ArticlePDF Available

Color constancy in natural scenes with and without an explicit illuminant cue

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Observers can generally make reliable judgments of surface color in natural scenes despite changes in an illuminant that is out of view. This ability has sometimes been attributed to observers' estimating the spectral properties of the illuminant in order to compensate for its effects. To test this hypothesis, two surface-color-matching experiments were performed with images of natural scenes obtained from high-resolution hyperspectral images. In the first experiment, the sky illuminating the scene was directly visible to the observer, and its color was manipulated. In the second experiment, a large gray sphere was introduced into the scene so that its illumination by the sun and sky was also directly visible to the observer, and the color of that illumination was manipulated. Although the degree of color constancy varied across this and other variations of the images, there was no reliable effect of illuminant color. Even when the sky was eliminated from view, color constancy did not worsen. Judging surface color in natural scenes seems to be independent of an explicit illuminant cue.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Color constancy in natural scenes with and without
an explicit illuminant cue
KINJIRO AMANO,
1
DAVID H. FOSTER,
1
and SÉRGIO M.C. NASCIMENTO
2
1Sensing, Imaging, and Signal Processing Group, School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University of Manchester,
Manchester, United Kingdom
2Department of Physics, Gualtar Campus, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
(Received March 8, 2006; Accepted March 8, 2006!
Abstract
Observers can generally make reliable judgments of surface color in natural scenes despite changes in an illuminant
that is out of view. This ability has sometimes been attributed to observers’ estimating the spectral properties of the
illuminant in order to compensate for its effects. To test this hypothesis, two surface-color-matching experiments
were performed with images of natural scenes obtained from high-resolution hyperspectral images. In the first
experiment, the sky illuminating the scene was directly visible to the observer, and its color was manipulated. In the
second experiment, a large gray sphere was introduced into the scene so that its illumination by the sun and sky
was also directly visible to the observer, and the color of that illumination was manipulated. Although the degree
of color constancy varied across this and other variations of the images, there was no reliable effect of illuminant
color. Even when the sky was eliminated from view, color constancy did not worsen. Judging surface color in
natural scenes seems to be independent of an explicit illuminant cue.
Keywords: Color constancy, Sky, Natural scenes, Illuminant estimate, Spatial cone-excitation ratios, Specular
highlights
Introduction
The spectrum of the light reaching the eye from a scene depends
both on the reflecting properties of surfaces within it and on the
spectrum of the light illuminating it. Consequently, to make an
accurate judgment of the color of a particular surface, an observer
must somehow discount the effects of the illuminant ~von Helm-
holtz, 1867!, even when the illuminant itself is not directly visible.
To a variable extent, human observers are successful in this task,
that is, color constancy holds, and there is a large literature,
partially summarized elsewhere ~Foster, 2003; Smithson, 2005!,
reporting estimates of the degree of color constancy with geomet-
ric stimuli, both planar ~e.g. Arend & Reeves, 1986!and three-
dimensional ~e.g. Kraft & Brainard, 1999; de Almeida et al., 2004!.
Some theories of color constancy assume that the observer
estimates the spectral properties of the illuminant in order to
compensate for its effects, an estimation that is theoretically pos-
sible in some circumstances ~D’Zmura & Iverson, 1993a, 1993b,
1994; Maloney, 1999!. Yet just because observers can estimate the
illuminant spectrum does not necessarily imply that they do.
Certainly, for matching colored papers in Mondrian-like patterns,
it appears to be unnecessary ~Amano et al., 2005!. But is it pos-
sible that explicit information about an illuminant, rather than
indirect inference, might influence judgments, particularly in those
natural scenes where the illuminant forms part of the normal field
of view?
To test this hypothesis, two surface-color-matching experi-
ments were performed with images of natural scenes obtained
from high-resolution hyperspectral images ~Foster et al., 2004!.
The cues to the illuminant in the scenes were manipulated using an
approach that was similar to the cue-perturbation method adopted
by Yang and Maloney ~2001!~see also Linnell & Foster, 1997!.
The images used here were of an urban and a rural scene, as shown
in Fig. 1. In the first experiment, the sky ~but not the sun!
illuminating the scene was made clearly visible to the observer,
and its color was varied. The rationale was that including the sky
in the scene may or may not improve performance, since its
spectrum was consistent with the radiance spectrum of the scene,
but changing its spectrum, so that it was inconsistent with the
radiance spectrum of the scene, ought to have a biasing effect. In
fact, although the degree of color constancy varied across this and
other changes to the images, there was no reliable effect of the
color of the sky. When the sky was eliminated from view, perfor-
mance was neither better nor worse. Despite the absence of any
reliable effect of a visible sky, it might be argued that observers
were able to separate the effects of the ambient illumination it
provided from the direct illumination by the sun, and ignore the
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Kinjiro Amano, Sens-
ing, Imaging, and Signal Processing Group, School of Electrical and
Electronic Engineering, University of Manchester, Manchester M60 1QD,
UK. E-mail: k.amano@manchester.ac.uk
Visual Neuroscience ~2006!,23, 351–356. Printed in the USA.
Copyright © 2006 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238006 $16.00
DOI: 10.10170S0952523806233285
351
former. Since there are practical problems in imaging a scene with
a visible sun, in the second experiment, a large gray sphere was
introduced into the scene so that its illumination by the sun ~and
sky!could be reliably inferred from its diffuse highlight, and the
color of the illumination on the sphere varied. Again, there was no
reliable effect of illuminant color.
Judging surface color in natural scenes seems to be indepen-
dent of explicit knowledge of the color of the illumination.
Materials and methods
Stimuli
Two natural scenes were selected from a hyperspectral image
database ~Foster et al., 2004; Foster et al., 2006a!. Hyperspec-
tral data were used to enable the accurate and independent
control of illuminant and reflectance spectra. The data were
obtained with a high-resolution hyperspectral imaging system,
based on a digital camera with a spatial resolution of 1344
1024 pixels ~Hamamatsu, model C4742-95-12ER, Hamamatsu
Photonics K.K., Japan!with a fast tuneable liquid-crystal filter
~VariSpec, model VS-VIS2-10-HC-35-SQ, Cambridge Research
& Instrumentation, Inc., MA!mounted in front of the lens,
together with an infrared blocking filter. Peak-transmission wave-
length of the filter was varied in 10-nm steps over 400–720 nm.
