ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

The Neuropsychological Profile of Mild Cognitive Impairment in Lewy Body Dementias

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Objectives: Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and Parkinson's disease dementia (PDD) have substantial clinical and biological overlap, with cognitive deficits typically observed in the executive and visuospatial domains. However, the neuropsychological profiles of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) associated with these disorders are not well understood. Methods: This systematic review examined existing literature on cognition in MCI due to LB disease (MCI-LB) and PD (PD-MCI) using an electronic search of seven databases (Medline, Embase, Psychinfo, PubMed, ProQuest, Scopus, and ScienceDirect). MCI-LB results were reviewed narratively given the small number of resulting papers (n = 7). Outcome variables from PD-MCI studies (n = 13) were extracted for meta-analysis of standardised mean differences (SMD). Results: In MCI-LB, executive dysfunction and slowed processing speed were the most prominent impairments, while visuospatial and working memory (WM) functions were also poor. MCI-LB scored significantly lower on verbal memory tests relative to controls, but significantly higher than patients with MCI due to Alzheimer's disease. Quantitative analysis of studies in PD-MCI showed a similar profile of impairment, with the largest deficits in visuospatial function (Benton Judgement of Line Orientation, SMD g = -2.09), executive function (Trail Making Test B, SMD g = -1.65), verbal ability (Naming Tests, SMD g = -0.140), and WM (Trail Making Test A, SMD g = -1.20). In both MCI-LB and PD-MCI, verbal and visuospatial memory retrieval was impaired, while encoding and storage appeared relatively intact. Conclusions: The findings of this systematic review indicate similar neuropsychological profiles in the MCI stages of DLB and PDD. Executive impairment may at least partially explain poor performance in other domains.
Content may be subject to copyright.
CRITICAL REVIEW
The Neuropsychological Profile of Mild Cognitive Impairment
in Lewy Body Dementias
Joanna Ciafone1, Bethany Little1, Alan J. Thomas1and Peter Gallagher1,2
1Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne NE2 4HH, United Kingdom
2Northern Centre for Mood Disorders, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
(RECEIVED March 20, 2019; FINAL REVISION July 5, 2019; ACCEPTED August 21, 2019; FIRST PUBLISHED ONLINE December 12, 2019)
Abstract
Objective: Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and Parkinsons disease dementia (PDD) have substantial clinical and
biological overlap, with cognitive deficits typically observed in the executive and visuospatial domains. However, the
neuropsychological profiles of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) associated with these disorders are not well understood.
Methods: This systematic review examined existing literature on cognition in MCI due to LB disease (MCI-LB) and
PD (PD-MCI) using an electronic search of seven databases (Medline, Embase, Psychinfo, PubMed, ProQuest, Scopus,
and ScienceDirect). MCI-LB results were reviewed narratively given the small number of resulting papers (n=7).
Outcome variables from PD-MCI studies (n=13) were extracted for meta-analysis of standardised mean differences
(SMD). Results: In MCI-LB, executive dysfunction and slowed processing speed were the most prominent
impairments, while visuospatial and working memory (WM) functions were also poor. MCI-LB scored significantly
lower on verbal memory tests relative to controls, but significantly higher than patients with MCI due to Alzheimers
disease. Quantitative analysis of studies in PD-MCI showed a similar profile of impairment, with the largest deficits in
visuospatial function (Benton Judgement of Line Orientation, SMD g=2.09), executive function (Trail Making Test
B, SMD g=1.65), verbal ability (Naming Tests, SMD g=0.140), and WM (Trail Making Test A, SMD g=1.20).
In both MCI-LB and PD-MCI, verbal and visuospatial memory retrieval was impaired, while encoding and storage
appeared relatively intact. Conclusion: The findings of this systematic review indicate similar neuropsychological
profiles in the MCI stages of DLB and PDD. Executive impairment may at least partially explain poor performance in
other domains.
Keywords: Dementia with Lewy bodies, Parkinsons disease dementia, Cognition, Visuospatial working memory, Verbal
learning and memory, Executive function
INTRODUCTION
The construct of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) encapsu-
lates the intermediatestage between normal ageing and demen-
tia in which cognitive decline is present yet activities of daily
living are preserved (Albert et al., 2011; Flicker, Ferris, &
Reisberg, 1991; Petersen et al., 1999). Development of the
concept of MCI has supported a proliferation of studies of
the prodromal stages of dementia, but most of the focus has
been in MCI later diagnosed as Alzheimersdisease
(MCI-AD). Less work has been devoted to prodromal demen-
tia with Lewy bodies (DLB),the second mostcommon form of
neurodegenerative dementia (Donaghy & McKeith, 2014).
MCI-LB refers to MCI in individuals who will eventually be
characterised as DLB, although DLB may have other prodro-
mal presentations, such as rapid eye movement sleep behav-
iour disorder (RBD). While DLB is diagnosed when dementia
precedes or accompanies parkinsonism (with the latter not
necessarily occurring), another Lewy body dementia (LBD),
Parkinsons disease dementia (PDD), is defined as dementia
occurring in the context of existing PD. The differentiation
between DLB and PDD is operationalised by a one year rule,
so that PDD is diagnosed when PD occurs a year or more before
dementia. LBDs share neuropathological hallmarks (Lewy
bodies and Lewy neurites; Aarsland, 2016; Boeve, 2012;
Braak et al., 2003) and appreciable overlap in clinical presen-
tation, including neuropsychiatric symptoms, autonomic
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Joanna Ciafone, Institute of
Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Biomedical Research Building, 3rd
Floor, Campus for Ageing and Vitality, Newcastle upon Tyne NE4 5PL,
United Kingdom. E-mail: joanna.ciafone@newcastle.ac.uk
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2020), 26, 210225
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2019.
doi:10.1017/S1355617719001103
210
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
features such as constipation and orthostatic dizziness, sleep
disturbances, and response to cholinergic therapy (Burn &
McKeith, 2003; Donaghy, OBrien, & Thomas, 2015; Thomas
et al., 2006). Indeed, the DLB/PDD Working Group has
suggested that there is no clinical symptom that absolutely
distinguishes DLB and PDD(Lippa et al., 2007, p. 817).
Cohort studies suggest neuropsychological impairment in
PD is not solely a late-stage issue, with subtle deficits detect-
able up to 7 years before diagnosis (Darweesh et al., 2017).
Because cognitive impairment may not always be detected or
reported at presentation (Aarsland et al., 2008; reviewed in
Goldman, Williams-Gray, Barker, Duda and Galvin, 2014),
many patients with an early alpha-synucleinopathy may be
labelled as PD-MCI once parkinsonism advances, and PDD
if loss of autonomy occurs, despite longer-standing cognitive
deficits. Indeed, updated PD diagnostic criteria state that
dementia at the time of parkinsonism onset is not an exclud-
ing factor for diagnosis (Postuma et al., 2015). Therefore, a
PD-MCI diagnosis may also capture prodromal DLB and
much existing work in PD-MCI may include prodromal
DLB patients (Fujishiro, Nakamura, Sato, & Iseki, 2015;
Massa et al., 2019). Massa et al. (2019), in recognition of
the relevance of this diagnostic issue, recently reported on
a small case series of patients that presented first with cogni-
tive impairment and later developed motor symptoms consis-
tent with PD (mean time to PD diagnosis =2.9 years, range
0.56.1), with nearly 50% having converted to dementia at
follow-up. Massa et al. (2019) note that these patients meet
both prodromal DLB (McKeith et al., 2017) and PD with cog-
nitive presentation (Postuma et al., 2015) criteria. Moreover,
utilising neuroimaging data, they conclude that, the distinc-
tion between prodromal DLB and PD with a cognitive presen-
tation seems blurry at the pathophysiological level, and these
patients might be labelled as affected with prodromal LBD, as
suggested by some authors (Donaghy & McKeith, 2014;
McKeith, 2016)(Massa et al., 2019, p. 14).
As such, a review of the neuropsychological profile of
MCI-LB should be informed by current research in cognition
in PD-MCI. In terms of neuropsychological presentation,
PDD and DLB may be indistinguishable when advanced, with
cognitive deficits typically observed in the executive function
(EF), visuospatial and memory domains (Aarsland, 2016).
However, some differences between LBD groups have been
reported, with DLB performing more poorly than PDD in tasks
requiring EF and attentional ability (Downes et al., 1998;
Gnanalingham, Byrne, Thornton, Sambrook, & Bannister,
1997; Mondon et al., 2007). The current systematic review aims
to evaluate existing research on cognition in MCI-LB and
PD-MCI.
METHODS
Search Strategy and Scope
The present review examined studies that used standardised tests
of domain-level cognitive impairment in MCI associated with
Lewy body disease. Key terms of the search (updated January
2019) of seven databases (Medline, Embase, Psychinfo,
PubMed, ProQuest, Scopus, ScienceDirect) were chosen to
target the three defining aspects of the studies of interest:
(1) patients with clinical symptoms consistent with Lewy body
neuropathology (Lewy bodies,), (2) patients in a predementia
stage (MCI), and (3) the use of neuropsychological methods
(neuropsychology). Synonyms were created for each term
and exploded with medical subject headings. Results were
limited to human studies in peer-reviewed English language
journals excluding reviews, meta-analyses, abstracts, case stud-
ies, commentaries, discussion papers, editorials, and confer-
ence proceedings. Articles that were primarily imaging or
eye-tracking studies were removed. Titles and abstracts were
reviewed independently by two authors (JC, BL) and discrepan-
cies were discussed to achieve consensus.
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
Included studies measured at least one cognitive domain with an
established neuropsychological task (previously published in
10 peer-reviewed journal articles). Composite scores of stand-
ardised tasks were accepted. Exclusion criteria were: unclear,
insufficient, or non-clinical PD diagnostic criteria; unspecified
MCI classification; n<10 per group; use of global measures
[e.g. Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE)]; domain-level
composite data only; or no healthy controls (HCs). Where studies
included a third comparison group, patient and HC data were
extracted. Baseline data were extracted from longitudinal studies.
Data Review, Extraction, and Synthesis
The following variables for each cognitive outcome measure
were extracted: first author, year of publication, country of pub-
lication, participant numbers, participant age means and standard
deviations (SD), PD diagnostic criteria, MCI criteria, disease
duration, and outcome measure means, SDsandp-values. The
direction of effect sizes (ES) was reversed as appropriate to
reflect deficits as negative ES. Bias-corrected ES Hedgesg
(Hedges & Olkin, 1985) and 95% confidence interval (CI) based
on the pooled estimate of SD were calculated and plotted by cog-
nitive domain. For commonly reported tasks, summary ES and
95% total CIs were calculated using Cochrane ReviewsReview
Manager (RevMan; The Nordic Cochrane Centre, 2014)fixed
effect model and inverse variance. Due to different testing param-
eters and scoring across studies (i.e. z-scores), the summary ES
were calculated as the standardised mean difference (SMD) to
avoid overestimation of the overall difference (Hedgesadjusted
g; Deeks & Higgins, 2010).
RESULTS
The Evidence Base
The database search resulted in >7000 articles; 6286 after
removing duplicates. Initial title review yielded 713 referen-
ces that were screened by abstract. Articles were typically
Ciafone-Neuropsychology of MCI in Lewy body dementias 211
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
removed for topic irrelevance, especially not targeting early
Lewy body disorders, only measuring cognition globally, or
being primarily an imaging study. The full text of the remain-
ing 73 articles was reviewed by both reviewers and any ambi-
guity discussed for consensus. Reasons for removal at full-text
screen were: absence of HCs (n=7), unclear or insufficient PD
diagnosis (n=11), irrelevant patient (n=10) or control (n=9)
groups, inappropriate cognitive tasks (n=5), 10 participants
tested (n=2), being primarily an imaging study (n=1), and
cohort redundancy with other retained papers (n=8; the paper
providing the most comprehensive cognitive task data was
chosen). Studies excluded for absences of HCs (n=7) gener-
ally compared PD-MCI with PD with normal cognition
(PD-NC) or normative data. They were reviewed informally
for relevancy (see supplementary table).
MCI-LB Results
Table 1displays details of retained papers. The search strat-
egy yielded only one study in MCI-LB that met full inclusion
criteria (Kemp et al., 2017). Six studies measured cognition in
MCI-LB, but did not use a HC group. One paper had only
nine patient subjects (Jicha et al., 2010). Given the small
number and heterogeneity of MCI-LB results, these papers
were retained for narrative review.
Kemp et al. (2017), the only paper meeting full inclusion
criteria, considers neuropsychological scores as well as social
cognitive ability. Patients are classified as prodromal DLB
(n=37) for meeting MCI criteria (Petersen, 2004) and having
at least two core symptoms of DLB (McKeith et al., 2005).
After adjusting for age, sex, and education level, prodromal
DLB scored significantly lower than HCs (n=29) on 14 of
21 extracted outcome measures, representing EF, attention
and processing speed, working memory (WM), visuospatial
memory, visuoconstructive ability, and verbal memory.
The largest ES were reported on semantic verbal fluency
(EF; g=1.65) and digit symbol substitution (attention/
processing speed, g=1.16) tasks. The other significant
ES were in the g=0.53 to 0.93 range. Prodromal DLB
showed impaired immediate and delayed free recall relative
to controls. However, the authors suggested intact real ver-
bal memory (i.e. storage)in the majority of their patients
(29 of 37; outcome measure not specified) and emphasised
retrieval deficits as related to executive impairments
(Kemp et al., 2017). Kemp et al. (2017) also concluded that
verbal memory (free and cued selective reminding test total
recall and recognition, g=0.60) was better preserved than
visual recognition memory [Delayed Matching to Sample-48
Items (g=0.79)] in their sample.
