ArticlePDF Available

A Critical Review of Horse-Related Risk: A Research Agenda for Safer Mounts, Riders and Equestrian Cultures

Authors:

Abstract

While the importance of improving horse-related safety seems self-evident, no comprehensive study into understanding or reducing horse-related risk has been undertaken. In this paper, we discuss four dimensions of horse-related risk: the risk itself, the horse, the rider and the culture in which equestrian activities takes place. We identify how the ways in which risk is constructed in each dimension affects the applicability of four basic risk management options of avoidance, transference, mitigation and acceptance. We find the acceptance and avoidance of horse-related risk is generally high, most likely due to a common construction of horses as irrevocably unpredictable, fearful and dangerous. The transference of risk management is also high, especially in the use of protective technologies such as helmets. Of concern, the strategy least utilised is risk mitigation. We highlight the potential benefit in developing mitigation strategies directed at: (a) improving the predictability of horses (to and by humans), and (b) improving riders’ competence in the physical skills that make them more resilient to injury and falls. We conclude with the presentation of a multidisciplinary agenda for research that could reduce accident, injury and death to horse-riders around the world.
Animals 2015,5, 561-575; doi:10.3390/ani5030372 OPEN ACCESS
animals
ISSN 2076-2615
www.mdpi.com/journal/animals
Article
A Critical Review of Horse-Related Risk: A Research Agenda
for Safer Mounts, Riders and Equestrian Cultures
Kirrilly Thompson 1,:,*, Paul McGreevy 2,:and Phil McManus 3,:
1The Appleton Institute, Central Queensland University, 44 Greenhill Road, Wayville, SA 5034,
Australia
2Faculty of Veterinary Science, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia;
E-Mail: paul.mcgreevy@sydney.edu.au
3School of Geosciences, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia;
E-Mail: phil.mcmanus@sydney.edu.au
:These authors contributed equally to this work.
*Author to whom correspondence should be addressed; E-Mail: kirrilly.thompson@cqu.edu.au;
Tel.: +61-08-8378-4512 (ext. 4512).
Academic Editor: Marina von Keyserlingk
Received: 24 June 2015 / Accepted: 7 July 2015 / Published: 17 July 2015
Abstract: While the importance of improving horse-related safety seems self-evident, no
comprehensive study into understanding or reducing horse-related risk has been undertaken.
In this paper, we discuss four dimensions of horse-related risk: the risk itself, the horse,
the rider and the culture in which equestrian activities takes place. We identify how the
ways in which risk is constructed in each dimension affects the applicability of four basic
risk management options of avoidance, transference, mitigation and acceptance. We find
the acceptance and avoidance of horse-related risk is generally high, most likely due to a
common construction of horses as irrevocably unpredictable, fearful and dangerous. The
transference of risk management is also high, especially in the use of protective technologies
such as helmets. Of concern, the strategy least utilised is risk mitigation. We highlight
the potential benefit in developing mitigation strategies directed at: (a) improving the
predictability of horses (to and by humans), and (b) improving riders’ competence in the
physical skills that make them more resilient to injury and falls. We conclude with the
presentation of a multidisciplinary agenda for research that could reduce accident, injury
and death to horse-riders around the world.
Animals 2015,5562
Keywords: horse-riding; risk; mitigation; culture; research; review; safety; behavior
change; eque-culture; motivator
1. Background
Horse-riding is a hazardous activity. Each year, horse riders are injured, hospitalized or killed as a
result of horse-related accidents and injuries. Despite technological advancements in equestrian safety
equipment [1], horse riding continues to be found more dangerous than motorcycling, skiing, football,
and rugby [2,3]. Whilst injury can occur simply from handling horses [47], falling from a horse
constitutes a dangerous fall from height, possibly at speed. A rider’s head can be elevated up to 3 m
from the ground and horses can travel at speeds around 50 km/h. one study of children under 15 years
of age found that a ‘mean Modified Injury Severity Scale score of injured riders was exceeded only by
that of pedestrians struck by a car’ [8] (p. 487).
Improving safety for the millions of horse-riders around the globe is significant for moral, economic,
socio-cultural, and public health reasons. While the importance of improving horse-related safety seems
self-evident, no rigorous study into understanding or reducing horse-related risk has been undertaken
internationally in the academic literature. This may be due to a historical legacy of horse-riding being
a pre-modern sport with a robust culture that accepts the dangers of riding. It may also be due to the
difficulty of analyzing and mitigating the risks inherent in, and generated by, a complex socio-technical
network and interspecies interaction that is historically, socially and culturally constructed. Nonetheless,
complexity must be addressed to enhance the safety of millions of equestrians around the globe.
In this paper, we discuss four inter-related dimensions of horse-related risk: the risk itself, the horse,
the rider and the cultural context through which equestrian activities become meaningful. For each
dimension, we present an overview of current knowledge and offer a list of important questions that
remain. Moreover, we identify the repercussions of how risk has been constructed for each dimension.
Risk management strategies are commonly presented as four options: avoidance, transference (of
responsibility to a third party), mitigation and acceptance [9]. As argued throughout this paper, the
acceptance and avoidance of horse-related risk is high. Horses are frequently constructed as irrevocably
unpredictable and dangerous. As a result, most horse-related safety information concentrates on
avoiding injury by keeping out of horse-related danger. Risk transference is also high, in relation to a
reliance on insurance companies, professional trainers to manage difficult horses or the use of protective
technologies such as helmets, body protectors or even sedative substances. Lowest of all is mitigation,
particularly mitigation directed at: (a) improving the predictability of horses (through making horses
more predictable and making riders more able to predict their behavior); and (b) improving riders’
competence in the physical skills with the explicit intention of making them more resilient to injury
and falls.
To address the questions raised throughout the paper, we conclude with a list of recommended
methods for future data collection. Together, these questions and methods comprise a multidisciplinary
agenda for research that could reduce accident, injury and death to millions of horse-riders around
the world.
Animals 2015,5563
2. Current Knowledge and Critical Questions
2.1. The Risk
As noted above, horses are dangerous and interacting with them is more or less risky. Horses are
often pushed to their physical and physiological limits in equestrian pursuits [10], resulting in risk to
both horses and riders. Research on horse-related risk is biased towards cross-sectional epidemiological
studies of injury type and severity [7], or the efficacy of technical interventions such as frangible pins on
jumping obstacles, helmets and back protectors [8,1113]. With very little exception [1315], a focus
on the causes and consequences of horse-related injury rather than prevention, could lead to researchers
being accused of ‘shutting the gate after the horse has bolted’.
Whilst risk analysis is an important aspect of risk research, especially for ranking risk, prioritizing
intervention and evaluating campaigns, risk-reduction requires changing the attitudes and behaviours of
participants in risky activities. Sandman’s model of risk communication is particularly useful in this
regard [16]. According to Sandman, ‘risk’ is most usefully understood not as the standard arithmetical
product of likelihood and consequence of an incident occurring (ISO 31000), but as the cultural sum
of ‘hazard + outrage’. Sandman discusses combinations of high/low risk/outrage on a 2X2 matrix and
their implications for crisis management. For example, the risk of autism being induced by childhood
immunization is a ‘low risk/high outrage’ concern. The cultural response of high outrage outweighs and
overshadows the objective risk but has had a significant impact on rates of failure to immunize.
Horse-related risk can be considered ‘high risk/low outrage’, suggesting a level of wide-spread
recognition of risk but a high degree of acceptance, complacency or inaction. The recommended strategy
for encouraging behaviours that reduce horse-related risk would then be to reduce the risks (see sections
on the rider and the horse below), whilst also increasing outrage (see ‘culture’, below). In particular,
outrage over horse-related injury needs to be increased amongst riders as well as the governments that
have the ability to financially support or legislate for change. As increasing outrage when there is no
available risk mitigation can lead to paralyzing fear and perpetuate complacency, there is a need to fortify
existing technical risk controls with behavioural, physical and cultural controls.
In relation to the risk dimension of horse-related risk, we therefore need to ask: How can horse-related
injuries be prevented?
(a) What contributes to complacency around horse-related risk?
(b) How can outrage over horse-related risk be usefully increased?
(c) What behavioural, physical and cultural controls can supplement existing technical controls?
2.2. The Horse
The risk of horse-related accidents and injury are well known and widely accepted. Risk is frequently
attributed to the ‘nature’ of horses as irrevocably unpredictable, fearful and flight-wired [17]. As
noted by Lawrence, even the most highly trained horses can be treated with suspicion, lest they go
‘feral’ and return to a state of unruly wildness [18]. This essentialist view of horses as more or
less ‘unchangeable’ could explain why horse-related risk intervention is largely technical, and risk
management is avoidance-based (as discussed in more detail below).
Animals 2015,5564
In equestrianism, there has been a strong focus on using personal protective equipment (e.g., helmet,
boots, gloves and, more recently, body protectors) and maintaining horse equipment to safe standards
(e.g., checking stitching on saddlery). Whilst technical intervention has an undeniable role in reducing
the likelihood and consequence of an adverse equestrian event, the voluntary use of basic protective
equipment such as helmets use is low to inconsistent [11,12,15]. Technical intervention should therefore
not overshadow attention to horse behavior, or rider skill (discussed in more detail below).
