ArticlePDF Available

Correlates and antecedents of theory of mind development during middle childhood and adolescence: An integrated model

Authors:

Abstract

Theory of Mind (ToM) is one of the core abilities that allows children to connect socially with others and to consider others' perspectives. Historically , most research on ToM development has focused on early childhood, but recent years have seen an increased focus on how children build this critical social understanding beyond the preschool timeframe. Given this burgeoning literature, we have identified and organized findings across a variety of domains of development to provide a cohesive theoretical framework depicting the correlates and antecedents of ToM development throughout middle childhood and adolescence. Thus, the present paper provides a synthesis and narrative review of the research to yield insights into important ways in which often-disparate lines of study (e.g., brain specialization, relational aggression, reading comprehension) relate to ToM and bidirectionally influence one another in the developing child. Specifically, we focused our analysis of the literature on identifying neural networks underlying ToM, the roles of executive function and emotional self-regulation on ToM, the socioemotional correlates of ToM, and relations between ToM and academic performance. We also provide a brief discussion of studies recognizing sociocultural, linguistic, and contextual influences on ToM. Our review provides evidence for both common and distinct processes and corollaries with age across these disparate literatures, with significant research indicating the important role of mediating and moderating processes when considering how advanced ToM impacts development. We end by proposing a theoretical, integrative framework and discussing the future directions for the field, including testable predictions generated by the framework that span often-disparate domains of inquiry.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
0273-2297/Published by Elsevier Inc.
Correlates and antecedents of theory of mind development during
middle childhood and adolescence: An integrated model
Amy A. Weimer
a
,
*
, Katherine Rice Warnell
b
, Idean Ettekal
c
, Kelly B. Cartwright
d
,
Nicole R. Guajardo
d
, Jeffrey Liew
c
a
School of Family and Consumer Sciences, Texas State University, San Marcos, TX, United States
b
Department of Psychology, Texas State University, San Marcos, TX, United States
c
Department of Educational Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
d
Department of Psychology, Christopher Newport University, Newport News, VA, United States
ABSTRACT
Theory of Mind (ToM) is one of the core abilities that allows children to connect socially with others and to consider othersperspectives. His-
torically, most research on ToM development has focused on early childhood, but recent years have seen an increased focus on how children build
this critical social understanding beyond the preschool timeframe. Given this burgeoning literature, we have identied and organized ndings
across a variety of domains of development to provide a cohesive theoretical framework depicting the correlates and antecedents of ToM devel-
opment throughout middle childhood and adolescence. Thus, the present paper provides a synthesis and narrative review of the research to yield
insights into important ways in which often-disparate lines of study (e.g., brain specialization, relational aggression, reading comprehension) relate
to ToM and bidirectionally inuence one another in the developing child. Specically, we focused our analysis of the literature on identifying neural
networks underlying ToM, the roles of executive function and emotional self-regulation on ToM, the socioemotional correlates of ToM, and relations
between ToM and academic performance. We also provide a brief discussion of studies recognizing sociocultural, linguistic, and contextual in-
uences on ToM. Our review provides evidence for both common and distinct processes and corollaries with age across these disparate literatures,
with signicant research indicating the important role of mediating and moderating processes when considering how advanced ToM impacts
development. We end by proposing a theoretical, integrative framework and discussing the future directions for the eld, including testable pre-
dictions generated by the framework that span often-disparate domains of inquiry.
Introduction
Theory of mind (ToM) is the ability to understand and take into account ones own and othersmental states (Premack & Woodruff,
1978). ToM is one of the core abilities that allows children to get along with others and to see othersperspectives. Traditionally, most
research on ToM development focused on when children acquire explicit ToM in early childhood (e.g., when children pass tests of false
belief understanding), but recent years have seen an increasing number of studies examining ToM throughout middle childhood and
adolescence (Apperly, Warren, Andrews, Grant, & Todd, 2011; Banerjee, Watling, & Caputi, 2011; Devine & Hughes, 2013; Hughes,
2016). This body of work indicates that ToM continues to develop beyond the preschool years as children advance in their under-
standing of the constructivist nature of knowledge and learn to comprehend nuanced aspects of social cognition such as irony, sarcasm,
and humor (e.g., Dumontheil, Apperly, & Blakemore, 2010; Weimer, Perault Dowds, Fabricius, Schwanenugel, & Suh, 2017).
Alongside research mapping out the development of these advanced social cognitive processes, there has been increased attention on
understanding the mechanisms by which these new abilities emerge (e.g., associated neural and cognitive processes; Astington &
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: amy.weimer@txstate.edu (A.A. Weimer).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Developmental Review
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/dr
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2020.100945
Received 26 November 2019; Received in revised form 7 December 2020;
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
2
Hughes, 2013; Hughes, 2016) and how the emergence of advanced ToM relates to other domains of development (e.g., social, aca-
demic; Dore, Amendum, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2018). A synthesis of the research on the antecedents, correlates, and consequences
of ToM development throughout middle childhood and adolescence is needed to spur both basic developmental science and applied
policy interventions aiming to improve academic and social outcomes. Thus, the present paper provides a conceptual review of the
growing body of research on ToM development beyond the preschool years, culminating in a dynamic developmental framework of
advanced ToM integrating neural, social, and cognitive domains (see Fig. 1). The goal of this paper and the framework is not a
comprehensive list of every potential individual or group-level variable that could intersect with ToM. Instead, we offer a synthesis and
narrative review of the research to yield insights into important ways in which these often-disparate lines of study (e.g., brain
specialization, relational aggression, reading comprehension) relate to ToM and inuence one another in the developing child. We end
by discussing future directions for the eld, including testable predictions generated by our proposed, integrative framework.
Overarchingly, an important consideration of the current review is whether and how relations between other processes and ToM
are different throughout middle childhood and adolescence as compared to older and younger age periods. Importantly, there might be
relations among domains of development that are unique during this timeframe as compared to prior developmental timepoints,
potentially inuenced by a variety of factors including differences in language ability (e.g., Im-Bolter, Agostino, & Owens-Jaffray,
2016), the complexity and saliency of interpersonal relationships (Carr, 2011; Somerville et al., 2013), and the neural processes
underlying social cognitive processes (Blakemore, 2012; Dai & Scherf, 2019). Throughout the paper, we emphasize the bidirectionality
of proposed relations in the framework to highlight the mutual inuences among the constructs described and discuss the potential
developmental changes throughout the review.
Alongside conceptual questions of developmental change are the more practical questions of operationalizing and measuring
advanced forms of ToM beyond assessments typically used in early childhood. Several recent papers have tackled this question
theoretically and psychometrically (e.g., Devine, White, Ensor, & Hughes, 2016; Hayward & Homer, 2017; Osterhaus, Koerber, &
Sodian, 2016), and a variety of new advanced ToM measures have been proposed (e.g., Devine & Hughes, 2013; Hayward & Homer,
2017; Rice & Redcay, 2015). In reviewing existing research, we are necessarily limited by the measures employed by past work, but we
acknowledge the ways in which measurement might inuence results, particularly given studies on how varied ToM measures do not
cohere in this age range (Schaafsma, Pfaff, Spunt, & Adolphs, 2015; Warnell & Redcay, 2019). In our discussion of future directions, we
also describe ways in which issues of measurement might be confronted in order to advance the eld.
Fig. 1. Interrelations among emotional self-regulation, theory of mind, social behavior, and academic achievement with sociocultural, linguistic,
and contextual factors and neurodevelopmental cascades shown as inuences.
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
3
We begin this review by addressing the neurodevelopmental processes underlying ToM development throughout middle childhood
and adolescence, with particular attention to bidirectional brain-behavior relations. There are numerous neurodevelopmental path-
ways underlying both ToM and ToM-related predictors, antecedents, and outcomes that we detail in the review. We next review
research highlighting the links among self-regulatory processes and ToM, considering both traditional executive function measures (e.
g., working memory) and measures focused on childrens ability to regulate their own emotional experiences (e.g., effortful control).
We argue that this interplay between ToM and self-regulation inuences links between ToM and other complex social and academic
outcomes. We next review how developing ToM inuences childrens social development, including both positive and negative social
outcomes. We follow this section with discussion of how ToM inuences childrens academic performance as they move through
middle and high school. Finally, based on this review, we synthesize across these sections and propose our integrated framework of
advanced ToM, ending with a discussion of future directions for the eld.
Measuring theory of mind beyond early childhood
Theory of mind development during early childhood has been the focus of extensive study. Using tasks that require participants to
consider their own mental states as well as those of others (i.e., rst-order beliefs, differential desires) researchers have revealed that
ToM development during early childhood represents an extended and progressive set of conceptual acquisitions (e.g., Wellman & Liu,
2004). Though less-studied, there are several important advances in mental state understanding that occur in later childhood (Devine
& Hughes, 2013). Advanced ToM tasks have been used to demonstrate that during middle childhood, children advance in their un-
derstanding of mental states, including intentions and perspectives (e.g., Happ´
e, 1994). Using tasks that assess advanced ToM de-
velopments, such as those that require social reasoning about others intentions (Filippova & Astington, 2008), perspectives
(Dumontheil et al., 2010), and social thinking (sometimes termed second-order false beliefs, Miller, 2009, 2012; Perner & Wimmer,
1985), researchers have documented developments in higher order ToM (Dumontheil et al., 2010; Vetter, Altgassen, Phillips, Mahy, &
Kliegel, 2013). While some research has focused on the social side of ToM development, revealing that adolescentsreasoning about
ambiguous social interactions and social understanding improves (e.g., empathy; Bosacki, 2000), others have shown that children
improve in the cognitive components of ToM across middle and late childhood, such as in the ability to understand how mental
processes affect knowledge construction (i.e., Constructivist ToM development; Weimer et al., 2017). As there is no single gold
standard advanced ToM assessment, this review examines a diverse array of ToM measures employed in middle childhood and
adolescence. We rst consider how the brain might support ToM processing in these ages.
Neural networks underlying ToM
Researchers have studied extensively the brain bases of ToM in adulthood, identifying a set of regions known as the ‘mentalizing
network.Although the exact regions engaged vary by task, recent meta-analyses have identied medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and
bilateral temporal parietal junction (TPJ) as core areas involved when thinking about the thoughts of others (Molenberghs, Johnson,
Henry, & Mattingley, 2016; Schurz, Radua, Aichhorn, Richlan, & Perner, 2014). Depending on the specic ToM assessment, other
structures commonly engaged during mentalizing include additional cortical midline regions (precuneus; dorsal anterior cingulate
cortex, dACC) as well as additional frontal and temporal regions (temporal poles; posterior superior temporal sulcus, pSTS; inferior
frontal gyrus, IFG; Mahy, Moses, & Pfeifer, 2014). Importantly, although the majority of neural research on ToM examines adults, the
bulk of behavioral ToM research examines younger age groups, resulting in comparatively less knowledge about how these behavioral
developments are reected in the brain.
The limited neuroimaging research examining ToM in preschoolers has found activation in similar regions to those activated in
adult ToM studies. For example, Richardson, Lisandrelli, Riobueno-Naylor, and Saxe (2018) measured neural activation while both
adults and children aged 312 years watched a naturalistic movie clip involving mental and physical states. They found that the brains
mentalizing and pain-processing networks were in place and distinct from age 3 onwards, becoming increasingly specialized with age.
Early differences in the mentalizing network also relate to ToM ability. In an EEG study of 4-year-olds, differences in alpha activity
in dMPFC and right TPJ were related to performance on explicit ToM items (Sabbagh, Bowman, Evraire, & Ito, 2009) and, between
ages 4 and 8 years, functional connectivity between nodes of the mentalizing network also related to advanced ToM (Xiao, Geng,
Riggins, Chen, & Redcay, 2019). Additionally, white matter structural maturity in the mentalizing network was found to correlate
positively with false belief reasoning in preschoolers (Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017). This developmental continuity in the mentalizing
network is consistent with infant studies indicating that frontal and temporal regions, similar to those engaged in adult mentalizing,
are activated when infants process social information (e.g., eye contact, emotion, biological motion; reviewed in Grossmann, 2015).
While the basic network for processing ToM is in place early, this network undergoes specialization for processing mental states
between early and middle childhood. Gweon and colleagues (2012) scanned adults and children aged 512 years while they listened to
stories involving mental state information, social but non-mental state information, and physical information. In general, children
recruited similar regions as adults when listening to mental state information versus physical information (i.e., MPFC, TPJ, and
precuneus). Neural selectivity for mental versus social stories increased in both left and right TPJ as children aged and specialization
for mental states in right TPJ was related to ToM performance outside of the scanner. Similarly, research involving naturalistic videos
found that neural responses were more variable in children than adults, with increased adult-like responding in left TPJ as children
aged (Moraczewski, Chen, & Redcay, 2018). Much is still unknown, however, about the mechanisms by which these neural changes
support improved ToM processing from early to middle childhood.
Although ToM improves throughout middle childhood (e.g., Devine & Hughes, 2013), the heightened social sensitivity of
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
4
adolescence makes it an especially important period for understanding the brain bases of ToM (see Blakemore, 2018; Blakemore &
Mills, 2014; and Somerville et al., 2013 for reviews). As with children, the core regions of the adult mentalizing network are active
when adolescents complete ToM tasks (e.g., Blakemore, den Ouden, Choudhury, & Frith, 2007; Sebastian et al., 2011; Vetter et al.,
2013; see Kilford, Garrett, & Blakemore, 2016 for review). There is evidence, however, that MPFC shows peak activation in adoles-
cence. Within MPFC, dorsal regions (dMPFC) have been linked to self-related processing and canonical ToM tasks, whereas ventral
regions (vMPFC) might be more involved in processing rewards and affective states (Bzdok et al., 2013). Several investigations of
affective ToM have found higher vMPFC activity for adolescents than adults (e.g., Moore et al., 2012; Sebastian et al., 2011; Vetter
et al., 2013), with evidence that peak activation might occur toward the beginning of adolescence (Moore et al., 2012; Rosen et al.,
2018). Outside of affective ToM, vMPFC activity also peaks in adolescence when engaging in self-evaluation (Pfeifer & Peake, 2012;
Pfeifer et al., 2013) and when winning rewards for disliked social partners (Braams, Peters, Peper, Güro˘
glu, & Crone, 2014). Ado-
lescents similarly show heightened dMPFC activity compared to adults when considering social versus basic emotions (Burnett, Bird,
Moll, Frith, & Blakemore, 2009) and when donating money while being observed (Van Hoorn, Van Dijk, Güro˘
glu, & Crone, 2016). This
heightened dMPFC activity that occurs when adolescents are being observed might be due to reecting on what the observer is
thinking about their choice to donate.
Adolescents even show increased prefrontal recruitment for tasks and conditions that lack explicit ToM demands. For example, in
an interactive game, adults only recruited dMPFC on trials that required perspective taking, whereas adolescents engaged dMPFC on
all trials with any social information (Dumontheil, Hillebrandt, Apperly, & Blakemore, 2012). This over-recruitment of dMPFC is
consistent with evidence for adolescent dMPFC activation even when individuals are not given a specic mentalizing task. When
individuals aged 822 years believed they were being observed, dorsal MPFC activation showed a curvilinear pattern, increasing into
adolescence before decreasing again (Sommerville et al., 2013). That is, even when simply lying in a scanner, feeling that one is the
object of social observation increases activity in a core region of the mentalizing network, potentially due to increased adolescent focus
on what others are thinking. Additionally, whereas adults recruit dMPFC more for live versus recorded speech, older children recruit
the region equally in both contexts (Rice & Redcay, 2016; Rice, Moraczewski, & Redcay, 2016). Although the exact mechanisms
underlying these neural changes are unknown, they might reect the increased salience of social situations and stimuli in adolescence,
consistent with a bidirectional interplay between brain and behavior.
Unfortunately, systematic assessments of single samples spanning childhood through adulthood are rare, making it difcult to
pinpoint exactly when MPFC activity peaks and whether this peak varies across ToM tasks. In particular, although there is some
evidence for increases from childhood to adolescence in MPFC activity (e.g., Sommerville et al., 2013; Warnell, Sadikova, & Redcay,
2018), most existing research compares adolescents to adults. Further, in contrast to a peak in frontal activity in adolescence, there is
some evidence that activation in temporal mentalizing regions (e.g., TPJ, pSTS) continues to increase from adolescence to adulthood,
although this is less robustly documented (e.g., van den Bos, Dijk, Westenberg, Rombouts, & Crone, 2010; see Blakemore, 2012 for
discussion and review).