Further details of the system and its calibration are given in
Foster et al. ~2004!and Foster et al. ~2006a!.
An urban scene was used for the first experiment and a rural
scene for the second experiment, as shown in Fig. 1. The urban
scene, from Porto, Portugal, consisted of a group of sixteenth–
eighteenth-century buildings and sky. The test surface was one of
the roofs in the middle of the image, which was illuminated by the
sky. The rural scene, from the Brufe region, Portugal, consisted of
fields and trees, some low farm buildings, distant mountains, and
sky. A large ~30-cm diam.!gray sphere ~painted with Munsell N7
matt emulsion paint @VeriVide Ltd., Leicester, UK#! was intro-
duced into the scene so that it reflected light from the sun, which
was to the right of the observer. The sphere was the brightest object
in the scene ~the intensity of its image was clipped in the printed
figure!, and the diffuse highlight on it gave a reliable cue to the
color of the direct illumination ~Yang & Maloney, 2001!. The test
surface was the roof of one of the farm buildings in the lower
center of the scene. In both scenes the sky occupied about a quarter
of the image and was clearly visible to observers.
Display system and calibration
Stimuli were produced on the screen of a 21-inch RGB color
monitor ~Trinitron Color Graphic Display, model GDM-F500R,
Sony Corp., Tokyo, Japan!, with spatial resolution 1600 1200
pixels, controlled by a color-graphics workstation ~Fuel V12,
Silicon Graphics, Inc., Mountain View, CA!whose 10-bit digital-
to-analog converters provided an intensity resolution of 1024
levels on each of the red, green, and blue guns. Each image was
limited to 80–85% of the displayable area of the screen. The
images of the two scenes subtended 1339 972 and 1339 1018
pixels. A calibrated telespectroradiometer ~SpectraColorimeter,
PR-650, Photo Research Inc., Chatsworth, CA!and photometer
~LMT, L1003, Lichtmesstechnik GmbH, Berlin, Germany!were
used to monitor and calibrate the display system. Calibration data
included the phosphor coordinates and voltage-intensity look-up
tables for the three guns. The monitor was allowed1htowarmup
before use.
Routine monitoring of the display system tested whether errors
in the displayed CIE ~x,y,Y!coordinates of a white test patch
were 0.005 in ~x,y!and 5% in Y~10% at low light levels!.
Tests of image fidelity used images from the experiments, as
described in Foster et al. ~2006b!. Errors for patches of width 20
pixels were 0.002 in ~u
'
,v
'
!coordinates, less than 15% of the
0.015 grid spacing in the ~u
'
,v
'
!plane used to sample observers’
responses. Since images were presented sequentially in the same
position on the screen, position-dependent chromatic errors in each
pair of images were the same. Other details of stimulus generation
and display are given in Foster et al. ~2006a!.
Stimulus variation
Images were prepared off-line. From the hyperspectral acquisition,
the color signal for each scene, that is, the original radiance image
C
0
~l;x,y!as a function of wavelength land position ~x,y!, was
recovered, along with the spectrum E
0
~l! of the global illumina-
tion on the scene, recorded at a particular reference point ~scenes
were recorded in direct sunlight under a cloudless sky or under a
sky with uniform cloud; Foster et al., 2004!. A new radiance image
C~l;x,y!was generated for a new global illuminant E~l! by
putting C~l;x,y!C
0
~l;x,y!E~l!0E
0
~l!. In effect, for each
surface in the scene, the new radiance at each point ~x,y!is the
same as that obtained by multiplying a spectral reflectance R~l;x,y!
at that point by the new global illuminant E~l!, where, by defi-
Fig. 1. Sample images of the ~a!urban and ~b!rural natural scenes used in Experiments 1 and 2. The test surface in each scene is
indicated by an arrow.
352 K. Amano, D.H. Foster, and S.M.C. Nascimento
nition, R~l;x,y!C
0
~l;x,y!0E
0
~l! ~an analysis of effective
reflectances with surfaces under direct and indirect illumination in
terms of bidirectional reflectance functions is given in Foster et al.
2006b!. For luminous and non-spectrally selective reflecting areas
in the scene, such as the sky or specular highlights, this global
illuminant change from C
0
~l;x,y!to C~l;x,y!has the critical
property that it preserves the spectral relationships between these
areas and the surfaces of the scene; for example, if the illumination
on the scene becomes more blue, then the sky and specular
highlights also become more blue.
In Experiment 1, the global illuminant E~l! was first a daylight
with correlated color temperature 25,000 K and then 6700 K; in
Experiment 2, the global illuminant was first 15,000 K and then
5700 K. This difference between the pairs of illuminants was due
to limits on the displayable color gamuts of the scenes, but the
color difference in 1976 CIE ~u
'
,v
'
!space between the members of
each pair of illuminants was the same.
The reflectance of the test surface in the first image was
manipulated independently of the global illuminant: five different
initial test-surface colors ~“yellowish,” “greenish,” etc.!were tested
in five separate blocks. In each block, the spectral reflectance of
the test surface in the second image varied randomly, from trial to
trial, in one of 65 ways ~all randomization was without replace-
ment!. This variation was achieved by a computational device, as
follows. Suppose that the initial spectral reflectance was R~l;x,y!
at each point ~x,y!of the surface and the global illuminant
spectrum was E~l!, so that the color signal was R~l;x,y!E~l!.
With a change in spectral reflectance to R
'
~l;x,y!, say, the color
signal becomes R
'
~l;x,y!E~l!. But the same color signal can be
achieved with the original R~l;x,y!by replacing E~l! locally by
a different daylight E
'
~l! such that R
'
~l;x,y!E~l! R~l;x,y!
