ArticlePDF Available

Cognitive behavioral therapy promotes mentalizing

Authors:
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
7
COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY
PROMOTES MENTALIZING
Thr¨
ostur Bj¨
orgvinsson and John Hart
Bateman and Fonagy defined mentalizing as
making sense of the actions of oneself and others on the basis of intentional mental
states, such as desires, feelings, and beliefs. It involves the recognition that what
is the mind is in the mind and reflects the knowledge of one’s own and other’s
mental states as mental states. (2004, p. 36)
They proposed that a lack of capacity to mentalize underlies borderline
personality disorder and perhaps most psychopathology, and they developed
mentalization-based therapy to enhance mentalizing capacity. Nonetheless,
Bateman and Fonagy explicitly note that there may be many roads to facilitate
mentalizing capacity and that facilitating mentalizing may be a common
thread throughout effective psychotherapeutic interventions (see also Munich,
Chapter 6).
Consistent with Bateman & Fonagy’s (2004) view, we build an explicit bridge
between cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and mentalizing, and we contend
that interventions employed in CBT are effective in promoting mentalizing. We
begin by noting how some concepts from cognitive therapy dovetail with mental-
izing and then illustrate how cognitive therapy interventions enhance mentalizing
capacity.
Handbook of Mentalization-Based Treatment. Edited by J. G. Allen and P. Fonagy.
2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
158 HANDBOOK OF MENTALIZATION-BASED TREATMENT
COGNITIVE THEORY
Although many cognitive and behavior models for therapeutic changes have
been developed, Dobson (2001) identified three fundamental propositions that
all CBTs share: cognitive activity affects behavior; cognitive activity may be
monitored and altered; and desired behavioral change may be effected through
cognitive change. Many pertinent theories have been developed, including cogni-
tive therapy (Beck, 1964, 1976), rational emotive behavior therapy (Ellis, 1962),
and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (Segal, Williams & Teasdale, 2004). In
this chapter, when we refer to CBT, we are considering evidence-based therapies
that share the three fundamental propositions Dobson (2001) identified.
We agree with Allen’s (Chapter 1) emphasizing the verb, mentalizing, to reflect
cognitive or mental activity. In a similar vein Hayes, Strosahl and Wilson (1999)
wrote,
When we speak of “minds” we are referring here to individuals’ repertoire of public
and private verbal activities (using our technical definition of verbal): evaluating,
categorizing, planning, reasoning, comparing, referring, and so on. Although we
will use the noun form, the mind is not a thing. The brain is a thing, replete with
white and gray matter, midbrain structures, and so on, but the mind is a repertoire,
not a place. “Minding” would be more accurate, if cumbersome, description. (p. 49)
Thus mentalizing is construed as an activity, involving a set of skills, in which
people engage in effective and adaptive functioning when relating to others as
well as to themselves.
Mentalizing can be implicit or explicit as skills can be declarative or proce-
dural. From a social-cognition standpoint, Fiske and Taylor (1991) described
people as cognitive misers, that is, as being unable to process the infinite num-
ber of perceptions, interactions, and events that occur throughout the day. Owing
to the cognitive limitations in fully processing every situation, we take short-
cuts. Although these shortcuts are intended to be an effective use of cognitive
resources, accuracy is often sacrificed. From this viewpoint, effective mentaliz-
ing is often done implicitly. Effective implicit mentalizing involves the automatic
selection of behavioral responses and assumptions based on such mental pro-
cesses as intuition, prototypes and exemplars, or heuristic biases; these can
be quick, efficient, and even protective when we must respond immediately
to threatening or dangerous stimuli. However, such responses can be and
often are completely wrong, at which time more explicit processes are needed.
Explicit mentalizing can override impulsive and risky urges that might get us
into trouble, and mentalizing can prevent us from jumping to conclusions before
we embark on an ill-advised behavioral course. Cognitive therapy is concerned
with hitting the metaphorical pause button (Allen, 2005); patients are asked to
stop and think. “What went through your mind before that happened?” is a com-
mon question cognitive therapists ask to make automatic thought process more
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY PROMOTES MENTALIZING 159
explicit. Cognitive therapy thus promotes mentalizing in making patients more
aware of the nature and function of their negative thinking.
As the foregoing implies, we are not only cognitive misers but also motivated
tacticians (Fiske, 2004; Fiske & Taylor, 1991). Depending on our motivation,
we can switch from implicit, resource-saving cognitive miserliness to more
thoughtful and thorough strategies for forming impressions and undertaking
actions. To be effective, we think only as hard as we must according to our
evaluation of the situational context. For example, we tend to be more moti-
vated to size up thoroughly our new internist than the person who comes to
install our cable television. Yet a person who had a previous traumatic experi-
ence with an assailant posing as service person coming to their home would be
motivated to make a more thorough assessment. Effective mentalizers are able
to adapt, being sensitive and flexible in utilizing cognitive resources to navigate
their interpersonal environment.
This selective marshaling of cognitive resources implies that explicit thought is
necessary to employ effective strategies. Yet Ferguson and Bargh (2004) review
recent research that indicates that automatic attitudes also can be sensitive to
context and flexible to novel situations. They suggest that automatic attitudes are
functional in that they provide us with evaluative information about an object
within the current situation that is sensitive to the meaning of the object and its
relevance to our goals. Moreover, they assert that we seem to have the ability to
automatically appraise and evaluate novel objects by integrating, outside of our
awareness, evaluative information from multiple sources to deliver an evaluation
about the whole object. Nonetheless, automatic attitudes about other persons,
which involve implicit mentalizing skills, do not do all the work; the importance
of developing explicit attitudes also has been demonstrated.
How do people become effective implicit mentalizers? Much of our behavior
has been shaped by contingency-based learning, such as the successive approxi-
mations of trial-and-error and practice that are employed in acquiring the target
skill of riding a bicycle. Other kinds of responding are more rule-driven, which
allows us to respond to stimuli in precise and effective ways when the slower
contingency-based learning would be maladaptive or even dangerous. We do
not use contingency-based learning to avoid certain types of danger, because it
would be too slow and ineffective. We typically employ rule-governed behavior
when behavioral consequences are subtle, future-oriented, cumulative, or prob-
abilistic. Rules are often remote from contingency learning inasmuch as they
are hypothetical and probabilistic (Hayes et al., 1999). Rule-governed behaviors
allow us to be effective cognitive misers, saving effortful thinking for the vari-
ous important matters that may arise throughout the day. According to Skinner
(1974), rule-following behavior is the veneer of society, whereas contingency-
shaped behavior “comes from the depths of the personality or mind” (p. 140).
Artists, composers, and poets may follow rules by imitating the work of others,
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
160 HANDBOOK OF MENTALIZATION-BASED TREATMENT
but they exercise their creative expression by acting in idiosyncratic ways and
are reinforced by their natural excitement or joy. As Picasso reportedly once
quipped, “Good taste is the enemy of creativity.”
