ArticlePDF Available

Ant Pupae Employ Acoustics to Communicate Social Status in Their Colony’s Hierarchy

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The possession of an efficient communication system and an ability to distinguish between young stages are essential attributes that enable eusocial insects to live in complex integrated societies. Although ants communicate primarily via chemicals, it is increasingly clear that acoustical signals also convey important information, including status, between adults in many species. However, all immature stages were believed to be mute. We confirm that larvae and recently formed pupae of Myrmica ants are mute, yet once they are sclerotized, the pupae possess a fully functioning stridulatory organ. The sounds generated by worker pupae were similar to those of workers but were emitted as single pulses rather than in the long sequences characteristic of adults; both induced the same range and intensity of benevolent behaviors when played back to unstressed workers. Both white and sclerotized pupae have a higher social status than larvae within Myrmica colonies, but the latter’s status fell significantly after they were made mute. Our results suggest that acoustical signals supplant semiochemicals as a means of identification in sclerotized pupae, perhaps because their hardened integuments block the secretion of brood pheromones or because their developing adult secretions initially differ from overall colony odors
Content may be subject to copyright.
Current Biology 23, 323–327, February 18, 2013 ª2013 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.010
Report
Ant Pupae Employ Acoustics
to Communicate Social Status
in Their Colony’s Hierarchy
Luca P. Casacci,
1
Jeremy A. Thomas,
2
Marco Sala,
1
David Treanor,
2
Simona Bonelli,
1
Emilio Balletto,
1
and Karsten Scho
¨nrogge
3,
*
1
Department of Life Sciences and Systems Biology,
University of Turin, Via Accademia Albertina, 10123 Turin, Italy
2
Department of Zoology, University of Oxford,
South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PS, UK
3
CEH Wallingford, NERC Centre for Ecology & Hydrology,
Maclean Building, Benson Lane, Wallingford OX10 8BB, UK
Summary
The possession of an efficient communication system and
an ability to distinguish between young stages are essential
attributes that enable eusocial insects to live in complex
integrated societies [1–4]. Although ants communicate
primarily via chemicals, it is increasingly clear that acous-
tical signals also convey important information, including
status, between adults in many species [5–9]. However, all
immature stages were believed to be mute [7]. We confirm
that larvae and recently formed pupae of Myrmica ants are
mute, yet once they are sclerotized, the pupae possess a fully
functioning stridulatory organ. The sounds generated by
worker pupae were similar to those of workers but were
emitted as single pulses rather than in the long sequences
characteristic of adults; both induced the same range and
intensity of benevolent behaviors when played back to
unstressed workers. Both white and sclerotized pupae
have a higher social status than larvae within Myrmica colo-
nies, but the latter’s status fell significantly after they were
made mute. Our results suggest that acoustical signals
supplant semiochemicals as a means of identification in
sclerotized pupae, perhaps because their hardened integu-
ments block the secretion of brood pheromones or because
their developing adult secretions initially differ from overall
colony odors [5, 10].
Results and Discussion
Pupal Sound Production
The main means of recognition and communication that permit
up to a million individuals in an ant society to function as
a single ‘‘superorganism’’ is by chemical cues, often modu-
lated by tactile stimuli [1, 2, 4]. Members of the same society
typically share a cocktail of hydrocarbons that provides a
distinctive ‘‘gestalt’’ odor across the colony, allowing workers
to discriminate between kin and strangers [5, 6]. Additional
variation between individuals’ profiles permits recognition
of—and appropriate responses to—nestmates of different
sex, caste, and developmental stage [10–14]. For example,
when a colony is perturbed, the workers quickly rescue and
retrieve the brood, including dummies treated with larval ex-
tracts [15, 16]. In the well-studied Myrmicine genus Myrmica,
brood recognition by pheromones is supplemented by tactile
cues, including larval turgidity, hairiness, size, shape, and
surface properties [4]; a social hierarchy exists between the
different young stages: small larvae are killed and fed to
larger larvae in times of food shortage, and a distinct order
of rescue occurs—starting with pupae, followed by large
larvae and finally by small larvae and eggs—whenever a colony
is disturbed [17, 18].
The role of acoustic signaling has recently received in-
creased interest [7–10], but, to our knowledge, there was no
evidence that the young stages of any ant could communicate
using sound. On the contrary, previous studies indicated that
the immature stages of Myrmica were mute [7, 19], although
older, sclerotized pupae may not have been investigated.
Scanning electron microscopy revealed, however, the pres-
ence of a fully formed stridulatory organ on the developing
imago within sclerotized ant pupae, similar to that on adult
workers and queens (Figure 1). The organ consists of a
minutely ridged file (Figures 1C and 1D; pars stridens), located
on the middorsal edge of the fourth abdominal segment, and
of a spike (plectrum) projecting from the rear edge of the
postpetiole. However, compared with adults, the scope for
the pupa to play one surface rapidly against the other was
constrained due to the thin pupal cuticle that encompassed
it (Figures 1Aand 1B). Emerging stridulatory organs were
also recognizable on the soft abdomens of newly formed white
pupae but were absent from larvae.
We recorded larvae and white pupae for a total of 40 hr, but
no sounds or substrate-borne vibrations were detected. In
contrast, sclerotized (nascent worker) pupae readily produced
acoustic signals which resembled those of adult workers and,
to a lesser extent, queens in their frequency and intensity, but
which consisted of single pulses rather than the streams of
‘‘song’’ emanating from both adult castes (Figure 2A). Using
a multivariate approach over three sound parameters, the
normalized Euclidean distances (mean 6SD) within samples
of M. scabrinodis pupae, queens, and workers were respec-
tively 0.88 60.32, 0.52 60.30, and 1.00 60.59 (Figure 2A).
Principal component analysis (PCA) was also conducted on
the three sound parameters recorded from groups of 6
M. scabrinodis sclerotized pupae, 1 individual worker, and 1
queen from each of 10 M. scabrinodis nests: the first and
the second principal components accounted for 79.1% and
20.9% respectively, i.e., explaining all the variance (Figure 2B).
Nested analysis of similarity (ANOSIM) of the Euclidean
distance matrix showed a clear separation between the
signals of sclerotized pupae, workers, and queens (overall:
R= 0.778, p < 0.001; for component distances: sclerotized
pupae: distance
workers
= 2.52 61.00, ANOSIM R= 0.941, p =
0.001; distance
queens
= 3.16 60.96, ANOSIM R=1,p=
0.001). As expected, the signals emitted by sclerotized
(nascent worker) pupae were significantly closer to the stridu-
lations of workers than to those of queens (two-sample t test:
t = 10.198, df = 198, p < 0.001).