Overall, these results suggest that the mentalizing network undergoes development through adolescence, but the specic neural
mechanisms underlying these developments are less clear. Two potential mechanisms explaining how neural changes support
behavioral changes are the timing of responses and the connectivity between regions. Evidence supporting the role of timing includes
ndings that anticipatory responding to mental state information increases from early to middle childhood (Richardson et al., 2018)
and that typical children and children with autism spectrum disorder show different temporal brain responses when processing mental
state information (Yuk et al., 2018). Given limitations of fMRI in measuring precise timing, future research combining fMRI with more
temporally sensitive neuroimaging measures may offer more insight into these processes. In addition to changes in timing, patterns of
connectivity between regions, both within and across networks, also might change with age (Richardson & Saxe, 2019; but see
Mccormick, Hoorn, Cohen, & Telzer, 2018). Although the exact impact of these changes on behavior is unknown, researchers have
found that stronger connectivity between nodes of the mentalizing network is related to advanced ToM abilities in childhood (Xiao
et al., 2019). Functional specialization also might be supported by neuroanatomical changes in ToM regions that extend into late
adolescence (Mills, Lalonde, Clasen, Giedd, & Blakemore, 2012). Future research should continue to use ne-grained analyses to better
map neural changes onto the ToM abilities that are acquired from middle childhood into adolescence.
Although understanding how the mentalizing network subserves development on classic ToM tasks is important, this network also
is recruited for a broader set of processes across development, including several of the domains discussed in this review. For example,
research has identied a coupling between the mentalizing network and regions activated during narrative processing (see Mar, 2011
for review), even for language without social content (Jacoby & Fedorenko, 2018). Additionally, increased adolescent academic self-
concept has been linked to increased activation in mentalizing regions during self-evaluation (van der Aar, Peters, van der Cruijsen, &
Crone, 2019). These overlaps could have implications for links between ToM and academic performance. Additionally, although the
mentalizing network is distinct from the executive functioning network, some studies have found areas of overlap, although the spatial
precision and developmental progression of this overlap is debated (see Wade et al., 2018 for review). For example, the mentalizing
network might be involved in self-referential components of emotional self-regulation, with evidence for developmental changes in
how frontal and temporal mentalizing regions support cognitive reappraisals (McRae et al., 2012). Thus, the mentalizing network
plays a role in a variety of cognitive processes throughout middle childhood and adolescence, with the extent of its contribution
dependent on the exact domain.
There also is increasing evidence for overlap between the mentalizing network and brain systems supporting social interaction
more broadly, including peer interaction. Even for tasks without explicit ToM demands, children and adults recruit the mentalizing
network during live interaction (see Redcay & Warnell, 2018 for review). For example, children aged 713 years show increased TPJ
activation for speech perceived to be live versus recorded, even when such speech contains no mental state content (Rice et al., 2016).
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
5
The mentalizing network also is activated when children believe they are interacting with a real-life peer, and the magnitude of this
activation increases from ages 812 years (Alkire, Levitas, Warnell, & Redcay, 2018; Warnell et al., 2018). Attesting to a broader role
for mentalizing in social behavior, mentalizing network activation also has been linked to both aggressive (Decety, Michalska,
Akitsuki, & Lahey, 2009) and prosocial (van den Bos et al., 2010; Van Hoorn et al., 2016) behaviors throughout development, with
different patterns in early versus late adolescence. Thus, the neural mechanisms underpinning conventional ToM tasks also might
broadly support real-world social outcomes throughout development.
In sum, the basic components of the mentalizing network are in place by early childhood, but patterns of activation and con-
nectivity continue to undergo renement as children age, with a particular social sensitivity manifesting in adolescence. The activation
in this network likely supports a variety of other components of cognitive and social development, which in turn affect childrens social
motivation and experiences, creating a developmental cascade that further shapes brain activation (cf. Masten & Cicchetti, 2010).
Investigating these neurodevelopmental cascades requires large scale, multi-modal, longitudinal studies that include multiple men-
talizing tasks. Given changes in activation even from early to late adolescence, researchers should target narrow age ranges and also
assess pubertal status in order to closely identify how brain changes relate to ToM performance during this timeframe (Goddings,
Burnett Heyes, Bird, Viner, & Blakemore, 2012). Such neuroimaging studies will be complemented by the development of new
behavioral assays to better capture ToM change and variability throughout middle childhood and adolescence. While there is some
evidence that age and puberty might each play a role, depending on task and region (Goddings et al., 2012; Moore et al., 2012), further
research is needed to identify specically how pubertal changes affect development in the mentalizing network. One theory is that
hormonal changes associated with puberty spur increased social motivation and social sensitivity (Forbes & Dahl, 2010), which also
might inuence neural development in regions associated with ToM. Overall, the eld should continue to investigate how the neural
and behavioral correlates of ToM intertwine.
Executive function, emotional self-regulation, and ToM
Self-regulation is a broad construct that includes both executive functions (EFs) and emotional self-regulation (Eisenberg, Hofer, &
Vaughan, 2007). EFs are higher-order cognitive and neurological processes, such as inhibitory control, cognitive exibility (also called
shifting), and working memory, which allow for reective or deliberate, goal-directed thought and action (Diamond, 2013). Emotion-
related or emotional self-regulation (often termed emotion regulation or ER for brevity in the literature) refers to processes used to
manage and change if, when, and how (e.g., how intensely) one experiences emotions and emotion-related motivational and physi-
ological states, as well as how emotions are expressed behaviorally(Eisenberg et al., 2007, p. 288). Henceforth, we use ER to refer to
emotional self-regulation given that emotional self-regulation is often synonymous with emotion regulation in the literature (Liew &
Spinrad, in press). ER often is measured using assessments of such abilities and traits as effortful control, emotionality, and reactivity.
Following Eisenberg et al. (2007), we use the term emotional self-regulation rather than emotion regulation to include the broader
processes involved in ER rather than on the amount of emotion experienced or expressed and also to emphasize the self-regulatory
(rather than external or interpersonal) processes involved in ER. Given that effortful control includes inhibitory control, activation
control (Evans & Rothbart, 2007), and some EF skills, there is overlap in the constructs of EF and ER. While many ToM studies often
evaluate EFs from a purely cognitive point of view, without including them in the macro concept of self-regulation, given the inter-
relatedness of these two constructs, it is important to draw from the literature on ER as well as EF to understand ToM development. The
two constructs are related throughout development, but as they often are studied separately in terms of ToM development, we address
them separately here.
Executive functions
EF is a non-unitary, multi-faceted construct including skills that improve rapidly in early childhood and continue to develop across
middle childhood, into adolescence, and into young adulthood (Davidson, Amso, Anderson, & Diamond, 2006; Huizinga, Dolan, & van
der Molen, 2006). EF consists of cool cognitive processes (e.g., inhibition, working memory, planning), which involve making
judgements in emotionally-neutral situations, and hot affective processes (e.g., ability to delay gratication, affective decision mak-
ing), which involve reasoning about emotionally-laden situations (Zelazo & Müller, 2002). These abilities have unique developmental
timelines. Some evidence, for example, suggests that whereas shifting reaches maturity by about 15 years of age, working memory and
some aspects of inhibitory control develop into young adulthood (Huizinga et al., 2006). Developmental performance differences vary,
though, depending upon the task used (see Davidson et al., 2006).
During preschool, EF skills measured at 24 months predict later ToM understanding at 39 months, suggesting that EF development
could enable children to attend to, process, and learn about mental states (e.g., Carlson, Mandell, & Williams, 2004). ToM under-
standing requires individuals to hold information in mind while continuing to process new information (working memory), suppress
their own knowledge to consider that of others (inhibition), and switch between what they know and what someone else may be
thinking (cognitive exibility). EFs also correlate with ToM performance throughout middle childhood both concurrently (Austin,
Groppe, & Elsner, 2014; Devine et al., 2016; Wilson, Andrews, Hogan, Wang, & Shum, 2018) and longitudinally (Austin et al., 2014),
though ndings are mixed regarding which domain of EF is most predictive of ToM. Whereas some have found that working memory
uniquely predicts ToM performance (Lecce & Bianco, 2018; Lecce, Bianco, Devine, & Hughes, 2017), others have indicated inhibitory
control and perhaps set-shifting are more important than working memory (Bock, Gallaway, & Hund, 2015; Cassetta, Pexman, &
Goghari, 2018; Vetter et al., 2013), though it is likely that multiple facets of EF are needed for onlineprocessing of others per-
spectives (Dumontheil et al., 2010). For example, Dumontheil et al. (2010) examined changes in ToM understanding across middle
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
6
childhood into young adulthood using a computerized version of the Director Task, based upon Keysar, Barr, Balin, and Brauner (2000)
original task. Performance on this task involves being able to hold directions in mind and suppress ones own perspective. Thus,
Dumontheil et al. (2010) propose that participantsimproved perspective taking into young adulthood suggests that EF is important for
ToM across middle childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood.
One reason for mixed ndings could be that advanced ToM measures differ across studies [e.g, second-order false belief, Strange
Stories (Happ´
e, 1994), Silent Films (Devine & Hughes, 2013)], and it is possible that the demands of these tasks vary such that different
subcomponents of EF are required. In fact, Valle, Massaro, Castelli, and Marchetti (2015) examined ToM development in adolescence
and early adulthood by examining correlates of participantsabilities a) to recognize the mental states of story characters at different
levels of recursive complexity (Imposing Memory Task), and b) to explain how a character would predict othersbehaviors based on
that characters understanding of mental states (higher-order false belief). They found support for differential relationships between EF
and ToM performance depending upon the abilities required in ToM tasks: working memory correlated with performance on the
Imposing Memory Task, but not to performance on a third-order false belief task.
Yet, it also is plausible that different components of EF underlie ToM understanding at a conceptual level, beyond task demands.
The positive association between EF and ToM has been explained using two main types of theories, the expressionaccount and the
emergenceaccount (Devine & Hughes, 2014; Moses, 2001; Wang, Devine, Wong, & Hughes, 2016). The expression account of ToM
development posits that the executive or higher-order cognitive demands of ToM tasks mask childrens true understanding of others
mental states. Thus, the relationship between EF and ToM is a consequence of task demands. By contrast, the emergence account of
ToM development suggests that EF is a developmental precursor to ToM that enables people to process and reect upon the mental
states of themselves and others (see Devine & Hughes, 2014).
Results in early childhood (Devine & Hughes, 2014) as well as middle childhood (Lecce & Bianco, 2018) support the emergence
account. For example, Lecce and Bianco (2018) conducted a training study with 9- to 11-year-old children. Working memory
moderated the effect of training such that children with higher working memory scores beneted most from training; more specically,
working memory enabled children to benet from conversations focused on mental states. These data suggest that EF is necessary, but
not sufcient for ToM understanding (see also Perner & Lang, 2000). Additionally, EF predicts ToM throughout middle childhood even
when ToM is assessed with complex, second-order false belief tasks; specically, cognitive exibility predicts ToM performance
beyond working memory, age, vocabulary, and inhibitory control (Bock et al., 2015). There also are likely bidirectional inuences
between EF and ToM across development throughout middle childhood and adolescence (Austin et al., 2014) so that gains in one
domain contribute to developments in the other. However, further research is needed to understand the bidirectional inuences of EF
and ToM on development of these skills.
Research also has proposed relations among mental attentional capacity, attentional inhibition, language, executive processes
(shifting and updating), and ToM (Im-Bolter et al., 2016). Im-Bolter et al. (2016) suggested that higher order ToM performance
throughout middle childhood might be more cognitively demanding, drawing more so on cognitive resources, perhaps due to a novice
level of understanding. Language and EF interact to support ToM performance in this age group, which ts with earlier ndings that
children from Hong Kong perform less well on ToM tasks despite stronger EF abilities (Wang et al., 2016). In early adolescence,
however, capacity itself becomes less important for ToM performance, perhaps given increases in capacity and greater familiarity with
ToM concepts. Capacity, however, continues to relate to language and shifting, which are both important for ToM performance.
Switching, in particular, is important for ToM performance in early adolescence (Im-Bolter et al., 2016). These results indicate the
importance of examining components of EF separately and for understanding how the relationships among aspects of EF and ToM can
vary with age.
Emotional self-regulation (ER)
Although links between EF and ToM have been extensively studied, less attention has been given to the links between ER and ToM
development. ER is required for self-determined or goal-directed behaviors and goal attainment (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Gross, 2015). ER
processes often have been conceptualized as consisting of two modes or dual systems. A dual systems model of emotional self-
regulation can be represented as consisting of reexive and reective systems (Carver, Johnson, & Joormann, 2009; Hofmann, Fri-
ese, & Strack, 2009), although researchers sometimes use different terms to refer to each of the systems. Terms that refer to the re-
exive and reective systems involved in ER include reactivity and self-regulation (Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981), social-emotional
and cognitive self-regulation (Bodrova & Leong, 2006), and implicit and explicit ER (Gyurak, Gross, & Etkin, 2011). While both re-
exive and reective systems are involved in ER, reective processes and higher-order cognition (Evans & Stanovich, 2013) are of
particular interest to the study of ToM because ToM requires the inhibition of automatic or reexive response tendencies to take ones
own perspective (i.e., executive inhibition) in order to reect on others mental states (Perner, Lang, & Kloo, 2002; van de Meer,
Groenewold, Nolen, Pijnenborg, & Aleman, 2011).
In preschoolers, Jahromi and Stifter (2008) have indicated that EFs, but not ER, predict false belief understanding. In contrast, Lane
et al. (2013) found that children who experienced greater physiological reactivity (indicative of emotional reactivity) demonstrated
better ToM. There are theoretical reasons to suspect links between ER and ToM during middle childhood, given the links between ER
and social competence (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2009; McKown, Gumbiner, Russo, & Lipton, 2009; Shields, Cicchetti, & Ryan, 1994), but
studies investigating these links directly during middle childhood are surprisingly sparse.
In one study of adults, Kalbe et al. (2007) examined whether the cognitive and emotional subcomponents of ToM correspond to
different levels in skin conductance responses in adults, who listened to ten cognitive ToM stories, ten affective ToM stories, and ten
non-ToM (control) stories. Results demonstrated signicantly elevated responses for affective ToM (i.e., ToM involving emotional
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
7
components) but not with cognitive ToM and control stories, suggesting the possibility that different facets of ToM might be differ-
entially supported by ER. Links between ER and ToM also could change throughout development. One possibility is that as children
engage in more complex and socially demanding situations, the ways in which socioemotional and behavioral development directly
inuence each other becomes increasingly evident. McKown et al. (2009) have shown that 4- to 14-year-old children with greater
socioemotional skills (e.g., awareness of nonverbal cues, ToM understanding, empathy, and social problem reasoning) and highly rated
ER abilities are more competent in their social interactions. Thus, in addition to research examining direct links between ER and
advanced ToM, it is important to consider feedback loops and bidirectionality.
Another important future direction is to examine effortful control. Effortful control and EF are distinct but related constructs
involved in inhibition of automatic or reexive response tendencies (Liew, 2012; Lin, Liew, & Perez, 2019; Nigg, 2016). One core
distinction between effortful control and EF is that working memory is a part of EF but not of effortful control. Working memory is not
typically considered a part of temperament or personality processes (e.g., effortful control), but it is for EF (Eisenberg & Zhou, 2016).
Effortful control (i.e., constraint or inhibition) can contribute to ER (e.g., when a child inhibits emotional expression), but ER involves
more than effortful control as it includes the ability to activate behavior and shift attention (Eisenberg et al., 2004). While studies on
ToM in early childhood often include measures of effortful control and/or executive functioning, studies on middle childhood or
adolescence typically focus on measures of EF (e.g., Vetter et al., 2013) but not effortful control (e.g., P´
erez-Edgar, 2015). The focus on
EF rather than on effortful control throughout middle childhood or adolescence might be because effortful control is a dimension of
temperament that emerges early in life and matures in early and middle childhood but tends to remain relatively stable across
development, while EF develops most rapidly during early childhood and continues to mature throughout adolescence, in line with
frontal maturation in the brain (Blakemore & Choudhury, 2006; Zelazo & Carlson, 2012). Without more research, however, it is
unknown if effortful control continues to impact childrens sophisticated social understanding as they become older.
Further, while the two components of self-regulation reviewed above, EFs and ER, have been linked in recent work with adolescents
(Effeney, Carroll, & Bahr, 2013; Follmer & Sperling, 2016), direct links among ToM, EFs, and ER are underexplored. Interestingly,
increasing evidence suggests that both variables play a role in academic achievement (e.g., Blair & Raver, 2015; Duckworth, Taxer,
Eskreis-Winkler, Galla, & Gross, 2019; Lockl, Ebert, & Weinert, 2017). Researchers have integrated both ToM and self-regulation in the
study of developmental and academic outcomes (e.g., Jahromi & Stifter, 2008; Olson, Lopez-Duran, Lunkenheimer, Chang, &
Sameroff, 2011). Despite these ndings, very few have differentiated the cognitive, motivational/emotional, and behavioral aspects of
regulation (e.g., effortful control, executive functioning) to examine relations with ToM, and how these self-regulatory and socio-
cognitive abilities uniquely or jointly contribute to social and academic success. Future work should address these relations.