Yet Hayes and associates (1999) pointed out that rule-governed behavior is not
without disadvantages. A number of studies demonstrate that excessive use
of rule-governed behavior can lead to insensitivity to the environment. Rule-
governed behavior is less sensitive to changes in the environment than behavior
guided by immediate experience. Rule-governed behavior often leads to rigidity
and diminishes flexible thinking and behavioral responses. Flexibility requires
mindfulness, a concept that overlaps with mentalizing (see Allen, Chapter 1).
Langer (1997)developed her concept of mindlessness through a series of stud-
Q1
ies of the pitfalls that automatic responding and inflexible thinking entail. In a
now famous study, Langer and her associates instructed a research subject to
make copies on a photocopier. An experimental confederate was instructed to
interrupt with one of three requests: “Excuse me, may I use the Xerox machine?”
“Excuse me, may I use the Xerox machine because I have to make copies?” Or,
“Excuse me, may I use the copy machine because I am in a rush?” Subjects
let the confederate interrupt when any reason was given, despite the nonsensi-
cal nature of the request to make copies since that is the only plausible reason
to use a copy machine. Why one would be mindless in such a situation has
been the source of much investigation and conjecture. Mindlessness and being
miserly often have unfortunate consequences. Mindlessness can take us out of
the moment and cause us to miss important experiences that are out of the rou-
tine. A more mindful approach is more open, aware, and flexible, such that it
can lead us in a new and creative direction.
Langer’s concept of mindfulness is similar to our understanding of explicit men-
talizing. According to Langer and Moldoveanu (2000), mindfulness consists
of several interrelating elements: openness to novelty; alertness to distinction;
sensitivity to different contexts; implicit, if not explicit, awareness of multiple
perspectives; and orientation to the present. To the extent that the object of atten-
tion is personal or interpersonal, all these facets of mindfulness are consistent
with mentalizing. In Langer’s construct, mindfulness is a psychological state that
uses “basic skills and information to guide our behavior in the present rather
than like a computer program” (p. 23). As philosophers Prinz & Clark (2004)
wrote, “A mind whose ideas are mixed like bricks rather than chemicals would
be an inhuman mind, and it would be a mind unfit for this world” (p. 61).
One key facet of mentalizing and mindfulness is self-awareness, which encom-
passes our capacity to become the object of our own attention and depends on
actively identifying, processing, and storing information about the self (Morin,
2005). According to Trapnell and Campbell (1999), thinking about the self can
take two forms: self-rumination and self-reflection. Self-rumination has been
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY PROMOTES MENTALIZING 161
linked to negative mood, whereas self-reflection is associated with more pos-
itive moods. The difference can be depicted in the statements “Oh no, why
did I do that?” as opposed to “Huh, I wonder why I did that?” Self-reflection
implies motivation stimulated by interest and curiosity in thinking about oneself
in contrast to the guilt, anger, and disappointment that is characteristic of self-
rumination. Grant (2001) extended this motivational theme to another closely
related concept, psychological mindedness (PM(), “a form of metacognition: a
predisposition to engage acts of affective and intellectual inquiry into how and
why oneself and/or others behave, think, and feel in the way that they do”
(p. 12). We contend that mentalizing represents the skills necessary to foster and
utilize PM as Grant construed it. Furthermore, Grant argued that “The proposed
model of PM is of relevance to clinical practice because the self-monitoring
and self-evaluation of one’s cognitions, emotions, and behaviors is central to
the successful practice of CBT (p. 14). Hence CBT requires and enhances
mentalizing capacity and PM.
Allen (2003) proposed that “We are not behaviorists; we learn intuitively to
postulate hidden mental causes to make sense of behavior, and behavior makes
no sense otherwise” (p. 98). We disagree. We are cognitive-behaviorists. What is
the benefit of knowing the mental states of others if not to anticipate the behavior
of others? We assume the benefit of the feeling of being loved by one’s significant
other is the security of the predictability that he or she continue to behave
lovingly toward us in the future. Despite the acknowledged improbability of a
spouse acting lovingly toward their mate without having loving mental states,
nonetheless what would be the difference? Mentalizing about our significant
others may be the glue that holds loving experiences together.
COGNITIVE THERAPY
The distinction between effective and ineffective mentalizing is fuzzy; does the
behavior or the consequence define the concept? Allen (Chapter 1) points out
that mentalizing is a value-laden concept, entangled with ethics and virtue. This
further complicates the picture: how badly or ineffectively does one have to think
about another person in order to be an ineffective mentalizer or to not mentalize
at all? How distorted or maladaptive can the person’s thoughts be before he
or she becomes an ineffective mentalizer? A successful con man or swindler
may be skilled in considering the thoughts, feelings, and desires of others with
the intention to deceive. Is the swindler mentalizing? Not entirely, according
to Allen’s view. Recognizing that mentalizing is a skill with many gradations,
we follow his lead that mentalizing entails adaptive and flexible mental activity
compatible with moral and ethical considerations, and we will use the word in
this sense.
We have argued that mentalizing indicates that the person has acquired sets of
skills. He or she is able to apply flexible thinking across various situations,
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
162 HANDBOOK OF MENTALIZATION-BASED TREATMENT
is socially adept, is able to be empathic, and is capable of dealing effectively
with interpersonal challenges. How does one foster mentalizing in persons who
are deficient in applying these skills? How do we address maladaptive or dis-
torted thinking about other persons, negative automatic thoughts that arise in
interpersonal situations, or the tendency to jump to conclusions? Challenging
maladaptive thinking patterns is the centerpiece of cognitive therapy (Clark &
Beck, 1999). Beck and Weishaar (1989) define this centerpiece as a “collabo-
rative process of empirical investigation, reality testing, and problem solving
between therapist and patient. The patient’s maladaptive interpretations and
conclusions are treated as testable hypotheses” (p. 285). Cognitive therapy is
an ongoing, fluid process in which collaboratively planned interventions are
intended to become part of the patient’s way of experiencing the world and him-
self or herself. Challenging the maladaptive negative thoughts extends beyond
examining the content of a belief or the truth and falsity of any particular thought
to how the process of thinking functions within a person’s life and the world
that he or she lives in.
Cognitive therapy focuses on supporting the patient to challenge maladaptive
thinking through collaborative empiricism and on helping patients evaluate the
evidence for their interpretations or meaning-making conclusions. We challenge
patients’ assumptions or negative thoughts by asking them to consider questions
such as: “What is the evidence that he or she did not like you?” “How do
you know that the person was angry with you?” “If the person were angry
with you, then how would you cope with the situation?” This process is not
intended to arrive at any particular correct answer inasmuch as there are many
possibilities; rather, the process provides practice in flexible and creative thinking
about alternative thoughts and strategies in response to a disturbing situation. In
the process, patients develop a keener sense of how their mind works and how
the ways their mind works influence their thoughts, feelings, and behavior.
Conceptualizing a patient within cognitive theory takes into consideration aspects
of the patient’s past that have contributed to the formation of the most central or
deep-seated maladaptive core self-beliefs or schemas (Beck, 1976; Beck et al.,
1979). Beck postulated that, when these maladaptive core beliefs or schemas
are activated by negative life events, these beliefs fuel symptoms of depression.