Overall, there seems to have been more discussion about why horses are dangerous (e.g.,
unpredictable, large and flighty), rather than if, how and to what extent those risk factors can be
mitigated or even controlled. In fact, research suggests that the unpredictability of horses can be reduced
through behavioural interventions and approaches [19]. This is particularly encouraging, given that
a taken-for-granted assumption that horses are irrevocably unpredictable may contribute to a lack of
outrage about current levels of horse-related accident, injury and death. Even if horses are irrevocably
unpredictable based on the innate fears or a prey animal, there has been no consideration of the extent to
which their own need to feel safe could be reconfigured to reduce risk to humans. As noted by McGreevy
and colleagues, ‘the value of safety to animals is often overlooked by trainers and handlers’ [20]. This is
a firm reminder that feelings of risk, fear and safety are at least as important to horses as well as people.
As these equine desires may contribute to risks where their importance goes unrecognized, the corollary
is that they could be used to reduce risk where their importance is accommodated and addressed.
The unpredictability of horses suggests that risks may be higher amongst people with low levels of
knowledge of horse ethology and behaviour, since this can help to anticipate natural but undesirable
equine behaviours and responses. Evidence of breed differences in personality [21] and vision [22] has
been revealed, but there is little guidance on how to use this information to reduce the risks associated
with handling and riding certain horses. One exception is the recent ‘Guide to managing risks when
new and inexperienced persons interact with horses’ [23], that singles out Thoroughbred horses as
unsuitable for beginners. This seminal guide highlights the potential risk reduction benefits of applying
WHS principles to equestrian cultures. In workplaces, workers are given guidance and decision-making
support tools to safely operate dangerous plant, machinery or equipment through the identification and
control of hazards. However, professional, amateur or leisure riders are rarely given decision-making
tools for riding a given horse on a given occasion (i.e., the equivalent of a pre-flight safety check).
Although evidence of its adoption is lacking, the following seven point pre-ride equestrian checklist has
been suggested by Guyton et al. [24]:
(1) Am I wearing adequate protective gear?
(2) Is the tack durable and well-fit to the horse?
(3) Are the environmental conditions (weather, ground footing) safe for riding?
(4) Does the arena contain unfamiliar objects, animals, or people that may alarm the horse?
(5) Is the horse healthy and prepared for riding? Is the mood or behavior of the horse uncharacteristic
or concerning?
(6) Am I healthy and prepared for riding? Do I have an emotional or physical condition that may
impair my ability to safely ride this horse?
(7) Do the horse and I have a healthy relationship? Do I have concerns about my ability to assert
myself with this horse? [24]
Animals 2015,5565
The list is entirely relevant, but can be significantly improved. The first two checks relate to
technology and are under direct control of the human. Checks Three and Four relate to the environment,
over which riders have less control, especially when riding in open spaces. Check Six relates to the rider
but ‘emotional or physical’ condition is rather broad, especially around the degree to which safety is
jeopardized. Checks Five and Seven relate to the horse, but require familiarity with individual horses or a
perceptive eye informed by knowledge of equine behavior. The idea of a ‘healthy relationship’ and being
‘assertive’ are both open to interpretation and relative to different equestrian disciplines. Moreover, being
able to interpret and predict equine behaviour may be a more useful risk-management tool than being
assertive. Check Six relates to the rider, but ‘emotional or physical’ condition is rather broad, especially
around the degree to which safety is jeopardized by particular conditions. Furthermore, Check Six does
not address specific physical skills essential for riding, such as those discussed in more detail below.
Overall, whilst the checklist comprises questions that are undoubtedly important to ask, most if not
all rely on a level of knowledge and expertise in their interpretation and assessment. There is particular
scope to incorporate objective checks of safety, quality of the human-horse relationship and resilience
to external or environmental stimuli. These can be undertaken first from the ground and then under
saddle in a confined area through testing basic responses such as deceleration, acceleration (sometimes
necessary to avoid hazards such as vehicles or other horses), reversing and turning.
Moreover, riders are not routinely given explicit advice on what to do if they assess a horse as unsafe,
i.e., additional controls, alternative (less dangerous) activities, strategies to improve the safety of a mount
(i.e., install robust deceleration responses and habituate the horses to common hazards). Whilst riders
may be given instruction that has the effect of improving safety, it may not be understood or framed
explicitly as a risk-management strategy. Rather, it may be seen as taken for granted tradition or
simply ‘just the way things are done’, as has been found in relation to risk management by volunteer
firefighters [25]. Riders too may already be engaging in risk-reduction practices that are tacit. The
explicit identification of practices that improve safety as risk-reduction strategies may encourage riders
not only to value, improve and maintain high standards of those practices and the horse’s response to
them, but to communicate those practices to other riders to create safer equestrian cultures.
It is tempting to construct the horse as an independent source of risk. Certainly, without a horse
there is no risk. However, from a safety systems perspective, there is a need to recognize the ways
in which risk is generated in, by and through socio-technical networks. Despite the importance of
matching riders with appropriate horses being acknowledged [24] and largely carried out on a basis of
experienced/inexperienced horse/rider [26], there is no widely accepted or validated means of assessing
experience for the purpose of determining safe horse-rider combinations. As environmental conditions
and stressors can impact the behavior of horses and riders, assessment needs to be undertaken in general
as well as on specific occasions, such as during competition, on return to work after an injury or in
unfamiliar surroundings.
In relation to the equine dimension of horse-related risk, we therefore need to ask:
(a) How well do riders understand horse behavior?
(b) How can increasing levels of knowledge about horse behavior improve safety?
(c) Which horses are most likely to be unpredictable for riders (e.g., age, breed, level of experience,
level of horse education, early preparation and history of unwelcome behaviour)
Animals 2015,5566
(d) What physical and behavioural conditioning increases a horse’s predictability or make a horse
reasonably safe to ride?
(e) How might a horse’s preference for ‘safety’ be used to reduce risk?
(f) What benefits may be taken from OHS approaches to safety?
(g) How can a rider assess, train, maintain and improve a horse’s level of risk?
i From the ground
ii From the saddle
(h) What tacit practices do riders currently engage in without being aware of their risk-reduction
benefits, which could be emphasized?
(i) What tools could enable safer matching of horse and rider?
2.3. The Rider
Most—if not all—riders are aware of the risk of equestrian sports and mindful of their own safety [27].
Pony club manuals are replete with references to safe practices around horses [2830] and the pony
club movement itself has a ‘safety first’ attitude. However, from a risk management perspective,
the most common strategies for improving safety around horses can be characterized as avoidance
strategies—keeping out of harm’s way. Aside from the safe use of safe equipment, being ‘horse safe’
usually refers to how to avoid being kicked, bitten, trampled or crushed. Once again, the reliance
on avoidance rather than control could be a repercussion of the construction of horses as irrevocably
unpredictable. As discussed above, there is very little overt discussion about how to manage or reduce
‘equinogenic’ risk.
Similarly, there is little overt discussion of the physical and postural skills that can increase a rider’s
resilience to injury or falls. Riders communicate to horses through ‘natural aids’; their hands (rein tension
increases for ‘stop’ and ‘turn’), legs (leg pressure increases for ‘go’), seat (classically conditioned weight
and balance cues for ‘stop’ or ‘go’), voice and various ‘artificial aids’ such as whips and spurs. The
self-awareness, timing and accuracy of the application of aids have been related to rider safety, especially
(1) rein pressure [tension and release], (2) leg stability and (3) balance and trunk stability [31]. When
these stimuli are applied incorrectly, inadvertently or simultaneously, they can cause miscommunication,
confusion, discomfort or pain for the horse. The horse may respond with evasive behaviour, resistance
or flight behaviour and anti-predator responses such as rearing, bucking or bolting, potentially leading
to rider injury or even death. Riders’ cues must be applied with awareness, accuracy, timing and
consistency to: (a) avoid or reduce the likelihood of a horse-related incident occurring; and (b) provide
the rider with the ability to safely respond to an untoward incident, thereby reducing the significance of
its consequences [32].
The mensuration of cues from riders’ hands and legs has commenced as part of equitation
science [33]. There may also be benefits in measuring levels of cognitive and somatic anxiety
in riders, as anxiety can impact horse-rider communication by affecting fine motor control and
decision-making [34,35]. High self-efficacy may reduce anxiety [36], although little is known about
riders’ subjective confidence in their proficiency in the application of those cues, based on self-report.
From a psychological perspective, high self-efficacy may mislead riders into over-estimating their safety
and/or taking increased risks [37,38].
Animals 2015,5567
Furthermore, there is a need to know if and how riders associate their physical skills with safety,
especially in some of the more artistic equestrian disciplines such as dressage where a rider’s skills have
significant aesthetic value. There are several well established approaches to improving riding position
such as ‘centred riding’ [39] and ‘riding with your mind’ [40] that focus on riders developing better
feel, relating better with the horse, and becoming more effective as well as feeling and looking more
aesthetically pleasing. The simultaneous effect of these approaches on improving safety is largely taken
for granted and remains to be determined, as does the usefulness of ‘improved safety’ as a means to
motivate riders to adopt such approaches.
In relation to the rider dimension of horse-related risk, we therefore need to ask:
(a) How riders acquire and objectively perform skills related to horse safety?