E
'
~l!; the change in reflectance k~l! is given by k~l!R
'
~l;x,y!0
R~l;x,y!E
'
~l!0E~l!. Varying the chromaticity of this local
illuminant E
'
~l! is closely related to varying the chromaticity of
the test surface, but the representation of changes in spectral
reflectances R
'
~l;x,y!0R~l;x,y!in terms of changes in local
illuminants E
'
~l!0E~l! has the advantage of a natural colorimetric
parameterization and of a quantification that is independent of the
initial spectral reflectance of the test surface, so that averages may
be calculated over stimuli ~see Foster et al., 2001a!.
These local illuminants were constructed from a linear combi-
nation of the daylight spectral basis functions ~Judd et al., 1964!
whose corresponding chromaticities were drawn from the gamut in
the ~u
'
,v
'
!diagram consisting of 65 locations, with spacing 0.015
in the u
'
and v
'
directions, shown by the small solid points in the
graphs of Fig. 2. The same technique was used to produce the five
different initial test-surface spectra, whose corresponding chroma-
ticities were shifted from the original or Munsell N5 or N7 by
~0.015, 0!,~0, 0.015!,~0.015, 0!,~0, 0.015!, and ~0, 0!.
Changes to the spectrum of the sky in Experiment 1 and to
the spectrum of the illumination on the gray reflecting sphere in
Experiment 2 were made in the same way; that is, the radiance
spectrum C~l;x,y!at each point ~x,y!of the sky or sphere was
replaced by the spectrum C~l;x,y!k~l!, making the sky or
sphere appear, for example, more blue or more red, depending
on k~l!. This illuminant change k~l! was the same for the first
and second images, for making inconsistent changes across the
two images is known to worsen color-constancy judgments ~Foster
et al., 2006a!.
Procedure
In each trial, two images of a particular scene were presented in
sequence on the screen of the color monitor, each for 1 s, with no
interval. The images differed in their global illuminants.As already
mentioned, during the global illuminant change, the spectral re-
flectance of the test surface in the second image also changed, by
a random amount. The observer’s task was to decide whether the
test surface in the successive images was the same or different, that
is, whether an illuminant change or an illuminant change accom-
panied by a change in the spectral reflectance of the test surface
had occurred ~Craven & Foster, 1992!. Responses were made with
mouse buttons connected to a computer. Observers were allowed
to move their eyes freely. At the beginning of the session, the
experimenter indicated the identity of the test surface to the
observer verbally and by pointing and gave a demonstration of
illuminant and varying sizes of reflectance changes.
In each experimental session, there was just one color change
to the sky or sphere and just one test-surface color. In all, 20
conditions were tested in Experiment 1 and 15 in Experiment 2. A
further control condition was introduced in Experiment 2, in which
the scene was cropped to remove both the sphere and sky.
The images on the screen of the monitor were viewed binoc-
ularly at 100 cm and subtended approximately 18 deg 13 deg
visual angle. The test surfaces subtended approximately 1 deg
0.5 deg and 3 deg 1 deg for Experiments 1 and 2, respectively.
The reflecting sphere of Experiment 2 subtended approximately 1
deg. Images were presented in a dark surround, and the luminance
at each pixel varied from 0 to 33 cd m
2
. Room luminance was
approximately 0.5 cd m
2
. Observers each performed at least 1300
trials in all.
Observers
Twelve observers, aged 23–34 years, took part in the experiments:
two male and four female for Experiment 1 and the same for
Experiment 2. All observers had normal color vision verified with
the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-Hue test; Ishihara pseudoisochro-
matic plates ~24-plates edition, 1964!; Rayleigh and Moreland
anomaloscopy; and luminance matching ~Interzeag Color Vision
meter 712, Schlieren, Switzerland!. All had normal or corrected-
to-normal visual acuity. The experiments were conducted in ac-
Fig. 2. Contour plots showing the relative frequency of “illuminant-
change” responses by observers in the CIE 1976 ~u',v'!chromaticity
diagram as a function of the chromaticity of the reflectance change of the
test surface. The square symbols show the first illuminant, a daylight with
correlated color temperature, 25,000 K in ~a!and 15,000 K in ~b!; the
circles the second illuminant, 6700 K in ~a!and 5700 K in ~b!; and the
triangles the mode, from which the color-constancy index was derived.
The line marked Lis the daylight locus.
Color constancy and a visible illuminant 353
cordance with principles embodied in the Declaration of Helsinki
~Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association!and were
approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of
Manchester. All observers were unaware of the purpose of the
experiment.
Analysis
Only one out of the 65 changes in the spectral reflectance of the
test surface was a null change, that is, corresponded to a pure
illuminant change on the scene. An observer with perfect color
constancy would therefore give “illuminant-change” responses
only to this stimulus combination and “reflectance-change” re-
sponses to all the others. The frequency of “illuminant-change”
responses in each condition was therefore plotted as a function of
the chromaticity of the local illuminant in the CIE 1976 ~u
'
,v
'
!
chromaticity diagram. This frequency plot was then smoothed by
a two-dimensional nonparametric locally weighted quadratic re-
gression ~“loess”; Cleveland & Devlin, 1988!and contour plots
derived as shown in Fig. 2 ~cf. Bramwell & Hurlbert, 1996, who
used a two-dimensional Gaussian model!. Each contour represents
a constant relative frequency, with differences between contours of
approximately 0.10–0.15. The position of the maximum of each
distribution was obtained numerically from the loess analysis
~shown by the triangles in Fig. 2!. If the observer had perfect color
constancy, that position would coincide with the position of the
second illuminant ~circles!.
To summarize the error in the surface-color judgment, that is,
the bias, a standard color-constancy index ~Arend et al., 1991!was
then derived. Thus, if ais the distance between the positions of the
maximum ~triangle!and the 6700-K, or 5700-K illuminant ~circle!
~the bias in observers’ responses!and bthe distance between the
positions of the 25,000-K or 15,000-K illuminant ~square!and
6700-K or 5700-K, respectively, illuminant, then the constancy
index is 1 a0b. The standard error ~SE!of this index was
estimated with a bootstrap procedure, based on 1000 replications,
with resampling over observers ~Efron & Tibshirani, 1993!. Per-
fect constancy corresponds to an index of unity. Perfect incon-
stancy corresponds to an index of 0, which occurs when the
response peak coincides with the first global illuminant.
