ArticlePDF Available

Relationship between academic pRocRastinaton and self-contRol: the mediational Role of self-esteem

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The present study examined the mediational effect of self-esteem on the relationship between self-control and academic procrastination. The sample consisted of 426 emerging adults (university students; 218 female, 208 male; M age = 21.3 years old, SD = 1.5) who completed reliable and valid academic procrastination, self-control, and self-esteem measures. Results revealed that academic procrastination was negatively predicted by self-esteem, and self-control. Moreover, findings suggested that self-esteem mediated the association of academic procrastination with self-control. Results are discussed in terms of prevention and intervention implications.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Relationship between academic
pRocRastinaton and self-contRol: the
mediational Role of self-esteem

Bahcesehir University, Department of Psychological Counseling and Guidance

Australian College of Applied Psychology

DePaul University
    
      
        
    M     SD    
       

   
    



Procrastination    

 

  
    
     
    
       
     
     
      
    
      
    
    
   

      

    
     
     
  
 
  
academic
procrastination   

 

    
      
     
2 / College Student Journal
to academic procrastination in a university
demonstrating that academic procrastination
is closely related to lower levels of self-con-
trol (Digdon & Howell, 2008), self-efficacy
(Klassen et al., 2008) and academic perfor-
mance with a concurrent increase in anxiety
levels (Chabaud, Ferrand, & Maury, 2010).
Researchers have focused on various
aspects of academic procrastination includ-
ing cognitive and behavioral components
(Kachgal, Hansen, & Kevin, 2001; Schow-
uenburg et al., 2004). Studies based on the
cognitive perspective investigated why stu-
dents make the conscious decision to delay al-
though it has negative consequences (Ferrari,
2010; Karas & Spada, 2009). This line of re-
search investigated the relationship between
self-esteem and academic procrastination
(e.g., Tice, 1991). Self-esteem is used as a per-
sonality (Brown & Marshall, 2006) variable
representing the way people generally feel
about themselves. One of the most popular
theories concerning the etiology of procrasti-
nation conceptualizes chronic procrastination
as a method of self-protection for fragile
self-esteem (Burka & Yuen, 1983; Ferrari &
Tibbett, 2017). Theory suggests performance
reflects ability, which is also a reflection of
self-worth. This assertion would then lead to
a link between performance, ability and self-
worth; hence, failure at a task may become an
indicator for lack of ability and low self-worth
in the absence of procrastination or self-hand-
icapping. Therefore, in an academic setting,
students may develop a fear of failure, fueling
their academic procrastination behaviours
because of the strong emphasis placed on
success when defining self-worth. As such,
academic procrastination may serve a protec-
tive function reinforcing the behaviour: since
performance has been impaired by time con-
straints, performance does not equal ability
and therefore does not equal self worth. In this
way, academic procrastination serves as an
ego defensive mechanism; therefore, it may
be used frequently as a protective device by
those with fragile self-esteem (Burka & Yuen,
1983). In this respect, numerous studies found
a significant inverse relationship between
procrastination and self-esteem (e.g., Ferrari,
1994; Uzun Ozer, Demir, & Harrington, 2012
such that low self-esteem and weak self-effi-
cacy are related to frequent academic-related
delays (Beck, Koons, & Milgrim, 2000; Fer-
rari, 1994; Sirois, 2004) whereby, feelings of
worthlessness cause task avoidance that may
result in failure (Ferrari, 2010). In this vein,
the findings of the previous research demon-
strate that student self-esteem may be a strong
predictor of academic-related task delays and
may serve a mediating role between academic
procrastination and other variables.
Some researchers investigating be-
havioural aspects of academic procrastina-
tion (Ferrari & Tice, 2000; Beck et al., 2000;
Van Eerde, 2003) argued that self-control
is one of the strongest behavioral predic-
tors of all forms of procrastination. Broadly
speaking, self-control refers to the capacity
to modify and adapt the self in order to op-
timize the fit between self and the external
environment (e.g., Rothbaum, Weisz, &
Snyder, 1982). Specifically, Vohs and Bau-
meister (2004) identified four key domains
of self-control which include the effective
control of thoughts, emotions, impulses, and
performance. Therefore, self-control involves
the ability to restrain oneself from engaging
in behaviors that are known to have negative
implications (e.g. smoking, purchasing be-
havior, breaking laws or procrastinating; e.g.,
Faber & Vohs, 2004). Ferrari (2010) suggests
procrastination is a ‘self-control failure of
performance’ (p. 391) in which procrastina-
tors fail to regulate their performance in situa-
tions of stress and high cognitive loads. Thus,
the process of self-control involves making
a comparison of one’s behavioral state to
an ideal or standard of behavior (Duval &
Wicklund, 1972). Concordantly, researchers
A Mediational Model of Academic Procrastination / 3
(e.g., Klassen, et al., 2008; Howell & Watson,
2007) believe procrastinators have problems
regulating their behaviors and are engaging
in undercontrol. Relatedly, a metanalysis