All studies in MCI-LB used clinical diagnostic criteria
except for Jicha et al. (2010), where neuropathological diagno-
ses were utilised. No significant differences between
MCI-LB and MCI-AD in global cognitive measures (e.g.
MMSE) were reported. Excluding Kemp et al. (2017), 28 of
the remaining 79 (35.44%) extracted outcome variables
showed differential performance between MCI-LB and
MCI-AD after bias correction (see description in Table 1
and individual statistics in the supplementary materials).
This tally does not include individual tasks removed that did
not meet a priori exclusion criteria for publication in at least
10 peer-reviewed journals. Twenty-one variables showed
poorer performance by MCI-LB relative to MCI-AD in visuo-
spatial, attention, and psychomotor speed. Both Bussè et al.
(2018) and Cagnin et al. (2015) reported poorer performance
in MCI-LB in WM, visuospatial and visuoconstructive ability,
and EF, with magnitudes ranging from medium (g=0.56,
Object Decision Visuospatial Test, Cagninet al., 2015)tolarge
(g=0.95, Digit Span Backwards, Cagnin et al., 2015)and
very large (g=3.45, Pentagon Test number of angles,
Busse, Hensel, Gühne, Angermeyer, & Riedel-Heller,
2006). Donaghy et al. (2018) showed deficits in MCI-LB rel-
ative to MCI-AD of medium ES in attention (Digit Vigilance,
g=0.56), EF (difference between Choice and Simple
Reaction Time, g=0.53), and visuospatial function
(Angle Task, g=0.55). Sadiq et al. (2017) reported impaired
scores on EF (letter fluency, g=0.62) and attention (Trails B,
g=0.51), while Yoon, Kim, Moon, Yong and Hong (2015)
only showed impairment inEF (g=0.72; Stroop Colour test)
and there were no significant differences in Jicha et al. (2010)
after bias correction. MCI-LBs performed significantly better
than MCI-AD on verbal memory outcome measures (immedi-
ate recall, delayed recall, and recognition) in three studies
(Bussè et al., 2018; Cagnin et al., 2015;Yoonetal.,2015)with
generally large ES (g=0.663.80, mean g=1.82).
The studies showing deficits in MCI-LB in the domains of
WM and attention, EF and visuospatial function were not
wholly consistent, as some outcome variables in the same
domains did not reach significance. For example, Donaghy
et al. (2018) reported significant differences in only one of
the nine outcome measures of attention and psychomotor
speed (Digit Vigilance).
Additionally, both Yoon et al.(2015)andSadiqetal.(2017)
also compared MCI-LB/DLB-MCI to an MCI-stable group at
follow-up. In Yoon et al. (2015; 5 years of follow-up), MCI-LB
was impaired in the domains of visuospatial ability and
memory [ReyOsterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) recognition
and copy], EF (Stroop Colour, verbal semantic fluencies),
WM/attention (Digit Span Forwards and Backwards), and ver-
bal learning and memory (Seoul Verbal Learning Test delayed
recall) relative to MCI that did not convert to dementia. Sadiq
et al. (2017; mean follow-up 2.8 years) found that DLB-MCI
patients are also impaired in EF (letter fluency) and visuocon-
struction (clock drawing test) relative to stable-MCI.
PD-MCI Evidence Base
Demographic and clinical characteristics of
participants
Details of quantitatively reviewed articles are summarised in
Table 1and group characteristics are summarised in Table 2.
Some studies did not provide details of age (but were
age-matched) or disease duration. For PD diagnosis, studies
212 J. Ciafone et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
Table 1. Details of all included studies (MCI-LB and PD-MCI)
First author
(year) Country
Patient group
Comparison
group
Design MCI criteria Diagnostic criteria
Disease
duration
(months)
Mean (SD)
Domains
measured
Outcome
measures
impairedType nType n
Bussè (2018) Italy Prodromal
DLB
19 Prodromal
AD
25 Cross-sectional with
diagnosis after at
least 3 years of
follow-up.
Petersen (2004) McKeith et al.
(2017), DLB
Possible.
EF 2 of 3
WM/att 2 of 3
Verbal 0 of 7
Visuospatial 4 of 5
Cagnin (2015) Italy MCI-LB 25 MCI-AD 28 Retrospective analysis
of records at first
(predementia) visit.
Absence of dementia
(MMSE >26) and
functional impairment in
everyday life. Exclusion:
inability to establish a
definite diagnosis at
follow-up and cognitive
decline due to
comorbidities.
McKeith et al.
(2005)
EF 2 of 2
WM/att 1 of 2
Verbal 0 of 2
Visuospatial 4 of 10
Donaghy (2018) UK MCI-LB 41 MCI-AD 24 Cross-sectional. Albert et al. (2011) McKeith et al.
(2017), DLB
Probable.
EF 1 of 1
WM/att 1 of 9
Verbal 0 of 3
Visuospatial 1 of 1
Jicha (2010) USA MCI-LB 9 MCI-AD 12 Retrospective analysis
of records of
neuropathically
determined groups.
Records reviewed for MCI
criteria of 2nd International
Consensus Panel on MCI,
Stockholm, Sweden
(Winblad et al., 2004)
including impairment of at
least 1.5 SDs below age
and education matched
controls on formal
testing(p. 1807).
Neocortical DLB
(below Braak
stage IV) with no
significant AD
pathology or
vascular disease),
EF 0 of 3
WM/att 0 of 2
Verbal 0 of 3
Kemp (2017) France Prodromal
DLB
37 HCs 29 Cross-sectional. Petersen (2004) McKeith et al.
(2005), DLB
Probable.
EF 4 of 4
WM/att 2 of 4
Verbal 3 of 3
Visuospatial 5 of 9
Sadiq (2017) UK DLB-MCI 21 AD-MCI 107 Retrospective,
unselected
sample.
Petersen et al. (1997) McKeith et al.
(2005), DLB
Probable.
21.6 (13.2) EF 1 of 2
WM/att 1 of 2
Verbal 0 of 2
Visuospatial 0 of 1
(Continued)
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
Table 1. (Continued )
First author
(year) Country
Patient group
Comparison
group
Design MCI criteria Diagnostic criteria
Disease
duration
(months)
Mean (SD)
Domains
measured
Outcome
measures
impairedType nType n
Yoon (2015) South
Korea
MCI-LB 18 MCI-AD 45 Prospective
cohort.
With regard to matched
controls: (1) scores below
16th percentile in at least
one of five cognitive
domains on the Seoul
Neuropsychological
Screening Battery (Yang,
2003), (2) scores above the
16th percentile on Korean
MMSE, and (3) no
impairments in Activities
of Daily Living.
McKeith et al.
(2005)
EF 1 of 5
WM/att 0 of 2
Verbal 0 of 6
Visuospatial 0 of 4
Anderson
(2013)
UK PD-MCI 19 HC 16 Matched on age Score of 93 or less on
Addenbrookes Cognitive
Examination-Revised, no
lower score limit
(Komadina, 2011)
Calne et al. (1992) Not given EF 3 of 4
Biundo (2014) Italy PD-MCI 49 HC 18 Adjusted normative
data: age, education
MDS level II criteria (Litvan
et al., 2012)
UKPDS (no
reference)
109.7 (72.0) EF 2 of 3
WM/att 1 of 4
Verbal all
Visuospatial all
Bocanegra
(2015)
Colombia PD-MCI 17 HC 17 Matched on age,
education, sex
MDS level I criteria (Litvan
et al., 2012)
UKPDS (Hughes
et al., 1992)
Not given Verbal all
Costa (2015) Italy PD-MCI 48 HC 20 Groups not matched MDS level II criteria (Litvan
et al., 2012)
UKPDS (Hughes
et al., 1992)
75.6 (49.2) EF all
WM/att 1 of 5
Verbal all
Visuospatial all
Dalrymple-
Alford (2011)
New
Zealand
PD-MCI 3 HC 34 Adjusted (z-scores):
age, education
Neuropsychological scores
1.52 SDs below
normative data in one
domain.
UKPDS (Hughes
et al., 1992)
Not given EF all
WM/att all
Verbal all
Visuospatial all
Galtier (2016) Spain PD-MCI 26 HC 20 Matched on age,
education, intelligence,
manual preference, sex
MDS level II criteria (Litvan
et al., 2012)
UKPDS (Hughes
et al., 1992)
105.7 (67.2) EF 2 of 4
WM/att 0 of 1
Verbal 3 of 4
Visuospatial all
(Continued)
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
Table 1. (Continued )
First author
(year) Country
Patient group
Comparison
group
Design MCI criteria Diagnostic criteria
Disease
duration
(months)
Mean (SD)
Domains
measured
Outcome
measures
impairedType nType n
Hessen (2016) Norway PD-MCI 13 HC 25 Groups not matched Clinical and
neuropsychological
criteria (National Institute
on Aging Alzheimers
Association). At least one
of the six
neuropsychological
subtests impaired (1.5 SD
or more below normative
scores)
UKPDS (Berardelli
et al., 2013)
25.2 (9.6) EF all
Verbal all
Peraza (2017) UK PD-MCI 37 HC 30 Groups not matched MDS level II criteria (Litvan
et al., 2012)
UKPDS (Hughes
et al., 2002)
5.81 (4.51) EF all
WM/att all
Poletti (2012) Italy PD-MCI 18 HC 100 Matched on age MDS level II criteria (Litvan
et al., 2012)
UKPDS (Hughes
et al., 1992)
11.0 (8.8) EF 7 of 8
WM/att all
Verbal all
Visuospatial all
Sanyal (2014) India PD-MCI 90 HC 280 Matched on age, sex Clinical Criteria. UKPDS
(Conditions,
2006)
34.8 (55.2) EF all
Verbal all
Visuospatial 1 of 2
Song (2008) Korea PD-MCI 20 HC 33 Groups not matched Neuropsychological scores of
at least 2 SD below control
group in at least one test.
UKPDS (Hughes
et al., 1992)
57.6 (29.9) EF all
WM/att 1 of 2
Verbal 2 of 4
Visuospatial 1 of 4
Wang (2015) China PD-MCI 96 HC 163 Matched on age,
gender, education
MDS level II criteria (Litvan
et al., 2012)
UKPDS (Hughes
et al., 1992)
61.2 (41.0) EF all
WM/att all
Verbal 3 of 4
Visuospatial all
Yu (2012) Taiwan PD-MCI 94 HC 84 Matched on age,
education, sex
Neuropsychological scores of
at least 1.5 SD below
normative mean (or below
the fifth percentile) of
normative samples in at
least one domain. Unclear
when norms of HC scores
were used.
UKPDS (Hughes
et al., 1992)
52.9 (27.1) EF all
WM/att 6 of 7
Verbal all
Visuospatial all
MCI =mild cognitive impairment; DLB =dementia with Lewy bodies; AD =Alzheimers disease; PD =Parkinsons disease; HC =healthy controls; MMSE =Mini-Mental State Examination; UKPDS =UK Parkinsons
Disease Society; MDS =Movement Disorder Society; SD =standard deviation; EF =executive function; WM/att =working memory/attention.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
cited United Kingdom PD Society Brain Bank criteria
(UKPDS; Hughes, Daniel, Kilford, & Lees, 1992) or equiv-
alent, and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and
Stroke criteria (Gelb et al., 1999). To define PD-MCI, seven
studies used MDS criteria (Litvan et al., 2012). The remaining
studies used a mix of clinical and neuropsychological diag-
nostic criteria, including both normative and control data
as impairment criteria.
Exclusion of tasks
Unclear task information or use of unestablished tasks
(<10 published peer-reviewed references) necessitated the
removal of variables from 8 of 13 studies. While not leading
to removal from analyses, details provided were often also insuf-
ficient regarding the length of delay in memory tasks. Only the
two most commonly reported measures per task and paper were
retained in order to focus synthesis and avoid redundancy (e.g.
for card sorting tasks, categories achievedand perseverative
errorswere used). Similarly, closely related variables adminis-
tered for more experimental rationales were removed to avoid
redundancy (e.g. from Costa et al. (2015), Drawings Copy
was extracted while Drawings with Landmarks Copywas
not). Sums were not extracted if separate scores were also
provided, for example, with Digit Span Total (Yu et al., 2012).
Domain-Level Cognitive Assessments
Outcome measures were organised into the domains of WM,
EF, visuospatial, and verbal function, corresponding to their
face validity. This conflicted with a number of papers that
assigned outcome measures to domains of which they were
not the primary target. For example, verbal fluency tasks were
referred to as language or memory tasks in five papers when
they are most commonly accepted as measures of EF
(MacCarthy, 1972). Overall, every study showed worse
performance in patients than HCs in at least one cognitive
domain. Table 2displays the number of outcome variables
retained in each domain for each group, after removal
described above, and how many were impaired. Table 3
displays summary ES of frequently reported outcome varia-
bles. See supplementary materials for individual ES and CI,
and for figures displaying data not included below. Across
domains, there was a pattern of smaller ES reported by studies
with larger sample sizes.
