ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Hippocampus-Dependent Memory Formation: Do Memory Type-Specific Mechanisms Exist?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Long-term memory (LTM) formation requires gene transcription and de novo protein synthesis. The transcription factor CREB is required for hippocampus-dependent LTM formation, and it is activated by several signaling pathways, including protein kinase A (PKA), the mitogen activated protein/extracellular signal-regulated kinases (MAPK or ERKs), and Ca(2+)/calmodulin kinases (CaMKs). However, it is unknown whether all types of hippocampus-dependent LTM use the same signaling to activate transcription, and whether the transcriptional output is the same. Here we present molecular genetic and behavioral studies to demonstrate that two types of hippocampus-dependent LTM formation, spatial and contextual, require different signaling molecules. This can be illustrated by the CaMK kinases, CaMKKalpha, and CaMKKbeta, which have converse roles. CaMKKalpha is required for contextual and CaMKKbeta is required for spatial LTM formation. This leads to the surprising conclusion that there are distinct types of hippocampus-dependent LTM, which differ in their underlying molecular mechanisms.
Content may be subject to copyright.
191
Journal of Pharmacological Sciences
©2005 The Japanese Pharmacological Society
Critical Review
J Pharmacol Sci 98, 191 – 197 (2005)
Hippocampus-Dependent Memory Formation: Do Memory
Type-Specific Mechanisms Exist?
Keiko Mizuno
1
and Karl Peter Giese
1,
*
1
Wolfson Institute for Biomedical Research, University College London, London, WC1E6BT, UK
Received April 13, 2005
Abstract. Long-term memory (LTM) formation requires gene transcription and de novo protein
synthesis. The transcription factor CREB is required for hippocampus-dependent LTM forma-
tion, and it is activated by several signaling pathways, including protein kinase A (PKA), the
mitogen activated protein/ extracellular signal-regulated kinases (MAPK or ERKs), and
Ca
2+
/ calmodulin kinases (CaMKs). However, it is unknown whether all types of hippocampus-
dependent LTM use the same signaling to activate transcription, and whether the transcriptional
output is the same. Here we present molecular genetic and behavioral studies to demonstrate that
two types of hippocampus-dependent LTM formation, spatial and contextual, require different
signaling molecules. This can be illustrated by the CaMK kinases, CaMKKα, and CaMKKβ,
which have converse roles. CaMKKα is required for contextual and CaMKKβ is required for
spatial LTM formation. This leads to the surprising conclusion that there are distinct types of
hippocampus-dependent LTM, which differ in their underlying molecular mechanisms.
Keywords: hippocampus, cAMP-responsive element binding protein, signaling,
Ca
2+
/ calmodulin kinase, mouse genetics
Introduction
The study of patient H.M. in the mid 1950s showed
that the formation of long-term memory (LTM) requires
the medial temporal lobe (brain areas including the
hippocampus, the amygdala, and part of the temporal
cortex). H.M. lost his ability to generate new LTM after
bilateral removal of the medial temporal lobe for treat-
ment of his severe seizures. Even though H.M. lost his
ability to generate new LTM, he kept his older memories
from before surgery, and he was able to have short-term
memory (STM). STM lasts for only a few minutes,
whereas LTM can last for days, weeks, or years. In
humans, LTM can be distinguished as explicit (declara-
tive) or implicit (non-declarative) memory. Explicit
memory is recalled by a deliberate and conscious effort,
such as factual knowledge of people, places, and things.
Implicit memory is involved in training, reflexive,
motor, and perceptual skills. The information from H.M.
and other similar patients have demonstrated an essential
role of the hippocampus in the formation of explicit but
not implicit memory. For example, H.M. can not
remember new faces and places, but he can learn new
motor skills (1). In rodents lesions of the hippocampus
impair memory for space and context, and the hippo-
campus is thought to be important for creating spatial
representations in the brain (2).
Using genetically modified mice, cellular and mole-
cular mechanisms underlying hippocampus-dependent
LTM formation have been studied intensively. Because
some genes such as N-methyl-
D-aspartate (NMDA)
receptors and the transcription factor cAMP-responsive
element binding protein (CREB) are needed for develop-
ment and survival, traditional null mutant mice are
not always suitable for studying mechanisms of LTM
formation. Therefore, some mutant mouse lines were
generated using the forebrain-specific αCaMKII-
promoter for spatial restriction, and occasionally, the
tetracycline-regulated system for temporal reversibility
in the expression of dominant-negative transgenes (3, 4).
Region-restricted deletions of genes, such as in CA1-
NR1-null mutant mice, were also generated using the
*Corresponding author. FAX: +44 207 916 5994
E-mail: p.giese@ucl.ac.uk
Invited article
K Mizuno and KP Giese192
phage P1-derived, Cre/ loxP recombination system
(Fig. 1).
Recently, the molecular genetic analysis of hippo-
campus-dependent LTM has shown that there are
distinct types, which require different signaling mole-
cules. Here we will review this evidence and focus on
studies that tested particular mutant mouse lines in both
the water maze and contextual conditioning and found
impaired LTM formation.
Different types of hippocampus-dependent behav-
ioral tasks
Several hippocampus-dependent LTM tasks exist, but
the water maze and contextual fear conditioning are the
most commonly used tasks in the field of mouse mole-
cular genetics.
1) Hippocampus-dependent spatial memory forma-
tion is frequently investigated in the hidden-platform
version of the water maze (5). In the water maze, mice
learn to navigate to a submerged platform by using
distal, spatial cues in the room. Mice are trained in serial
sessions and at the end of training, spatial memory
formation is assessed in a probe trial where the platform
is removed from the pool. If the mouse has formed
normal spatial memory, it will spend significant time in
the target quadrant where the platform was located. As a
control for performance deficits, a visible platform
version of this task is used, which does not require the
hippocampus. The water maze is a multi-trial task in
which mice learn slowly. It is impossible to distinguish
between impairment in learning versus memory with the
exception of a few particular LTM impairments (e.g., 6);
and furthermore over-training, depending on the training
protocol, could potentially miss the detection of an
abnormal phenotype.
