembering with relaxation led to the White team winning. This study’s difference in
concentration during memory encoding might be interpreted as a difference in load during the
memory retention process. Increases in theta activity in the frontal midline area have been reported
when engaging in tasks that require focused attention or high-load memory tasks [101103]. The
hippocampus is known to be strongly connected with the prefrontal cortex [104,105], and is suggested
to contribute to cortical theta activity through their interaction [40]. Furthermore, one study reported
an increase in MTL theta power during memory retention with increasing memory load [86]. Taking
these into account, when concentration was needed for memory, theta power might have increased,
resulting in a rise in NF signal values and the Red team winning, while in a more relaxed state, theta
power might have decreased, leading to a decrease in NF signal values and the White team winning.
The reason why the NF signal values were higher than the baseline in trial 1, regardless of Up/Down
in the first sessions, might be due to the increase in theta power as P02 still needed to focus on
remembering. Additionally, as the scores in the recognition task 2 were lower in the first sessions
compared to other sessions, it can be inferred that the memory task imposed a high load during these
first sessions.
4.3. Discussion on the Results of P03
Interestingly, in the first and intermediate sessions of NF training for P03, the NF signal values
and LTRi were higher in Down (session 2, 4) than in Up (session 1, 3). Given the negative correlation
trend between the transitions in NF signal values and transitions in frequency band powers, it can be
inferred that the transitions in NF signal values was more heavily influenced by the power in other
frequency bands than by low theta power. Particularly, delta power exhibited larger differences
between conditions (Up > Down) in the first and intermediate sessions than low theta power, with
the magnitude of this power difference being similar to that in the total band. Additionally, in the
first session comparison of the time-frequency map between conditions, we observed clusters
indicating intermittently lower delta power in Down than in Up. Considering these, we can infer that
delta power significantly influenced LTR and NF signal values in P03, similar to P02. Although we
were unable to confirm any similarities or negative correlations between the transitions of neural
activity and the transitions of scores in the memory task in P03, the differences between NF conditions
were most apparent in the delta band. This suggests that delta power in P03 might be deeply
associated with the memory encoding function, similar to or even more than low theta power. Some
studies have reported modulation of delta power during encoding [45,52,53,93], and Lega et al. [45]
have asserted its functional significance, noting the similarity between oscillations below 4Hz in
human MTL and memory-related theta oscillations observed in the hippocampus of animals. If delta
power contributed to encoding more than low theta power in P03, then, given the observed negative
correlation trend between NF signal values and delta power, we may have unintentionally conducted
NF that decreased delta power in Up and increased it in Down.
To investigate the causes of the differences in NF signal values and LTRi (Up < Down) in the first
and intermediate sessions, we examined the within-session transitions in delta power and low theta
power, as shown in Figure 28.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Figure 28. Changes of band power of P03 within session. (a) delta power. (b) low theta power
In Up, delta power transitioned higher than low theta power. This suggests that delta power had
a significant impact on NF signal values. In Up, delta power remained high for the first few trials but
later declined to values similar to trial 1. In the first session of Up, NF signal values were lower than
trial 1 for the first few trials but later transitioned to values similar to or higher than trial 1.
Considering these, it is conceivable that in P03, the loss of the red team in the first few trials in Up
became a negative reinforcement in NF learning, causing a decline in delta power to a level similar
to trial 1 and changing the NF signal values. However, LTRi and NF signal values were not elevated
significantly beyond the baseline. This might be due to the loss of negative reinforcement when
escaping from the losing situation of the red team and the subsequent repetitions of the increase and
decrease of NF signal values (down to the level of trial 1), making it difficult for positive
reinforcement to occur. Furthermore, the inability to observe an increase in LTRi and NF signal values
in the session comparison of Up could be attributed to the potential retrograde effect of interposing
Down session between Up sessions.
On the other hand, in Down, delta power remained lower and more stable than in Up
throughout the session, and NF signal values were high throughout the session after trial 1, with
continuous losses for the supported white team serving as feedback. The fact that delta power in
Down remained lower and more stable than in Up can be interpreted in two ways. One possibility is
the anterogradely carryover effect of lowering delta power in the previous Up session. The other is
that the motivation for NF decreased due to the continuous losses of the white team, potentially
hindering NF learning within the session. However, delta power showed an increasing trend across
Down sessions. This could imply that the continuous losses of the white team served as negative
reinforcers in between-session NF learning. In Down, as delta power increased, low theta power also
increased, and no differences in LTRi were observed between sessions. Nonetheless, NF signal values
declined with each Down session. A direct cause of this could be the magnitude of LTR1. In Down,
the values of LTR1 increased, being 20.69% in session 2, 25.01% in session 4, and 34.81% in session 6.
This might be a carryover effect of trying to increase NF signal values and thereby decreasing delta
power in the previous Up session.
4.4. Limitation and Future Prospects
This study has several limitations. Firstly, we adopted the band power ratio rather than band
power as NF signal values. Based on the previous studies that suggest the importance of frequencies
below 4 Hz in the human hippocampus for encoding [45,47,49,83], we set low theta (2-6 Hz) as the
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
frequency band targeted by NF. However, due to variations in frequency power between individuals
and the possibility of diurnal and inter-day changes, we thought that using the band power and
visualising it for the tug-of-war game would be challenging. Therefore, we calculated the LTR, where
the total power of 0.5-32Hz was used as the denominator, and used this in NF. We believed that
adjusting this LTR would lead to adjustments in low theta power. However, the large fluctuations in
delta power for P02 and P03 significantly influenced the changes in LTR, and it is possible that NF
was conducted in a way that decreased the band power contributing to encoding in Up and increased
it in Down. Especially for P03, differences between conditions were more pronounced in delta power
than low theta power, suggesting that delta power might have been a more critical frequency band
for encoding. Considering these points, future memory NF studies might benefit from individual
optimization of the target frequency band by conducting frequency analysis of the MTL when
participants are performing another memory task beforehand. Also, it might be important to focus
only on a specific frequency band power for NF to prevent the influence of other frequency activities.
Nevertheless, while we found areas for improvement, using the MTL's frequency band power ratio
as the NF signal allowed us to recognise the importance of frequency band power other than low
theta in encoding, and that memory NF could potentially influence multiple frequency bands.
Secondly, we switched the direction of NF for each session for conducting bidirectional NF.
Bidirectional NF is a powerful control condition for excluding potential confounding factors [72].