These symptoms are maintained by automatic negative thoughts and maladaptive
behaviors (e.g., procrastination and avoidance). According to Beck (1964, 1976),
these core beliefs or schemas tend to fall into two broad categories: helplessness
and un-lovability (see J. S. Beck, 1995 for detailed discussion). Specifically,
Beck postulated that these maladaptive schemas or beliefs can be organized
around four views: self, others, the world, and the future. Persons, Davidson and
Tompkins (2000) argued that the most prominent maladaptive beliefs or schemas
pertain to self and others the territory of mentalizing. Depressed patients, for
example, are likely to have distorted and negative core beliefs or schemas, such
as “I am inadequate,” “I am inept,” or “I am a failure.” These patients often
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY PROMOTES MENTALIZING 163
experience automatic negative thoughts about various situations triggered by
serious life events, such as “I cannot handle this!” or “Why bother? This isn’t
going to work.” These automatic negative thoughts that stem from distorted
schemas in turn fuel depressive mood, hopelessness, and procrastination.
Cognitive therapy emphasizes the automatic aspect of maladaptive thoughts that
often arise without the awareness of the patient, without any effort or inten-
tion. Several techniques, which are beyond the scope of this paper, have been
developed to address automatic negative thoughts (see J. S. Beck, 1995; Persons
et al., 2000). It is important to note, as Gluhoski (1994) admonished, that cog-
nitive therapy is not merely a collection of techniques. Rather, the patient and
therapist develop a collaboration intended to promote an inquisitive, exploratory
attitude toward thinking in effect, a mentalizing attitude. Two main therapeutic
tools are frequently used to challenge automatic thoughts (a process called cog-
nitive restructuring): the Socratic method and the thought record. The Socratic
method entails guided questioning to help patients become more aware of their
maladaptive interpretations of events. The thought record, which mirrors Beck’s
cognitive theory, entails documenting the situation that triggered the automatic
negative thoughts in conjunction with emotions and behaviors. Most importantly,
the thought record promotes alternative ways of thinking about events, reinforc-
ing the experience of multiple mental perspectives. Several different versions of
the thought records exist, and it is unclear what particular method is most effec-
tive (Beck, 1995; Persons et al., 2000). Clearly, however, employing thought
records in some form is effective in alleviating depressive symptoms (DeRubeis
& Crits-Christoph, 1998).
To illustrate how cognitive therapy potentially facilitates mentalizing, we present
a case illustration of a patient who became very defensive when he met with
his inpatient treatment team at the Menninger Clinic. The treatment team had
told this middle-aged lawyer that he used humor and intellectualization to avoid
emotional experience. The patient had said that he genuinely wanted help, to
be understood, and to collaborate with the treatment team members to reach
these goals. In cognitive therapy terms, we established that he had an intense,
pervasive fear and avoidance of humiliation and embarrassment; therefore, he
had a strong urge to be right. This is illustrated in the example below, where the
patient became angry when there was a diversion from his expectation; a trivial
issue became major power struggle. The core belief or schema that was activated
in the following illustration was his basic sense of inadequacy and vulnerability.
This core belief was reflected in his conditional assumptions, “If I am wrong,
then I will get criticized” and “If people disagree with me then they are out to
get me.” Conditional assumptions are typically expressed in contingent “if then”
statements. Maladaptive if–then statements are associated with imperatives or
rigid rules of thought and behavior not only for the patient but also for those in
the patient’s interpersonal world. For example, this patient had the imperative
that no one should ever get the best of him and that he must never be wrong.
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
164 HANDBOOK OF MENTALIZATION-BASED TREATMENT
The following illustrates a therapeutic interaction between the patient and his
cognitive therapist following a meeting with the treatment team.
PATIE N T : “When I meet with my team I get very defensive, and
yesterday I got very angry. In our individual meeting,
my doctor said that I needed to stay in the hospital
one week longer, but later in the clinical rounds she
said that I only needed to stay a couple of days.”
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“And that made you angry?”
PATIE N T : “Yes, so I called her on it.”
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“What reason would she have to say that?”
PATIE N T : “She was just trying to save face in the team.”
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“So it came down to her own thoughts and feelings
about herself being more important than your
treatment. Is that how you understood it?”
PATIE N T : “Well she was wrong. What she said was one week and
I wanted to make sure the rest of the treatment team
knew it.”
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“So what I understand you to be saying is that you
wanted to make sure that the treatment team knew
that you were right?”
PATIE N T : “Yeah, this is like we discussed before. If I am wrong I
feel embarrassed or humiliated.”
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“And what does it mean to be embarrassed?”
PATIE N T : “I don’t know. I may have forgotten. If it’s a work deal,
then I can get angry and go on the attack. If I’m with
friends, then I just make a big joke out of it.”
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“Do you think it was the doctor’s intention to embarrass
or humiliate you?”
PATIE N T : “I guess it felt like it, but my experience of her is that
sheisnotthekindofpersontodothat.
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“So, your initial thoughts and feelings about what was
going on actually turned out to be contrary to how
you experience your doctor?”
PATIE N T : “Yeah.”
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“So I wonder why you reacted so strongly on your first
interpretation of this situation, rather than basing it on
your previous experience with your doctor?”
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY PROMOTES MENTALIZING 165
PATIE N T : “I guess that’s what I’m here for. It wouldn’t be too
hard to figure out that this has something to do with
my dad, who always thought I was wrong and didn’t
hold back from telling me.”
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“Did this seem similar to experience you might have had
with your father?”
PATIE N T : “My doctor is obviously not my father.”
COGNITIVE
THERAPIST:
“Do you think your approach to this was effective?”
PATIE N T : “Oh no, it was completely ineffective I got nowhere.”
This illustration demonstrates the patient’s misreading the mind of his doctor, a
failure of mentalizing. He attributed motivations and intentions to his doctor that
were contrary to his previous experience of her. The patient’s core belief is that
he is inept or inadequate. When this basic belief is activated, he feels embar-
rassed and inadequate. This patient feels vulnerable when he is wrong or makes
mistakes. Therefore, he operates under the conditional assumption that, if he is
wrong, he will be criticized. Feeling criticized, he becomes overwhelmed with
embarrassment and humiliation. The therapy did not focus primarily on the con-
tent of his thoughts that led to the threat of embarrassment or humiliation (e.g.,
viewing his doctor as if she were his father); rather, the interventions emphasized
the patient’s relationship to these feelings, that is, what these emotions mean to
him, how he interprets them, what behaviors they trigger, and what other inter-
pretations he might consider. In addition, the therapist focused on the function of
his thoughts and feelings and how they thwarted him in reaching his stated goals
(“I got nowhere”). His goals shifted from being understood and maintaining a
collaborative, helping relationship with his doctor and treatment team to avoid-
ing embarrassment and humiliation. The therapist then challenged the patient
to explore if he was willing to commit to his stated goal of being understood
and working together and at the same time allowing himself to experience the
feelings of embarrassment and humiliation.