Socioemotional correlates of ToM
Researchers increasingly have focused attention on identifying the extent to which ToM is associated with childrens social be-
haviors and peer relationships. Given the particular salience and inuence of peer relationships in adolescence (Suleiman & Deardorff,
2015), we have focused our review on a discussion of ToM and social relationships outside the family. In spite of this focus, we note
that family relations are foundational to later social development and that ToM has been found to relate to interactions with parents
(Meins et al., 2003) and siblings (e.g., McAlister & Peterson, 2007), and we encourage longitudinal studies examining how early links
between ToM and family relations are related to later peer outcomes.
Research on the socioemotional correlates of advanced ToM has focused on both adaptive and maladaptive behavioral outcomes
such as prosocial behavior and aggression, as well as indicators of childrens social status within their larger peer group (e.g., peer
group acceptance and rejection). In general, researchers have proposed that more advanced understanding of mental states should
have a positive effect on childrens behavioral adjustment and social status (Hughes & Devine, 2015; Hughes & Leekam, 2004).
Consistent with this proposition, several researchers have found positive associations between more advanced mentalizing skills
and indicators of social competence, prosocial behavior, and peer acceptance (Banerjee et al., 2011; Bosacki & Astington, 2001;
Caputi, Lecce, Pagnin, & Banerjee, 2012; Cassidy, Werner, Rourke, Zubernis, & Balaraman, 2003; Fink, Begeer, Hunt, & de Rosnay,
2014; Imuta, Henry, Slaughter, Selcuk, & Ruffman, 2016; Lalonde & Chandler, 1995; Slaughter, Imuta, Peterson, & Henry, 2015;
Weimer et al., 2017). Extending this proposition, children with less advanced understanding of mental states, compared to peers, are at
greater risk for behavioral and relational problems. More specically, one line of investigation has examined the associations between
ToM and childrens aggressive and bullying behaviors. Applying a social information processing perspective, investigators have
evaluated a social skills decit hypothesis, according to which aggressive behaviors are largely the result of decient and maladaptive
social cognitive biases (Crick & Dodge, 1994). Consistent with this viewpoint are ndings from several studies that have reported
negative associations between ToM and aggression, such that less advanced ToM skills are associated with greater rates of aggression
among children and adolescents from 10 to 15 years old (Bosacki & Astington, 2001; Gomez-Garibello & Talwar, 2015; Kokkinos,
Voulgaridou, Mandrali, & Parousidou, 2016; Shakoor et al., 2012; Weimer et al., 2017).
Contrary to these ndings that indicate an inverse association between ToM and aggression, some researchers have posited that
there may be a positive association between ToM and aggression, such that children who display more advanced ToM skills may be at
greater risk for engaging in aggressive and bullying behaviors (Caravita, Di Blasio, & Salmivalli, 2010; Gini, 2006; Sutton et al., 1999a,
1999b). More specically, having more advanced ToM and perspective taking skills may facilitate the use of more sophisticated forms
of aggression such as indirect and relational aggression. That is, children and adolescents might be able to engage more effectively in
behaviors such as social exclusion, rumor spreading, gossiping and friendship manipulation if they are more competent at under-
standing others mental states (Gomez-Garibello & Talwar, 2015; Renouf et al., 2009). In addition to research linking ToM with
different forms of aggression, researchers have posited that ToM might be positively linked with different functions of aggression, in
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
8
particular proactive and instrumental aggression, including bullying behaviors (Gasser & Keller, 2009; Renouf et al., 2010; Sutton
et al., 1999a, 1999b).
Attempts to explain these divergent ndings point to several considerations, each of which reect fruitful areas for further
investigation. First, there is a need to better account for potential developmental differences in the associations between ToM and
childrens behavioral adjustment in early versus middle childhood. Accordingly, Hughes and Devine (2015) propose two alternative
hypotheses. According to the developmental lag hypothesis, there are individual differences in the age at which children attain certain
mentalizing skills. One implication of this hypothesis is that mentalizing skills in early childhood (e.g., false-belief) will have a stronger
association with concurrent social adjustment, but are less likely to be longitudinally associated with childrens social adjustment
throughout middle childhood (since most children will have attained these skills by that period). According to the genuine variation
hypothesis, individual differences in early mentalizing skills are indicative of the ease and uency with which children attain men-
talizing skills and reect more sustained and persistent variations in mental reasoning. Thus, early individual differences might have
long-term effects on childrens social adjustment (Fink et al., 2014; Shakoor et al., 2012). It is important to note that one potential
limitation of these hypotheses is that they point to individual differences in when children attain mentalizing abilities, and thus, they
speak less directly to how children use or apply them (Caputi et al., 2012; Hughes, 2011).
Consequently, a second consideration that relates more directly to applications or performance of mentalizing abilities is the
proposition that ToM is a neutral tool or skill that can be applied in positive or negative ways (Astington, 2004; Hughes & Devine,
2015). As a corollary to this premise, it would be expected that the effects of ToM are moderated by other social-cognitive or emotion
processes. For instance, there is evidence that children (ages 56 years old) who exhibit more prosocial tendencies are less inclined to
engage in relational aggression, even when they exhibit the ToM skills to effectively engage in these behaviors (Renouf et al., 2009).
Research on emotional processes indicates that among children who were either 6 or 10 years old, those with less advanced ToM were
at greater risk for engaging in aggressive behaviors if they also exhibited higher levels of callous-unemotional traits (Song, Waller,
Hyde, & Olson, 2016). In addition to social-cognitive and emotion processes, the effects of ToM also might be moderated by salient
contextual processes. For instance, investigators have examined whether childrens peer relationships could alter the associations
between ToM and aggression. For example, Renouf et al. (2010) found that ToM was negatively associated with reactive aggression,
and positively associated with proactive aggression when children were making the transition to kindergarten (i.e., about 6 years old),
but only among children who experienced high levels of peer victimization.
In addition to potential moderating processes, a third consideration relates to potential mediating mechanisms that provide insights
into how ToM is primarily indirectly associated with childrens social adjustment. Support for this premise has been garnered from
several studies that have investigated how childrens social-cognitions and behavioral styles might have a more proximal association
with their social adjustment. With respect to social cognitions, ToM has been found to facilitate the use of social problem-solving skills,
which in turn, reduced the risks for aggressive behaviors among adolescents who were about 1415 years old (Weimer et al., 2017).
Furthermore, lower ToM increased the risks for moral disengagement, which in turn, was associated with more frequent aggressive
behaviors among children who were 1012 years old (Kokkinos et al., 2016). With respect to behavioral styles, there is evidence that
prosocial behavior functions as a mediator between ToM and peer acceptance and rejection in the early grade school years (Caputi
et al., 2012).
Taken together, one implication of these ndings is that the association between ToM and childrens peer relationships is
dependent on the application or usage of mentalizing abilities to promote the use of cooperation or helping behaviors, which in turn
foster more harmonious social interactions. As previously noted, however, there might be important social-cognitive, emotional or
contextual processes that moderate the extent to which children use their mentalizing skills in order to engage in prosocial behaviors
(Caputi et al., 2012; Imuta et al., 2016). Although there is evidence that ToM is positively associated with childrens peer relationships
in early childhood (Slaughter et al., 2015), a second implication of these ndings is that these associations might have an even more
pronounced effect in middle childhood, after children have transitioned to formal schooling and must successfully integrate them-
selves into a more diverse, complex, and larger peer group (see Imuta et al., 2016).
Expanding on the viewpoint that there are individual differences in the attainment and use of mentalizing skills, a fourth
consideration relates to how specic tasks used to measure different components of ToM might have differential associations with
childrens behavioral adjustment and peer relationships. For instance, Banerjee and colleagues (2011) have proposed that traditional
ToM tasks utilized in early childhood (e.g., false-belief understanding) might not capture the forms of mentalizing abilities that relate
most directly to childrens social adjustment. These investigators contend that more advanced tasks typically used in middle child-
hood, such as faux pas,more accurately reect aspects of mentalizing abilities that are directly pertinent for childrens social in-
teractions, and consequently are more strongly associated with their social adjustment. Consistent with this viewpoint, they have
found that performance on faux pas tasks was positively associated with childrens sociability (Banerjee & Henderson, 2001) and peer
acceptance (Banerjee et al., 2011; Caputi et al., 2012), and negatively associated with peer rejection (Banerjee & Watling, 2005;
Banerjee et al., 2011), particularly among older children (ages 911 years old) than younger children (ages 68 years old).
These ndings imply that faux pas tasks, or other socially-relevant tasks, more directly assess aspects of emotion understanding that
are important determinants of childrens social adjustment, and which are not assessed by more traditional ToM tasks that focus on
beliefs and cognitive representations (Banerjee et al., 2011). Moreover, these ndings suggest potential developmental variations in
the associations between ToM and childrens social adjustment (Hughes & Devine, 2015). More specically, ndings from Banerjee
and colleagues (2011) support the viewpoint that the effects of faux pas are more pronounced in middle and late childhood (as opposed
to early childhood), when these forms of mentalizing abilities become more normative. That is, older children who continue to exhibit
difculties in these forms of understanding appear to lack important prerequisite or foundational skills in order to engage in positive
social interactions, which in turn, would increase their risks for peer rejection.
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
9
Although much of the research described has considered the role of ToM in impacting childrens peer relationships, it is important
to recognize that peer relationships also might function as an important socialization context for the development of ToM (Hughes &
Leekam, 2004; Hughes & Devine, 2015). This premise is consistent with social constructivist perspectives, according to which the
development of childrens mental states occurs in the context of their social interactions (Caputi et al., 2012; Carpendale & Lewis,
2004). Although the family context and parent-child and sibling interactions are also important inuences on ToM development in
early childhood (Devine & Hughes, 2018), several researchers have evaluated how childrens friendships and social status among peers
(e.g., peer acceptance and rejection) are associated with their mentalizing abilities (Badenes, Estevan, & Bacete, 2000; Banerjee et al.,
2011; Peterson & Siegal, 2002; Slaughter, Dennis, & Pritchard, 2002). From a conceptual standpoint, it is plausible that peer so-
cialization effects are more likely to be pronounced after children have made the transition to formal schooling and spend a
considerable amount of time with classmates.
Longitudinal studies, however, have found mixed empirical evidence for links between early peer status and later ToM. Bosacki
(2013) found that children whose drawings were of cooperative activities at age 8 later scored more highly on ToM than children who
drew competitive activities, suggesting that prosocial interactions might relate positively with ToM. By contrast, Banerjee and col-
leagues (2011) found that peer rejection, but not peer acceptance, was prospectively associated with childrens ToM. These ndings
suggest that children who are actively disliked and excluded from their peer group are deprived of more adaptive socialization ex-
periences, which could undermine the development of ToM. These ndings are also consistent with research on childrens social
information processing, such that peer rejection has been found to be predictive of biases in encoding social cues and hostile attri-
butions (Lansford, Malone, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 2010). In contrast to the ndings reported by Banerjee et al., Caputi and colleagues
(2012) reported that peer acceptance and rejection were not prospectively associated with ToM. Similarly, using a measure of social
preference to assess childrens social status, Fink and colleagues (2014) reported that social preference was not signicantly predictive
of ToM. Although the reasons for these discrepancies are unclear, it is possible that they are due to differences in the ages of the samples
assessed or to the forms of ToM that were measured (as previously noted, Banerjee et al. assessed faux pas). With respect to age-related
or developmental differences, Caputi et al. and Fink et al. examined longitudinal samples who were followed from roughly ages 57
years old. Notably, Banerjee et al. examined a longitudinal sample from ages 611, but the effects of peer rejection on ToM emerged
between the ages of 710. Taken together, these ndings suggest that there may be specic developmental periods in middle child-
hood, but not necessarily in early childhood, in which peer socialization effects are most pronounced.
In sum, the relations between ToM and peer relations are complex and dynamic throughout middle childhood, particularly as ToM
becomes more advanced. Links between social outcomes and social cognition remain a primary question of interest in the eld,
particularly as these relations between ToM and social behavior are unfolding alongside childrens developing cognitive abilities,
which include dynamic couplings among ToM, executive functions, and academic skills.
ToM and academics
In addition to impacts on social development in childhood and adolescence, ToM also has been linked to academic achievement.
For example, in a longitudinal study, Lecce, Caputi, Pagnin, & Banerjee (2017) found that social competence mediates the relationship
between early ToM and later school achievement (a latent factor of reading, math, and teacher rated academic competence), inde-
pendently of verbal abilities and current ToM. Further, Lockl et al. (2017) found that ToM in rst graders signicantly predicts
teachersratings of childrens reading and mathematical competencies, even controlling for socioeconomic status, gender, nonverbal
abilities, working memory, and language abilities. Additionally, Weimer et al. (2017) have found that adolescent academic
achievement (as measured via grade point average) relates signicantly with constructivist ToM, an advanced form of ToM that in-
volves understanding the interpretive processes embedded in knowledge construction.
Although these ndings suggest relations between ToM and general academic abilities across childhood and adolescence, other
ndings point more specically to links between ToM and developing reading skills. For example, Blair and Razza (2007) demon-
strated preschool ToM was predictive of kindergarten literacy skills, but not math skills. Similarly, in a path analytic study of 7- to 10-
year-old children, Cantin, Gnaedinger, Gallaway, Hesson-Mcinnis, and Hund (2016) found reading comprehension contributed to ToM
(suggesting a bidirectional relation that we expand on below), but no signicant relations emerged between ToM and mathematics
achievement. These ndings suggest reading seems to hold a special status in relation of academic abilities to ToM. This is not sur-
prising for two reasons. First, neurobiological evidence indicates considerable overlap between the core mentalizing network un-
derlying ToM and networks involved in processing of story comprehension (Gweon et al., 2012; Mar, 2011). Additionally, reading
comprehension skill underlies academic achievement in other areas, such as science (Reed, Petscher, & Truckenmiller, 2016) and
mathematics (Vilenius-Tuohimaa, Aunola, & Nurmi, 2008) in middle childhood and adolescence, pointing to readings special status
among academic skills more generally.
Why might ToM be related to reading development? One possibility involves the development of EF skills. EFs contribute
signicantly and directly to the development of ToM, as we noted above. Reading comprehension is inherently complex, involving
simultaneous integration of multiple processes, features, and perspectives in text (e.g., Cartwright & Duke, 2019; Duke & Cartwright,
2019; Kintsch, 1988; RAND, 2002); thus, it necessarily recruits EFs (e.g., Locascio, Mahone, Eason, & Cutting, 2010). Readers must
shift exibly among word-level features and text meaning (cognitive exibility; Cartwright et al., 2017), suppress attention to in-
formation not relevant to the ongoing comprehension of text (inhibitory control; Borella, Carretti, & Pelegrina, 2010), and update a
continually evolving model of text meaning in mind while reading a text (working memory; Garcia-Madruga et al., 2013). The relation
of ToM to developing reading comprehension may therefore be related to shared relations of reading and ToM to EF skills. However,
studies consistently indicate signicant relations between ToM and reading skills beyond contributions of EF in preschool (e.g., Blair &
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
10
Razza, 2007) and middle childhood (e.g., Cantin et al., 2016; Guajardo & Cartwright, 2016).
A second possibility is that reading comprehension involves ToM abilities directly. Indeed, reading comprehension not only re-
quires reasoning about events and actions in texts, but also reasoning about authorsand charactersperspectives and internal mental
states to facilitate social inferences about text meaning (Cartwright, 2015; Dore et al., 2018). Neurobiological, correlational, and
experimental evidence converge to support this possibility. For example, ToM and narrative comprehension are aligned at a neuro-
biological level, as well, pointing to the tight relation between the two processes, which recruit the same neural networks (see Gweon,
Dodell-Feder, Bedny, & Saxe, 2012; Mar, 2011). In correlational work, ToM contributes directly and longitudinally to successful
reading comprehension. For example, Kim (2017) found ToM contributed signicantly to reading comprehension in 7-year-olds
beyond vocabulary, working memory, inference, grammar, and monitoring abilities. Similarly, Boerma and colleagues (2017)
found ToM contributes uniquely to reading comprehension in 8- to 11-year-olds, beyond home literacy environment, print exposure,
and verbal ability. Furthermore, preschool ToM predicts reading comprehension throughout middle childhood (Atkinson, Slade,
Powell, & Levy, 2017; Guajardo & Cartwright, 2016), and school-age childrens social-emotional comprehension of mental processes
used in social situations relates to their reading performance (McKown, Russo-Ponsaran, Allen, Johnson, & Warren-Khot, 2016).