Since we could not determine in advance how many training sessions the patients could participate
in, we adopted this control condition. This resulted in having only 2-4 sessions per condition. It has
been suggested that the intermittent NF takes time to learn [106], and the limited number of sessions
per condition might have influenced the results of our NF training. Besides, there might have been
anterograde and retrograde effects that could have interfered with NF learning. From the results of
P02 and P03, we speculated that conducting NF in the opposite direction in the previous session
influenced the subsequent session's trial 1 and the entire session. However, this also could suggest
that the effects of memory NF could continue, even after some time, if conducted for 1 session of 20
trials. In the future, we could try to conduct training for one condition first before switching to the
other condition. This would allow us to understand the accumulation of new NF effects in the next
condition though the NF effects from the previous condition remain. If we can recruit more
participants in the future, we could increase the number of sessions per participant and exclude the
effects of anterograde and retrograde effects by conducting group comparisons by assigning
participants to Up and Down groups.
Finally, this study, regarding the relationship between the transition of neural activity and the
changes of scores in the memory task, we only qualitatively stated that similarities and negative
correlation trends were observed. Especially, because we conducted the frequency band power
calculations after confirming the NF signal values and LTR transitions as exploratory analyses,
conducting correlation analyses for all and applying multiple comparison corrections could increase
the probability of committing type II errors, leading to erroneous conclusions, so we did not do it.
This study had too few sessions in NF training to conduct correlation analyses and evaluate
quantitatively. Thus, even if we had conducted correlation analyses, it would be hard to say that they
were evaluated correctly. However, considering the physical and mental burden on the patients who
participated in the study and the number of electrode placement days, it was challenging to secure
the number of sessions needed to conduct a quantitative evaluation.
5. Conclusions
In this study, we constructed a NF system to control the neural activity in the MTL
bidirectionally (Up and Down), and we evaluated whether we could control neural activity in both
directions after several days of training and how memory function would change. Specifically, we
constructed a memory NF system that could control neural activity bidirectionally by acquiring the
low theta power ratio in the MTL during encoding from intracranial electrodes while participants
played a tug-of-war game. We then examined the effects on MTL neural activity and memory
function.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
As a result of conducting NF training for three patients with intractable epilepsy, we confirmed
that the difference in NF signal conditions (UpDown) became larger as sessions progressed.
However, the direction in which all participants could control neural activity was unidirectional, and
they could not control it in both directions. Moreover, we found that NF could potentially influence
memory retention function and various cognitive functions related to memory, such as prediction,
attention processing processes, and inhibitory top-down control of sensory processing.
Furthermore, we suggested the possibility that frequency bands below 2Hz, which have not
been as focused on in human memory research as the theta frequency band, might contribute to
encoding. We also confirmed that there were similarities and a negative correlation tendency between
the transition of neural activity and memory function.
Our study represents an important step in understanding the application of NF in the context of
memory and offers valuable insights that may lead to the development of more targeted and effective
memory enhancement strategies using NF.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, K.K., N.K., K.U., K.N., S.F. and S.S.; Data curation, K.K. and K.N.;
Formal analysis, K.K.; Funding acquisition, K.K.; Investigation, K.K., K.N. and S.F.; Methodology, K.K., N.K. and
K.U.; Project administration, M.N.; Resources, N.K.; Software, K.K. and N.K.; Supervision, N.K. and K.U.;
Validation, K.K., N.K., K.U., K.N., S.F. and S.S.; Visualization, K.K.; Writing original draft, K.K.; Writing
review & editing, N.K., K.U., K.N., S.F., S.S. and M.N..
Funding: This study was funded by JSPS KAKENHI (Grant Number JP21J11552).
Informed Consent Statement: Written informed consent has been obtained from the patient(s) to publish this
paper.
Data Availability Statement: Due to the nature of this research, participants of this study did not agree for their
data to be shared publicly, so supporting data is not available.
Acknowledgments: We acknowledge that the experimental part of the work presented in this article was
conducted as a part of the thesis work of K.K. at the University of Tokyo in Japan.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1. Aggleton, J.P.; Shaw, C.; Gaffan, E.A. The Performance of Postencephalitic Amnesic Subjects on Two
Behavioural Tests of Memory: Concurrent Discrimination Learning and Delayed Matching-To-Sample.
Cortex 1992, 28, 359–372, doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80146-3.
2. Buffalo, E.A.; Reber, P.J.; Squire, L.R. The Human Perirhinal Cortex and Recognition Memory. Hippocampus
1986, 8, 330–339, doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:4.
3. Holdstock, J.S.; Shaw, C.; Aggleton, J.P. The Performance of Amnesic Subjects on Tests of Delayed
Matching-to-Sample and Delayed Matching-to-Position. Neuropsychologia 1995, 33, 1583–1596,
doi:10.1016/0028-3932(95)00145-X.
4. Holdstock, J.S.; Gutnikov, S.A.; Gaffan, D.; Mayes, A.R. Perceptual and Mnemonic Matching-To-Sample in
Humans: Contributions of The Hippocampus, Perirhinal and Other Medial Temporal Lobe Cortices. Cortex
2000, 36, 301–322, doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70843-8.
5. Owen, A.M.; Sahakian, B.J.; Semple, J.; Polkey, C.E.; Robbins, T.W. Visuo-Spatial Short-Term Recognition
Memory and Learning after Temporal Lobe Excisions, Frontal Lobe Excisions or Amygdalo-
Hippocampectomy in Man. Neuropsychologia 1995, 33, 1–24, doi:10.1016/0028-3932(94)00098-A.
6. Sasaki, T.; Piatti, V.C.; Hwaun, E.; Ahmadi, S.; Lisman, J.E.; Leutgeb, S.; Leutgeb, J.K. Dentate Network
Activity Is Necessary for Spatial Working Memory by Supporting CA3 Sharp-Wave Ripple Generation and
Prospective Firing of CA3 Neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2018 212 2018, 21, 258–269, doi:10.1038/s41593-017-0061-
5.
7. Bohbot, V.D.; Kalina, M.; Stepankova, K.; Spackova, N.; Petrides, M.; Nadel, L. Spatial Memory Deficits in
Patients with Lesions to the Right Hippocampus and to the Right Parahippocampal Cortex.
Neuropsychologia 1998, 36, 1217–1238, doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00161-9.
8. Suthana, N.; Haneef, Z.; Stern, J.; Mukamel, R.; Behnke, E.; Knowlton, B.; Fried, I. Memory Enhancement
and Deep-Brain Stimulation of the Entorhinal Area. N. Engl. J. Med. 2012, 366, 502–510,
doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1107212.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
9. Hescham, S.; Lim, L.W.; Jahanshahi, A.; Blokland, A.; Temel, Y. Deep Brain Stimulation in Dementia-
Related Disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 2666–2675, doi:10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2013.09.002.