Fig. 1. Genetic techniques to restrict mutations. Left: The tetracycline-dependent regulatory system is used for inducible
expression of a transgene. The tetracycline transactivator (tTA) is used as a switch ON/OFF transcription to drive the expression
of a gene of interest. A mouse line (Mouse A) that expresses tTA protein under a tissue-type specific promoter (αCaMKII), and a
mouse line (Mouse B) that expresses a gene of interest placed downstream of the tet operator (tetO) promoter are intercrossed.
The tTA protein binds to the tetO promoter and induces transcription. Whenever the tetracycline (doxycyclin DOX) is present in
either drinking water or foods, it blocks transcription. Right: Spatial restriction by the Cre/ loxP system is a widely used
technique. Cre is a site-specific DNA recombinase derived from the P1 bacteriophage and recognizes loxP sites. Cre catalyzes the
deletion of DNA that is flanked by a pair of loxP sites (floxed), creating a null mutation of the gene of interest. A mouse line
(Mouse A) that expresses Cre driven by a tissue-specific promoter (αCaMKII) and a mouse line (Mouse B) that contains the
floxed gene are intercrossed. Restricted expression of Cre such as expression only in the hippocampal area CA1 or CA3 leads to a
region-restricted deletion of the floxed gene.
Distinct Hippocampal Memory Types 193
2) Another frequently used task, contextual condition-
ing, is also hippocampus-dependent (7). In this task,
mice can learn a new environment (training box) as a
whole context, not as context elements. In contextual
conditioning, a mouse is exposed to a novel environ-
ment, which is the context. A mild foot shock is pro-
vided and consequently the mouse associates the context
with the shock. When placed back into the context, the
mouse remembers the context-shock association and
exhibits ‘freezing’. Freezing is scored at 1 2 h (protein
synthesis-independent, STM) and 24 h or longer time
points (protein synthesis-dependent, LTM) after the
training. Contextual conditioning with a tone presenta-
tion requires the dorsal hippocampus and amygdala and
also permits testing for amygdala-dependent tone fear
conditioning (8). If a particular mutant is impaired in
both contextual and tone conditioning tests, it is
impossible to establish whether the impaired contextual
memory formation resulted from dysfunction of either
the hippocampus or the amygdala. Another point to
consider is that in some mutants, contextual condition-
ing may be normal even in the presence of hippocampal
dysfunctions because non-hippocampal systems can
support contextual conditioning (7). In this case, context
discrimination, a more difficult task (9), may be more
sensitive and therefore detect impairments in hippo-
campus-dependent memory formation. On the other
hand, the contextual discrimination task may test for
another hippocampus-dependent memory type.
Molecular mechanisms of LTM formation
The process of LTM formation requires gene tran-
scription and de novo protein synthesis in contrast to
STM, which requires post-translational modification of
proteins (10). The transcription factor CREB is required
for hippocampus-dependent LTM formation (11); and
signaling by protein kinase A (PKA), the mitogen
activated protein/ extracellular signal-regulated kinases
(MAPK/ ERKs), and Ca
2+
/ calmodulin kinases (CaMKs)
have been implicated in activation of CREB, by phos-
phorylating Serine 133. The transcriptional co-activator
CREB-binding protein (CBP) also needs to be phos-
phorylated to activate CREB-mediated transcription
(11, 12). CBP plays a role in cognitive function not
only as a platform protein, but also as a chromatin
remodelling histone acetyltransferase (13). The current
view of LTM formation in the hippocampus involves an
activation of CREB and CBP. However, it is unknown
whether all types of hippocampus-dependent LTM use
the same signaling to activate transcription and whether
the transcriptional output is the same.
Common signaling for hippocampus-dependent
spatial and contextual LTM formation
Several molecular genetic studies have shown that
there is common signaling between spatial and contex-
tual LTM formation.
CREB was the first transcription factor to be studied
intensively in LTM formation. CREB hypomorphic
mutant mice have impairments in both spatial and
contextual LTM formation (14). However, even though
these mutants lack expression of the predominant α- and
δ-isoforms of CREB, they have a compensatory up-
regulation in CREB β and the cAMP-responsive element
modulator (CREM) (15). Additional mutants with
inducible expression of a dominant inhibitor of all
CREB/ ATF transcription factors confirmed a role for
CREB in LTM formation (16, 17). Although CREB is
important for LTM formation, the phenotypes of the
CREB hypomorphic mutants and some other CREB-
specific transgenic manipulations are inconsistent. We
believe that this is due to the large number of
CREB/ ATF transcription factors including CREM and
ATF-1 being expressed in the hippocampus. There is a
tight regulation between these transcription factors,
probably depending on the genetic background, which
influences the behavioral phenotype.
Among the CREB-activating kinases, some
Ca
2+
/ calmodulin-dependent protein kinases (CaMKs)
directly connect Ca
2+
-signaling and gene expression
(12, 18). One of these kinases is CaMKIV, which
phosphorylates CREB, and has a relatively well-studied
role in hippocampal LTM. Forebrain-restricted domi-
nant-negative CaMKIV mutant mice (dnCaMKIV) are
impaired in spatial and contextual memory formation
(19). Contextual memory is impaired 7 days, but not
1 day after conditioning, and the impairment in contex-
tual LTM formation does not seem to be caused by
amygdala dysfunction. This is because memory of cued
conditioning is not affected. In contrast with these
findings, CaMKIV null mutant mice have been
described as normal in spatial memory formation (20).
However, the testing conditions may not have been
sufficiently sensitive to detect an abnormal phenotype.
The CaMKIV null mutants are impaired in contextual
memory formation, but this impairment is likely to have
resulted from amygdala dysfunction because the mutants
are also affected in cued conditioning (21).
The neuronal response to a Ca
2+
stimulus is a complex
process involving direct Ca
2+
/ calmodulin (CaM) actions
as well as secondary activation of multiple signaling
pathways such as cAMP and ERK (12, 18). Transgenic
mice, expressing a dominant inhibitor of CaM selec-
tively in the nuclei of adult forebrain neurons, are
K Mizuno and KP Giese194
impaired in contextual memory 1 day but not 1 h after
conditioning LTM formation (22). These mutants are
also impaired in spatial memory formation.
The MAP kinase pathway contains at least three
protein kinases: the extracellular signal-regulated kinases
1/ 2 (ERK1 and ERK2), the immediate upstream of the
MAP/ERK kinase (MEK or MKK), and the Raf family
as MEKK (23). Conditional transgenic mice expressing
dominant-negative MEK1 in the postnatal forebrain are
impaired in contextual memory 1 day but not 1 h after
conditioning LTM formation (24). These mutants are
also impaired in spatial memory formation.