Another theme was at work with this patient: he was willing to sacrifice accuracy
for coherence. Despite the fact that he was arguing over whether he was right
or not, he was willing to overlook the accuracy of his actual experience of his
doctor for the sake of maintaining the coherence of his basic belief system. That
is, he maintained that people would take advantage of him, particularly when
he perceived that he was being questioned, challenged, or otherwise not taken
seriously. Research by Swann and his associates (Swann, Rentfrow & Guin,
2002) has demonstrated that we are motivated by self-verification. That is, to
satisfy needs for prediction and control, we seek to evaluate ourselves in ways
that will verify our existing self-concept. We will distort information not to make
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
166 HANDBOOK OF MENTALIZATION-BASED TREATMENT
ourselves feel more positive but rather to gain a better sense of consistency and
coherence as to what we already believe about ourselves. This is all well and
good if the self-views are positive but, according to Swann, people will gain a
better sense of security and predictability even if they confirm negative beliefs
about themselves if they hold a generally negative self-concept. The patient
we described seemed to be motivated to confirm his self-concept that he was
a vulnerable person, not be taken seriously, easily humiliated, and basically
ineffective in getting his needs met. His petty and argumentative attitude was
directed toward verifying negative core beliefs about himself.
Although people are motivated to seek self-verification, this motivation can be
overridden when they feel it is in their best interest. A fundamental task of
cognitive therapy is to help people become more effective motivated tacticians.
To experience himself in a new light, the patient must be willing to feel the
tension and negative feelings that dissonance with his self-concept creates. We
encouraged the patient to reappraise his feelings as part and parcel of a desired
process of change rather than automatically deflecting disconfirming evidence
of negative self-beliefs. Cognitive therapy helped this patient to develop the
adaptive notion that people can have flexible thinking or be able to take others’
perspectives. Hence we were encouraging him to have others’ minds in mind,
that is, to mentalize.
Segal, Williams and Teasdale (2002, 2004) have developed a cognitive therapy
for depression enhanced by mindfulness training, an approach that overlaps con-
siderably with mentalizing (Allen, 2006). In this therapy, patients are trained
to purposefully pay attention to the present moment in specific, non-judgmental
ways. Mindfulness training involves learning de-centering skills in which patients
come to realize that thoughts are just thoughts and not fully representative of
one’s reality. One of the tenets of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy holds
that negative thoughts underlying a dysphoric mood involve critical process-
ing modes that are automatic in nature. These critical processing modes have
become automatic through well-practiced, habituated, cognitive routines; but
they are nonetheless extremely dependent on highly controlled central processing
attentional resources. Mindfulness training endeavors to shift to a cognitive-
processing mode that is fully in the present and attentive to moment-to-moment
experience regardless of the emotional valence. According to Segal and his
colleagues, this mindfulness mode encourages the free and open experience of
thoughts, feelings, and bodily functions; hence it reduces the experiential avoid-
ance that constitutes various forms of psychopathology.
Keeping in mind Bateman and Fonagy’s (2004) definition of mentalizing as “mak-
ing sense of the actions of oneself and others on the basis of intentional mental
states, such as desires, feelings, and beliefs” (p. 36), we propose that experien-
tial avoidance is commensurate with ineffective mentalizing particularly with
what Fonagy and colleagues (2002) call mentalized affectivity, namely, feeling
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY PROMOTES MENTALIZING 167
and thinking about feeling simultaneously. Pivotal in experiential avoidance is
the intolerance of emotion. In Leahy’s (2003) cognitive behavioral model, our
responses to feeling states are organized around emotional schemas. By definition,
schemas organize abstract forms of behavior, desires, attitudes, and attributions
in contrast to the more prolonged process of recalling a multitude of specifics that
guide even the most basic social interaction (Moscowitz, 2004). Leahy (2003)
advanced a model in which emotional experience reflects universal concepts
and processes while allowing for individual differences in particular beliefs and
response strategies. The nature of these beliefs and strategies will determine how
problematic emotional experiences will become.
Habitually, the socially phobic person ineffectively mentalizes the emotional
schemas of others. For example, persons with social phobia often hold the belief
that being anxious means being weak or incompetent; consequently, they may
misread the minds of others by assuming that others hold this same set of beliefs.
Under the scrutiny of others, an emotional schema can be activated that guides an
ineffective response associated with various safety behaviors. A socially phobic
person may wish to be approached by others in a social situation but may expend
a great deal of energy in looking calm, cool, and collected an effort that comes
off looking aloof and angry. Such a person will look unapproachable, reinforcing
negative beliefs about self and others. The initial detection of anxious feelings
will signal vulnerability to negative evaluation.
Consider the case of a college instructor who had socially anxious reactions dur-
ing his classes. He believed that, if his students knew he was anxious, then they
would conclude that he was ineffective as teacher, because teachers that are anx-
ious do not know what they are talking about. Despite this man’s best effort to
hide his emotional state, he was unable to do so. One of his particularly troubling
and uncontrollable physiological symptoms was profuse sweating prominently
on his forehead that was hard to hide even if, as a safety behavior, he wore
short-sleeve shirts in the middle of the winter. To promote effective mentalizing,
he was encouraged to reflect on alternative perspectives from his vantage point as
well as that of his students. Because of his intolerance of uncertainty, no amount
of challenging maladaptive assumptions would produce the certainty to which
he aspired. In fact he became quite good at recognizing and challenging his mal-
adaptive assumptions by becoming a better mind reader. Nonetheless he retained
residual anxiety whenever he entered a classroom. Practicing mindfulness skills
helped him observe and accept his experience of anxiety. To address directly
his emotional avoidance, exposure techniques were employed. These techniques
activated his emotional schemas, facilitating both acceptance and change. Thus,
in a variety of ways, the treatment improved his mentalizing capacity.
Thus we are working with what Leahy (2003) called individuals’ theory of
emotions, that is, the theories built on the basis of emotional experience. Indi-
viduals build theories about a variety of mental activities, including intrusive
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
168 HANDBOOK OF MENTALIZATION-BASED TREATMENT
thoughts, recurring traumatic imagery, the nature of worry, or the meaning
of panic symptoms. For example, the cognitive activity of worry serves to
reduce the physiological response of anxious arousal, including such physiolog-
ical responses as heart and respiration rates as well as galvanic skin temperature
(Borkovec, Alcaine & Behar, 2004). Worry is linguistic in nature and is typi-
cally experienced as self-talk. The relative left-hemisphere activation involved
in anxious self-talk might help to dampen right-hemisphere processing of more
intensely emotional experience associated with imagery and visceral responses.
Worry is reinforced by patient’s excessive focus on catastrophic fears that rarely
come true, thus reinforcing the perceived protective nature of worry. In addition,
worry is a mental activity that interferes with active problem solving; chronic
worriers would rather fret than engage in effective activities. CBT encourages
people to engage in real and effective problem solving despite increased anxi-
ety. Hence, excessive worriers are not being effective in keeping their mind in
mind. And excessive worries about avoided situations prevent them from experi-
encing disconfirming evidence that is invaluable in modifying their rigidly held
schemas.