10. Lacruz, M.E.; Valentín, A.; Seoane, J.J.G.; Morris, R.G.; Selway, R.P.; Alarcón, G. Single Pulse Electrical
Stimulation of the Hippocampus Is Sufficient to Impair Human Episodic Memory. Neuroscience 2010, 170,
623–632, doi:10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2010.06.042.
11. McLachlan, R.S.; Pigott, S.; Tellez-Zenteno, J.F.; Wiebe, S.; Parrent, A. Bilateral Hippocampal Stimulation
for Intractable Temporal Lobe Epilepsy: Impact on Seizures and Memory. Epilepsia 2010, 51, 304–307,
doi:10.1111/J.1528-1167.2009.02332.X.
12. Miatton, M.; Van Roost, D.; Thiery, E.; Carrette, E.; Van Dycke, A.; Vonck, K.; Meurs, A.; Vingerhoets, G.;
Boon, P. The Cognitive Effects of Amygdalohippocampal Deep Brain Stimulation in Patients with Temporal
Lobe Epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2011, 22, 759–764, doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.09.016.
13. Schoenberg, P.L.A.; David, A.S. Biofeedback for Psychiatric Disorders: A Systematic Review. Appl.
Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 2014, 39, 109–135, doi:10.1007/S10484-014-9246-9.
14. Lofthouse, N.; Arnold, L.E.; Hersch, S.; Hurt, E.; DeBeus, R. A Review of Neurofeedback Treatment for
Pediatric ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2012, 16, 351–372, doi:10.1177/1087054711427530.
15. Wang, J.R.; Hsieh, S. Neurofeedback Training Improves Attention and Working Memory Performance. Clin.
Neurophysiol. 2013, 124, 2406–2420, doi:10.1016/J.CLINPH.2013.05.020.
16. Brandmeyer, T.; Delorme, A. Meditation and Neurofeedback. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 688–688,
doi:10.3389/FPSYG.2013.00688/BIBTEX.
17. Brandmeyer, T.; Delorme, A. Closed-Loop Frontal Midlineθ Neurofeedback: A Novel Approach for
Training Focused-Attention Meditation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 246–246,
doi:10.3389/FNHUM.2020.00246/BIBTEX.
18. Papez, J.W. A Proposed Mechanism of Emotion. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 1937, 38, 725–743,
doi:10.1001/archneurpsyc.1937.02260220069003.
19. Herff, C.; Krusienski, D.J.; Kubben, P. The Potential of Stereotactic-EEG for Brain-Computer Interfaces:
Current Progress and Future Directions. Front. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 123–123, doi:10.3389/fnins.2020.00123.
20. Gharabaghi, A.; Naros, G.; Khademi, F.; Jesser, J.; Spüler, M.; Walter, A.; Bogdan, M.; Rosenstiel, W.;
Birbaumer, N. Learned Self-Regulation of the Lesioned Brain with Epidural Electrocorticography. Front.
Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00429.
21. Khanna, P.; Swann, N.C.; De Hemptinne, C.; Miocinovic, S.; Miller, A.; Starr, P.A.; Carmena, J.M.
Neurofeedback Control in Parkinsonian Patients Using Electrocorticography Signals Accessed Wirelessly
with a Chronic, Fully Implanted Device. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 2017, 25, 1715–1724,
doi:10.1109/TNSRE.2016.2597243.
22. Bichsel, O.; Stieglitz, L.H.; Oertel, M.F.; Baumann, C.R.; Gassert, R.; Imbach, L.L. Deep Brain Electrical
Neurofeedback Allows Parkinson Patients to Control Pathological Oscillations and Quicken Movements.
Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1–10, doi:10.1038/s41598-021-87031-2.
23. Yamin, H.G.; Gazit, T.; Tchemodanov, N.; Raz, G.; Jackont, G.; Charles, F.; Fried, I.; Hendler, T.; Cavazza,
M. Depth Electrode Neurofeedback with a Virtual Reality Interface. Brain-Comput. Interfaces 2017, 4, 201–
213, doi:10.1080/2326263X.2017.1338008.
24. Koizumi, K.; Kunii, N.; Ueda, K.; Takabatake, K.; Nagata, K.; Fujitani, S.; Shimada, S.; Nakao, M. Intracranial
Neurofeedback Modulating Neural Activity in the Mesial Temporal Lobe During Memory Encoding: A
Pilot Study. Res. Sq. 2022, doi:10.21203/RS.3.RS-1954990/V1.
25. Sherlin, L.H.; Arns, M.; Lubar, J.; Heinrich, H.; Kerson, C.; Strehl, U.; Sterman, M.B. Neurofeedback and
Basic Learning Theory: Implications for Research and Practice. J. Neurother. 2011, 15, 292–304,
doi:10.1080/10874208.2011.623089.
26. Davelaar, E.J. Mechanisms of Neurofeedback: A Computation-Theoretic Approach. Neuroscience 2018, 378,
175–188, doi:10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2017.05.052.
27. Hinterberger, T.; Veit, R.; Wilhelm, B.; Weiskopf, N.; Vatine, J.J.; Birbaumer, N. Neuronal Mechanisms
Underlying Control of a Brain–Computer Interface. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005, 21, 3169–3181, doi:10.1111/J.1460-
9568.2005.04092.X.
28. Johnston, S.J.; Boehm, S.G.; Healy, D.; Goebel, R.; Linden, D.E.J. Neurofeedback: A Promising Tool for the
Self-Regulation of Emotion Networks. NeuroImage 2010, 49, 1066–1072,
doi:10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2009.07.056.
29. Skottnik, L.; Sorger, B.; Kamp, T.; Linden, D.; Goebel, R. Success and Failure of Controlling the Real-Time
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Neurofeedback Signal Are Reflected in the Striatum. Brain Behav.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
2019, 9, e01240–e01240, doi:10.1002/BRB3.1240.
30. Enriquez-Geppert, S.; Huster, R.J.; Figge, C.; Herrmann, C.S. Self-Regulation of Frontal-Midline Theta
Facilitates Memory Updating and Mental Set Shifting. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 420–420,
doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00420.
31. Eschmann, K.C.J.; Bader, R.; Mecklinger, A. Improving Episodic Memory: Frontal-Midline Theta
Neurofeedback Training Increases Source Memory Performance. NeuroImage 2020, 222, 117219–117219,
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117219.