However, as Bruner noted (1988, 1990), application of ToM abilities to reading tasks is difcult for children. For example, in one
study, 7- to 11-year-olds had difculty inferring charactersinternal motivations unless they were stated explicitly in text (Shannon,
Kameenui, & Baumann, 1988). Similarly, 9- to 11-year-olds rarely include characters internal motivations in retellings of stories,
focusing instead on charactersactions (McConaughy, 1985), suggesting that physical (i.e., non-mental) inferences about characters
are easier for children than mental state inferences about characters. Restrictions in EFs may make it more difcult for children to focus
on both mental and physical content simultaneously, or children may not yet be able to apply developing ToM abilities in a complex
reading task.
Promising lines of work suggest training ToM improves reading comprehension for students who struggle to understand characters
perspectives throughout middle childhood (Lysaker, Tonge, Gauson, & Miller, 2011; Shanahan & Shanahan, 1997). Other work
suggests bidirectional relations between ToM and reading ability, such that reading comprehension also might contribute to the
development of ToM throughout middle childhood (Cantin et al., 2016). The ndings of Cantin et al. (2016) suggest developing
reading comprehension skill may facilitate the development of ToM because inferential comprehension of charactersmental states in
texts may provide practice with increasingly sophisticated ToM abilities. Indeed, Boerma, Mol, and Jolles (2017) provided evidence for
reciprocal relations between ToM and reading in middle childhood. Home literacy environment, an indicator of exposure to books and
reading experiences, contributed directly to childrens ToM, which, in turn, contributed directly to reading comprehension. The
reciprocal, mutually facilitative relations between ToM and reading likely persist into late adolescence and adulthood, as evidenced by
ndings that ction reading improves ToM abilities in 18- to 75-year-olds (Kidd & Castano, 2013). Being better able to take the
perspective of others can facilitate reading comprehension, and through reading, people learn the degree to which perspectives can
vary. Further research is warranted to clarify the exact mechanisms linking reading ability, ToM, and EFs.
Sociocultural, linguistic, and contextual inuences
Sociocultural, linguistic, and contextual variables also exert bidirectional inuences on ToM development. Although a full review
of these factors is outside the scope of this manuscript, we note their importance in understanding ToM in middle childhood and
adolescence. Research focused on cross-cultural comparisons in preschoolers has found that children in Anglo-Western cultures (e.g.,
Australia, Canada, USA) follow similar trajectories, but that children in non-Western cultures (e.g., China, Singapore, Iran) develop
ToM understanding in an alternative sequence (Peterson & Slaughter, 2017; Shahaeian, Peterson, Slaughter, & Wellman, 2011;
Wellman & Liu, 2004; Wellman, Fang, Liu, Zhu, & Liu, 2006; Zhang, Shao, & Zhang, 2016). Similarly, cross-cultural research on ToM
development in young children has shown that in cultural contexts that discourage mental state talk, children show developmental lags
in ToM development (Mayer & Tr¨
auble, 2014). Kuntoro, Saraswati, Peterson, and Slaughter (2013) also found a signicant link be-
tween authoritarian parenting and slower rates of ToM development among young children in non-Western cultural contexts. A recent
systematic review of studies focused on cultural variations in ToM and related constructs (e.g., empathy, perspective-taking) suggests
that development in these areas might differ cross-culturally due to differences in linguistic factors, value preferences, and parenting
characteristics (Aival-Naveh, Rothschild-Yakar, & Kurman, 2019). Further, researchers have identied links between sociodemo-
graphic factors and ToM (Devine et al., 2016; Renouf et al., 2010), and suggested that relations between these might differ during
middle childhood (Miller, Reavis, & Avila, 2018). Other studies have found different processes among ToM and social variables (e.g.,
self-concept) when comparing adolescents in different cultural contexts (e.g., Canadian and Polish adolescents; Bosacki, Bialecka-
Pikul, & Szpak, 2013). Furthermore, Antonietti, Liverta-Sempio, Marchetti, and Astington (2006) and Olson, Antonietti, Liverta-
Sempio, and Marchetti (2006) have examined relations among ToM constructs such as mental language across cultures. Their
research reveals that mental language and understanding of epistemic and emotional mental states continues to develop into early
adulthood and varies across cultural contexts (e.g., Canada, Italy, Serbia, Tanzania). Thus, it is important to consider the role of culture
in ToM research focused on children in middle childhood and adolescence.
Relations between language competence and ToM development have been well-established (e.g., Milligan, Astington, & Dack,
2007) and longitudinal studies focused on children in early childhood have indicated that language skills predict ToM development
(Astington & Jenkins, 1999; de Villiers & Pyers, 2002). Recent research also has focused on relations among language prociency and
ToM. Some researchers have suggested that bilingualism enhances ToM (Berguno & Bowler, 2004; Farhadian et al., 2010; Goetz, 2003;
Javor, 2016; Kov´
acs, 2012), although associations between bilingualism and ToM overlap with broader language skills (Diaz & Farrar,
2018; Nguyen & Astington, 2014) and sociocultural factors (Weimer & Gasquoine, 2016). Bilingual language development is likely
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
11
inuenced by a number of factors such as age and process of language acquisition, prociency level attained in languages, amount of
exposure, motivation, type of learning experience, and the degree of similarity between the two languages (Costa & Sebasti´
an-Gall´
es,
2014). To understand how language prociency relates to ToM, these variables, and other social cognitive factors should be explored
further. Some research has offered promising insight. For example, Diaz and Farrar (2018) have found that while language predicted
ToM in both monolingual and bilingual children, EF was only associated with ToM in monolingual children (not bilingual children).
Similarly, Buac and Kaushanskaya (2020) found that language ability, not EF skills, predicted ToM in simultaneous bilinguals (those
acquiring English and Spanish at the same time). They also indicated that different patterns of relations among the variables occurred
in bilinguals with English as a rst language and monolinguals when compared to simultaneous bilinguals, suggesting that language
acquisition histories inuence ToM development. Further research is needed to advance our understanding of the relations among
correlates of ToM (e.g., sociocultural factors, language competence, EF) and ToM development among bilinguals and children with
unique language histories.
It also is important to consider broader sociocontextual factors in investigations of ToM and language prociency. Pelletier (2006)
found that vocabulary predicted performance on reading and story comprehension tasks among monolingual and high-achieving
students, but ToM predicted reading and story comprehension in lower-achieving second-language learners in middle childhood
and in younger lower language learners. Thus, to understand relations among correlates of ToM development, children from diverse
sociocultural, linguistic, and contextual backgrounds should be included in investigations.
A guiding framework for future research
Although some research has identied links among advanced ToM and other components of neural, social, and cognitive devel-
opment, a conceptual framework to describe relations among these constructs would serve as a useful guide for future research. To
date, models of school-aged children and adolescents have been evaluated to assess the socioemotional and sociocognitive mechanisms
underlying ToM development. Across studies, models have considered mediating (indirect) and moderating (interactive) mechanisms
but are limited in how they integrate different factors. For example, while some examined relations among ToM and social behavior
during this developmental time frame, they have failed to include measures of language prociency, or socioeconomic status, which
should be accounted for as important sociocultural factors. Further, few scholars have offered models that integrate the possible
neurodevelopmental components underlying ToM with other behavioral factors.
One illustration of the complicated relation between ToM and other aspects of behavior comes from the social-emotional literature.
Rather than a straightforward relation (i.e., main effect) between ToM and aggression throughout middle childhood and adolescence,
the direction of effect varies depending on the form of aggression assessed (e.g., proactive vs reactive) and the social context, implying
the need to assess for potential (person by environment) interaction effects (Renouf et al., 2010). To add to this complexity, these
associations might be further mediated or moderated by additional person-level factors (e.g., social cognitive and emotion processes)
that could alter how children utilize ToM in their behavioral responses (Song et al., 2016; Weimer et al., 2017). Collectively, these
studies suggest that a new conceptual framework for understanding and testing the linkages among these constructs is needed. In
particular, the mixed ndings could suggest that main-effects models, which assess the direct effects of ToM on aggression, might be
limited in their predictive utility (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2001). Future research should examine the hypothesis that ToM mediates
relations between aspects of ER (e.g., effortful control, EFs) and social and academic outcomes.
Consequently, we propose a conceptual framework that includes many feedback loops and depicts multiple possible mechanisms
underlying ToM development during middle childhood and early adolescence, a timeframe in which there are many neuro-
developmental, socioemotional, and cognitive advances. Fig. 1 shows that self-regulation, comprised of effortful control and executive
functions, relates to ToM development, which relates to prosocial and negative social outcomes, all critical domains of academic
achievement. We chose to use the term emotional self-regulation to emphasize that both the reexive (emotionality/reactivity) and
reective (effortful control and executive functioning) processes are important factors for ToM, interpersonal and social outcomes, and
academic achievement. The feedback loops are illustrative of the bidirectional nature of these relationships. In addition to examining
relations between behavioral assays, the proposed framework also considers bidirectional brain-behavior links, embedding the
developing child within a series of neurodevelopmental cascades. The basic neural components necessary for ToM during middle
childhood and early adolescence are in place by early childhood, but patterns of activation and connectivity continue to undergo
renement as children age and are inuenced by a variety of other changing components of social and cognitive developmental
processes (e.g., language processing, prosocial behavior).
There are many applications of the framework proposed herein that will guide future research. Below we discuss in more detail
specic hypotheses, but rst we review some broad principles of the framework. The framework underscores the importance of
examining the directionality among these proposed transactional relationships, including via longitudinal and experimental studies.
Relations between ToM correlates should also be explored, with investigators examining paths that might be indirect, or conditional on
(i.e., moderated by) other social and emotional factors. For example, given the ndings on relations among negative social behavior
and ToM (Song et al., 2016), researchers could examine if preadolescentsER moderates associations between ToM and aggression. It
also would be of interest to explore how internal motivations and aspects of personality (e.g., temperament, emotion perception,
motivation) mediate ToM and academic/social outcomes.
Similarly, it is hoped that our framework inspires researchers to take an integrative approach that encompasses variables at
multiple levels of analysis and use this framework to clarify further how the neurodevelopmental, cognitive, sociocultural, and
contextual factors underlie ToM development and how ToM relates to broader domains of development. For example, researchers
should examine how sociocultural and linguistic factors (e.g., parenting practices, family constellation, language prociency,
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
12
bilingualism) relate to ToM, social, and academic outcomes in children of diverse backgrounds. Given that bilingual children have
been shown to have unique language developmental proles, sociolinguistic factors might interrelate with cognitive components in
predicting ToM development. For example, in global regions in which bilingualism is valued and common, ToM might be enhanced,
whereas, in cultural contexts that perceive bilingualism negatively (e.g., immigrants suppressing their native tongue due to
discrimination), there might be relatively little effect of bilingualism on ToM development.
Moreover, the framework could guide work that addresses current limitations in ToM measurement. Given that there are incon-
sistent ndings across studies relying on disparate measures (e.g., more cognitive vs. more affective ToM tasks) and methodologies, in
order to increase the ecological validity of investigations of ToM, researchers should measure development in naturalistic settings as
much as possible and assess not only ToM capacity but also the motivation and ability to apply ToM. Currently, much research on ToM
capacity is conducted in the laboratory using decontextualized assays (e.g., predicting what story characters will do next), whereas
peer relations and classroom behavior are unfolding in real-time and often involve multiple interacting individuals. Bridging these
methodological disconnects could better capture variability in the real-world correlates depicted in our framework. However, natu-
ralistic research should avoid confounding factors simply because they are confounded in the real world. Thus, naturalistic research
need not supercially imitate reality but, instead, should explain it.
We hope our framework also brings together conventionally disparate domains of developmental research (e.g., social and cognitive
development) and guides interdisciplinary investigations and interventions (e.g., among educators, school counselors, and psychol-
ogists collectively seeking to improve social outcomes for children). For example, while research has shown that childrens poor
effortful regulation skills predict later behavioral problems (Morris et al., 2013), it is possible that adolescents in school contexts that
emphasize socioemotional learning (e.g., that include curricular approaches focused on increasing ToM, empathy, and related skills)
will nevertheless develop prosocial behavior patterns and correspondingly experience academic success. Ultimately, there is a need for
studies that incorporate not only multiple predictors and antecedents of ToM, but also those that measure these constructs from
multiple lenses. We need increased investigations that examine how the links among naturalistic neuroimaging, facets of emotional
regulation, and social behavior (i.e., prosocial and aggression) interrelate and predict academic outcomes.
Specic hypotheses for future research
Based on this review and the proposed framework, we have developed several viable directions for future researchers and proposed
hypotheses. This is not an exhaustive list, but rather a set of suggestions to guide thinking. We hypothesize that ToM and other
socioemotional processes might affect relations between emotional self-regulation (i.e., effortful control, EFs) and social behavior
(prosociality, aggression, peer status), as well as between emotional self-regulation and academic variables, especially reading
comprehension. Further we expect that pathways between these factors will differ when comparing frameworks of children across
diverse sociocultural, linguistic, and contextual backgrounds.
Our review of the literature on the neurodevelopmental underpinnings of ToM reveals several directions for future research.
Studies should continue to examine age-related changes in mentalizing network activity from early childhood through adolescence,
ideally using longitudinal designs, given inconsistencies in past work as to whether linear, quadratic, or null changes in network
recruitment are expected. Potentially, age-related changes could be dependent on the regions studied (e.g., frontal versus temporal)
and the tasks used, such that adolescent mentalizing network activity would peak for tasks that involved direct social interaction and
appraisal but not for more conventional false belief tasks. Such research could also use behavioral assays to more directly target
proposed links between heightened MPFC activity and heightened subjective salience of social information, including othersmental
states. This research should also more clearly dissociate relative contributions of age versus pubertal status to frontal region activity
given previous mixed ndings about developmental changes in social processing (see Dai & Scherf, 2019 for a meta-analysis). One
concern of many prior studies of brain-behavior relations is that results may be hampered by small sample sizes (Cremers et al., 2017).
Thus, studies with larger samples are essential. Indeed, one recent study examining self-evaluation in girls aged 1013 found no
relation between MPFC activity and age or puberty (Barendse et al., 2020), but different results may be found in studies of how dMPFC
activity developmentally changes when more directly reecting on others mental states.
Future neuroimaging work should also continue to incorporate naturalistic and spontaneous ToM measures, given evidence that
interactive contexts alter neural processing (see Redcay & Schilbach, 2019 for review). The incorporation of new tasks, on which
children and adolescents are not at ceiling, will also allow for better understanding of how individual differences in ToM performance
relate to neural variability. One hypothesis is that links between neural activation and real-world social outcomes (e.g., sociometric
status) will be stronger for more realistic, socially-interactive tasks than conventional false belief reasoning or even reasoning about
faux pas of characters in stories. Similarly, studies should examine the neural bases of a variety of ToM tasks in a single sample, akin to
studies which have administered a suite of tasks to assay the role of pSTS (e.g., Deen et al., 2015; Dasgupta et al., 2018). This research
should also include different ages, as one possibility is that increased neural specialization could lead to less neural overlap among
tasks as children move from early childhood through adolescence.
Studies using naturalistic ToM tasks could also further explore the hypothesis that adolescence corresponds to increased
engagement of mentalizing regions, even in contexts in which mentalizing is not required (Redcay & Warnell, 2018). Although there
are a handful of suggestive studies nding such heightened activation, researchers could systematically manipulate the perception of
social presence and judgement while measuring activity in ToM regions. For example, researchers could examine the inuence of being
observed by a peer versus an adult, or being recorded to being observed later versus being observed in real-time. Importantly, regions
of the mentalizing network are active during a variety of tasks, even those which do not involve mental state reasoning. To help isolate
the role of mentalizing network regions in social interaction, researchers could incorporate ToM localizers, use more sophisticated
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
13
analytical techniques, and develop more precise tasks, such as paradigms that systematically increase mentalizing demands in
interactive contexts (e.g., Alkire et al., 2018).
Our specic proposed framework also suggests several directions for future work combining neuroimaging research with studies of
self-regulation, social behavior, and academic achievement. Longitudinal research could help disentangle questions of causality in
relations between social behavior and neural activity. For example, researchers could test whether sustained positive or negative
experiences with peers precede changes in mentalizing network activity when anticipating peer feedback or handling peer pressure (e.
g., Falk et al., 2014). Similarly, researchers could develop assessments of emotional self-regulation that systematically vary in their
ToM demands, to determine how activation in regulatory and ToM networks interplays throughout development. Finally, although
there is evidence for neural overlap in narrative processing and ToM networks (Mar, 2011), most of this work has been done in adults.
Expanding this work to younger ages, and adding in explicit assessments of academic reading ability, might help explain individual
differences in academic achievement.
The results of our review also indicate several future directions in the study of self-regulation and ToM. First, that there is a need to
examine EF abilities separately to better understand relations among the components of EF and ToM and how these vary with age. It is
hypothesized that disparate components of EF will relate to ToM uniquely at various time points across early childhood to adolescence.