Emotions provide us with information concerning the state of affairs of others,
our environment, and ourselves. Emotional awareness mentalized affectiv-
ity promotes change, and avoidance of emotion blocks change. Inadequate
or maladaptive emotional processing inhibits our ability to communicate effec-
tively about what is on our minds. Constricted emotions and impaired emotion
regulation makes emotional experience a poor source of information and not
useful in navigating patients’ internal and external worlds. Emotional avoidance
often takes the form of maladaptive covert and overt strategies to avoid unwanted
and intolerable emotional experience as seen in the form of unnecessarily risky
behaviors, obsessive-compulsive rituals, substance abuse, and so forth (Hol-
land, 2003).
Mentalization-based treatments build on creating agency within the patient and
a framework for understanding their mind as they keep other persons’ minds in
mind (see Allen, Chapter 1 and Munich, Chapter 6). One of the main strengths
of cognitive behavior therapy is the easily understood framework it provides
for therapy. In our experience, patients are very receptive to this framework;
it makes intuitive sense and provides a platform to tackle maladaptive thinking
patterns. The collaborative work that takes place is aimed at helping the patient
be his or her own cognitive behavioral therapist (Heimberg & Becker, 2002).
CONCLUSION
Plainly, the ability to understand the minds of others as well to have some clar-
ity about what is happening in one’s own mind promotes adaptive functioning.
In this chapter, we have conceptualized mentalizing as a psychological process
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPY PROMOTES MENTALIZING 169
that uses sets of cognitive skills that promote personal wellbeing and adaptive
interpersonal functioning. In addition, we have illustrated how one might con-
strue the therapeutic action of CBT as promoting mentalizing; in effect, cognitive
therapy provides highly structured ways of practicing mentalizing.
“Mentalizing” has become a part of the vernacular of the Menninger Clinic. Even
without full appreciation of the concept’s roots in psychoanalysis, attachment
theory, and developmental psychopathology (Fonagy et al., 2002), patients and
staff members seem to use “mentalizing” in a meaningful pragmatic way to
describe a more flexible and reflective style of thinking. Hence, as we construe
it, CBT interfaces well with the institution’s emphasis on mentalizing as well
as providing systematic and well-researched strategies that effectively promote
mentalizing.
REFERENCES
Allen, J. G. (2003). Mentalizing. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic,67, 91–112.
Allen, J. G. (2005). Coping with Trauma: Hope through Understanding (2nd ed.). Wash-
ington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Allen, J. G. (2006). Coping with Depression: From Catch-22 To Hope. Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Publishing.
Bateman, A. W. & Fonagy, P. (2004). Mentalization-based treatment of BPD. Journal
of Personality Disorders,18, 36– 51.
Beck, A. T. (1964). Thinking and depression: II. Theory and therapy. Archives of General
Psychiatry,10, 561–571.
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. New York: Inter-
national Universities Press.
Beck, A. T. & Weishaar, M. E. (1989). Cognitive therapy. In R. J. Corsini & D. Wedding
(Eds.), Current psychotherapies (4th ed., pp. 285– 320). Itasca, Il: Peacock.
Beck, A. T., Rush, J., Shaw, B. & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression.
New York: Guilford.
Beck, J. S. (1995). Cognitive Therapy: Basics and Beyond. New York: Guilford.
Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O. M. & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and
generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk & D. S. Mennin (eds),
Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Advances in Research and Practice (pp. 77 109). New
York: Guilford.
Clark, D. A. & Beck, A. T. (1999). Scientific Foundations of Cognitive Theory and Ther-
apy of Depression. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
DeRubeis, R. J. & Crits-Christoph, P. (1998). Empirically supported individual and group
psychological treatments for adult mental disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology,66, 37–52.
Dobson, K. S. (2001). Handbook of Cognitive-Behavioral Therapies (2nd ed.) New York:
Guilford.
Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy. New York: Stuart.
Ferguson, M. J. & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Sensitivity and flexibility: exploring the knowl-
edge function of automatic attitudes. In L. F. Barrett & P. Salovey (eds), The Wisdom
of Feeling: Psychological Processes in Emotional Intelligence (pp. 383 405). New
York: Guilford.
Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social Beings: A Core Motives Approach to Social Psychology.New
York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
170 HANDBOOK OF MENTALIZATION-BASED TREATMENT
Fiske, S. T. & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social Cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. & Target, M. (2002). Affect Regulation, Mentalization,
and the Development of the Self. New York: Other Press.
Gluhoski, V. L. (1994). Misconceptions of cognitive therapy. Psychotherapy,31,
594–600.
Grant, A. M. (2001). Rethinking psychological mindedness: metacognition, self-reflection
and insight. Behavior Change,18, 8–17.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D. & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment Ther-
apy: An Experiential Approach to Behavior Change. New York: Guilford.
Heimberg, R. G. & Becker, R. E. (2002). Cognitive-Behavioral Group Therapy for Social
Phobia: Basic Mechanisms and Clinical Strategies. New York: Guilford.
Holland, S. J. (2003). Avoidance of emotion as an obstacle to progress. In R. L. Leahy
(ed.), Roadblocks in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy: Transforming Challenges into
Opportunities for Change (pp. 116 134). New York: Guilford.
Langer, E. J. & Moldoveanu, M. (2000). The construct of mindfulness. Journal of Social
Issues,56, 1–11.
Leahy, R. L. (2003). Emotional schemas and resistance. In R. L. Leahy (ed.), Road-
blocks in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy: Transforming Challenges into Opportunities
for Change (pp. 91 115). New York: Guilford.
Morin, A. (2005). Possible links between self-awareness and inner speech: theoretical
background, underlying mechanisms, and empirical evidence. Journal of Conscious-
ness Studies,12, 115–134.
Moscowitz, G. B. (2004). Social Cognition: Understanding Self and Others.NewYork:
Guilford.
Persons, J. B., Davidson, J. & Tompkins, M. A. (2000). Essential Components
of Cognitive-Behavior Therapy for Depression. Washington, DC: American Psycho-
logical Association.
Prinz, J. & Clark, A. (2004). Putting concepts to work: some thoughts for the twenty-first
century. Mind and Language,19, 57–69.
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G. & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive
Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. New York: Guilford.
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G. & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Mindfulness-based cognitive
therapy: theoretical rationale and empirical status. In S. C. Hayes, V. M. Follette
& M. M. Linehan (eds), Mindfulness and Acceptance: Expanding the Cognitive-
Behavioral Tradition (pp. 45– 65). New York: Guilford.
Skinner, B. F. (1974). About Behaviorism. New York: Vintage.
Swann, W. B., Rentfrow, P. J. & Guin, J. S. (2002). Self-verification: the search for
coherence. In M. Leary & J. Tagney (eds), Handbook of Self and Identity (pp.
367–383). New York: Guilford.
Trapnell, P. D. & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor
model of personality: distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,76, 284–304.
UNCORRECTED PROOFS
Allen c07.tex V1 - April 26, 2006 2:19 P.M.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
Queries in Chapter 7
Q1. This reference has not been listed in the reference list. Please provide the
reference details.