The adults of many ant species stridulate to nestmates
[20, 21], although acoustical communication by their immature
stages has not been previously described. Because the
active organ formed part of a nascent adult developing inside
the sclerotized M. scabrinodis pupa, we might expect to find
*Correspondence: ksc@ceh.ac.uk
similar acoustical communications, after the first few days
of pupal lives, among the four subfamilies of ants which
also possess a stridulatory organ, namely the Ponerinae,
Nothomyrmecinae, Pseudomyrmecinae, Myrmicinae.
Worker Ant Responses to Pupal Sounds
The responses of otherwise undisturbed M. scabrinodis
workers to recordings of the sounds emitted by their sclero-
tized pupae were compared with playbacks of their own
(worker) recordings and of white noise in three randomly as-
signed containers, simultaneously replicated twenty times.
No antagonistic or alarmed ant behavior occurred during
playback experiments, but five benevolent responses were
observed, the first two involving attraction and the rest involv-
ing reactions: (1) walking—the worker was attracted to the
speaker but walked over it without stopping on it; (2) alerting—
the worker abruptly changed direction to pass onto the
speaker; (3) antennating—the worker antennated the speaker
for at least 3 s; (4) guarding—the workers rested in an alert
on-guard poise (sensu; [7]) on the speaker for at least 5 s; (5)
digging—the worker dug into the soil surrounding the speaker.
Linear mixed-effect models showed that worker reactions to
the three sound stimuli were significantly different for all
observed behaviors except digging, which, however, was
never elicited by white noise (Figure 3). Thus, compared with
white noise, both pupal and worker sounds always induced
significantly more instances of walking, alerting, antennating,
and guarding by Myrmica worker ants, with values of p ranging
from 0.019 to <0.0001. Yet despite the fact that pupal calls
consisted of single pulses, whereas worker stridulations
were broadcast in streams, no significant difference was found
in worker responses to these two sound stimuli (Figure 3).
The results are consistent with other observations within the
genus Myrmica that stridulations are caste specific rather than
species specific [8], and, unsurprisingly, the structure of the
stridulation organ we found in M. scabrinodis worker pupae
was identical to that of eclosed adult workers (Figures 1 and
2). Similarly, we predict that the stridulatory organ of a gyne
pupa will produce sounds similar to an adult queen and will
induce similar royal treatment from nurse workers [7]. The
constraint of an enveloping integument may explain why the
pupal sounds occurred in single pulses rather than the
complex repetitions that characterize an adult ant’s diagnostic
patterns. The fact that both types of adult and pupal stridula-
tions triggered the same intensity and range of benevolent
responses suggests that the frequency at which pulses of
sounds occur is not important for conveying information. It is
worth noting, however, that our test environment was simple
and constant, and that in nature adult ants are capable of
both producing different sounds [22] and reacting in different
ways to the same acoustics [23], depending on the context
in which the signal is transmitted or received. Furthermore,
our acoustics were tested in isolation, whereas in nature they
may be modulated by chemical or tactile cues, and vice versa
[1, 2]. Thus, we suspect that tended pupae in natural colonies
may possess a wider acoustical repertoire than observed here
and that worker responses to them may be more complex.
Social Status of Normal and Mute Myrmica Pupae
As has been reported for other Myrmica species [17, 18], we
found that M. scabrinodis workers rescued living pupae, as
Figure 1. The Stridulatory Organ of Sclerotized Pupae of Myrmica
scabrinodis
(A and B) Location of the acoustical organ (arrow) beneath the integument of
an intact pupa.
(C) Pupa with integument removed.
(D) Pars stridens on pupa with integument removed.
Figure 2. Comparison of the Acoustics of Queen,
Worker, and Sclerotized Pupae of Myrmica
scabrinodis
(A) Oscillogram, spectrogram, and single pulse
parameters.
(B) Combined effect of the three sound parame-
ters (pulse length, frequency, and intensity)
shown as the first and second component plot
of a principal components analysis over all indi-
vidual pulse measurements.
Current Biology Vol 23 No 4
324
a class (i.e., brown + white), significantly more quickly than
their larvae (Wilcoxon Mann-Whitney, Z= 6.822, p = 0.009)
after their nest was disturbed (Figure 4). However, within
these assays, using normal (i.e., nonmuted) brood items, the
white pupae were rescued ahead of both sclerotized pupae
(Z= 2.118, p = 0.026) and larvae (Z=23.177, p < 0.001), with
no significant difference being found between sclerotized
pupae and larvae (Z=21.399, p = 0.168), although the latter
were, on average, rescued last (Figure 4).
The pattern of rescue changed with recently killed brood:
i.e., brood still coated with its full cocktail of recognition
pheromones [24, 25] but which was mute and immobilized
(Figure 4). The mute sclerotized pupae were the last to be
rescued, significantly behind white pupae (Z= 3.326, p <
0.001) and larvae (Z= 2.306, p = 0.021). White pupae were on
average rescued first, but not significantly ahead of larvae
(Z= 1.5875, p = 0.107). Wilcoxon signed rank tests were also
used to directly compare the shift in order in each brood
type between the normal and mute trials: sclerotized pupae
shifted to being rescued significantly after the other brood in
the mute trials (Z=224.500, df = 10, p = 0.0098), but there
was no significant shift in the order of recovery of white pupae
or larvae between the two experiments (Z=24.500, df = 10,
p = 0.6719 and Z=215.500, df = 10, p = 0.1309, respectively).
It was impractical to record the acoustics of Myrmica pupae
during the rescue experiment, but the shift in rank for the
brown pupae that could and could not stridulate indicates
that this is linked to the stridulations. Of course, dead brood
cannot move, either; e.g., larvae cannot beg, but the lack of
any significant difference in the relative order of rescue of
white pupae and larvae during the mute assays compared
with the living trials supports previous conclusions [24, 25]
that the chemical and tactile signals involved in brood recog-
nition are not compromised by this treatment.
The preference afforded to living white pupae after colony
perturbation was unexpected. We predicted that the calls of
sclerotized pupae would attract preferential worker attention,
perhaps explaining why pupae as a group were selected
ahead of larvae or eggs in previous ant rescue experiments
[17, 18]. A possible explanation is that, rather than elevating
the social level of sclerotized pupae through the possession
of an additional cue, their acoustics may replace brood-recog-
nition pheromones, perhaps because the hardened integu-
ment blocks the secretions from their own glands or reduces
their ability to absorb colony odors. An alternative explanation
is that hydrocarbons secreted by the developing imago within
a sclerotized pupa not only replace or overscore the phero-
mones of brood with soft cuticles, but—like the secretions of
the callow adults that they will shortly become—differ some-
what from the overall gestalt odor of their colony, making
them less recognizable as nestmates using chemical cues
alone.