(b) How riders subjectively self-evaluate the skills essential to horse safety?
(c) If and how riders and trainers associate physical ability with safety?
(d) What is the impact of existing rider position programmes on safety?
2.4. The Culture
As acknowledged by Sandman’s hazard/outrage model descried above, subjective perceptions of risk
do not always align with objective risk calculations [41], and not all strategies that reduce fear actually
reduce risk. Perceptions of risk can have a significant impact on the uptake of safety behaviours and
protective equipment. ‘Safety culture’ is a contested term that has been used in studies of organizational
culture to describe the ways in which workplaces do (or do not) value safety [42,43] or perform on
measures thought to indicate an organisation’s ‘safety climate’ [44]. The concept can be used to consider
the ways in which various equestrian cultures or ‘eque-cultures’ [45] impact on the safety of equestrians,
through cultural variation in risk perceptions, attitudes, beliefs and behaviours. In relation eque-culture
in general, safety is often poorly evaluated especially at the point of sale [14,46]. This is despite a
growing appreciation of the horse’s need to feel safe [20] and the range of responses a horse offers when
threatened [47]. In relation to racing eque-cultures, the common rehoming of racehorses to novice riders
is one example of a contemporary eque-cultural practice that presents risks to horses and humans. Whilst
the aim is often to reduce the risk of horses being unnecessarily euthanized, this practice may increase
the risk of a horse-related risk or injury to a human, and the potential mistreatment of a horse that is
subsequently labeled ‘dangerous’.
From a more socio-historical standpoint, the romantic cultural construction of horse riding as an
art can also conflict with practical considerations of risk. Since the Classical Greek period and
especially in the renaissance in Europe, equitation has been constructed as an art [4850]. As with
any artistic endeavor, there is reverence for those who seem blessed with a natural affinity or ‘feel’ that
eludes explanation. In equestrian culture, there has always been an acceptance that some equestrians
have a natural ability to communicate seemingly telepathically with horses such that the two become
one [51,52]. These riders are said to have natural ‘feel’ [26]. However, equitation science is
helping to demystify many of the qualities that distinguish such individuals [32,5355]. For example,
communication from rider to horse always relies upon at least some form of pressure cues, however
subtle [55]. These cues appear imperceptible in the best cases having been reduced from larger operant
Animals 2015,5568
cues [19] by a process of classical conditioning [56]. Different eque-cultures have different tolerances
for the ‘volume’ of cues to the horse and their visibility to bystanders.
Whilst equitation science has the potential to significantly reduce horse-related injury and death, it
would be disingenuous to present it as a panacea, at least uncritically. In any given population, there will
be varying levels of scientific literacy and diverse attitudes towards ‘scientific’ modes of thought, ranging
from supporters to skeptics. For some riders, the elusive experience of harmony is precisely what attracts
them to horse-riding [51]. For others, instruction from elite competitors is highly valued [26,27]. Some
resist a scientific framework of horse training, preferring leadership narratives that place them above
their horses in a perceived hierarchy [57]. Moreover, research has identified that the desire to achieve a
‘good’ human-horse relationship (or at least to ‘perform’ the achievement of a ‘good’ relationship) can
be a barrier to precautionary behaviour when such riders consider their ‘good’ relationship as evidence
of reduced risk and therefore a reduced need to wear protective equipment such as helmets [15].
This heterogeneity of styles of engagement with equestrianism, or ‘equestrian dispositions’, suggests
that safety information needs to be communicated in forms that resonate with riders of a multitude
of dispositions including scientific and artistic approaches to equitation. Understanding the cultural
specificity and generalizability of eque-cultures and equestrianism more broadly will be crucial to
effectively applying or adapting longstanding behavior change theories and models from health
psychology [5861]; as well as theories of decision-making and threat and error management from
human factors and safety science [62]. Whilst different equestrian dispositions have been operationalized
from researcher experience [57] and through factor analysis [63], there remains a need to evaluate their
resonance with equestrians themselves. There is a particular need to understand what motivates the
behaviour of different equestrians to identify useful ‘irrelevant motivators’ [64] that could encourage the
adoption of protective behaviours in the short term and cultivate safety values in the medium to long
term. For example, signaling being fashionable or professional may be a powerful motivator for some
riders to wear helmets [65]. Others might be more motivated to wear helmets if they become a symbol of
the ‘toughness’ of their discipline, especially in traditional cowboy equestrian disciplines associated with
masculinity, bravery and resilience [66]. Regardless of the motivation, many riders will be influenced
by their peers, and could be engaged to replicate desired precautionary behaviours through ‘participant
modeling’ [36] by their role models.
In relation to the cultural dimension of horse-related risk, we therefore need to ask:
(a) How do riders perceive equestrianism in general and equitation science in particular?
(b) What are the different styles of engagement with equestrianism?
(c) What are the most powerful motivators for equestrians?
(d) How do riders consume safety-related information?
(e) How are the causes of horse-related injuries and deaths understood by riders?
(f) How is the use of horse safety equipment represented in the horse community?
(g) What are the socio-cultural dimensions and determinants of risk and safety?
(h) What are the socio-cultural barriers and enablers to improve rider safety?
(i) What are the elements of effective behavior change campaigns and programmes that increase
rider safety?
Animals 2015,5569
3. Discussion
In this paper we critically reviewed four dimensions important to horse-related risks to horse riders.
Whilst they were presented discretely, they are mutually inclusive within a complex socio-technical
network and interspecies interaction that is historically, socially and culturally constructed. In relation to
the dimensions of risk, horses, riders and culture, we found that research on risk explains the intricacies
of what can go wrong, how often and what the consequences are. Research on horses is also problem
focused, arguing why horses are a source of risk. These biases favour risk-management options of
acceptance and avoidance. To take full advantage of risk-management strategies of mitigation, further
research is required on the extent to and ways in which the behavior of horses can be made more
predictable, and riders can be made more capable of predicting their behaviour. There is also a need
for research evaluating rider proficiency at performing and self-assessing the physical skills that increase
resilience to horse-related injury whilst riding. Finally, we considered the unavoidable and omnipotent
cultural context that affects riders’ behaviours, values, attitudes and beliefs regarding risk and safety.
Inconsistent levels of voluntary helmet use suggest that increasing the safety of eque-cultures and
equestrianism in general will require external legislation and internal transformation. Overall, we found
enormous potential for reducing horse-related risk through the risk management strategy of mitigation.
For each element of horse-related risk, we presented a list of research questions. Together, they
comprise a multidisciplinary agenda for further research that could significantly reduce accident, injury
and death to millions of horse-riders around the world. As these questions are multi-disciplinary, so too
do they require data from various sources, including but not limited to:
Surveys and questionnaires—to obtain a wider perspective on a range of issues, and to generate
quantitative results for policy development and advocacy around horse and rider safety [67,68].
Interviews and focus groups—to explore controversial subjects that pertain to risk and safety of
both the horse and rider [45,69].
Ethnographic research—to study the actual practices of equestrians; and identify risk management
strategies consistent with the motivations, beliefs and values of eque-cultures [26,7073].
Media analysis—to identify how particular incidents and risks are reported (or not reported) and
relationships to the values, beliefs and practices of equestrians [15,67,74,75].
Physiometry—to measure rider position and identify physical attributes positively correlated with
safety or resistance to being unseated [33,7678].
Psychometric research into fear [79], risk-taking propensity [80,81] and sensation seeking [82]
amongst riders and equestrian discipline—to identify target groups and tailor behavior change
interventions [38,83].
Analysis of accident and injury reports (i.e., from inquests, insurance records and hospital
admission data)—to enable triangulation of objective and self-report data, especially around
risk [6,84,85].
Inferential modeling - to determine predictors of risk and safety, animal attachment and target
group archetyping [86,87].
Animals 2015,5570
To ensure that data are translated into effective safety intervention tools that can reduce numbers
of horse-related injury and death, researchers should focus on developing initiatives that (a) increase
outrage about preventable horse-related injury and death, and (b) reduce horse-related risk, such as:
Ethical techniques and behavioral interventions to increase the predictability of horses
Interventions to improve riders’ ability to predict horse behaviour
Horse safety assessment and decision-making support tools
Rider safety skills assessment tools
Validated measure of horse training/riding style
Behavior change for safe equestrian cultures
4. Conclusion: Horse/Human-Related Risk/Safety
As demonstrated throughout this paper, horse-related risk is generated through a complex
socio-technical network of risk, horses, humans and culture. Whilst these dimensions have
been recognized and in some cases researched, horse-related risk has typically been constructed
anthropocentrically; it originates in horses and it impacts humans. Moreover, safety is largely seen as a
concern for humans only, despite a desire for safety being a powerful driver of ‘unpredictable’ behavior
in horses (and an excellent reward for behavioural interventions). To overcome these biases, there is
apparent advantage in the widespread adoption of a more anthrozoological approach to horse-related
risk that includes human-related risk as well as human/horse-related safety. With human-horse safety
as the ultimate goal, this paper has identified unrealised opportunity to mitigate horse-related risk with
behavioural, physical and socio-cultural interventions that could make horses safer mounts, humans safer
riders, and equestrianism a safer culture.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Holly Bowen for her assistance in the preparation and proofreading
of this manuscript.