Executive function, working memory, and attention
A variety of EF and WM tasks were used, with ES ranging
from moderate (g=0.45; Yu et al., 2012, semantic fluency)
to very large (g=2.47; Sanyal, Banerjee, & Rao, 2014;
semantic fluency). EF was the most frequently targeted
Table 2. Summary of impairments in PD-MCI from retained paper (n=13) across neuropsychological domains
PD-MCI group Control group
n
Age
Mean (SD) n
Age
Mean (SD) Domain
Proportion of studies with at least one
impaired variable relative to controls
Proportion of outcome variables
impaired relative to controls
530 63.8 (8.3) 840 61.7 (9.3) Executive function 100% (13/13) 89% (39/44)
WM/Attention 88.9% (8/9) 68% (21/31)
Visuospatial L&M 88.9% (8/9) 91% (30/33)
Visuospatial WM 75.0% (3/4) 67% (4/6)
Verbal L&M 100% (11/11) 88% (30/34)
MCI-LB group Control group
Domain
Proportion of studies with at least one
impaired variable relative to controls
Proportion of outcome variables
impaired relative to controlsn
Age
Mean (SD) n
Age
Mean (SD)
37 67.2 (8.6) 29 68.8 (7.9) Executive function 100.0% (1/1) 100.0% (4/4)
WM/Attention 100.0% (1/1) 50.0% (2/4)
Visuospatial L&M 100.0% (1/1) 55.6% (5/9)
Visuospatial WM ––
Verbal L&M 100.0% (1/1) 100.0% (3/3)
MCI-LB group MCI-AD group
Domain
Proportion of studies with at least one
impaired variable relative to controls
Proportion of outcome
variables impaired relative to
controlsn
Age
Mean (SD) n
Age
Mean (SD)
133 76.0 (7.92) 241 75.0 (8.0) Executive function 83.3% (5/6) 43.8% (7/16)
WM/Attention 67.0% (4/6) 25.0% (5/20)
Visuospatial L&M 60.0% (3/5) 42.9% (9/21)
Visuospatial WM 100.0% (1/1) 100.0% (1/1)
Verbal L&M 0.0% (0/6) 0.0% (0/20)
SD =standard deviation; PD =Parkinsons Disease; MCI =mild cognitive impairment; WM =working memory, L&M =learning and memory.
216 J. Ciafone et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
domain. Commonly reported outcome measures were from
card sorting, verbal fluency, and Stroop tasks, with patients
showing summary impairments in the medium (g=0.63)
to large (g=1.30) range (Table 3). Verbal fluency tasks,
commonly used measures of EF (Baddeley, 1996), can be
separated into semantic and phonemic fluency paradigms.
Both showed large ES (Figure 1a and 1b), but PD-MCI
was somewhat more impaired in semantic fluency tasks.
PD-MCI showed a lower proportion of only 21 of 30
impaired WM variables, but with a similar range in summary
ES (g=0.52 to 1.65) as in EF. Six studies included WM
measures that in particular require psychomotor speed, including
Trails A and B. While both summary ES are large, there was
greater impairment on Trails B, which requires EFs, than
Trails A. All four studies that used Trails B reported significant
and large deficits (above 1 SD), with a SMD of g=1.65
(1.93, 1.37). A similar divergence was observed between
Digit Span Forwards (g=0.52) and Backwards (g=0.69).
Visuospatial domain
Patients performed significantly worse than controls on 30 of
the 33 visuospatial memory variables. The largest SMD com-
puted from the evidence base overall was in judgement of line
orientation (JOLO) (g=2.09; Benton, Hamsher, Varney, &
Spreen, 1983), making visuospatial function the most frequently
Table 3. Results of tests for commonly reported outcome measures for PD-MCI participants
Domain Outcome measure Studies
Patient
n
Control
n
Std. Mean Difference
(95% CI)
Test for overall
effect Heterogeneity
EF Card sorting categories
achieved
4 136 224 1.30 (1.55, 1.04) z=9.93
(p<.001)
Chi² =3.36, df =3
(p=.34); =11%
Card sorting perseverative
errors
4 110 204 0.63 (0.89, 0.37) z=4.72
(p<.001)
Chi² =0.07, df =2
(p=.97); =0%
Phonemic fluency 6 267 384 0.85 (1.02, 0.67) z=9.48
(p<.001)
Chi² =9.58, df =5
(p=.09); =48%
Semantic fluency 8 379 748 1.07 (1.21, 0.93) z=14.90
(p<.001)
Chi² =131.77, df =7
(p<.001); =95%
Stroop interference
condition
3 127 288 0.73 (0.95, 0.51) z=6.54
(p<.001)
Chi² =3.81, df =2
(p=.15); =48%
TMT B 4 151 188 1.65 (1.93, 1.37) z=11.51
(p<.001)
Chi² =7.74, df =3
(p=.05); =61%
WM Digit span forwards 5 257 318 0.69 (0.87, 0.51) z=7.47
(p<.001)
Chi² =32.25, df =4
(p<.001); =88%
Digit span backwards 5 234 320 0.52 (0.70, 0.35) z=5.78
(p<.001)
Chi² =7.99, df =4
(p=.09); =50%
TMT A 5 195 272 1.20 (1.42, 0.98) z=10.67
(p<.001)
Chi² =20.33, df =4
(p<.001); =80%
Verbal Naming tasks 6 220 468 1.40 (1.59, 1.21) z=14.50
(p<.001)
Chi² =36.80, df =5
(p<.001); =86%
Word list (immediate free
recall)
5 195 341 0.96 (1.15, 0.76) z=9.53
(p<.001)
Chi² =28.61, df =4
(p<.001); =86%
Word list (delayed free
recall)
7 257 411 1.25 (1.43, 1.08) z=13.80
(p<.001)
Chi² =9.31, df =6
(p=.16); =36%
Word list (recognition) 3 142 216 0.07 (0.29, 0.14) z=0.69
(p=.49)
Chi² =0.39, df =2
(p=.82); =0%
Word list (similarities) 3 189 265 0.50 (0.70, 0.30) z=4.97
(p<.001)
Chi² =3.44, df =2
(p=.18); =42%
VS Benton JOLO 4 176 333 2.09 (2.33, 1.85) z=17.28
(p<.001)
Chi² =56.06, df =3
(p<.001)); =95%
Figure copy 4 283 481 1.56 (1.74, 1.38) z=16.68
(p=.003)
Chi² =153.64, df =3
(p=.18); =98%
Drawing immediate recall 4 130 237 0.94 (1.19, 0.70) z=7.57
(p<.001)
Chi² =13.28, df =3
(p=.004); =77%
Drawing delayed recall 7 305 585 1.03 (1.19, 0.87) z=12.79
(p<.001)
Chi² =25.83, df =6
(p<.001); =77%
ROCF 6 231 258 1.42 (1.65, 1.19) z=12.24
(p<.001)
Chi² =93.53, df =5
(p<.001); =95%
PD =Parkinsons disease; MCI =mild cognitive impairment; EF =executive function; WM =working memory; VS =visuospatial; TMT =Trail Making Test;
JOLO =judgement of line orientation; ROCF =ReyOsterrieth complex figure.
Ciafone-Neuropsychology of MCI in Lewy body dementias 217
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
and severely impaired domain. Only Song, Kim, Jeong, Song
and Lee (2008) reported a lack of difference in visuospatial
function (ROCF copy, immediate and delayed recall). Figure
copying and recall was a common visuospatial paradigm, but
recall conditions varied substantially in the length of delay.
Summary ES for the PD-MCI group showed more pronounced
deficits in figure copying than in either immediate or delayed
recall. Song et al. (2008) also tested delayed figure recognition
and found a moderate deficit in PD-MCI relative to controls
(g=0.60). However, potential outliers (Poletti et al., 2012;
Sanyal et al., 2014) complicate interpretation (Figure 2).
Six outcomes from four studies targeted the visuospatial
component of WM, per fractionated models proposed by
Baddeley and Hitch (1974), Logie and Pearson (1997),
and others. These include the Corsi Test (Corsi, 1972)
and Pattern Recognition Memory (Robbins et al., 1998).
A summary ES computed from Corsi Tests administered
byPolettietal.(2012), Costa et al. (2015), and Biundo
et al. (2014) suggested a moderate impairment in PD-
MCI relative to controls, with Poletti et al. (2012)having
the only significant finding.
Verbal domain
Patients performed significantly worse than controls in 30 out
of 34 of the verbal domain variables. Each study that tested
Fig. 1. Bias-corrected effect sizes of (a) phonemic verbal fluency scores and (b) semantic verbal fluency scores in PD-MCI relative to controls
in individual studies (circles) and as a summary effect size by group (diamonds).
218 J. Ciafone et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
verbal learning and memory reported at least one significant
difference between groups. However, three studies measur-
ing verbal learning and memory did not find a significant
difference between groups: California Verbal Learning
Test in Galtier, Nieto, Lorenzo and Barroso (2016), Hopkins
Verbal Learning Test (HVLT) and Boston Naming Test in
Song et al. (2008), and HVLT in Wang et al. (2015). Two-
thirds of significant outcome variables pertained to word list
recall tasks. Most immediate and delayed free recall scores
indicated significant impairment, but the three studies meas-
uring recognition did not show a significant difference
between patients and controls. Very large ES were also found
in naming tasks (g=1.40).
DISCUSSION
Summary of Findings
MCI-LB
In this structured review, only one study on the cognitive
profile of MCI-LB utilised a control group. Kemp et al.
(2017) reported prodromal DLB to have a profile of diffuse
impairments in the domains of EF, processing speed, atten-
tion, WM, visuospatial ability and visuospatial construction,
verbal memory and language. Kemp et al. (2017) provided
evidence of immediate and delayed verbal recall deficits in
prodromal DLB compared to controls, although recognition
Fig. 2. Bias-corrected effect sizes of visuospatial copying and recall tasks in PD-MCI relative to controls in both individual studies
(circles) and as a summary effect size by group (diamonds). BVMT =Brief Visuospatial Memory Test; ROCF =ReyOsterrieth
Complex Figure.
Ciafone-Neuropsychology of MCI in Lewy body dementias 219
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
memory was intact. This study indicates that a substantial
number of MCI-LB will present with verbal memory impair-
ment at this early stage and it should not therefore be taken as
a reliable indicator of prodromal AD (Kemp et al., 2017).
However, ES of verbal memory impairment reported in
MCI-LB (g=058; Kemp et al., 2017) were almost half that
of the pooled estimate for PD-MCI (g=1.25).
Given that the largest deficits overall in prodromal DLB
were in EF (g=1.65) and processing speed (g=1.16),
it is possible that domain-general impairments could at least
partly account for deficits across other cognitive functions
(Kemp et al., 2017). Slowed speed of processing, executive
dysfunction, or praxic and perceptual impairments in prodro-
mal DLB could confound interpretation of deficits in more
complex tasks such as visual construction or naming tests,
as well as the greater impairments on visual versus verbal
memory scores reported by Kemp et al. (2017). Executive
dysfunction, for example, has the potential to impair cogni-
tion at various stages, including encoding and retrieval in
the case of memory function, thereby compounding the
impact (Bryan et al., 1999) Kemp et al.s(2017) emphasis
on the role of EF in prodromal DLB is in line with a recent
report that another measure of EF (verbal fluency) had the
highest predictive accuracy for conversion to DLB in
idiopathic RBD patients (Génier Marchand et al., 2018).
EF similarly emerges as a potential explanatory factor in
PD-MCI results, as discussed further below.
Only six other studies (Bussè et al., 2018;Cagninetal.,
2015; Donaghy et al., 2018; Jicha et al., 2010; Sadiq et al.,
2017; Yoon et al., 2015) resulted from the systematic review
and used MCI-AD rather than a control group for comparison.
This illustrates both the dearth of neuropsychological research
in MCI-LB and the current focus on AD in the field. However,
the small number of results in this key population may also
relate to the challenge of identifying MCI-LB patients. Both
DLB and PDD are under-diagnosed, and regional (geographic)
differences in prevalence rates suggest varying clinical aware-
ness of Lewy body disease (Kane et al., 2018). The retained
studies provided limited evidence for MCI-LB and MCI-AD
differentiation, as only about a third of extracted measures
differed between groups and the findings within domains
are sometimes inconsistent, with MCI-LB statistically differ-
ing from MCI-AD on some but not all tasks in the same
domain. Verbal learning and memory appeared preserved in
MCI-LB relative to MCI-AD, in line with the pronounced
memory encoding deficits of AD (Lange et al., 2002;
Martin, Brouwers, Cox, & Fedio, 1985). However, without
the use of HCs, it cannot be inferred that verbal impairment
relative to controls should not be expected in MCI-LB cohorts.
PD-MCI
Our quantitative analyses emphasise that there are significant
deficits in PD-MCI relative to HCs. Overall, the highest
proportion and most severe of deficits was observed in the
visuospatial domain, although the verbal and EF domains
were also consistently impaired. One of the largest
differences between PD-MCI and controls was in Bentons
JOLO task (Benton et al., 1983). JOLO, unlike many other
visuospatial tasks, requires minimal motor skill and is free
of practice effects (Montse, Pere, Carme, Francesc, &
Eduardo, 2001). As such it may be particularly useful in
tracking cognitive decline with PD progression. There was
less consistency in the attention and WM data, with half of
the papers showing deficits in some but not all reported tasks.