General Conclusions
Our results support a growing body of work—facilitated by the
increased sophistication of affordable sound equipment—that
suggests that acoustical communication plays a greater and
more varied role in influencing ant social behavior than was
previously thought (e.g., see [7, 8, 20–22, 26]).
The recognition, not only of brood in ant societies but also of
different types of brood, including nestmate and nonnestmate
brood, has received much recent attention. Although it seems
clear that chemical, tactile, behavioral, and now acoustic cues
can be important in brood recognition [4, 10, 25], the precise
role of each cue is still poorly understood. For instance, on
current evidence we have suggested that acoustical signals
are caste specific but not species (let alone kin) specific.
On the other hand, the cuticular hydrocarbon signatures
described on brood are often impoverished and dominated
by saturated alkanes that are not thought to convey informa-
tion [27, 28]. If this were the case, brood would be chemically
transparent [10, 29] and distinctive to workers only if other
Figure 3. Responses of Myrmica scabrinodis Workers to Broadcasts of
Worker and Pupal Acoustics and White Noise
Five benevolent but no antagonistic behaviors were observed: the same
letter indicates no significant difference within each type of behavior;
different letters indicate a significantly different response. Compared with
white noise, linear mixed-effect model likelihood ratios are (1) walking
LR
pupa
= 11.082, df = 4, p = 0.001; LR
worker
= 8.097, df = 4, p = 0.004; (2)
alerting LR
pupa
= 23.232, df = 4, p < 0.0001; LR
worker
= 20.518, df = 4,
p < 0.0001; (3) antennating LR
pupa
= 8.425, df = 4, p = 0.004; LR
worker
=
17.154, df = 4, p < 0.0001; and (4) guarding LR
pupa
= 5.476, df = 4, p =
0.019; LR
worker
= 11.419, df = 4, p = 0.001. Likelihood ratios comparing pupal
and worker acoustics are (5) walking LR = 0.296, df = 4, p = 0.587; (6) alerting
LR = 0.145, df = 4, p = 0.704; (7) antennating LR = 2.278, df = 4, p = 0.131; and
(8) guarding LR = 1.441, df = 4, p = 0.230.
Figure 4. The Hierarchical Status of Myrmica Brood Items
Box plots illustrate the order in which worker ants rescued sclerotized
(brown) pupae, young white pupae, and larvae after their nest was perturbed
by exposure to light: vertical line = median rank of rescue, box = 25
th
–75
th
percentiles, whiskers = one standard deviation below and above the
mean of the data. White boxes show ‘‘normal’’ live ant brood (overall
Kruskal-Wallis H
n
= 11.182, df = 2, p = 0.003), and gray boxes show results
for ‘‘mute’’ recently dead brood (H= 26.347, df = 2, p < 0.001).
Acoustic Signaling of Ant Brood Status
325
undescribed brood pheromones exist [30]. Recent studies,
however, have shown that nonnestmate brood is often
adopted into an ant society as a quick and efficient way of
increasing the workforce, whereas behavioral experiments
show that any kin brood is always chosen first, indicating a
clear ability among workers to recognize immature nestmates
within their species [10, 31].
Notwithstanding the predominant use of semiochemicals in
ant communications, many species generate acoustical
signals through a stridulatory organ or by drumming their
gaster. Once considered a weak form of communication,
restricted to spreading alarm or modulating responses to
other signals [1, 32–34], it is increasingly clear that acoustics
is used to convey a greater variety of information between
nestmates as well as to signal an individual’s social status
[7, 8]. We suspect that acoustics may be a more flexible means
of signaling and conveying information between both adult
and immature ants than is generally recognized [22].
Experimental Procedures
Field Collection and Culture
Myrmica scabrinodis nests (n = 10) were collected in July 2011 at Walling-
ford (UK), set as standardized laboratory ant colonies with >100 workers
in 12.5 cm 38cm32 cm Perspex containers, and maintained on a diet
of sugar and Drosophila larvae [35]. All colonies contained a minimum of
ten larvae, ten white pupae, and ten sclerotized pupae.
Scanning Electron Microscopy
We used dissection and scanning electron microscopy to investigate the
presence of stridulatory organs on ant brood. Two M. scabrinodis larvae
and two white and two sclerotized pupae from two ant colonies were kept
in 70% ethanol, and one item per category was dissected between the post-
petiole and the abdomen to expose the pars stridens and the plectrum. The
whole individuals and the two ant parts were mounted on the same steel
stub and coated with gold, and the samples were scanned using a Cam-
bridge Stereoscan S360 scanning electron microscope. M. scabrinodis
white pupae and larvae were dried in hexamethyldisilazane to avoid cell
structure disruption before coating. The SEM operated at 20–25 kV.
Sound Recordings
We recorded sounds of clusters of six M. scabrinodis larvae and six white
and six sclerotized pupae from ten M. scabrinodis nests. Separate record-
ings were made of individual queens and workers taken from the same test
colonies. The recording equipment consisted of a 12.5 cm 38cm32cm
recording chamber with a moving-coil miniature microphone attached
through the center. A second microphone of the same type was used to
record ambient noise but in antiphase. An amplifier was attached to each
microphone and calibrated to maximize the noise cancellation of ambient
noise from the two microphones, leaving the signal from the recording
chamber. The resulting signal was processed through two-stage low-noise
amplification before being digitally recorded on a laptop computer, using
Audacity 1.3 Beta (http://audacity.sourceforge.net/). To further reduce
ambient noise and interference, the equipment was powered by a 12 V gel
cell battery, and the recording chamber and microphones were placed
inside an anechoic chamber. Sounds were recorded for 20 min periods
starting 10 min after items were introduced into the recording chamber.
Recordings were sampled at 44.10 kHz and 32-bit resolution. Frequency
information was obtained through fast Fourier transformation (FFT; width
1,024 points). Spectrograms were obtained at Hanning window function
with 512 bands resolution. We selected 20 good quality pulses from each
track and measured dominant frequency (Hz), pulse length, and sound
amplitude (dB) using Audacity 1.3 Beta. Based on the three sound parame-
ters, single pulses were ordinated by principal components analysis (PCA).