For example, working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive exibility might become more important in adolescence as ToM tasks
grow more complex. It also is proposed that these aspects of EF will account for more variability in applied task performance and
everyday application of ToM concepts compared to explicit, laboratory task performance. Indeed, performance and task demands also
may vary with the context and nature of social interaction. Differential relations also may arise as researchers explore meaningful
differences between cognitive and affective ToM or between naturalistic and non-naturalistic ToM measures. For example, EF might be
more related to canonical false belief tasks, as compared to ER, which might relate with naturalistic or spontaneous measures inte-
grated into real-world interaction.
Given that research has indicated both positive association between ToM and prosocial behavior and negative associations between
ToM and aggression (i.e., less advanced ToM skills are associated with greater rates of aggression) there are several hypotheses to be
tested around ToM and social development, many of which involve ER. Using the proposed framework, researchers could examine the
interrelations among ER, ToM, and social behavior at multiple time points from early childhood into late adolescence. For example,
using latent growth curve modeling, research could model individual developmental trajectories from culturally and linguistically
diverse groups of children to map changeover time. Such analyses could help determine the effect of the predictors over and above SES,
parental strategies, or other covariates. We hypothesize potential developmental variations in the associations between ToM and
childrens social behavior across language and cultural groups, above and beyond SES.
Additional hypotheses generated from our proposed framework might help to explain inconsistencies in prior studies that indicate
that ToM is both positively and negatively associated with childrens aggressive behaviors. More specically, by integrating childrens
ER, we hypothesize that the effects of ToM on childrens aggression are moderated by ER processes. That is, considering the viewpoint
that ToM is a neutral tool that can be applied in both positive and negative ways, we expect that ToM is more likely to be positively
associated with aggression among children who exhibit emotion and self-regulatory difculties (e.g., low effortful or inhibitory
control; high emotional reactivity). In contrast, when children have higher levels of ER, ToM is hypothesized to be negatively asso-
ciated with aggression.
We also expect that there might be important contextual (e.g., peer relational) moderators. More specically, in the context of
maladaptive peer relationships (e.g., peer rejection or peer victimization), we hypothesize that children could be more inclined to use
their ToM to facilitate the use of more sophisticated forms of relational aggression. In contrast, when children experience social
contexts that are more harmonious (e.g., a child is well accepted; peer group norms do not support the use of aggression), we hy-
pothesize that more advanced ToM would be more strongly associated with prosocial behaviors. Finally, we expect that these asso-
ciations would be more pronounced in adolescence, compared to childhood, when peer groups become more salient and inuential.
In the realm of academic achievement, our review also generates several suggestions for future research. As we noted previously,
ToM is related to general academic achievement (Lecce et al., 2017; Lockl et al., 2017; Weimer et al., 2017). However, research
disaggregating the various types of achievement domains (e.g. reading and math) shows signicant relations of ToM to reading but not
to mathematics skill (Blair & Razza, 2007; Cantin et al., 2016), suggesting a unique relation between ToM and reading. More research
is needed, however, to further explore relations of ToM to subcomponents of these domains of academic achievement, such as various
types of reading (e.g., literal vs. inferential reading comprehension, or narrative vs. expository text comprehension) and mathematics
(e.g., computational vs. applied problems) tasks. Mentalizing may be more important for some academic task types, such as inferential
narrative texts or mathematical word problem comprehension, than for others. Further, given these positive relations, ToM could offer
a unique target of intervention for reading comprehension difculties, which has been tested in small studies (i.e., Lysaker et al., 2011),
but should be further explored in more extensive training work.
In addition to ToM contributing directly to reading comprehension (e.g., Kim, 2017), research has suggested possible bidirectional
relations between ToM and reading, such that reading may facilitate ToM abilities (Cantin et al., 2016). Reading ctional texts (i.e.,
those with social content) might provide the opportunity to practice applying ToM skills to make social inferences, which might
facilitate ToM skills as well as improve reading. Boerma et al. (2017) provided preliminary evidence to support this hypothesis, nding
that home literacy environment, an indicator of exposure to books and reading experiences, contributed directly to ToM in middle
childhood, which, in turn, contributed directly to reading comprehension. Kidd and Castano (2013) provided similar correlational
evidence with adults, nding that adults who read ction (i.e., texts that offer opportunities to consider charactersperspectives) more
frequently had better ToM abilities. These ndings are promising, but randomized controlled trials are necessary to determine whether
reading directly inuences ToM abilities. Guajardo and Watson (2002) presented promising evidence from a training study with
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
14
preschoolers for whom a narrative comprehension intervention improved ToM. These ndings suggest text-based interventions might
improve ToM in middle childhood, a direction that should be explored in future work.
Why might reading comprehension be tied to ToM development? Our review revealed several reasons why this may be the case.
First, research has revealed a coupling between the mentalizing network and regions activated during narrative processing (Gweon
et al., 2012; Mar, 2011). Mentalizing network development enables new abilities, which in turn affect academic performance. That
academic practice may then feed back into brain development, a hypothesis that should be tested in future work.
Behaviorally, development of EF skills occurs alongside the development of ToM and of reading. The relation of ToM to developing
reading comprehension may therefore be inuenced by shared relations of reading and ToM to EF skills. However, studies consistently
indicate signicant relations between ToM and reading skills beyond contributions of EF in preschool (e.g., Blair & Razza, 2007) and
middle childhood (e.g., Cantin et al., 2016; Guajardo & Cartwright, 2016). These ndings suggest reading comprehension may involve
ToM abilities directly. During middle childhood, children advance in their understanding of mental states, including intentions,
perspectives, and social thinking (Dumontheil et al., 2010; Filippova & Astington, 2008; Happ´
e, 1994; Vetter et al., 2013). At the same
time, childrens reading comprehension development shifts, such that inferential and language comprehension processes come to
dominate their processing of the meanings of texts, in contrast to early childhood when word decoding processes are the dominant
contributors to reading comprehension (Gough, Hoover, & Peterson, 1996; Lonigan, Burgess, & Schatschneider, 2018). Comparable
studies of the relative contributions of ToM to reading comprehension across development, from early to middle childhood and
beyond are scarce. Longitudinal evidence is necessary to understand how changes in ToM skill interact with, and support, im-
provements in inferential reading comprehension skill across development.
Finally, bidirectional relations between self-regulatory abilities, such as emotion regulation (ER) and executive functioning (EF),
and reading comprehension may partially account for relations of ToM to reading comprehension and other types of academic
achievement. Few studies have examined all of these variables, however, within the same sample. Recently, Kim (2017) used structural
equation modeling to examine contributions of a number of variables, including ToM and working memory (one of the three core EFs)
in a sample of 2nd grade students, nding that both variables contributed signicant variance to reading comprehension. Themodel
precluded examination of whether ToM mediated relations between reading and working memory and did not examine potential
reciprocal relations between these variables, a direction that should be explored in future work that includes both EF and ER variables.
Finally, longitudinal research using our framework could explore which components of mentalizing predict various academic abilities;
for example, mental state vocabulary - an indicator of ToM knowledge - may contribute differently to reading comprehension than the
social inferential ability involved in belief-desire reasoning assessed in false belief tasks. These should be explored in future work.
Although this review focuses on neurotypical development, there are many applications of the framework to work in ToM and
psychopathology including autism and social anxiety. Recent research throughout middle childhood has suggested a curvilinear
relation between ToM ability and social anxiety, such that both very high and very low levels of ToM can link to greater social anxiety
(Nikoli´
c et al., 2019), with increased self-consciousness mediating links between social anxiety and high ToM. Similarly, different
components of ToM show different patterns of performance in autism, including in late childhood and adolescence (e.g., Fitzpatrick
et al., 2018; Schaller & Rauh, 2017; Sodian, Schuwerk, & Kristen, 2015). One potential explanation for the lack of straightforward ToM
ndings in psychopathology is that early differences in motivation or basic social processing might differentially affect components of
ToM as children age, which in turn affects other social processes (e.g., Chevallier, Kohls, Troiani, Brodkin, & Schultz, 2012). Thus, the
complex, transactional framework proposed here is also relevant to understanding atypical developmental processes, including in
other disorders linked to altered ToM such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder, and depression. Future
work should directly compare ToM-relevant developmental pathways (e.g., links between ToM and social success) among clinical and
non-clinical groups and also examine severity of clinical traits as potential moderators.
Conclusion
After decades of focus on ToM in early childhood, developmental scientists are increasingly focusing on how ToM continues to
develop through middle childhood and adolescence. Here, we synthesized this growing literature, focusing on brain bases, the roles of
executive function and emotional self-regulation, how developing social cognitive capacities affect the childs social world, and how
ToM relates to academic performance. Across these areas, there is evidence for both common and distinct processes and corollaries
with age, with signicant literature indicating the important role of mediating and moderating processes when considering how
advanced ToM impacts development. Thus, while the rudimentary components of the mentalizing network emerge in early childhood,
patterns of activation and neural connectivity particularly in areas of social sensitivity undergo further development across childhood.
These developmental changes likely facilitate childrens abilities to regulate their own emotions and engage in increased degrees of
both positive and negative social interaction, which may in turn enhance social cognitive and academic abilities. Correspondingly,
improved self-regulation enables ToM, empathy, perspective-taking, and related social cognitive abilities not only because it leads to
social interactions that provide data-gathering opportunities for children to consider how their peersmental states might differ from
their own, but also enables children to engage in prosocial behavior when they are motivated to do so. Further research is needed to
investigate the acquisition versus application of ToM. Such research focused on understanding how social outcomes could be both
positively and negatively linked to ToM in this age range should continue to explore moderators (e.g., gender, role models, temper-
ament). Further, while research has suggested that ToM is an important aspect of childrens social and academic success, it is difcult
to disentangle the underlying developmental mechanisms.
In sum, by synthesizing studies from across these disparate areas of research, we have proposed an integrated transactional
framework, explaining how neural and behavioral changes in the childs ToM inuence, and are inuenced by, other cognitive
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
15
processes, emotional capacities, peer relations, and school contexts. We hope that future research on advanced ToM continues to
bridge multiple domains of study in order to explain how this core cognitive capacity inuences children across development.
References
Aival-Naveh, E., Rothschild-Yakar, L., & Kurman, J. (2019). Keeping culture in mind: A systematic review and initial conceptualization of mentalizing from a cross-
cultural perspective. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 26(4), e12300. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.12300
Alkire, D., Levitas, D., Warnell, K. R., & Redcay, E. (2018). Social interaction recruits mentalizing and reward systems in middle childhood. Human Brain Mapping, 39,
39283942. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24221
Antonietti, A., Liverta-Sempio, O., Marchetti, A., & Astington, J. W. (2006). Mental language and understanding of epistemic and emotional mental states: Contextual
aspects. In A. Antonietti, O. Liverata, & A. Marchetti (Eds.), Theory of Mind and Language in Different Developmental Contexts (pp. 123). New York: Springer.
Apperly, I. A., Warren, F., Andrews, B. J., Grant, J., & Todd, S. (2011). Developmental continuity in theory of mind: Speed and accuracy of belief-desire reasoning in
children and adults. Child Development, 82(5), 16911703. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01635.x
Arsenio, W., & Lemerise, E. A. (2001). Varieties of childhood bullying: Values, emotion processes, and social competence. Social Development, 10, 5973. https://doi.
org/10.1111/1467-9507.00148
Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5),
13111320. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311
Astington, J. W. (2004). Sometimes necessary, never sufcient: False-belief understanding and social competence. In B. Repacholi, & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual
differences in theory of mind: Implications for typical and atypical development (pp. 1338). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press.
Astington, J. W., & Hughes, C. (2013). ToM: Self-reection and social understanding. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), Oxford library of psychology. The Oxford Handbook of
Developmental Psychology, Vol. 2. Self and other (pp. 398-424). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
Atkinson, L., Slade, L., Powell, D., & Levy, J. P. (2017). Theory of mind in emerging reading comprehension: A longitudinal study of early indirect and direct effects.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 164, 225238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.007
Austin, G., Groppe, K., & Elsner, B. (2014). The reciprocal relationship between executive function and ToM in middle childhood: A 1-year longitudinal perspective.
Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00655
Badenes, L. V., Estevan, R. A. C., & Bacete, F. J. G. (2000). Theory of mind and peer rejection at school. Social Development, 9(3), 271283. https://doi.org/10.1111/
1467-9507.00125
Banerjee, R., Watling, D., & Caputi, M. (2011). Peer relations and the understanding of faux pas: Longitudinal evidence for bidirectional associations. Child
Development, 82(6), 18871905. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01669.x
Barendse, M. E., Cosme, D., Flournoy, J. C., Vijayakumar, N., Cheng, T. W., Allen, N. W., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2020). Neural correlates of self-evaluation in relation to age
and pubertal development in early adolescent girls. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 100799. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100799
Banerjee, R., & Henderson, L. (2001). Social-cognitive factors in childhood social anxiety: A preliminary investigation. Social Development, 10(4), 558572. https://
doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00180
Banerjee, R., & Watling, D. (2005). Childrens understanding of faux pas: Associations with peer relations. Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 2(1), 2745.
Berguno, G., & Bowler, D. M. (2004). Communicative interactions, knowledge of a second language, and theory of mind in young children. The Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 165(3), 293309. https://doi.org/10.3200/GNTP.165.3.293-309
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 711731. https://
doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten.
Child Development, 78(2), 647663. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x
Blakemore, S.-J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Brain development during puberty: State of the science. Developmental Science, 9(1), 1114. https://doi.org/10.1111/
j.1467-7687.2005.00456.x
Blakemore, S.-J., den Ouden, H., Choudhury, S., & Frith, C. (2007). Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions. Social Cognitive and
Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), 130139. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm009
Blakemore, S.-J. (2018). Avoiding social risk in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(2), 116122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417738144
Blakemore, S.-J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 187207. https://doi.org/
10.1146/annurev-psych- 010213-115202
Blakemore, S.-J. (2012). Development of the social brain in adolescence. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 105(3), 111116. https://doi.org/10.1258/
jrsm.2011.110221
Bock, A. M., Gallaway, K. C., & Hund, A. M. (2015). Specifying links between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle childhood: Cognitive exibility
predicts social understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 509521. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2014.888350
Bodrova, E. L. E. N. A., & Leong, D. J. (2006). Vygotskian perspectives on teaching and learning early literacy. Handbook of Early Literacy Research, 2, 243256.
Boerma, I. E., Mol, S. E., & Jolles, J. (2017). The role of home literacy environment, mentalizing, expressive verbal ability, and print exposure in third and fourth
gradersreading comprehension. Scientic Studies of Reading, 21(3), 179193. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2016.1277727
Borella, E., Carretti, B., & Pelegrina, S. (2010). The specic role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 43(6), 541552. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219410371676
Bosacki, S. L. (2000). Theory of mind and self-concept in preadolescents: Links with gender and language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(4), 709. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.709
Bosacki, S., & Astington, J. W. (2001). Theory of mind in preadolescence: Relations between social understanding and social competence. Social Development, 8,
237255. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00093
Bosacki, S. L. (2013). Theory of mind understanding and conversational patterns in middle childhood. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 174, 170191. https://doi.org/
10.1080/00221325.2012.659233
Bosacki, S., Bialecka-Pikul, M., & Szpak, M. (2013). The adolescent mind in school: Theory of mind and self-concept in Canadian and Polish youth. International
Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 20, 113. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2013.804423
Braams, B. R., Peters, S., Peper, J. S., Güro˘
glu, B., & Crone, E. A. (2014). Gambling for self, friends, and antagonists: Differential contributions of affective and social
brain regions on adolescent reward processing. NeuroImage, 100, 281289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.020
Bruner, J. (1988). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Burnett, S., Bird, G., Moll, J., Frith, C., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2009). Development during adolescence of the neural processing of social emotion. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 21(9), 17361750. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21121
Bzdok, D., Langner, R., Schilbach, L., Engeman, D., Laird, A., Fox, P., & Ekhoff, S. B. (2013). Segregation of the human medial prefrontal cortex in social cognition.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 232. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00232
Buac, M., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2020). Predictors of theory of mind performance in bilingual and monolingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(2),
339359. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006919826866
Cantin, R. H., Gnaedinger, E. K., Gallaway, K. C., Hesson-Mcinnis, M. S., & Hund, A. M. (2016). Executive functioning predicts reading, mathematics, and theory of
mind during the elementary years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 6678. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.014
Caputi, M., Lecce, S., Pagnin, A., & Banerjee, R. (2012). Longitudinal effects of theory of mind on later peer relations: The role of prosocial behavior. Developmental
Psychology, 48, 257270.