Author Contributions
Thompson conceived this article. All authors contributed to the writing and approval of the
article’s content.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
1. Foote, C.; Gibson, T.; McGuaran, P. Health and Safety in Australian Racing-Evaluation of Safety
Vests; Rural Industries Research and Development Corporation: Canberra, Australia, 2014.
2. Ball, C.G.; Ball, J.E.; Kirkpatrick, A.W.; Mulloy, R.H. Equestrian injuries: Incidence, injury
patterns, and risk factors for 10 years of major traumatic injuries. Amer. J. Surg. 2007,193,
636–640. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Animals 2015,5571
3. Paix, B.R. Rider injury rates and emergency medical services at equestrian events. Brit. J. Sport.
Med. 1999,33, 46–48. [CrossRef]
4. Nunnink, L.; Abu-Zidan, F. Accidental carotid artery injury caused by a horse rope. Brit. J. Sports
Med. 2003,37, 460–461. [CrossRef]
5. Exadaktylos, A.K.; Eggli, S.; Inden, P.; Zimmermann, H. Hoof kick injuries in unmounted
equestrians. Improving accident analysis and prevention by introducing an accident and emergency
based relational database. Emerg. Med. J. 2002,19, 573–575. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
6. Thomas, K.E.; Annest, J.L.; Gilchrist, J.; Bixby-Hammett, D.M. Non-fatal horse related injuries
treated in emergency departments in the united states, 2001–2003. Brit. J. Sports Med. 2006,40,
619–626. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
7. Carmichael, S.P., II; Davenport, D.L.; Kearney, P.A.; Bernard, A.C. On and off the horse:
Mechanisms and patterns of injury in mounted and unmounted equestrians. Injury 2014,45,
1479–1483.
8. Bond, G.R.; Christoph, R.A.; Rodgers, B.M. Pediatric equestrian injuries: Assessing the impact of
helmet use. Pediatrics 1995,95, 487–489. [PubMed]
9. Hillson, D. Extending the risk process to manage opportunities. Int. J. Project Manag. 2002,20,
235–240. [CrossRef]
10. McLean, A.N.; McGreevy, P.D. Ethical equitation: Capping the price horses pay for human glory.
J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2010,5, 203–209. [CrossRef]
11. Nelson, D.E.; Rivara, F.P.; Condie, C. Helmets and horseback riders. Am. J. Prev. Med. 1994,10,
15–19. [PubMed]
12. Finch, C.; Watt, G. Locking the Stable Door: Preventing Equestrian Injuries; Monash University
Accident Research Centre: Clayton, Australia, 1996.
13. Condie, C.; Rivara, F.P.; Bergman, A.B. Strategies of a successful campaign to promote the use of
equestrian helmets. Public Health Rep. 1993,108, 121–126. [PubMed]
14. Hawson, L.A.; Oddie, C.; McLean, A.N.; McGreevy, P.D. Is safety valued in the australian pony
market? J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2010,6, 254–260. [CrossRef]
15. Haigh, L.; Thompson, K. Helmet use amongst equestrians: Harnessing social and attitudinal factors
revealed in online forums. Animals 2015,5, 576–591.
16. Sandman, P.M. Responding to Community Outrage: Strategies for Effective Risk Communication;
The American Industrial Hygiene Association: Fairfax, VA, USA, 1993.
17. LeGuin, E. Man and horse in harmony. In The Culture of the Horse: Status, Discipline, and
Identity in the Early Modern World; Raber, K., Tucker, T.J., Eds.; Palgrave Macmillan: New York,
NY, USA, 2005; pp. 175–196.
18. Lawrence, E.A. Rodeo: An anthropologist looks at the wild and the tame; University of Chicago
Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1982.
19. Hawson, L.A.; McLean, A.N.; McGreevy, P.D. The roles of equine ethology and applied learning
theory in horse-related human injuries. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2010,5, 324–338.
[CrossRef]
Animals 2015,5572
20. McGreevy, P.D.; Henshall, C.; Starling, M.J.; McLean, A.N.; Boakes, R.A. The importance of
safety signals in animal handling and training. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2014,9, 382–387.
[CrossRef]
21. Lloyd, A.S.; Martin, J.E.; Bornett-Gauci, H.L.I.; Wilkinson, R.G. Horse personality: Variation
between breeds. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008,112, 369–383. [CrossRef]
22. Evans, K.; McGreevy, P. The distribution of ganglion cells in the equine retina and its relationship
to skull morphology. Anat. Histol. Embryol. 2007,36, 151–156. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
23. Safe Work Australia. Guide to Managing Risks When New and Inexperienced Persons Interact with
Horses; Safe Work Australia: Canberra, Australia, 2014.
24. Guyton, K.; Houchen-Wise, E.; Peck, E.; Mayberry, J. Equestrian injury is costly, disabling, and
frequently preventable: The imperative for improved safety awareness. Amer. Surg. 2013,79,
76–83.
25. Dawson, D.; Mayger, K.; Thomas, M.J.; Thompson, K. Fatigue risk management by volunteer
fire-fighters: Use of informal strategies to augment formal policy. Accid. Anal. Prevent. 2015. in
press.
26. Thompson, K.; Birke, L. The horse has got to want to help: Human-animal habituses and networks
in amateur show jumping. In Sport, Animals, and Society; Gillett, J., Gilbert, M., Eds.; Routledge:
New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 69–84.
27. Thompson, K.; Nesci, C. Over-riding concerns: Developing safe relations in the high-risk
interspecies sport of eventing. Int. Rev. Sociol. Sport 2013, 2013. [CrossRef]
28. Knox-Thompson, E.; Dickens, S. Pony Club Manual No. 1: D and C Certificates; Ray Richards
Publisher: Auckland, New Zealand, 1994.
29. Knox-Thompson, E.; Dickens, S. Pony Club Manual No. 2: C+, B, A And H Certificates; Ray
Richards Publisher: Auckland, New Zealand, 1994.
30. Knox-Thompson, E.; Dickens, S. Pony Club Manual No. 3: For Instructors; Ray Richards
Publisher: Auckland, New Zealand, 1993.
31. Wolframm, I. The Science of Equestrian Sports: Theory, Practice and Performance of the
Equestrian Rider; Routledge: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2013.
32. McGreevy, P.D.; Murphy, J. Equitation science offers new horizons in the understanding of equine
performance and horse-human relationships. Vet. J. 2009,181, 1–5. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
33. Pierard, M.; Hall, C.; von Borstel, U.K.; Averis, A.; Hawson, L.; McLean, A.; Nevison, C.;
Visser, K.; McGreevy, P. Evolving protocols for research in equitation science. J. Vet. Behav.
Clin. Appl. Res. 2015,10, 255–266. [CrossRef]
34. Miu, A.C.; Heilman, R.M.; Houser, D. Anxiety impairs decision-making: Psychophysiological
evidence from an iowa gambling task. Biol. Psychol. 2008,77, 353–358. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
35. Paus, T. Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nat.
Rev. Neurosci. 2001,2, 417–424. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
36. Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977,84,
191. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
37. Rimal, R.N.; Real, K. Perceived risk and efficacy beliefs as motivators of change. Hum. Commun.
Res. 2003,29, 370–399. [CrossRef]
Animals 2015,5573
38. Beauchamp, M.R.; Whinton, L.C. Self-efficacy and other-efficacy in dyadic performance: Riding
as one in equestrian eventing. J. Sport Exercise Psychol. 2005,27, 245–252.
39. Swift, S. Centred Riding; William Heinemann Ltd.: London, UK, 1985.
40. Wanless, M. Ride with Your Mind: A Right Brain Approach to Riding; Methuen Publishing Ltd.:
York, UK, 1987.
41. Lupton, D. Risk; Routledge: London, UK, 1999.
42. Hopkins, A. Studying organisational cultures and their effects on safety. Saf. Sci. 2006,44,
875–889. [CrossRef]
43. Haukelid, K. Theories of (safety) culture revisited-an anthropological approach. Saf. Sci. 2008,46,
413–426. [CrossRef]
44. Cooper, M.D.; Phillips, R.A. Exploratory analysis of the safety climate and safety behavior
relationship. J. Safety Res. 2004,35, 497–512. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
45. McManus, P.; Albrecht, G.; Graham, R. The Global Horseracing Industry: Social, Economic,
Environmental and Ethical Perspectives; Routledge: London, UK, 2013.
46. McGreevy, P.D.; Oddie, C.F.; Hawson, L.A.; McLean, A.N.; Evans, D.L. Do vendors value safety
in thoroughbred horses in the australian recreational riding horse market? J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl.
Res. 2014,10, 153–157. [CrossRef]
47. McGreevy, P.D.; Oddie, C.; Burton, F.L.; McLean, A.N. The horse-human dyad: Can we align
horse training and handling activities with the equid social ethogram? Vet. J. 2009,181, 12–18.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
48. Raber, K.; Tucker, T.J. The Culture of the Horse: Status, Discipline, and Identity in the Early
Modern World; Palgrave Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 2005.
49. van Orden, K. From gens d’armes to gentilshommes: Dressage, civility, and the ballet à cheval. In
The Culture of the Horse: Status, Discipline, and Identity in the Early Modern World; Raber, K.,
Tucker, T.J., Eds.; Palgrave Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 197–222.