Similarly, the magnitude of deficit on semantic verbal fluency
tasks differed widely between studies, suggesting unreliabil-
ity when quantifying the extent of executive dysfunction in
patients. Our review found a slightly larger ES of impairment
in semantic than phonemic verbal fluency tasks in PD-MCI,
in line with findings from a meta-analysis in PD (Henry &
Crawford, 2004). Semantic fluency tasks make demands
on temporal functions and are typically challenging for AD
patients (Henry, Crawford, & Phillips, 2004), while pho-
nemic verbal fluency is more closely associated with frontal
abilities (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander, & Stuss,
1998). This suggests firstly that caution should be taken when
utilising semantic and phonemic verbal fluency tasks inter-
changeably as indicative of executive dysfunction, for exam-
ple, in the case of current PD-MCI criteria (Litvan et al.,
2012). Secondly, this could suggest that co-occurring AD
pathology accounts for semantic fluency impairment in
PD-MCI. However, recent work by El-Nazer et al. (2019)
concluded that PD and PD/AD mixed pathologies do not
differ clinically and that cognitive deficits are associated with
both LB and AD pathological burden. Specifically, greater
Lewy body density was found in limbic and cortical struc-
tures in PD patients with poor verbal fluency, but higher
AD pathology was on trend in many of the same areas.
Recall Versus Recognition in PD-MCI
PD-MCI-associated deficits in figure copying (g=1.56)
were more pronounced than in recall (immediate g=0.91
and delayed SMD g=1.03 recall), although all ES were
large. Conversely, visuospatial recognition is much less
impaired (Song et al., 2008), as also emerged in the MCI-
LB results. It is possible that complex figure recall is a more
challenging task for both patients and controls, and thus less
difference between groups is observed. The use of retention
scores (the percentage of information lost or gained between
copy and recall conditions) might provide more nuanced
information on differential abilities (Shin, Park, Park, Seol,
& Kwon, 2006). Recognition memory for word list stimuli
seems similarly intact in PD-MCI, in contrast to deficits in
both immediate and delayed recall conditions. This pattern
of dysfunction in both the visuospatial and verbal domains
in PD-MCI suggests impaired retrieval and relatively intact
encoding and storage mechanisms (Shin et al., 2006). This
supports the established retrieval deficit hypothesis of
memory impairment in PD (Tröster & Fields, 1995;
Whittington, Podd, & Kan, 2000), which argues that the cause
220 J. Ciafone et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
of memory impairment is inability to retrieve material on
demand rather than encoding or retention ability (Mahurin,
Feher, Nance, Levy, & Pirozzolo, 1993). Given that a similar
pattern emerged in MCI-LB (Kemp et al., 2017), this model
may apply to other early alpha-synucleinopathies.
However, there has been increasing scrutiny of this hypoth-
esis and evidence of recognition impairment in some patients
(Higginson, Wheelock, Carroll, & Sigvardt, 2005; Whittington
et al., 2000). Whittington et al. (2000) found impairment in
recognition in PD participants without dementia, but not in
newly diagnosed PD patients, and Bronnick, Alves, Aarsland,
Tysnes and Larsen (2011) found that early PD patients
performed poorly on free recall, cued recall, and recognition
memory. Bronnick et al. (2011) attribute this impairment to
encoding failure due to poor EF, rather than to impaired
retrieval. Indeed, PD participants used fewer semantic cluster-
ing strategies to enhance their recall, and both strategy and EF
explained significant variance in learning. Earlier, Gershberg
and Shimamura (1995) and Hirst and Volpe (1988)had
demonstrated that frontal lobe lesion patients were unable to
capitalise on the potential semantic organisation present in
word lists. It is likely that the prominent EF impairments in
PD-MCI at least partly explain poorer performance on tasks
of greater complexity, regardless of domain.
Executive Function Weighting Impairs
Performance in PD-MCI
Impairment was found on only two-thirds of visuospatial
WM measures despite the high proportion of visuospatial
learning and memory tasks (91%) that show deficits in
PD-MCI. One question is why visuoconstructional ability
is impaired but not visuospatial WM? Figure copying such
as ROCF is a complex task that requires EFs such as
sustained attention, planning, and organisation (Shin
et al., 2006), in addition to the visuospatial perception
and processing that is required in visuospatial WM tasks like
Corsi blocks. If executive demands are responsible for this
divergence, this emphasises the methodological importance
of locating the executive processes within tasks with face
validity in other domains. However, even simple WM tasks
such as Digits Forward could be argued to have an EF
component (e.g. chunking), so this issue may speak more
to the debate regarding multicomponent versus unitary
models of WM (Cowan, 1999; Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin,
& Conway, 1999) than to a salient difference in a patient
population.
MCI Classification
Defining MCI neuropsychologically using domain cut-off
scores may also have the potential for circularity in analyses
in which neuropsychological performance is the phenotype
of interest. In the PD-MCI, all but one study (Anderson,
Simpson, Channon, Samuel, & Brown, 2013) defined groups
using neuropsychological criteria. PD-MCI studies tend to
utilise Movement Disorder Society (MDS) diagnostic criteria
for MCI, which is based on neuropsychological cut-off scores
by individual cognitive domain, typically between 1 and 2
SDs. However, the specific magnitude of deficit required
to be considered impairmenthas critical implications.
Dalrymple-Alford et al. (2011) found that while only 14%
of a PD sample was considered PD-MCI when defined as
two SDs below normative scores in at least two tests in a
domain, this number increases to 89% if considered at one
SD or more. In the latter scenario, 70% of clinically defined
HCs were also identified as MCI. To capture the breadth of
impairment, MDS criteria stipulate that cognition should
ideally be measured in five domains by at least two tasks,
but this increases the likelihood of reaching significance on
at least one measure. Few studies correct for multiple compar-
isons. Such a large amount of neuropsychological testing can
also become unwieldy to report in entirety and encourages
selection biases in presenting only significant results. Thus,
choosing a battery poses a problem: decreasing the breadth
may omit relevant individuals, but increased breadth often
leads to contradictory findings within domains. The use of
composite domain scores may help to overcome this meth-
odological challenge and has been suggested to bypass task
idiosyncrasy, avoid multiple comparisons, and increase
power (Crane et al., 2012; Gibbons et al., 2012).
While results suggest similarities in the neuropsychologi-
cal profiles of MCI-LB and PD-MCI by domain, future work
comparing these patient groups directly is warranted. For
example, poorer episodic memory ability might be expected
in MCI-LB given evidence of greater co-occurrence of amy-
loid and tau pathology in DLB versus PDD (Jellinger &
Korczyn, 2018). However, ES of verbal memory impairment
reported in MCI-LB (g=0.58; Kemp et al., 2017) were
almost half that of the pooled estimate for PD-MCI
(g=1.25) in the present study. A potential explanation
for this finding may be the greater variability expected in
DLB (Nelson et al., 2010) and MCI-LB. Indeed, the validity
of such direct comparisons of MCI subtypes may be chal-
lenged given the different definitional criteria for MCI cur-
rently in use.
The present review focused on studies with HC compari-
son groups that would provide closer matching between local
patients and controls. However, this approach does not
assume that important and complementary insights will not
be found in studies using normative data or other relevant
patient groups, such as stable or reverting MCI patients
(see supplementary table). Yarnall et al. (2014), for example,
found a high prevalence of MCI in their incidence cohort and
suggested that prominent executive dysfunction may account
for poor performance in other domains, such as memory
tasks, in line with the present reviews suggestions.
Longitudinal studies also critically offer insight into cogni-
tive trajectories in PD-MCI, and suggested that low baseline
EF, processing speed, visuospatial function, and verbal
memory were predictive of poorer outcomes at follow-up
(e.g. see Pigott et al., 2015; Williams-Gray, Foltynie,
Brayne, Robbins, & Barker, 2007).
Ciafone-Neuropsychology of MCI in Lewy body dementias 221
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
In conclusion, growing interest in MCI due to Lewy body
disease necessitates a broader understanding of cognition in
early disease. The present study aimed to capture existing
literature in neuropsychological functioning by focusing on
studies in MCI-LB and PD-MCI. MCI-LB studies are limited,
but provide evidence for impaired EF, speed of processing,
visuospatial, WM, and attentional abilities in comparison
to controls and MCI-AD. However, results within domains
are at times inconsistent. PD-MCI shows deficits in visuospa-
tial, executive, and verbal tasks. However, these results
should be interpreted with caution due to issues of circularity.
MCI as a stable finding has also been called into question,
with 9% of individuals with established MCI later reverting
to normal cognition (Copeland & Schiess, 2013). Thus, while
PD-MCI may retain utility in clinical settings, it may be inap-
propriate as an a priori definition of a patient group in neuro-
psychological research. Defining MCI clinically [e.g. by
assessing independent function in daily activities, input from
family members, neurological examination, global cognitive
scales (MMSE) to rule out advanced or absent impairment, or
biomarkers], rather than neuropsychologically, will serve to
better capture the cognitive heterogeneity of early LBDs and
allow for more nuanced analyses of the potential interrelated-
ness of deficits.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was supported by the NIHR Newcastle
Biomedical Research Centre, Alzheimers Research UK
(A.T., grant number BH142057) and a Medical Research
Council studentship (J.C., MR/K501396/1).
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
A.T. has received support from GE Healthcare for investigator-
led research. No other authors have conflicts of interest to
disclose.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
REFERENCES
Aarsland, D. (2016). Cognitive impairment in Parkinsons disease
and dementia with Lewy bodies. Parkinsonism & Related
Disorders,22, S144S148.
Aarsland, D., Kurz, M., Beyer, M., Bronnick, K., Piepenstock Nore,
S., & Ballard, C. (2008). Early discriminatory diagnosis of
dementia with Lewy bodies. The emerging role of CSF and
imaging biomarkers. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive
Disorders,25(3), 195205.
Albert, M.S., DeKosky, S.T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman,
H.H., Fox, N.C., Gamst, A., Holtzman, D.M., Jagust, W.J.,
Petersen, R.C., Snyder, P.J., Carrillo, M.C., Thies, B., &
Phelps, C.H. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment
due to Alzheimers disease: Recommendations from the National
Institute on Aging-Alzheimers Association workgroups on diag-
nostic guidelines for Alzheimers disease. Alzheimers&
Dementia,7(3), 270279.
Anderson, R.J., Simpson, A.C., Channon, S., Samuel, M., & Brown,
R.G. (2013). Social problem solving, social cognition, and mild
cognitive impairment in Parkinsons disease. Behavioral
Neuroscience,127(2), 184192.
Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A,49(1), 528.
Baddeley, A.D. & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology
of Learning and Motivation,8,4789.
Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K.D., Varney, N.R., & Spreen, O. (1983).
Judgment of Line Orientation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Biundo, R., Weis, L., Facchini, S., Formento-Dojot, P., Vallelunga,
A., Pilleri, M., & Antonini, A. (2014). Cognitive profiling of
Parkinson disease patients with mild cognitive impairment and
dementia. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders,20(4), 394399.
Bocanegra, Y., García, A.M., Lopera, F., Pineda, D., Baena, A.,
Ospina, P., Alzate, D., Buriticá, O., Moreno, L., Ibá˜nez, A., &
Cuetos, F. (2017). Unspeakable motion: Selective action-verb
impairments in Parkinsons disease patients without mild cogni-
tive impairment. Brain and Language,168,3746.
Boeve, B.F. (2012). Mild cognitive impairment associated with
underlying Alzheimers disease versus Lewy body disease.
Parkinsonism and Related Disorders,18(Suppl. 1), S41S44.
Braak, H., Del Tredici, K., Rüb, U., de Vos, R.A.I., Steur, E.N.H.J.,
& Braak, E. (2003). Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic
Parkinsons disease. Neurobiology of Aging,24(2), 197211.
Bronnick, K., Alves, G., Aarsland, D., Tysnes, O.-B., & Larsen, J.P.
(2011). Verbal memory in drug-naive, newly diagnosed
Parkinsons disease. The retrieval deficit hypothesis revisited.
Neuropsychology,25(1), 114124.
Bryan, J., Luszcz, M.A., & Pointer, S. (1999). Executive function
and processing resources as predictors of adult age differences
in the implementation of encoding strategies. Aging,
Neuropsychology, and Cognition,6(4), 273287.
Burn, D.J. & McKeith, I.G. (2003). Current treatment of dementia
with Lewy bodies and dementia associated with Parkinsons
disease. Movement Disorders,18(S6), 7279.
Busse, A., Hensel, A., Gühne, U., Angermeyer, M., & Riedel-Heller,
S. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment long-term course of four
clinical subtypes. Neurology,67(12), 21762185.
Bussè, C., Caffarra, P., Rossi, A., Zorzi, G., Fragiacomo, F.,
Camporese, G., Pompanin, S., Di Bernardo, G.A. & Cagnin, A.
(2018). Testing hippocampal memory in prodromal dementia with
Lewy bodies. Journal of Alzheimers Disease,64(2), 349353.
Cagnin, A., Bussè, C., Gardini, S., Jelcic, N., Guzzo, C., Gnoato, F.,
Mitolo, M., Ermani, M., & Caffarra, P. (2015). Clinical and
cognitive phenotype of mild cognitive impairment evolving to
dementia with Lewy bodies. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive
Disorders Extra,5(3), 442449.