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
16
Cremers, H. R., Wager, T. D., & Yarkoni, T. (2017). The relation between statistical power and inference in fMRI. PloS one, 12(11), e0184923. Plos one, 12(11),
e0184923. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184923
Caravita, S. C. S., Di Blasio, P., & Salmivalli, C. (2010). Early adolescentsparticipation in bullying: Is ToM involved? Journal of Early Adolescence, 30, 138170.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431609342983
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40,
11051122. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105
Carpendale, K. I., & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of childrens social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 27, 79151. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000032
Carr, A. (2011). Social and emotional development in middle childhood. In D. Skuse, H. Bruce, L. Dowdney, & D. Mrazek (Eds.), Child psychology and psychiatry:
Frameworks for practice (2nd ed., pp. 5661). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.
Cartwright, K. B. (2015). Executive skills and reading comprehension: A guide for educators. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Cartwright, K. B., Coppage, E. A., Lane, A. B., Singleton, T., Marshall, T. R., & Bentivegna, C. (2017). Cognitive exibility decits in children with specic reading
comprehension difculties. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 50, 3344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.01.003
Cartwright, K. B., & Duke, N. K. (2019). The DRIVE model of reading: Making the complexity of reading accessible. The Reading Teacher, 73, 715. https://doi.org/
10.1002/trtr.1818
Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., & Joormann, J. (2009). Two-mode models of self-regulation as a tool for conceptualizing effects of the serotonin system in normal
behavior and diverse disorders. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 195199. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01635.x
Cassetta, B. D., Pexman, P. M., & Goghari, V. M. (2018). Cognitive and affective theory of mind and relations with executive function in middle childhood. Merrill-
Palmer Quarterly, 64, 514538. https://doi.org/10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.64.4.0514
Cassidy, K., Werner, R. S., Rourke, M., Zubernis, L., & Balaraman, G. (2003). The relationship between psychological understanding and positive social behaviors.
Social Development, 12, 198221. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00229
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231239.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007
Costa, A., & Sebasti´
an-Gall´
es, N. (2014). How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain? Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 15(5), 336345. https://doi.org/10.1038/
nrn3709
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information- processing mechanisms in childrens social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin,
115, 74101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74
Dai, J., & Scherf, K. S. (2019). Puberty and functional brain development in humans: Convergence in ndings? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 39, 100690.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100690
Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from
manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 20372078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006
Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., Akitsuki, Y., & Lahey, B. B. (2009). Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: A functional MRI
investigation. Biological Psychology, 80(2), 203211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004de
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The whatand whyof goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227268.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1104_01
Dasgupta, S., Srinivasan, R., & Grossman, E. D. (2018). Multivariate pattern analysis of the human pSTS: A comparison of three prototypical localizers.
Neuropsychologia, 120, 5058. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.10.004
Deen, B., Koldewyn, K., Kanwisher, N., & Saxe, R. (2015). Functional organization of social perception and cognition in the superior temporal sulcus. Cerebral Cortex,
25(11), 45964609. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv111
de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding.
Cognitive Development, 17(1), 10371060. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00073-4
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2013). Silent lms and strange stories: ToM, gender, and social experiences in middle childhood. Child Development, 84(3), 9891003.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12017
Devine, R. T., White, N., Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2016). Theory of mind in middle childhood: Longitudinal associations with executive function and social
competence. Developmental Psychology, 52, 758771. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000105
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2018). Family correlates of false belief understanding in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 89(3), 971987. https://doi.
org/10.1111/cdev.12682
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Diaz, V., & Farrar, M. J. (2018). Do bilingual and monolingual preschoolers acquire false belief understanding similarly? The role of executive functioning and
language. First Language, 38(4), 382398. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723717752741
Dore, R. A., Amendum, S. J., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2018). ToM: A hidden factor in reading comprehension? Educational Psychology Review, 30,
10671089. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-018-9443-9
Duckworth, A. L., Taxer, J. L., Eskreis-Winkler, L., Galla, B. M., & Gross, J. J. (2019). Self-control and academic achievement. Annual Review of Psychology, 70,
373399. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103230
Duke, N. K., & Cartwright, K. B. (2019). The DRIVE model of reading: Deploying reading in varied environments. In D. E. Alvermann, N. Unrau, M. Sailors, &
R. B. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of literacy (7th ed., pp. 118135). New York, NY: Routledge.
Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13(2),
331338. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00888.x
Dumontheil, I., Hillebrandt, H., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2012). Developmental differences in the control of action selection by social information. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(10), 20802095. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00268
Effeney, G., Carroll, A., & Bahr, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning and executive function: Exploring the relationships in a sample of adolescent males. Educational
Psychology, 33, 773796. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2013.785054
Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., & Vaughan, J. (2007). Effortful control and its socioemotional consequences. In J. J. Gross (ed.) Handbook of Emotion Regulation (pp.
287306). New York, NY: Guilford.
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland, A., Shepard, S. A., Thompson, M. (2004). The relations of effortful control and impulsivity to
childrens resiliency and adjustment. Child Development, 75(1), 2546. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00652.x
Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T. L., Cumberland, A., Liew, J., Reiser, M., Losoya, S. H. (2009). Longitudinal relations of childrens effortful control,
impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 9881008.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016213
Eisenberg, N., & Zhou, Q. (2016). Conceptions of executive functioning and regulation: When and to what degree do they overlap? In J. A. Grifn, L. S. Freund, &
P. McCardle (Eds.), Executive Function in Preschool Age Children: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopment and Translational Research (pp. 115136). Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for adult temperament. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(4), 868888. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jrp.2006.11.002
Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223241. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
17
Farhadian, M., Abdullah, R., Mansor, M., Redzuan, M. A., Gazanizadand, N., & Kumar, V. (2010). Theory of mind in bilingual and monolingual preschool children.
Journal of Psychology, 1(1), 3946. https://doi.org/10.1080/09764224.2010.11885444
Falk, E. B., Cascio, C. N., ODonell, M. B., Carp, J., Tinney, F. J., Jr., Bingham, C. R., Simons-Morton, B. G., et al. (2014). Neural responses to exclusion predict
susceptibility to social inuence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54(5), s22s31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.12.035
Fink, E., Begeer, S., Hunt, C., & de Rosnay, M. (2014). False-belief understanding and social preference over the rst 2 years of school: A longitudinal study. Child
Development, 85, 23892403. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12302
Fitzpatrick, P., Frazier, J. A., Cochran, D., Mitchell, T., Coleman, C., & Schmidt, R. C. (2018). Relationship between theory of mind, emotion recognition, and social
synchrony in adolescents with and without autism. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01337
Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 126138.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01115.x
Follmer, D. J., & Sperling, R. A. (2016). The mediating role of metacognition in the relationship between executive function and self-regulated learning. British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 86, 559575. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12123
Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). Pubertal development and behavior: Hormonal activation of social and motivational tendencies. Brain and Cognition, 72(1), 6672.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.007
García-Madruga, J. A., Elosúa, M. R., Gil, L., G´
omez-Veiga, I., Vila, J. ´
O., Orjales, I., Duque, G. (2013). Reading comprehension and working memorys executive
processes: An intervention study in primary school students. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(2), 155174. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.44
Gasser, L., & Keller, M. (2009). Are the competent the morally good? Perspective taking and moral motivation of children involved in bullying. Social Development, 18
(4), 798816. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00516.x
Gini, G. (2006). Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: Whats wrong? Aggressive Behavior, 32, 528539. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20153
Goddings, A., Burnett Heyes, S., Bird, G., Viner, R. M., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2012). The relationship between puberty and social emotion processing. Developmental
Science, 15(6), 801811. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01174.x
Goetz, P. J. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1017/
S1366728903001007P
Gomez-Garibello, C., & Talwar, V. (2015). Can you read my mind? Age as a moderator in the relationship between theory of mind and relational aggression.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 39, 552559. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415580805
Grossmann, T. (2015). The development of social brain functions in infancy. Psychological Bulletin, 141(6), 12661287. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000002
Gough, P. B., Hoover, W. A., & Peterson, C. L. (1996). Some observations on a simple view of reading. In C. Cornoldi, & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension
difculties: Processes and intervention (pp. 113). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 126. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781
Grosse Wiesmann, C., Schreiber, J., Singer, T., Steinbeis, N., & Friederici, A. D. (2017). White matter maturation is associated with the emergence of theory of mind in
early childhood. Nature Communications, 8, 14692. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14692
Guajardo, N. R., & Cartwright, K. B. (2016). The contribution of theory of mind, counterfactual reasoning, and executive function to pre-readerslanguage
comprehension and later reading awareness and comprehension in elementary school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 2745. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.004
Guajardo, N. R., & Watson, A. C. (2002). Narrative discourse and theory of mind development. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(3), 305325. https://doi.org/
10.1080/00221320209598686
Gweon, H., Dodell-Feder, D., Bedny, M., & Saxe, R. (2012). Theory of mind performance in children correlates with functional specialization of a brain region for
thinking about thoughts. Child Development, 83(6), 18531868. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01829.x
Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., & Etkin, A. (2011). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A dual- process framework. Cognition and Emotion, 25(3), 400412. https://doi.
org/10.1080/02699931.2010.544160
Happ´
e, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding story charactersthoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal
children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129154. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172093
Hayward, E. O., & Homer, B. D. (2017). Reliability and validity of advanced theory-of-mind measures throughout middle childhood and adolescence. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 35(3), 454462. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12186
Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(2), 162176. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1745- 6924.2009.01116.x
Hughes, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reections and new directions for studies of typical and
atypical development. Social Development, 13(4), 590619. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00285.x
Hughes, C. (2011). Social understanding and social lives: From toddlerhood through to the transition to school. London, UK: Psychology Press.
Hughes, C., & Devine, R. T. (2015). A social perspective on theory of mind. In M. R. M. Lamb (Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science (7th ed.,,
pp. 564610). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Vol 3.
Hughes, C. (2016). Theory of mind grows up: Reections on new research on theory of mind in middle childhood and adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 149, 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.017
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis.
Neuropsychologia, 44, 20172036. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010
Im-Bolter, N., Agostino, A., & Owens-Jaffray, K. (2016). Theory of mind throughout middle childhood and early adolescence: Different from before? Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 98115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.006
Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B., & Ruffman, T. (2016). Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: A meta-analytic review. Developmental
Psychology, 52(8), 11921205. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000140
Jacoby, N., & Fedorenko, E. (2018). Discourse-level comprehension engages medial frontal Theory of Mind brain regions even for expository texts. Language, Cognition
and Neuroscience, 117. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2018.1525494
Jahromi, L. B., & Stifter, C. A. (2008). Individual differences in preschoolers self-regulation and ToM. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54(1), 125150. https://doi.org/
10.1353/mpq.2008.0007
Javor, R. (2016). Bilingualism, theory of mind and perspective-taking: The effect of early bilingual exposure. Sciences, 5(6), 143148. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.
pbs.20160506.13
Kalbe, E., Grabenhorst, F., Brand, M., Kessler, J., Hilker, R., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2007). Elevated emotional reactivity in affective but not cognitive components of
theory of mind: A psychophysiological study. Journal of Neuropsychology, 1(1), 2738. https://doi.org/10.1348/174866407X180792
Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Bulletin,
11, 3238. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00211
Kidd, D. C., & Castano, E. (2013). Reading literary ction improves theory of mind. Science, 342(6156), 377380. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1239918
Kilford, E. J., Garrett, E., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2016). The development of social cognition in adolescence: An integrated perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews, 70, 106120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.016
Kim, Y. S. G. (2017). Why the simple view of reading is not simplistic: Unpacking component skills of reading using a direct and indirect effect model of reading
(DIER). Scientic Studies of Reading, 21(4), 310333. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1291643
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163182. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0033- 295X.95.2.163
Kokkinos, C. M., Voulgaridou, I., Mandrali, M., & Parousidou, C. (2016). Interactive links between relational aggression, theory of mind, and moral disengagement
among early adolescents. Psychology in the Schools, 53, 253-269. doi:10.1002/pits.21902.
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
18
Kov´
acs, ´
A. M. (2012). Early bilingualism and theory of mind: Bilingualsadvantage in dealing with conicting mental representations. In M. Siegal, & L. Surian (Eds.),
Access to language and cognitive development (pp. 192218). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Kuntoro, I., Saraswati, L., Peterson, C., & Slaughter, V. (2013). Micro-cultural inuences on theory of mind development: A comparative study of middle-class and
Pemulung children in Jakarta, Indonesia. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37, 266273. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413478258
Lalonde, C. E., & Chandler, M. J. (1995). False belief understanding goes to school: On the social-emotional consequences on coming early or late to a rst theory of
mind. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 167186. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939508409007
Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S. L., Miller, A. L., Wang, L., & Tardif, T. (2013). Relations between temperament and theory of mind development in the United
States and China: Biological and behavioral correlates of preschoolers false-belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 49(5), 825836. https://doi.org/
10.1037/a0028825
Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2010). Developmental cascades of peer rejection, social information processing biases, and
aggression during middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 593602. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579410000301
Lecce, S., & Bianco, F. (2018). Working memory predicts changes in childrens theory of mind during middle childhood: A training study. Cognitive Development, 47,
7181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.04.002
Lecce, S., Bianco, F., Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2017). Relations between theory of mind and executive function in middle childhood: A short-term longitudinal
study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 163, 6986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.06.11
Lecce, S., Caputi, M., Pagnin, A., & Banerjee, R. (2017). Theory of mind and school achievement: The mediating role of social competence. Cognitive Development, 44,
8597. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.010
Liew, J. (2012). Effortful control, executive functions, and education: Bringing self-regulatory and social-emotional competencies to the table. Child Development
Perspectives, 6, 105111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00196.x
Liew, J., & Spinrad, T. L. (in press). Emotional self-regulation processes as foundation for social-emotional competencies and whole-child school success. In D. Fisher
(Ed.), Routledge Encyclopedia of Education. (Online). Taylor & Francis: New York.
Lin, B., Liew, J., & Perez, M. (2019). Measurement of self-regulation in early childhood: Relations between laboratory and performance-based measures of effortful
control and executive functioning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 47, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.10.004
Locascio, G., Mahone, E. M., Eason, S. H., & Cutting, L. E. (2010). Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension decits. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 43(5), 441454.
Lockl, K., Ebert, S., & Weinert, S. (2017). Predicting school achievement from early ToM: Differential effects on achievement tests and teacher ratings. Learning and
Individual Differences, 53, 93102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.11.007
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Schatschneider, C. (2018). Examining the simple view of reading with elementary school children: Still simple after all these years.
Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 260273. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932518764833
Lysaker, J. T., Tonge, C., Gauson, D., & Miller, A. (2011). Reading and social imagination: What relationally oriented reading instruction can do for children. Reading
Psychology, 32(6), 520566. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711.2010.507589
Mahy, C. E. V., Moses, L. J., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2014). How and where: Theory-of-mind in the brain. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 6881. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.dcn.2014.01.002
Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 103134. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-
120709-145406
Morris, A., Aesha, J., Halliburton, A., Morris, M., Robinson, L., Myers, S., & Terranova, A. (2013). Effortful control, behavior problems, and peer relations: What
predicts academic adjustment in kindergartners from low-income families? Early Education and Development, 24, 813828. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10409289.2013.744682
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491495. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222
Mayer, A., & Tr¨
auble, B. E. (2014). The weird world of cross-cultural false-belief research: A true- and false-belief study among Samoan children based on commands.
Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 650665. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2014.926273
McAlister, A., & Peterson, C. (2007). A longitudinal study of child siblings and theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, 22(2), 258270. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.009
McConaughy, S. H. (1985). Good and poor readers comprehension of story structure across different input and output modalities. Reading Research Quarterly, 20,
219232. https://doi.org/10.2307/747757
Mccormick, E. M., Hoorn, J. V., Cohen, J. R., & Telzer, E. H. (2018). Functional connectivity in the social brain across childhood and adolescence. Social Cognitive and
Affective Neuroscience, 13(8), 819830. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy064
McRae, K., Gross, J. J., Weber, J., Robertson, E. R., Sokol-Hessner, P., Ray, R. D., Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The development of emotion regulation: An fMRI study of
cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 1122. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr093
McKown, C. I., Russo-Ponsaran, N. M., Allen, A., Johnson, J. J., & Warren-Khot, H. K. (2016). Socialemotional factors and academic outcomes among elementary-
aged children. Infant and Child Development, 25, 119136. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1926
McKown, C. I., Gumbiner, L. M., Russo, N. M., & Lipton, M. (2009). Social-emotional learning skill, self-regulation, and social competence in typically developing and
clinic-referred children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38(6), 858871. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410903258934
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark Carter, D., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2003). Pathways to understanding mind: Construct validity and
predictive validity of maternal mind-mindedness. Child Development, 74, 11941211. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00601
Miller, S. A. (2009). Childrens understanding of second-order mental states. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 749773. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016854
Miller, S. A. (2012). Theory of mind: Development beyond the preschool years. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Miller, S. E., Reavis, R. E., & Avila, B. N. (2018). Associations between theory of mind, executive function, and friendship quality in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 64(3), 397426. https://doi.org/10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.64.3.0397
Milligan, K., Astington, J., & Dack, L. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding.