32. Eschmann, K.C.J.; Mecklinger, A. Improving Cognitive Control: Is Theta Neurofeedback Training
Associated with Proactive Rather than Reactive Control Enhancement? Psychophysiology 2022, 59, e13873–
e13873, doi:10.1111/PSYP.13873.
33. Reiner, M.; Rozengurt, R.; Barnea, A. Better than Sleep: Theta Neurofeedback Training Accelerates Memory
Consolidation. Biol. Psychol. 2014, 95, 45–53, doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.10.010.
34. Reiner, M.; Lev, D.D.; Rosen, A. Theta Neurofeedback Effects on Motor Memory Consolidation and
Performance Accuracy: An Apparent Paradox? Neuroscience 2018, 378, 198–210,
doi:10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2017.07.022.
35. Reis, J.; Portugal, A.M.; Fernandes, L.; Afonso, N.; Pereira, M.; Sousa, N.; Dias, N.S. An Alpha and Theta
Intensive and Short Neurofeedback Protocol for Healthy Aging Working-Memory Training. Front. Aging
Neurosci. 2016, 8, doi:10.3389/fnagi.2016.00157.
36. Rozengurt, R.; Shtoots, L.; Sheriff, A.; Sadka, O.; Levy, D.A. Enhancing Early Consolidation of Human
Episodic Memory by Theta EEG Neurofeedback. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2017, 145, 165–171,
doi:10.1016/J.NLM.2017.10.005.
37. Tseng, Y.H.; Tamura, K.; Okamoto, T. Neurofeedback Training Improves Episodic and Semantic Long-Term
Memory Performance. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1–11, doi:10.1038/s41598-021-96726-5.
38. Guderian, S.; Düzel, E. Induced Theta Oscillations Mediate Large-Scale Synchrony with Mediotemporal
Areas during Recollection in Humans. Hippocampus 2005, 15, 901–912, doi:10.1002/HIPO.20125.
39. Herweg, N.A.; Apitz, T.; Leicht, G.; Mulert, C.; Fuentemilla, L.; Bunzeck, N. Theta-Alpha Oscillations Bind
the Hippocampus, Prefrontal Cortex, and Striatum during Recollection: Evidence from Simultaneous EEG–
FMRI. J. Neurosci. 2016, 36, 3579–3587, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3629-15.2016.
40. Hsieh, L.T.; Ranganath, C. Frontal Midline Theta Oscillations during Working Memory Maintenance and
Episodic Encoding and Retrieval. NeuroImage 2014, 85, 721–729, doi:10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2013.08.003.
41. Hanslmayr, S.; Spitzer, B.; Bäuml, K.H. Brain Oscillations Dissociate between Semantic and Nonsemantic
Encoding of Episodic Memories. Cereb. Cortex 2009, 19, 1631–1640, doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn197.
42. Osipova, D.; Takashima, A.; Oostenveld, R.; Fernández, G.; Maris, E.; Jensen, O. Theta and Gamma
Oscillations Predict Encoding and Retrieval of Declarative Memory. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2006,
26, 7523–7531, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1948-06.2006.
43. Fell, J.; Ludowig, E.; Staresina, B.P.; Wagner, T.; Kranz, T.; Elger, C.E.; Axmacher, N. Medial Temporal
Theta/Alpha Power Enhancement Precedes Successful Memory Encoding: Evidence Based on Intracranial
EEG. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 5392–5397, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3668-10.2011.
44. Herweg, N.A.; Solomon, E.A.; Kahana, M.J. Theta Oscillations in Human Memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020,
24, 208–227, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2019.12.006.
45. Lega, B.C.; Jacobs, J.; Kahana, M. Human Hippocampal Theta Oscillations and the Formation of Episodic
Memories. Hippocampus 2012, 22, 748–761, doi:10.1002/HIPO.20937.
46. Lin, J.J.; Rugg, M.D.; Das, S.; Stein, J.; Rizzuto, D.S.; Kahana, M.J.; Lega, B.C. Theta Band Power Increases
in the Posterior Hippocampus Predict Successful Episodic Memory Encoding in Humans. Hippocampus
2017, 27, 1040–1053, doi:10.1002/HIPO.22751.
47. Miller, J.; Watrous, A.J.; Tsitsiklis, M.; Lee, S.A.; Sheth, S.A.; Schevon, C.A.; Smith, E.H.; Sperling, M.R.;
Sharan, A.; Asadi-Pooya, A.A.; et al. Lateralized Hippocampal Oscillations Underlie Distinct Aspects of
Human Spatial Memory and Navigation. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 1–12, doi:10.1038/s41467-018-04847-9.
48. Sederberg, P.B.; Kahana, M.J.; Howard, M.W.; Donner, E.J.; Madsen, J.R. Theta and Gamma Oscillations
during Encoding Predict Subsequent Recall. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 10809–10814, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-
34-10809.2003.
49. Solomon, E.A.; Lega, B.C.; Sperling, M.R.; Kahana, M.J. Hippocampal Theta Codes for Distances in
Semantic and Temporal Spaces. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2019, 116, 24343–24352, doi:10.1073/pnas.1906729116.
50. Fell, J.; Klaver, P.; Elfadil, H.; Schaller, C.; Elger, C.E.; Fernández, G. Rhinal–Hippocampal Theta Coherence
during Declarative Memory Formation: Interaction with Gamma Synchronization? Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003,
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
17, 1082–1088, doi:10.1046/J.1460-9568.2003.02522.X.
51. Gruber, M.J.; Hsieh, L.T.; Staresina, B.P.; Elger, C.E.; Fell, J.; Axmacher, N.; Ranganath, C. Theta Phase
Synchronization between the Human Hippocampus and Prefrontal Cortex Increases during Encoding of
Unexpected Information: A Case Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 30, 1646–1656, doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01302.
52. Ezzyat, Y.; Kragel, J.E.; Burke, J.F.; Levy, D.F.; Lyalenko, A.; Wanda, P.; O’Sullivan, L.; Hurley, K.B.; Busygin,
S.; Pedisich, I.; et al. Direct Brain Stimulation Modulates Encoding States and Memory Performance in
Humans. Curr. Biol. 2017, 27, 1251–1258, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.028.
53. Ezzyat, Y.; Wanda, P.A.; Levy, D.F.; Kadel, A.; Aka, A.; Pedisich, I.; Sperling, M.R.; Sharan, A.D.; Lega, B.C.;
Burks, A.; et al. Closed-Loop Stimulation of Temporal Cortex Rescues Functional Networks and Improves
Memory. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 365–365, doi:10.1038/s41467-017-02753-0.
