ArticlePDF Available

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/authorsrights
Author's personal copy
Number skills are maintained in healthy ageing
Marinella Cappelletti
a,b,
, Daniele Didino
c,1
, Ivilin Stoianov
d,e,1
, Marco Zorzi
d,f
a
UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK
b
Goldsmiths College, University of London, New Cross London, SE14 6NW, UK
c
Department of Cognitive and Education Science, University of Trento, via Tartarotti 7, Rovereto, Trento, Italy
d
Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy
e
Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, National Research Council, Rome, Italy
f
IRCCS San Camillo Hospital, Venice-Lido, Italy
article info
Article history:
Accepted 29 November 2013
Available online 11 January 2014
Keywords:
Numerical cognition
Ageing
Numerosity perception
Number acuity
Computational modelling
abstract
Numerical skills have been extensively studied in terms of their
development and pathological decline, but whether they change
in healthy ageing is not well known. Longer exposure to numbers
and quantity-related problems may progressively refine numerical
skills, similar to what happens to other cognitive abilities like ver-
bal memory. Alternatively, number skills may be sensitive to age-
ing, reflecting either a decline of number processing itself or of
more auxiliary cognitive abilities that are involved in number
tasks. To distinguish between these possibilities we tested 30 older
and 30 younger participants on an established numerosity discrim-
ination task requiring to judge which of two sets of items is more
numerous, and on arithmetical tasks. Older participants were
remarkably accurate in performing arithmetical tasks although
their numerosity discrimination (also known as ‘number acuity’)
was impaired. Further analyses indicate that this impairment
was limited to numerosity trials requiring inhibiting information
incongruent to numerosity (e.g., fewer but larger items), and that
this also correlated with poor inhibitory processes measured by
standard tests. Therefore, rather than a numerical impairment,
poor numerosity discrimination is likely to reflect elderly’s impov-
erished inhibitory processes. This conclusion is supported by sim-
ulations with a recent neuro-computational model of numerosity
perception, where only the specific degradation of inhibitory
processes produced a pattern that closely resembled older partici-
pants’ performance. Numeracy seems therefore resilient to ageing
but it is influenced by the decline of inhibitory processes
0010-0285/$ - see front matter Ó2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.11.004
Corresponding author. Fax: +44 (0)207 916 8517.
E-mail address: m.cappelletti@ucl.ac.uk (M. Cappelletti).
1
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Cognitive Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cogpsych
Author's personal copy
supporting number performance, consistent with the ‘Inhibitory
Deficit’ Theory.
Ó2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Does our ability to use numbers and arithmetical concepts change with ageing? Are these changes
specific to numeracy or do they rather reflect decline of more general cognitive processes such as
attention or inhibitory processes? Numerical skills have been extensively studied in children and
young adults, both in terms of development or impairment following brain lesions (Ansari, 2008;
Cappelletti, 2011). However, little is known about the impact of healthy ageing on numerical skills,
and the few studies that investigated this issue focused mostly on arithmetical abilities, i.e. those
required when solving problems such as 8 9 or 243 + 39. These studies concurred to show that
although older participants can learn new ways to solve arithmetical problems, they show a smaller
repertoire of strategies and are less efficient than younger participants in selecting among them (e.g.
Duverne & Lemaire, 2005;Lemaire & Arnaud, 2008;Geary & Lin, 1998; Salthouse & Kersten, 1993), or
that they do not equally engage the same brain regions as younger participants when performing
arithmetical tasks (El Yagoubi, Lemaire, & Besson, 2005). However, these tasks are typically
multi-componential, requiring several processes such as the retrieval of arithmetic facts, the use of
procedures and the ability to monitor the steps of the problem (Cappelletti & Cipolotti, 2011). It
may therefore be difficult to isolate which specific component may be affected by ageing.
An alternative approach to test the impact of ageing on numeracy skills is to assess other simpler skills
(sometimes referred to as ‘biologically primary skills’, Geary & Lin, 1998) which are thought to be founda-
tional to more complex, education- and language-based numerical and arithmetical abilities. One such
foundational skill isthought to be our capacity to represent approximate number, which is basedon encod-
ing numerosities as analog magnitudes (e.g. Izard, Dehaene-Lambertz, & Dehaene, 2008; Stoianov & Zorzi,
2012), and relies on an ‘approximate number system’ (ANS, Feigenson, Dehaene, & Spelke, 2004). The ANS
is often measured in terms of the ability to discriminate numerosities (e.g. which set has more elements),
also referred to as ‘number acuity’ (Halberda,Mazzocco, & Feigenson, 2008). Number acuity is expressed as
Weber fraction (wf), which reflects the amount of noise in the underlying approximate number represen-
tation (Halberdaet al., 2008; Piazza,Izard, Pinel, Le Bihan, & Dehaene, 2004).The wf is highly variable across
individuals (Halberda, Lya, Wilmerb, Naimana, & Germine, 2012; Halberda et al., 2008; Piazza et al., 2004),
and it refines progressively from infancy to adulthood (Halberda et al., 2008; Halberda & Feigenson, 2008;
Lipton & Spelke, 2003, 2012; Piazza et al., 2010). Whether it continues to improve with age is an open ques-
tion: longer exposure to numbers may refine the approximate number system further, similarly to what
happens to other cognitive abilities like vocabulary and semantic memory (e.g. Hedden & Gabrieli,
2004). Notably, number acuity has been found to correlate with math achievement in children (Halberda
et al., 2008; Mazzocco, Feigenson, & Halberda, 2011a) and to be impaired in children with developmental
dyscalculia (Mazzocco, Feigenson, & Halberda, 2011b; Piazza et al., 2010),
A few previous studies have focused on how healthy ageing participants are able to represent approx-
imate large numerosities (i.e., more than 10 elements), which has sometimes been reported to be well
maintained (e.g. Gandini, Lemaire, & Dufau, 2008; Gandini, Lemaire, & Michel, 2009; Lemaire & Lecac-
heur, 2007; Trick, Enns, & Brodeur, 1996; Watson, Maylor, & Bruce, 2005; Watson, Maylor, & Manson,
2002). Some of these previous results, however, are difficult to interpret. This is because in some cases
the focus was mainly on the strategies used to perform the numerosity task, without reporting finer
quantitative details of older participants’ performance (e.g. Gandini et al., 2008). In other studies, the
long presentation of the stimuli (6 s or even unlimited) may have encouraged processes different from
numerosity estimation, like counting (Gandini et al., 2009; Lemaire & Lecacheur, 2007; Watson, Maylor,
& Bruce, 2007). Likewise, some experimental designs did not control for continuous variables which
inevitably vary when manipulating the numerosity of the display, like the total area covered by the dots,
i.e. cumulative area (for discussion see Piazza et al., 2004). If these continuous variables are not taken into
account, for example if in all trials an increase in numerosity always corresponds to an increase in
26 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
cumulative area (e.g., Gandini et al., 2008), it is unclear whether participants judged changes in numer-
osity or in these continuous variables. Finally, participants’ performance cannot be fully characterised
when only measured as percentage of correct answers rather than in terms of finer psychophysical mea-
sures like the wf (Gandini et al., 2008, 2009; Lemaire & Lecacheur, 2007;Watson et al., 2005).
A recent internet-based mega-study showed a different patter of results, indicating that the ability
to discriminate numerosities (as indexed by the wf) may indeed be sensitive to ageing (Halberda et al.,
2012). This age-related deterioration, which was nevertheless not explained by the authors, may re-
flect either the decline of the approximate number system itself or of more peripheral cognitive pro-
cesses that are involved in discriminating numerosities. For instance, working memory, attention and
inhibitory processes are all critical when discriminating numerosity, for example when retrieving the
quantity of a standard set of items to be compared to a test set or when inhibiting task irrelevant infor-
mation, such as the area or the density of elements correlating with numerosity. Since working mem-
ory, attention and inhibitory processes tend to decline with age (Grady, 2012; Hedden & Gabrieli,
2004; Nyberg, Lövdén, Riklund, Lindenberger, & Bäckman, 2012; Salthouse, Atkinson, & Berish,
2003), they may in turn affect performance in numerosity discrimination tasks. In particular, inhibi-
tory processes have been suggested to decline with age and to underlie age-related impairments in
many cognitive functions according to the ‘Inhibitory Deficit’ Theory (Hasher, Lustig, & Zacks, 2007;
Hasher & Zacks, 1988; Hasher, Zacks, & May, 1999). Among the separate functions of inhibition, the
‘restraint function’ (also called ‘inhibition of dominant responses’, Miyake et al., 2000) is the ability
to control strong responses so that others more appropriate for the task goal can be used (Hasher
et al., 2007). This function, which is sensitive to age decline (Kane & Engle, 2003; May & Hasher,
1998), may be particularly relevant in the numerosity discrimination task. This could be the case of
trials where some continuous variables change orthogonally to numerosity, for instance when fewer
large-size dots have a bigger cumulative area than smaller-size but more numerous dots. In this case,
the task-irrelevant but salient information about cumulative area (Hurewitz, Gelman, & Schnitzer,
2006) has to be controlled in order to correctly discriminate numerosity.
Here we studied young and ageing participants in order to explore the precision of their ANS mea-
sured in terms of number acuity in a numerosity discrimination task. Our study is novel not only be-
cause it systematically measured number acuity in older people but also because it aimed to
determine what underlies the pattern of spared or impaired numerosity processing. We reasoned that
if number acuity does not differ between older and young participants, this may be suggestive of
maintained ANS. In contrast, age-related differences in number acuity may reflect impairments spe-
cific to the number system, or alternatively decline of more general cognitive processes. To distinguish
between these two possibilities, our plan was twofold: first, using established neuropsychological
measures, we planned to investigate arithmetical abilities, which are thought to be linked to the
ANS (Halberda et al., 2008) and are therefore expected to be impaired if the ANS is impaired. Second,
using dedicated and well established tasks, we set to investigate the integrity of older participants’
inhibitory processes and in particular of restraint functions which might contribute to any age-related
difference in numerical abilities. Besides studying numerical abilities in older participants in depth
using behavioural measures based on psychophysics and neuropsychology, a second novel aspect of
our research is to combine these measures with a computational approach. This aimed to establish
the type of condition that may resemble our participants’ performance in the numerosity discrimina-
tion task. Our goal was to better understand whether any impairment in numerosity discrimination
may be due to a global deterioration of the number system or to impaired peripheral processes, spe-
cifically those on which inhibitory functions rely on.
2. Behavioral study
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Participants
Sixty right-handed neurologically healthy, education-matched participants with normal or
corrected-to-normal vision gave written consent and were paid to participate in our study which
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 27
Author's personal copy
was approved by the local research Ethics Committee. Participants were selected from the UCL Insti-
tute of Cognitive Neuroscience database because of their age: the 30 young participants had a mean
age of 24.8 years (range 19–36; 13 males); the 30 older participants had a mean age of 65.77 years
(range 60–75; 12 males). Ageing participants were considered neurologically normal on the basis of
self-report and on their performance in the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE, Folstein, Folstein,
& McHugh, 1975; see Table 1). Information on participants’ education and mathematical education in
particular was collected in order to assess the possible impact of these factors on performance in the
experimental tasks. Data in all participants were collected in one or two testing sessions (in the latter
case, they were 7–10 days apart).
2.2. Numerosity discrimination: experimental tasks and stimuli
Stimulus presentation and data collection were controlled using the Cogent Graphics toolbox
(http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/Cogent) and MATLAB 7.3 software on a Sony S2VP laptop computer with
video mode of 640 480 pixels, and 60 Hz refresh rate.
Following earlier studies that investigated number acuity across the lifespan (Halberda et al.,
2012), we used a version of the numerosity discrimination task that required to judge which of two
intermixed collections of coloured dots was more numerous. Sets of dots were presented in blue
and yellow in each display, with 5–16 dots for each colour. The ratios between the larger and the smal-
ler number of dots were 2:1, 4:3, 6:5 and 8:7, with 40 trials presented for each of the easiest ratios (i.e.