Results
Recall that these experiments were designed to test for a system-
atic effect on surface-color judgments of initial shifts in color of a
visible sky or of the color of illumination on a visible sphere. The
distributions of observers’ responses without these color shifts are
shown in Figs. 2a and b. Fig. 3 shows mean color constancy
indices for the six observers of Experiment 1 in which the color of
the sky was shifted in increments along the daylight locus. Data are
grouped according to the shift in sky color. Within groups, each
data point corresponds to a different initial color of the test surface.
Vertical bars show 61 SE of the mean. The control condition is
discussed later.
Color constancy indices ranged from 0.56 to 0.88, with an
overall mean of 0.71. Neither the initial shift in sky color nor the
initial test-surface color seemed to affect performance. This was
confirmed by a repeated-measures analysis of variance ~ANOVA!.
Thus, sky-color shift had no significant effect ~F~3,15!1.3, P
0.3!; nor did test-surface color ~F~4, 20!0.8, P0.5!; nor was
there an interaction between the two ~F~12,60!1.0, P0.4!.
Since data from the same scene location were also available
from a separate experiment in which the same kinds of surface-
color judgments were made without a visible illuminant ~Foster
et al., 2006a!, the mean level of color constancy is also included
in Fig. 3 for comparison ~labeled “control”!. Despite the absence
of the sky, and a different group of observers, performance was
similar. An ANOVA showed formally that the visible sky had no
significant effect ~F~4,135!1.1, P0.4!and neither did the
initial test-surface color ~F~4, 135!0.5, P0.8!.
Fig. 4 shows mean color constancy indices for the six observers
of Experiment 2, in which the color of the light illuminating the
sphere in the scene was shifted in increments along the daylight
locus. As in Fig. 3, data are grouped according to the shift of the
sphere illuminant color. Within groups, each data point corre-
Fig. 3. Color-constancy indices from Experiment 1 in which the color of
the sky in the scene ~Fig. 1a!was shifted in increments along the daylight
locus. Data are grouped according to the shift in initial sky color; within
groups, each data point corresponds to a different shift in initial color of
the test surface. Data are averaged over six observers. Vertical bars show
61 SE of the mean.
Fig. 4. Color-constancy indices from Experiment 2 in which the color of
the light from the sun and sky illuminating the sphere in the scene ~Fig. 1b!
was shifted in increments along the daylight locus. Other details as for
Fig. 3.
354 K. Amano, D.H. Foster, and S.M.C. Nascimento
sponds to a different initial color of the test surface. Vertical bars
show 61 SE of the mean.
Color constancy indices ranged from 0.60 to 0.81, with an
overall mean of 0.70. As with the main experiment, neither the
initial shift in illuminant color nor the initial test-surface color
seemed to affect performance. This was confirmed by a repeated-
measures ANOVA. Thus, sphere-illuminant color shift had no
significant effect ~F~2,10!0.6, P0.5!; nor did test-surface
color ~F~4,20!1.7, P0.2!; nor was there an interaction
between the two ~F~8,40!1.0, P0.4!.
Results for the control condition, in which the scene was
cropped to remove both the sphere and sky, were similar. A
repeated-measures ANOVA showed formally that the presence of
the sphere had no significant effect ~F~1,5!0.3, P0.5!and
neither did the initial test-surface color ~F~4, 20!1.0, P0.4!.
Discussion
If the gamut of surfaces in a scene is sufficiently large, then, in
theory, it is possible to make a reliable estimate of the color of the
illumination, for example, by assuming that it coincides with a
spatial average color of the scene ~Buchsbaum, 1980!, or with the
color of the highest-luminance surface ~Land & McCann, 1971!,
or by appealing to other statistical properties of the image ~Fin-
layson et al., 2001; Golz & MacLeod, 2002!. In experiments with
Mondrian-like patterns, observers are able to estimate the color of
an illuminant on a scene ~Linnell & Foster, 2002!, and, as ex-
pected, the accuracy of estimation improves as the number of
surfaces in the patterns increases. Space-average scene color may
also be used as the cue in some surface-color matching experi-
ments in which both the illuminant and test-surface position vary
~Amano & Foster, 2004!. Even so, there are clearly conditions
where space-average scene color gives an unreliable cue to the
illuminant, most notably where the color gamut is limited. Making
information about the illuminant on a scene explicit, rather than
inferential, ought therefore to influence judgments, particularly in
natural scenes.
As was shown here, however, shifting the color of a directly
visible sky along the daylight locus so that it differed from the true
illuminant spectrum on the scene had no reliable effect on the ac-
curacy of surface-color judgments, nor did removing the sky from
the field of view. Shifting the color of the light from the sun and sky
illuminating a clearly visible sphere in the scene so that it, too,
differed from the true illuminant spectrum on the scene also had no
reliable effect. It might be argued that in the images of Experiment
2~Fig. 1b!, the right-hand side of the house whose roof was the test
surface could also have been used to infer the color of the direct
illumination, but it was less bright than the sphere and had no dif-
fuse highlight that allowed the illuminant color to be inferred.
These shifts in color of the sky and the light on the sphere were
substantial, in the 1976 CIE ~u
'
,v
'
!color diagram of the same
order as the color difference between the two global illuminants.
Moreover, performance was no worse when both the sky and the
sphere were eliminated from the field of view. The mean levels of
color constancy of 0.71 and 0.70 found here were compatible with
those reported for a much larger population of rural and urban
scenes ~Foster et al., 2006a!.
The simplest conclusion is that judging surface color in natural
scenes does not depend on explicit knowledge of the color of the
illumination ~cf. Yang & Maloney, 2001!. This conclusion is
consistent with another experiment ~Amano et al., 2005!in which
observers made asymmetric color matches between pairs of simul-
taneously presented Mondrian-like patterns of colored papers un-
der different daylights. The patterns had either 49 surfaces or a
minimal 2 surfaces, too few for an accurate estimate of the
illuminant to be formed. Yet color-constancy indices were almost
identical ~0.73 and 0.72 for 1 deg paper squares and closely similar
to the mean values reported here!.