To test whether sound differed between groups, we calculated the pairwise
normalized Euclidean distance over all three parameters and used a nested
(‘‘colony’’ within ‘‘group’’) ANOSIM implemented in Primer v6 (Primer-E
Ltd.). The sound parameters were log(x+1) transformed. We calculated
the average pairwise distances and used a two-sample t test to compare
differences between group distances.
Worker Ant Responses to Sound Recordings
Behavioral assays were carried out in three 7 cm 37cm35 cm Perspex
arenas with the speaker attached at the bottom of the box and sealed on
the outside with Blu-Tack. The speaker was covered with a thin layer of
slightly wet soil. Ten workers from the same M. scabrinodis colony were
placed in each arena and allowed to settle for 10 min before being played
one of the three test sounds (M. scabrinodis worker, sclerotized pupae
sound, and white noise). The sounds were produced by MP3 players playing
loops of the original recordings, with each volume adjusted to the natural
level by attaching the speaker to the microphone of the recording equip-
ment and by calibrating to the same levels reached during recording.
Each trial lasted 30 min: counts were made of all instances of antagonistic
or attractive behaviors, during periods of one minute for each box, and in
sequence between the three treatments, i.e., S10 min for each sound per
trial. Each playback experiment was repeated 20 times, using fresh ants
from ten different M. scabrinodis colonies (i.e., twice for each colony). The
source of sound for each arena was randomly assigned before each trial
was replicated to control for possible positional effects. Between each trial,
new soil was introduced and all the equipment, including speakers and
arenas, was cleaned with absolute alcohol and rinsed with distilled water.
The effect of sound stimulus on the five worker ant behaviors was analyzed
in a linear mixed-effects model with ‘‘colonies’’ as a random factor using the
software R-2.15.0 [36].
Experiment to Measure the Order in which Workers Rescued Different
Brood Items
The arena used for the brood-rescue experiment consisted of two adja-
cent chambers of 7 32 cm communicating at one end. We placed eight
Myrmica larvae, eight white pupae, eight sclerotized pupae, and ten
workers on a 0.4 cm
3
moist sponge (to maintain humidity) at the end of
one chamber, which was then covered with a transparent glass. The other
chamber was covered with a dark glass. After 10 min of resting in the dark,
we shone a 60 W light placed 10 cm away onto the chamber containing the
worker ants and brood, to create a high level of stress which induced
workers to rescue the exposed brood and carry it into the dark chamber.
The order in which each item of brood was rescued was recorded. The
experiment was then repeated after placing all brood items from a colony
in a freezer (220C) for 20 min, thus killing the brood to make them mute
(and immobile). Brood items were then left at room temperature for 5 min
to return to normal temperature. Immediately after this period, the same
procedures as before were used to make rescue experiments. Previous
studies [24, 25] have established that in assays conducted only a short
time after immature ants are killed, the chemicals responsible for brood
recognition remain present in approximately the same quantities as in
the live brood.
Statistical analyses were performed using the package ‘‘coin’’ provided
with the software R-2.15.0 [36, 37]. Kruskal-Wallis tests were used to
compare the rescue orders of different brood categories between nonmute
and mute treatments. Subsequent pairwise comparisons of median
rescue order between brood categories within the same treatment
were made using Wilcoxon Mann-Whitney tests; p values were calculated
against a null distribution generated from data using a Monte Carlo
resampling. Direct comparisons of the same brood categories between
normal and mute treatments were made using paired Wilcoxon signed
rank tests.
Acknowledgments
Research was funded within the project CLIMIT (Climit Change Impacts on
Insects and their Mitigation; Settele and Ku¨ hn [2009] [38], Thomas et al.
[2009] [39]), funded by Deutsches Zentrum fu¨ r Luft-und Raumfahrt-Bundes-
ministerium fu¨r Bildung und Forschung (Germany); Natural Environment
Research Council (NERC) and Department for Environment, Food, and Rural
Affairs (UK); Agence nationale de la recherche (France); Formas (Sweden);
and Swedish Environmental Protection Agency (Sweden) through the
FP6 BiodivERsA Eranet. Part of the research was funded by the Italian
Ministry of Education, University, and Research (MIUR) within the project
‘‘A multitaxa approach to study the impact of climate change on the biodi-
versity of Italian ecosystems.’’
Received: December 3, 2012
Revised: January 2, 2013
Accepted: January 2, 2013
Published: February 7, 2013
Current Biology Vol 23 No 4
326
References
1. Ho
¨lldobler, B.E., and Wilson, E.O. (1990). The Ants (Cambridge, MA:
Belknap Press of Harvard University Press).
2. Ho
¨lldobler, B.E., and Wilson, E.O. (2009). The Superorganism: The
Beauty, Elegance, and Strangeness of Insect Societies (New York:
W.W. Norton & Company).
3. Wilson, E.O. (1971). The Insect Societies (Cambridge, MA: Belknap
Press of Harvard University Press).
4. Brian, M.V. (1975). Larval recognition by workers of the ant Myrmica.
Anim. Behav. 23, 745–756.
5. Vander Meer, R.K., and Morel, L. (1998). Nestmate recognition in ants. In
Pheromone Communication in Social Insects, R.K. Vander Meer, M.
Breed, M. Winston, and K.E. Espelie, eds. (Boulder, CO: Westview
Press), pp. 79–103.
6. Lenoir, A., D’Ettorre, P., Errard, C., and Hefetz, A. (2001). Chemical
ecology and social parasitism in ants. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46, 573–599.
7. Barbero, F., Thomas, J.A., Bonelli, S., Balletto, E., and Scho
¨nrogge, K.
(2009). Queen ants make distinctive sounds that are mimicked by
a butterfly social parasite. Science 323, 782–785.
8. Barbero, F., Bonelli, S., Thomas, J.A., Balletto, E., and Scho
¨nrogge, K.
(2009). Acoustical mimicry in a predatory social parasite of ants.
J. Exp. Biol. 212, 4084–4090.
9. Thomas, J.A., Scho
¨nrogge, K., Bonelli, S., Barbero, F., and Balletto, E.
(2010). Corruption of ant acoustical signals by mimetic social parasites:
Maculinea butterflies achieve elevated status in host societies by
mimicking the acoustics of queen ants. Commun.Integr. Biol. 3, 169–171.
10. Fouks, B., D’Ettorre, P., and Nehring, V. (2011). Brood adoption in the
leaf-cutting ant Acromyrmex echinatior: adaptation or recognition
noise? Insectes Soc. 58, 479–485.
11. Sudd, J.H. (1967). An Introduction to the Behavior of Ants (New York: St.
Martin’s Press).