(Ferrari, 2010) investigating cause and effect,
determined chronic everyday procrastination
was strongly predicted by trait characteristics
such as self-control (i.e. distractibility, organi-
zation, and achievement) and impulsiveness.
Thus, a strong body of evidence supports the
strong inverse relationship between self-con-
trol and forms of procrastination, suggesting
lower levels of self-control are related to
higher levels of academic procrastination
(e.g., Uzun Ozer, et. al., 2014).
These facts demonstrate the need for
close examination of academic procrastina-
tion among emerging adults (i.e., university
students) because of its frequency and debil-
itating impact. Yet, detailed models explain-
ing the contributing dynamics of academic
procrastination are limited; thus, numerous
questions remain with regards to root causes
and operating mechanisms. In this study we
explored causal correlates, putting forth a pre-
liminary model investigating the relationship
between academic procrastination, self-es-
teem and self-control in greater depth.
Taken together, low self-esteem exists in
the presence of low self-control may serve
as a powerful mediator between maladaptive
behaviours that increase the frequency of
academic procrastination among emerging
adults in university settings. Thus, the current
study built upon existing literature by pro-
viding a better understanding of the link be-
tween self-esteem, self-control and academic
procrastination.
Considering previous research (see Fer-
rari, 2010; Ferrari & Emmons, 1995; Ferrari
& Tibbett, 2017), it was hypothesized that
self-control would be negatively correlated
with academic procrastination. Similarly,
self-esteem was hypothesized to be negatively
associated with academic procrastination and
positively related to self-control, suggesting
that students with lower levels of self-esteem
will have lower levels of self-control.
Method
Participants
Data were collected from undergraduate
students enrolled in various departments at a
major-state funded, urban university in Tur-
key. For the sample selection, proportional
sampling created five subgroups from five
faculties selected for the sample in the same
proportion, as they exist in the general pop-
ulation. The sample of 426 undergraduate
students included 218 female and 208 male
students. The participants consisted of 107
first year students, 78 sophomore, 160 junior,
and 81 senior students. The mean age of our
emerging adult participants was 21.3 years
old (SD = 1.5) with an age range between 18
and 25.
Data Collection Instruments
In the present study, variables were as-
sessed utilizing self-report reliable and valid
questionnaires, namely.
Tuckman procrastination scale (TPS;
Tuckman, 1991) assesses college students’
academic procrastination tendencies. It is 16
16 items reliable (α = 0.89; Tuckman, 2007)
and valid (r = -.47 with the General Self-Ef-
ficacy Scale) and demonstrated high internal
consistency (0.88), retest stability (0.82).
In the Turkish adaptation study, Uzun Özer,
Sackes, and Tuckman (2013) found the scale
to be reliable and valid (α = 0.90; r = 22 with
self-efficacy and r = 23 with self-esteem).
Self Control Schedule (SCS; Rosenbaum,
1980) assessed students’ tendencies to exert
self-control methods to resolve behavioral
problems. The scale consists of 36 items.
In the Rosenbaum’s study, an alpha coeffi-
cient was computed on six different samples
ranging from 0.78 to 0.86, indicating a high
4 / College Student Journal
internal consistency among items. The SCS’s
evidence for validity was provided by correla-
tions with Croskey’s Measure of Communi-
cation Apprehension (r = -.37; Rosenbaum,
1980). In a Turkish adaptation study, Siva
(1991) found the scale to be reliable and valid
= 0.79; test-retest r = 0.80; r = -22 with
locus of control).
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES;
Rosenberg, 1965) assessed participants’ glob-
al self-esteem or self-worth. It is a 10 items
scale demonstrating high internal consistency
= .80). Evidence for convergent validity
was provided by positive correlations with the
Copersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (r = .60)
and the Health Self Image Questionnaire (r
= .83). In a Turkish adaptation studies, Celik
(2004) reported the scale to be reliable and
valid (α = 0.84; r = -22 with locus of control).
Procedures
Demographic questionnaires including an
explanation of the study and a set of scales
were administered to the participants during
regular classroom hours. After obtaining per-
missions from the Human Research Ethical
Committee and the instructor of each class,
volunteer students were asked to respond to
the scales.
Results
To test our hypothesized model, the
mediational role of self-esteem on the rela-
tionship between self-control and procras-
tination, a confirmatory factor analysis was
first conducted to ensure the model was an
acceptable fit to the data. Then the structural
model was tested to assess the hypothesized
relationship. The Maximum Likelihood
method in AMOS 25 (Byrne, 2016) was
utilized to examine the measurement and
structural model. Four indecies were used to
assess the goodness of fit of the models on
the basis of the traditional crriteria for good
fit: the comparative fit index, the incremental
fit index, the standardized root mean square
residual, and the root mean square error of
approximation (Hu & Bentler, 1999).
Latent Variables
To test the model, indicators of the la-
tent construct were observed. The observed
indicators of the latent variables of procras-