50. Xenophon. The Art of Horsemanship; J.A. Allen & Co.: London, UK, 1962.
51. Thompson, K. Theorising rider-horse relations: An ethnographic illustration of the centaur
metaphor in the spanish bullfight. In Theorising Animals; Taylor, N., Signal, T., Eds.; Brill: Leiden,
the Netherlands; Boston, MA, USA, 2011; pp. 221–253.
52. Game, A. Riding: Embodying the centaur. Body Soc. 2001,7, 1–12. [CrossRef]
53. Goodwin, D.; McGreevy, P.; Waran, N.; McLean, A. How equitation science can elucidate and
refine horsemanship techniques. Vet. J. 2009,181, 5–11. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
54. Henshall, C.; McGreevy, P.D. The role of ethology in round pen horse training-a review. Appl.
Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014,155, 1–11. [CrossRef]
55. Boot, M.; McGreevy, P.D. The x files: Xenophon re-examined through the lens of equitation
science. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2015,8, 367–375. [CrossRef]
56. McGreevy, P.; Boakes, R.; Boakes, R.A. Carrots and Sticks: Principles of Animal Training;
Darlington Press: Sydney, Australia, 2011.
57. DeAraugo, J.; McLean, A.; McLaren, S.; Caspar, G.; McLean, M.; McGreevy, P. Training
methodologies differ with the attachment of humans to horses. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res.
2014,9, 235–241. [CrossRef]
Animals 2015,5574
58. Gielen, A.C.; Sleet, D. Application of behavior-change theories and methods to injury prevention.
Epidemiol. Rev. 2003,25, 65–76. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
59. Ajzen, I. From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In Action Control: From
Cognition to Behavior; Kuhl, J., Beckman, J., Eds.; Springer: Heidelberg, Germany, 1985;
pp. 11–39.
60. Prochaska, J.O.; DiClemente, C.C.; Norcross, J.C. In search of how people change: Applications
to addictive behaviors. Amer. Psychol. 1992,47, 1102–1114. [CrossRef]
61. Fishbein, M. A theory of reasoned action: Some applications and implications. Nebr. Symp. Motiv.
1980,27, 65–116. [PubMed]
62. Helmreich, R.L.; Davies, J.M. Culture, threat, and error: Lessons from aviation. Can. J. Anesth.
2004,51, R1–R4. [CrossRef]
63. Visser, E.K.; Van Wijk-Jansen, E.E.C. Diversity in horse enthusiasts with respect to horse welfare:
An explorative study. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2012,7, 295–304. [CrossRef]
64. Sandman, P.M. Media Campaigns. Environmental Education & Communication for a Sustainable
World; Academy for Educational Development: Washington, DC, USA, 2000; pp. 79–84.
65. Veblen, T. The Theory of a Leisure Class; Penguin Books: New York, NY, USA, 1899.
66. Busick, J. Getting workers to wear ppe: Communication is key. Safety Compliance Letter, 2005,
Available online: http://www.psandman.com/articles/Busick.pdf(accessed on 14 July 2015).
67. McManus, P.; Graham, R. Horse racing and gambling: Comparing attitudes and preferences of
racetrack patrons and residents of sydney, australia. Leisure Stud. 2014,33, 400–417. [CrossRef]
68. Thompson, K.; Matthews, C. Inroads into equestrian safety: Rider-reported factors contributing to
horse–related accidents and near misses on Australian roads. Animals 2015,5, 592–609.
69. Chapman, C.; Musselwhite, C.B.A. Equine road user safety: Public attitudes, understandings and
beliefs from a qualitative study in the united kingdom. Accid. Anal. Prevent. 2011,43, 2173–2181.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
70. Cassidy, R.L. Horse People: Thoroughbred Culture in Lexington and Newmarket; John Hopkins
University Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2010.
71. Krumeich, A.; Weijts, W.; Reddy, P.; Meijer-Weitz, A. The benefits of anthropological approaches
for health promotion research and practice. Health Educ. Res. 2001,16, 121–130. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
72. Thompson, K. For pets’ sake, save yourself: Motivating emergency and disaster preparedness
through relations of animal guardianship. Autr. J. Emerg. Manag. 2015,30, 43–46.
73. Brandt, K. A language of their own: An interactionist approach to human-horse communication.
Soc. Anim. 2004,12, 299–316. [CrossRef]
74. Montoya, D.; McManus, P.; Albrecht, G. Jumping to conclusions? Media coverage of jumps racing
debates in australia. Soc. Anim. 2012,20, 273–293. [CrossRef]
75. McManus, P.; Montoya, D. Toward new understandings of human-animal relationships in sport: A
study of australian jumps racing. Soc. Cult. Geogr. 2012,13, 399–420. [CrossRef]
76. Eckardt, F.; Münz, A.; Witte, K. Application of a full body inertial measurement system in dressage
riding. J. Equine. Vet. Sci. 2014,34, 1294–1299. [CrossRef]
Animals 2015,5575
77. Janura, M.; Peham, C.; Dvorakova, T.; Elfmark, M. An assessment of the pressure distribution
exerted by a rider on the back of a horse during hippotherapy. Hum. Movement Sci. 2009,28,
387–393. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
78. Peham, C.; Kotschwar, A.B.; Borkenhagen, B.; Kuhnke, S.; Molsner, J.; Baltacis, A. A comparison
of forces acting on the horse’s back and the stability of the rider’s seat in different positions at the
trot. Vet. J. 2010,184, 56–59. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
79. Tsohantaridis, V.A. Development of the Jewel Equestrian Scale. Unpublished Thesis, George Fox
University, Portland, OR, USA, 2004.
80. Lyng, S. Edgework: A social psychological analysis of voluntary risk taking. Amer. J. Sociol.
1990,95, 851–886. [CrossRef]
81. Giulianotti, R. Risk and sport: An analysis of sociological theories and research agendas. Sociol.
Sport J. 2009,26, 540–556.
82. Zuckerman, M.; Neeb, M. Sensation Seeking and Psychopathology; Hogrefe & Huber Publishers:
Washington, DC, USA, 1979.
83. Wolframm, I.A.; Shearman, J.; Micklewright, D. A preliminary investigation into pre–competitive
mood states of advanced and novice equestrian dressage riders. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 2010,22,
333–342. [CrossRef]
84. McManus, P.; Graham, R.; Ruse, K. The construction of human-animal relations. National jumps
day 2013 at Te Rapa, Hamilton, New Zealand. N. Z. Geographer 2014,70, 190–200. [CrossRef]
85. Lawrence, K.; McManus, P. Towards household sustainability in sydney? Impacts of two
sustainable lifestyle workshop programs on water consumption in existing homes. Geogr. Res.
2008,46, 314–332. [CrossRef]
86. Trigg, J.; Smith, B.; Thompson, K. Does emotional closeness to pets motivate their inclusion in
bushfire survival plans? Implications for emergency communicators. Aust. J. Emerg. Manag.
2015,30, 24–30.
87. Trigg, J.; Thompson, K.; Smith, B.; Bennett, P. Engaging pet owners in disaster risk and
preparedness communications: Simplifying complex human–animal relations with archetypes.
Environ. Hazards 2015. [CrossRef]
© 2015 by the authors; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
... and vast opportunities for individuals to engage in physical activity across their lifespan, but equestrian activities come with an inherent risk (Keener et al., 2023;Loder, 2008). Epidemiological studies evaluating emergency department (ED) and public health databases have highlighted that these activities have higher hospital admission rates compared to participation in rugby or football, skiing, or riding motorcycles (Ball et al., 2007;Buckley et al., 1993;Ekberg et al., 2011;Sorli, 2000;Thompson et al., 2015;Zuckerman et al., 2015). However, these studies are limited in scope. ...
... Organisational culture often dictates how individuals interact and communicate (Wreathall, 1995). Equestrian culture places higher importance on the horse's health and safety than the rider's (Kuhl et al., 2014;Thompson et al., 2015). Equestrians are often encouraged to get back in the saddle immediately after a fall unless severely hurt or impaired (Broshek, 2001;Kuhl et al., 2014). ...
... Future research should utilise reporting systems within equestrian organisations for a more comprehensive understanding of injury and pain in equestrians. Individual equestrian organisations have started implementing injury-reporting systems, such as the USEF and the United States Pony Club (USPC) (Haines et al., 2022;Thompson et al., 2015). The USPC is the only organisation publicising that they are tracking injuries at all meetings, lessons, rallies (shows), and other USPCsanctioned events. ...