... Standard cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) puts a strong focus on automatic and maladaptive thought patterns, challenging patients' implicit and explicit assumptions. Because these automatic thoughts can also relate to other people (e.g., "my colleagues have not contacted me since I'm in the hospital, that's because they do not like me"), it has been proposed that CBT targets mentalising almost by default (66,67), even though enhancing mentalising is normally no explicit goal of CBT. However, empirical support for the assumed improvements in mentalising is lacking. ...
Article
Full-text available
Major depression is one of the most common mental disorders, affecting millions of people around the globe. In recent years, researchers increasingly investigated social cognition in depression and discovered pronounced alterations. A special focus has been put on mentalising or Theory of Mind, the ability to recognize and understand another person’s thoughts and feelings. While there is behavioral evidence for deficits in this ability in patients with depression as well as specialized therapeutic interventions, the neuroscientific substrates are only beginning to be understood. In this mini-review, we take a social neuroscience perspective to analyse the importance of altered mentalising in depression and whether it can help to understand the origins and perpetuation of the disorder. We will put a special focus on treatment options and corresponding neural changes to identify relevant paths for future (neuroscientific) research.
... conduct disorder) have been reported (Blair et al., 2004;Sharp et al., 2011). Given their daily critical role and importance for clinical diagnostics, mentalizing concepts have been key targets of different therapy settings (Björgvinsson and Hart, 2006;Fonagy and Allison, 2014;Fonagy et al., 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
The ability to understand mental states of others is referred to as mentalizing and enabled by our Theory of Mind. This social skill relies on brain regions comprising the mentalizing network, as robustly observed in adults, but also in a growing number of developmental studies. We summarized and compared neuroimaging evidence in children/adolescents and adults during mentalizing using coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses to inform about brain regions consistently or differentially engaged across age categories. Adults (N = 5286) recruited medial prefrontal and middle/inferior frontal cortices, precuneus, temporoparietal junction and middle temporal gyri during mentalizing, which were functionally connected to bilateral inferior/superior parietal lobule and thalamus/striatum. Conjunction and contrast analyses revealed that children and adolescents (N = 479) recruit similar, but fewer regions within core mentalizing regions. Subgroup analyses revealed an early continuous engagement of middle medial prefrontal cortex, precuneus and right temporoparietal junction in younger children (8–11y) and adolescents (12–18y). Adolescents additionally recruited the left temporoparietal junction and middle/inferior temporal cortex. Overall, the observed engagement of the medial prefrontal cortex, precuneus and right temporoparietal junction during mentalizing across all ages reflects an early specialization of some key regions of the social brain.
... Whereas CBT was developed to treat specific symptoms, the third wave cognitive therapies, like MBT, have shifted their focus on the thinking process underlying symptoms rather than on the content of that thinking process (Lana et al. 2017). In other words, they have shifted their focus from what people think to how people think (Björgvinsson and Hart 2006). Social cognitive therapies for example tend to focus on specific aspects that underlie other-oriented mentalizing, such as affect recognition, social attribution biases or theory of mind (see Lana et al. 2017 for an overview of different approaches). ...
Article
Full-text available
Social functioning can be severely impaired in non-affective psychotic disorder (NAPD). Current models of psychosis pathogenesis do not tend to focus on social dysfunction and pharmacological treatment fails to ameliorate it. In this article, we propose that mentalization theory provides a valuable contribution to the understanding and treatment of NAPD. Impaired mentalizing may contribute to both positive and negatives symptoms as well as social dysfunction observed in NAPD. Furthermore, impaired mentalizing may help explain the relation between childhood abuse, insecure attachment and psychosis. Mentalization based treatment may contribute to the functional recovery of NAPD patients as it targets the social cognitive processes underlying social interaction. The article includes a description of the principles of MBT in general, specific characteristics of using MBT with patients with NAPD and a clinical vignette to illustrate these principles.
... Continuing with the cognitive theory of PTSD, the other main factor contributing to the sense of current threat is the presence of negative appraisals of the traumatic event (e.g., 'What happened was my fault' or 'My family despises me because of what I did/didn't do) (Ehlers and Clark, 2000). CT--PTSD identifies those appraisals and changes them through a variety of techniques that use our mentalisation processes (Björgvinsson and Hart, 2008); that is, our ability to understand the mental states that underlie our own and others overt behaviour. Specifically, the dmPFC DMN + is considered to support mentalisation ( this preprint (which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in The copyright holder for . ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Very little is known about the role of effective cognitive therapy in reversing imbalances in brain activity after trauma. We hypothesised that exaggerated threat perception characteristic of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and subsequent recovery from this disorder, are underpinned by changes in the dynamics of large-scale brain networks. Here, we use a novel data-driven approach with high temporal precision to find recurring brain networks from fMRI data and estimate when these networks become active during exposure to either trauma reminders or neutral pictures. We found that PTSD patients spend less time in two default mode sub-networks in contrast to trauma-exposed healthy controls, and that PTSD symptom severity correlates positively with time spent in the salience network during exposure to trauma reminders. The former are important for different aspects of self-referential processing and the latter for detection of threat. Importantly, the decreased time in the default mode sub-networks is rebalanced after successful cognitive therapy for PTSD. Our results show that remittance of PTSD through trauma-focused cognitive therapy is associated with the successful reinstatement of a healthy balance in self-referential and threat detection brain networks.
... A subsequent study found that students who were psychologically minded demonstrated higher ability in both cognitive and affective components of empathy, which is both perspective taking and feelings of sympathy (Beitel et al. 2005). Different theorists perceive psychological mindedness to be related to enhanced awareness of internal psychological states, processes, and needs, and as such, it contributes to affect regulation (Björgvinsson and Hart 2006;Fonagy and Bateman 2006;Nyklíček and Denollet 2009). In cognitive therapy, depressed patients are encouraged to methodically examine the way they create inferences and to consciously switch from an automatic to intentional mode of assessing the accuracy of negative judgments (Evans and Hollon 1988;Hollon and Garber 1990). ...