2:1 and 4:3), and 120 for each of the most difficult ratios (6:5 and 8:7) (see Table S1). We used a larger
number of trials in the most difficult ratios in order to increase statistical power (i.e., to obtain more
reliable estimates of individual wf) since we expected lower accuracy in these ratios. A total of 320
trials was presented in 10 blocks, and in each ratio the ‘larger’ set was equally assigned to the two
colours.
For each ratio there was also an equal number of congruent (dot-size controlled) and incongruent
(area controlled) trials presented in random order. Dot-size controlled trials were those in which the
average diameter of the dots in the larger set was equal to the average diameter of the smaller set.
The diameter of a dot ranged approximately between 0.57°and 1.17°of visual angle from a distance
of 57 cm, the average diameter being 0.87°. In these trials the cumulative area of the larger set was
always larger than the cumulative area of the smaller one. In contrast, area controlled trials were those
in which the average diameter of the larger set (which ranged approximately between 0.57°and 1.17°
of visual angle, i.e. 0.87 ± 35%) was smaller than the average diameter of the smaller set (which ranged
approximately between ±35% of the average diameter of the smaller set itself). The average diameter
of the smaller set was selected so that the cumulative area of the two sets was equal.
2.2.1. Procedure
Each trial started with a fixation point for 1500 ms followed by a display of blue and yellow dots for
200 ms after which a question mark appeared to prompt participants to respond (see Fig. 1). Partici-
pants were instructed to make an unspeeded answer indicating which group of dots (blue or yellow)
was more numerous by pressing one of two predefined computer keys (M or N), whose assignment
was randomised between participants. Once an answer was made, the following trial started imme-
diately with the 1500 ms fixation.
2.2.2. Data analysis
For all data, the Shapiro–Wilk test confirmed the normality of the distribution, and the data were
analysed using parametric tests (multiple regressions, Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) and t-tests). The
data sphericity was tested using the Mauchy Test, and for significant results Greenhouse–Geisser
correction was applied in case of sphericity violation. Significance was set at a pvalue of 0.05.
Following earlier studies, we fitted the response distribution of each participant in the numerosity
discrimination task to obtain individual estimates of number acuity (i.e., the precision of the underly-
ing numerical representation), expressed as internal Weber fraction (wf, e.g. Halberda et al., 2012,
2008; Piazza et al., 2004, 2010. See SI for a detailed description of how the wf was calculated).
Although speed was not stressed in the instructions given to participants, we analysed response times
28 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
Table 1
Older and young participants’ (A) demographic information, and performance in (B) background tasks, (C) number and arithmetic tasks,
(D) executive and inhibitory processes. Percentile,reaction time(RTs), percent corrector Weber Fraction (wf) with sta ndard deviation ( SD).
Task/information Young participants (N= 30) Older participants (N= 30)
A. Demographic information
Age 24.8 years (range 19–36) 65.77 years (range 60–75)
Gender 13 males 12 males
Years of education 18 16
Years of mathematical education 13 12
B. Background
Full IQ (WAIS-R)
a
116.2 (10.8) 118.3 (13.4)
Mini Mental State Examination
b
nt 29.4 (0.6)
Stimulus identification 293.1 ms (33.72) 348.2 ms (34.0)
Vocabulary
a
83%ile (18.5) 94%ile (6.3)
Digit span
a
82%ile (19.6) 86%ile (12.3)
Door recognition
c
87%ile (15.1) 83%ile (14.7)
C. Number and arithmetic
Numerosity discrimination based on
All trials (wf) 0.24 (0.04) 0.30 (0.07)
Congruent trials (wf) 0.238 (0.06) 0.255 (0.08)
Incongruent trials (wf) 0.256 (0.06) 0.367 (0.15)
Number comparison
Accuracy (% correct) 97.1 (2.5) 98.9 (1.6)
RTs small/large distance
d
576 ms (141)/488 ms (76) 698 ms (112)/607 ms (65)
Arithmetic tasks
Arithmetic verification
Accuracy (% correct); RTs 94.2 (4.4); 1127 ms (241) 97.3 (2.9); 1409 ms (387)
Graded Difficulty Arithmetic Test
e
80.6%ile (18.2) 86.3%ile (19.9)
WAIS-R math sub-test
a
83.1%ile (16.4) 86.3%ile (22.8)
Reading and writing numbers 99.5% (1.9) 98.7% (2.1)
D. Executive and inhibitory processes
Number Stroop task
Magnitude comparison
Congruent 99.3%; 478 ms (67) 99.8%; 745 ms (99)
Neutral 97.17%; 514 ms (64) 99.6%; 789 ms (112)
Incongruent 89.1%; 544 ms (66) 96.4%; 866 ms (120)
Physical comparison
Congruent 99.1%; 457 ms (71) 99.8%; 600 ms (76)
Neutral 99.8%; 470 ms (62) 99.8%; 615 ms (79)
Incongruent 93.1%; 505 ms (82) 99%; 687 ms (84)
Word Stroop task
f
Word target
Congruent 98.7%; 462 ms (55) 99.3%; 602 ms (82)
Neutral 99.7%; 472 ms (48) 99.3%; 612 ms (72)
Incongruent 93.3%; 513 ms (68) 98.0%; 696 ms (72)
Colour target
Congruent 98.3%; 467 ms (102) 99.3%; 611 ms (103)
Neutral 95.6%; 473 ms (114) 99.8%; 610 ms (93)
Incongruent 94.8%; 470 ms (80) 98.8%; 725 ms (161)
Attention network test (ANT)
g
Congruent; Incongruent 448 ms (86); 533 ms (59) 658 ms (82); 766 ms (90)
Conflict: incongruent–congruent 85 ms (20) 98 ms (26)
nt = not tested; ms = milliseconds.
a
Wechsler (1995).
b
Folstein et al. (1975); max score: 30.
c
Baddeley A. D., H., & I., 1994.
d
Longer RTs to small number distances indicate normal distance effect [r= 0.4, F(1, 29) = 14.8, p< 0.001], not different from
young participants [t(58) = 8.4, p< 0.001].
e
Jackson and Warrington (1986).
f
Stroop, 1935.
g
Fan et al. (2002).
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 29
Author's personal copy
to further characterise participants’ performance following other studies using a similar experimental
paradigm and procedure (e.g. Halberda et al., 2012; Piazza et al., 2010).
2.2.3. Results of numerosity discrimination
Accuracy differed across the four numerosity ratios [percent correct, ratio 2:1, Young = 96%
(sd = 0.4), Older = 89% (sd = 0.7); ratio 4:3, Young = 78% (sd = 0.9), Older = 77% (sd = 0.6); ratio 6:5,
Young = 71% (sd = 0.4), Older = 66% (sd = 0.5); ratio 8:7, Young = 65% (sd = 0.6), Older = 62% (sd = 0.4)].
An analysis of the wf in young participants indicated a large variability, such that the wf was on
average 0.24, ranging from 0.16 to 0.33, consistent with previous studies (e.g. Halberda et al., 2008,
2012). Likewise, number acuity in older participants showed large individual differences, being on
average 0.30 and ranging from 0.20 to 0.49.
In a regression analysis with wf as dependent variable and group as independent one, we found
that group was a significant predictor of number acuity [beta = 0.38, SE = 0.16, t= 3.1, p=0.003], as
confirmed by a significant difference between older and young participants [t(58) = 3.2, p= 0.002,
see Fig. 2 left panel]. A more specific analysis looking at the variability of performance in the older
sample showed that within the older group 21 out of 30 participants showed a wf that was one or
two standard deviations below the average of the younger group, indicating defective performance.
This suggests that the group difference we observed was not simply driven by a few participants,
thereby mirroring the results of Halberda et al.’s (2012) internet mega-study.
We also tested whether education in general and mathematical education in particular may predict
number acuity. This is because mathematical expertise (e.g. in accountants and bookkeepers) has pre-
viously been shown to explain differences in memory ability for arbitrary numbers and for grocery-
store prices between expert and non-expert ageing participants (Castel, 2005, 2007). Although our
Fig. 1. Numerosity discrimination task. (A) Participants saw a centrally presented fixation cross for 1500 ms immediately
followed by a brief (200 ms) presentation of a display of yellow and blue dots (for the purpose of the figure displayed in black
and white). Participants were instructed to indicate with a button press whether each trial contained more blue or yellow dots,
with no time constrains. The following trial was displayed immediately after the answer. (B) Example of area-controlled
(incongruent) and dot-size controlled (congruent) trials. Participants were presented with 320 trials; half of these trials
contained yellow and blue dots having on average the same cumulative area and whose diameter was inversely correlated to
numerosity, implying that the least numerous set had the larger dot diameter (incongruent trials, bottom panel of B). The other
half of the trials contained yellow and blue dots of approximately the same average diameter such that the larger display had
larger cumulative area (congruent trials, top panel of B).
30 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
sample did not include mathematical experts, we nevertheless explored whether education and
mathematical education may in part explain the difference in number acuity we observed. However,
we found that neither of these predictors were significant [education: beta = 0.08, SE = 0.004, t= 0.58,
p= 0.6; mathematical education: beta = 0.009, SE = 0.004, t= 0.06, p= 0.9].
The difference in wf might suggest that relative to younger participants, older participants needed a
larger discrepancy between the two stimulus sets to be able to identify the larger, which may be due
to a deteriorated representation of numerosity.
2.3. Arithmetical tasks
Impaired performance in the numerosity discrimination task may reflect a deteriorated represen-
tation of numerosity, which has been suggested to be linked to arithmetical skills, following the pro-
posal that number acuity is foundational to these skills (Halberda et al., 2008). We therefore tested
older and younger participants’ arithmetical abilities, predicting a possible impairment in older par-
ticipants if these skills are linked to ANS.
2.3.1. Stimuli and methods for the arithmetical tasks
Stimuli for the arithmetical tasks were Arabic numbers in the form of single to 3 digits presented
either on the computer, on paper or read aloud by the experimenter. We used the following four tasks:
arithmetic verification, number comparison, multi-digit mental arithmetic, and arithmetical problem
solving.
Arithmetic verification required participants to indicate as fast as possible whether an arithmetic
problem displayed the correct or incorrect answer (using the ‘F’ or ‘J’ keys of the keyboard). Twenty
single-digit problems for each type of operation (addition, subtraction and multiplication) were pre-
sented in separate blocks with no repetition of the same items in immediately successive trials. All
problems had operands below 10; results of the addition and subtraction problems were between 1
and 18, and of multiplication problems were between 6 and 36. Following a 500 ms central fixation
Fig. 2. Behavioural results. Psychometric functions (PF) indicating young (in light gray) and older (in black) participants’
performance in the numerosity discrimination task measured in terms of wf in: (A) all conditions (i.e. average of dot-size
and area-controlled trials), (B) dot-size controlled trials only, and (C) area-controlled trials only. The psychometric
functions plot the probability of choosing ‘more blue’ dots (y-axis) as a function of the ratio between blue and yellow
dots (x-axis).
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 31
Author's personal copy
cross, each operation was presented for up to 7 s during which participants could answer. When pre-
sented with incorrect results, these were either 1 or 2 units apart from the correct result for addition
and subtraction problems (e.g. 9 + 6 = 13 or 7–2 = 3) or 2 units apart for multiplication problems (e.g.
63 = 16).
Number Comparison required indicating as fast as possible the larger of two Arabic numbers pre-
sented to the left and right of a central fixation point (using the ‘F’ and ‘J’ keys of the keyboard to indi-
cate the larger number appearing on the left or on the right respectively). Thirty-six pairs of single-
digit Arabic numbers (1–9) were individually presented.