Article
Full-text available
The use of trauma databases for epidemiological study of equestrian injuries is well established. However, such databases fail to capture minor injuries or injuries when medical attention is not sought. Additionally, concerns about chronic pain in equestrians have only been explored in small samples. The current study is the first nationwide survey of American equestrians 12 years and older participating in all equestrian disciplines. This study aimed to evaluate the patterns of self-reported acute injuries, behaviours of seeking medical attention for acute injuries, and chronic pain. A convenience sample of 2,573 American equestrians completed an anonymous online survey with questions on acute injury anatomical region, type, and if they sought medical attention. Additional questions identified frequency and location of chronic pain. In total, 2,049 survey responses were complete, with 96% female and representation from all regions of the United States. Participation status (PS) was categorised as professionals (19%), amateurs (43%), and recreational (38%). PS was associated with acute injury and chronic pain frequency. Only 3.9% had never experienced an acute injury from equestrian activity. Of all injuries, foot injuries were most reported (13% of all injuries; 52% of participants) with only 39% seeking medical attention. Professionals had higher odds of experiencing acute injuries in most anatomical regions. 42% indicated they had encountered a concussion; of those, 71% sought medical assistance due to a suspected concussion. Chronic back pain frequency was reported at 27.5%. This study is the first to evaluate self-reported injury in a large convenience sample of American equestrians. The current study found a higher rate of lower extremity injuries in equestrians than previously captured in trauma databases. Prior concussion and acute injury increased the odds of individuals experiencing regional chronic pain, suggesting that future studies and interventions should focus on improving preventative and rehabilitative care.
... In addition, psychosocial risk factors for the development of MDP in veterinarians have been documented in various studies, namely high job stress and low job satisfaction [10] or personal and workplace issues [11]. Furthermore, animal-induced accidents are a major risk for veterinarians [48,[61][62][63][64][65][66][67][68][69]. The anxiety of being seriously injured could enhance the development of MDP, as anxiety is associated with the prevalence of MDP [70]. ...
... For future interventions, veterinary students should be provided with effective practical advice for the prevention of injuries and accidents with horses (and other large animals) and with safety measures, e.g., sedation [66]. Improving the predictability of horses could not only advance safety in human-horse interactions [69,83,84] but also improve the feeling of being in control when veterinary students handle horses. Hence, it may work as an adaptive coping strategy for potentially dangerous humananimal interactions. ...
... This could be protective not only regarding accidents during human-animal interactions but could also enhance coping with other stressful situations. It must be noted that with acceptance of horse-related injuries and deaths in the equestrian culture [66,69,83,84], bravery and risk-taking in veterinarians, as well as the culture of working through pain, might also hinder safe occupational working styles [53]. While improvement in proper patient safety seems self-evident [86], the lack of adequate safety measures also leads to veterinarians having a high incidence of workrelated accidents [48,[61][62][63][64][65][66][67][68][69]87]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary A relevant number of veterinarians leave the profession within the first years after graduation. In a preliminary study, we investigated the effects of very short, active interventions, called microbreaks, in 36 students of veterinary medicine. Students were encouraged to integrate these microbreaks into their days over 6 weeks, within the total observational period of 12 weeks. Additionally, information on the ergonomics of a variety of veterinary tasks was given in weekly interactive talks with practicing clinicians and a physiotherapist. At the start, many veterinary students reported musculoskeletal discomfort in the neck and the lower back. All students were able to incorporate the microbreaks well during their study activities and found them relieving. After 12 weeks, the participating students reported fewer painful body regions, and that their self-efficacy in potentially painful, risky, or dangerous interactions with animals in a veterinary setting had increased. The feeling of control over dangerous situations increased with dogs but decreased with horses. Most students wanted to continue the microbreaks in the future and judged the topic of the present study to be relevant to their profession. Abstract Occupational hazards, such as psychosocial stressors, physical injuries from human–animal interactions, and physically demanding work tasks, are common in the veterinary profession, and musculoskeletal discomfort and pain (MDP) may already be present in veterinary undergraduates. This preliminary study investigates the effects of very short, active interventions, called microbreaks, in 36 veterinary students. At the start, participants had a high prevalence of MDP, especially in the neck and lower back. Within a 12-week observational period, 6 weeks of active intervention comprised teaching microbreaks (nine strengthening, stretching, and relaxation exercises; 30–90 s each) and a weekly veterinary-specific ergonomics education and discussion. After the intervention, participants reported fewer painful body regions and an increase in their self-efficacy in potentially painful, risky, or dangerous human–animal interactions. After the 12-week observational period, participants had increased self-efficacy in the maintenance of physical health and self-protection but decreased self-efficacy in healing injuries after veterinary human–animal interactions. Participants felt to have increased and decreased control over dangerous situations with dogs and horses, respectively, although self-efficacy in handling horses increased. Participants integrated microbreaks well into their undergraduate activities and rated the topic relevant to their (later) profession. This should encourage the inclusion of similar programs in undergraduate curricula.
... Poor compatibility between horse and rider, known as a "mismatch" [9], can compromise safety and may also compromise both horse and human welfare. The equestrian industry has traditionally focussed on the use of personal protective equipment (PPE) such as helmets, boots, gloves and body protectors to minimise the consequences of equestrianrelated injuries [10]. However, it has been suggested that PPE alone is insufficient as a sole risk-management strategy [10]. ...
... The equestrian industry has traditionally focussed on the use of personal protective equipment (PPE) such as helmets, boots, gloves and body protectors to minimise the consequences of equestrianrelated injuries [10]. However, it has been suggested that PPE alone is insufficient as a sole risk-management strategy [10]. Higher-level safety controls to further mitigate risk include appropriate rider-horse matching and understanding which horses (age, breed, experience, Animals 2023, 13, 1481 3 of 23 education and history) are most likely to be unpredictable, and therefore less suitable, for certain riders [10,11]. ...
... However, it has been suggested that PPE alone is insufficient as a sole risk-management strategy [10]. Higher-level safety controls to further mitigate risk include appropriate rider-horse matching and understanding which horses (age, breed, experience, Animals 2023, 13, 1481 3 of 23 education and history) are most likely to be unpredictable, and therefore less suitable, for certain riders [10,11]. For these reasons, it is important to understand what niche feral horses occupy in the recreational riding horse market to optimise horse-human welfare outcomes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Feral horses, also known as brumbies, are widely distributed across Australia with some populations being managed largely by human intervention. Rehoming of suitable feral horses following passive trapping has wide community acceptance as a management tool. However, there is little information about the number and relative economic value of feral horses compared with cohorts in the riding horse market. We examined 15,404 advertisements of horses for sale in 53 editions of Horse Deals, published from February 2017 to July 2022. Despite the considerable media attention and public scrutiny surrounding feral horse management, rehomed feral horses represented only a tiny fraction of the horse market in the current study. Of the 15,404 advertisements examined, only 128 (0.0083%) were for feral horses. We recorded phrases used to describe behavioural characteristics and other variables. The following variables were found to be not independent: Ridden Status, Height, Age, Sex, Colour, and Warning terms/more work. Using descriptive statistics to describe basic features of the data, the average price for feral horses ($1408) was lower than that for domestic horses ($1790) with the maximum price for a domestic horse being nearly twice the maximum for a feral horse. Univariate analysis showed feral horses were over represented among “Unbroken” horses and underrepresented among “Ridden”, “Broodmare” and “Harness” horses compared with domestic bred horses (p < 0.001). Feral horses appeared over-represented at shorter heights, among younger age groups (3 years or younger and 3.1 to 6 years) (p < 0.001) and in the dilute colour category (p = 0.008). The multivariable mixed model on price revealed that for domestic horses, the highest estimated marginal mean price averaged across the colour categories was for ridden horses aged 6.1–10-year-old at $1657.04 (95% CI $1320.56–$2074.66). In contrast, for feral horses, the multivariable mixed model demonstrated the similar highest estimated marginal mean averaged was for green broken 3–6-year-old horses that have undergone foundation training under saddle at $2526.97 (95% CI $1505.63–$4208.27). Australian feral horses were valued differently from similar domestic horses in the recreational riding horse market and further research is warranted to determine appropriate target markets and boost the sustainability of rehoming as a feral horse management tool.
... However, there is an increasing understanding of the paradigm of shared human-animal risk. This concept examines injury risk in terms of "one welfare", whereby when animals are at risk, so are people [10][11][12][13]. The corollary is that reducing the risk and preventing injury to one species may also prevent injury to the other [14]. ...
... Based on this and other recently published works on the injury of horses during transport by the authors and others, a review of education strategies that address this shared "one welfare" risk in the industry is prudent [1,52,53]. Any approach to reducing the shared risk of human and equine injury is most likely facilitated by addressing issues of horse welfare [14], multidisciplinary approaches to understanding horse-related risk [11], and engagement of the principles of equitation science to improve the safety of horse-human interactions [54]. Horse and handler training, personal protective equipment such as helmets and gloves, and safety awareness strategies should be developed that are specific to the at-risk demographic and industry activity [1,43]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary There is an increased understanding of shared human–animal risk in terms of “one welfare”, whereby when animals are at risk, so are people. Reducing the risk and preventing injury to one species may also prevent injury to the other. The authors of the current manuscript considered this approach to study road equine transport-related injuries to humans in New Zealand and so aimed to determine their frequency and associated factors. New Zealand horse industry participants were surveyed on their horse industry, activities, and road transport experiences and asked if they had experienced horse-related self-injury. There were 112/1067 (10.5%) owners and carers injured while preparing, loading, traveling, or unloading. Of these, four in ten had multiple injury types, and a third had several body regions affected. Hand injury was most common, followed by the foot, arm, head, or face. Injuries were associated with the responder’s industry educational background, driving experience, and reporting a horse injured during road transport in the past two years. Findings support wearing helmets and gloves, and adopting strategies designed to eliminate equine injuries associated with the road transport of horses to reduce the risk of injury to their handlers. Abstract There is an increased understanding of shared human–animal risk in terms of “one welfare”, whereby when animals are at risk, so are people, so preventing injury to one species may also prevent injury to the other. Because transport-related injuries to horses are common, the authors considered this paradigm to study road equine transport-related injuries to humans in New Zealand. The aim was to determine their frequency and associated factors by distributing a survey to horse industry participants through industry organisations asking about their horse activities, road transport experiences, and any related self-injury. There were 112/1067 (10.5%) handlers injured while preparing (13/112), loading (39/112), traveling (6/112), or unloading (33/112). Of these, 40% had multiple injury types, and 33% had several body regions affected. Hand injuries were most common (46%), followed by the foot (25%), arm (17%), and head or face (15%). Median recovery time was 7 days. Injuries were associated with the responder’s industry educational background, years of driving experience, and reporting a horse injured during road transport in the past two years. Wearing helmets and gloves, and adopting strategies designed to eliminate equine injuries associated with the road transport of horses to reduce the risk of injury to their handlers are recommended.