Centre, T.N.C. (2014). Review Manager (RevMan) [Computer
program] (Version Version 5.3). Copenhagen: The Cochrane
Collaboration.
Copeland, B.J. & Schiess, M.C. (2013). Can mild cognitive impair-
ment in Parkinson disease predict the development of dementia?
JAMA Neurology,70(5), 553555.
Corsi, P.M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region
of the brain. Dissertation Abstracts International,34(819B).
Costa, A., Zabberoni, S., Peppe, A., Serafini, F., Scalici, F.,
Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G.A. (2015). Time-based
222 J. Ciafone et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
prospective memory functioning in mild cognitive impairment
associated with Parkinsons disease: Relationship with autono-
mous management of daily living commitments. Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience,9, 333.
Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working
memory. Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active
Maintenance and Executive Control,20, 506.
Crane, P.K., Carle, A., Gibbons, L.E., Insel, P., Mackin, R.S., Gross,
A., Jones, R.N., Mukherjee, S., Curtis, S.M., Harvey, D., Weiner,
M., Mungas, D., & Alzheimers Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
(2012). Development and assessment of a composite score for
memory in the Alzheimers Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
(ADNI). Brain Imaging and Behavior,6(4), 502516.
Dalrymple-Alford, J.C., Livingston, L., MacAskill, M.R., Graham,
C., Melzer, T.R., Porter, R.J., Watts, R., & Anderson, T.J. (2011).
Characterizing mild cognitive impairment in Parkinsons disease.
Movement Disorders,26(4), 629636.
Darweesh, S.K.L., Verlinden, V.J.A., Stricker, B.H., Hofman, A.,
Koudstaal, P.J., & Ikram, M.A. (2017). Trajectories of prediagnos-
tic functioning in Parkinsons disease. Brain,140(2), 429441.
Deeks, J.J. & Higgins, J.P.T. (2010). Statistical algorithms in review
manager 5. Statistical Methods Group of the Cochrane
Collaboration,1(11), 111.
Donaghy, P.C. & McKeith, I.G. (2014). The clinical characteristics
of dementia with Lewy bodies and a consideration of prodromal
diagnosis. Alzheimers Research & Therapy,6, 46.
Donaghy, P.C., OBrien, J.T., & Thomas, A.J. (2015). Prodromal
dementia with Lewy bodies. Psychological Medicine,45(02),
259268.
Donaghy, P.C., Taylor, J.P., OBrien, J.T., Barnett, N., Olsen, K.,
Colloby, S.J., Lloyd, J., Petrides, G., McKeith, I.G., &
Thomas, A.J. (2018). Neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive
profile in mild cognitive impairment with Lewy bodies.
Psychological Medicine,48(14), 23842390.
Downes, J.J., Priestley, N.M., Doran, M., Ferran, J., Ghadiali, E., &
Cooper, P. (1998). Intellectual, mnemonic, and frontal functions
in dementia with Lewy bodies: A comparison with early and
advanced Parkinsons disease. Behavioural Neurology,11(3),
19981999.
El-Nazer, R., Adler, C.H., Beach, T.G., Belden, C.M., Artz, J.,
Shill, H.A., Driver-Dunckley, E., Mehta, S.H., Sabbagh,
M.N., Serrano, G.E., Sue, L.I., Zamrini, E., & Benge, J.F.
(2019). Regional neuropathology distribution and verbal flu-
ency impairments in Parkinsons disease. Parkinsonism &
Related Disorders.
Engle, R.W., Tuholski, S.W., Laughlin, J.E., & Conway, A.R.A.
(1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid
intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General,128(3), 309.
Flicker, C., Ferris, S.H., & Reisberg, B. (1991). Mild cognitive
impairment in the elderly predictors of dementia. Neurology,
41(7), 10061006.
Fujishiro, H., Nakamura, S., Sato, K., & Iseki, E. (2015). Prodromal
dementia with Lewy bodies. Geriatrics & Gerontology
International,15(7), 817826.
Galtier, I., Nieto, A., Lorenzo, J.N., & Barroso, J. (2016). Mild
cognitive impairment in Parkinsons disease: Diagnosis and
progression to dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology,38(1), 4050.
Gelb, D.J., Oliver, E., & Gilman, S. (1999). Diagnostic criteria for
Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology,56(1), 3339.
Génier Marchand, D., Postuma, R.B., Escudier, F., De Roy, J.,
Pelletier, A., Montplaisir, J., & Gagnon, J.F. (2018). How does
dementia with Lewy bodies start? Prodromal cognitive changes
in REM sleep behavior disorder. Annals of Neurology,83(5),
10161026.
Gershberg, F.B. & Shimamura, A.P. (1995). Impaired use of organi-
zational strategies in free recall following frontal lobe damage.
Neuropsychologia,33(10), 13051333.
Gibbons, L.E., Carle, A.C., Mackin, R.S., Harvey, D., Mukherjee, S.,
Insel, P., Curtis, S.M., Mungas, D., Crane, P.K., & Alzheimers
Disease Neuroimaging. (2012). A composite score for executive
functioning, validated in Alzheimers Disease Neuroimaging
Initiative (ADNI) participants with baseline mild cognitive impair-
ment. Brain Imaging and Behavior,6(4), 517527.
Gnanalingham, K.K., Byrne, E.J., Thornton, A., Sambrook, M.A., &
Bannister, P. (1997). Motor and cognitive function in Lewy body
dementia: Comparison with Alzheimers and Parkinsonsdiseases.
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry,62(3),
243252.
Goldman, J.G., Williams-Gray, C., Barker, R.A., Duda, J.E., &
Galvin, J.E. (2014). The spectrum of cognitive impairment in
Lewy body diseases. Movement Disorders,29(5), 608621.
Hedges, L. & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical Methods for Meta-
Analysis. New York: Academic Press.
Henry, J.D. & Crawford, J.R. (2004). Verbal fluency deficits in
Parkinsons disease: A meta-analysis. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society,10(4), 608622.
Henry, J.D., Crawford, J.R., & Phillips, L.H. (2004). Verbal fluency
performance in dementia of the Alzheimers type: A meta-analysis.
Neuropsychologia,42(9), 12121222.
Hessen, E., Stav, A.L., Auning, E., Selnes, P., Blomsø, L.,
Holmeide, C.E., Johansen, K.K., Eliassen, C.F., Reinvang, I.,
Fladby, T., & Aarsland, D. (2016). Neuropsychological profiles
in mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimers and
Parkinsons diseases. Journal of Parkinsons Disease,6(2),
413421.
Higginson, C.I., Wheelock, V.L., Carroll, K.E., & Sigvardt, K.A.
(2005). Recognition memory in Parkinsons disease with and
without dementia: Evidence inconsistent with the retrieval deficit
hypothesis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology,27(4), 516528.
Hirst, W. & Volpe, B.T. (1988). Memory strategies with brain
damage. Brain and Cognition,8(3), 379408.
Hughes, A., Daniel, S., Kilford, L., & Lees, A. (1992). Accuracy of
clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinsons disease. A clinico-
pathological study of 100 cases. Journal of Neurology,
Neurosurgery & Psychiatry,55, 181184.
Jellinger, K.A. & Korczyn, A.D. (2018). Are dementia with Lewy
bodies and Parkinsons disease dementia the same disease?
BMC Medicine,16(1).
Jicha, G., Schmitt, F., Abner, E., Nelson, P., Cooper, G., Smith, C.,
& Markesbery, W. (2010). Prodromal clinical manifestations of
neuropathologically confirmed Lewy body disease.
Neurobiology of Aging,31(10), 18051813.
Kane, J.P., Surendranathan, A., Bentley, A., Barker, S.A., Taylor,
J.P., Thomas, A.J., Allan, L.M., McNally, R.J., James, P.W.,
McKeith, I.G., & Burn, D.J. (2018). Clinical prevalence of
Lewy body dementia. Alzheimers Research & Therapy,
10(1), 19.
Kemp, J., Philippi, N., Phillipps, C., Demuynck, C., Albasser, T.,
Martin-Hunyadi, C., Schmidt-Mutter, C., Cretin, B., & Blanc, F.
Ciafone-Neuropsychology of MCI in Lewy body dementias 223
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
(2017). Cognitive profile in prodromal dementia with Lewy
bodies. Alzheimers Research & Therapy,9(1), 19.
Lange, K.L., Bondi, M.W., Salmon, D.P., Galasko, D., Delis, D.C.,
Thomas, R.G., & Thal, L.J. (2002). Decline in verbal memory
during preclinical Alzheimers disease: Examination of the effect
of APOE genotype. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society,8(7), 943955.
Lippa, C.F., Duda, J.E., Grossman, M., Hurtig, H.I., Aarsland, D.,
Boeve, B.F., Brooks, D.J., Dickson, D.W., Dubois, B., Emre, M.,
Fahn, S., Farmer, J.M., Galasko, D., Galvin, J.E., Goetz, C.G.,
Growdon, J.H., Gwinn-Hardy, K.A., Hardy, J., Heutink, P.,
Iwatsubo, T., Kosaka, K., Lee, V.M., Leverenz, J.B., Masliah,
E., McKeith, I.G., Nussbaum, R.L., Olanow, C.W., Ravina,
B.M., Singleton, A.B., Tanner, C.M., Trojanowski, J.Q.,
Wszolek, Z.K., & DLB/PDD Working Group (2007). DLB and
PDD boundary issues diagnosis, treatment, molecular pathology,
and biomarkers. Neurology,68(11), 812819.
Litvan, I., Goldman, J.G., Tröster, A.I., Schmand, B.A., Weintraub, D.,
Petersen, R.C., Mollenhauer, B., Adler, C.H., Marder, K., Williams
Gray, C.H., Aarsland, D., Kulisevsky, J., Rodriguez-Oroz, M.C.,
Burn, D.J., Barker, R.A., & Emre, M. (2012). Diagnostic criteria
for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinsons disease: Movement
Disorder Society Task Force guidelines. Movement Disorders,
27(3), 349356.
Logie, R. & Pearson, D. (1997). The inner eye and the inner scribe of
visuo-spatial working memory: Evidence from developmental
fractionation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,9(3),
241257.
MacCarthy, D. (1972). Manual for the McCarthy Scales of
Childrens Abilities. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Mahurin, R.K., Feher, E.P., Nance, M.L., Levy, J.K., & Pirozzolo,
F.J. (1993). Cognition inParkinsons disease and related disorders,
In R.W. Parks, R.F. Zec, & R.S. Wilson (Eds.), Neuropsychology
of Alzheimers disease and other dementias (pp. 308349).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Martin, A., Brouwers, P., Cox, C., & Fedio, P. (1985). On the nature
of the verbal memory deficit in Alzheimers disease. Brain and
Language,25(2), 323341.
Massa, F., Arnaldi, D., De Cesari, F., Girtler, N., Brugnolo, A.,
Grazzini, M., Bauckneht, M., Meli, R., Morbelli, S., Pardini,
M., Sambuceti, G., De Carli, F., Tiraboschi, P., & Nobili, F.
(2019). Neuroimaging findings and clinical trajectories of
Lewy body disease in patients with MCI. Neurobiology of
Aging,76,917.
McKeith, I.G., Boeve, B.F., Dickson, D.W., Halliday, G., Taylor,
J.-P., Weintraub, D., Aarsland, D., Galvin, J., Attems, J.,
Ballard, C.G., Bayston, A., Beach, T.G., Blanc, F., Bohnen,
N., Bonanni, L., Bras, J., Brundin, P., Burn, D., Chen-Plotkin,
A., Duda, J.E., El-Agnaf, O., Feldman, H., Ferman, T.J.,
Ffytche, D., Fujishiro, H., Galasko, D., Goldman, J.G.,
Gomperts, S.N., Graff-Radford, N.R., Honig, L.S., Iranzo, A.,
Kantarci, K., Kaufer, D., Kukull, W., Lee, V.M.Y., Leverenz,
J.B., Lewis, S., Lippa, C., Lunde, A., Masellis, M., Masliah,
E., McLean, P., Mollenhauer, B., Montine, T.J., Moreno, E.,
Mori, E., Murray, M., OBrien, J.T., Orimo, S., Postuma, R.B.,
Ramaswamy, S., Ross, O.A., Salmon, D.P., Singleton, A.,
Taylor, A., Thomas, A., Tiraboschi, P., Toledo, J.B.,
Trojanowski, J.Q., Tsuang, D., Walker, Z., Yamada, M., &
Kosaka, K. (2017). Diagnosis and management of dementia
with Lewy bodies Fourth consensus report of the DLB
Consortium. Neurology,89(1), 88100. doi: 10.1212/WNL.
0000000000004058.
McKeith, I.G., Dickson, D.W., Lowe, J., Emre, M., OBrien, J.T.,
Feldman, H., Cummings, J., Duda, J.E., Lippa, C., Perry, E.K.,
Aarsland, D., Arai, H., Ballard, C.G., Boeve, B., Burn, D.J.,
Costa, D., Del Ser, T., Dubois, B., Galasko, D., Gauthier, S.,
Goetz, C.G., Gomez-Tortosa, E., Halliday, G., Hansen, L.A.,
Hardy, J., Iwatsubo, T., Kalaria, R.N., Kaufer, D., Kenny,
R.A., Korczyn, A., Kosaka, K., Lee, V.M., Lees, A., Litvan, I.,
Londos, E., Lopez, O.L., Minoshima, S., Mizuno, Y., Molina,
J.A., Mukaetova-Ladinska, E.B., Pasquier, F., Perry, R.H.,
Schulz, J.B., Trojanowski, J.Q., Yamada, M., & Consortium on
DLB. (2005). Diagnosis and management of dementia with
Lewy bodies third report of the DLB consortium. Neurology,
65(12), 18631872.