tination, self-control and self-esteem were
parceled following the recommendation of
Russell, Kahn, Spoth, and Althmaier (1998).
Four parcels were created for each scale items
seperately. Item parcelling was chosen for the
variables in order to reduce the number of
parameters that would result from using indi-
vidual items thereby improving the estimation
of the effect (Russell et al., 1998). Bandalos
(2008) and Nasser and Wisenbaker (2003)
also asserted that parcel scores are more like-
ly to be distributed normally than those of sin-
gle items. Thus, ‘the resulting reduction in the
complexity of measurement models should
lead to more realistic models’ (Nasser and
Wisenbaker, 2003; p.730). The correlations
among the study variables were small to mod-
erate in magnitude (r = -.20 and r = -.48). As
expected, self-esteem and self-control were
negatively associated with procrastination.
Measurement Model
A test of the measurement model resulted
in a good fit to the data, scaled X2 (50, N =
426) = 134.67, p < 0.001, CFI = .96, IFI = .96,
RMSEA = .63. All of the variables were sig-
nificantly loaded on their perspectives latent
variables and appear to have been adequately
measured by their perspective indicators.
Structural Model
The structural model used to test the
hypothesis (see Figure 1) showed an excel-
lent fit to the data, scaled X2 (50, N = 426)
= 102.98, p < 0.001, CFI = .97, IFI = .97,
RMSEA = .50. Self-control and self-esteem
predicted procrastination. Seven percent of
A Mediational Model of Academic Procrastination / 5
the variance in self-esteem and 46% of the
variance in procrastination was accounted
for in the model. Thus, the hypothesized
model was the best fitting model and used in
the subsequent boostrapping and invariance
testing analysis.
Boostraping
Significant levels of indirect effects for the
meditational model were tested by following
the boostrapt procedure recommended by
Shrout and Bolger (2002). Analysis showed
that indirect effects were normally distributed.
The indirect effects specified in the hypothe-
ses were estimated via bootstrapping (set at
1000), and bias corrected bootstrap (BC) and
95% confidence intervals were requested.
The bootstrap values provided significant me-
diated paths from self-control through self-es-
teem to procrastination (β = .31, p < .01).
Discussion
The present study investigated the media-
tional affects of self esteem in the relationship
between self-control and academic procrasti-
nation. Results showed both self-esteem and
self control negatively predicted academic
procrastination, indicating lower levels
of self-esteem and self-control predicted
higher levels of academic procrastination.
Self-control was also a significant predictor
of self-esteem suggesting an indirect effect
on academic procrastination via self-esteem.
Results demonstrated that the hypothesized
relationship was well supported using the cur-
rent data. The findings, both statistically and
theoretically, supported the important effect
of self-esteem in the relationship between
self-control and academic procrastination
among emerging adults.
Figure 1. Standardized parameter estimates of the structural model. Ps are the parcels
for latent variables. Numbers next to arrows indicate the relationships between the latent
and measured variables.
6 / College Student Journal
The present findings suggest individuals’
cognitions about themselves lead to their
behavioral control, which in turn directly pre-
dicted procrastination. Specifically, findings
revealed that students’ low levels of self-es-
teem led to decrease self-control, which re-
sulted in an increase in procrastination. The
results were consistent with the previous find-
ings demonstrating a relationship between
self-esteem and self-control (Steel, Brothen,
& Wambach, 2001; Tice & Bratslavsky,
2000). The findings also supported the theory
that contingencies of self-worth may serve
a self-regulatory function. Accordingly, in-
creased self-worth in an academic setting may
serve to enhance self-control, which in turn
decreases academic procrastination (Roth-
man, Baldwin, & Hertel., 2004).
Another important finding of the present
study may be the behavioral control features
of self-control. As suggested earlier (e.g.,
Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2004), self-esteem
mechanisms may provide students with con-
trolling thoughts, feelings and behavior. In
line with self-control theory, it has been seen
as willpower or effortful control in cogni-
tive-affective processing systems, which is a
dynamic of delay of gratification (Mischel &
Ayduk, 2004).
Several implications may be drawn from
the findings of the present study for counsel-
ors and educators. The present study explored
the relationships between behavioral factors
such as self-control and its ability to mediate
personal cognitive factors among a represen-
tative sample of university students enrolled
in various departments at a major state funded
university. Therefore, the present study has
the potential to generate meaningful infor-
mation for understanding student procrasti-
nation associated with factors contributing
to cognitive and behavioral patterns. Hence,
the results of the present study may provide
valuable cues for both university counselors
and staff to develop new programs that may
reduce the negative effects of procrastination.
From an applied standpoint, the current
findings suggest interventions targeting both
the cognitive and behavioral components of
procrastination may be effective. In support,
interventions focused on enhancing self-con-