Child Development, 78, 622646. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x
Mills, K. L., Lalonde, F., Clasen, L. S., Giedd, J. N., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2012). Developmental changes in the structure of the social brain in late childhood and
adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 123131. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss113
Molenberghs, P., Johnson, H., Henry, J. D., & Mattingley, J. B. (2016). Understanding the minds of others: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews, 65, 276291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.020
Moore, W. E., Pfeifer, J. H., Masten, C. L., Mazziotta, J. C., Iacoboni, M., & Dapretto, M. (2012). Facing puberty: Associations between pubertal development and
neural responses to affective facial displays. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 3543. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr066
Moraczewski, D., Chen, G., & Redcay, E. (2018). Inter-subject synchrony as an index of functional specialization in early childhood. Scientic Reports, 8(1). https://doi.
org/10.1038/s41598-018-20600-0
Moses, L. J. (2001). Executive accounts of theory-of-mind development. Child Development, 72(3), 688690. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00306
Nikoli´
c, M., van der Storm, L., Colonnesi, C., Brummelman, E., Kan, K. J., & B¨
ogels, S. (2019). Are socially anxious children poor or advanced mindreaders? Child
Development, 90(4), 14241441. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13248
Nigg, J. T. (2016). Annual research review: On the relations among self-regulation, self- control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control,
impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361383. https://doi.org/10.1111/
jcpp.12675
Nguyen, T., & Astington, J. (2014). Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17,
396409.
Olson, D. R., Antonietti, A., Liverta-Sempio, O., & Marchetti, A. (2006). The mental verbs in different conceptual domains and in different cultures. In A. Antonietti,
O. Liverata, & A. Marchetti (Eds.), Theory of Mind and Language in Different Developmental Contexts (pp. 3152). New York: Springer.
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
19
Olson, L. S., Lopez-Duran, N., Lunkenheimer, E. S., Chang, H., & Sameroff, A. J. (2011). Individual differences in the development of early peer aggression: Integrating
contributions of self-regulation, ToM, and parenting. Developmental Psychopathology, 23, 253266. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000775
Osterhaus, C., Koerber, S., & Sodian, B. (2016). Scaling of advanced theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 87(6), 19711991. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12566
Pelletier, J. (2006). Relations among theory of mind, metacognitive language, reading skills and story comprehension in L1 and L2 learners. In A. Antonietti,
O. Liverata, & A. Marchetti (Eds.), Theory of Mind and Language in Different Developmental Contexts (pp. 7792). New York: Springer.
P´
erez-Edgar, K. (2015). Effortful control in adolescence: Individual differences within a unique developmental window. In G. Oettingen, & P. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Self-
regulation in adolescence (pp. 78102). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2000). Theory of mind and executive function: Is there a developmental relationship? In S. Baron-Cohen, T. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.),
Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience (pp. 150181). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Perner, J., Lang, B., & Kloo, D. (2002). Theory of mind and self-control: More than a common problem of inhibition. Child Development, 73(3), 752767. https://doi.
org/10.1111/1467-8624.00436
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). John thinks that Mary thinks thatattribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 39(3), 437471. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7
Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Mindreading and moral awareness in popular and rejected preschoolers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 205224.
https://doi.org/10.1348/026151002166415
Peterson, C. C., & Slaughter, V. (2017). Culture and the sequence of developmental milestones toward theory-of-mind mastery. In V. Slaughter, & M. de Rosnay (Eds.),
Theory of mind in context (pp. 2540). Oxford, UK: Routledge.
Pfeifer, J., & Peake, S. (2012). Self-development: Integrating cognitive, socioemotional, and neuroimaging perspectives. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2,
5569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.07.012
Pfeifer, J. H., Kahn, L. E., Merchant, J. S., Peake, S. J., Veroude, K., Masten, C. L., Dapretto, M. (2013). Longitudinal change in the neural bases of adolescent social
self-evaluations: Effects of age and pubertal development. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Ofcial Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(17), 74157419.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4074-12.2013
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Problem-solving in the chimpanzee: Test for comprehension. Science, 202, 532535. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.705342
RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for Understanding: Toward an R&D program in Reading Comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.
Redcay, E., & Warnell, K. R. (2018). A social-interactive neuroscience approach to understanding the developing brain. In J. B. Benson (Ed.), Advances in Child
Development and Behavior, 54 (pp. 144). San Diego, CA, US: Elsevier Academic Press.
Reed, D. K., Petscher, Y., & Truckenmiller, A. J. (2016). The contribution of general reading ability to science achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 52(2),
253266. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.158
Redcay, E., & Schilbach, L. (2019). Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 495505.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0179-4
Renouf, A., Brendgen, M., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Zelazo, P. D., Boivin, M., S´
eguin, J. R. (2009). Relations between theory of mind and indirect and physical
aggression in kindergarten: Evidence of the moderating role of prosocial behaviors. Social Development, 19(3), 535555. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
9507.2009.00552.x
Renouf, A., Brendgen, M., S´
eguin, J. R., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., P´
erusse, D. (2010). Interactive links between theory of mind, peer victimization, and
reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(8), 11091123. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9432-z
Rice, K., & Redcay, E. (2016). Interaction matters: A perceived social partner alters the neural processing of human speech. NeuroImage, 129, 480488. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.041
Rice, K., Moraczewski, D., & Redcay, E. (2016). Perceived live interaction modulates the developing social brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(9),
13541362. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw060
Richardson, H., Lisandrelli, G., Riobueno-Naylor, A., & Saxe, R. (2018). Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years. Nature Communications, 9(1),
1027. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-03399-2
Richardson, H., & Saxe, R. (2019). Development of predictive responses in theory of mind brain regions. Developmental Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12863
Rosen, M. L., Sheridan, M. A., Sambrook, K. A., Dennison, M. J., Jenness, J. L., Askren, M. K., McLaughlin, K. A. (2018). Salience network response to changes in
emotional expressions of others is heightened during early adolescence: Relevance for social functioning. Developmental Science, 21(3), e12571. https://doi.org/
10.1111/desc.12571
Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual difference in temperament. In M. E. Lamb, & A. L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in Developmental
Psychology (pp. 3786). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sabbagh, M. A., Bowman, L. C., Evraire, L. E., & Ito, J. M. B. (2009). Neurodevelopmental correlates of theory of mind in preschool children. Child Development, 80(4),
11471162. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01322.x
Schaafsma, S. M., Pfaff, D. W., Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2015). Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(2), 6572.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.007
Schaller, U. M., & Rauh, R. (2017). What difference does it make? Implicit, explicit and complex social cognition in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 47(4), 961979. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-3008-x
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009
Sebastian, C. L., Fontaine, N. M. G., Bird, G., Blakemore, S.-J., Brito, S. A. D., Mccrory, E. J. P., & Viding, E. (2011). Neural processing associated with cognitive and
affective Theory of Mind in adolescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 5363. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr023
Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., & Wellman, H. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 47,
12391247. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023899
Shakoor, S., Jaffee, S. R., Bowes, L., Ouellet-Morin, I., Andreou, P., Happ´
e, F., Arseneault, L. (2012). A prospective longitudinal study of childrens theory of mind
and adolescent involvement in bullying. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 254261. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02488.x
Shanahan, T., & Shanahan, S. (1997). Character perspective charting: Helping children to develop a more complete conception of story. The Reading Teacher, 50(8),
668677.
Shannon, P., Kameenui, E. J., & Baumann, J. F. (1988). An investigation of childrens ability to comprehend character motives. American Educational Research Journal,
25(3), 441462. https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312025003441
Shields, A. M., Cicchetti, D., & Ryan, R. M. (1994). The development of emotional and behavioral self-regulation and social competence among maltreated school-age
children. Development and Psychopathology, 6(1), 5775. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400005885
Slaughter, V., Imuta, K., Peterson, C. C., & Henry, J. D. (2015). Meta-analysis of ToM and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Development,
86(4), 11591174. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12372
Slaughter, V., Dennis, M. J., & Pritchard, M. (2002). Theory of mind and peer acceptance in preschool children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20(4),
545564. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151002760390945
Sodian, B., Schuwerk, T., & Kristen, S. (2015). Implicit and spontaneous theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders. In M. Fitzgerald (Ed.), Autism
spectrum disorderRecent advances. InTech. doi: 10.5772/59393.
Somerville, L. H., Jones, R. M., Ruberry, E. J., Dyke, J. P., Glover, G., & Casey, B. J. (2013). The medial prefrontal cortex and the emergence of self-conscious emotion
in adolescence. Psychological Science, 24(8), 15541562. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613475633
Song, J. H., Waller, R., Hyde, L. W., & Olson, S. L. (2016). Early callous-unemotional behavior, theory-of-mind, and a fearful/inhibited temperament predict
externalizing problems in middle and late childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 12051215. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0099-3
Suleiman, A. B., & Deardorff, J. (2015). Multiple dimensions of peer inuence in adolescent romantic and sexual relationships: A descriptive, qualitative perspective.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(3), 765775. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0394-z
A.A. Weimer et al.
Developmental Review 59 (2021) 100945
20
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999a). Bullying and theory of mind: A critique of the social skills decit view of anti-social behavior. Social
Development, 8, 117127. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00083
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999b). Social cognition and bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 17, 435450. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151099165384
Valle, A., Massaro, D., Castelli, I., & Marchetti, A. (2015). Theory of mind development in adolescence and early adulthood: The growing complexity of recursive
thinking ability. Europes Journal of Psychology, 11(1), 112124. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v11i1.829
van der Aar, L. P. E., Peters, S., van der Cruijsen, R., & Crone, E. A. (2019). The neural correlates of academic self-concept in adolescence and the relation to making
future-oriented academic choices. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 15, 1017. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2019.02.003
van de Meer, L., Groenewold, N. A., Nolen, W. A., Pijnenborg, M., & Aleman, A. (2011). Inhibit yourself and understand the other: Neural basis of distinct processes
underlying Theory of Mind. NeuroImage, 56(4), 23642374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.053
van den Bos, W., Dijk, E. V., Westenberg, M., Rombouts, S. A., & Crone, E. A. (2010). Changing brains, changing perspectives. Psychological Science, 22(1), 6070.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610391102
Van Hoorn, J., Van Dijk, E., Güro˘
glu, B., & Crone, E. A. (2016). Neural correlates of prosocial peer inuence on public goods game donations during adolescence.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(6), 923933. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw013
Vetter, N. C., Altgassen, M., Phillips, L., Mahy, C. E. V., & Kliegel, M. (2013). Development of affective theory of mind across adolescence: Disentangling the role of
executive function. Developmental Neuropsychology, 38, 114125. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2012.733786
Vilenius-Tuohimaa, P. M., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2008). The association between mathematical word problems and reading comprehension. Educational
Psychology, 28(4), 409426. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410701708228
Wade, M., Prime, H., Jenkins, J. M., Yeates, K. O., Williams, T., & Lee, K. (2018). On the relation between theory of mind and executive functioning: A developmental
cognitive neuroscience perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(6), 21192140. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1459-0
Wang, Z., Devine, R. T., Wong, K. K., & Hughes, C. (2016). Theory of mind and executive function during middle childhood across cultures. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 149, 622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.028
Warnell, K. R., Sadikova, E., & Redcay, E. (2018). Lets chat: Developmental neural bases of social motivation during real-time peer interaction. Developmental Science,
21(3), e12581. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12581
Warnell, K. R., & Redcay, E. (2019). Minimal coherence among varied theory of mind measures in childhood and adulthood. Cognition, 191, 103997. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.cognition.2019.06.009
Weimer, A. A., Perault Dowds, S. J., Fabricius, W. V., Schwanenugel, P. J., & Suh, G. W. (2017). Development of constructivist ToM from middle childhood to early
adulthood and its relation to social cognition and behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 154, 2845.
Weimer, A. A., & Gasquoine, P. G. (2016). Belief reasoning and emotion understanding in balanced bilingual and language-dominant Mexican American young
children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 17(2), 3343. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2016.1138793
Wellman, H. M., Fang, F., Liu, D., Zhu, L., & Liu, G. (2006). Scaling of theory-of-mind understandings in Chinese children. Psychological Science, 17, 10751081.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01830.x
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
Wilson, J., Andrews, G., Hogan, C., Wang, S., & Shum, D. H. K. (2018). Executive function in middle childhood and the relationship with theory of mind.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 43, 163182.
Xiao, F., Geng, F., Riggins, T., Chen, G., & Redcay, E. (2019). Neural correlates of developing theory of mind competence in early childhood. Neuroimage, 184,
707716. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.079
Yuk, V., Urbain, C., Pang, E. W., Anagnostou, E., Buchsbaum, D., & Taylor, M. J. (2018). Do you know what Im thinking? Temporal and spatial brain activity during a
theory-of-mind task in children with autism. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 34, 139147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.08.001
Zhang, T., Shao, Z., & Zhang, Y. (2016). Developmental steps in theory of mind of typical Chinese children and Chinese children with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 23, 210220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.10.005
Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6,
354360. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x
Zelazo P. D., Müller U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswamis (ed.) Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive
Development (pp. 445- 469). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.
A.A. Weimer et al.
... Additional research probing the associations between hardship and behavior problems during adolescence is crucial for two reasons. First, major social cognitive and sociocultural changes occur in adolescence, including improvements in perspective-taking and metacognition, elevated emotion reactivity, and increased understanding of societal structures around socioeconomic status (Crone & Dahl, 2012;Kraus et al., 2012;Stephens et al., 2014;Weimer et al., 2021). Second, adolescence is a time of high vulnerability for mood and emotional issues, substance abuse, and psychiatric disorders. ...
... These processes include: (a) understanding the mental states of other individuals (theory of mind); (b) thinking about thinking (metacognition); and (c) emotion reactivity. Growth in these capacities enables adolescents to understand, explain, and predict the actions and cognitions of others (Crone & Dahl, 2012;Keating, 2004;Weimer et al., 2021). Several studies have documented adolescents' rapid development in perspective-taking processes, resulting in greater accuracy compared to children in their ability to assess the mental states of others (Bosco et al., 2009;Choudhury et al., 2006;Dumontheil et al., 2010;Lonigro et al., 2017). ...
... Given the host of changes occurring during adolescence, youth may be uniquely sensitive to information or cues that convey their socioeconomic standing, like economic hardship (Somerville, 2013). Moreover, as adolescents take more active roles in their development and make gains in cognitive abilities-for example, more advanced planning, problem solving, and perspective taking-their own views of life experiences play an increasingly important role in predicting their behavior (Crone & Dahl, 2012;Keating, 2004;Weimer et al., 2021). Importantly, adolescence is also the time when youth begin to assess and plan for their own economic futures (e.g., making educational and career choices) and, thus, may pay closer attention to their families' economic circumstances (Brown & Larson, 2009;Flanagan & Gallay, 2014;Hagquist, 2007;Schoon & Heckhausen, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Understanding how youth perceive household economic hardship and how it relates to their behavior is vital given associations between hardship and behavioral development. Yet, most studies ignore youth’s own perceptions of economic hardship, instead relying solely on caregiver reports. Moreover, the literature has tended to treat economic hardship as a stable force over time, rather than a volatile one that varies month-to-month. This study addressed extant limitations by collecting monthly measures of economic hardship, specifically caregiver- and youth-reported material deprivation and youth-reported financial stress, and youth internalizing and externalizing problems from 104 youth–caregiver dyads (youth: 14–16 years, 55% female, 37% Black, 43% White) over nine months. We examined month-to-month variability of these constructs and how youth-reports of material deprivation and financial stress predicted their behavior problems, controlling for caregiver-reports of material deprivation. We found that hardship measures varied month-to-month (ICCs = 0.69–0.73), and youth-reported material deprivation positively predicted internalizing when examining both within- and between-individual variability ( β = .19–.47). Youth-reported financial stress positively predicted within-individual variation in externalizing ( β = .18), while youth reports of material deprivation predicted externalizing when looking between families ( β = .41). Caregiver-reported material deprivation was unrelated to youth behavior when accounting for youth perceptions of economic hardship.
... Some studies find that working memory is the only skill that predicts ToM performance (Lecce and Bianco 2018; Lecce et al. 2017), while others reveal that it is predicted by inhibitory control (Bock et al. 2015;Cassetta et al. 2018;Vetter et al. 2013). Weimer et al. (2021) proposed that these differences could be due to the measures themselves, as these would require different subcomponents of executive function. However, in studies with participants with ADHD, where a poorer ToM would be predicted because of limitations in executive function, the degree of prediction and predictability of executive function are extremely heterogeneous between different studies (Pineda-Alhucema et al. 2018). ...