The cAMP-PKA pathway is involved in CREB acti-
vation. Dominant-negative PKA mutants have impaired
LTM but normal STM in contextual and cued condition-
ing, and impaired spatial memory formation (25).
The role of c-Fos, an immediate-early gene and a
target gene of CREB, was studied in conventional c-fos
null mutants, but these mice suffer from developmental
malformations (26) and are not suitable for studying
learning and memory. A CNS-specific c-fos null mutant
(c-fos δ
CNS
) is impaired in both spatial and contextual
LTM formation (27). However, interestingly when the
fos-related antigen 1 (Fra-1), a member of c-fos family
of transcription factors, was knocked-in into the c-fos
locus, it can only replace the function of contextual, but
not spatial memory formation (28). This suggests that
there are different molecular requirements for spatial
and contextual LTM formation.
Molecules required for either spatial or contextual
LTM
Recent findings show that spatial and contextual LTM
require for their formation different signaling to regu-
late gene transcription and they need distinct transcrip-
tional outputs. The Ca
2+
/ calmodulin kinase kinases,
CaMKKα and CaMKKβ, can both phosphorylate
CaMKIV and this phoshorylation event enhances
CaMKIV activity (18). Studies with CaMKKβ null
mutants revealed that this kinase is required for the
activation of CREB by spatial training as well as spatial
memory formation in the water maze in male mice (29).
However, CaMKKβ is not needed for contextual LTM
formation. Thus, for spatial but not contextual LTM
formation, CaMKKβ may activate CaMKIV to phos-
phorylate CREB. In contrast to CaMKKβ, CaMKKα is
required for contextual but not spatial LTM in male mice
(30). The requirement for contextual LTM is hippo-
campus-dependent because CaMKKα is not needed for
cued conditioning. It remains to be investigated whether
CaMKKα activates CaMKIV to phosphorylate CREB
during contextual but not spatial LTM formation. The
studies with CaMKKα and CaMKKβ mutants show
that there is a double dissociation for the signaling
mechanisms required for spatial and contextual LTM
formation. This dissociation between spatial and
contextual LTM formation has also been observed in
several other mutant mouse lines.
Earlier studies of CREB in LTM formation focused
only on CREB. However, CREB can heterodimerize
with the other CREB family transcription factors CREM
and ATF1, and their functions are not restricted to
hippocampus-dependent memory formation (12). To
overcome these difficulties, a transgenic mouse line
expressing KCREB was generated. KCREB is a mutant
of human CREB that is a potent dominant-negative
inhibitor, and importantly, it also inhibits all three CREB
family transcription factors. dCA1-KCREB mice have
restricted expression of KCREB in area CA1 of the
dorsal hippocampus (17). These mutants are impaired in
spatial LTM formation, but have normal contextual
LTM. Importantly, when the KCREB gene is switched
off, spatial LTM impairment is rescued. Furthermore,
dorsally restricted dCA1-KCREB provided evidences
that the dorsal hippocampus is needed for spatial LTM,
but not contextual LTM. These results support previous
lesion studies that total hippocampal lesions disrupt both
the water maze and contextual fear conditioning,
whereas ventral lesions disrupt fear conditioning while
sparing performance in the water maze (31), and dorsal
hippocampal lesions disrupt performance in the water
maze only (32). A similar phenotype, impaired spatial
and normal contextual LTM, was observed in brain-
specific CREB null mutants, which have up-regulated
CREM expression (33).
CBP is an important factor to form LTM and func-
tions as a transcriptional co-activator as well as histone
acetyltransferase (HAT), which can remodel the chro-
matin structure. Dominant-negative CBP transgenic
mice that specifically lack HAT activity (CBP{HAT-})
are impaired in spatial memory formation but they are
normal in contextual LTM (34).
As one of the mechanisms to stimulate ERK, the
PKA-dependent activation of the Ras-related small
G protein Rap1 and B-Raf were identified. Transgenic
mice expressing a temporally controlled dominant-
negative Rap1 (iRap1) in the forebrain showed that
Rap1 is required for spatial memory formation (35).
Mice with iRap1 exhibit deficits in contextual discrimi-
nation but not in contextual and cued fear conditioning
tasks.
The pituitary adenylate cyclase activating polypeptide
(PACAP) type I receptor (PAC1) is a G-protein coupled
receptor. Null mutants as well as forebrain-specific
mutants both have a deficit in contextual but not cued
Distinct Hippocampal Memory Types 195
fear conditioning, but normal spatial LTM (36).
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is a target
gene of CREB (37). BDNF expression is regulated in the
hippocampus after spatial training as well as contextual
conditioning (38, 39). Since BDNF and its receptor
TrkB are expressed throughout the brain from develop-
ment, it is difficult to know the function specifically in
learning and memory using traditional null mutants. To
study the specific role of BDNF, forebrain-restricted
BDNF null mutants (Emx-BDNFKO mice) were
generated (40). Emx-BDNFKO mice are impaired in
spatial memory formation but they are normal in contex-
tual LTM. However, in these BDNF mutants, contextual
freezing was enhanced in comparison to WT mice,
and the mutants generalize their context-shock associa-
tion to other contexts. Thus, there is the possibility that
contextual learning in mutant mice is not hippocampus-
dependent. Indeed pre-training lesions suggest that
“contextual conditioning” can occur without the hippo-
campus, possibly resulting from discrete cue-shock
associations (9).