trol by creating routines, timetables and
predetermined activities have demonstrated
efficacy (Ferrari, 2010).
From a cognitive behavioral perspective,
cognitive restructuring, combined with be-
havioural experiments, has demonstrated
effictiveness and has been associated with
concurrent increases in self-esteem (Van
Eerde, 2003). In conclusion, taking an idio-
graphic approach, combining behavioral and
cognitive components and exploring alterna-
tive methods of delivery, may be a promising
avenue for future research, as they are theo-
retically supported and adventageous from
a practical standpoint, due to scalability and
cost effectiveness.
A limitation of the current study was the
use of a homogenous, university sample.
Further research with larger and more de-
mographically diverse populations would
strengthen the findings of the current study.
Therefore, it is suggested to conduct future
studies with samples from different universi-
ties and in different regions.
A Mediational Model of Academic Procrastination / 7
References
Bandalos, D. L. (2002). Is parcelling really necessary?
A comparison of results from item parcelling
and categorical variable methodology. Structural
Equation Modeling, 15, 211-240. Doi: 10.1207/
S15328007SEM0901_5
Beck, B. L., Koons, S. R., & Milgrim, D. L. (2000).
Correlates and consequences of behavioral procras-
tination: The effects of academic procrastination,
self-consciousness, self-esteem and self-handicap-
ping. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality,
15(5), 3-13.
Brown, J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2006). The three faces
of self-esteem. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Self-esteem: Is-
sues and answers (pp. 4-9). New York: Psychology
Press.
Burka, J. B., & Yuen, L. M. (1983). Procrastination: Why
do you do it, what to do about it. CA: Addison- Wes-
ley Publishing Company.
Byrne, B. M. (2016). Structural equation modeling with
AMOS : Basic concepts, applications, and program-
ming (3rd edition). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chabaud, P., Ferrand, C., & Maury, J. (2010). Individual
diffrerences in undergraduate student athletes: The
roles of perfectionism and trait anxiety perception of
procrastination behavior. Social Behavior & Person-
ality: An International Journal, 38(8), 1041-1056.
Digdon, N. L., & Howell, A. J. (2008). College students
who have an eveningness preference report lower
self-control and greater procrastination. Chronobi-
ology International: The Journal of Biological &
Medical Rhythm Research, 25(6), 1029-1046.
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objec-
tive self-awareness. NY: Academic Press.
Ellis, A., & Knaus, W. (1977). Overcoming procrastina-
tion. NY: Signet.
Faber, R. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). To buy or not to buy?
Self control and self-control failure in purshase be-
havior. In R. F. Baumesister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.),
Handbook of self-control: Research, theory and ap-
plications. NY: The Guilford Press.
Ferrari, J. R. (1994). Dysfunctional procrastination and
Its relationship with self-esteem, interpersonal de-
pendency, and self-defeating behaviors. Personality
and Individual Differences, 17(5), 673-679.
Ferrari, J. R., & Emmons, R. A. (1995). Methods of
procrastination and their relation to self-control and
self-reinforcement: An exploratory study. Journal of
Social Behavior and Personality, 10(1), 135.
Ferrari, J.R. (2010). Still Procrastinating? The No Re-
grets Guide to Getting It Done. New York: John
Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Ferrari, J. R., Johnson, J. L., & McCown, W. G. (Eds.).
(1995). Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory
Research, and Treatment. NY: Plenum Press.
Ferrari, J., & Pychyl, T. (2008). Procrastination: Current
Issues and New Directions, 15(5).
Ferrari, J. R., & Tice, D. M. (2000). Procrastination as a
self-handicap for men and women: A task-avoidance
strategy in a laboratory setting. Journal of Research
in Personality, 34(1), 73-83.
Ferrari, J. R., & Tibbett, T. P. (2017). Procrastination. In
V. Zeigler-Hill & T.K. Shackelford (Eds). Encyclo-
pedia of Personality and Individual Differences, pp.
1–8. New York: Springer Meteor Press.
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Automatic
self-control. In R. F. Baumesister & K. D. Vohs
(Eds.), Handbook of self-control: Research, theory
and applications. NY: The Guilford Press.
Howell, A. J., & Watson, D. C. (2007). Procrastination:
Associations with achievement goal orientation and
learning strategies. Personality and Individual Dif-
ferences, 43(1), 167-178.
Howell, A. J., Watson, D. C., Powell, R. A., & Buro, K.
(2006). Academic procrastination: The pattern and
correlates of behavioural postponement. Personality
and Individual Differences, 40(8), 1519-1530.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit in-
dexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional
criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation
Modeling, 6, 1-55.
Kachgal, M. M., Hansen, L. S., & Kevin, N. J. (2001).
Academic procrastination prevention/intervention
strategies and recommendations. Journal of Devel-
opmental Education, 25(1), 14-20.
Karas, D., & Spada, M. M. (2009). Brief cognitive-be-
havioural coaching for procrastination: a case series.
Coaching: An International Journal of Theory, Re-
search & Practice, 2(1), 44-53.
Klassen, R. M., Krawchuk, L. L., & Rajani, S. (2008).
Academic procrastination of undergraduates: Low
self-efficacy to self-regulate predicts higher levels of