2
Stimuli pair were centred along the horizon-
tal line of the computer screen and displayed for 500 ms each to the left or the right of the fixation cross;
they were replaced by a black screen for a maximum of 2500 ms during which participants made an an-
swer. After this, the next trial started immediately. The following numerical distances were used: 1 (e.g.
7 vs 8), 2 (e.g. 3 vs 1), 3 (e.g. 5 vs 2), 4 (e.g. 1 vs 5), 5 (e.g. 4 vs 9). The larger number in each pair was
equally presented to the left and to the right; likewise, correct answers were equally assigned to the left
or the right digit in each pair.
Multi-digit mental arithmetic was tested with the Graded Difficulty Arithmetical Test (GDA, Jack-
son & Warrington, 1986), a standardised test based on two separate blocks of twelve 2 to 3-digit addi-
tion and twelve 2 to 3-digit subtraction problems of progressive difficulty (e.g. from ‘13 + 15’ to
‘243 + 149’). These were orally presented one at a time for an oral answer, which scored 1 point if cor-
rectly produced within 10 s.
Arithmetical problem solving was assessed with the arithmetical subtest of the Wechsler Intelli-
gence Scale (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1995), consisting of twenty arithmetical problems embedded in a text
and orally presented for an oral answer. Correct answers produced within a maximum time (spanning
from 15 to 60 s depending on the problem) were assigned 1 point.
Performance in these numerical and arithmetical tasks was expressed as accuracy and/or reaction
times (RTs) of correct answers only; reaction times were collected in the number comparison and the
arithmetic verification tasks only.
Since performance in arithmetical skills are often more sensitive to speed than accuracy, and since
speed tends to increase with age (e.g. Salthouse, 1991), we measured older participants’ general speed
of performance in two ways. The first way was based on a simple perceptual task, which required par-
ticipants to respond as rapidly as possible to a dot stimulus appearing on the computer monitor at var-
ious locations. Forty trials were presented, each displaying a dot for 200 ms in random locations on the
left or right of a computer monitor and following an ISI randomly selected between 500 ms and 2 s.
Participants were instructed to make speeded responses whenever the stimulus appeared by pressing
a pre-defined key. Accuracy and response times were recorded.
The second way consisted of measuring older participants’ speed of performance using a two-
choice non-numerical task whose modality of response resembled the type of choice participants
had to make in the number and arithmetical tasks. Specifically, we used responses made in the Stroop
‘neutral’ conditions of the Word Stroop task (averaged across the case where the target colour was
presented on the neutral string ‘XXX’, and where the target word was presented in the neural colour
grey; see also Section 2.4.2.4).
2.3.2. Results of arithmetical tests
Remarkably, we found that young and older participants accuracy did not significantly differ in any
of the number and arithmetical tasks (all P> 0.4, see Table 1). In older participants, proficiency in the
numerosity task correlated with accuracy in the arithmetical tasks [arithmetic verification: r= 0.51,
F(1,29) = 9.7, p< 0.004; multi-digit arithmetic (GDA): r= 0.42, F(1, 29) = 6.1, p< 0.02; arithmetic prob-
lem solving (WAIS): r= 0.36, F(1, 29) = 4.2, p< 0.05], but not in number comparison [r= 0.15,
F(1,29) = 0.6, p= 0.4, ns].
Despite maintained accuracy, older participants were significantly slower than young to perform
the number comparison and the arithmetic verification tasks [t(58) = 13.1, p< 0.001 and t(58) = 3.2,
2
The pairs of Arabic numbers were as follows; distance 1: 1–2, 3–2, 4–3, 5–4, 6–7, 7–8, 8–9; distance 2: 3–1, 2–4, 4–6, 5–3, 7–5,
6–8, 9–7; distance 3: 4–1, 2–5, 3–6, 7–4, 8–5, 6–9, 5–2, 1–4; distance 4: 1–5, 3–7, 4–8, 5–9, 6–2, 7–3, 8–4, 9–5; distance 5: 1–6, 2–
7, 8–3, 9–4.
32 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
p< 0.01 respectively). We therefore tested whether this slowness was specific for the number tasks or
whether instead it reflected general slowness. We run a fixed-entry hierarchical regression with the
basic RTs, the choice RTs and group entered as independent variables in different steps, and RTs (sep-
arate for arithmetic verification and number comparison) as the dependent variable. Results showed a
significant effect of group [arithmetical verification: beta = 275.6, SE = 133.5, t= 2.0, p< 0.05;
number comparison: beta = 245.6, SE = 49.2, t= 4.9, p< 0.001], suggesting that older participants
were generally slower than younger. However, a non-significant effect of RTs in the basic perceptual
speed task [arithmetical verification: beta = 1.8, SE = 1.0, t= 1.7, p= 0.09, ns; number comparison:
beta = 0.5, SE = 0.38, t= 1.3, p= 0.18, ns] suggests that basic RTs did not explain participants’ slowness
in the number and arithmetic tasks. Instead, a significant effect of two-choice RTs [arithmetic verifi-
cation: beta = 1.9, SE = 0.39, t= 4.8, p< 0.001; number comparison: beta = 0.32, SE = 0.14, t= 2.2,
p= 0.03] which contributed to 65% and 88% of the variance in the two tasks respectively, accounted
for participants’ slowness in the number and arithmetic tasks.
2.4. The role of other cognitive processes in number acuity
Our results suggest that despite a larger wf, older participants’ numerical and arithmetical skills
were well maintained.This therefore does not support one of our initial hypotheses that a larger wf
may reflect a more general impairment in number and arithmetical processing.
An alternative possibility is that a larger wf may be explained by impairments in inhibiting task-
irrelevant information, an hypothesis that has been previously put forward to account for elderly’s im-
paired performance in other cognitive functions (Hasher & Zacks, 1988; Hasher et al., 1999, 2007). If
this is the case, differences between congruent and incongruent numerosity trials may be expected,
since incongruent trials required inhibiting task-irrelevant information coming from continuous
variables such as the size of the dots that negatively correlated with numerosity (Dakin, Tibber, Green-
wood, Kingdom, & Morgan, 2011; Gebuis & Reynvoet, 2012; Hurewitz et al., 2006). In addition, we also
examined older participants’ inhibitory skills independently from number acuity.
2.4.1. Congruent vs incongruent numerosity trials
To investigate whether older participants’ larger wf may reflect a difficulty to inhibit information
that may be irrelevant to numerosity judgments, separate wf for congruent (dot-size controlled)
and incongruent (area controlled) trials in the numerosity discrimination task were calculated and
compared within and between groups using paired and independent t-tests respectively. Relative to
congruent trials, incongruent numerosity trials resulted in a larger wf in both groups (older: congru-
ent = 0.255 and incongruent = 0.367; young: congruent = 0.238 and incongruent = 0.256), but with a
significant difference between trial types only in the older [t(29) = 3.9, p< 0.001] but not in the young
participants [t(29) = 0.9, p= 0.3, ns], see Fig. 2 middle panel. The wf differed between the two groups
only in the incongruent [t(58) = 3.7, p< 0.001] but not in the congruent trials [t(58) = 0.1, p= 0.4, ns;
see Fig. 2 right panel].
In older participants, age correlated with performance in the incongruent numerosity trials [r= 0.3,
F(1,29) = 2.9, p<0.04] but not with the congruent ones [r= 0.12, F(1, 29) = 0.4, p=0.5, ns].
2.4.2. Numerosity discrimination task with blocked congruent and incongruent trials
Having found that older participants were specifically impaired in the incongruent numerosity
trials as indexed by a significantly larger wf relative to young participants, we aimed to identify which
factors may account for such impairment. One possibility is that older participants’ impaired incon-
gruent numerosity trials might be related to the strategies used to perform these trials. For instance,
in order to identify the more numerous set older participants may have used a strategy based on just
numerosity, and also other strategies, for example based on a salient non-numerical visual feature
(like the area covered by the dots or amount of colour as a proxy for numerosity). These numerosity
and area-based strategies may have been successfully used in the case of incongruent and congruent
trials respectively, but switching between them may have been particularly difficult for ageing partic-
ipants. We reasoned that a difficulty in switching among strategies may be ameliorated by keeping the
congruent and incongruent numerosity trials blocked. We therefore administered participants the
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 33
Author's personal copy
same numerosity task but with the congruent and incongruent trials in blocked rather than random-
ized order, and with a trial-by-trial feedback to help maintaining correct performance. Since we only
found a large wf in the older participants, we did not test younger participants in this blocked and
feedback-based version of the numerosity task.
3
2.4.2.1. Stimuli and method. We used the same stimuli, design and procedure previously employed (see
Section 2.2), with the following changes. First, the same number (N= 320) and type of trials previously
used were now presented in random order in 5 blocks of 32 congruent trials and 5 blocks of 32 incon-
gruent trials. The 5 blocks of congruent and the 5 blocks of incongruent trials were grouped and the
order was counterbalanced across participants. Second, to encourage correct performance, trial-by-
trial feedback was introduced at the end of each trial, indicating whether participants made a correct
or wrong answer. All the other features of the task were exactly the same as before (see Section 2.2).
Performance was calculated in terms of wf with the same procedure previously used; we also mea-
sured response times, although speed was not stressed in the instructions to participants.
2.4.2.2. Results. The wf and response times obtained in the numerosity task with blocked and with the
random trial presentation were compared in independent t-tests. There was no significant advantage
in presenting trials in a blocked fashion and with feedback relative to the canonical random presen-
tation, indicated by an almost unchanged wf (average random condition: 0.29 vs blocked condition:
0.31; t(11) = 0.3, p= 0.7, ns). This suggests that it is unlikely that higher wf in older participants is sim-
ply due to problems in switching among strategies, for instance from a numerosity-based to an area-
based rule. Further support to this suggestion comes from the analysis of the response times, which
were examined in an ANOVA with presentation type (blocked vs random) and trial type (congruent
vs incongruent) as within-subject factor. This showed no main effect of presentation [F(1,11) = 0.8,
p= 0.3, ns] and of trial type [F(1,11) = 0.7, p= 0.4, ns], and no significant interaction [F(1, 11) = 0.02,
p= 0.8, ns], therefore suggesting no difference between the blocked and the random conditions.
2.4.2.3. Analysis of response times in the numerosity task. The pattern of response times in the numer-
osity discrimination task (data from the main experiment, see Section 2.2) was analysed to assess the
hypothesis that group differences may be due to a speed-accuracy trade-off for the older participants.
We used a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with response times and accuracy (wf) for the
congruent and incongruent trials entered as dependent variables, and group (young and older) and
type of trial (congruent and incongruent) as fixed factors. Both group and trial type were significant
[respectively: F(2,115) = 16.04, p< 0.001, Wilk’s
K
= 0.78; and F(2,115) = 15.05, p< 0.001, Wilk’s
K
= 0.79], as well as their interaction [F(2,115) = 4.8, p< 0.01, Wilk’s
K
= 0.9]. Specifically, group
had a significant impact on both accuracy [F(1, 116) = 7.3, p< 0.001] and response times
[F(1,116) = 26.3, p< 0.001], and likewise trial type significantly affected accuracy [F(1, 116) = 21.9,
p< 0.00] and response times [F(1, 116) = 9.7, p= 0.002]. Group and trial type also interacted with accu-
racy [F(1,116) = 5.1, p< 0.02] and with response times [F(1,116) = 4.1, p= 0.04]. This is because young
participants were more accurate than older in incongruent (wf: 0.256 vs 0.367; t(58) = 3.7, p<0.001)
but not in congruent trials (wf: 0.238 vs 0.255; t(58) = 0.1, p=0.4, ns), and response times for congru-
ent and incongruent numerosity trials differed in the older [617 ms vs 649 ms, t(29) = 2.3, p=0.02] but
not in the young participants [415 ms vs 427 ms, t(29) = 0.9, p=0.3, ns]. This RTs significant difference
in trial type in the ageing group was maintained even when older participants’ general slowness was
taken into account in a regression analysis. Here response times for the congruent and incongruent
numerosity trials were separately entered as dependent variable in the analysis, with group and
two-choice RTs as independent variables. There was a significant effect of group in the incongruent
trials only [congruent: beta = 0.3, SE = 65.7, t= 1.5, p= 0.13, ns; incongruent: beta = 3.9, SE = 71.8,
t= 2.0, p= 0.048], suggesting that older participants were slower than younger only in the incongruent
trials. In these trials there was no main effect of choice RTs [beta = 0.25, SE = 0.22, t= 1.2, p= 0.2, ns],
indicating that slowness in performing incongruent trials could not be accounted for by general
3
Only 12 older participants could be tested on this additional version of the numerosity task.