Conclusion
The transcription factor CREB was recognised as a
molecular switch for LTM formation in the hippo-
campus. Original findings suggested that CREB is
required for LTM formation in general. However, some
difficulties were encountered in these earlier experi-
ments to elucidate the detailed roles of CREB in
different types of LTM formation. Using region-specific
mutations, recent results suggest that not only CREB
but also other transcription factors might be important
for LTM formation. CREB is more involved in spatial
LTM formation, but perhaps other transcription factors
are required for contextual LTM formation. For
example, c-Rel, a transcription factor of the nuclear
factor κB family, is required in contextual LTM forma-
tion (41). Further supporting this idea, we found that
CREB activation was regulated during spatial LTM
formation and required CaMKKβ (29), but we failed to
detect any CREB activation during contextual LTM
formation (our unpublished data). This might suggest
Fig. 2. Summary of the signaling pathways involved in spatial and/ or contextual long-term memory (LTM) formation
discussed in this review. Excitatory neurotransmitters, ligands for GPCRs, and neuronal growth factors are among the stimuli
that activate signaling pathways during hippocampus-dependent LTM formation. It is thought that these signals activate CREB
kinases and CREB. Stimulus-dependent kinases include PKA, CaMKIV, and downstream-kinases of ERK such as pp90RSK
(RSK), and MSK families of protein kinases are CREB kinases. Some signaling components are required for both spatial and
contextual LTM formation (circled) and others are required for either spatial or contextual LTM formation (boxed). Abbrevia-
tions: AC: adenylate cyclase, BDNF: brain-derived neurotrophic factor, CaM: Ca
2+
/calmodulin, CaMKK: Ca
2+
/calmodulin
kinase kinase, CBP: CREB binding protein, CTX: contextual LTM formation, ERK: extracellular signal-regulated kinase,
GPCR: G protein-couple receptor, MEK: MAP/ ERK kinase, NMDAR: N-methyl-
D-aspartate receptor, PAC1: PACAP type I
receptor, PKA: cAMP-dependent protein kinase, RTK: receptor tyrosine kinase, VSCC: voltage-sensitive calcium channels.
K Mizuno and KP Giese196
that contextual LTM formation requires another tran-
scription factor(s) and CaMKKα.
We have discussed the signaling molecules whose
roles in spatial and contextual LTM formation has been
tested using genetically modified mice and this is
summarised in Fig. 2. It seems that molecules upstream
of CREB are differentially required for distinct types of
hippocampus-dependent LTM; CaMKKβ and Rap1 are
involved in spatial, whereas CaMKKα and PAC1 are
needed for in contextual LTM formation. Therefore, we
conclude that the formation of two types of hippo-
campus-dependent LTM, spatial and contextual, uses
different signaling molecules and perhaps distinct
transcription factors.
Acknowledgments
We thank L. Drinkwater for helpful comments and are
grateful for the generous support from the Wellcome
Trust and British Medical Research Council.
References
1 Milner B, Squire LR, Kandel ER. Cognitive neuroscience and
the study of memory. Neuron. 1998;20:445–468.
2 O’Keefe J, Nadel L. The hippocampus as a cognitive map.
Oxford: Clarendon Press; 1978.
3 Mayford M, Kandel ER. Genetic approaches to memory storage.
Trends Genet. 1999;15:463–470.
4 Nakazawa K, McHugh TJ, Wilson MA, Tonegawa, S. NMDA
receptors, place cells and hippocampal spatial memory. Nat Rev
Neurosci. 2004;5:361–372.
5 Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O’Keefe J. Place navigation
impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 1982;297:
681–683.
6 Remondes M, Schuman EM. Role for a cortical input to hippo-
campal area CA1 in the consolidation of a long-term memory.
Nature. 2004;431:699–703.
7 Anagnostaras SG, Gale GD, Fanselow MS. Hippocampus and
contextual fear conditioning: recent controversies and advances.
Hippocampus. 2001;11:8–17.
8 Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Lesions of the dorsal hippocampal
formation interfere with background but not foreground
contextual fear conditioning. Learn Mem. 1994;1:34–44.
9 Frankland PW, Cestari V, Filipkowski RK, McDonald RJ, Silva
AJ. The dorsal hippocampus is essential for context discrimina-
tion but not for contextual conditioning. Behav Neurosci.
1998;112:863–874.
10 Silva AJ, Giese KP. Plastic genes are in! Curr Opin Neurobiol.
1994;4:413–420.
11 Silva AJ, Kogan JH, Frankland PW, Kida S. CREB and
memory. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1998;21:127–148.
12 Lonze BE, Ginty DD. Function and regulation of CREB family
transcription factors in the nervous system. Neuron. 2002;35:
605–623.
13 Levenson JM, Sweatt JD. Epigenetic mechanisms in memory
formation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:108–118.
14 Bourtchuladze R, Frenguelli B, Blendy J, Cioffi D, Schutz G,
Silva AJ. Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted
mutation of the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell.
1994;79:59–68.
15 Hummler E, Cole TJ, Blendy JA, Ganss R, Aguzzi A, Schmid
W, et al. Targeted mutation of the CREB gene: compensation
within the CREB/ ATF family of transcription factors. Proc Natl
Acad Sci USA. 1994;91:5647–5651.
16 Kida S, Josselyn SA, de Ortiz SP, Kogan JH, Chevere I,
Masushige S, et al. CREB required for the stability of new and
reactivated fear memories. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:348–355.
17 Pittenger C, Huang YY, Paletzki RF, Bourtchouladze R, Scanlin
H, Vronskaya S, et al. Reversible inhibition of CREB/ ATF
transcription factors in region CA1 of the dorsal hippocampus
disrupts hippocampus-dependent spatial memory. Neuron. 2002;
34:447–462.
18 Soderling TR. The Ca
2+
-calmodulin-dependent protein kinase
cascade. Trends Biochem Sci. 1999;24:232–236.
19 Kang H, Sun LD, Atkins CM, Soderling TR, Wilson MA,
Tonegawa S. An important role of neural activity-dependent
CaMKIV signaling in the consolidation of long-term memory.
Cell. 2001;106:771–783.
20 Ho N, Liauw JA, Blaeser F, Wei F, Hanissian S, Muglia LM,
et al. Impaired synaptic plasticity and cAMP response element-
binding protein activation in Ca
2+
/calmodulin-dependent protein
kinase type IV/ Gr-deficient mice. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6459–
6472.
21 Wei F, Qiu CS, Liauw J, Robinson DA, Ho N, Chatila T, et al.
Calcium calmodulin-dependent protein kinase IV is required
for fear memory. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:573–579.
22 Limback-Stokin K, Korzus E, Nagaoka-Yasuda R, Mayford M.
Nuclear calcium/ calmodulin regulates memory consolidation.
J Neurosci. 2004;24:10858–10867.
23 Sweatt JD. Mitogen-activated protein kinases in synaptic
plasticity and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:311–317.