12. Bagne
`res, A.G., Lorenzi, M.C., Dusticier, G., Turillazzi, S., and Clement,
J.L. (1996). Chemical usurpation of a nest by paper wasp parasites.
Science 272, 889–892.
13. Singer, T.L. (1998). Roles of hydrocarbons in the recognition systems of
insects. Am. Zool. 38, 394–405.
14. van Zweden, J.S., Brask, J.B., Christensen, J.H., Boomsma, J.J.,
Linksvayer, T.A., and d’Ettorre, P. (2010). Blending of heritable recogni-
tion cues among ant nestmates creates distinct colony gestalt odours
but prevents within-colony nepotism. J. Evol. Biol. 23, 1498–1508.
15. Glancey, B.M., Stringer, C.E., Craig, C.H., Bishop, P.M., and Martin, B.B.
(1970). Pheromone may induce brood tending in the fire ant, Solenopsis
saevissima. Nature 226, 863–864.
16. Akino, T., Knapp, J.J., Thomas, J.A., and Elmes, G.W. (1999). Chemical
mimicry and host specificity in the butterfly Maculinea rebeli, a social
parasite of Myrmica ant colonies. Proc. Biol. Sci. 266, 1419–1426.
17. Thomas, J.A., Elmes, G.W., and Wardlaw, J.C. (1998). Polymorphic
growth in larvae of the butterfly Maculinea rebeli, a social parasite of
Myrmica ant colonies. Proc. Biol. Sci. 265, 1895–1901.
18. Thomas, J.A., Elmes, G.W., Sielezniew, M., Stankiewicz-Fiedurek, A.,
Simcox, D.J., Settele, J., and Scho
¨nrogge, K. (2013). Mimetic host shifts
in an endangered social parasite of ants. Proc. Biol. Sci. 280, 20122336.
19. DeVries, P.J., Cocroft, R.B., and Thomas, J.A. (1993). Comparison of
acoustical signals in Maculinea butterfly caterpillars and their obligate
host Myrmica ants. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Lond. 49,229–238.
20. Markl, H. (1973). The evolution of stridulatory communication in ants. In
Proceedings IUSSI VIIth International Congress, London, September
10–15, 1973(Southampton, UK: University of Southampton), pp.258–265.
21. Hickling, R., and Brown, R.L. (2001). Response to ‘‘Ants are deaf’’.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109, 3083.
22. Chiu, Y.K., Mankin, R.W., and Lin, C.C. (2011). Context-dependent strid-
ulatory responses of Leptogenys kitteli (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) to
social, prey, and disturbance stimuli. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 104,
1012–1020.
23. Ferreira, R.S., Poteaux, C., Delabie, J.H.C., Fresneau, D., and Rybak, F.
(2010). Stridulations reveal cryptic speciation in neotropical sympatric
ants. PLoS ONE 5, e15363.
24. Robinson, S.W., and Cherrett, J.M. (1974). Laboratory investigations to
evaluate the possible use of brood pheromones of the leaf-cutting ant
Atta cephalotes (L.) (Formicidae, Attini) as a component in an attractive
bait. Bull. Entomol. Res. 63, 519–529.
25. Walsh, J.P., and Tschinkel, W.R. (1974). Brood recognition by contact
pheromone in the red imported fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Anim.
Behav. 22, 695–704.
26. Hickling, R., and Brown, R.L. (2000). Analysis of acoustic communica-
tion by ants. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108, 1920–1929.
27. d’Ettorre, P., and Lenoir, A. (2010). Nestmate recognition. In Ant
Ecology, L. Lach, C. Parr, and K. Abbott, eds. (Oxford: Oxford
University Press), pp. 194–208.
28. van Zweden, J.S., and d’Ettorre, P. (2010). The role of hydrocarbons in
nestmate recognition. In Insect Hydrocarbons: Biology, Biochemistry,
and Chemical Ecology, G.J. Blomquist and A.-G. Bagne
´res, eds.
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), pp. 222–243.
29. Scho
¨nrogge, K., Wardlaw, J.C., Peters, A.J., Everett, S., Thomas, J.A.,
and Elmes, G.W. (2004). Changes in chemical signature and host spec-
ificity from larval retrieval to full social integration in the myrmecophilous
butterfly Maculinea rebeli. J. Chem. Ecol. 30, 91–107.
30. Morel, L., and Vander Meer, R.K. (1988). Do ant brood pheromones
exist? Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 81, 705–710.
31. Solazzo, G., Moritz, R.F.A., and Settele, J. (2012). Choice behaviour
of Myrmica rubra workers between ant larvae and larvae of their
Phengaris (Maculinea) nausithous nest parasites. Insectes Soc. 60,
57–64. Published online November 11, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1007/s00040-012-0265-3.
32. Kirchner, W.H. (1997). Acoustical communication in social insects . In
Orientation and Communication in Arthropods, L. Lehrer, ed. (Basel:
Birkenha
¨user Verlag), pp. 273–300.
33. Markl, H., and Ho
¨lldobler, B. (1978). Recruitment and food-retrieving
behavior in Novomessor (Formicidae, Hymenoptera). II: Vibration
signals. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 4, 183–216.
34. Santos, J.C., Korndo
¨rfer, A.P., and Del-Claro, K. (2005). Defensive
behavior of the weaver ant Camponotus (Myrmobrachys) senex
(Formicidae, Formicinae):drumming and mimicry. Sociobiology 46, 1–10.
35. Wardlaw, J.C., Elmes, G.W., and Thomas, J.A. (1998). Techniques for
studying Maculinea butterfies. I. Rearing Maculinea caterpillars with
Myrmica ants in the laboratory. J. Insect Conserv. 2, 79–84.
36. RDevelopment Core Team (2012). R: A Language and Environment for
Statistical Computing (Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing).
http://www.R-project.org.
37. Hothorn, T., Hornik, K., van de Wiel, M.A., and Zeileis, A. (2008).
Implementing a class of permutation tests: the coin package. J. Stat.
Softw. 28, 1–23.
38. Settele, J., and Ku¨hn, E. (2009). Ecology. Insect conservation. Science
325, 41–42.
39. Thomas, J.A., Simcox, D.J., and Clarke, R.T. (2009). Successful conser-
vation of a threatened Maculinea butterfly. Science 325, 80–83.
Acoustic Signaling of Ant Brood Status
327
... We recorded and analysed the stridulation produced by M. scabrinodis workers and queens and from M. karavajevi queens, for a total of 1204 pulses. Ants' stridulation patterns were similar to those described in previous works carried out on the genus Myrmica [17,21,48]. The stridulations were constituted by series (trains) of a variable number of pulses ( Figure 3). ...