procrastination. Contemporary Educational Psychol-
ogy, 33(4), 915-931.
Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cog-
nitive-affective processing system: The dynamics
of delay gratification. In R. F. Baumesister & K. D.
Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-control: Research,
theory and applications. NY: The Guilford Press.
Nasser, F., & Wisenbaker, J. (2006). A Monte Carlo study
investigating the impact of item parcelling on mea-
sures of fit in confirmatory factor analysis. Educa-
tional and Psychological Measurement, 63, 729-757.
Doi: 101177/0013164403258228.
Roseberg, M. (1965). Society and adolescence self-im-
age. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University.
8 / College Student Journal
Rosenbaum, M. (1980). A schedule for assessing
self-control behaviors: Preliminary findings. Behav-
ior Therapy, 11(1), 109-121.
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982).
Changing the world and changing the self: A
two-process model of perceived control. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 5.
Rothman, A. J., Baldwin, A., & Hertel, A. (2004).
Self-control and behavior change: Disentangling
behavioral initiation and behavioral maintenance.
In K. Vohs & R. Baumeister (Eds.), The handbook
of self-control (pp. 130-148). New York: Guilford
Press.
Russell, D. W., Kahn, J. H., Spoth, R., & Altmaier, E.
M. (1998). Analyzing data from experimental stud-
ies: A latent variable structural equation modeling
approach. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45,
18–29.
Schowuenburg, H. C., Lay, C. H., Pychyl, T. A., & Fer-
rari, J. R. (2004). Counseling the procrastinator in
academic settings. Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association.
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in ex-
perimental and non- experimental studies: New
procedures and recommendations. Psychological
Methods, 7, 422–445.
Sirois, F. M. (2004). Procrastination and counterfactual
thinking: Avoiding what might have been. British
Journal of Social Psychology 43, 269-286.
Siva, A. (1991). İnfertilede stresle başetme, öğrenilmiş
güçlülük ve depresyonun incelenmesi [Examining
stress coping, learned resourcefullness, and depres-
sion in infertility]. Unpublished Doctoral Disserta-
tion, Hacettepe University, Ankara.
Steel, P., Brothen, T., & Wambach, C. (2001). Procrasti-
nation and personality, performance, and mood. Per-
sonality and Individual Differences, 30(1), 95-106.
Tan, J., Ma, Z., & Li, X. (2015). Global self-esteem
mediates the effect of general self-efficacy on Chi-
nese undergraduates’ general procrastination. Social
Behavior and Personality: An international Journal,
43(8), 1265-1271.
Tice, D. M., & Bratslavsky, E. (2000). Giving in to feel
good: The place of emotion control in the context of
general self-control. Psychological Inquiry, 11(3),
149-159.
Tuckman, B. W. (1991). The development and concurrent
validity of the procrastination scale. Educational &
Psychological Measurement, 51(2), 473.
Tuckman, B. W. (2007). The effect of motivational
scaffolding on procrastinators’ distance learning
outcomes. Computers & Education, 49(2), 414-422.
Uzun Özer, B., Demir, A., & Harrington, N. (2012).
Psychometric properties of Frustration Discomfort
Scale in a Turkish Sample. Psychological Reports:
Measures & Statistics, 111(1), 117-128.
Uzun Özer, B., O’Callaghan, J., Bokszczanin, A., Eder-
er, E., & Essau, C. A. (2014). Dynamic interplay
of depression, perfectionism, and self-control on
procrastination. British Journal of Guidance and
Counselling, 42(3), 309-319.
Uzun Özer, B., Saçkes, M., & Tuckman, W. B. (2013).
Psychometric Properties of the Tuckman Procrasti-
nation Scale in a Turkish Sample. Psychological Re-
ports: Measurement and Statistics, 113(3), 874-884.
Van Eerde, W. (2003). Procrastination: Self-regulation in
initiating aversive goals. Applied Psychology, 49(3),
372-389. Doi: 10.1111/1464-0597.00021
Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Understanding
self-control: An introduction. Handbook of self-con-
trol: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 1–9).
... 34,96 Conversely, academic procrastination is a consequence of reduced or absent capacity for self-discipline, autonomy and self-management, and therefore, all dimensions of life autonomy are associated with academic procrastination. [97][98][99] Therefore, it is suggested that schools can provide online training for students in emotional regulation. Relevant research findings have shown that this type of training can enhance students' general emotional regulation skills and significantly reduce procrastination behavior. ...
... 98,102 Accordingly, autonomous students are more likely to be motivated to complete learning tasks and avoid academic procrastination. 97,98 Of course, teachers or schools can increase opportunities for students to participate and more choices through curriculum design or school activity planning, allowing students to practice and improve their autonomous decision-making and proactive problem-solving skills in a safe environment, thereby improving A sense of belonging to the school. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction This study aims to investigate the impact of COVID-19 on undergraduate students’ academic procrastination and negative emotions. It seeks to examine the relationship between negative affect and academic procrastination among university students, considering the mediating role of life autonomy and the moderating effect of a sense of school belonging. Methods The study sample consisted of 776 university students from the Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China. Data collection involved the use of the Negative Affect Scale, Academic Procrastination Scale, Life Autonomy Scale, and Sense of School Belonging Scale. SPSS version 26.0 and PROCESS V3.5⁸⁰ to conduct relationship analysis, test the mediation model (model 4) and the moderated mediating model (model 7). Results The findings revealed significant relationships among the variables. Negative emotions were negatively related with life autonomy and sense of school belonging, and positively related with academic procrastination. Academic procrastination showed a negative correlation with both life autonomy and sense of school belonging. Sense of school belonging exhibited a positive relationship with life autonomy. The study also identified life autonomy as a mediator in the relationship between negative emotions and academic procrastination. Additionally, the sense of school belonging moderated the initial segment of the negative emotion-life autonomy-academic procrastination pathway. Discussion This study contributes to a better understanding of the association between negative affect and academic procrastination. It highlights that negative emotions have a direct and indirect influence on academic procrastination through the mediating role of life autonomy. Moreover, the moderating role of sense of school belonging suggests that the relationship between negative affect and life autonomy is stronger when the sense of school belonging is weaker.
... This inclination toward postponement is often accompanied by subjective distress and is not merely a matter of low responsibility and time management but represents a genuine issue of self-regulation on cognitive, emotional, and behavioral levels (Klingsieck, 2013). The propensity to procrastinate can be exacerbated by factors such as perceived self-competence (Haghbin et al., 2012), low self-control (Uzun et al., 2020), fear of failure (Zhang et al., 2018), depression (Kınık and Odacı, 2020), low self-esteem (Hajloo, 2014), or anxiety (Spada et al., 2006), among others. (O'Connor and Paunonen, 2007;Vedel, 2014;Lamas, 2015;Sorić et al., 2017;Stajkovic et al., 2018;Hidalgo-Fuentes et al., 2021), consistently found that the factor of conscientiousness (known as tenacity in the HFM) is frequently associated with academic performance and serves as a powerful, significant predictor thereof. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The High Five Model (HFM) categorizes five positive human characteristics-erudition, peace, joviality, honesty, and tenacity-utilizing an inductive psycholexic approach. This study examines the predictive power of HFM on academic performance among university students, hypothesizing that it surpasses conventional predictors such as academic motivation, exam anxiety, and academic procrastination. Methods A non-experimental cross-sectional correlational design was implemented using a non-probabilistic convenience sample of 1,007 Ecuadorian university students (403 females). Self-reported measures of the “high factors,” academic motivation, exam anxiety, and academic procrastination were collected. Linear regression analysis was utilized to evaluate the predictive capacity of the HFM on academic performance. Results The analysis revealed that the high factors of the HFM significantly predict academic performance, demonstrating a stronger predictive ability than traditional psychological predictors. Discussion The findings suggest that incorporating the HFM into academic settings could enhance understanding and prediction of student performance. This could potentially inform targeted interventions that leverage these high factors, thereby fostering better academic outcomes. Further research could explore the integration of the HFM with other educational strategies and its applicability across diverse educational contexts.
... This inclination toward postponement is often accompanied by subjective distress and is not merely a matter of low responsibility and time management but represents a genuine issue of self-regulation on cognitive, emotional, and behavioral levels (Klingsieck, 2013). The propensity to procrastinate can be exacerbated by factors such as perceived self-competence (Haghbin et al., 2012), low self-control (Uzun et al., 2020), fear of failure (Zhang et al., 2018), depression (Kınık and Odacı, 2020), low self-esteem (Hajloo, 2014), or anxiety (Spada et al., 2006), among others. (O'Connor and Paunonen, 2007;Vedel, 2014;Lamas, 2015;Sorić et al., 2017;Stajkovic et al., 2018;Hidalgo-Fuentes et al., 2021), consistently found that the factor of conscientiousness (known as tenacity in the HFM) is frequently associated with academic performance and serves as a powerful, significant predictor thereof. ...
Article
Full-text available
The High Five Model (HFM) categorizes five positive human characteristics-erudition, peace, joviality, honesty, and tenacity-utilizing an inductive psycholexic approach. This study examines the predictive power of HFM on academic performance among university students, hypothesizing that it surpasses conventional predictors such as academic motivation, exam anxiety, and academic procrastination.