34 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
slowness, although this seemed to be case for congruent trials where choice RTs were marginally sig-
nificant [beta = 3.5, SE = 0.2, t= 1.8, p= 0.06]. These findings suggest that older participants did not
trade off their accuracy for response speed. Indeed, both accuracy and RT data revealed a specific dif-
ficulty of older participants in processing incongruent trials.
These results suggest that larger wf in older participants were due to differences in wf in the incon-
gruent but not the congruent trials, possibly because these trials relied upon inhibitory skills. We
therefore aimed to explore these skills in more details in our older participants. We focused on inhi-
bition not only because it is critical for processing the incongruent numerosity trials but also because
it has been suggested to decline with age and to underlie age-related impairments in many other cog-
nitive functions (Hasher & Zacks, 1988; Hasher et al., 1999, 2007).
2.4.2.4. Older participants’ performance in inhibitory tasks. Using standard tests, we specifically focused
on the restraint function among inhibitory processes, i.e. the ability to control strong responses such that
others more appropriate for the task goal can be used (Hasher, Lustig, & Zacks, 2007). We reasoned that
this function may be particularly relevant in the numerosity discrimination task and specifically in the
incongruent trials where information about the area covered by the dots had to be controlled in order to
correctly discriminate numerosity. We used the following standard tasks to assess inhibitory processes
in older participants: Word Stroop, Number Stroop, Attention Network test (ANT).
The word Stroop task requires participants to read as quickly as possible either a word ignoring the
colour of the ink it is printed on (for instance ‘RED’ whether printed in colour red, blue or in grey for a
neutral condition), or to name the colour in which words are printed ignoring their meaning (for in-
stance to name the colour red whether displayed on the word ‘RED’, ‘BLUE’ or on ‘XXX’ for a neutral
condition). There were 60 trials for each task (word or colour). For the word task, stimuli were the
word ‘RED’ or ‘BLUE’, which could appear in red, blue or gray colour; this therefore resulted in a con-
gruent, incongruent or neutral condition (20 trials for each type), depending on whether, for example,
the word ‘RED’ appeared in colour red, or in colour blue or in gray. In the colour task, stimuli were the
word ‘RED’, ‘BLUE’ or a string of ‘XXX’. These could appear in colour red or blue, therefore resulting in a
congruent, incongruent or neutral condition (20 trials for each type), depending on whether, for exam-
ple, the colour red appeared on the word RED or on the word BLUE or on the string XXX.
In each trial, participants saw a centrally presented 500 ms fixation cross, followed by a word stim-
ulus displayed until the participant made an answer or for a maximum of 4000 ms. After this, the fol-
lowing trial started immediately. Participants were asked to decide as quickly as possible whether the
stimulus was the word ‘RED’ or ‘BLUE’ irrespective of the colour (in the word task), or whether it was
displayed in red or blue font irrespective of the meaning of the word (colour task); they were in-
structed to press the left and right arrow keys for blue (word or colour) and red (word or colour)
respectively. The two tasks were presented separately, and the order of the tasks was counterbalanced
across participants.
For each task (word or colour), accuracy and response times were calculated for the three conditions:
neutral (corresponding to the target word printed in grey or the target colour printed on XXX), congru-
ent (target word printed on the corresponding colour or the target colour on the corresponding word),
and incongruent (target word printed on a different colour like ‘RED’ printed in blue, or the target colour
printed on a different word, like colour red on the word blue). These three conditions allowed calculating
an index of ‘congruity’ (i.e. response times of correct answers only in incongruent–congruent trials), a
standard measure of participants’ ability to inhibit task irrelevant information (Stroop, 1935).
The number Stroop task is based on an established paradigm that assesses the automatic process-
ing of numbers as well as inhibitory processes using experimental stimuli that contain congruent and
incongruent information (Henik & Tzelgov, 1982). In two separate tasks, participants viewed a total of
336 pairs of 1–9 Arabic numbers (168 per block) that could vary in magnitude (e.g. 3 vs 2) or physical
size (e.g. 3 vs 2). There were therefore three types of stimuli (each of 36 trials per task): a congruent
stimulus corresponded to a pair of digits in which a given digit was larger in both the relevant and the
irrelevant dimensions; a neutral stimulus was a pair of digits that differed only on the relevant dimen-
sion (magnitude or physical size); an incongruent stimulus consisted of a pair of digits in which one of
the digits was at the same time larger in one dimension (e.g. magnitude) and smaller in the other (e.g.
physical size). Each number stimulus could be paired to itself, therefore consisting of a neutral
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 35
Author's personal copy
stimulus for the physical size condition (e.g. 2 vs 2), or to another number stimulus which could be
between 1 and 4 units apart. Moreover, the two number stimuli could be of the same physical size,
therefore consisting of the neutral stimulus for the magnitude condition, or they could vary along
two levels of physical size, as stimuli could appear in a vertical visual angle of 0.7°or 0.9°centred
along the horizontal line of the computer screen to the left or the right of the fixation cross.
Participants were required to indicate the larger number in either magnitude or physical size by
pressing either the left or the right arrow if the larger number was presented either to the left or to
the right. Following a 500 ms fixation cross, the number stimuli were presented until the participant
made an answer or for a maximum of 4000 ms. After this, the next fixation cross appeared and the
following trial started immediately. For each task (magnitude or physical size), accuracy and response
times were recorded. Using this experimental design, it is common to find a congruity effect which
consists of facilitation, i.e. faster responding to stimuli in which information about the magnitude
and the physical size of the stimuli is congruent (e.g. 3 vs 2) relative to neutral trials (e.g. 3 vs 3 for
physical comparisons or 3 vs 2 for numerical comparisons), and of interference namely slower re-
sponses to stimuli in which information about the magnitude and the physical size of the stimuli is
incongruent (e.g. 3 vs 2) relative to neutral trials (Henik & Tzelgov, 1982). These effects are thought
to reflect automatic processing of numerical and physical size even when magnitude or size are
task-irrelevant; this is suggested by studies showing that irrespective of which dimension is relevant,
response times for congruent trials are always shorter than response times for incongruent trials, and
for incongruent trials shorter than for neutral trials, although an advantage for congruent relative to
neutral trials has only been observed in the magnitude condition (Henik & Tzelgov, 1982; Tzelgov,
Meyer, & Henik, 1992). This task therefore served the double purpose of measuring participants’ auto-
matic number processing and, critically, also their ability to inhibit task-irrelevant information.
The Attention Network Test (ANT, Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002) examines exec-
utive and inhibitory processes using a variation of the flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974) whereby
participants attend to one object while ignoring others (Posner, 1980). In the version used here, a cu-
ing task and a flanker task were combined such that participants responded to cued or un-cued central
targets while ignoring flanking distractors. A total of 288 trials were presented in 3 blocks of 96 trials
each. The stimuli consisted of a target arrow flanked by two arrows on either side, which could appear
in the same direction as the target arrow (congruent condition e.g. ) or in the opposite
direction (incongruent condition e.g. ? ). Following Fan and colleagues’ paradigm (2002),
each arrow was presented at 0.55°of visual angle and separated from the adjacent arrows by 0.06°
of visual angle. The stimuli (the central arrow and the four flankers) consisted of a total 3.08°of visual
angle. Participants were instructed to attend to the middle arrow and to decide whether it was point-
ing to the left or to the right. A trial consisted of the following events: a central fixation cross was first
presented for a random duration between 400 and 1600 ms, and followed by either a 100 ms warning
asterisk cue in the cued trials or by a longer fixation in the un-cued trials, and by a second 400 ms fix-
ation period after which the target and the flankers appeared simultaneously and centrally, at 1.06°of
visual angle either above or below the fixation point. The cue could appear centrally, hence corre-
sponding to a spatially neutral condition or it could precede the target and flankers in the same posi-
tion (the cue was always valid), i.e. at 1.06°of visual angle above or below the fixation point, which
corresponded to a spatially orienting condition. The target and flankers remained on the screen until
the participant responded or for a maximum of 1700 ms. After a response was made, the next trial be-
gan immediately.
Participants had to press as quickly as possible a left-hand key if the central arrow pointed left and a
right-hand key if it pointed right. The task allows measuring three indexes of performance based on
how response times of correct answers are influenced by alerting cues, spatial cues, and flankers: alertness
(cued vs un-cued trials), orienting (central cue vs spatialcue), and conflict(congruent vs incongruent trials
averaged across cued and un-cued, and central vs spatial cue). As conflict is the critical component we
aimed to measure, we limited the analysis of the performance to this index of behaviour.
2.4.2.5. Results of tasks assessing inhibitory processes. Older participants significantly differed from
younger participants in the ability to suppress task-irrelevant information (see Table 1). In both tasks
of the Word Stroop (i.e. where the target was either a word or a colour), older participants were
36 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
significantly slower in incongruent than congruent trials relative to young (word Stroop task:
t(58) = 3.2, p=0.002; colour Stroop task: t(58) = 7.5, p<0.001). Likewise in both the Number Stroop
tasks and in the ANT, older participants showed a larger congruity effect relative to young participants
[Number Stroop tasks: magnitude comparison, t(58) = 3.7, p<0.001; physical size comparison,
t(58) = 2.1, p<0.04; ANT, t(58) = 2.7, p=0.008]. Accuracy was near ceiling in both groups (98.6%
and 98.9% correct in older and young respectively), and did not differ between groups [t(58) = 0.76,
p= 0.54, ns].
Critically, older participants’ poor performance in these tasks correlated with wf in the incongruent
numerosity trials. Hence, the worse the elderly’s performance in these incongruent relative to congruent
trials, the longer weretheir response times in the incongruent trials of the Stroop tasks and of the Attention
Network Test [Word Stroop (averaged over word and colour target conditions): r=0.38,p< 0.04; Number
Stroop (averaged over magnitude and physical conditions): r=0.4,p=0.03;ANT:r=0.37,p=0.04].This
was not the case of performance in the congruent trials which did not correlate with any measure of inhi-
bition p rocesses [Word Stroop : r=0.24,p<0.2,ns;NumberStroop:r=1.5,p=0.4,ns;ANT:r= 0.06, p=0.7,
ns]. The equivalent analyses in young participants showed no link between wf in congruent or incongruent
numerosity trials and response times in any of the above tasks [all P>0.1].
These results suggest a link between wf in the incongruent trials and older participants’ perfor-
mance in tests of inhibitory processes. This in turn may reflect deteriorated inhibitory processes rather
than impoverished number processing, as this would have likely resulted in impaired performance in
other arithmetical tasks. To further support the hypothesis that larger wf may reflect deteriorated
inhibitory processes, we performed a set of computer simulations based on a recent neurocomputa-
tional model of numerosity perception (Stoianov & Zorzi, 2012).