54. Fellner, M.C.; Gollwitzer, S.; Rampp, S.; Kreiselmeyr, G.; Bush, D.; Diehl, B.; Axmacher, N.; Hamer, H.;
Hanslmayr, S. Spectral Fingerprints or Spectral Tilt? Evidence for Distinct Oscillatory Signatures of
Memory Formation. PLOS Biol. 2019, 17, e3000403–e3000403, doi:10.1371/JOURNAL.PBIO.3000403.
55. Greenberg, J.A.; Burke, J.F.; Haque, R.; Kahana, M.J.; Zaghloul, K.A. Decreases in Theta and Increases in
High Frequency Activity Underlie Associative Memory Encoding. NeuroImage 2015, 114, 257–263,
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.077.
56. Kragel, J.E.; Ezzyat, Y.; Sperling, M.R.; Gorniak, R.; Worrell, G.A.; Berry, B.M.; Inman, C.; Lin, J.J.; Davis,
K.A.; Das, S.R.; et al. Similar Patterns of Neural Activity Predict Memory Function during Encoding and
Retrieval. NeuroImage 2017, 155, 60–71, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.042.
57. Long, N.M.; Kahana, M.J. Successful Memory Formation Is Driven by Contextual Encoding in the Core
Memory Network. NeuroImage 2015, 119, 332–337, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.073.
58. Solomon, E.A.; Stein, J.M.; Das, S.; Gorniak, R.; Sperling, M.R.; Worrell, G.; Inman, C.S.; Tan, R.J.; Jobst, B.C.;
Rizzuto, D.S.; et al. Dynamic Theta Networks in the Human Medial Temporal Lobe Support Episodic
Memory. Curr. Biol. 2019, 29, 1100-1111.e4, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2019.02.020.
59. Nan, W.; Rodrigues, J.P.; Ma, J.; Qu, X.; Wan, F.; Mak, P.I.; Mak, P.U.; Vai, M.I.; Rosa, A. Individual Alpha
Neurofeedback Training Effect on Short Term Memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2012, 86, 83–87,
doi:10.1016/J.IJPSYCHO.2012.07.182.
60. Hosseini, S.M.H.; Pritchard-Berman, M.; Sosa, N.; Ceja, A.; Kesler, S.R. Task-Based Neurofeedback
Training: A Novel Approach Toward Training Executive Functions. NeuroImage 2016, 134, 159–159,
doi:10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2016.03.035.
61. Liu, N.; Cliffer, S.; Pradhan, A.H.; Lightbody, A.; Hall, S.S.; Reiss, A.L. Optical-Imaging-Based
Neurofeedback to Enhance Therapeutic Intervention in Adolescents with Autism: Methodology and Initial
Data. Neurophotonics 2016, 4, doi:10.1117/1.NPH.4.1.011003.
62. Kunii, N.; Kamada, K.; Ota, T.; Kawai, K.; Saito, N. A Detailed Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance
Imaging in the Frontal Language Area: A Comparative Study with Extraoperative Electrocortical
Stimulation. Neurosurgery 2011, 69, 590–597, doi:10.1227/NEU.0b013e3182181be1.
63. Binder, J.R.; Sabsevitz, D.S.; Swanson, S.J.; Hammeke, T.A.; Raghavan, M.; Mueller, W.M. Use of
Preoperative Functional MRI to Predict Verbal Memory Decline after Temporal Lobe Epilepsy Surgery.
Epilepsia 2008, 49, 1377–1394, doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.01625.x.
64. Asahara, M. Word Familiarity Rate for “Word List by Semantic Principles (WLSP).”
65. Asahara, M. Word Familiarity Rate Estimation Using a Bayesian Linear Mixed Model. In Proceedings of
the Conference Proceedings of the First Workshop on Aggregating and Analysing Crowdsourced
Annotations for NLP; 2019; pp. 6–14.
66. Chang, C.Y.; Hsu, S.H.; Pion-Tonachini, L.; Jung, T.P. Evaluation of Artifact Subspace Reconstruction for
Automatic Artifact Components Removal in Multi-Channel EEG Recordings. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 2020,
67, 1114–1121, doi:10.1109/TBME.2019.2930186.
67. Mullen, T.R.; Kothe, C.A.E.; Chi, Y.M.; Ojeda, A.; Kerth, T.; Makeig, S.; Jung, T.P.; Cauwenberghs, G. Real-
Time Neuroimaging and Cognitive Monitoring Using Wearable Dry EEG. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 2015,
62, 2553–2567, doi:10.1109/TBME.2015.2481482.
68. Kadosh, K.C.; Staunton, G. A Systematic Review of the Psychological Factors That Influence
Neurofeedback Learning Outcomes. NeuroImage 2019, 185, 545–555,
doi:10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2018.10.021.
69. Seitz, A.R.; Yamagishi, N.; Werner, B.; Goda, N.; Kawato, M.; Watanabe, T. Task-Specific Disruption of
Perceptual Learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2005, 102, 14895–14900, doi:10.1073/PNAS.0505765102.
70. Sing, G.C.; Smith, M.A. Reduction in Learning Rates Associated with Anterograde Interference Results
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
from Interactions between Different Timescales in Motor Adaptation. PLOS Comput. Biol. 2010, 6, e1000893–
e1000893, doi:10.1371/JOURNAL.PCBI.1000893.
71. Cortese, A.; Amano, K.; Koizumi, A.; Lau, H.; Kawato, M. Decoded FMRI Neurofeedback Can Induce
Bidirectional Confidence Changes within Single Participants. NeuroImage 2017, 149, 323–337,
doi:10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2017.01.069.
72. Sorger, B.; Scharnowski, F.; Linden, D.E.J.; Hampson, M.; Young, K.D. Control Freaks: Towards Optimal
Selection of Control Conditions for FMRI Neurofeedback Studies. NeuroImage 2019, 186, 256–265,
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.004.
73. Lotte, F.; Larrue, F.; Mühl, C. Flaws in Current Human Training Protocols for Spontaneous Brain-Computer
Interfaces: Lessons Learned from Instructional Design. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 0, 568–568,
doi:10.3389/FNHUM.2013.00568/BIBTEX.
74. Enriquez-Geppert, S.; Huster, R.J.; Herrmann, C.S. EEG-Neurofeedback as a Tool to Modulate Cognition
and Behavior: A Review Tutorial. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 51–51, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00051.