... The SO of M. karavajevi is described for the first time within the present work ( Figure 6). Overall, it is very similar to the organ present in all other species of the genus Myrmica studied so far [16,17,21,48,49]. Table S4). ...
... The SO of M. karavajevi is described for the first time within the present work ( Figure 6). Overall, it is very similar to the organ present in all other species of the genus Myrmica studied so far [16,17,21,48,49]. However, M. karavajevi SO differs in some morphological characteristics from the SOs of the host castes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social parasitism represents a particular type of agonistic interaction in which a parasite exploits an entire society instead of a single organism. One fascinating form of social parasitism in ants is the “inquilinism”, in which a typically worker-less parasitic queen coexists with the resident queen in the host colony and produces sexual offspring. To bypass the recognition system of host colonies, inquilines have evolved a repertoire of deceiving strategies. We tested the level of integration of the inquiline Myrmica karavajevi within the host colonies of M. scabrinodis and we investigated the mechanisms of chemical and vibroacoustic deception used by the parasite. M. karavajevi is integrated into the ant colony to such an extent that, in rescue experiments, the parasite pupae were saved prior to the host’s brood. M. karavajevi gynes perfectly imitated the cuticular hydrocarbon profiles of M. scabrinodis queens and the parasite vibroacoustic signals resembled those emitted by the host queens eliciting the same levels of attention in the host workers during playback experiments. Our results suggest that M. karavajevi has evolved ultimate deception strategies to reach the highest social status in the colony hierarchy, encouraging the use of a combined molecular and behavioural approach when studying host–parasite interactions.
... Direct observation indicates ants can readily locate and manipulate their brood in different settings, implying that the same pheromones (and other cues) may be exploitable by brood-specific natural enemies. However, the unambiguous identification of ant brood-specific recognition signals has remained controversial (Morel and Vander Meer, 1988;Casacci et al., 2013). Evidence indicates at least some post-embryonic developmental ant stages emit some form of chemical (Walsh and Tschinkel, 1974;Brian, 1975) and even sound (Casacci et al., 2013). ...
... However, the unambiguous identification of ant brood-specific recognition signals has remained controversial (Morel and Vander Meer, 1988;Casacci et al., 2013). Evidence indicates at least some post-embryonic developmental ant stages emit some form of chemical (Walsh and Tschinkel, 1974;Brian, 1975) and even sound (Casacci et al., 2013). It is therefore possible that these associates Johnson et al., 1996 Host and parasite nest sharing is indicated with yes/no and the location of nest and trail are described. ...
Article
Full-text available
Chemical communication is a fundamental, highly complex component of social insect societies. Ants in particular employ a remarkable diversity of chemical signals to maintain social cohesion among nestmates, gain essential resources through coordinated foraging, and warn of danger. Although the chemicals used can be functionally specific, they are vulnerable to exploitation by eavesdropping natural enemies (e.g., parasitoids, predators, parasites) and other associates (e.g., myrmecophiles). Ant nests are nutrient hotspots due to their collection of resources warranting keen defense systems; yet the heavily defended hideouts are frequently invaded. Many organisms exploit ant species, but how they locate hosts—including what host-derived cues are used—is still poorly understood. Here, we review current knowledge about how ant chemical communication systems can be exploited by unintended receivers. We take a case study approach and illustrate the diversity of ant associates and host traits that may predispose ants to exploitation. We identify knowledge gaps by reviewing host systems and listing: (1) the types of associates (e.g., fly, wasp, beetle) where eavesdropping is likely occurring, organized by the host communication system that is being exploited; (2) the ant parasites that exploit trail pheromones; and (3) the experimentally determined chemicals (i.e., alarm/defensive pheromones), used by eavesdroppers. At least 25 families of arthropods (10 orders) potentially eavesdrop on ant communication systems and nearly 20 host ant species are vulnerable to trail parasite ant species. We also propose future research that will improve our understanding of community assembly by examining host traits (e.g., latitude, nest characteristics, trail system) that influence their susceptibility to eavesdropping associates.
... The social structure is maintained through a complex communication system based on chemical [5,6], acoustical [7][8][9], visual [10,11], and tactile signals [12], as well as benevolent (e.g., trophallaxis or grooming behavior) or aggressive interactions to establish dominance dynamics in the colony hierarchy. Multimodal signals [13,14] are perceived by various sensors and integrated into the central nervous system in the brain of colony members. ...
Article
Full-text available
Eusociality represents the higher degree of interaction in insects. This complex social structure is maintained through a multimodal communication system that allows colony members to be flexible in their responses, fulfilling the overall society’s needs. The colony plasticity is supposedly achieved by combining multiple biochemical pathways through the neuromodulation of molecules such as biogenic amines, but the mechanisms through which these regulatory compounds act are far from being fully disentangled. Here, we review the potential function of major bioamines (dopamine, tyramine, serotine, and octopamine) on the behavioral modulation of principal groups of eusocial Hymenoptera, with a special focus on ants. Because functional roles are species- and context-dependent, identifying a direct causal relationship between a biogenic amine variation and behavioral changes is extremely challenging. We also used a quantitative and qualitative synthesis approach to summarize research trends and interests in the literature related to biogenic amines of social insects. Shedding light on the aminergic regulation of behavioral responses will pave the way for an entirely new approach to understanding the evolution of sociality in insects.
... In social insects, the main recognition cues and signals are chemical substances [12][13][14] , although other signalling modes may also be of importance (vibroacoustic e.g., 12,15,16 ; visual 17,18 ; tactile 19 ). The discrimination between strangers (both con-and allospecific) and nestmates is largely based on complex mixtures of long-chain cuticular hydrocarbons (CHC), which cover the surface of individuals. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many parasites interfere with the behaviour of their hosts. In social animals, such as ants, parasitic interference can cause changes on the level of the individual and also on the level of the society. The ant-parasitic fungus Rickia wasmannii influences the behaviour of Myrmica ants by expanding the host’s nestmate recognition template, thereby increasing the chance of the colony accepting infected non-nestmates. Infected ants consistently show an increase of the alkane tricosane (n-C23) in their cuticular hydrocarbon profiles. Although experimental application of single compounds often elicits aggression towards manipulated ants, we hypothesized that the increase of n-C23 might underlie the facilitated acceptance of infected non-nestmates. To test this, we mimicked fungal infection in M. scabrinodis by applying synthetic n-C23 to fresh ant corpses and observed the reaction of infected and uninfected workers to control and manipulated corpses. Infected ants appeared to be more peaceful towards infected but not uninfected non-nestmates. Adding n-C23 to uninfected corpses resulted in reduced aggression in uninfected ants. This supports the hypothesis that n-C23 acts as a ‘pacifying’ signal. Our study indicates that parasitic interference with the nestmate discrimination of host ants might eventually change colony structure by increasing genetic heterogeneity in infected colonies.