3. Computational modelling study
The connectionist model of numerosity perception developed by Stoianov and Zorzi (2012) is a
deep neural network with two hierarchically organised layers of (hidden) neurons. The first layer re-
ceives input from the image and it consists of uniformly spread center-On local detectors (high fre-
quency spatial filters). The second layer consists of local numerosity detectors that receive input
from the center-On neurons normalised by an inhibitory signal representing the image cumulative
area. The activity of numerosity detectors (layer 2 neurons) is used as input to a simple linear classifier
that is trained to decide whether the input numerosity is greater than a reference number. Perfor-
mance of the model in the numerosity comparison task is evaluated in the same way as human par-
ticipants, that is by computing the model’s number acuity (wf) from the accuracy data.
The main aim of our simulations was to establish what type of impairment would produce the dis-
sociation between intact performance on dot-size controlled (‘‘congruent’’) trials and declined perfor-
mance on area-controlled (‘‘incongruent’’) trials observed in the elderly population. Rather than fitting
the participants’ performance, the simulation aimed to offer an explanation at the level of the mech-
anisms that subtend numerosity discrimination in the model. To provide a closer match to the ANS
task used in the present behavioral study, the model was extended to simulate the comparison of
two variable visual numerosities.
3.1. Computer simulations
3.1.1. Stimuli
Using the method described in Stoianov and Zorzi (2012), we created visual numerosity images of
size 30 30 pixels, each containing up to 32 randomly placed non-overlapping rectangular objects of
variable size and shape
4
(see samples in Fig. 3). Object size varied within each pattern according to a
Gaussian term with mean = 0 and sd = 0.5. We created a database of images containing objects with
4
Note that the range of numerosities (with upper limit of 32 objects) was selected by Stoianov and Zorzi (2012) to allow the
simulation of the typical experimental setting of numerosity comparison studies (e.g., Piazza et al., 2004). This requires a minimum
image size of 30 30 pixels to keep all objects spatially separated by at least one pixel in the largest numerosity condition.
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 37
Author's personal copy
variable cumulative area that ranged from 32 to 256 pixels at 8 levels, with 200 images for each level of
numerosity (n= 32) and cumulative area (n= 8). We then compiled a training set by drawing 10240 pairs
of images from the image database, ensuring that numerosity and cumulative area were balanced. The
training set was used to train the linear classifier on the numerosity comparison task, while the model’s
performance (i.e., wf) was assessed using an independent set of test images that were not used for train-
ing (as in Stoianov & Zorzi, 2012).
We also created a database of test images that had the same characteristics of the stimuli used in
Experiment 1. In particular, area-controlled test images had constant cumulative area of 100 pixels,
whereas size-controlled test images contained objects with constant size (3 3 pixels). Cumulative
area was not predictive of numerosity in the area-controlled images but it was correlated with the
numerosity in the size-controlled ones. The database contained 200 images for each of the numeros-
ities and conditions (size-controlled vs. area-controlled) of the behavioural study. The test set was cre-
ated by drawing 1100 pairs of test images for each of the two conditions, yielding the same
numerosity pairs (and ratios) used in the behavioural study.
3.1.2. Model details
The simulations extended the simplified version of the model described in the Supplementary
Material of Stoianov and Zorzi (2012) where the numerosity perception network is hard-wired and
training involves only the numerosity discrimination task. The model is depicted in Fig. 3. It is worth
noting that the architecture and the parameters of the model (e.g., size of the receptive fields) were
Fig. 3. Computational model. (A) Hierarchical network for numerosity perception (Stoianov & Zorzi, 2012). The input image
(30 30 pixels) is processed by one layer of center-on detectors with small-size receptive fields. Numerosity detectors at the
top layer have larger receptive fields and compute a local numerosity signal by combining the activity of center-on detectors
with an inhibitory normalization signal conveying the image’s cumulative area (see text for details). Poor numerosity
discrimination in the elderly population can be accounted for by degraded inhibitory normalization. (B) Numerosity comparison
task. Activity of the numerosity detectors obtained for each image of a pair was used as input to a linear classifier, that is a
simple network trained to decide which of the two numerosites was larger.
38 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
identical to those reported in the original study and they were chosen as to faithfully represent the
structure emerged (i.e., self-organised) in the learning model. For the present purposes, a key advan-
tage of the simplified mathematical model is that it clearly identifies the functionality of each model
component, such as the role of inhibitory signals (see below).
The numerosity perception network has two hierarchically organised layers of neurons above the
visual input layer. The first layer (size: 13 13 neurons) receives input from a binary image Iand it
consists of uniformly spread center-On local detectors (i.e. high frequency spatial filters) activated
according to the following equation:
O
ij
¼fðXV
ij
IþbÞ ð1Þ
where V
ij
are 2D-gaussian-shaped receptive fields (
r
= 2) over the image I,bis a bias term (fixed to
1), and f(.) is the logistic function. The second layer (size: 6 6 neurons) consists of uniformly spread
local numerosity detectors activated according to the following equation:
N
kl
¼XW
kl
Ocð2Þ
where W
kl
are 2D-Gaussian-shaped receptive fields (
r
= 6) over the center-ON neurons and cis a
(inhibitory) normalisation term based on the image cumulative area,
c¼log 1 þPI
c
max
 ð3Þ
where c
max
is the maximal cumulative area across the image database. The activity of the numerosity
detectors coarsely represents numerosity. Indeed, Stoianov and Zorzi (2012) showed that the response
profile of the population activity of layer 2 numerosity neurons closely resembled the monotonic cod-
ing of numerosity observed in monkey LIP neurons (Roitman, Brannon, & Platt, 2007) and that it was
invariant to perceptual properties of the stimuli, such as cumulative area.
Stoianov and Zorzi (2012) showed that the activity of numerosity detectors supported human-like
performance in numerosity comparison when used as input to a linear classifier that was trained to
compare numerosities to a fixed reference. More specifically, the activity of the numerosity detectors
elicited by a given input image was fed to a decision layer encoding a ‘‘smaller’’ vs. ‘‘larger’’ judgment.
This simple linear network was trained using the delta rule, which is formally equivalent to the Resc-
orla-Wagner learning rule and has been widely used to account for human learning (Sutton & Barto,
1981). Thus, the model assumes that numerosity comparison is a simple linear task when the input to
the decision is an abstract representation of numerosity. This is line with Dehaene and Changeux
(1993) computational model of the development of early numerical abilities and it fits well with
the observation that the classic numerosity comparison paradigm (with explicit judgment/response)
can be carried out even by 3–4 year old children (Halberda et al., 2008; Piazza et al., 2010). The model
responses, when plotted as a function of numerical ratio, yielded a psychometric function that per-
fectly mirrored those of adult observers, thereby obeying Weber’s law. Moreover, the wf computed
from the model’s response distribution was virtually identical to the mean value reported for adult
observers (e.g., Piazza et al., 2010). The model’s descriptive adequacy was therefore supported by
its match to both behavioral (i.e., human psychophysics) and neural (i.e., single cell recording) data
(see Stoianov & Zorzi, 2012, for further details).
Here we simulated the comparison of two variable numerosities (as in the behavioral paradigm of
Experiment 1 and in the studies of Halberda et al., 2008, 2012) by feeding two internal numerosity
representations (i.e., the activity of the numerosity detectors for each image of a given pair in the
training set) to a linear classifier that was trained to decide which of the two numerosities was larger.
Learning was based on the delta rule and the training database was presented for 100 epochs (when
learning became markedly asymptotic). Model’s performance after training (i.e., model’s wf) was as-
sessed on the image pairs contained in the test set. Because the representation of each numerosity
(activity of numerosity detectors) is intrinsically noisy, we expected greater response variability
(i.e., a higher wf) in this version of the task relative to the original simulations of Stoianov and Zorzi
(2012) where a single numerosity was compared to a fixed reference number.
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 39
Author's personal copy
3.2. Elderly model
To simulate the declined performance of elderly population, we implemented a degradation to the
network connections by applying a stochastic decay term to the synaptic strengths (i.e., weight val-
ues). This stochastic reduction of synaptic efficacy results in neurons’ decreased responsivity to affer-
ent signals. Decreased responsivity of neurons, which turns into a weaker signal-to-noise ratio (i.e.,
increased neuronal noise) is also critical in the neuromodulation model of ageing of Li and colleagues
(Li, Lindenberger, & Bäckman, 2010; Li, Lindenberger, & Sikström, 2001). In their model, this is ob-
tained by flattening the slope of the neurons’ sigmoid activation function, which is contolled by a sto-
chastic gain parameter. Li and colleagues link the value of the gain parameter to dopaminergic
modulation, which is known to be altered in elderly population.
We implemented two different types of impairment: a global degradation of the numerosity per-
ception network, which involved all network synapses, or a selective degradation of the inhibitory
synapses (in line with the inhibition deficit hypothesis). Inhibition in the model is critical for abstract-
ing numerosity from continuous visual properties, specifically cumulative area (see Eq. (2)). Indeed,
the normalisation signal represents an inhibitory input to the numerosity detectors that is conveyed
through feed-forward inhibitory connections (see Fig. 3). Feed-forward inhibition is known to act as
input normalisation mechanism in real neurons (e.g., Pouille, Marin-Burgin, Adesnik, Atallah, & Scan-
ziani, 2009). In the present model, the excitatory input to numerosity detectors increases both with
numerosity and cumulative area (because large objects will tend to activate more center-on detectors
than small objects) but is normalised by an inhibitory input whose strength also increases with cumu-
lative area (see Stoianov & Zorzi, 2012, for detailed analyses).
Synaptic degradation was obtained by scaling the connection weights by a random value (range [0–
1]) drawn from a Gaussian distribution with mean = mand s.d. = 0.1 (smaller values of mdetermine
stronger impairment). Numerosity comparison in the model was adjusted after the impairment
through a short re-training (5 epochs) on a new dataset created just as the training set and containing
5120 image pairs. The re-training was meant to capture a gradual adjustment of the decision process
to compensate for the aging effect. Indeed, aging is known to modify the decision criteria in two-
choice decision tasks (Ratcliff, Thapar, & McKoon, 2006). The severity of each type of impairment, con-
trolled by the parameter m, was set to yield an average decline in performance (across 30 replications)
that matched the decline observed in human participants. This procedure yielded a global impairment
characterised by m=0.20 and, alternatively, an inhibitory impairment with m=0.25.
We performed 30 replications of each type of impairment (i.e. global vs. inhibitory) and we as-
sessed the model’s performance in numerosity comparison on the test dataset. Individual wfs were
separately computed for the two conditions (size-controlled vs. area-controlled) of the test dataset.
A control population of 30 unimpaired networks was created and assessed after applying the same
readjustment procedure used for the impaired networks (to ensure that the observed differences were
not due differences in training regimen). Test and re-training dataset were independently drawn for
each replication.
4. Results of the computational model
The wf values obtained in the simulation (computed for each replication) were submitted to a
mixed ANOVA with condition (area-controlled vs. dot size-controlled) as within-subject factor and
impairment type (unimpaired, inhibitory, global) as between-subject factor. Both factors, and impor-
tantly, their interaction, affected numerosity discrimination (impairment: F
(2,87)
= 26, p< 0.001; condi-
tion: F
(1,87)
= 212, p< 0.001; interaction: F
(2,87)
= 187, p< 0.001). Planned contrasts revealed that both
types of impairment worsened discrimination performance relative to the unimpaired models, with
overall wf (i.e., across conditions) increasing from 0.249 to 0.313 after global impairment
(t(58) = 9.8, p< 0.001) and to 0.315 after inhibition impairment (t
(58)
= 5.5, p< 0.001), see Fig. 4. Per-
formance of the two types of impaired model was not statistically different (t
(58)
= 0.2, p= 0.85). Crit-
ically, the inhibition impairment specifically worsened discrimination on area-controlled stimuli
(wf = 0.384) relative to dot-size controlled stimuli (wf = 0.246; t
(29)
= 20.6, p< 0.001). Global
40 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
impairment yielded similar discrimination performance in both conditions (area-controlled:
wf = 0.311, dot-size controlled: wf = 0.315; t
(29)
= 0.5, p= 0.59). The unimpaired models showed a
performance comparable to that of our young adult participants in both conditions, with a slight
advantage on the congruent (wf = 0.244) relative to the incongruent (wf = 0.253) stimuli (t
(29)
= 2.1,
p= 0.044). Finally, the inhibition impairment deteriorated performance (relative to that of the
unimpaired models) in the incongruent condition (t
(29)
= 8.6, p< 0.001) but not in the congruent
condition (t
(29)
= 0.2, p= 0.85).