As has been argued elsewhere ~Foster, 2003!, a possible expla-
nation for the insensitivity of surface-color judgments to informa-
tion about the illuminant on a scene is observers’ use of “relational
color constancy” ~Foster & Nascimento, 1994!. This refers to the
constancy of perceived color relations between surfaces under
different illuminants, as distinct from color constancy, which refers
to the constancy of perceived colors of surfaces. Thus, when
discriminating between illuminant and material changes in scenes,
observers simply compare how the color of the test surface relates
to the color of one or more other surfaces in the scene or, indeed,
to the scene as a whole, first under the one illuminant and then
under the other.
There is a ready physiological substrate for these comparisons:
the ratios of cone-photoreceptor excitations generated in response
to light reflected from pairs of surfaces or groups of surfaces. Such
ratios, which can also be calculated across postreceptoral combi-
nations and spatial averages of cone signals, have the remarkable
property of being almost exactly invariant under changes in illu-
minant both with natural surfaces ~Nascimento et al., 2002!and
with colored papers ~Foster & Nascimento, 1994!.
More generally, when surface-color judgments were measured
with a much larger group of scenes ~Foster et al., 2006a!, the
variation in color-constancy indices across the scenes, from 0.69 to
0.97, was best explained by spatial ratios of cone excitations being
calculated globally for each scene, that is, over all possible pairs of
points, rather than just between the test surface and one or more
other surfaces in the scene.
In everyday viewing, we are able to make rapid and relatively
accurate judgments about the colors of things, an ability that seems
not to require attentional effort ~Foster et al., 2001b!. Yet the
illuminant is often not visible, and our percepts are much the same
as when the illuminant is visible, an intuition that seems confirmed
by the present experiments.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the EPSRC ~grant nos. GR0R39412001
and EP0B00025701!.
References
Amano, K. & Foster, D.H. ~2004!. Colour constancy under simultaneous
changes in surface position and illuminant. Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London Series B 271, 2319–2326.
Amano, K., Foster, D.H. & Nascimento, S.M. ~2005!. Minimalist surface-
colour matching. Perception 34, 1009–1013.
Arend, L. & Reeves, A. ~1986!. Simultaneous color constancy. Journal of
the Optical Society of America A. Optics, Image Science, and Vision
3, 1743–1751.
Arend, L.E., Jr., Reeves, A., Schirillo, J. & Goldstein, R. ~1991!.
Simultaneous color constancy: Papers with diverse Munsell values.
Journal of the Optical Society of America A. Optics, Image Science,
and Vision 8, 661–672.
Bramwell, D.I. & Hurlbert, A.C. ~1996!. Measurements of colour
constancy by using a forced-choice matching technique. Perception 25,
229–241.
Buchsbaum, G. ~1980!. A spatial processor model for object colour
perception. Journal of the Franklin Institute 310,126.
Color constancy and a visible illuminant 355
Cleveland, W.S. & Devlin, S.J. ~1988!. Locally weighted regression: An
approach to regression analysis by local fitting. Journal of the Ameri-
can Statistical Association 83, 596– 610.
Craven, B.J. & Foster, D.H. ~1992!. An operational approach to colour
constancy. Vision Research 32, 1359–1366.
de Almeida, V.M.N., Fiadeiro, P.T. & Nascimento, S.M.C. ~2004!.
Color constancy by asymmetric color matching with real objects in
three-dimensional scenes. Visual Neuroscience 21, 341–345.
D’Zmura, M. & Iverson, G. ~1993a!. Color constancy. I. Basic theory of
two-stage linear recovery of spectral descriptions for lights and sur-
faces. Journal of the Optical Society of America A. Optics, Image
Science, and Vision 10, 2148–2165.
D’Zmura, M. & Iverson, G. ~1993b!. Color constancy. II. Results for
two-stage linear recovery of spectral descriptions for lights and sur-
faces. Journal of the Optical Society of America A. Optics, Image
Science, and Vision 10, 2166–2180.
D’Zmura, M. & Iverson, G. ~1994!. Color constancy. III. General linear
recovery of spectral descriptions for lights and surfaces. Journal of the
Optical Society of America A. Optics, Image Science, and Vision 11,
2389–2400.
Efron, B. & Tibshirani, R.J. ~1993!.An Introduction to the Bootstrap.
New York: Chapman and Hall.
Finlayson, G.D., Hordley, S.D. & Hubel, P.M. ~2001!. Color by corre-
lation: A simple, unifying framework for color constancy. IEEE Trans-
actions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence 23, 1209–1221.
Foster, D.H. ~2003!. Does colour constancy exist? Trends in Cognitive
Sciences 7, 439– 443.
Foster, D.H., Amano, K. & Nascimento, S.M. ~2006a!. Color constancy
in natural scenes explained by global image statistics. Visual Neuro-
science,23, 341–349.
Foster, D.H., Amano, K. & Nascimento, S.M.C. ~2001a!. Colour con-
stancy from temporal cues: Better matches with less variability under
fast illuminant changes. Vision Research 41, 285–293.
Foster, D.H., Amano, K., Nascimento, S.M.C. & Foster, M.J. ~2006b!.
Frequency of metamerism in natural scenes. Journal of the Optical
Society of America A. Optics, Image Science, and Vision ~in press!.
Foster, D.H. & Nascimento, S.M.C. ~1994!. Relational colour constancy
from invariant cone-excitation ratios. Proceedings of the Royal Society
of London Series B 257, 115–121.
Foster, D.H., Nascimento, S.M.C. & Amano, K. ~2004!. Information
limits on neural identification of colored surfaces in natural scenes.
Visual Neuroscience 21, 331–336.
Foster, D.H., Nascimento, S.M.C., Amano, K., Arend, L., Linnell,
K.J., Nieves, J.L., Plet, S. & Foster, J.S. ~2001b!. Parallel detection
of violations of color constancy. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the U.S.A. 98, 8151–8156.
Golz, J. & MacLeod, D.I.A. ~2002!. Influence of scene statistics on
colour constancy. Nature 415, 637–640.