McKeith, I., Taylor, J.P., Thomas, A., Donaghy, P., & Kane, J.
(2016). Revisiting DLB diagnosis: A consideration of prodromal
DLB and of the diagnostic overlap with Alzheimer disease.
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology,29(5), 249253.
Mondon, K., Gochard, A., Marqué, A., Armand, A., Beauchamp,
D., Prunier, C., Jacobi, D., de Toffol, B., Autret, A., Camus,
V., & Hommet, C. (2007). Visual recognition memory differen-
tiates dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinsons disease demen-
tia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry,78(7),
738741.
Montse, A., Pere, V., Carme, J., Francesc, V., & Eduardo, T. (2001).
Visuospatial deficits in parkinsons disease assessed by judgment
of line orientation test: Error analyses and practice effects. Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,23(5), 592598.
Nelson, P.T., Jicha, G.A., Kryscio, R.J., Abner, E.L., Schmitt, F.A.,
Cooper, G., Xu, L.O., Smith, C.D., & Markesbery, W.R. (2010).
Low sensitivity in clinical diagnoses of dementia with Lewy
bodies. Journal of Neurology,257(3), 359366.
Peraza, L.R., Nesbitt, D., Lawson, R.A., Duncan, G.W., Yarnall,
A.J., Khoo, T.K., Kaiser, M., Firbank, M.J., OBrien, J.T.,
Barker, R.A., & Brooks, D. J. (2017). Intra- and inter-network
functional alterations in Parkinsons disease with mild cognitive
impairment. Human Brain Mapping,38(3), 17021715.
Petersen, R.C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic
entity. Journal of Internal Medicine,256(3), 183194.
Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R.J., Kokmen, E.,
& Tangelos, E.G. (1997). Aging, memory, and mild cognitive
impairment. International Psychogeriatrics,9(S1), 6569.
Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R.J., Tangalos,
E.G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment:
Clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology,
56(3), 303308.
Pigott, K., Rick, J., Xie, S.X., Hurtig, H., Chen-Plotkin, A., Duda,
J.E., Morley, J.F., Chahine, L.M., Dahodwala, N., Akhtar, R.S.,
Siderowf, A., Trojanowski, J.Q., & Weintraub, D. (2015).
Longitudinal study of normal cognition in Parkinson disease.
Neurology,85(15), 12761282.
Poletti, M., Frosini, D., Pagni, C., Baldacci, F., Nicoletti, V.,
Tognoni, G., Lucetti, C., Del Dotto, P., Ceravolo, R., &
Bonuccelli, U. (2012). Mild cognitive impairment and cogni-
tive-motor relationships in newly diagnosed drug-naive patients
with Parkinsons disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery
& Psychiatry,83(6), 601606.
Postuma, R.B., Berg, D., Stern, M., Poewe, W., Olanow, C.W.,
Oertel, W., Obeso, J., Marek, K., Litvan, I., Lang, A.E.,
Halliday, G., Goetz, C.G., Gasser, T., Dubois, B., Chan, P.,
Bloem, B.R., Adler, C.H., & Deuschl, G. (2015). MDS clinical
diagnostic criteria for Parkinsons disease. Movement
Disorders,30(12), 15911601.
224 J. Ciafone et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
Robbins, T.W., James, M., Owen, A.M., Sahakian, B.J., Lawrence,
A.D., McInnes, L., & Rabbitt, P.M. (1998). A study of perfor-
mance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal
lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers:
Implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive
aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,
4(5), 474490.
Sadiq, D., Whitfield, T., Lee, L., Stevens, T., Costafreda, S., &
Walker, Z. (2017). Prodromal dementia with Lewy bodies and
prodromal Alzheimers disease: A comparison of the cognitive
and clinical profiles. Journal of Alzheimers Disease,58(2),
463470.
Sanyal, J., Banerjee, T.K., & Rao, V.R. (2014). Dementia and cog-
nitive impairment in patients with Parkinsons disease from India:
A 7-year prospective study. American Journal of Alzheimers
Disease and other Dementias,29(7), 630636.
Shin, M.-S., Park, S.-Y., Park, S.-R., Seol, S.-H., & Kwon, J.S.
(2006). Clinical and empirical applications of the ReyOsterrieth
complex figure test. Nature Protocols,1(2), 892899.
Song, I.-U., Kim, J.-S., Jeong, D.-S., Song, H.-J., & Lee, K.-S.
(2008). Early neuropsychological detection and the characteris-
tics of Parkinsons disease associated with mild dementia.
Parkinsonism & Related Disorders,14(7), 558562.
Thomas, A.J., Hendriksen, M., Piggott, M., Ferrier, I.N., Perry, E.,
Ince, P., & OBrien, J.T. (2006). A study of the serotonin
transporter in the prefrontal cortex in latelife depression
and Alzheimers disease with and without depression.
Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology,32(3), 296303.
Tröster, A.I. & Fields, J.A. (1995). Frontal cognitive function and
memory in Parkinsons disease: Toward a distinction between
prospective and declarative memory impairments? Behavioural
Neurology,8(2), 5974.
Troyer, A.K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, M.P., &
Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency:
The effects of focal frontal-and temporal-lobe lesions.
Neuropsychologia,36(6), 499504.
Wang, Y.Q., Tang, B.S., Yan, X.X., Chen, Z.H., Xu, Q., Liu,
Z.H., Wang, K., & Guo, J.F. (2015). A neurophysiological pro-
file in Parkinsons disease with mild cognitive impairment and
dementia in China. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience,22(6),
981985.
Whittington, C.J., Podd, J., & Kan, M.M. (2000). Recognition
memory impairment in Parkinsons disease: power and meta-
analyses. Neuropsychology,14(2), 233.
Williams-Gray, C., Foltynie, T., Brayne, C., Robbins, T., & Barker,
R. (2007). Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident
Parkinsons disease cohort. Brain: A Journal of Neurology,
130(7), 17871798.
Yarnall, A.J., Breen, D.P., Duncan, G.W., Khoo, T.K., Coleman, S.Y.,
Firbank, M.J., Nombela, C., Winder-Rhodes, S., Evans, J.R., Rowe,
J.B., Mollenhauer, B., Kruse, N., Hudson, G., Chinnery, P.F.,
OBrien, J.T., Robbins, T.W., Wesnes, K., Brooks, D.J., Barker,
R.A., Burn, D.J., & ICICLE-PD Study Group (2014).
Characterizing mild cognitive impairment in incident Parkinson dis-
ease: The ICICLE-PD Study. Neurology,82(4), 308316.
Yoon, J.H., Kim, M., Moon, S.Y., Yong, S.W., & Hong, J.M.
(2015). Olfactory function and neuropsychological profile to dif-
ferentiate dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimers disease
in patients with mild cognitive impairment: A 5-year follow-up
study. Journal of the Neurological Sciences,355(12), 174179.
Yu, R.L., Wu, R.M., Tai, C.H., Lin, C.H., Cheng, T.W., & Hua,
M.S. (2012). Neuropsychological profile in patients with early
stage of Parkinsons disease in Taiwan. Parkinsonism &
Related Disorders,18(10), 10671072.
Ciafone-Neuropsychology of MCI in Lewy body dementias 225
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001103
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Newcastle University, on 31 Mar 2020 at 15:10:21, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms.
... The cognitive profile of DLB is typically marked by greater deficits in attention, executive functioning, and visuospatial abilities relative to AD, which is typically characterised by impairment in memory [44,45]. Similar patterns have been observed in MCI-LB and MCI-AD, respectively [29,44,46,47]. Amnestic MCI is characterised by a predominant impairment in memory, while nonamnestic MCI is characterised by impairment in non-memory domains, including visuospatial abilities and executive functions. ...
... Visuospatial problems may manifest as pareidolic misperceptions [37,40,49]. Some individuals with MCI-LB may also demonstrate reduced verbal fluency [40], inattentive errors on timed tasks [29], slowed processing speed [46], with cognitive impairment, psychiatric symptoms, and/ or recurrent episodes of delirium. Therefore, pDLB is an umbrella term which includes, but is not limited to, presentations with mild cognitive impairment and executive dysfunction [27,46,50]. ...
... Some individuals with MCI-LB may also demonstrate reduced verbal fluency [40], inattentive errors on timed tasks [29], slowed processing speed [46], with cognitive impairment, psychiatric symptoms, and/ or recurrent episodes of delirium. Therefore, pDLB is an umbrella term which includes, but is not limited to, presentations with mild cognitive impairment and executive dysfunction [27,46,50]. Individuals with MCI-LB may demonstrate memory impairment relative to cognitively healthy older adults [29,50]; however, memory impairments in MCI-LB are typically less severe compared to MCI-AD, often with preserved recognition of previously learned material [27,47]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This pragmatic review synthesises the current understanding of prodromal dementia with Lewy bodies (pDLB) and prodromal Alzheimer’s disease (pAD), including clinical presentations, neuropsychological profiles, neuropsychiatric symptoms, biomarkers, and indications for disease management. The core clinical features of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB)—parkinsonism, complex visual hallucinations, cognitive fluctuations, and REM sleep behaviour disorder are common prodromal symptoms. Supportive clinical features of pDLB include severe neuroleptic sensitivity, as well as autonomic and neuropsychiatric symptoms. The neuropsychological profile in mild cognitive impairment attributable to Lewy body pathology (MCI-LB) tends to include impairment in visuospatial skills and executive functioning, distinguishing it from MCI due to AD, which typically presents with impairment in memory. pDLB may present with cognitive impairment, psychiatric symptoms, and/or recurrent episodes of delirium, indicating that it is not necessarily synonymous with MCI-LB. Imaging, fluid and other biomarkers may play a crucial role in differentiating pDLB from pAD. The current MCI-LB criteria recognise low dopamine transporter uptake using positron emission tomography or single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), loss of REM atonia on polysomnography, and sympathetic cardiac denervation using meta-iodobenzylguanidine SPECT as indicative biomarkers with slowing of dominant frequency on EEG among others as supportive biomarkers. This review also highlights the emergence of fluid and skin-based biomarkers. There is little research evidence for the treatment of pDLB, but pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatments for DLB may be discussed with patients. Non-pharmacological interventions such as diet, exercise, and cognitive stimulation may provide benefit, while evaluation and management of contributing factors like medications and sleep disturbances are vital. There is a need to expand research across diverse patient populations to address existing disparities in clinical trial participation. In conclusion, an early and accurate diagnosis of pDLB or pAD presents an opportunity for tailored interventions, improved healthcare outcomes, and enhanced quality of life for patients and care partners.
... Deficits in spatial orientation and spatial memory are early signs of cognitive decline, e.g., in Alzheimer's disease (AD, [1,2]) and other forms of dementia [3,4]. Since they often are the first cognitive symptoms with a long time span of preclinical stages of dementia [5] in which therapeutical interventions might be possible, the importance of testing spatial orientation and memory becomes clinically relevant. ...
... ", items 6,7,8,10,13), questions on absolute self-assessed function (e.g., "I am very good at judging distances. ", items 1, 2, 3,4,9,10,14), and questions where the score does not necessarily indicate higher or lower orientational performance but possibly individual preferences (e.g., "I tend to think of my environment in terms of cardinal directions (N, S, E, W). ", items 5,12,15). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Deficits in spatial memory, orientation, and navigation are often neglected early signs of cognitive impairment or loss of vestibular function. Real-world navigation tests require complex setups. In contrast, simple pointing at targets in a three-dimensional environment is a basic sensorimotor ability which provides an alternative measure of spatial orientation and memory at bedside. The aim of this study was to test the reliability of a previously established 3D-Real-World Pointing Test (3D-RWPT) in patients with cognitive impairment due to different neurodegenerative disorders, bilateral vestibulopathy, or a combination of both compared to healthy participants. Methods The 3D-RWPT was performed using a static array of targets in front of the seated participant before and, as a transformation task, after a 90-degree body rotation around the yaw-axis. Three groups of patients were enrolled: (1) chronic bilateral vestibulopathy (BVP) with normal cognition (n = 32), (2) cognitive impairment with normal vestibular function (n = 28), and (3) combined BVP and cognitive impairment (n = 9). The control group consisted of age-matched participants (HP) without cognitive and vestibular deficits (n = 67). Analyses focused on paradigm-specific mean angular deviation of pointing in the azimuth (horizontal) and polar (vertical) spatial planes, of the preferred pointing strategy (egocentric or allocentric), and the resulting shape configuration of the pointing array relative to the stimulus array. Statistical analysis was performed using age-corrected ANCOVA-testing with Bonferroni correction and correlation analysis using Spearman’s rho. Results Patients with cognitive impairment employed more egocentric pointing strategies while patients with BVP but normal cognition and HP used more world-based solutions (pBonf 5.78 × 10-3**). Differences in pointing accuracy were only found in the azimuth plane, unveiling unique patterns where patients with cognitive impairment showed decreased accuracy in the transformation tasks of the 3D-RWPT (pBonf < 0.001***) while patients with BVP struggled in the post-rotation tasks (pBonf < 0.001***). Overall azimuth pointing performance was still adequate in some patients with BVP but significantly decreased when combined with a cognitive deficit. Conclusion The 3D-RWPT provides a simple and fast measure of spatial orientation and memory. Cognitive impairment often led to a shift from world-based allocentric pointing strategy to an egocentric performance with less azimuth accuracy compared to age-matched controls. This supports the view that cognitive deficits hinder the mental buildup of the stimulus pattern represented as a geometrical form. Vestibular hypofunction negatively affected spatial memory and pointing performance in the azimuth plane. The most severe spatial impairments (angular deviation, figure frame configuration) were found in patients with combined cognitive and vestibular deficits.