... Despite this difference in experience, horse-related accidents do often occur to both professional and non-professional horse people (Hausberger et al. 2008). In any case, an interspecific misunderstanding has been found to be the main reason for these horse-related human injuries (Thompson et al. 2015). It is worth highlighting that the TM technique found an association between 'work' and 'lesion' and between 'rider' and 'accident'. ...
Article
Full-text available
Animal welfare protection is a challenge for which policy-makers are demanding scientific-based evidence. Considering horses, the challenge is even greater, as their role in society varies from production to companion animals. This review aimed to describe the evolution and geographical distribution of horse welfare literature over the years, to identify the most studied research topics and to highlight the still present gaps in knowledge. A search on Scopus V R bibliometric database was performed using different welfare-related keywords. Predetermined filters were posed on time range, language and subject area. A total of 6584 records were downloaded and screened retaining only the ones dealing with horse welfare and behavioural sciences. Descriptive statistics, text mining (TM) and topic analysis (TA) were performed on the abstract of 801 eligible records. The results showed the number of studies on horse welfare is exponentially increasing, especially in the European continent. 'Transport' was the most frequent word, but also 'behaviour' and 'pain' emerged. Nine topics were identified and covered different subject areas: stress analysis, pain assessment, equitation science, human-horse interactions, breeding management, transport, working equids and health. The evaluation of stress and pain, horse social behaviour and the human-horse relationship resulted to be the most studied aspects related to animal welfare. On the contrary, the concept of positive welfare and the welfare assessment at slaughter of horses were underrepresented, confirming the limited literature on these subjects. This review confirmed the growing impact of research on horse welfare and highlighted how some important areas still do not provide sufficient scientific evidence.
... For this reason, the intervention of a person who does not know the behavioral manifestations of horses can lead to serious failures. Conversely, if man is familiar with their ethological features, he can discover new ways to increase their productivity and discover new benefits from their use (Hausberger & Muller, 2002;Hausberger et al., 2008;Thompson et al., 2015;Hall et al., 2018;Butler et al., 2019;Kelly et al., 2021). The purpose of the research is to study basic ethological, functional signs of karakachan horses from the national gene pool in ecological and biocompatible breeding. ...
Article
Full-text available
The observation was made on a herd of Karakachan horses, bred freely in the area of the village of Prisadets, Southern Bulgaria. The study covers a period of one year, with 24-hour field surveys conducted every month. The analysis shows that horses drink water relatively rarely. Although not statistically proven, the highest percentage of horses drink water at moderate ambient temperatures-about 22 o C, while at high temperatures around 36 o C, when standing in the shade, the percentage of horses drinking water drops to 46.15%. Karakachan horses spend most of their time grazing. Most horses graze at noon (91.28%), and the least at night (40.90%). The factor time of the day had a significant effect on the following traits of behavior: grazing (P <0.001), sleeping / resting lying down (P <0.001), sleeping/resting standing (P <0.01), standing on alert (P <0.01) and standing in the shade (P <0.01). Ambient temperature affects resting behavior: lying down (P <0.01) and standing (P <0.01), chasing insects with a tail and head (P <0.001), and standing on alert (P < 0.001).
Article
In equestrian sport, women are popularly thought to compete on equal terms with men. This ideal has been critiqued by studies documenting how women's participation is impacted by gendered equestrian cultures and society more broadly. However, one physiological process has been overlooked-menstruation. This is the first study to focus specifically on the impacts of menstruation and menstrual concerns on women's participation in horse sport and to locate those concerns within broader equestrian traditions of attire and discourses of control. The authors conducted a cross-sectional survey of 328 female riding members of Pony Club Australia aged 10-17 years. Findings challenge the idea of equestrian as a sport where men and women compete "on equal terms." In particular, they illustrate how traditional dress codes of white-/light-colored riding pants exacerbate menstrual concerns, limit/prevent participation, and pose safety concerns due to distraction. The majority (80%) of participants supported changing the formal uniform to allow darker-colored riding pants. However, longstanding equestrian associations between white pants, prestige, and respect are a barrier to the adoption and therefore normalization of period-friendly attire. While discriminatory rules or standards need to be revised, they are insufficient to challenge entrenched gendered, socio-historical, and esthetic constructions of some organized sports. The authors therefore recommend that rule changes be supported by other practical and educational initiatives necessary to create period-friendly cultures for equestrian sport.
Article
The aim of the study was to evaluate the personality components of Arabian and Thoroughbred horses (TB) using the Horse Personality Questionnaire (HPQ) and examine their relationship with breed, sex, and racing performance. Within the scope of the research, race data of 96 horses from Thoroughbred (n = 56) and Arabian horse (n = 40) breeds that participated in flat races were taken from the official websites of Turkish Jockey Club. HPQ was constructed using 26 behaviorally defined adjectives. Thereafter, latent class analysis was carried out to determine distinct classes (subgroups) by using the questionnaire that described the personalities of horses. Three distinct personality classes of both TB and Arabian horses were compared. While there was a significant difference in sex distribution among the personality classes of TB (P = 0.023), this difference was not observed in the classes of Arabian horses (P = 0.580). There was no significant difference in terms of age and some racing characteristics in the per- sonality classes of both Arabian and TB horses (P > 0.05). In conclusion, it was seen that some personality classes of the horses could not be determined in the type of management system where there was no paddock and horses cannot display some of their intra-species social behaviors. For understanding horse personality, it is crucial to observe their intra-species interactions Simultaneously, by evaluating the data on horse personality from a scientific perspective and appraising the sportive performance of horses with a holistic approach, ethical equitation would be possible.