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive behavior models present major depressive disorder (MDD) as a failure in emotional processing associated with disrupted metacognitive skills. Self-observation, a metacognitive ability, plays a vital role in emotion regulation. Studies have noted the role of mindfulness in cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT); however, few address psychological mindedness, the interest and ability to understand behaviors, thoughts, and feelings, or alexithymia, the inability to identify and describe emotions. Seventy-one depressed outpatients with mean age M = 42.5; (SD = 13.7) received 14 CBT sessions. Longitudinal regression showed that baseline psychological mindedness significantly predicted symptom reduction across the course of treatment (β = − 8.43, 95% CI − 14.33, − 2.52, p = 0.01). Baseline alexithymia was unrelated to remission, yet positively correlated with symptom reduction through treatment. Treatment response may correlate more strongly with analytical than affective metacognitive ability, though affective skills increased during treatment. Further research can improve our understanding of processing affective information in depression and disruptions in emotion regulation and inform the selection of CBT strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Háttér és célkitűzések A mentalizáció kulcsfontosságú a szociokognitív képességek integrációjához és fejlett interperszonális készségek kialakításához. Hagelquist (2017) pedagógiai és pszichoterápiás modelljében a sikeres mentalizációt építőkövek hierarchikus rendszerébe helyezte. Célunk a Reflektív Funkció Kérdőív megbízhatóságának vizsgálata és a mentalizáció hagelquisti modelljének igazolása egészséges serdülő mintán. Jelen kutatásban azt vizsgáltuk, hogy az észlelt társas támogatás és a mentalizáció színvonala milyen kapcsolatot mutat a rezilienciával és a problémaviselkedés kialakulásával. Módszer A vizsgálatban 444 serdülő vett részt ( M = 16,3 év, SD = 1,15 év), akik a Reflektív Funkció Kérdőív mellett kitöltötték a Serdülő Reziliencia Kérdőív magyar változatát, a Képességek és Nehézségek Kérdőívet, valamint a Multidimenzionális Társas Támogatás Kérdőívet. Az adatok elemzéséhez klaszteranalízist végeztünk a mentalizáció és az észlelt társas támogatás dimenzióinak bevonásával. Eredmények Az észlelt problémaviselkedés gyenge, illetve közepes erősségű kapcsolatban áll a mentalizáció sérülésével, a mentalizáció dimenziói közül a bizonytalan mentalizációval és az érzelmi elárasztottság skálájával, azaz az egyén csökkent képessége arra, hogy saját szándékait értelmezze, pozitív együtt járást mutat a problémaviselkedés kialakulásával. A mentalizáció és az észlelt társas támogatás dimenzióinak bevonásával három klasztert alakítottunk ki. A kiegyensúlyozott csoportba tartozó egyének szignifikánsan kevesebb problémaviselkedést jeleztek, és magasabb szintű rezilienciáról számoltak be. A magárahagyottak csoportja a reziliencia és a problémaviselkedés területén is a legerősebb sérüléseket jelzi, míg az érzelemvezérelt csoport esetén a problémaviselkedés emelkedett mértéke jellemző. Következtetések A mentalizáció kapcsolati beágyazottsága befolyásolja a reziliencia színvonalát és a problémaviselkedés kialakulásának esélyét, ezzel igazolódott Hagelquist modelljének azon része, amely a sikeres mentalizáció egyik feltételének a környezeti-kapcsolati biztonságot jelöli ki. Background Mentalization is a key concept in integrating socio-cognitive skills and developing advanced interpersonal skills. In his pedagogical and psychotherapeutic model, Hagelquist (2017) placed successful mentalization in a hierarchical system of building blocks. Our aim is to investigate the reliability of the Reflective Function Questionnaire and to validate the Hagelquist model of mentalization in a healthy adolescent sample. In our study, we examined the relationship of percieved social support and mentalization focusing on the development of resilience or problem behavior. Methods 444 adolescents ( M = 16.3 years, SD = 1.15 years) completed the Hungarian version of the Resilience Scale for Adolescents, the Strenghts and Difficulties Questionnaire, The Mutidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support and the Reflective Function Questionnaire. Data analisys was performed by cluster analysis involving the dimensions of mentalization and perceived social support. Results Perceived problem behavior is weakly or moderately associated with mentalization impairment. The individual's decreased ability to interpret his or her own intentions is positively associated with the development of problem behavior. As a result of the cluster analysis involving the dimensions of mentalization and perceived social support, we formed three clusters. Individuals in „the balanced” group reported significantly less problem behaviors and reported higher levels of resilience. The group „left alone” shows the strongest impairments in both resilience and problem behavior, while the „emotion driven” group has an increased rate of problem behavior. Conclusions Relational embeddedness of mentalization influences the level of resilience and the chances of developing problem behavior, thus confirming the part of Hagelquist's model that identifies environmental-relational security as a prerequisite for successful mentalization.
Book
Full-text available
The philosophical thought of modern times, which I have encompassed under the notion of Harmony in previous works, was based on three pillars that, still today, are the crucial axis of contemporary thought: 1) the human mind has access to reality as it is, it can know the true essences that underlie the existence of the particulars; 2) the human mind can know universal ethical truths, which are at the root of human rights, as well as of political and economic individual freedoms; 3) the social universe does not have an essential order, therefore it cannot be known through reason; it is the outcome of social will, consequence of political freedom. Since social will is expressed nationally, under the logic of Harmony, once democracy in every nation and global economic freedom are achieved, human prosperity will necessarily follow. However, reality has not behaved like that. The lack of global governance has created all sort of problems, ranging from the mismanagement of the 2008 financial crisis and the 2020 pandemic, to global poverty, pollution and crime. Something is wrong. Harmony as a conceptual system of thought is not compatible with the reality of the contemporary world. science has shown that the human mind does not have access to any essential truths, not to universal ethical principles. To be consequential, in this book we propose to substitute the pre assumed initial essential truths of Harmony with the accumulated scientific knowledge. This simple shift allows us to develop a new conceptual system of thought, which we are calling The Philosophy of Belonging. And with this framework we review traditional philosophical thinking in areas such as: metaphysics, epistemology, ethics, the philosophy of justice, aesthetics, political philosophy, the philosophy of self, and the philosophy of economics. Among many other results, The Philosophy of Belonging shows that Harmony is a conceptual system that corresponds to a particular historical time of the Western societies. Other societies have evolved different conceptual systems; and therefore are, and will be, unwilling to embrace Harmony. Thus, Harmony is unsuitable to face the problems of the globalized world of today. The globalization of information, communications, technology, science, life styles, industrial production, and so on, has brought us together; in these times, it is imperative to construct a new institutional arrangement that truly reflects the diversity in national interests, and in conceptual systems of thought, that the world has. The main proposal in this manuscript is: that it is time to let Harmony go, and embrace the new conceptual system of Belonging.