... In both PD and DLB, individuals at the dementia and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stages often experience symptoms such as anxiety, apathy, depression, delusions and hallucinations [5,10,11], and share similar cognitive deficits, such as attention impairments and executive dysfunction [4,12,13]. The relatively high prevalence of NPS within LBD groups compared with other common dementia subtypes, such as Alzheimer's disease (AD) [4,8], suggests that such symptoms, rather than simply reflecting a comorbidity of cognitive decline, or a consequence of living with dementia, may result from specific aetiological mechanisms underlying the LBD dementia syndrome. ...
... This may suggest that despite the limited relationship between cognition and individual NPS, overall neuropsychiatric burden may be an additional marker of disease severity, in line with similar findings in PD and AD [3,5,6]. Furthermore, anxiety sub-scores demonstrated an association with poorer cognitive performance, both at baseline and across time points, in the LBD related domains of attention and visuospatial function [4,13]. Despite evidence suggesting anxiety may be a risk factor of incident cognitive decline in older adults [20], studies in DLB and MCI have often failed to find associations between anxiety and global cognitive decline in the early stages of dementia [21]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) in Lewy body dementias (LBD) occur frequently and early in disease progression. Such symptoms are associated with worse quality of life, caregiver burden and functional limitations. Limited evidence exists, however, outlining the longitudinal relationship between NPS and cognitive decline in prodromal LBD. Methods: 123 participants were derived from three cohort studies. Patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) relating to probable dementia with Lewy bodies (MCI-LB, n = 67) and Parkinson's disease (PD-MCI, n = 56) completed comprehensive cognitive and neuropsychiatric assessment and were followed up longitudinally. Linear regression and mixed effects models assessed the relationship between baseline NPS and cognition at baseline and over time. Results: In MCI-LB, overall NPS burden was associated with declines over time in executive function (p = 0.026) and processing speed (p = 0.028) and baseline aberrant motor behaviour was associated with declines in attention (p < 0.025). Anxiety was significantly associated with poorer visuospatial functioning (p = 0.016) at baseline and poorer attention both at baseline (p = 0.017) and across time points (p = 0.024). In PD-MCI, psychosis was associated with poorer executive functioning at baseline (p = 0.008) and across time points (p = 0.002) but had no association with changes longitudinally. Conclusions: Core neuropsychiatric components of LBD are not strongly associated with cognition in prodromal disease. This may suggest that neuropathological mechanisms underlying NPS may not be the same as those underlying cognitive impairment. Non-core NPS, however, may be more directly associated with cognitive change. Future studies utilising neuroimaging techniques are needed to explore the neuropathological basis of NPS in prodromal LBD.
... In both PD and DLB, individuals at the dementia and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stages often experience symptoms such as anxiety, apathy, depression, delusions and hallucinations [5,10,11], and share similar cognitive deficits, such as attention impairments and executive dysfunction [4,12,13]. The relatively high prevalence of NPS within LBD groups compared with other common dementia subtypes, such as Alzheimer's disease (AD) [4,8], suggests that such symptoms, rather than simply reflecting a comorbidity of cognitive decline, or a consequence of living with dementia, may result from specific aetiological mechanisms underlying the LBD dementia syndrome. ...
... This may suggest that despite the limited relationship between cognition and individual NPS, overall neuropsychiatric burden may be an additional marker of disease severity, in line with similar findings in PD and AD [3,5,6]. Furthermore, anxiety sub-scores demonstrated an association with poorer cognitive performance, both at baseline and across time points, in the LBD related domains of attention and visuospatial function [4,13]. Despite evidence suggesting anxiety may be a risk factor of incident cognitive decline in older adults [20], studies in DLB and MCI have often failed to find associations between anxiety and global cognitive decline in the early stages of dementia [21]. ...
Article
Background Neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) in Lewy body diseases, including dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and Parkinson’s disease (PD) dementia, occur frequently and at an early stage in disease progression. Such symptoms are significantly associated with quality of life, caregiver burden and functional limitations. Limited evidence exists, however, outlining the relationship between NPS and cognitive decline. Methods Data from 217 participants were derived from three cohort studies. Patients diagnosed with either MCI with Lewy bodies (MCI‐LB, n = 67), PD‐MCI (n = 56) or Alzheimer’s disease (MCI‐AD, n = 39) and a group of healthy older adults (n = 55) completed comprehensive neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric assessment and were followed up longitudinally for a median of three years. NPS identified at baseline by the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) were included as predictors in linear regressions and linear mixed effects models to assess the relationship between NPS and performance on a range of cognitive tests both at baseline and longitudinally. Results At baseline, NPS were most prevalent and severe among the MCI‐LB group (49% with NPI score >4 in at least one symptom) and least prevalent in controls (7% with NPI score >4). Total NPI score was not related to baseline cognitive function in any domain in any group. Greater baseline total NPI score was associated with steeper rates of decline among PD‐MCI patients on a measure of processing speed (p = .01). Among healthy adults, only sleep disturbance was associated with steeper rates of decline on global cognition (p = .005). None of the patient groups showed any association between individual NPS and decline on a measure of global cognitive function. Conclusions The lack of a significant relationship found between NPS and either global cognitive function or decline in global cognition seen among MCI patients suggests that at this prodromal stage of disease, NPS appear to be of limited clinical impact and indicates that the mechanisms of cognitive and neuropsychiatric symptoms may differ in early Lewy body disease. Future work will investigate these mechanisms using multimodal imaging data from this cohort.
... This study had certain limitations that should be acknowledged. First, the MCI classification model in this study did not encompass a diverse range of individuals with varying racial characteristics [77] or include those with neurodegenerative diseases such as Parkinson disease [78] and Lewy body diseases [79], which could potentially impact cognitive impairment in older adults. Future studies investigating these aspects may aid in the development of screening tools for neurodegenerative diseases among older adults, expanding the potential user pool to include different racial groups. ...
Article
Background Early detection of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), a transitional stage between normal aging and Alzheimer disease, is crucial for preventing the progression of dementia. Virtual reality (VR) biomarkers have proven to be effective in capturing behaviors associated with subtle deficits in instrumental activities of daily living, such as challenges in using a food-ordering kiosk, for early detection of MCI. On the other hand, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) biomarkers have demonstrated their efficacy in quantifying observable structural brain changes that can aid in early MCI detection. Nevertheless, the relationship between VR-derived and MRI biomarkers remains an open question. In this context, we explored the integration of VR-derived and MRI biomarkers to enhance early MCI detection through a multimodal learning approach. Objective We aimed to evaluate and compare the efficacy of VR-derived and MRI biomarkers in the classification of MCI while also examining the strengths and weaknesses of each approach. Furthermore, we focused on improving early MCI detection by leveraging multimodal learning to integrate VR-derived and MRI biomarkers. Methods The study encompassed a total of 54 participants, comprising 22 (41%) healthy controls and 32 (59%) patients with MCI. Participants completed a virtual kiosk test to collect 4 VR-derived biomarkers (hand movement speed, scanpath length, time to completion, and the number of errors), and T1-weighted MRI scans were performed to collect 22 MRI biomarkers from both hemispheres. Analyses of covariance were used to compare these biomarkers between healthy controls and patients with MCI, with age considered as a covariate. Subsequently, the biomarkers that exhibited significant differences between the 2 groups were used to train and validate a multimodal learning model aimed at early screening for patients with MCI among healthy controls. Results The support vector machine (SVM) using only VR-derived biomarkers achieved a sensitivity of 87.5% and specificity of 90%, whereas the MRI biomarkers showed a sensitivity of 90.9% and specificity of 71.4%. Moreover, a correlation analysis revealed a significant association between MRI-observed brain atrophy and impaired performance in instrumental activities of daily living in the VR environment. Notably, the integration of both VR-derived and MRI biomarkers into a multimodal SVM model yielded superior results compared to unimodal SVM models, achieving higher accuracy (94.4%), sensitivity (100%), specificity (90.9%), precision (87.5%), and F1-score (93.3%). Conclusions The results indicate that VR-derived biomarkers, characterized by their high specificity, can be valuable as a robust, early screening tool for MCI in a broader older adult population. On the other hand, MRI biomarkers, known for their high sensitivity, excel at confirming the presence of MCI. Moreover, the multimodal learning approach introduced in our study provides valuable insights into the improvement of early MCI detection by integrating a diverse set of biomarkers.
... Brønnick et al. (2016),Ciafone et al. (2020),Ferman et al. (2013),Hamilton et al. (2021a, b, c),Kemp et al. (2017a, b),Martini et al. (2020), Pigott et al. (2015), Sadiq et al. (2017), Tiraboschi et al. (2006), Wallace et al. (2022), Williams-Gray et al. (2007), Yoon et al. (2015) Impaired attention, executive function, visuospatial deficits, less memory impairment, daytime sleepiness, subtle parkinsonism, neuropsychiatric symptoms Cagnin et al. (2015), Ferman et al. (2018), Payne et al. (2022) Autonomic symptoms (gastrointestinal), RBD, episodic confusion, hyp(an) osmia, olfactory dysfunction Belden et al. (2015), Donaghy et al. (2015), Hamilton et al. (2022), Kasanuki et al. (2017), Liu et al. (2021), Thomas et al. (2022a, b) Autonomic symptoms, photophobia, slowed cognition, behavioral disorders Blanc et al. (2022), Hamilton et al. (2022) Deficits visual attention, oscillations, autonomic features, EF deficits, RBD, oscillations, soft extrapyramidal symptoms Cagnin et al. (2015), Galvin et al. (2021), Payne et al. (2022) Other symptoms Deficits executive function, attention, non-amnestic symptoms, depression Szeto et al. (2017) Frequent autonomic symptoms (gastrointestinal, cardiovascular, hypotension), RBD Hamilton et al. (2022), Liu et al. (2022), Oka et al. (2020) Fluctuating attention, episodes of confusion, RBD, neuropsychiatric symptoms, less memory impairment Donaghy et al. (2017) Color vision impairment Flanigan et al. (2018) Progressive fluent aphasia, anomic aphasia Watanabe et al. (2023) EF, attention, hallucinations Hansen et al. (2022), Utsumi et al. (2021) Slower non-decision times, impaired perceptional encoding/visual performance ...
Article
Full-text available
Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), the second most common degenerative neurocognitive disorder after Alzheimer disease (AD), is frequently preceded by a period of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), in which cognitive decline is associated with impairment of executive functions/attention, visuospatial deficits, or other cognitive domains and a variety of noncognitive and neuropsychiatric symptoms, many of which are similar but less severe than in prodromal AD. While 36–38% remain in the MCI state, at least the same will convert to dementia. Biomarkers are slowing of the EEG rhythms, atrophy of hippocampus and nucleus basalis of Meynert, temporoparietal hypoperfusion, signs of degeneration of the nigrostriatal dopaminergic, cholinergic and other neurotransmitter systems, and inflammation. Functional neuroimaging studies revealed disturbed connectivity of frontal and limbic networks associated with attention and cognitive controls, dopaminergic and cholinergic circuits manifested prior to overt brain atrophy. Sparse neuropathological data showed varying Lewy body and AD-related stages associated with atrophy of entorhinal, hippocampal, and mediotemporal cortices. Putative pathomechanisms of MCI are degeneration of limbic, dopaminergic, and cholinergic systems with Lewy pathology affecting specific neuroanatomical pathways associated with progressing AD-related lesions, but many pathobiological mechanisms involved in the development of MCI in LBD remain to be elucidated as a basis for early diagnosis and future adequate treatment modalities to prevent progression of this debilitating disorder.
... O uso mais difundido das tarefas de orientação de linhas é mediante o JLOT e esta é considerada uma medida relativamente pura da capacidade de julgamento visuoespacial (Benton, 1994). Apesar de haver um importante viés de lateralização no JLOT (Treccani & Cubelli, 2011), tarefas semelhantes parecem ser capazes de identificar déficits na PE de pacientes com CCL misto e disexecutivo (Wasserman et al., 2020), CCL por Doença de Parkinson (Ciafone et al., 2020) e no diagnóstico diferencial entre CCL por Corpos de Lewy e CCL por DA, com o desempenho do primeiro significativamente mais baixo que o do segundo (Hamilton et al., 2021). Tendo em vista a relação de regiões parietais e parieto-occipitais com tarefas de orientação de linhas (Thiyagesh et al., 2009;Tranel et al., 2009), os resultados do presente estudo indicam que essas regiões não apenas estão relacionadas à PE, mas poderiam indicar um possível comprometimento das mesmas regiões ainda na fase prodrômica da DA. ...