Judd, D.B., MacAdam, D.L. & Wyszecki, G. ~1964!. Spectral distribu-
tion of typical daylight as a function of correlated color temperature.
Journal of the Optical Society of America 54, 1031–1040.
Kraft, J.M. & Brainard, D.H. ~1999!. Mechanisms of color constancy
under nearly natural viewing. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the U.S.A. 96, 307–312.
Land, E.H. & McCann, J.J. ~1971!. Lightness and retinex theory. Journal
of the Optical Society of America 61, 1–11.
Linnell, K.J. & Foster, D.H. ~1997!. Space-average scene colour used to
extract illuminant information. In John Dalton’s Colour Vision Legacy,
eds. Dickinson, C., Murray, I. & Carden, D., pp. 501–509. London:
Taylor and Francis.
Linnell, K.J. & Foster, D.H. ~2002!. Scene articulation: Dependence
of illuminant estimates on number of surfaces. Perception 31,
151–159.
Maloney, L.T. ~1999!. Physics-based approaches to modeling surface
color perception. In Color Vision: From Genes to Perception, eds.
Gegenfurtner, K.R. & Sharpe, L.T., pp. 387–416. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Nascimento, S.M.C., Ferreira, F.P. & Foster, D.H. ~2002!. Statistics of
spatial cone-excitation ratios in natural scenes. Journal of the Optical
Society of America A 19, 1484–1490.
Smithson, H.E. ~2005!. Sensory, computational and cognitive components
of human colour constancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society B 360, 1329–1346.
von Helmholtz, H. ~1867!.Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik, Vol. 2.
Second edition. Leipzig: Leopold Voss. Translated as Helmholtz’s
Treatise on Physiological Optics, ed. Southall, J.P.C., pp. 286–287.
Third edition. Washington, DC: Optical Society of America, 1924.
Reprint, New York: Dover Publications, 1962.
Yang, J.N. & Maloney, L.T. ~2001!. Illuminant cues in surface color
perception: Tests of three candidate cues. Vision Research 41,2581
2600.
356 K. Amano, D.H. Foster, and S.M.C. Nascimento
... However, this by itself does not justify the claim that the psychological capacity itself is representational and 'possesses' that content in any strong metaphysical sense. Indeed, different, non-representational theories of color constancy have been proposed and seem to be equally empirically successful: for instance, the theory called Relational color constancy (Amano et al., 2006;Foster, 2003;Foster & Nascimento, 1994), according to which the visual system, instead of recovering 'objective' surface colors, merely tracks the invariance in color contrast relations among adjacent surfaces under the same lighting conditions. On this relational view, the representation of the 'objective' surface color of a specific surface is taken to be either produced at a later stage of processing or not produced by any sub-personal system at all, resulting instead from explicit reasoning (i.e. a judgment). ...
Article
Full-text available
Can philosophical theories of perception defer to perceptual science when fixing their ontological commitments regarding the objects of perception? Or in other words, can perceptual science inform us about the nature of perception? Many contemporary mainstream philosophers of perception answer affirmatively. However, in this essay I provide two arguments against this idea. On the one hand, I will argue that perceptual science is not committed to certain assumptions, relevant for determining perceptual ontology, which however are generally relied upon by philosophers when interpreting such science. On the other hand, I will show how perceptual science often relies on another assumption, which I call the ‘Measuring instrument conception’ of sensory systems, which philosophers of perception should clearly reject. Given these two symmetric lines of argument, I will finally suggest that we ought to think differently about the relationship between perceptual science and the philosophy of perception.
... On the other hand, the relational color constancy hypothesis, consistent with the Structural psychological thesis, characterizes the mechanism in question as a mechanism that tracks how the (apparent) chromaticity of the target surface relates to that of adjacent surfaces as the illumination shed on the whole scene changes. (Foster & Nascimento, 1994;Foster, 2003;Amano et al. 2005Amano et al. , 2006Zaidi 1998Zaidi , 2002. If the changes are such that relative chromatic differences among adjacent surfaces remain unchanged, the visual system will both register the change in apparent colors and the fact that nothing in the physical makeup of the surfaces has changed, thus influencing both conscious color experience and the subsequent judgment/report (Arend & Reeves, 1986;Davies, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
It is a familiar experience to perceive a material object as maintaining a stable shape even though it projects differently shaped images on our retina as we move with respect to it, or as maintaining a stable color throughout changes in the way the object is illuminated. We also perceive sounds as maintaining constant timbre and loudness when the context and the spatial relations between us and the sound source change over time. But where does this perceptual invariance ‘come from’? What is it about our perceptual systems that makes them able to ‘transform’ incoming unstable and fluctuating sensory inputs into generally stable and coherent conscious experiences? And what exactly do we experience as invariant in cases like those described above? There are two main approaches to the Problem of Perceptual Invariance: the Local-Inferential approach and the Global-Structural approach. Although both approaches include an account of the sub-personal perceptual mechanisms ‘stabilizing’ variant and invariant components in incoming sensory stimulation and a proposal regarding the phenomenology of perceptual invariance, in this paper I argue that the latter provides a better solution to the problem overall.
... The system is focused on the distribution of colors that are present in an image as the major assumption. In the next section, a framework is discussed generating different color constancy methods [2], where each method is based on a specific assumption about the presence of colors and color edges in images. ...
... By far the best performance was achieved with a large background and long adaptation period [13,19]. This suggests that computations performed by higher cortical areas concerned with lightness and color constancy are not crucially dependent on any explicit evaluation of illuminants as suggested in a variety of experiments [10,25]. Indeed, as shown by Barbur and Spang [26], normal levels of rapid or instantaneous color constancy are obtained when the subject is unaware of any changes in the illuminant. ...
Article
Full-text available
The link between chromatic constancy (compensation for hue and saturation shifts) and lightness constancy (compensation for a change in surface reflectance) was tested theoretically by computing cone contrasts and by asymmetric matching experiments. The effect of a thin achromatic line (a frame) around the test sample was tested empirically. When the samples were outlined by the frame, lightness constancy was increased and chromatic constancy reduced ( p < 0.001 ). Changes in luminance are more likely to be compensated when the luminance contrast edge around the test stimulus is disturbed as with the addition of an achromatic frame.