In sum, just as in the elderly human participants, specific impairment of inhibition caused a large
decrease of performance on stimuli in which task-relevant and irrelevant features compete. Con-
versely, an equally strong global impairment caused a decline in performance that was identical across
conditions. Reduced inhibition of irrelevant information is therefore critical to explain the specific pat-
tern of numerosity discrimination performance in elderhood.
5. Discussion
We investigated whether numerical and arithmetical skills may be impaired in healthy ageing. We
first studied whether normally ageing participants had impaired number acuity – which tests a foun-
dational numerical skill measured in terms of Weber fraction, wf – and found that older participants
showed a significantly larger wf relative to young participants. This is in line with the results of Hal-
berda et al.’s internet mega-study (Halberda et al., 2012), and may indicate impaired numerosity pro-
cessing. However, a novel and more detailed analysis showed that this larger wf, i.e. impaired
numerosity processing, could be due to a specific inability to process trials in which numerosity
was incongruent with other measures of continuous quantity (i.e., where the least numerous set con-
tained dots of larger size), whilst congruent trials (i.e., those where the more numerous set had a lar-
ger cumulative area) were performed equally accurately in both populations. This impairment in
incongruent trials did not correspond to difficulties in other numerical or arithmetical problems which
were exceptionally well maintained in older participants.
Whether older participants performed the congruent numerosity trials by processing just numer-
osity or whether they successfully combined information about numerosity with information about
other continuous dimensions like the area covered by the dot stimuli may be difficult to establish. In-
deed, even if numerosity can effectively be extracted in the context of other dimensions such as cumu-
lative area or dot size (e.g., Stoianov & Zorzi, 2012), information from these dimensions can influence
numerosity judgments (Gebuis & Reynvoet, 2012; Hurewitz et al., 2006; Stoianov & Zorzi, submitted
for publication). However, older and younger participants’ wf were very similar in the congruent num-
erosity trials therefore suggesting that either both groups consistently used an area-based strategy to
perform the numerosity task, or alternatively that they both mainly used a numerosity-based strategy.
The first possibility seems unlikely as participants would have had to continuously switch between
Fig. 4. Computational model results. Psychometric functions (PF) indicating the model’s numerosity discrimination perfor-
mance (averaged across networks) for each type of impairment (unimpaired, global and inhibition impaired) and test dataset
(area-controlled vs. size-controlled).
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 41
Author's personal copy
strategies (e.g. between an area-based and a non area-based strategy), which seems inefficient, and
was not corroborated by differences in response times. Moreover, in older participants this switching
might have resulted in faster response times in the numerosity task with blocked conditions, which
we did not find. The second possibility is that both groups used a numerosity-based strategy which
had to be efficiently combined with inhibitory processes in the numerosity incongruent trials. We sug-
gest that ageing participants may have used a numerosity-based strategy to perform the numerosity
task and that their larger wf can be explained by impoverished inhibitory processes.
This hypothesis was supported by the results of tasks assessing inhibitory processes independent
from numerosity. We found that elderly’s performance in these tasks was significantly different from
younger participants and that it correlated with performance in the incongruent numerosity trials.
Therefore, the more difficult it was to inhibit task-irrelevant information, the worse was the perfor-
mance in the incongruent numerosity trials. We further tested the hypothesis that performance in
incongruent numerosity trials may depend on the integrity of inhibitory processes using a computa-
tional model. This showed that a general degradation of the number system resulted in a larger wf
with no difference between dot-size and area-controlled trials. However, a more specific degradation
of the inhibitory processes resulted in a significantly larger impairment in processing the area-con-
trolled numerosity trials, which resembled our older participants’ performance.
Approximating larger numerosities (i.e. more than 10 elements) or ‘number acuity’ (Halberda et al.,
2008) has been shown to be either well maintained (Gandini et al., 2008; Trick et al., 1996; Watson
et al., 2005) or conversely impaired in older participants (Halberda et al., 2012). However, different
aims or sub-optimal methodological aspects of some of these previous studies make it difficult to
interpret some of the reported results. Similar to these earlier studies, we aimed to investigate the pat-
tern of spared or impaired numeracy skills in elderly participants, but we also sought to provide fur-
ther and more specific information on what exactly lies behind elderly’s number performance. For the
first time, our results of ageing participants’ maintained performance in congruent numerosity trials
but impaired in incongruent ones points at two fundamental processes intrinsic to numerosity dis-
crimination: the abstraction of numerosity in a set and the inhibition of task-irrelevant information.
We suggest that the first of these two processes was maintained in the ageing group as indicated
by an unchanged wf in numerosity congruent trials relative to younger participants and by maintained
arithmetical performance. In contrast, inhibitory processes were partially defective in ageing partici-
pants and we suggest that this is what affected performance in the incongruent numerosity trials.
Such distinction between abstraction and inhibitory processes in numerosity discrimination is likely
to be undetected in younger populations, or even in elderly if the experimental design does not allow
distinguishing between congruent and incongruent trials, or if performance measures do not allow fi-
ner analyses.
Inhibitory processes have so far been suggested to account for elderly’s impaired performance in
several processes, for instance memory and language, based on the ‘Inhibitory Deficit’ Theory (Hasher
et al., 2007; Healey, Campbell, & Hasher, 2008). Here we suggest that these processes may also ac-
count for the impoverished performance we observed in the numerosity trials that required inhibitory
processes in order to be accurately judged. It may also be argued that in ageing participants the
impairment lies at the encoding level (Stoltzfus, Hashe, & Zacks, 1996). That is, older participants
may not have encoded numerosity information properly and this in turn may account for the larger
wf. However, this seems less likely because an encoding problem, which would make information dif-
ficult to retrieve, would have also affected the congruent numerosity trials.
Our novel, combined approach allowed us to avoid the misleading conclusion that ageing affects
numerosity discrimination. Instead, intact number acuity and arithmetical skills in ageing participants
adds to previous observations that despite cognitive decline in some functions, the ageing brain is able
to maintain other skills like vocabulary, syntax and semantic memory (e.g. Hedden & Gabrieli, 2004).
However, differently from these education-related skills whose maintenance may partly be due to
increasing practice with age, undamaged number abilities are likely to reflect the preservation of a
system that is at least partially unrelated to education and language and that is thought to be robustly
embedded in the human brain (Feigenson et al., 2004). This is in line with the evidence that quanti-
fication processes, albeit in a more rudimental form, are already present in children and infants
(Lipton & Spelke, 2003), and are relatively better maintained than other processes following brain
42 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
lesions (Cappelletti, Butterworth, & Kopelman, 2012). The idea that numerical and arithmetical skills
are resilient to normal ageing because they rely on a primitive and approximate number system (see
Hasher & Zacks, 1979 for a similar view), is consistent with the idea that primitive systems tend to be
more robust to ageing (Lemaire & Lecacheur, 2007; Trick et al., 1996). This is because primitive
systems tend to be innate or to be acquired earlier in life and this may put them in a stronger position
relative to more complex and recently acquired skills (Trick et al., 1996). It is also possible that the
number system relies on brain areas that are naturally less affected by ageing (Nyberg et al., 2012)
or where decline begins later, a working hypothesis for future studies.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by a Royal Society Dorothy Hodgkin Fellowship, a Royal Society and a
British Academy Research Grants (M.C), and by Grant No. 210922 from the European Research Council
(M.Z.). We thank Brian Butterworth for his comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.
Appendix A. Supplementary material
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.11.004.
References
Ansari, D. (2008). Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
9, 278–291.
Baddeley A. D., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). Doors and people: A test of visual and verbal recall and recognition. Bury St.
Edmunds, England: Thames Valley Test Company.
Cappelletti, M., & Cipolotti, L. (2011). The neuropsychological assessment and treatment of calculation disorders. In Halligan,
Kischka, & Marshall (Eds.). Handbook of clinical neuropsychology. Oxford University Press.
Cappelletti, M., Butterworth, B., & Kopelman, M. (2012). Numerical abilities in patients with focal and progressive neurological
disorders: A neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology, 26, 1–19.
Castel, A. D. (2005). Memory for grocery prices in younger and older adults: The role of schematic support. Psychology and Aging,
20, 718–721.
Castel, A. D. (2007). Aging and memory for numerical information: The role of specificity and expertise in associative memory.
Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 62, 194–196.
Dakin, S., Tibber, M., Greenwood, J., Kingdom, F. A., & Morgan, M. (2011). A common visual metric for approximate number and
density. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 19552–19557.
Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (1993). Development of elementary numerical abilities: A neuronal model. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 5, 390–407.
Duverne, S., & Lemaire, P. (2005). Arithmetic split effects reflect strategy selection: An adult age comparative study in addition
verification and comparison tasks. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 262–278.
El Yagoubi, R., Lemaire, P., & Besson, M. (2005). Effects of aging on arithmetic problem solving: An event-related brain potential
study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 37–50.
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identication of a target letter in a nonsearch task.
Perception & psychophysics, 16, 143–149.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional
networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347.
Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 307–314.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). ‘‘Mini-mental state’’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of
patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.
Gandini, D., Lemaire, P., & Dufau, S. (2008). Older and young adults’ strategies in approximative quantification. Acta Psychologica,
129, 175–189.
Gandini, D., Lemaire, P., & Michel, B. F. (2009). Approximate quantification in young, healthy older adults, and Alzheimer
patients. Brain and Cognition, 70, 53–61.
Geary, D. C., & Lin, J. (1998). Numerical cognition: Age-related differences in the speed of executing biologically primary and
biologically secondary processes. Experimental Aging Research, 24, 101–137.
Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2012). The role of visual information in numerosity estimation. PLoS One, 7, e37426.
Grady, C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nature Review Neuroscience, 20, 491–505.
Halberda, J., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the ‘‘Number Sense’’: The Approximate Number
System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1457–1465.
Halberda, J., Lya, R., Wilmerb, J. B., Naimana, D. Q., & Germine, L. (2012). Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a
massive Internet-based sample. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.http://
dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1200196109.
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 43
Author's personal copy
Halberda, J., Mazzocco, M., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths
achievement. Nature, 455, 665–668.
Hasher, L., Lustig, C., & Zacks, R. T. (2007). Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane,
A. Miyake, & A. J. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 227–249). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108,
356–388.
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.).
The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., & May, C. P. (1999). Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention
& performance, XVII, cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Healey, M. K., Campbell, K. L., & Hasher, L. (2008). Cognitive aging and increased distractibility: Costs and potential benefits. In
W. S. Sossin, J. C. Lacaille, V. F. Castellucci, & S. Belleville (Eds.). Progress in brain research (Vol. 169, pp. 353–363).
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Hedden, T., & Gabrieli, J. (2004). Insight into the aging mind: A view from cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5,
87–97.
Henik, A., & Tzelgov, J. (1982). Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks.
Memory & Cognition, 10, 389–395.
Hurewitz, F., Gelman, R., & Schnitzer, B. (2006). Sometimes area counts more than number. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 19599–19604.
Izard, V., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Dehaene, S. (2008). Distinct cerebral pathways for object identity and number in human
infants. PLoS Biology, 6(2), e11.
Jackson, M., & Warrington, E. K. (1986). Arithmetic skills in patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Cortex, 22, 611–620.
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect,
response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70.