24 Kelleher RJ 3rd, Govindarajan A, Jung HY, Kang H, Tonegawa
S. Translational control by MAPK signaling in long-term
synaptic plasticity and memory. Cell. 2004;116:467–479.
25 Abel T, Nguyen PV, Barad M, Deuel TA, Kandel ER,
Bourtchouladze R. Genetic demonstration of a role for PKA in
the late phase of LTP and in hippocampus-based long-term
memory. Cell. 1997;88:615–626.
26 Johnson RS, Spiegelman BM, Papaioannou V. Pleiotropic
effects of a null mutation in the c-fos proto-oncogene. Cell.
1992;71:577–586.
27 Fleischmann A, Hvalby O, Jensen V, Strekalova T, Zacher C,
Layer LE, et al. Impaired long-term memory and NR2A-type
NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity in mice lacking
c-Fos in the CNS. J Neurosci. 2003;23:9116–9122.
28 Gass P, Fleischmann A, Hvalby O, Jensen V, Zacher C,
Strekalova T, et al. Mice with a fra-1 knock-in into the c-fos
locus show impaired spatial but regular contextual learning and
normal LTP. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2004;130:16–22.
29 Peters M, Mizuno K, Ris L, Angelo M, Godaux E, Giese KP.
Loss of Ca
2+
/calmodulin kinase kinase beta affects the formation
of some, but not all, types of hippocampus-dependent long-term
memory. J Neurosci. 2003;23:9752–9760.
30 Mizuno K, Peters M, Giese KP. The role of Ca
2+
/calmodulin-
dependent protein kinase kinases in learning and memory. Soc
Neuroscience. 2003;Abstract No. 836.1.
Distinct Hippocampal Memory Types 197
31 Richmond MA, Yee BK, Pouzet B, Veenman L, Rawlins JN,
Feldon J, et al. Dissociating context and space within the
hippocampus: effects of complete, dorsal, and ventral excito-
toxic hippocampal lesions on conditioned freezing and spatial
learning. Behav Neurosci. 1999;113:1189–1203.
32 Moser E, Moser MB, Andersen P. Spatial learning impairment
parallels the magnitude of dorsal hippocampal lesions, but is
hardly present following ventral lesions. J Neurosci.
1993;13:3916–3925.
33 Balschun D, Wolfer DP, Gass P, Mantamadiotis T, Welzl H,
Schutz G, et al. Does cAMP response element-binding protein
have a pivotal role in hippocampal synaptic plasticity and
hippocampus-dependent memory? J Neurosci. 2003;23:6304–
6314.
34 Korzus E, Rosenfeld MG, Mayford M. CBP histone acetyl-
transferase activity is a critical component of memory consolida-
tion. Neuron. 2004;42:961–972.
35 Morozov A, Muzzio IA, Bourtchouladze R, Van-Strien N,
Lapidus K, Yin D, et al. Rap1 couples cAMP signaling to a
distinct pool of p42/ 44MAPK regulating excitability, synaptic
plasticity, learning, and memory. Neuron. 2003;39:309–325.
36 Otto C, Kovalchuk Y, Wolfer DP, Gass P, Martin M, Zuschratter
W, et al. Impairment of mossy fiber long-term potentiation and
associative learning in pituitary adenylate cyclase activating
polypeptide type I receptor-deficient mice. J Neurosci.
2001;21:5520–5527.
37 Tao X, Finkbeiner S, Arnold DB, Shaywitz AJ, Greenberg ME.
Ca
2+
influx regulates BDNF transcription by a CREB family
transcription factor-dependent mechanism. Neuron. 1998;20:
709–726.
38 Yamada K, Mizuno M, Nabeshima T. Role for brain-derived
neurotrophic factor in learning and memory. Life Sci. 2002;70:
735–744.
39 Tyler WJ, Alonso M, Bramham CR, Pozzo-Miller LD. From
acquisition to consolidation: on the role of brain-derived
neurotrophic factor signaling in hippocampal-dependent learn-
ing. Learn Mem. 2002;9:224–237.
40 Gorski JA, Balogh SA, Wehner JM, Jones KR. Learning deficits
in forebrain-restricted brain-derived neurotrophic factor mutant
mice. Neuroscience. 2003;121:341–354.
41 Levenson JM, Choi S, Lee SY, Cao YA, Ahn HJ, Worley KC,
et al. A bioinformatics analysis of memory consolidation
reveals involvement of the transcription factor c-rel. J Neurosci.
2004;24:3933–3943.
... Previous studies demonstrated that the protein kinase A or Ca 2+ /calmodulin kinase pathways may be involved in CREB phosphorylation, which enhances c-Fos expression. 44,45 The number of capsaicin-induced c-Fos and p-ERK immunolabeled neuronal profiles in the Vo was significantly increased on both sides after LIN-TS or IAN-TS. This increase may be attributed to changes in neuronal excitability. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Sequential changes in brainstem and spinal cord neurons after traumatic injury to peripheral nerves are related to neuropathic pain symptoms. Purpose This study was conducted to elucidate the influence of nerve insult on stimulus-induced c-Fos expression and ERK phosphorylation by brainstem neurons. Methods The brainstem trigeminal sensory nuclear complex (BTSNC) was examined for neuronal profiles immunolabeled with c-Fos and phosphorylated ERK (p-ERK) antibodies elicited by stimulation of the tongue with capsaicin after lingual or inferior alveolar nerve (IAN) injury. Results Abundant neuronal profiles immunolabeled for c-Fos and p-ERK elicited by capsaicin were distributed in the spinal trigeminal nucleus caudalis (Vc) without nerve injury. The spinal trigeminal nucleus oralis (Vo) contained limited numbers of these neuronal profiles after stimulation of the tongue. A significant reduction of these neuronal profiles in the ipsilateral Vc was detected after lingual nerve injury. After IAN injury, an increased number of neuronal profiles immunolabeled for c-Fos elicited by capsaicin was noted, while that of p-ERK was left unchanged in the ipsilateral Vc. On the both sides of the Vo, an increased number of capsaicin-induced neuronal profiles immunolabeled for c-Fos and p-ERK was detected after lingual or IAN injury. Conclusion Differential effects of lingual or IAN injury on stimulus-induced c-Fos expression and ERK phosphorylation by Vo and Vc neurons may be involved in the complex nature of symptoms of trigeminal neuralgia.