... While effects of the rearing environment on brood development, and particularly on sex and caste allocation, have been extensively studied in social insects [29][30][31][32][33], whether and how the brood can affect worker behaviour has received comparatively little attention. Previous work in other systems has shown that the brood of social insects can produce chemical [34], behavioural [35] and acoustic [36] cues, some of which influence worker behaviour [37,38]. For example, honeybee larvae secrete a pheromone that affects worker physiology and behaviour [37,39,40]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many social animals display collective activity cycles based on synchronous behavioural oscillations across group members. A classic example is the colony cycle of army ants, where thousands of individuals undergo stereotypical biphasic behavioural cycles of about one month. Cycle phases coincide with brood developmental stages, but the regulation of this cycle is otherwise poorly understood. Here, we probe the regulation of cycle duration through interactions between brood and workers in an experimentally amenable army ant relative, the clonal raider ant. We first establish that cycle length varies across clonal lineages using long-term monitoring data. We then investigate the putative sources and impacts of this variation in a cross-fostering experiment with four lineages combining developmental, morphological and automated behavioural tracking analyses. We show that cycle length variation stems from variation in the duration of the larval developmental stage, and that this stage can be prolonged not only by the clonal lineage of brood (direct genetic effects), but also of the workers (indirect genetic effects). We find similar indirect effects of worker line on brood adult size and, conversely (but more surprisingly), indirect genetic effects of the brood on worker behaviour (walking speed and time spent in the nest).
... In social insects, the main recognition cues and signals are chemical substances [12][13][14], although other signalling modes may also be of importance (vibroacoustic: e.g., [12,[15][16]; visual: [17][18]; tactile: [19]). The discrimination between strangers (both con-and allospeci c) and nestmates is largely based on complex mixtures of long-chain cuticular hydrocarbons (CHC), which cover the surface of individuals. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Many parasites interfere with the behaviour of their hosts. In social animals, such as ants, parasitic interference can cause changes on the level of the individual and also on the level of the society. The ant-parasitic fungus Rickia wasmannii influences the behaviour of Myrmica ants by expanding the host’s nestmate recognition template, thereby increasing the chance of the colony accepting infected non-nestmates. Infected ants consistently show an increase of the alkane tricosane ( n -C23) in their cuticular hydrocarbon profiles. Although experimental application of single compounds often elicits aggression towards manipulated ants, we hypothesized that the increase of n- C23 might underlie the facilitated acceptance of infected non-nestmates. To test this, we mimicked fungal infection in M. scabrinodis by applying synthetic n -C23 to fresh ant corpses and observing the reaction of infected and uninfected workers to control and manipulated corpses. Infected ants appeared to be more peaceful towards infected but not uninfected non-nestmates. Adding n -C23 to uninfected corpses resulted in reduced aggression by uninfected ants. This supports the hypothesis that n -C23 acts as a “pacifying” signal. Parasitic interference with the nestmate discrimination of host ants might eventually change colony structure by increasing genetic heterogeneity and thus might significantly affect social evolution.
... For instance, the larvae of the social parasite wasp Polistes sulcifer are cared by host workers more than host brood [100]. Among ants, Myrmica schencki workers protect queen-destined pupae more than worker pupae in case of nest disturbances [101] and exhibit higher protection responses towards queens than workers; when colonies are parasitized by Maculinea rebeli butterflies, parasite offspring deserve the same level of protection as host queens [102]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social parasites have evolved adaptations to overcome host resistance as they infiltrate host colonies and establish there. Among the chemical adaptations, a few species are chemically “insignificant”; they are poor in recognition cues (cuticular hydrocarbons) and evade host detection. As cuticular hydrocarbons also serve a waterproofing function, chemical insignificance is beneficial as it protects parasites from being detected but is potentially harmful because it exposes parasites to desiccation stress. Here I tested whether the social parasites Polistes atrimandibularis employ behavioral water-saving strategies when they live at Polistes biglumis colonies. Observations in the field showed that parasites were less active than their cohabiting host foundresses, spent more time at the nest, and rested in the shadowy, back face of the nest, rather than at the front face, which contradicted expectations for the use of space for dominant females—typically, dominants rest at the nest front-face. These data suggest that behavioral adaptations might promote resistance to desiccation stress in chemical insignificant social parasites.
... The transitional pupal phase is an essential element of this strategy. In many eusocial insects, other colony members oversee the developmental transition between larva and pupa; e.g., worker ants carry larvae to pupal locations within the nest, and worker bees enclose pre-pupal larvae in cells (H€ olldobler & Wilson, 1990;Bourke, 1999;Casacci et al., 2013). In solitary species, however, each individual insect must make the journey from larval feeding site to pupation site on its own (Stehr, 2009a). ...
Article
Although many organisms experience relatively predictable environmental conditions throughout development, others, as a result of ontogenetic changes, inhabit very different environments across life stages, requiring them to navigate into habitats or microhabitats that they have not previously encountered. For holometabolous insects, late instars must often locate a safe site before undergoing pupation. In Lepidoptera, many pre‐pupae cease feeding and wander in search of an appropriate pupation site; their preferences display a shift in behavioral responses to habitat‐specific environmental cues, relative to those of feeding larvae. Having previously determined that pre‐pupae of Epargyreus clarus (Cramer) (Lepidoptera: Hesperiidae) move from their Fabaceae host plant down into the leaf litter, where they construct a pupal refuge out of dried leaves and silk, we investigated the sensory cues used by E. clarus in selecting a pupation location. We further explored how responses to these cues vary between actively feeding larvae and non‐feeding pre‐pupae, both for individual insects and for stage‐based cohorts. Using Y‐tube assays, we determined that pre‐pupae used visual (light) and gravitational cues to move from host plants into the leaf litter, whereas the odor cues we tested did not appear to influence their decision‐making. Directional responses to both light and gravity reversed over ontogeny, allowing the two life stages to make use of the same cues to produce stage‐appropriate adaptive behaviors.
... So far, this form of acoustical mimicry has been found only in larvae and pupae of Maculinea spp. lycenid butterflies (Barbero et al., 2009a, b;Casacci et al., 2013), guests of Myrmica spp. ants, and in adults of the ant nest beetle Paussus favieri (Di Giulio et al., 2015), guest of Pheidole pallidula. ...