Thesis
Full-text available
Achtsamkeit wird mit vielen positiven Effekten für das psychische Wohlbefinden assoziiert, wobei Fähigkeiten wie Emotionsregulation (ER) und soziale Kognition (SC) zu den wichtigsten Mechanismen gehören. In der vorliegenden Doktorarbeit wurde die Beziehung zwischen Achtsamkeit, ER und SC mit verschiedenen methodischen Ansätzen untersucht. In Studie I wurde mithilfe von Literatur und empirischen Modellen die Beziehung zwischen Achtsamkeit und ER ausgearbeitet und verschiedene psychologische und neurokognitive Mechanismen diskutiert. Studie II zielte darauf ab den ER-Mechanismus bei „Trait-Achtsamkeit“ zu entschlüsseln. Hier zeigte sich, dass es sowohl bei Probanden mit einer Borderline-Persönlichkeitsstörung als auch bei gesunden Teilnehmern einen mediierenden Effekt von Selbstmitgefühl gab, der Achtsamkeit mit ER-Merkmalen verband. Studie III untersuchte den Zusammenhang zwischen ER und SC mit Hilfe von Verhaltens- und Neuroimaging-Experimenten, mit Fokus auf dem Konzept der sozialen ER (die Fähigkeit, die Emotionen anderer zu modulieren). Es zeigte sich, dass bei der Regulierung der Emotionen anderer der eigene Stress reduziert wird, wobei wichtige "soziokognitive" Hirnregionen (z.B. Precuneus) an der Vermittlung dieser Effekte beteiligt sind. Studie IV untersuchte im Rahmen einer Neuroimaging-basierten randomisierten Kontrollstudie ER-Mechanismen während einer achtsamkeitsbasierten Intervention (MBI). Die Studie zeigte eine durch die MBI induzierte ER-Verhaltensplastizität im Gehirn, sowohl für die Eigen- als auch für die soziale ER. Ein Effekt im Vergleich zu SC (kognitive und emotionale Empathie) wurde nicht gezeigt. Unter Einbezug aller Ergebnisse wurde ein Modell postuliert, das den Austausch und die Regulierung von Emotionen im Kontext von sozialen Interaktionen integriert. Die Dissertation bietet neue Einblicke in die ER-Mechanismen der Achtsamkeit und beleuchtet die individuellen Determinanten sozialer Prozesse durch das Zusammenbringen von ER und SC.
Thesis
The aim of this thesis has been to analyze the official definitions of mental disorder (i.e. those routinely used in the Health System to make diagnosis), to demonstrate that they are biological and individual-based. A mental disorder is essentially conceived and measured as a biological phenomenon embedded in an individual. Thanks to a logical and conceptual process rooted in empirical evidences, though, I suggest here to reconsider psychopathology as a fundamentally social phenomenon: sociality could be the “proprium” or even the “definiens” of what we call mental disorders. This operation would present many difficulties, both practical and theoretical, which I tried to take into account.
Chapter
Full-text available
In this chapter, we describe socio-emotional developmental psychological abilities such as self-regulation, mentalization, and empathy in childhood and adolescence and examine how contemplative practice may foster and enhance such capacities, contributing to building personal and social resilience. Development is understood as the result of biological (genetic) and environmental (cultural, social, interpersonal factors) interactions. The main notion at stake is the plasticity of people’s socio-emotional capacities throughout developmental stages, from childhood to adolescence. In other words, from a dynamic systems approach, development is a state of flux led by multicausal interactions between the physical, environmental, and psychosocial domains. Such a complex process results in the emergence of variability regarding psychological and biological skills (e.g. self-regulation); within this context, other mental facets including the sense of self and personality develop as well (Smith and Thelen in Trends Cogn Sci 7(8):343–348, 2003). We describe the adolescent mind along with the psychological challenges and neurological developmental specificities of young people, speculating that contemplative practices, among other interventions, may lay the groundwork for self and social resilience by targeting specific psychological needs in connection with socio-emotional development. Furthermore, we refer to early parenting, psychotherapy, and educational interventions as social prevention strategies which open up new opportunities for family and children in this particularly sensitive context. Finally, we share some views and suggestions for future research regarding the integration of contemplative practices from an individual to a communal level.
Article
Full-text available
A distinction between ruminative and reflective types of private self-attentiveness is introduced and evaluated with respect to L. R. Goldberg's (1982) list of 1,710 English trait adjectives (Study 1), the five-factor model of personality (FFM) and A. Fenigstein, M. F. Scheier, and A. Buss's(1975) Self-Consciousness Scales (Study 2), and previously reported correlates and effects of private self-consciousness (PrSC; Studies 3 and 4). Results suggest that the PrSC scale confounds two unrelated motivationally distinct disposition-rumination and reflection-and that this confounding may account for the "self-absorption paradox" implicit in PrSC research findings: Higher PrSC sources are associated with more accurate and extensive self-knowledge yet higher levels of psychological distress. The potential of the FFM to provide a comprehensive Framework for conceptualizing self-attentive dispositions, and to order and integrate research findings within this domain, is discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Psychological mindedness (PM) has long been considered to be an important mediator of therapy outcome. However, to date, definitions of PM have been typified by linguistic imprecision and lack of conceptual clarity. Further, most definitions and measures of PM have approached the task from a psychodynamic perspective, thus limiting the use of this construct by clinicians and researchers from other theoretical perspectives. In this paper, previous definitions and self-report measures are reviewed and a new definition proposed. It is argued that PM is best conceptualised as a form of metacognition: a predisposition to engage in metacognitive acts of inquiry into how and why people behave, think, and feel in the way that they do. A new model, based on this definition, suggests that PM may be assessed by measuring individuals' metacognitive processes of self-reflection and insight, circumventing many of the problems associated with previous self-report measures of PM. Research into individual differences in propensity for PM, self-reflection, and insight may well provide the clinician with additional tools with which to facilitate purposeful, directed change in both clinical and nonclinical populations.
Book
An ACT Approach Chapter 1. What is Acceptance and Commitment Therapy? Steven C. Hayes, Kirk D. Strosahl, Kara Bunting, Michael Twohig, and Kelly G. Wilson Chapter 2. An ACT Primer: Core Therapy Processes, Intervention Strategies, and Therapist Competencies. Kirk D. Strosahl, Steven C. Hayes, Kelly G. Wilson and Elizabeth V. Gifford Chapter 3. ACT Case Formulation. Steven C. Hayes, Kirk D. Strosahl, Jayson Luoma, Alethea A. Smith, and Kelly G. Wilson ACT with Behavior Problems Chapter 4. ACT with Affective Disorders. Robert D. Zettle Chapter 5. ACT with Anxiety Disorders. Susan M. Orsillo, Lizabeth Roemer, Jennifer Block-Lerner, Chad LeJeune, and James D. Herbert Chapter 6. ACT with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Alethea A. Smith and Victoria M. Follette Chapter 7. ACT for Substance Abuse and Dependence. Kelly G. Wilson and Michelle R. Byrd Chapter 8. ACT with the Seriously Mentally Ill. Patricia Bach Chapter 9. ACT with the Multi-Problem Patient. Kirk D. Strosahl ACT with Special Populations, Settings, and Methods Chapter 10. ACT with Children, Adolescents, and their Parents. Amy R. Murrell, Lisa W. Coyne, & Kelly G. Wilson Chapter 11. ACT for Stress. Frank Bond. Chapter 12. ACT in Medical Settings. Patricia Robinson, Jennifer Gregg, JoAnne Dahl, & Tobias Lundgren Chapter 13. ACT with Chronic Pain Patients. Patricia Robinson, Rikard K. Wicksell, Gunnar L. Olsson Chapter 14. ACT in Group Format. Robyn D. Walser and Jacqueline Pistorello
Article
Traces the development of the cognitive approach to psychopathology and psy hotherapy from common-sense observations and folk wisdom, to a more sophisticated understanding of the emotional disorders, and finally to the application of rational techniques to correct the misconceptions and conceptual distortions that form the matrix of the neuroses. The importance of engaging the patient in exploration of his inner world and of obtaining a sharp delineation of specific thoughts and underlying assumptions is emphasized. (91/4 p ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)