... CREB plays a role in the regulation of short-and long-term memory formation (Bourtchuladze et al., 1994). It is essential to the formation of spatial memory (Mizuno and Giese, 2005) and plays a positive role in axonal transport and synaptic plasticity (Abel and Nguyen, 2008;Lin and Holt, 2008). In the adult brain, CREB is involved in learning, memory, and neuronal plasticity. ...
Article
Full-text available
HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) remain an unsolved problem that persists despite the use of antiretroviral therapy. We have obtained data showing that HIV-gp120 protein contributes to neurodegeneration through metabolic reprogramming. This led to decreased ATP levels, lower mitochondrial DNA copy numbers, and loss of mitochondria cristae, all-important for mitochondrial biogenesis. gp120 protein also disrupted mitochondrial movement and synaptic plasticity. Searching for the mechanisms involved, we found that gp120 alters the cyclic AMP response element-binding protein (CREB) phosphorylation on serine residue 133 necessary for its function as a transcription factor. Since CREB regulates the promoters of PGC1 and BDNF genes, we found that CREB dephosphorylation causes PGC1 and BDNF loss of functions. Our data was validated in vitro and in vivo. The negative effect of gp120 was alleviated in cells and animals in the presence of rolipram that inhibits the phosphodiesterase protein 4 (PDE4) and restores CREB phosphorylation. We concluded that HIV-gp120 protein contributes to HAND via inhibition of CREB protein function.
... Indeed, several pathways of communication between the gut and the brain exist: through the neuronal, endocrine or immune systems 2,58-60 . In rodents, it has been demonstrated that alterations of gut microbiota were correlated with changes in brain-derived neurotropic factor and c-fos proteins concentrations, both known for their implications in memory functions [61][62][63] . In pigs as well, supplementation for a 28-day period with a higher crude protein diet showed an increase in dopamine concentration in the brain stem 64 . ...
Article
Full-text available
The influence of feed supplements on behavior and memory has been recently studied in livestock. The objectives of the study were to evaluate the effects of a synbiotic on: an episodic-like (SOR: Spontaneous Object Recognition), a working (BARR: Fence barrier task), a long-term (TMAZE: Spatial T-maze task) memory test and on gut microbiota composition. Eighteen female piglets were supplemented from 1 to 28 days of age with a synbiotic (SYN), while 17 served as control (CTL). Feces were collected on days 16, 33 and 41 for 16S rRNA gene composition analyses. In the SOR, SYN piglets interacted more quickly with the novel object than CTL piglets. In the BARR, SYN piglets had shorter distances to finish the test in trial 3. In the TMAZE, SYN piglets were quicker to succeed on specific days and tended to try the new rewarded arm earlier during the reversal stage. Difference of microbiota composition between treatments was nonexistent on D16, a tendency on D33 and significant on D41. The synbiotic supplement may confer memory advantages in different cognitive tasks, regardless of the nature of the reward and the memory request. Difference in memory abilities can potentially be explained by differences in microbiota composition.
... That would be more in line with the idea that MWM performance and hippocampal LTP are normal in most Ras-GRF1 mutant lines (Brambilla's and GENA53 are normal in MWM, Brambilla's and Giese's have normal LTP) and that MWM is also normal in a Ras-GRF1 over-expressing line, whose behavioral characterization has already been reported (Fasano et al., 2009). However, it is also important to consider the fact that the hippocampal circuitry and/or the hippocampal signaling mechanisms involved in contextual fear conditioning may be distinct from those used in spatial learning, leaving the possibility that other components of the hippocampal formation may be specifically impaired in fear learning in the Ras-GRF1 deficient mice, as previously suggested for other mutant lines (Mizuno and Giese, 2005) or based on lesion studies (Phillips and LeDoux, 1992, 1994, 1995. ...
Chapter
Issue included in: Neuronal Cell Signaling and Behavior (2013) Editors: Riccardo Brambilla and Gilberto Fisone ISBN: 978-2-88919-082-9 ISSN: 1664-8714 DOI: 10.3389/978-2-88919-082-9
... CREB plays a role in the regulation of short-and long-term memory formation (16). It is essential to the formation of spatial memory (17) and plays a positive role in axonal transport and synaptic plasticity (18,19). CREB protects the neurons through the regulation of the PGC-1, Bcl-2, and BDNF promoters. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) remains an unsolved problem in the clinical management of HIV-1 carriers, because existing anti-retroviral therapy while suppressing viral replication, do not prevent neurocognitive impairment (e.g. spatial memory loss). HIV-1 gp120 protein has been proposed to contribute to HAND because it is shed by infected cells and the use of antibodies revealed its presence in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) even in the combinatory antiretroviral therapy (cART) era. The cyclic AMP response element-binding protein (CREB) has long been known to be a star player in memory. CREB exerts its effect partially through regulating the genes for peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor gamma coactivator (PGC)-1α and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). CREB, PGC-1α, and BDNF levels are low in the brains of patients with neurodegenerative diseases and a dearth of either protein is associated with cognitive decline. We have obtained data showing that gp120 contributes to neurodegeneration by altering CREB phosphorylation on serine residue 133 thus disrupting mitochondrial movement and synaptic plasticity leading to spatial memory loss. Inhibition of CREB function was also associated with a decrease of ATP levels and lower mitochondrial DNA copy numbers. Our data was validated in vitro (primary mouse neurons and neuronal cell line, SH-SY5Y) and in vivo (gp120-tg mice and mice injected with gp120). The negative effect of gp120 was alleviated in cells and animals in the presence of Rolipram. Hence, we conclude that HIV-1 gp120 protein contributes to spatial memory impairment via inhibition of CREB protein activity.