Article
Full-text available
O Comprometimento Cognitivo Leve amnésico (CCLa) é a fase prodrômica da Doença de Alzheimer (DA). Embora as habilidades visuoespaciais (HVs) estejam comprometidas em vários tipos de demências, pouco se sabe sobre como essa função é afetada no CCLa. A Bateria para Habilidades Visuoespaciais (BVA) avalia vários aspectos das HVs por meio de oito subtestes, enquanto busca manter a mínima interferência de outras funções cognitivas. A BVA inclui a mensuração da Percepção Espacial (PE), a qual se caracteriza por tarefas mais elementares, que exigem o mínimo possível de demais processos cognitivos e ainda, a mensuração do Pensamento Espacial (PsE), o qual demanda habilidades mais complexas que requerem integração e manipulação da informação espacial. O objetivo do presente estudo foi avaliar as HVs de adultos maiores com CCLa e verificar quais aspectos dessa função são afetados pela condição. Doze participantes (60-90 anos) com CCLa e treze (60-84 anos) cognitivamente saudáveis (grupo saudável-GS) passaram por uma avaliação neuropsicológica, a qual incluiu a aplicação da BVA. Quando comparados ao GS, os participantes com CCLa tiveram desempenho significativamente menor em dois subtestes que examinam a PE (orientação da linha e posição dos pontos) e todos os subtestes que examinam o PsE (rotação mental, figura complexa, figura escondida e construção mental). Em conclusão, os resultados do presente estudo sugerem que as HVs parecem estar comprometidas em pacientes com CCLa, sendo o subdomínio de PsE foi o mais afetado. Portanto, recomenda-se que o PsE seja avaliado para detectar de forma precoce essa condição.
Article
Full-text available
The Free and Cued Selective Reminding test (FCSRT) was used to assess memory in 19 patients with prodromal dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and 25 Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients. DLB scored better than AD in selective measures of the FCSRT: immediate total recall (p = 0.01) and index of sensitivity of cueing (p = 0.001), while free delayed and total memory scores were similarly impaired. The index of sensitivity of cueing held a sensitivity of 76% and specificity of 79% in distinguishing DLB. FCSRT could help in disentangling hippocampal memory deficits from memory impairment due to ineffective recall strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and Parkinson's disease dementia (PDD), which share many clinical, neurochemical, and morphological features, have been incorporated into DSM-5 as two separate entities of major neurocognitive disorders with Lewy bodies. Despite clinical overlap, their diagnosis is based on an arbitrary distinction concerning the time of onset of motor and cognitive symptoms, namely as early cognitive impairment in DLB and later onset following that of motor symptoms in PDD. Their morphological hallmarks - cortical and subcortical α-synuclein/Lewy body plus β-amyloid and tau pathologies - are similar, but clinical differences at onset suggest some dissimilar profiles. Based on recent publications, including the fourth consensus report of the DLB Consortium, a critical overview is provided herein. Discussion: The clinical constellations of DLB and PDD include cognitive impairment, parkinsonism, visual hallucinations, and fluctuating attention. Intravitam PET and postmortem studies have revealed a more pronounced cortical atrophy, elevated cortical and limbic Lewy body pathologies, higher Aβ and tau loads in cortex and striatum in DLB compared to PDD, and earlier cognitive defects in DLB. Conversely, multitracer PET studies have shown no differences in cortical and striatal cholinergic and dopaminergic deficits. Clinical management of both DLB and PDD includes cholinesterase inhibitors and other pharmacologic and non-drug strategies, yet with only mild symptomatic effects. Currently, no disease-modifying therapies are available. Conclusion: DLB and PDD are important dementia syndromes that overlap in many clinical features, genetics, neuropathology, and management. They are currently considered as subtypes of an α-synuclein-associated disease spectrum (Lewy body diseases), from incidental Lewy body disease and non-demented Parkinson's disease to PDD, DLB, and DLB with Alzheimer's disease at the most severe end. Cognitive impairment in these disorders is induced not only by α-synuclein-related neurodegeneration but by multiple regional pathological scores. Both DLB and PDD show heterogeneous pathology and neurochemistry, suggesting that they share important common underlying molecular pathogenesis with Alzheimer's disease and other proteinopathies. While we prefer to view DLB and PDD as extremes on a continuum, there remains a pressing need to more clearly differentiate these syndromes and to understand the synucleinopathy processes leading to either one.
Article
Full-text available
Background: The prevalence of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and dementia in Parkinson's disease (PDD) in routine clinical practice is unclear. Prevalence rates observed in clinical and population-based cohorts and neuropathological studies vary greatly. Small sample sizes and methodological factors in these studies limit generalisability to clinical practice. Methods: We investigated prevalence in a case series across nine secondary care services over an 18-month period, to determine how commonly DLB and PDD cases are diagnosed and reviewed within two regions of the UK. Results: Patients with DLB comprised 4.6% (95% CI 4.0-5.2%) of all dementia cases. DLB was represented in a significantly higher proportion of dementia cases in services in the North East (5.6%) than those in East Anglia (3.3%; χ2= 13.6, p < 0.01). DLB prevalence in individual services ranged from 2.4 to 5.9%. PDD comprised 9.7% (95% CI 8.3-11.1%) of Parkinson's disease cases. No significant variation in PDD prevalence was observed between regions or between services. Conclusions: We found that the frequency of clinical diagnosis of DLB varied between geographical regions in the UK, and that the prevalence of both DLB and PDD was much lower than would be expected in this case series, suggesting considerable under-diagnosis of both disorders. The significant variation in DLB diagnostic rates between these two regions may reflect true differences in disease prevalence, but more likely differences in diagnostic practice. The systematic introduction of more standardised diagnostic practice could improve the rates of diagnosis of both conditions.
Article
Background: Verbal fluency deficits are common in patients with Parkinson's disease. The association of these impairments with regional neuropathological changes is unexplored. Objectives: Determine if patients with verbal fluency impairments have greater neuropathological burden in frontal, temporal, and limbic regions and if Lewy bodies or neurofibrillary tangles were associated with verbal fluency impairments. Methods: Data was derived from the Arizona Study of Aging and Neurodegenerative Disorders. 47 individuals who completed phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks and met clinicopathological criteria for Parkinson's disease (with and without comorbid Alzheimer's disease) were included. Impairment on fluency tasks was defined by normative data, and the density of neuropathology in temporal, limbic, and frontal regions was compared between groups. Results: Individuals with semantic fluency impairments had greater total pathology (Lewy bodies + neurofibrillary tangles) in limbic structures (W = 320.0, p = .033, rpb = .33), while those who had phonemic fluency impairments had increased total neuropathology in frontal (W = 364.5, p = .011, rpb = .37), temporal (W = 356.5, p = .022, rpb = .34), and limbic regions (W = 357.0, p = .024, rpb = .34). Greater Lewy body density was found in those with verbal fluency impairments, though trends for greater neurofibrillary tangle density were noted as well. Conclusions: Impaired phonemic fluency was associated with higher Lewy body and tangle burden in frontal, temporal, and limbic regions, while impaired semantic fluency was associated with greater limbic pathology. Though neurofibrillary tangles trended higher in several regions in those with impaired verbal fluency, higher Lewy body density in general was associated with verbal fluency deficits. Implications for research and clinical practice are discussed.
Article
Elderly patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) may develop a Lewy body disease; their neuroimaging features at presentation are largely unknown. We present an intriguing group of 13 patients with MCI preceding (2.9 ± 1.9 years) parkinsonism (MCI-P), and eventually dementia 4.6 ± 1.6 years later (6 patients), whereas 7 patients remained dementia free after 4.7 ± 2.7 years. Neuropsychological tests, dopamine transporter (DAT) single photon emission computed tomography, and ¹⁸F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography were compared with healthy controls and with cognitively normal patients with Parkinson's disease (PD-MOT). Compared to controls, MCI-P but not PD-MOT showed significant posterior temporo-parieto-occipital hypometabolism. Basal ganglia DAT uptake was similar between MCI-P and PD-MOT. Patients who converted to dementia were older, tended to have higher movement disorder society-unified Parkinson's disease rating scale scores and developed at least another clinical core feature fulfilling the criteria for probable dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). Concurrent impairment of Corsi span and semantic verbal fluency, or of temporal lobe hypometabolism at baseline and reduced putamen-to-caudate ratio on DAT-SPECT at parkinsonism onset, both predicted (p < 0.001) the evolution to dementia. The constructs of Park cognitive subtype and prodromal Lewy body dementia partially overlap; functional imaging and neuropsychology may help in characterizing the patients and in tracking the risk toward dementia.
Article
Objective: We describe the progression of cognitive decline and identify the predictive values of cognitive tests in three groups of REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD) patients classified at their last follow-up as having Parkinson's disease (PD), dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), or still-idiopathic. Methods: Patients (n = 109) underwent polysomnographic, neurological, and neuropsychological assessments. We used linear mixed-model analyses to compare the progression of cognitive test performance between the three groups over a 3-year prodromal period, and performed linear regressions for a 6-year prodromal period. We compared the proportions of patients with clinically impaired performance (z scores < -1.5). DLB patients were pair-matched according to age, sex, and education to healthy controls (2:1 ratio), and receiver operating characteristic curves were performed to identify the psychometric properties of cognitive tests to predict dementia. Results: At follow-up, 38 patients (35%) developed a neurodegenerative disorder: 20 had PD and 18 DLB. Cognitive performance changes over time were strongly associated with later development of dementia. Clear deficits in attention and executive functions were observed 6 years before diagnosis. Verbal episodic learning and memory deficits started later, deviating from normal approximately 5 to 6 years and becoming clinically impaired at 1 to 2 years before diagnosis. Visuospatial abilities progressed variably, with inconsistent prodromal latencies. The Trail Making Test (part B), Verbal Fluency (semantic), and Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (total, immediate, and delayed recalls) were the best predictors for dementia (area under the curve = 0.90-0.97). Interpretation: Prodromal DLB is detectible up to 6 years before onset. For clinical utility, the Trail Making Test (part B) best detects early prodromal dementia stages, whereas Verbal Fluency (semantic) and verbal episodic learning tests are best for monitoring changes over time. Ann Neurol 2018;83:1016-1026.
Article
Background: The prevalence of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and dementia in Parkinson’s disease (PDD) in routine clinical practice is unclear. Prevalence rates observed in clinical and population-based cohorts and neuropathological studies vary greatly. Small sample sizes and methodological factors in these studies limit generalisability to clinical practice. Method: We investigated prevalence in a case series across nine secondary care services over an 18-month period, to determine how commonly DLB and PDD cases are diagnosed and reviewed within two regions of the UK. Results: Patients with DLB comprised 4.6% (95% CI 4.0-5.2%) of all dementia cases. DLB was represented in a significantly higher proportion of dementia cases in services in the North East (5.6%) than those in East Anglia (3.3%; χ2=13.6, p<0.01). DLB prevalence in individual services ranged from 2.4% to 5.9%. PDD comprised 9.7% (95% CI 8.3-11.1%) of PD cases. No significant variation in PDD prevalence was observed between regions or between services. Conclusion: We found that the frequency of clinical diagnosis of DLB varied between geographical regions in the UK, and that the prevalence of both DLB and PDD were much lower than would be expected in this case series, suggesting considerable under-diagnosis of both disorders. The significant variation in DLB diagnostic rates between these two regions may reflect true differences in disease prevalence, but more likely differences in diagnostic practice. The systematic introduction of more standardised diagnostic practice could improve the rates of diagnosis of both conditions.
Article
Background The accurate clinical characterisation of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is becoming increasingly important. The aim of this study was to compare the neuropsychiatric symptoms and cognitive profile of MCI with Lewy bodies (MCI-LB) with Alzheimer's disease MCI (MCI-AD). Methods Participants were ⩾60 years old with MCI. Each had a thorough clinical and neuropsychological assessment and 2β-carbomethoxy-3β-(4-iodophenyl)-N-(3-fluoropropyl)-nortropane single photon emission computed tomography FP-CIT SPECT). MCI-LB was diagnosed if two or more diagnostic features of dementia with Lewy bodies were present (visual hallucinations, cognitive fluctuations, motor parkinsonism, rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder or positive FP-CIT SPECT). A Lewy body Neuropsychiatric Supportive Symptom Count (LBNSSC) was calculated based on the presence or absence of the supportive neuropsychiatric symptoms defined by the 2017 DLB diagnostic criteria: non-visual hallucinations, delusions, anxiety, depression and apathy. Results MCI-LB ( n = 41) had a higher LBNSSC than MCI-AD ( n = 24; 1.8 ± 1.1 v. 0.7 ± 0.9, p = 0.001). 67% of MCI-LB had two or more of those symptoms, compared with 16% of MCI-AD (Likelihood ratio = 4.2, p < 0.001). MCI-LB subjects scored lower on tests of attention, visuospatial function and verbal fluency. However, cognitive test scores alone did not accurately differentiate MCI-LB from MCI-AD. Conclusions MCI-LB is associated with neuropsychiatric symptoms and a cognitive profile similar to established DLB. This supports the concept of identifying MCI-LB based on the presence of core diagnostic features of DLB and abnormal FP-CIT SPECT imaging. The presence of supportive neuropsychiatric clinical features identified in the 2017 DLB diagnostic criteria was helpful in differentiating between MCI-LB and MCI-AD.