... En revanche, les individus du Cluster1 ayant des moyennes d'IE-Trait plus faibles que les autres pour les traits Self-Control, Expressivité et Adaptabilité déclarent plus souvent que les autres avoir des difficultés à maintenir leur attention durant tout un cours en travaux dirigés (TD) ou en amphi, s' encourager, évaluer l'utilité d'un travail désagréable à faire au regard de leurs objectifs, savoir exactement quoi faire quand ils se retrouvent devant leurs cours, savoir ce qui est visé en matière de connaissances/compétences pour la plupart des modules. Des études ont montré que le trait Self-Control est un bon prédicteur de la procrastination (Uzun et al., 2020 ;Kim et al., 2017). Les individus de ce cluster ont possiblement un profil de procrastinateur. ...
... Ferari (2010) states that procrastination is a tendency to delay actions and decisions which is not a new phenomenon (Uzun, B. et al., 2021). Furthermore, Solomon and Rothblum (1984) define procrastination as behavior or actions that slow down work for no definite reason to the point of discomfort experienced (Ahmad, R. & Mudjiran, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study is objective to describe the task aversiveness and procrastination of academics, to analyze the influences between task aversiveness to procrastination of academics and to analyze the relationship between task aversiveness to procrastination of academics. This research uses descriptive quantitative research methods. Identification of the problem in the study, there is a correlation between procrastination and task aversiveness separately strong and stable. The Purpose of study is test of connection task aversiveness student with procrastination academic. In this study, researchers use a quantitative method with a descriptive correlative approach. The population in their study were 575 students.Amount sample in this study was 236 students which was taken by using Slovin formula. The Sample was taken from each different class, then the Proportional Stratified Random Sampling technique was used. On this research, there are two types of variables. They are variable free (independent) and variable bound (dependent). As for the free variable or the independent variablein this study is task aversiveness, while the bound variable or the dependent variable is academic procrastination. In this study, the data used is interval data. As for variable in this study is task aversiveness and procrastination academic. The research instrument used is a questionnaire using Likert Scale. The results of this research show 64.41 % of students have high task aversiveness. This shows that students who perceive task aversion bothered or are not comfortable with duty are in the high category. On procrastination academic shows 60.59 % have high academic procrastination behavior. This shows the behavior of procrastination academic student high category because many procrastinating students do academic duty in the period of time you want. From the hypothesis it is found that task aversion influences significantly to the procrastination of academics. We conclude that they are connection task aversiveness significantly to academic procrastination behavior . That is, the more and more high task aversiveness, then the behavior of academic procrastination will also increase, and vice versa .
... La autoeficacia tiene una relación con la procrastinación académica (Estrada, 2021;Uzun et al., 2020), aunque no es posible establecer con claridad si los bajos niveles de autoeficacia percibidos provienen, generan o mantienen la procrastinación. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introducción: en el alumnado la procrastinación académica se asocia con dificultades de bajo rendimiento, repitencia, conflictos familiares y salud. Se busca caracterizar la procrastinación y sus motivos, y diferencias por curso, género, niveles de autorregulación y autoeficacia. Metodología: participan 451 alumnos de secundaria, de sexto a once, 44.8% hombres y 55.2% mujeres, edad M =13.99; DE= 1.91. Se aplican pruebas de procrastinación, autoeficacia y autorregulación. Se realizan análisis descriptivos de frecuencia y motivos para procrastinar; pruebas t y ANOVA, por autoeficacia, autorregulación, género, curso y su interacción. Resultados: los motivos más frecuentes para procrastinar son perfeccionismo, ansiedad a la evaluación y búsqueda de entusiasmo. El motivo de búsqueda de entusiasmo es más frecuente en hombres. El motivo de falta de energía y autocontrol incrementa en los cursos superiores. Para los motivos de poca confianza y de ansiedad, se encontró interacción entre cursos y género; son más frecuentes en hombres de curso 6, y mujeres de 9 y 10. La procrastinación incrementa significativamente en los grupos de menor autoeficacia y autorregulación. Conclusión: para disminuir la procrastinación en el alumnado sería recomendable: la detección de su motivación, y el fomento de la autorregulación y la autoeficacia, teniendo presentes las diferencias motivacionales por curso y género. Enfocándose, en los cursos superiores en la falta de energía y en las mujeres de cursos superiores, en la falta de confianza y la ansiedad a la evaluación.
... Ahondando en este aspecto, desde las teorías de la cognición social, ciertos atributos de regulación o referenciación interna como las emociones, la autoestima y las creencias como las de eficacia se suelen asociar con la problemática de la procrastinación en diferentes contextos en especial ante la ausencia o déficit de estos componentes (Moreta-Herrera & Durán-Rodríguez, 2018; Uzun et al., 2020). En este aspecto, llama de particular interés las construcciones o creencias de realización y logro que direccionan el pensamiento de los estudiantes en el mundo académico, pues es un elemento que incide en las tendencias a postergar o desplazar actividades educativas. ...