... Interestingly, the regulation of processes involved in learning and memory, such as protein synthesis, may vary across the day and night (Aten et al., 2018;Frank, 2016;Gerstner & Yin, 2010;Jilg et al., 2010;Kim et al., 2019;Nakanishi et al., 1997;Ramm & Smith, 1990;Rawashdeh, Jilg, Maronde, Fahrenkrug, & Stehle, 2016;Snider, Sullivan, & Obrietan, 2018). For example, hippocampal cAMP levels as well as the phosphorylation of mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK), and cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) show daily oscillations, which may suggest circadian regulation of cAMP/MAPK/CREB pathways (Eckel-Mahan et al., 2008;Mizuno & Giese, 2005;Rawashdeh et al., 2016;Snider et al., 2018;Trifilieff et al., 2006). Importantly, the regulation of mammalian target of rapamycin complex 1 (mTORC1) signaling pathway, which is key for protein synthesis by initiating translation, also shows daily oscillations (Jouffe et al., 2013;Robles, Humphrey, & Mann, 2017;Saraf, Luo, Morris, & Storm, 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
It is widely acknowledged that de novo protein synthesis is crucial for the formation and consolidation of long‐term memories. While the basal activity of many signaling cascades that modulate protein synthesis fluctuate in a circadian fashion, it is unclear whether the temporal dynamics of protein synthesis‐dependent memory consolidation varies depending on the time of day. More specifically, it is unclear whether protein synthesis inhibition affects hippocampus‐dependent memory consolidation in rodents differentially across the day (i.e., the inactive phase with an abundance of sleep) and night (i.e., the active phase with little sleep). To address this question, male and female C57Bl6/J mice were trained in a contextual fear conditioning task at the beginning or the end of the light phase. Animals received a single systemic injection with the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin or vehicle directly, 4 hours, 8 hours, or 11.5 hours following training, and memory was assessed after 24 hours. Here, we show that protein synthesis inhibition impaired the consolidation of context‐fear memories selectively when the protein synthesis inhibitor was administered at the first three time points, irrespective of timing of training. Even though the basal activity of signaling pathways regulating de novo protein synthesis may fluctuate across the 24h cycle, these results suggest that the temporal dynamics of protein synthesis‐dependent memory consolidation are similar for day‐time and night‐time learning.
Article
Increasing evidence indicates that glyphosate (GlyP)-based herbicides (GBHs) induce developmental neurotoxicity. The present study investigated the developmental exposure effect of GlyP and GBH on hippocampal neurogenesis in rats. Dams were treated from gestational day 6 to day 21 post-delivery on weaning with a diet containing 1.5% or 3.0% GlyP or drinking water with 1.0% GBH (containing 0.36% GlyP). Dams in the 1.5%-GlyP, 3.0%-GlyP, and GBH groups received 1.04, 2.16, and 0.25 g GlyP/kg body weight (BW)/day during gestation, and 2.27, 4.65, and 0.58 g GlyP/kg BW/day during lactation, respectively. On weaning, 3.0% GlyP- and GBH-exposed offspring decreased the BW, and the latter also decreased the brain weight. Both compounds suppressed neural progenitor cell proliferation in the neurogenic niche, and GlyP-exposed offspring showed a decreased number of TUBB3⁺ immature granule cells. In contrast, both compounds increased the number of ARC⁺ granule cells, suggesting increased synaptic plasticity. Both compounds downregulated antioxidant genes (Cat and Sod2) in the dentate gyrus, suggestive of increased sensitivity to oxidative stress, which might be related to the suppression of neurogenesis. At the adult age, GBH alone sustained decreases in body and brain weights. Both compounds increased hippocampal malondialdehyde levels and upregulated Cat in the dentate gyrus, suggesting induction of oxidative stress. Both compounds upregulated Casp9, and GBH increased neural progenitor cell apoptosis, suggesting disruption of neurogenesis related to oxidative stress. GBH increased the number of COX2⁺ granule cells, and both compounds upregulated Arc, suggesting increased synaptic plasticity. These results suggest that GlyP and GBH might cause similar effects on disruption of neurogenesis accompanying compensatory responses and induction of oxidative stress responses through the adult age in the hippocampus. However, effects on adult age were more evident with GBH, suggesting that the surfactants contained in GBH might have contributed to the enhanced neurotoxicity of GlyP, similar to the enhanced general toxicity.
Article
The study used the method of cobalt epilepsy, which allows rats with long-term implanted electrodes in the cortical and subcortical structures of the brain to monitor the dynamics of the formation and migration of Epi-foci for a long time. It was found that in the control at the 1st stage of development of the Epi system, Epi activity is most pronounced in the electrocorticograms of the ipsilateral cortex, and at the 2nd, stable stage of development of the Epi system – in the contralateral cortex and subcortical structures. The compound GIZH-290 (the original structural analogue of levetiracetam) reduces the number of Epi discharges and their duration at the 2nd, stable stage of the development of the Epi system. The target structure of the GIZH – 290 compound was the hippocampus. The compound GIZH -290 selectively statistically significantly reduces both the number and duration of Epi – discharges only in the hippocampus and does not affect the foci of epileptic activity in the ipsi-and contralateral cortex and hypothalamus.
Article
Exercise is recognized to increase the expression of neurotrophic genes in the hippocampus and prevent cognitive impairment. Histone deacetylase (HDAC) inhibitor acetylate histones and enhance gene transcription in epigenetic regulation. HDAC inhibitors are expected to be an efficacious pharmacological treatment for cognitive function. This study aimed to examine the effect of HDAC inhibitors and exercise on epigenetic markers and neurotrophic gene expression in the hippocampus to find a more enriched brain conditioning for cognitive function based on the synergic effects of pharmacological treatment and behavioral therapy. Thirteen-week-old male mice were divided into four groups. Intraperitoneal administration of an HDAC inhibitor (1.2 g/kg sodium butyrate, NaB) and treadmill exercise (approximately 10 m/min for 60 min) were performed 5 days a week for 4 weeks. NaB administration increased the expression of an immediate-early gene, a neurotrophin, and a neurotrophin receptor in the hippocampus. These results indicate that HDAC inhibition could present an enriched platform for neuronal plasticity in the hippocampus and cognitive function. The novel object recognition test showed that NaB administration increased the score. Notably, the step-through passive avoidance test showed improved learning and memory in the presence of exercise and exercise, indicating that the mice acquired fear memory, specifically in the presence of NaB administration plus exercise. This study found that repetitive administration of HDAC inhibitors improved cognitive function and HDAC inhibitor administration plus exercise has a synergic effect on learning and memory, accompanying the enhancement of crucial gene transcriptions for neuronal plasticity in the hippocampus.