Article
Full-text available
Presents an integrative theoretical framework to explain and to predict psychological changes achieved by different modes of treatment. This theory states that psychological procedures, whatever their form, alter the level and strength of self-efficacy. It is hypothesized that expectations of personal efficacy determine whether coping behavior will be initiated, how much effort will be expended, and how long it will be sustained in the face of obstacles and aversive experiences. Persistence in activities that are subjectively threatening but in fact relatively safe produces, through experiences of mastery, further enhancement of self-efficacy and corresponding reductions in defensive behavior. In the proposed model, expectations of personal efficacy are derived from 4 principal sources of information: performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and physiological states. Factors influencing the cognitive processing of efficacy information arise from enactive, vicarious, exhortative, and emotive sources. The differential power of diverse therapeutic procedures is analyzed in terms of the postulated cognitive mechanism of operation. Findings are reported from microanalyses of enactive, vicarious, and emotive modes of treatment that support the hypothesized relationship between perceived self-efficacy and behavioral changes. (21/2 p ref)
Book
Full-text available
Throughout history animals have been used to assist humans in work and play or simply to satisfy our curiosity. Several paintings from Ancient Egypt demonstrate that we have been charming, cajoling and exploiting animals for many thousands of years. One example depicts men hand-feeding hyenas that are shown lying on their backs, a feature that strongly suggests that they were tame. There is evidence from the same source that gazelles, ibex and oryx were equally relaxed in human company. In view of the enormous investment of time required for the gentling of non-domesticated species, it is fascinating to speculate about the jobs these animals performed in Ancient Egypt. Some of the uses to which animals have been put in the past may seem unacceptable by modern ethical standards. For example, the Romans tied songbirds to bushes in their gardens and even used animals to torture and execute their enemies. Animals have long been used to keep vermin such as rats away from human households or grain stores and to act as guards warning of possible intruders. Across different cultures such guards have included geese, guinea fowl and pigs, as well as dogs. Large species such as horses, donkeys and cattle have for many millennia been used as sources of power. In its crudest form this means traction, as in pulling ploughs, sleds or carts. Later, animals were also used to provide power for primitive machines designed, for example, for milling grain or for raising water from deep wells. Similarly, dogs were forced to run in large wall-mounted wheels to turn roasting-spits. None of the forms of work mentioned so far required large changes in the animals’ behaviour. In contrast to these relatively simple uses of animals, in the domains of hunting and herding humans since pre-history have sought to increase their efficiency by investing considerable time in training animals. Training means changing the frequency with which animals show certain behaviours. Unwelcome behaviours become less likely, while desirable ones become more likely. Ancient Egyptians even tamed cheetahs for hunting and the work that these big cats performed may have been seen as the most sophisticated and effective hunting tool then available. However, this is a very unusual example. More generally, hunting and herding were the domains in which the dog truly came to the fore as the most trainable of all species. The role of animals in warfare and in the relative success of different human cultures is often under-estimated. The cultures that have prevailed from ancient origins are those that most fully exploited a variety of animals in combat, especially horses. Chief among the peoples that owed their success to the horse were the Mongol hordes. These excellent equestrians used their horses as sources of milk and meat when they were not exploiting their fleetness of foot for lightning raids on unmounted victims. The training of horses to perform specific behaviours useful in warfare eventually gave rise to the emergence of military riding academies. The haute-ecole dressage movements that the Lippizaner stallions of Vienna now perform in their displays were first developed some four hundred years ago to vanquish enemies in face-to-face combat. Training and riding skills contributed to the success of armies and the survival of individuals. Horses are not the only species to have been conscripted into human conflicts. Dogs and pigeons were used to carry messages during the trench warfare of the World War I. In World War II the Russians used carefully selected dogs as anti-tank operatives, while the U.S. Navy trained dolphins to place explosive devices on the hulls of ships. The same war prompted research into the deployment of pigeons to guide what was intended as the world's first smart missile. Three pigeons were strapped into position and trained to peck a spot on recognition of approximations to their target, this peck being transmitted to the guidance system of a missile that was actually never used. This long tradition of involving animals in human conflict still continues. Dolphins were used to search for mines in the second Gulf War and dogs are used to detect landmines and are trained to search buildings for terrorists with tiny cameras strapped to their foreheads. Explosive detection is becoming ever more sophisticated these days with techniques that concentrate volatile substances from a single site and seal them in small air-tight capsules so that these can be sent to the dog for his opinion. Although the behaviour of intensively trained animals can fascinate us, the animals with which most of us have frequent contact are those that have come into our homes as companions. We may be using animals less in the workplace, but we are not necessarily spending less time with them. Even highly domesticated companion animals need to be trained, although the level of dedication and expertise needed is far below that required to train a Lippizaner stallion or mine-detecting dolphin to perform at a high level. Over the very long history of training animals, a variety of expert traditions have developed. The language used to describe them is just as varied. For example, the way a shepherd describes how to train his dog is very different from the accounts of how they train their animals that might be given by a falconer or by an elephant trainer. The ways in which these different animals are trained also appears to differ enormously and in turn seems quite different from the advice given in a booklet on ‘How to train your pet’. However, the basic idea behind Carrots and Sticks is that these differences are superficial ones and that the same general principles apply to any kind of animal training. We reached this conclusion by different routes. One of us (PMcG) trained as a veterinarian and specialized in animal behaviour, with a particular interest in and love of dogs and horses. The other (RAB) trained as a research psychologist, with a particular interest in comparative psychology and learning theory. This book is a result of our collaboration in trying to make clear what we believe these general principles to be. One set of principles has to do with behaviour that is largely determined by what kind of animal it is. We refer to this as instinctive behaviour. Although this is an old fashioned and ambiguous term, nevertheless it is better than any other label for denoting behaviour more strongly determined by an animal’s genes – its nature - than by its experience – its nurture. Chapter One discusses those aspects of instinctive behaviour that are important from the perspective of an animal trainer, and also the way that instinctive behavior changes as a result of experience; hence the title Instincts and their modification. One of the core principles of training is that based on positive reward; the ‘carrot’. The properties of such learning have been extensively studied by psychologists using various kinds of conditioning methods. This research had led to the principles of importance to animal training that are described in Chapter Two. A related set of principles, described in Chapter Three, have been derived from conditioning studies that have employed aversive events – ‘sticks’ – to find out how punishment works (and sometimes doesn’t work) and how avoidance behaviour is learned. Many attempts at training fail because the trainer assumes that animals have very human-like ways of perceiving and thinking about the world. The limitations of this assumption and the realities of animal intelligence are the main topics in Chapter Four. You are invited to approach the two parts of the book in different ways. The first part can be read in the conventional way from beginning to end, while the second part has a quite different format. It contains a range of case histories to illustrate how the basic principles have been put into practice by trainers. The cases are intended for browsing in no particular order. Since the overall goal of this book is to take the mystery out of training, in the case studies we have unpicked the various processes by which the animals acquired their sometimes amazing behaviours. The accounts of their training are offered as illustrations of training practices. They are not intended as models for readers to emulate. The performances you see represent the end-points of a long process of behavioural modification that may have begun when the animals were very young. Having considered various approaches, you will be better able to decide for yourself whether it is right or wrong that animals are used in these ways. Are certain behaviours undignified? How can animal welfare be ensured when animals are required to work for a living? Should zoos require their animals to perform? Can this enrich their lives? It is possible that your informed response to these questions may then be at odds, say, with your views on riding horses. Regardless of these dilemmas, the information in this book should add to your fascination with the non-human animals with whom we share the world. The main theme of this book is that, despite huge diversity in the aims of different kinds of training and in the way that trainers explain their methods, all successful training depends largely on the principles we discuss in the four chapters of Part One.
Article
Full-text available
One-day equestrian eventing comprises three disciplines: dressage, showjumping, and cross-country. Participants in the present study were 187 riders (38 M, 149 F) competing in one of two intermediate-level 1-day competitions. Participants' perceptions of their own (self-efficacy) and their horses' abilities (other-efficacy) were assessed 30 minutes prior to each stage of competition and examined in relation to subsequent riding performance. For dressage, self efficacy (beta = -.20, p < .05) and other-efficacy (beta = -.26, p < .01) were each able to explain unique variance in dressage performance (adj. R-2 = .16). However, for both show-jumping and cross-country disciplines, neither form of efficacy was associated with the corresponding measure of riding performance. The results for show-jumping and cross-country may be explained by the lack of variability and heavily skewed nature of the performance data in these two contexts. Consistent with previous research, the results for dressage suggest that self-efficacy may be an important predictor of performance in sport. However, in line with theorizing by Lent and Lopez (2002), the results suggest that other-efficacy may also be an important predictor of behavioral enactment within performing dyads.
Chapter
It is only within the last century—very suddenly and very recently, given their long career—that horses are no longer intrinsic to the basic operations of Western society; few of us any longer rely on them for anything we consider indispensable. Horsemanship is still tightly woven into our speaking and our thinking, however, in countless figures that make up a kind of thoroughgoing metaphorical fabric. We speak of being back (or tall) in the saddle, being spurred to do something, reining in someone or something. We transpose equine experience into human terms with concepts such as “keeping pace,” “hitting one’s stride,” “getting off on the wrong foot,” “kicking up one’s heels,” “feeling one’s oats.” Such metaphors are used regularly by people who have never come near a horse to address matters of intention, control, and enactment, and they show remarkably few signs of being supplanted by automotive imagery. Given the readiness with which language adapts to social and technological change, it seems unlikely that the persistence of horsemanship metaphors in modern English is merely an odd pocket of resistance. I would instead suggest that it has to do with the intricate and as yet irreplaceable ways in which horses have represented human embodiment in Western culture.
Book
"It is not thought as such that can move anything, but thought which is for the sake of something and is practical." This discerning insight, which dates back more than 2000years to Aristotle, seems to have been ignored by most psycholo­ gists. For more than 40years theories of human action have assumed that cogni­ tion and action are merely two sides of the same coin. Approaches as different as S-O-R behaviorism,social learning theory, consistency theories,and expectancy­ value theories of motivation and decision making have one thing in common: they all assume that "thought (or any other type of cognition) can move any­ thing," that there is a direct path from cognition to behavior. In recent years, we have become more and more aware of the complexities in­ volved in the relationship between cognition and behavior. People do not always do what they intend to do. Aside from several nonpsychological factors capable of reducing cognition-behavior consistency, there seems to be a set of complex psychological mechanisms which intervene between action-related cognitions, such as beliefs, expectancies, values, and intentions,and the enactment of the be­ havior suggested by those cognitions. In our recent research we have focused on volitional mechanismus which presumably enhance cognition-behavior consistency by supporting the main­ tenance of activated intentions and prevent them from being pushed aside by competing action tendencies.
Book
This volume fills an important gap in the analysis of early modern history and culture by reintroducing scholars to the significance of the horse. A more complete understanding of the role of horses and horsemanship is absolutely crucial to our understanding of the early modern world. Each essay in the collection provides a snapshot of how horse culture and the broader culture - that tapestry of images, objects, structures, sounds, gestures, texts, and ideas - articulate. Without knowledge of how the horse figured in all these aspects, no version of political, material, or intellectual culture in the period can be entirely accurate.
Chapter
In 1594, the great horseman Antoine de Pluvinel opened France?s first military academy, aiming, he said, to render his students ?capable of serving their Prince well, whether in peace or in war.?1 The academy filled a genuine need, as before that time young French nobles keen to fulfill the military calling of their estate had had to travel to Italy for training, where they studied at the riding school of Cesare Fiaschi in Ferrara or in Naples with Federico Grisone, Giovanni Battista Pignatelli (at whose school Pluvinel had studied for six years), or Cesare Mirabbello. After a stay that usually began no earlier than age 14 and lasted one or two years, students returned to France, where they commanded respect through their poise in the saddle.