Article
Full-text available
El objetivo de la investigación fue determinar la relación existente entre la Autoeficacia Académica y la Procrastinación Académica en una muestra de adolescentes de la ciudad de Quito, Ecuador. La investigación presenta un enfoque descriptivo, correlacional de corte transversal por medio de las escalas de Procrastinación Académica, Procrastinación Académica de Tuckman, Autoeficacia General y Autoeficacia Percibida Específica de Situaciones Académicas. Participaron 424 adolescentes (49,8% hombres, 50,2% Mujeres) entre 12 a 17 años (M= 14 años; DT= 1,71). Estudiantes de diversas instituciones educativas secundarias (73,8% privado, 25,2% pública y 0,9% Fiscomisional) de la ciudad de Quito, Ecuador. La Autoeficacia General y Académica es moderada, mientras que la Procrastinación es baja; únicamente se presentan diferencias por género en la Autoeficacia General, siendo los hombres quienes evidencian más autoeficacia. Existe relación entre la Autoeficacia Académica y la Procrastinación Académica. En conclusión, la Autoeficacia Académica es un factor relevante que permite identificar el desempeño de la Procrastinación Académica en adolescentes de la ciudad de Quito.
Article
Student academic procrastination has been established as a strong predictor of several unfavorable consequences such as poor academic performance, low academic self-efficacy, and negative emotional experience. Increasing knowledge on what variables affect academic procrastination can assist nurturers and educators in minimizing student academic procrastination in the primary school setting. Based on the temporal decision model, this study examined the link between self-control and academic procrastination via the mediational role of task aversiveness and outcome value. The model was tested using structural equation modeling and cross-sectional data from 3028 primary school students (50.8% boys) in grades 3–6. The results showed a positive association between the impulse system and academic procrastination, and a negative association between the control system and academic procrastination among primary school students. Moreover, both task aversiveness and outcome value were found to mediate the link between self-control and academic procrastination among primary school students. These findings suggest that encouraging and guiding students to think more about the positive outcome and less about the negative engagement of tasks may be an effective strategy to reduce their academic procrastination.
Article
Full-text available
Claims that attributions and their related behaviors may reflect a type of perceived control that is generally overlooked. People attempt to gain control by bringing the environment into line with their wishes (primary control) and by bringing themselves into line with environmental forces (secondary control). Four manifestations of secondary control are considered: (a) Attributions to severely limited ability can serve to enhance predictive control and protect against disappointment; (b) attributions to chance can reflect illusory control, since people often construe chance as a personal characteristic akin to an ability ("luck"); (c) attributions to powerful others permit vicarious control when the individual identifies with these others; and (d) the preceding attributions may foster interpretive control, in which the individual seeks to understand and derive meaning from otherwise uncontrollable events in order to accept them. (5½ p ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed at investigating the intercorrelation among affective, cognitive and behavioural components on procrastination. A total of 402 undergraduate students (115 males, 286 females, 1 not indicated) participated in the study. Students completed the General Procrastination Scale, Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale, Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale and Self-regulation Inventory. A constructed hypothesised model was tested by utilising path analysis. Overall, the analysis indicated that the model sufficiently explained the data. The findings showed the multicorrelation among the variables, ranging from 14 to 58. Squared multiple correlation coefficients (R2) of procrastination revealed that endogenous and mediator variables explained 33% of the total variance in procrastination. The results demonstrated the importance of affect, cognition and behaviour on problematic delay referred to as procrastination.
Article
We investigated the mediating role of global self-esteem in the relationship between general self-efficacy and general procrastination among a sample of 304 Chinese undergraduate students. An online survey method was employed for data collection and willing participants completed an online survey consisting of the General Self-Efficacy Scale, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, and the Aitken Procrastination Inventory. The results showed that procrastination was negatively related to self-efficacy and self-esteem, and that self-efficacy was positively correlated with self-esteem. Mediation analysis revealed that self-esteem completely mediated the effect of self-efficacy on procrastination. Thus, we suggest that general self-efficacy decreases general procrastination because high self-efficacy fosters high self-esteem. As a result, it is necessary for preventive therapy in the context of procrastination to be focused on the enhancement of self-efficacy in order to cultivate a sense of self-worth in Chinese undergraduate student procrastinators.