Article
Astragaloside IV (AS-IV), a natural product derived from Radix Astragali (Astragalus membranaceus), is beneficial for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), but the mechanisms underlying this benefit are not completely understood. Peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor gamma (PPARγ) and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) are potential therapeutic targets for AD. In this study, we found that amyloid β protein fragment 1–42 oligomers (AβO) suppressed BDNF and PPARγ expression, and inhibited tyrosine receptor kinase B (TrkB) phosphorylation in cultured hippocampal neurons; these changes were ameliorated by treatment with AS-IV. Inhibition of PPARγ by genetic and pharmacological methods also blocked the effect of AS-IV on BDNF expression in AβO-treated cells. Importantly, exogenous BDNF protected against neurotoxicity and apoptosis induced by AβO, whereas inhibition of PPARγ reversed protective effects of AS-IV against these outcomes. In vivo data further revealed that AS-IV improved AβO-induced memory impairment and reduced apoptosis of hippocampal neurons. Moreover, AS-IV suppressed the AβO-induced reduction in BDNF by promoting PPARγ activation in the hippocampus. Taken together, these results indicate that AS-IV prevents AβO-induced memory impairment and hippocampal neuronal apoptosis, probably by promoting the PPARγ/BDNF signaling pathway.
Article
Full-text available
Dorsal hippocampal (DH) lesions produce a severe deficit in recently, but not remotely, acquired contextual fear without impairing memory of discrete training stimuli, i.e., DH lesions produce an anterograde and time-limited retrograde amnesia specific to contextual memory. These data are consistent with the standard model which posits temporary involvement of the hippocampus in recent memory maintenance. However, three recent controversies apparently weaken the case for a selective mnemonic role for the hippocampus in contextual fear. First, although retrograde amnesia (from posttraining lesions) is severe, anterograde amnesia (from pretraining lesions) may be mild or nonexistent. Second, a performance, rather than mnemonic, account of contextual freezing deficits in hippocampal-lesioned animals has been offered. Third, damage to the entire hippocampus, including the ventral hippocampus, can produce a dramatic and temporally stable disruption of context and tone fear. These data are reviewed and explanations are offered as to why they do not necessarily challenge the standard model of hippocampal memory function in contextual fear. Finally, a more complete description of the hippocampus' proposed role in contextual fear is offered, along with new data supporting this view. In summary, the data support a specific mnemonic role for the DH in the acquisition and consolidation of contextual representations. Hippocampus 2001;11:8–17.
Article
Full-text available
The cAMP responsive element binding protein (CREB) is a nuclear protein that modulates the transcription of genes with cAMP responsive elements in their promoters. Increases in the concentration of either calcium or cAMP can trigger the phosphorylation and activation of CREB. This transcription factor is a component of intracellular signaling events that regulate a wide range of biological functions, from spermatogenesis to circadian rhythms and memory. Here we review the key features of CREB-dependent transcription, as well as the involvement of CREB in memory formation. Evidence from Aplysia, Drosophila, mice, and rats shows that CREB-dependent transcription is required for the cellular events underlying long-term but not short-term memory. While the work in Aplysia and Drosophila only involved CREB function in very simple forms of conditioning, genetic and pharmacological studies in mice and rats demonstrate that CREB is required for a variety of complex forms of memory, including spatial and social learning, thus indicating that CREB may be a universal modulator of processes required for memory formation.
Article
Full-text available
Electrophysiological studies have shown that single cells in the hippocampus respond during spatial learning and exploration1-4, some firing only when animals enter specific and restricted areas of a familiar environment. Deficits in spatial learning and memory are found after lesions of the hippocampus and its extrinsic fibre connections5,6 following damage to the medial septal nucleus which successfully disrupts the hippocampal theta rhythm7, and in senescent rats which also show a correlated reduction in synaptic enhancement on the perforant path input to the hippocampus8. We now report, using a novel behavioural procedure requiring search for a hidden goal, that, in addition to a spatial discrimination impairment, total hippocampal lesions also cause a profound and lasting placenavigational impairment that can be dissociated from correlated motor, motivational and reinforcement aspects of the procedure.
Article
Rats with complete excitotoxic hippocampal lesions or selective damage to the dorsal or ventral hippocampus were compared with controls on measures of contextually conditioned freezing in a signaled shock procedure and on a spatial water-maze task. Complete and ventral lesions produced equivalent, significant anterograde deficits in conditioned freezing relative to both dorsal lesions and controls. Complete hippocampal lesions impaired water-maze performance; in contrast, ventral lesions improved performance relative to the dorsal group, which was itself unexpectedly unimpaired relative to controls. Thus, the partial lesion effects seen in the 2 tasks never resembled each other. Anterograde impairments in contextual freezing and spatial learning do not share a common underlying neural basis; complete and ventral lesions may induce anterograde contextual freezing impairments by enhancing locomotor activity under conditions of mild stress.
Article
The cAMP-responsive element-binding protein (CREB) has been implicated in the activation of protein synthesis required for long-term facilitation, a cellular model of memory in Aplysia. Our studies with fear conditioning and with the water maze show that mice with a targeted disruption of the alpha and delta isoforms of CREB are profoundly deficient in long-term memory. In contrast, short-term memory, lasting between 30 and 60 min, is normal. Consistent with models claiming a role for long-term potentiation (LTP) in memory, LTP in hippocampal slices from CREB mutants decayed to baseline 90 min after tetanic stimulation. However, paired-pulse facilitation and posttetanic potentiation are normal. These results implicate CREB-dependent transcription in mammalian long-term memory.
Article
The c-fos proto-oncogene has been implicated as a central regulatory component of the nuclear response to mitogens and other extracellular stimuli. Embryonic stem cells targeted at the c-fos locus have been used to generate chimeric mice that have transmitted the mutated allele through the germline. Homozygous mutants show reduced placental and fetal weights and significant loss of viability at birth. Approximately 40% of the homozygous mutants survive and grow at normal rates until severe osteopetrosis, characterized by foreshortening of the long bones, ossification of the marrow space, and absence of tooth eruption, begins to develop at approximately 11 days. Among other abnormalities, these mice show delayed or absent gametogenesis, lymphopenia, and altered behavior. Despite these defects, many live as long as their wild-type or heterozygous littermates (currently 7 months). These data indicate that c-fos is not required for the growth of most cell types but is involved in the development and function of several distinct tissues.