75. Ros, T.; Enriquez-Geppert, S.; Zotev, V.; Young, K.D.; Wood, G.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Wan, F.; Vuilleumier,
P.; Vialatte, F.; Van De Ville, D.; et al. Consensus on the Reporting and Experimental Design of Clinical and
Cognitive-Behavioural Neurofeedback Studies (CRED-Nf Checklist). Brain 2020, 143, 1674–1685,
doi:10.1093/BRAIN/AWAA009.
76. Hattie, J.; Timperley, H. The Power of Feedback: Rev. Educ. Res. 2007, 77, 81–112,
doi:10.3102/003465430298487.
77. Craik, F.I.M. Effects of Dividing Attention on Encoding and Retrieval Processes. Nat. Rememb. Essays Honor
Robert G Crowder 2004, 55–68, doi:10.1037/10394-004.
78. Gardiner, J.M.; Parkin, A.J. Attention and Recollective Experience in Recognition Memory. Mem. Cogn. 1990
186 1990, 18, 579–583, doi:10.3758/BF03197100.
79. Buzsáki, G. Theta Oscillations in the Hippocampus. Neuron 2002, 33, 325–340, doi:10.1016/S0896-
6273(02)00586-X.
80. Hanslmayr, S.; Volberg, G.; Wimber, M.; Raabe, M.; Greenlee, M.W.; Bäuml, K.H.T. The Relationship
between Brain Oscillations and BOLD Signal during Memory Formation: A Combined EEG–FMRI Study.
J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 15674–15680, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3140-11.2011.
81. Khader, P.H.; Jost, K.; Ranganath, C.; Rösler, F. Theta and Alpha Oscillations during Working-Memory
Maintenance Predict Successful Long-Term Memory Encoding. Neurosci. Lett. 2010, 468, 339–343,
doi:10.1016/J.NEULET.2009.11.028.
82. Klimesch, W.; Doppelmayr, M.; Russegger, H.; Pachinger, T. Theta Band Power in the Human Scalp EEG
and the Encoding of New Information. Neuroreport 1996, 7, 1235–1240, doi:10.1097/00001756-199605170-
00002.
83. Staudigl, T.; Hanslmayr, S. Theta Oscillations at Encoding Mediate the Context-Dependent Nature of
Human Episodic Memory. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, 1101–1106, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.074.
84. Marks, V.S.; Saboo, K.V.; Topçu, Ç.; Lech, M.; Thayib, T.P.; Nejedly, P.; Kremen, V.; Worrell, G.A.; Kucewicz,
M.T. Independent Dynamics of Low, Intermediate, and High Frequency Spectral Intracranial EEG
Activities during Human Memory Formation. NeuroImage 2021, 245, 118637–118637,
doi:10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2021.118637.
85. Wianda, E.; Ross, B. The Roles of Alpha Oscillation in Working Memory Retention. Brain Behav. 2019, 9,
e01263–e01263, doi:10.1002/BRB3.1263.
86. Boran, E.; Fedele, T.; Klaver, P.; Hilfiker, P.; Stieglitz, L.; Grunwald, T.; Sarnthein, J. Persistent Hippocampal
Neural Firing and Hippocampal-Cortical Coupling Predict Verbal Working Memory Load. Sci. Adv. 2019,
5, doi:10.1126/SCIADV.AAV3687/SUPPL_FILE/AAV3687_SM.PDF.
87. Meeuwissen, E.B.; Takashima, A.; Fernández, G.; Jensen, O. Increase in Posterior Alpha Activity during
Rehearsal Predicts Successful Long-term Memory Formation of Word Sequences. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011,
32, 2045–2045, doi:10.1002/HBM.21167.
88. Burke, J.F.; Zaghlou, K.A.; Jacobs, J.; Williams, R.B.; Sperling, M.R.; Sharan, A.D.; Kahana, M.J. Synchronous
and Asynchronous Theta and Gamma Activity during Episodic Memory Formation. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33,
292–304, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2057-12.2013.
89. Solomon, E.A.; Kragel, J.E.; Sperling, M.R.; Sharan, A.; Worrell, G.; Kucewicz, M.; Inman, C.S.; Lega, B.;
Davis, K.A.; Stein, J.M.; et al. Widespread Theta Synchrony and High-Frequency Desynchronization
Underlies Enhanced Cognition. Nat. Commun. 2017 81 2017, 8, 1–14, doi:10.1038/s41467-017-01763-2.
90. Weidemann, C.T.; Kragel, J.E.; Lega, B.C.; Worrell, G.A.; Sperling, M.R.; Sharan, A.D.; Jobst, B.C.;
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
Khadjevand, F.; Davis, K.A.; Wanda, P.A.; et al. Neural Activity Reveals Interactions Between Episodic and
Semantic Memory Systems During Retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2019, 148, 1–1, doi:10.1037/XGE0000480.
91. Guderian, S.; Schott, B.H.; Richardson-Klavehn, A.; Düzel, E. Medial Temporal Theta State before an Event
Predicts Episodic Encoding Success in Humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2009, 106, 5365–5370,
doi:10.1073/PNAS.0900289106/SUPPL_FILE/0900289106SI.PDF.
92. Long, N.M.; Burke, J.F.; Kahana, M.J. Subsequent Memory Effect in Intracranial and Scalp EEG. NeuroImage
2014, 84, 488–494, doi:10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2013.08.052.
93. Serruya, M.D.; Sederberg, P.B.; Kahana, M.J. Power Shifts Track Serial Position and Modulate Encoding in
Human Episodic Memory. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 24, 403–413, doi:10.1093/CERCOR/BHS318.
94. Voss, J.L.; Gonsalves, B.D.; Federmeier, K.D.; Tranel, D.; Cohen, N.J. Hippocampal Brain-Network
Coordination during Volitional Exploratory Behavior Enhances Learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2010 141 2010, 14,
115–120, doi:10.1038/nn.2693.
95. Castelhano, J.; Duarte, I.; Bernardino, I.; Pelle, F.; Francione, S.; Sales, F.; Castelo-Branco, M. Intracranial
Recordings in Humans Reveal Specific Hippocampal Spectral and Dorsal vs. Ventral Connectivity
Signatures during Visual, Attention and Memory Tasks. Sci. Rep. 2022 121 2022, 12, 1–10,
doi:10.1038/s41598-022-07225-0.
96. Sederberg, P.B.; Schulze-Bonhage, A.; Madsen, J.R.; Bromfield, E.B.; McCarthy, D.C.; Brandt, A.; Tully, M.S.;
Kahana, M.J. Hippocampal and Neocortical Gamma Oscillations Predict Memory Formation in Humans.
Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 1190–1196, doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl030.