... Although we have cast our interpretation of average constancy and variability in terms of illuminant estimation, we note that the available evidence suggests that observers do not explicitly represent the illuminant (Rutherford and Brainard, 2002;Amano et al., 2006;Granzier et al., 2009). Despite this, the language of illuminant estimation implicit in discussions of perceptual segregation may be functionally useful. ...
Article
Full-text available
For the surface reflectance of an object to be a useful cue to object identity, judgments of its color should remain stable across changes in the object's environment. In 2D scenes, there is general consensus that color judgments are much more stable across illumination changes than background changes. Here we investigate whether these findings generalize to real 3D objects. Observers made color matches to cubes as we independently varied both the illumination impinging on the cube and the 3D background of the cube. As in 2D scenes, we found relatively high but imperfect stability of color judgments under an illuminant shift. In contrast to 2D scenes, we found that background had little effect on average color judgments. In addition, variability of color judgments was increased by an illuminant shift and decreased by embedding the cube within a background. Taken together, these results suggest that in real 3D scenes with ample cues to object segregation, the addition of a background may improve stability of color identification.
... Despite diurnal and seasonal variations in the composition and the intensity of the light that illuminates surfaces, animals and humans do not perceive much variation in the lightness and the colors of objects. The visual system has evolved so as to make this correction independent of explicit information about the spectrum of the light source (Amano et al., 2006). These perceptual constancies are no doubt valuable adaptations, without which we could not reliably distinguish degrees of fruit ripeness under different kinds of illumination. ...
Article
Full-text available
At VSS 2005 we reported that percussionists use visual gestures to alter audience perception of musical note length (Schutz & Kubovy). This conflicts with previous research suggesting vision does not alter auditory judgments of tone duration (Walker & Scott, 1981). In order to determine why our results differ from prior research, we built a point light simulation of a percussionist's movements tracking the long and short gestures used to strike percussion instruments. After pilot studies demonstrated the simulation to be successful, a series of four experiments revealed: • When using alternate sounds, the visual influence: • Persists with percussive sounds (piano). • Does not persist with non-percussive sounds (clarinet, trumpet, voice). • Visual influence is lost when the moving dot pauses at the moment of impact before rebounding. • Influence is lost when the motion path is rotated 180 degrees after impact to simulate moving “through the bar” rather than rebounding off of it. • A static dot presented simultaneously with the percussive sound produces effects similar to those observed with gestures. We conclude our initial results were driven by the perceived causal relationship between modalities. Only sounds caused by impacts (e.g. percussive sounds) are influenced by visual impact gestures (Experiment 1). Manipulations which obscure the perception of a visual impact have no effect on auditory judgments (Experiments 2). The most important aspect of the gesture is the sudden change of direction at the moment of impact (Experiment 3). However, the motion itself is important only in that it depicts a change. A spatially static visual change (e.g. the appearance of a dot on a previously blank screen) alters perception as reliably as the original gestures (Experiment 4). Together these studies show the importance of the relationship between the auditory and visual information in understanding the nature of sensory integration.
... Despite diurnal and seasonal variations in the composition and the intensity of the light that illuminates surfaces animals and humans do not perceive much variation in the lightness and the colors of objects. The visual system has evolved so as to make this correction independent of explicit information about the spectrum of the light source (Amano et al., 2006). These perceptual constancies are no doubt valuable adaptations, without which we could not reliably distinguish degrees of fruit ripeness under different kinds of illumination. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this paper we offer a theory of cross-modal objects. To begin, we discuss two kinds of linkages between vision and audition. The first is a duality. The the visual system detects and identifies emphsurfaces; the auditory system detects and identifies emphsources. Surfaces are illuminated by sources of light; sound is reflected off surfaces. However, the visual system discounts sources and the auditory system discounts surfaces. These and similar considerations lead to the Theory of Indispensable Attributes that states the conditions for the formation of gestalts in the two modalities. The second linkage involves the formation of audiovisual objects, integrated cross-modal experiences. We describe research that reveals the role of cross-modal causality in the formation of such objects. These experiments use the canonical example of a causal link between vision and audition: a visible impact that causes a percussive sound.
Article
The light‐emitting diode (LED)‐based light sources have been widely applied across numerous industries and in everyday practical uses. Recently, the LED‐based light source consisting of red, green and blue LEDs with narrow spectral bands (RGB‐LED) has been a more preferred illumination source than the common white phosphor LED and other traditional broadband light sources because the RGB‐LED can create many types of illumination color. The color rendering index of the RGB‐LED, however, is considerably lower compared to the traditional broadband light sources and the multi‐band LED light source (MB‐LED), which is composed of several LEDs and can accurately simulate daylight illuminants. Considering 3 relatively narrow spectral bands of the RGB‐LED light source, the color constancy, which is referred to as the ability of the human visual system to attenuate influences of illumination color change and hold the perception of a surface color constant, may be worse under the RGB‐LED light source than under the traditional broadband light sources or under the MB‐LED. In this study, we investigated categorical color constancy using a color naming method with real Munsell color chips under illumination changes from neutral to red, green, blue, and yellow illuminations. The neutral and 4 chromatic illuminants were produced by the RGB‐LED light source. A modified use of the color constancy index, which describes a centroid shift of each color category, was introduced to evaluate the color constancy performance. The results revealed that categorical color constancy under the 4 chromatic illuminants held relatively well, except for the red, brown, orange, and yellow color categories under the blue illumination and the orange color category under the yellow illumination. Furthermore, the categorical color constancy under red and green illuminations was better than the categorical color constancy under blue and yellow illuminations. The results indicate that a color constancy mechanism in the visual system functions in color categories when the illuminant emits an insufficient spectrum to render the colors of reflecting surfaces accurately. However, it is not recommended to use the RGB‐LED light source to produce blue and yellow illuminations because of the poor color constancy.