Lemaire, P., & Arnaud, L. (2008). Young and older adults’ strategies in complex arithmetic. American Journal of Psychology, 121,
1–16.
Lemaire, P., & Lecacheur, M. (2007). Aging and numerosity estimation. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 62,
P305–P312.
Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2010). Dopaminergic modulation of cognition across the life span. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 625–638.
Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., & Sikström, S. (2001). Aging cognition: From neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 5, 479–486.
Lipton, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2003). Origins of number sense: Large number discrimination in human infants. Psychological Science,
15, 396–401.
May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 363–379.
Mazzocco, M. M. M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011a). Preschoolers’ precision of the approximate number system predicts
later school mathematics performance. PLoS One, 6(9), e23749.
Mazzocco, M. M. M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011b). Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies
mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). Child Development, 82, 1224–1237.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive
functions and their contributions to complex ‘‘frontal lobe’’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.
Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 16, 292–305.
Piazza, M., Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Berteletti, I., Conte, S., Lucangeli, D., et al (2010). Developmental trajectory of number
acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia. Cognition, 116, 33–41.
Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human
intraparietal sulcus. Neuron, 44, 547–555.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 19–45.
Pouille, F., Marin-Burgin, A., Adesnik, H., Atallah, B. V., & Scanziani, M. (2009). Input normalization by global feedforward
inhibition expands cortical dynamic range. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1577–1585.
Ratcliff, R., Thapar, A., & McKoon, G. (2006). Aging and individual differences in rapid two-choice decisions. Psychonomic Bulletin
and Review, 13, 626–635.
Roitman, J., Brannon, E. M., & Platt, M. L. (2007). Monotonic coding of numerosity in macaque lateral intraparietal area. PLoS
Biolology, 5, e208.
Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions in working memory and speed of
processing. Psychological Science, 2, 179–183.
Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive
decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 566–594.
Salthouse, T. A., & Kersten, A. W. (1993). Decomposing adult age differences in symbol arithmetic. Memory & Cognition, 2,
699–710.
Stoianov I., & Zorzi, M. (submitted for publication). The influence of continuous visual properties on numerosity estimation: No
challenge for the Approximate Number System.
Stoianov, I., & Zorzi, M. (2012). Emergence of a ‘‘visual number sense’’ in hierarchical generative models. Nature Neuroscience, 15,
194–196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2996.
Stoltzfus, E. R., Hashe, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1996). Working memory and aging: Current status of the inhibitory view. In J. T. E.
Richardson, R. W. Engle, L. Hasher, R. H. Logie, E. R. Stoltzfus, & R. T. Zacks (Eds.), Counterpoints in cognition: Working memory
and human cognition (pp. 66–68). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
44 M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45
Author's personal copy
Sutton, R. S., & Barto, A. G. (1981). Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: Expectation and prediction. Psychological
Review, 88, 135–170.
Trick, L. M., Enns, J. T., & Brodeur, D. A. (1996). Life span changes in visual enumeration: The number discrimination task.
Developmental Psychology, 32, 925–932.
Tzelgov, J., Meyer, J., & Henik, A. (1992). Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 166–179.
Watson, D. G., Maylor, E. A., & Bruce, L. A. M. (2005). Effects of age on searching for and enumerating targets that cannot be
detected efficiently. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 1119–1142.
Watson, D. G., Maylor, E. A., & Bruce, L. A. M. (2007). The role of eye movements in subitizing and counting. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1389–1399.
Watson, D. G., Maylor, E. A., & Manson, N. J. (2002). Aging and enumeration: A selective deficit for the subitization of targets
among distractors. Psychology and Aging, 17, 496–504.
Wechsler, D. (1995). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence.The Psychological Corporation. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace &
Company.
M. Cappelletti et al. / Cognitive Psychology 69 (2014) 25–45 45
... On the other hand, less is known about discrimination processing during ageing or specific neurological conditions. An important aspect to clarify is whether magnitude processing is sensitive to ageing, due to a decline of a specific system as happens for other functions or, alternatively, whether the age-related decline is the result of deterioration of other cognitive processes involved in magnitude discrimination (Cappelletti et al. 2014;Halberda et al., 2012). ...
... duration or numerosity), and never the comparison between the three dimensions (e.g. Capizzi et al. 2022;Cappelletti et al. 2014). Most of the studies have been conducted investigating temporal abilities in ageing and showed lower temporal abilities and higher variability in old compared to younger participants (Block et al. 1998;Wearden 2005). ...
... The results showed a flatter psychometric curve with decreasing MMSE scores and increasing age, while no shift of the psychometric function was observed, indicating that lower temporal abilities observed were mainly caused by a deficit at the level of cognitive control functions rather than of altered temporal processing per se (see also, Turgeon et al. 2016). Similarly, it has been proposed that a decline of the inhibitory processes could be responsible for a lower performance in numerosity discrimination tasks in elderly compared to young participants (Cappelletti et al. 2014;Norris et al. 2015). Length processing during ageing was less investigated. ...
Article
Full-text available
People constantly process temporal, numerical, and length information in everyday activities and interactions with the environment. However, it is unclear whether quantity perception changes during ageing. Previous studies have provided heterogeneous results, sometimes showing an age-related effect on a particular quantity, and other times reporting no differences between young and elderly samples. However, three dimensions were never compared within the same study. Here, we conducted two experiments with the aim of investigating the processing of duration, numerosity and length in both healthy and pathological ageing. The experimental paradigm consisted of three bisection tasks in which participants were asked to judge whether the presented stimulus (i.e. a time interval, a group of dots, or a line) was more similar to the short/few or long/many standards. The first study recruited healthy young and elderly participants, while the second recruited healthy elderly participants and patients with Parkinson’s disease, a clinical condition commonly associated with temporal impairments. The results of both experiments showed that discrimination precision differed between domains in all groups, with higher precision in the numerosity task and lower sensitivity in judging duration. Furthermore, while discrimination abilities were affected in healthy elderly and, even more so, in Parkinson’s disease group, no domain-specific impairments emerged. According to our research, reduced discrimination precision might be explained by an alteration of a single system for all quantities or by an age-related general cognitive decline.
... Some researchers show that reduced mathematical abilities lead to lower income and less financial security in everyday life (Butterworth et al., 2011). According to numerosity comparison, several studies have demonstrated age-related differences in the performance of such tasks (Cappelletti et al., 2014;Norris et al., 2015). A study described by Halberda et al. (2012), with more than 10.000 participants aged 11-85 showed that this ability begins to decline after age 30, which is probably attributed to a deficit in executive functions (Gandini et al., 2009). ...
... Also in line with these results are the findings of an earlier study (Roquet and Lemaire, 2019), in which the ability of numerosity comparison was tested in two age groupsamong adults (mean age = 21.1) and among older people (mean age = 74.8). The results of this study showed that older people were significantly more likely to choose simpler cognitive strategies (based on only one visual aspect of the presented stimuli) than younger adults -this difference was evident in the non-concurrent variant of the task, which requires participants to engage more of their available cognitive resources (Cappelletti et al., 2014;Clayton et al., 2015;Gilmore et al., 2016). Due to cognitive load during executing non-concurrent variant of the task, this result may provide indirect evidence of a link between the cognitive functioning of different age groups and the cognitive strategies they use. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Studies have shown age-related differences in numerical cognition, for example, in the level of numerosity comparison ability. Moreover, some studies point out individual differences in the cognitive strategies employed during the performance of numerosity comparison tasks and reveal that they are related to the aging process. One probable cause of these differences is the level of cognitive functioning. The aim of our study was to determine the relationships among numerosity comparison ability, the cognitive strategies utilized in the performance of numerosity comparison tasks and the general cognitive functioning in older people. Methods Forty-seven elderly people participated in the study. The participants were examined using overall cognitive functioning scales and computerized numerosity comparison task. Results The results showed many correlations between the participants’ level of cognitive functioning and the percent of correct responses (PCR) and response time (RT) during numerosity comparison, as well as with the cognitive strategies applied by the participants. Task correctness was positively related to the level of performance in the attention and executive function tasks. In contrast, the long-term memory resources index and visuospatial skills level were negatively correlated with RT regarding numerosity comparison task performance. The level of long-term memory resources was also positively associated with the frequency of use of more complex cognitive strategies. Series of regression analyses showed that both the level of general cognitive functioning and the cognitive strategies employed by participants in numerosity comparison can explain 9–21 percent of the variance in the obtained results. Discussion In summary, these results showed significant relationships between the level of cognitive functioning and proficiency in numerosity comparison measured in older people. Moreover, it has been shown that cognitive resources level is related to the strategies utilized by older people, which indicates the potential application for cognitive strategy examinations in the development of new diagnostic tools.
... In contrast, an age-related decrease in the distance effect was found when controlling for the higher variance in older adults by using standardization of speed (Study 2). This suggests that aging might indeed affect basic symbolic number skills, probably more so in older age, as it was shown for non-symbolic number skills 2,23 . ...
Article
Full-text available
While general cognitive skills decline during aging, numerical skills seem to be mainly preserved. Such skills are essential for an independent life up to old age, e.g., when dealing with money or time. Operating with numbers usually requires number magnitude and place-value processing. The question is whether these processes are negatively affected by aging due to the general cognitive decline or positively affected due to lifelong experience with numbers. Therefore, we investigated age-related changes in the distance and compatibility effects in single-digit, two-digit, and four-digit number comparison. On the one hand, older adults took longer for number processing and showed a smaller distance effect, indicating altered number magnitude representations. On the other hand, older adults were better in place-value processing as indicated by a smaller compatibility effect than in younger adults. We conclude that aging differentially affects basic numerical skills.
... No studies have directly examined this possibility; however, some evidence supports it. For example, researchers found the congruency effect when dot arrays were presented for 200 ms (Cappelletti et al., 2014;Norris & Castronovo, 2016;Zhang et al., 2019) or 300 ms (Gebuis & Reynvoet, 2012;Lindskog et al., 2021;Pekár & Kinder, 2020); however, no congruency effect was observed in the study by Odic et al. (2013), in which dot arrays were presented for 1500 ms. Therefore, we hypothesized that a long array presentation time might induce more inhibitory control in participants than a short presentation condition. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies have found that inhibitory control plays an important role in non-symbolic numerical processing. However, this role may be influenced by the visual cue control method or the stimulus' presentation time. We investigated these questions by conducting three experiments using a priming paradigm to compare the level of inhibitory control in a sequential dot comparison task with single-dimensional and multi-dimensional control of visual cues under two presentation time conditions (300 ms and 1500 ms). We found that neither the method of visual cue control nor the presentation time of dot arrays affected the level of inhibitory control in the dot comparison task. These results reveal a stable role of inhibitory control in non-symbolic numerical processing, providing further evidence for integrating numerical and visual information during non-symbolic numerical processing.
... However, the authors do not provide a specific explanation as to which cognitive mechanism is responsible for the filtering process, e.g., whether it is domain-specific or domain-general. One possibility would be a domain-general inhibitory system that controls the interference of visual and numerical information (Houdé et al., 2011;Cappelletti et al., 2014). Another possibility is that filtering is domainspecific and, as a consequence, a domain-specific inhibitory mechanism is involved. ...