97. Van Wingerden, M.; Vinck, M.; Lankelma, J.; Pennartz, C.M.A. Theta-Band Phase Locking of Orbitofrontal
Neurons during Reward Expectancy. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 7078–7087, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3860-09.2010.
98. Adcock, R.A.; Thangavel, A.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Knutson, B.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Reward-Motivated
Learning: Mesolimbic Activation Precedes Memory Formation. Neuron 2006, 50, 507–517,
doi:10.1016/J.NEURON.2006.03.036.
99. Wittmann, B.C.; Schott, B.H.; Guderian, S.; Frey, J.U.; Heinze, H.J.; Düzel, E. Reward-Related FMRI
Activation of Dopaminergic Midbrain Is Associated with Enhanced Hippocampus- Dependent Long-Term
Memory Formation. Neuron 2005, 45, 459–467, doi:10.1016/J.NEURON.2005.01.010.
100. Gruber, M.J.; Watrous, A.J.; Ekstrom, A.D.; Ranganath, C.; Otten, L.J. Expected Reward Modulates
Encoding-Related Theta Activity before an Event. NeuroImage 2013, 64, 68–74,
doi:10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2012.07.064.
101. Gevins, A.; Smith, M.E.; McEvoy, L.; Yu, D. High-Resolution EEG Mapping of Cortical Activation Related
to Working Memory: Effects of Task Difficulty, Type of Processing, and Practice. Cereb. Cortex 1997, 7, 374–
385, doi:10.1093/CERCOR/7.4.374.
102. Gomarus, H.K.; Althaus, M.; Wijers, A.A.; Minderaa, R.B. The Effects of Memory Load and Stimulus
Relevance on the EEG during a Visual Selective Memory Search Task: An ERP and ERD/ERS Study. Clin.
Neurophysiol. 2006, 117, 871–884, doi:10.1016/J.CLINPH.2005.12.008.
103. Missonnier, P.; Deiber, M.P.; Gold, G.; Millet, P.; Gex-Fabry Pun, M.; Fazio-Costa, L.; Giannakopoulos, P.;
Ibáñez, V. Frontal Theta Event-Related Synchronization: Comparison of Directed Attention and Working
Memory Load Effects. J. Neural Transm. 2006, 113, 1477–1486, doi:10.1007/S00702-005-0443-9.
104. Barbas, H.; Blatt, G.J. Topographically Specific Hippocampal Projections Target Functionally Distinct
Prefrontal Areas in the Rhesus Monkey. Hippocampus 1995, 5, 511–533, doi:10.1002/HIPO.450050604.
105. Goldman-Rakic, P.S.; Selemon, L.D.; Schwartz, M.L. Dual Pathways Connecting the Dorsolateral Prefrontal
Cortex with the Hippocampal Formation and Parahippocampal Cortex in the Rhesus Monkey. Neuroscience
1984, 12, 719–743, doi:10.1016/0306-4522(84)90166-0.
106. Oblak, E.F.; Lewis-Peacock, J.A.; Sulzer, J.S. Self-Regulation Strategy, Feedback Timing and Hemodynamic
Properties Modulate Learning in a Simulated FMRI Neurofeedback Environment. PLOS Comput. Biol. 2017,
13, e1005681–e1005681, doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005681.
Preprints (www.preprints.org) | NOT PEER-REVIEWED | Posted: 28 June 2023 doi:10.20944/preprints202306.1815.v2
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Invasive brain recordings using many electrodes across a wide range of tasks provide a unique opportunity to study the role of oscillatory patterning and functional connectivity. We used large-scale recordings (stereo EEG) within and beyond the human hippocampus to investigate the role of distinct frequency oscillations during real-time execution of visual, attention and memory tasks in eight epileptic patients. We found that activity patterns in the hippocampus showed task and frequency dependent properties. Importantly, we found distinct connectivity signatures, in particular concerning parietal-hippocampal connectivity, thus revealing large scale synchronization of networks involved in memory tasks. Comparing the power per frequency band, across tasks and hippocampal regions (anterior/posterior) we confirmed a main effect of frequency band (p = 0.002). Gamma band activity was higher for visuo-spatial memory tasks in the anterior hippocampus. Further, we found that alpha and beta band activity in posterior hippocampus had larger modulation for high memory load visual tasks (p = 0.004). Three functional connectivity task related networks were identified: (dorsal) parietal-hippocampus (visual attention and memory), ventral stream- hippocampus and hippocampal-frontal connections (mainly tasks involving face recognition or object based search). These findings support the critical role of oscillatory patterning in the hippocampus during visual and memory tasks and suggests the presence of task related spectral and functional connectivity signatures. These results show that the use of large scale human intracranial recordings can validate the role of oscillatory and functional connectivity patterns across a broad range of cognitive domains.
Article
Full-text available
Understanding and improving memory are vital to enhance human life. Theta rhythm is associated with memory consolidation and coding, but the trainability and effects on long-term memory of theta rhythm are unknown. This study investigated the ability to improve long-term memory using a neurofeedback (NFB) technique reflecting the theta/low-beta power ratio on an electroencephalogram (EEG). Our study consisted of three stages. First, the long-term memory of participants was measured. In the second stage, the participants in the NFB group received 3 days of theta/low-beta NFB training. In the third stage, the long-term memory was measured again. The NFB group had better episodic and semantic long-term memory than the control group and significant differences in brain activity between episodic and semantic memory during the recall tests were revealed. These findings suggest that it is possible to improve episodic and semantic long-term memory abilities through theta/low-beta NFB training.