... Regarding cooperation in play contexts, Etel and Slaughter (2019) evaluated its association with ToM in preschool-age children, finding that those children who showed better performance on the ToM task were related to cooperation among peers in the context of independent play, but not in the context of group play. In general, all this suggests that children in preschool and school age with a lower understanding of mental states, compared to their peers, are at increased risk of developing behavioral and relational problems (Weimer et al. 2021). Similarly, higher rates of aggression among children and adolescents appear to be associated with poorer ToM skills (Gomez-Garibello and Talwar 2015;Weimer et al. 2017). ...
... Furthermore, other studies have found a relationship between ToM and reading comprehension (Atkinson et al. 2017;Boerma et al. 2017;Guajardo and Cartwright 2016;Kim 2017). This could be the result of the shared relationships of reading and ToM with EF skills (Weimer et al. 2021). Atkinson et al. (2017) explored the relationship between ToM and emergent reading comprehension in 80 children with typical development through a 2-year longitudinal study. ...
Chapter
Theory of Mind (ToM) is a construct that reflects people’s ability to understand the mental states of others. In our daily lives, we continuously infer what others are thinking, understanding, feeling, and interpreting, especially with respect to us and other persons. The present chapter is focused on different definitions and concepts of ToM, its development in children and adolescents, and factors related to individual differences. We also analyse how ToM has been assessed in different studies and the methodological issues surrounding its measurement.
... Саморегуляция в социальных ситуациях важна для развития социальной компетентности (Weimer et al., 2021). Более высокий уровень исполнительных функций связан с более высокими оценками социальной компетентности . ...
... (Wilson et al., 2021) была обнаружена связь модели психического и просоциального поведения, а недостаточность исполнительных функций предсказывала нарушения социального поведения в работе, посвященной изучению роли исполнительных функций в адаптации детей в средней школе . Неоднородность полученных результатов может быть связана как с различиями в методах оценки исследуемых параметров (в большей мере это касается социальной компетентности и регуляции) (Wilson et al., 2021;Weimer et al., 2021), так и с возрастными особенностями соотношения рассмотренных способностей с социальной компетентностью (Zorza et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Социальная компетентность в подростковом возрасте играет решающую роль в социализации и психологическом благополучии. Имеющиеся данные говорят о значительном вкладе в социальную компетентность способности к социальному познанию (модель психического) и саморегуляции (контроль поведения) в младшем возрасте, однако исследований их роли в подростковом возрасте пока недостаточно. Цель исследования - изучение роли контроля поведения и модели психического в социальной компетентности подростков. Выборку составили 106 учеников 6-8 классов (из них девочек - 51) в возрасте 12-15 лет ( Ме = 13, SD = 0,87). Для оценки контроля поведения использовался опросник BRIEF. Модель психического оценивалась с использованием заданий на понимание неверных мнений высшего порядка. Для внешней оценки социальной компетентности применялась анкета для учителя и социометрический метод. Проводилось сравнение групп подростков, разделенных по медианному критерию, с использованием общего индекса регуляции BRIEF и общего балла понимания неверных мнений. Наибольшие различия в оценках социальной компетентности учителями и сверстниками обнаружены между контрастными группами - с высоким уровнем модели психического и контроля поведения и с низким уровнем этих способностей. При этом в оценках педагогов различия наблюдаются как для положительных, так и для отрицательных оценок, а в социометрических индексах - только для отрицательных. Учителя считают более социально компетентными подростков с высоким уровнем контроля поведения, вне зависимости от уровня их социального познания. Для сверстников уровень контроля поведения имеет значение только при слабом умении оценивать психические состояния других людей. Результаты показывают важность роли социального познания и контроля поведения для реализации социально компетентного поведения у подростков. В то же время полученные данные свидетельствует о неоднородности вклада этих способностей в социальную компетентность подростков, оцененную сверстниками и педагогами.
... Indeed, both contribute to a child's ability to self-regulate (Efklides, 2008;Lyons and Zelazo, 2011) and behave in a goal-directed manner, however one is thought of as slower and more deliberate (metacognition) and other more automatic (executive functions). Self-regulation as a broader concept, which has also been known to relate to other factors such as a child's temperament (Chae, 2022), has also been linked to higher school achievement (Blair and Razza, 2007;Pianta et al., 2017;Weimer et al., 2021). In her framework, Roebers (2017) argued that executive functions lay the groundwork for metacognitive abilities; indeed, inhibition may explicitly contribute to metacognitive monitoring, as it allows an individual to pause and reflect on their answer (Bryce et al., 2015), and shifting and updating may be needed to keep in mind the goal of the task and decide, based on what was monitored, whether any control strategies need to be implemented to improve performance. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research suggests that metacognition (the knowledge and skills related to knowledge acquisition) and executive functions (skills needed to plan and execute goals) are possible predictors of academic performance, including math and reading abilities. This study sought to clarify the relationship between school readiness and these abilities. A visual identification task was used to measure preschool children's metacognitive skills, specifically their ability to monitor their confidence on their answers (explicit) and ability to ask for a clue only when necessary (implicit). Response time to answering was also measured to obtain a non-verbal implicit measure of metacognition. Executive functions were measured using the Flanker and Dimensional Change Card Sorting (DCCS) tasks from the NIH toolbox. It was hypothesized that both metacognition and executive functions would predict school readiness and that implicit metacognitive skills would be more highly related to school readiness than explicit skills. A hierarchical linear regression was run with age and sex as control variables, and with executive function and metacognition (implicit and explicit) as predictors. Results indicated that both implicit and explicit metacognition remained significant predictors of school readiness scores beyond age and sex. In addition, we found correlations between explicit metacognition and executive functions and a relationship between response time and explicit metacognitive skill. Results highlight the importance of early metacognitive abilities beyond other cognitive skills and the importance of being able to effectively use metacognitive strategies from a young age. The implications relating to academic abilities are discussed.
... Social understanding is critical to navigating the increasingly complex social world of adolescents (Weimer et al. 2021) and dealing with the higher vulnerability to mental health problems stemming from hormonal, neurological, social, and psychological changes that cumulate in adolescence (Blakemore 2019). Therefore, it is reasonable to attempt to support adolescents in developing SU, not only in atypically developing or especially vulnerable groups, but also in typically developing youths representative of the general population. ...
Article
Full-text available
Adolescence is a developmental period characterized by significant changes and intensified social interactions. The role of parents decreases and the importance of peer groups increases. Peers, especially friends, may deliver instrumental aid and emotional support; they may also promote a sense of security and be a significant source of affection and intimacy. Additionally, peer relations provide a testing ground for exercising many competencies necessary in complex social situations, such as social problem-solving, conflict resolution, and negotiation. The intensified contact with peers may also enhance adolescents’ social understanding skills. Therefore, practicing social understanding skills within a peer group may enhance one’s social functioning in adolescence. For these practical and educational reasons, we aimed to confirm the effectiveness of conversation-based training in these skills and identify what factors potentially support or hinder its effectiveness. Social understanding, the ability to understand oneself and others in various social situations, develops in childhood and adolescence. As this ability impacts satisfactory social functioning in adolescence and develops in a social context, a training process was proposed with the aim of enhancing the development of this ability based on the social-constructivist approach to social understanding. The efficacy of the training to enhance the understanding of one’s own and others’ mental states was verified using a sample of 65 Polish adolescents (mean age: 14.6 years). They participated in nine one-hour sessions and were divided into an experimental group (social understanding, n = 26) and two control groups: attention/perception (n = 17) and film/text literacy (n = 22). Although no direct effect of the theory of mind training was found, the results provided important observations for further work on adolescent social understanding training programs.
... To date, studies investigating social experiences and theory of mind have focused largely on early childhood and on experiences in the family (Devine & Hughes, 2018). The expansion of theory of mind research into middle childhood and adolescence over the past decade provides new opportunities to investigate how wider social experiences in school contribute to the ongoing development of theory of mind beyond early childhood Weimer et al., 2021). There are compelling grounds for investigating associations between classroom ethnic diversity and children's theory of mind. ...
Article
This study examined the link between classroom ethnic diversity, cross‐ethnic friendships, and children's theory of mind. In total, 730 children in the United Kingdom (54.7% girls, 51.5% White) aged 8 to 13 years completed measures of theory of mind in 2019/2020. Controlling for verbal ability, executive function, peer social preference, and teacher‐reported demographic characteristics, greater classroom ethnic diversity provided opportunities for cross‐ethnic friendships, and children with cross‐ethnic friendships performed better than peers without cross‐ethnic friendships on theory of mind. These results extend accounts of intergroup contact by using direct assessments of children's theory of mind and advance social accounts of theory of mind by demonstrating how experiences outside the family are linked with theory of mind.
Article
Full-text available
The course paper looks at relationship between theory of mind and language and language development. The aim of the work is to find out the relationship between the theory of mind and language and language development. The tasks of this work included selecting the literature, analysis of literature, summarizing the information and making conclusions and suggestions. The research question was: "Is there a connection between theory of mind and language and language development?".
Article
Full-text available
Fifty-nine 3-year-olds were tested 3 times over a period of 7 months in order to assess the contribution of theory of mind to language development and of language to theory-of-mind development (including the independent contributions of syntax and semantics). Language competence was assessed with a standardized measure of reception and production of syntax and semantics (the Test of Early Language Development). Theory of mind was assessed with false-belief tasks and appearance–reality tasks. Earlier language abilities predicted later theory-of-mind test performance (controlling for earlier theory of mind), but earlier theory of mind did not predict later language test performance (controlling for earlier language). These findings are consistent with the argument that language is fundamental to theory-of-mind development.
Article
Full-text available
Pathways of relations of language, cognitive, and literacy skills (i.e., working memory, vocabulary, grammatical knowledge, inference, comprehension monitoring, word reading, and listening comprehension) to reading comprehension were examined by comparing four variations of direct and indirect effects model of reading. Results from 350 English-speaking second graders revealed that language and cognitive component skills had direct and indirect relations to listening comprehension, explaining 86% of variance. Word reading and listening comprehension completely mediated the relations of language and cognitive component skills to reading comprehension and explained virtually all the variance in reading comprehension. Total effects of component skills varied from small to substantial. The findings support the direct and indirect effects model of reading model and indicate that word reading and listening comprehension are upperlevel skills that are built on multiple language and cognitive component skills, which have direct and indirect relations among themselves. The results underscore the importance of understanding nature of relations.
Chapter
Full-text available
Emotional self-regulation is a highly popular and influential topic amongst researchers, educators, and practitioners. In the fields of developmental, education or learning, and affective sciences, some confusion and disagreement remain as to how to define and measure emotional self-regulation. For example, the terms self-control, effortful control, executive functioning, and emotion regulation are often used interchangeably or haphazardly, although there are important distinctions between these terms or constructs. Thus, we clarify definitional issues and review major theoretical frameworks in the study of emotional self-regulation. We also present a heuristic model on the role of emotional self-regulation processes in school success. As part of our heuristic model, we call for a holistic view of school success so that engagement and achievements in the social, emotional, academic, and school-based extracurricular domains are all valued as components of success at school. We provide empirical support for the pathways in our model through review of the extant research literature, showing that emotional self-regulation processes are linked to school success indirectly through social-emotional and behavioral competencies. We conclude by providing recommendations for future directions for research, intervention, and practice. Specifically, we call for the use of evidence-based approaches that integrate whole-school, whole-class, and whole-child supports and social emotional learning (SEL) interventions aimed at emotional self-regulation processes and social-emotional and behavioral skills in the context of in-and out-of-school contexts and across academic and extracurricular domains.
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigates the link between bilingualism, Theory of Mind, and empathy among 240 mono-and bilinguals. We hypothesize that bilinguals have better theory of mind capabilities and empathic skills. Possible explanations for this evidence of a bilingual advantage are the early exposure to a second language and/or greater inhibitory control. 240 individuals, divided into two groups, participated in this study: Hungarian-Serbian bilinguals and Hungarian monolinguals. They filled out two questionnaires, one for the language profiles and Davis's Interpersonal Reactivity Index; and they participated in the Adult Theory of Mind test. Our hypotheses have been proven to be true. Bilinguals really have better empathic skills and the stories in the Theory of Mind test showed the difference in favor of bilinguals, so we can assume a correlation between bilingualism, theory of mind, and empathy, it is a trend and it is important for further upcoming bilingual researches.
Article
Full-text available
In this review, we call for a cross-cultural examination of mentalizing. To this end, we first outline theoretical directions for understanding mentalizing in the context of the universalism–relativism debate. Next, we systematically review cross-cultural studies of five concepts, each of which overlaps with separate dimensions of mentalizing: Theory of Mind, empathy, perspective-taking, alexithymia, and mindfulness. Based on healthy and clinical samples investigated across more than 45 cultures, we draw several conclusions. First, mentalizing profiles may vary between cultures (e.g., self > other mentalizing in individualistic cultures, self < other mentalizing in collectivistic cultures). Second, linguistic factors, value preferences, and parenting characteristics may explain these differences. Finally, the data generally support the link between mentalizing and mental health across cultures, yet further research is needed.
Article
Full-text available
Early adolescence is marked by puberty, and is also a time of flux in self-perception. However, there is limited research on the neural correlates of self-evaluation in relation to pubertal development. The current study examined relationships between neural activation during self-evaluation of social traits and maturation (age and pubertal development) in a community sample of female adolescents. Participants (N = 143; age M = 11.65, range = 10.0-13.0) completed a functional MRI task in which they judged the self-descriptiveness of adjectives for prosocial, antisocial and social status-related traits. Pubertal development was based on self-report, and was also examined using morning salivary testosterone, dehydroepiandrosterone, and estradiol. Contrary to preregistered hypotheses, neither age nor pubertal development were related to neural activation during self-evaluation. We further examined whether activation in two regions-of-interest, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and perigenual anterior cingulate (pgACC), was associated with trial-level self-evaluative behavior. In line with preregistered hypotheses, higher vmPFC and pgACC activation during self-evaluation were both associated with a higher probability of endorsing negative adjectives, and a lower probability of endorsing positive adjectives. Future studies should examine neural trajectories of self-evaluation longitudinally, and investigate the predictive value of the neural correlates of self-evaluation for adolescent mental health.
Article
Full-text available
Although there is a long history of studying the influence of pubertal hormones on brain function/structure in animals, this research in human adolescents is young but burgeoning. Here, we provide a comprehensive review of findings from neuroimaging studies investigating the relation between pubertal and functional brain development in humans. We quantified the findings from this literature in which statistics required for standard meta-analyses are often not provided (i.e., effect size in fMRI studies). To do so, we assessed convergence in findings within content domains (reward, facial emotion, social information, cognitive processing) in terms of the locus and directionality (i.e., positive/negative) of effects. Face processing is the only domain with convergence in the locus of effects in the amygdala. Social information processing is the only domain with convergence of positive effects; however, these effects are not consistently present in any brain region. There is no convergence of effects in either the reward or cognitive processing domains. This limited convergence in findings across domains is not the result of null findings or even due to the variety of experimental paradigms researchers employ. Instead, there are critical theoretical, methodological, and analytical issues that must be addressed in order to move the field forward.
Article
Theory of mind—or the understanding that others have mental states that can differ from one’s own and reality—is currently measured across the lifespan by a wide array of tasks. These tasks vary across dimensions including modality, complexity, affective content, and whether responses are explicit or implicit. As a result, theoretical and meta-analytic work has begun to question whether such varied approaches to theory of mind should be categorized as capturing a single construct. To directly address the coherence of theory of mind, and to determine whether that coherence changes across development, we administered a diverse set of theory of mind measures to three different samples: preschoolers, school-aged children, and adults. All tasks showed wide variability in performance, indicating that children and adults often have inconsistent and partial mastery of theory of mind concepts. Further, for all ages studied, the selected theory of mind tasks showed minimal correlations with each other. That is, having high levels of theory of mind on one task did not predict performance on another task designed to measure the same underlying ability. In addition to showing the importance of more carefully designing and selecting theory of mind measures, these findings also suggest that understanding others’ internal states may be a multidimensional process that interacts with other abilities, a process which may not occur in a single conceptual framework. Future research should systematically investigate task coherence via large-scale and longitudinal efforts to determine how we come to understand the minds of others.
Article
In this review, we call for a cross‐cultural examination of mentalizing. To this end, we first outline theoretical directions for understanding mentalizing in the context of the universalism–relativism debate. Next, we systematically review cross‐cultural studies of five concepts, each of which overlaps with separate dimensions of mentalizing: Theory of Mind, empathy, perspective‐taking, alexithymia, and mindfulness. Based on healthy and clinical samples investigated across more than 45 cultures, we draw several conclusions. First, mentalizing profiles may vary between cultures (e.g., self > other mentalizing in individualistic cultures, self < other mentalizing in collectivistic cultures). Second, linguistic factors, value preferences, and parenting characteristics may explain these differences. Finally, the data generally support the link between mentalizing and mental health across cultures, yet further research is needed.