Article
The social parasitic beetle Paussus favieri (Coleoptera, Carabidae, Paussini) performs different types of stridulations, which selectively mimic those emitted by different ant castes of its host Pheidole pallidula (Hymenoptera, Formicidae, Myrmicinae). However, the significance of this acoustical mimicry for the success of the parasitic strategy and the behaviors elicited in the host ants by stridulations was unknown. We reared Paussus favieri in Pheidole pallidula colonies and filmed their interacting behaviors. We analyzed in slow motion the behavior of ants near a stridulating beetle. We analyzed separately trains of pulse (Pa+Pb, produced by repeated rubbings) and single pulse (Pc, produced by a single rubbing) of stridulations, clearly recognizable from the shaking up and down of the beetle hind legs, and associated them with different ant responses. The full repertoire of sounds produced by P. favieri elicited benevolent responses both in workers and soldiers. We found that different signals elicit different (sometimes multiple) behaviors in ants, with different frequency in the two ant castes. However, Pc (alone or in conjunction with other types of pulses) appears to be the type of acoustic signal mostly responsible for all recorded behaviors. These results indicate that the acoustic channel plays a pivotal role in the host‐parasite interaction. Finding that a parasite uses the acoustical channel so intensively, and in such a complicated way to trigger ant behaviors, indicates that acoustic signals may be more important in ant societies than commonly recognized. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved
Article
Full-text available
Insect societies require an effective communication system to coordinate members’ activities. Although eusocial species primarily use chemical communication to convey information to conspecifics, there is increasing evidence suggesting that vibroacoustic communication plays a significant role in the behavioural contexts of colony life. In this study, we sought to determine whether stridulation can convey information in ant societies. We tested three main hypotheses using the Mediterranean ant Crematogaster scutellaris: (i) stridulation informs about the emitter’caste; (ii) workers can modulate stridulation based on specific needs, such as communicating the profitability of a food resource, or (iii) behavioural contexts. We recorded the stridulations of individuals from the three castes, restrained on a substrate, and the signals emitted by foragers workers feeding on honey drops of various sizes. Signals emitted by workers and sexuates were quantitatively and qualitatively distinct as was stridulation emitted by workers on different honey drops. Comparing across the experimental setups, we demonstrated that signals emitted in different contexts (restraining vs feeding) differed in emission patterns as well as certain parameters (dominant frequency, amplitude, duration of chirp). Our findings suggest that vibrational signaling represents a flexible communication channel paralleling the well-known chemical communication system.
Article
Full-text available
Ants have an enormous variety of defensive and offensive techniques to use in the most varied situations of food search and colony protection, including drumming. The drumming consists of beating the gaster multiple times against a substratum. Camponotus senex is a weaver ant that builds its nests with the silk expelled by its larvae on tree species of the Neotropical forests. Drumming behavior in C. senex seems to be an important defensive trait related to the structure of the nest. We describe the defensive behavior and test the effect of a potential predator on the modulation of the defensive answers by C. senex, especially drumming. A nest (35 x 28cm) was collected and conditioned in an experimental garden. To simulate potential predator attack, 64 disturbances were performed using two cylindrical pots with the same volume (10ml), however with different masses: one of 11g (M1, n = 32 tests) and another of 38g (M2, n = 32 tests). The masses were loosened from a height of 15cm to fall on top of the nest, with a 5-minute interval between the disturbance with M1 and M2, and with an interval of 20 minutes between that with M2, followed by M1. The number of ants in the external part and the resonant effect provoked by the drumming were quantified before and after the disturbances. The results showed that the average number of ants outside of the nest after each treatment was significantly larger (p < 0.001) than control (no disturbance) and no difference was observed among treatments (P = 0.115). However, for the duration of resonant effect, the drumming answer was stronger with increasing disturbance (M2) and time (p < 0.001). The defensive behavior of C. senex is quite elaborate, with tactics similar to those observed in other Camponotus species, such as C. herculeanus and C. ligniperta. Here it is suggested that drumming in C. senex could act as an aposematic sound signal against potential predators, mimicking Polibia wasps.
Chapter
Airborne and substrate-borne sounds play a crucial role in intraspecific communication in many social insect species, and often also in interspecific information transfer. Acoustical signals are involved in a variety of social interactions and serve several functions, such as alarming or warning nestmates, recruiting nestmates to a profitable foraging site in the near or the far surrounding of the nest, or to a site within the nest where urgent activity is required. Sounds are also used for communication among members of the reproductive castes. Some sound signals enhance the effect of other, non-acoustical signals, and some have still unknown functions that remain to be unravelled. The present chapter reviews results of early and recent studies on the various mechanisms of sound production and sound perception in different social insects, on the physical parameters of the acoustical signals involved, and on the nature and the capacities of the respective sensory organs. It also describes experimental results demonstrating the various functions of acoustical communication in social insects, and discusses their evolutionary and ecological significance.
Article
Many bioassays have shown that cuticular hydrocarbons are used in the recognition systems of both solitary and social insects. The function of insect recognition systems is to enable an insect to recognize, and possibly discriminate, its own species, sex, or kin from that of other insects. The primary function of cuticular hydrocarbons is to protect the insects from desiccation. Hydrocarbons can be removed from insect cuticles and characterized with gas chromatography/ mass spectrometry. Studies using such analytical techniques have revealed that insect hydrocarbon compositions are species-specific, sex-specific and, in social insects, colony- and caste-specific. Furthermore, recognition bioassays have confirmed that certain components of the cuticle of some insect species are sex attractants as well as aphrodisiacs or sex inhibitors. Other bioassays have shown that hydrocarbons are important in facilitating colony structure in social insects. In addition, the hydrocarbons of some parasitic insects appear to mimic those of their host species. Thus, hydrocarbons are proving to be very important in the everyday activities of many insect species.
Chapter
The ability to recognize group members is a key characteristic of social life. Ants are typically very efficient in recognizing non-group members and they aggressively reject them in order to protect their colonies. There are a range of different recognition mechanisms including prior association, phenotype matching, and recognition alleles. The concept of kin recognition should be considered different from that of nestmate recognition. Most of the available studies address the nestmate recognition level, namely the discrimination of nestmates from non-nestmates, independently of actual relatedness. Indirect and direct evidence identify long-chain cuticular hydrocarbons as the best candidates to act as recognition cues in ants, even if other chemical substances could also play a role, at least in some ant species. The relative importance of genetic and environmental factors on the expression and variation of the cuticular hydrocarbon profile vary among species and is linked to life history strategies.