Article
Full-text available
Misidentifying materials-such as mistaking soap for pâté, or vice versa-could lead to some pretty messy mishaps. Fortunately, we rarely suffer such indignities, thanks largely to our outstanding ability to recognize materials-and identify their properties-by sight. In everyday life, we encounter an enormous variety of materials, which we usually distinguish effortlessly and without error. However, despite its subjective ease, material perception poses the visual system with some unique and significant challenges, because a given material can take on many different appearances depending on the lighting, viewpoint and shape. Here, I use observations from recent research on material perception to outline a general theory of material perception, in which I suggest that the visual system does not actually estimate physical parameters of materials and objects. Instead-I argue-the brain is remarkably adept at building 'statistical generative models' that capture the natural degrees of variation in appearance between samples. For example, when determining perceived glossiness, the brain does not estimate parameters of the BRDF. Instead, it uses a constellation of low- and mid-level image measurements to characterize the extent to which the surface manifests specular reflections. I argue that these 'statistical appearance models' are both more expressive and easier to compute than physical parameters, and therefore represent a powerful middle way between a 'bag of tricks' and 'inverse optics'.
Article
In this paper, I defend a representationalist account of the phenomenal character of color experiences. Representationalism, the thesis that phenomenal character supervenes on a certain kind of representational content, so-called phenomenal content, has been developed primarily in two different ways, as Russellian and Fregean representationalism. While the proponents of Russellian and Fregean representationalism differ with respect to what they take the contents of color experiences to be, they typically agree that colors are exhaustively characterized by the three dimensions of the color solid: hue, saturation, and lightness. I argue that a viable version of representationalism needs to renounce this restriction to three dimensions and consider illumination to be a genuine phenomenal dimension of color. My argument for this thesis falls into two parts. I first consider the phenomenon of color constancy in order to show that neither Russellian nor Fregean representationalism can do justice to the phenomenal significance of local illumination. I subsequently formulate a version of representationalism that accounts for illumination by taking it as a phenomenal dimension of color.
Article
Full-text available
Arend and Reeves [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 3, 1743 (1986)] described measurements of color constancy in computer simulations of arrays of colored papers of equal Munsell value under 4000-, 6500-, and 10,000-K daylight illuminants. We report an extension of those experiments to chromatic arrays spanning a wide range of Munsell values. The computer-simulated scene included a standard array of Munsell papers under 6500-K illumination and a test array, an identical array of the same papers under 4000 or 10,000 K. Observers adjusted a patch in the test array in order to match the corresponding patch in the standard array by one of two criteria. They either matched hue and saturation or they made surface-color matches, in which the test patch was made to “look as if it were cut from the same piece of paper as the standard patch.” The test and the standard patches were surrounded by a single color (annulus display) or by many colors (Mondrian display). The data agreed with those of our previous equal-value experiment. The paper matches were often approximately color constant. The hue-saturation matches were in the correct direction for constancy but were always closer to. a chromaticity match (no constancy) than to the chromaticity required for hue-saturation constancy.
Conference Paper
If surfaces in a scene are to be distinguished by their color, their neural representation at some level should ideally vary little with the color of the illumination. Four possible neural codes were considered: von-Kries-scaled cone responses from single points in a scene, spatial ratios of cone responses produced by light reflected from pairs of points. and these quantities obtained with sharpened (opponent-cone) responses. The effectiveness of these codes in identifying surfaces was quantified by information-theoretic measures. Data were drawn from a sample of 25 rural and urban scenes imaged with a hyperspectral camera, which provided estimates of Surface reflectance at 10-nm intervals at each of 1344 X 1024 pixels for each scene. In computer simulations, scenes were illuminated separately by daylights of correlated color temperatures 4000 K, 6500 K, and 25,000 K. Points were sampled randomly in each scene and identified according to each of the codes. It was found that the maximum information preserved under illuminant changes varied with the code, but for a particular code it was remarkably stable across the different scenes. The standard deviation over the 25 scenes was, on average, approximately 1 bit, suggesting that the neural coding of surface color can be optimized independent of location for any particular range of illuminants.
Article
Spectral distributions of 622 samples of daylight (skylight, and sunlight plus skylight) have been subjected to characteristic vector analysis, as composite data and in three subgroups (99 distributions measured by Budde; 249, by Condit; and 274, by Henderson and Hodgkiss). The chromaticity coordinates (x,y) computed from these distributions have been compared with direct visual determinations of chromaticity coordinates of daylight by Nayatani and Wyszecki, and by Chamberlin, Lawrence, and Belbin. It was found that the chromaticities indicated by the spectral distributions and by direct visual colorimetry cluster about the curve: y = 2.870x-3.000x2-0.275. This curve of typical daylight chromaticities falls slightly on the green side of the Planckian locus. From the mean and the first two characteristic vectors of the composite data, spectral distribution curves have been reconstituted by choice of scalar multiples of the vectors such that the chromaticity points fall on the curve of typical daylight chromaticities at places corresponding to correlated color temperatures of 4800°, 5500°, 6500°, 7500°, and 10000° K. The representative character of these reconstituted spectral-distribution curves has been established by comparison with the measured curves from each subgroup yielding the closest approximation to the same chromaticities. The agreement so found suggests that this family of curves is more representative of the various phases of daylight between correlated color temperatures 4800° and 10000°K than any previously derived distributions.
Article
Locally weighted regression, or loess, is a way of estimating a regression surface through a multivariate smoothing procedure, fitting a function of the independent variables locally and in a moving fashion analogous to how a moving average is computed for a time series. With local fitting we can estimate a much wider class of regression surfaces than with the usual classes of parametric functions, such as polynomials. The goal of this article is to show, through applications, how loess can be used for three purposes: data exploration, diagnostic checking of parametric models, and providing a nonparametric regression surface. Along the way, the following methodology is introduced: (a) a multivariate smoothing procedure that is an extension of univariate locally weighted regression; (b) statistical procedures that are analogous to those used in the least-squares fitting of parametric functions; (c) several graphical methods that are useful tools for understanding loess estimates and checking the assumptions on which the estimation procedure is based; and (d) the M plot, an adaptation of Mallow's Csubp/sub procedure, which provides a graphical portrayal of the trade-off between variance and bias, and which can be used to choose the amount of smoothing.