Article
Full-text available
Parkinsonian motor symptoms are linked to pathologically increased beta-oscillations in the basal ganglia. While pharmacological treatment and deep brain stimulation (DBS) reduce these pathological oscillations concomitantly with improving motor performance, we set out to explore neurofeedback as an endogenous modulatory method. We implemented real-time processing of pathological subthalamic beta oscillations through implanted DBS electrodes to provide deep brain electrical neurofeedback. Patients volitionally controlled ongoing beta-oscillatory activity by visual neurofeedback within minutes of training. During a single one-hour training session, the reduction of beta-oscillatory activity became gradually stronger and we observed improved motor performance. Lastly, endogenous control over deep brain activity was possible even after removing visual neurofeedback, suggesting that neurofeedback-acquired strategies were retained in the short-term. Moreover, we observed motor improvement when the learnt mental strategies were applied 2 days later without neurofeedback. Further training of deep brain neurofeedback might provide therapeutic benefits for Parkinson patients by improving symptom control using strategies optimized through neurofeedback.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive and neurofeedback training (NFT) studies have demonstrated that training-induced alterations of frontal-midline (FM) theta activity (4-8 Hz) transfer to cognitive control processes. Given that FM theta oscillations are assumed to provide top-down control for episodic memory retrieval, especially for source retrieval, that is, accurate recollection of contextual details of prior episodes, the present study investigated whether FM theta NFT transfers to memory control processes. It was assessed (1) whether FM theta NFT improves source retrieval and modulates its underlying EEG characteristics and (2) whether this transfer extends over two posttests. Over seven NFT sessions, the training group who trained individual FM theta activity showed greater FM theta increase than an active control group who trained randomly chosen frequency bands. The training group showed better source retrieval in a posttraining session performed 13 days after NFT and their performance increases from pre- to both posttraining sessions were predicted by NFT theta increases. Thus, training-induced enhancement of memory control processes seems to protect newly formed memories from proactive interference of previously learned information. EEG analyses revealed that during pretest both groups showed source memory specific theta activity at frontal and parietal sites. Surprisingly, training-induced improvements in source retrieval tended to be accompanied by less prestimulus FM theta activity, which was predicted by NFT theta change for the training but not the control group, suggesting a more efficient use of memory control processes after training. The present findings provide unique evidence for the enhancement of memory control processes by FM theta NFT.
Article
Full-text available
Neurofeedback has begun to attract the attention and scrutiny of the scientific and medical mainstream. Here, neurofeedback researchers present a consensus-derived checklist that aims to improve the reporting and experimental design standards in the field.
Article
Full-text available
Stereotactic electroencephalogaphy (sEEG) utilizes localized, penetrating depth electrodes to measure electrophysiological brain activity. It is most commonly used in the identification of epileptogenic zones in cases of refractory epilepsy. The implanted electrodes generally provide a sparse sampling of a unique set of brain regions including deeper brain structures such as hippocampus, amygdala and insula that cannot be captured by superficial measurement modalities such as electrocorticography (ECoG). Despite the overlapping clinical application and recent progress in decoding of ECoG for Brain-Computer Interfaces (BCIs), sEEG has thus far received comparatively little attention for BCI decoding. Additionally, the success of the related deep-brain stimulation (DBS) implants bodes well for the potential for chronic sEEG applications. This article provides an overview of sEEG technology, BCI-related research, and prospective future directions of sEEG for long-term BCI applications.
Article
A wide spectrum of brain rhythms are engaged throughout the human cortex in cognitive functions. How the rhythms of various frequency ranges are coordinated across the space of the human cortex and time of memory processing is inconclusive. They can either be coordinated together across the frequency spectrum at the same cortical site and time or induced independently in particular bands. We used a large dataset of human intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG) to parse the spatiotemporal dynamics of spectral activities induced during formation of verbal memories. Encoding of words for subsequent free recall activated low frequency theta, intermediate frequency alpha and beta, and high frequency gamma power in a mosaic pattern of discrete cortical sites. A majority of the cortical sites recorded activity in only one of these frequencies, except for the visual cortex where spectral power was induced across multiple bands. Each frequency band showed characteristic dynamics of the induced power specific to cortical area and hemisphere. The power of the low, intermediate, and high frequency activities propagated in independent sequences across the visual, temporal and prefrontal cortical areas throughout subsequent phases of memory encoding. Our results provide a holistic, simplified model of the spectral activities engaged in the formation of human memory, suggesting an anatomically and temporally distributed mosaic of coordinated brain rhythms.
Article
Frontal-midline (FM) theta activity (4-8 Hz) is proposed to reflect a mechanism for cognitive control that is needed for working memory retention, manipulation, and interference resolution. Modulation of FM theta activity via neurofeedback training (NFT) demonstrated transfer to some but not all types of cognitive control. Therefore, the present study investigated whether FM theta NFT enhances performance and modulates underlying EEG characteristics in a delayed match to sample (DMTS) task requiring mainly proactive control and a color Stroop task requiring mainly reactive control. Moreover, temporal characteristics of transfer were explored over two posttests. Across seven 30-min NFT sessions, an FM theta training group exhibited a larger FM theta increase compared to an active control group who upreg-ulated randomly chosen frequency bands. In a posttest performed 13 days after the last training session, the training group showed better retention performance in the DMTS task. Furthermore, manipulation performance was associated with NFT theta increase for the training but not the control group. Contrarily, behavioral group differences and their relation to FM theta change were not significant in the Stroop task, suggesting that NFT is associated with proactive but not reactive control enhancement. Transfer to both tasks at a posttest one day after training was not significant. Behavioral improvements were not accompanied by changes in FM theta activity, indicating no training-induced modulation of EEG characteristics. Together, these findings suggest that NFT supports transfer to cognitive control that manifests late after training but that other training-unspecific factors may also contribute to performance enhancement.
Article
Decreases in low frequency power (2-30 Hz) alongside high frequency power increases (>40 Hz) have been demonstrated to predict successful memory formation. Parsimoniously this change in the frequency spectrum can be explained by one factor, a change in the tilt of the power spectrum (from steep to flat) indicating engaged brain regions. A competing view is that the change in the power spectrum contains several distinct brain oscillatory fingerprints, each serving different computations. Here, we contrast these two theories in a parallel MEG-intracranial EEG study where healthy participants and epilepsy patients, respectively, studied either familiar verbal material, or unfamiliar faces. We investigated whether modulations in specific frequency bands can be dissociated in time, space and by experimental manipulation. Both, MEG and iEEG data, show that decreases in alpha/beta power specifically predicted the encoding of words, but not faces, whereas increases in gamma power and decreases in theta power predicted memory formation irrespective of material. Critically, these different oscillatory signatures of memory encoding were evident in different brain regions. Moreover, high frequency gamma power increases occurred significantly earlier compared to low frequency theta power decreases. These results speak against a “spectral tilt” and demonstrate that brain oscillations in different frequency bands serve different functions for memory encoding.
Article
Theta frequency (4-8 Hz) fluctuations of the local field potential have long been implicated in learning and memory. Human studies of episodic memory, however, have provided mixed evidence for theta's role in successful learning and remembering. Re-evaluating these conflicting findings leads us to conclude that: (i) successful memory is associated both with increased narrow-band theta oscillations and a broad-band tilt of the power spectrum; (ii) theta oscillations specifically support associative memory, whereas the spectral tilt reflects a general index of activation; and (iii) different cognitive contrasts (generalized versus specific to memory), recording techniques (invasive versus noninvasive), and referencing schemes (local versus global) alter the balance between the two phenomena to make one or the other more easily detectable.