Article
Full-text available
It has long been debated how humans estimate the numerosity of sets of elements and what role continuous visual properties play in this process. The dot comparison task, in which the more numerous of two dot arrays must be selected, is a dominant method to investigate this phenomenon. It has been shown that the visual properties of the two dot patterns strongly influence the comparison. This influence can be systematically investigated by manipulating visual properties congruently and incongruently with numerosity. However, it remains unclear how learning and prior experience affect the influence of the visual properties. To address this question, we introduced feedback into the classical dot comparison task: during the learning phase, participants in the experimental group received feedback after each trial indicating whether their answer was correct whereas participants in the control group did not. After the learning phase, neither group received feedback. The convex hull of the dot patterns and the average dot diameter were manipulated congruently and incongruently with numerosity. Our results show that feedback had no effect on overall performance. However, when manipulated separately, dot diameter no longer affected performance in the experimental group after the learning phase, but it did in the control group. Moreover, this effect remained visible even when diameter and convex hull were manipulated simultaneously. This pattern of results is consistent with the notion of sensory integration which proposes that weights are assigned to different visual cues and that numerical judgments depend on an additive combination of these weights. We also found a correlation between performance on an arithmetic task and performance on trials in which dot size was manipulated incongruently with numerosity. However, there were no correlations between an inhibition task and performance in the dot comparison task. Taken together, the current results suggest that learning with feedback may affect some visual properties but not others. Future studies should further investigate a wider range of visual properties to examine which of them can be influenced by learning and under what conditions learning occurs.
... Visual features positively correlate with numerosity in the congruent trials, while in the incongruent trials, they are manipulated to correlate negatively with numerosity. In general, participants tend to be more accurate and faster on congruent than incongruent trials (e.g., Cappelletti et al., 2014), but this effect is not always significant (e.g., Odic et al., 2013). Gilmore et al. (2016) suggest that differences between congruent and incongruent trials can be explained partly by the lack of control of the stimuli's visual properties across studies since different algorithms have been used throughout the years. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Number sense has been systematically measured using dot comparison tasks. However, recent studies have reported that performance on dot comparison might be influenced inhibitory control and visual properties of dot arrays. In the present study, we analysed the influence of continuous magnitude, inhibitory control, and numerical ratio on the dot comparison performance of preschool children. Methods Participants were 517 preschool children from 13 different schools in Chile. Children completed a dot comparison and two inhibitory control tasks. Gebuis and Reynvoet method was used to create well-controlled dot arrays for use in the dot comparison task. A logistic mixed effects model was conducted to predict participants’ dot comparison accuracy. Continuous magnitude and ratio were entered as level-1 predictors and inhibitory control as level-2 predictors. Results The results showed that all predictors made a significant contribution to dot comparison accuracy. Furthermore, a significant double interaction (inhibitory control x continuous magnitude) and a triple interaction (inhibitory control x continuous magnitude x ratio) showed that the contribution of inhibitory control skills in dot comparison accuracy depends on the continuous properties of dot arrays and ratio. Discussion These findings suggest that preschool children rely more on continuous magnitudes than numerosity in dot comparison tasks. They also indicate that the greater children’s inhibitory control, the more able they are to respond based on numerosity in fully incongruent trials, particularly when ratio is low (easiest items). Taken together, the above findings support the competing processes account provided that both ANS and inhibitory control skills influence performance on dot comparison tasks.
... The increasing aging population has raised an urgent need for research on methods that will enable early and effective prediction and diagnosis of cognitive deficits that occur with age. Several studies have demonstrated age-related differences in the performance of tasks used to measure basic numerical capacities (Cappelletti et al., 2014;Norris et al., 2015). It might indicate that this skills can be a predictor of cognitive function impairment. ...
Poster
Full-text available
Recent studies have shown age-related differences in the level of such basic numerical skills as numerosity comparison (Norris et al., 2015). It has been shown that there are individual differences in the cognitive strategies used during this task performance (Roquet & Lemaire, 2019). However, the principles of these differences are still unknown, but the level of cognitive functioning is likely one of them. The aim of the short pilot study is to determine the relationship between general cognitive functioning and basic numerical skills in two groups: younger (aged 63-67) and older (aged 73-77) seniors. The study will involve 50 elderly people with no neurological or psychiatric problems, divided into the two groups mentioned above. Participants will be asked to attend a meeting in which their general cognitive functioning (with the use of psychological tests) and basic numerical skills (numerosity comparison in a computerized task) with simultaneous registration of eye movements will be examined. Two standard screening tools will be used to assess multiple cognitive functions: the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). Reaction time and correctness of response in the performance of numerosity comparisons will be measured and analyzed, and analysis of parameters such as the number of fixations and saccades and the duration of fixation will provide markers of the cognitive strategies chosen by participants. Thus, the study will test the effectiveness of a method programmed to measure basic numerical skills (comparing sets of dots) while simultaneously measuring the participant’s eye movements.
Article
Theories have ventured to explain how the human brain has gained the unique ability to develop mathematical concepts and theories. One theory states that mathematics has appeared as a by-product of the human language faculty (Chomsky, 2006). Alternatively, recent cognitive neuroscience research have postulated that mathematics arose from non-linguistic intuitions of numbers and space (Dehaene, 2011; Dillon et al., 2013). Indeed, studies have observed that infants (Starkey & Cooper, 1980; Starkey et al., 1983) and Amazonian indigenous adults with no education in Mathematics and who do not possess number words (Dehaene et al. 2008, Pica et al., 2004) display abstract proto-mathematical intuitions of arithmetic and geometry. These ‘core knowledges’ (Spelke & Kinzler, 2007) have been theorized to be the ontological building blocks that scaffold our ability for representing numbers symbolically and performing exact arithmetic (Starr et al., 2013). They have been found to be predictive of later, more complex mathematical abilities (Gilmore et al., 2010; Halberda et al., 2008; Starr et al., 2013), supporting the idea that complex mathematics would arise from core representations of number and space (Amalric & Dehaene, 2016). Extensive research has shown that numerical and spatial cognition were tightly connected (Hubbard et al., 2005). In some contexts, this relation between number and visuospatial dimensions, such as spatial extent or density, can lead to interferences which can hinder the development of math abilities.
Article
In this handbook, the world's leading researchers answer fundamental questions about dyslexia and dyscalculia based on authoritative reviews of the scientific literature. It provides an overview from the basic science foundations to best practice in schooling and educational policy, covering research topics ranging from genes, environments, and cognition to prevention, intervention and educational practice. With clear explanations of scientific concepts, research methods, statistical models and technical terms within a cross-cultural perspective, this book will be a go-to reference for researchers, instructors, students, policymakers, educators, teachers, therapists, psychologists, physicians and those affected by learning difficulties.
Article
Full-text available
There has long been interest in how the mind represents numerical magnitude, particularly in the absence of symbols. For humans and nonhuman animals, number represents a core dimension of perceptual experience by which objects in the physical world are delineated. The physical world is also well characterized by other dimensions, many of which covary with number. Yet, the general consensus is that number is perceived independently of other magnitudes that co-occur with it. Here, we present evidence against the independence of number perception. In particular, we use evidence from neuroimaging, computational modeling, visual illusions, and psychophysics to introduce a novel theory of visual number encoding, wherein nonnumerical magnitude information such as cumulative surface area is encoded along with number and sustained throughout visual perception. Moreover, we propose that the experience of number per se reflects the readout of a multidimensional (i.e., integral) representation vis-à-vis selective attention, not the independent encoding of number. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter discusses the status of the inhibitory view of working memory and its relation to aging. It considers evidence for the conceptualizations of working memory that stress its purported limited capacity and presents an alternative view which focuses on the inhibitory control of the contents of working memory. The chapter also discusses current research evidence concerning adult age differences in attention, memory, and language.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract Despite their lack of language, human infants and several animal species possess some elementary abilities for numerical processing. These include the ability to recognize that a given numerosity is being presented visually or auditorily, and, at a later stage of development, the ability to compare two nume-rosities and to decide which is larger. We propose a model for the development of these abilities in a formal neuronal network. Initially, the model is equipped only with unordered numerosity detectors. It can therefore detect the numerosity of an input set and can be conditioned to react accordingly. In a later stage, the addition of a short-term memory network is shown to be sufficient for number comparison abilities to develop. Our computer simulations account for several phenomena in the numerical domain, including the distance effect and Fechner's law for numbers. They also demonstrate that infants' numerosity detection abilities may be explained without assuming that infants can count. The neurobiological bases of the critical components of the model are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This chapter focuses on a set of attentional or executive control processes, all inhibitory, that operate in the service of an individual's goals to narrow and constrain the contents of consciousness to be goal relevant. An uncluttered or narrowly focused "working memory," rather than a large one, is the ideal processing system. The narrow focus maximizes the speed and accuracy of on-line processing because it reduces the likelihood of switching attention to goal-irrelevant representations. The work is similar to that of other investigations in its focus on executive processes as a critical source of working memory variation as well as variation in many cognitive domains. The emphasis on inhibitory processes may be the characteristic that most differentiates their work from others.
Article
Full-text available
Publisher Summary This chapter discusses the theoretical and empirical literature that addresses aging and discourse comprehension. A series of five studies guided by a particular working memory viewpoint regarding the formation of inferences during discourse processing is described in the chapter. Compensatory strategies may be used with different degrees of likelihood across the life span largely as a function of efficiency with which inhibitory mechanisms function because these largely determine the facility with which memory can be searched. The consequences for discourse comprehension in particular may be profound because the establishment of a coherent representation of a message hinges on the timely retrieval of information necessary to establish coreference among certain critical ideas. Discourse comprehension is an ideal domain for assessing limited capacity frameworks because most models of discourse processing assume that multiple components, demanding substantially different levels of cognitive resources, are involved. For example, access to a lexical representation from either a visual array or an auditory message is virtually capacity free.
Chapter
Full-text available
In 1966 the first meeting of the Association for the Study of Attention and Performance was held in the Netherlands to promote the emerging science of cognitive psychology. This volume is based on the most recent conference, held in Israel thirty years later. The focus of the conference was the interaction between theory and application. The organizers chose the specific topic, cognitive regulation of performance, because it is an area where contemporary theories of cognitive processes meet the everyday challenges posed by human interactions with complex systems. Present-day technological systems impose on the operator a variety of supervisory functions, such as input and output monitoring, allocation of cognitive resources, choice of strategies, and regulation of cognitive operations. A challenge for engineers and designers is to accommodate the cognitive requirements called for by these systems. The book is divided into four sections: the presentation and representation of information, cognitive regulation of acquisition and performance, consciousness and behavior, and special populations: aging and neurological disorders. Contributors Nicole D. Anderson, Moshe Bar, Lynn Bardell, Alice E. Barnes, Irving Biederman, Robert A. Bjork, Richard A. Block, Fergus I. M. Craik, Heiner Deubel, John Dunlosky, Ido Erev, Ronald Fisher, John M. Flach, Barry Goettl, Morris Goldsmith, Daniel Gopher, Lynn Hasher, Okihide Hikosaka, Larry L. Jacoby, Peter Kalocsai, Colleen Kelley, David E. Kieras, Roberta Klatzky, Asher Koriat, Arthur F. Kramer, Elisabetta Ladavas, John L. Larish, Susan J. Lederman, John Long, Cynthia P. May, Guiliana Mazzoni, Brian McElree, David Meyer, Satoru Miyauchi, Neville Moray, Louis Narens, Thomas O. Nelson, Raymond S. Nickerson, Lynne Reder, J. Wesley Regian, Ian Robertson, Wolfgang Schneider, Christian D. Schunn, Wayne Shebilske, Shinsuke Shimojo, Suresh Subramaniam, Tom N. Trainham, Jehoshua Tsal, Timothy A. Weber, Christopher Wickens, Rose T. Zacks, Dan Zakay Bradford Books imprint
Article
The term subitization has been used to refer to the fast and accurate enumeration of up to about 4 items. Beyond 4 items, enumeration becomes more serial and less accurate, with each additional item adding to the total enumeration time. The effects of aging on visual enumeration of targets (Os) were assessed with and without the presence of distractors (Xs). Young participants (n = 30; 18-27 years) showed the usual subitization-counting enumeration function, both with and without the presence of distractors. In contrast, for older participants (n = 35; 65-79 years), evidence for subitization was found only when distractors were absent. This occurred even though they could detect an individual target among distractors in parallel across the field. The results are discussed in relation to recent theories of visual selection and enumeration.